You are on page 1of 10

I

*
;

and Psydmmmtk~

Introduction

Special Article
Psychother Psychosom 1996;65: 14-23

Carol D. Ryp Burton Singerb


a

Department of Psycholow. university of ~isconsin:~adison, Wisc., Oflice of Population Research, Princeton University, Princeton, N.J., USA

PsychologicalWell-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and implications for Psychotherapy Research

............................................... ..................................................................................................
Key Words
Abstract

Self-acceptance Purpose in life Positive relationships Personal growth Autonomy Environmental mastery . Sociodemographic differences Vulnerability Resilience Quality of life

......................

A model of positive psychological functioning that emerges from diverse domains of theory and philosophy is presented. Six key dimensions of wellness are defined, and empirical research summarizing their empirical translation and sociodemographic correlates is presented. Variations in well-being are explored via studies of discrete life events and enduring human experiences. Life histories of the psychologically vulnerable and resilient, defined via the cross-classification of depression and well-being, are summarized. Implications of the focus on positive functioning for research on psychotherapy, quality of life, and mindlbody linkages are reviewed.

Historically, mental health research is dramatically weighted on the side of psychological dysfunction. That is, studies of positive psychological functioning are minuscule compared to investigations of mental problems. In fact, the meaning of basic terms, such as mental health, are negatively biased - typical usage equates health with the absence of illness rather than the presence of wellness. Such formulations ignore human capacities and needs for flourishing as well as the protective features associated with being well. This article summarizes the development of a multidimensional model of positive psychological functioning and reviews empirical research folllowing froni it. The key components of well-being are first presented, with emphasis given to their theoretical and philosophical underpinnings. Operational definitions of the six dimensions are provided, and empirical research regarding their mea-

surement properties and sociodemographic correlates is briefly described. Life event and life history studies attempting to explain variations in well-being are also reviewed. Pertinent to psychotherapy research, final sections examine the implications of a focus on well-being for studies of vulnerability to psychological dysfunction, recovery from psychological disorders (resilience), and the need to expand quality of life indicators to include assessment of the positive in people's lives.

The Meaning and Measurement of Well-Being

An extensive theoretical literature has addressed the meaning of positive psychological functioning. This work includes Maslow's [I] conception of self-actualization Roger's [2] view of the fully functioning person, Jung's [3, 41 formulation of individuation, and Allport's [5] concep tion of maturity. A further domain of theory for defining
Carol D. Ryff, PhD Institute or Aging 2245 Medical Sciences Center 1300 Un~versity Avenue Mad~son, 53706 (USA) WI

KARGER
EMail karger@karger.ch Fax+4161 3061234

Q 1996 S. Karger AG, Basel

I
I

psychological well-being comes from life-span develop mental perspectives, which emphasize the differing challenges confronted at various phases of the life cycle. Included here are Erikson's [6] psychosocial stage model, Buhler's [7, 81 basic life tendencies that work toward the fulfillment of life and Neugarten's [9,101 descriptions of personality change in adulthood and old age. Jahoda's [I I] positive criteria of mental health, generated to replace definitions of well-being as the absence of illness, also offer extensive descriptions of what it means to be in good psychological health. When one reviews the characteristics of well-being described in these formulations [12], it becomes apparent that many theorists were writing about similar features of positive psychological functioning. These points of convergence in the prior theories constitute the core dimensions of the model of well-being pursued in the following program of research. Each dimension is briefly described below. SelfAcceptance. The most recurrent criterion of wellbeing evident in the previous perspectives is the individual's sense of self-acceptance. This is defined as a central feature of mental health as well as characteristic of selfactualization, optimal functioning, and maturity. Life span theories also emphasize acceptance of one's self and one's past life. Thus, holding positive attitudes toward oneself emerges as a central characteristic of positive psychological functioning. Positive Relations with Others. Many of the preceding theories emphasize the importance of warm, trusting interpersonal relations. The ability to love is viewed as a central component of mental health. Self-actualizers are described as having strong feelings of empathy and affecton for all human beings and as being capable of greater love, deeper friendship, and more complete identification with others. Warm relating to others is posed as a criterion of maturity. Adult developmental stage theories also emphasize the achievement of close unions with others (intimacy) and the guidance and direction of others (generativity). Thus, the importance of positive relations with others is repeatedly stressed in conceptions of psychological well-being. Autonomy. There is considerable emphasis in the prior literature on such qualities as self-determination, independence, and the regulation of behavior from within. Self-actualizers are described as showing autonomous functioning and resistance to enculturation. The fully functioning person is described as having an internal locus of evaluation, whereby one does not look to others for approval, but evaluates oneself by personal standards.
Psychological Well-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and Implications for Psychotherapy Research

Individuation is seen to involve a deliverance from convention, in which the person no longer clings to the collective fears, beliefs, and laws of the masses. The process of turning inward in the later years is also seen by life-span developmentalists to give the person a sense of freedom from the norms governing everyday life. Environmental Mastery. The individual's ability to choose or create environments suitable to his or her psychic conditions is defined as a characteristic of mental health. Maturity is seen to require participation in a significant sphere of activity outside of oneself. Life-span development is described as requiring the ability to manipulate and control complex environments. These theories emphasize one's ability to advance in the world and change it creatively through physical or mental activities. Successful aging also emphasizes the extent to which the individual takes advantage of environmental opportunities. These combined perspectives suggest that active participation in and mastery of the environment are key ingredients in an integrated framework of positive psychological functioning. Purpose in Life. Mental health is defined to include beliefs that give one the feeling that there is purpose and meaning to life. The definition of maturity also emphasizes clear comprehension of life's purpose, a sense of directedness, and intentionality. The life-span develop ment theories refer to a variety of changing purposes or goals in life, such as being productive and creative or achieving emotional integration in later life. Thus, one who functions positively has goals, intentions, and a sense of direction, all of which contribute to the feeling that life is meaningful. Personal Growth. Optimal psychological functioning requires not only that one achieve the prior characteristics, but also that one continue to develop one's potential, to grow and expand as a person. The need to actualize oneself and realize one's potential is central to clinical perspectives on personal growth. Openness to experience, for example, is a key characteristic of the fully functioning person. Such an individual is continually developing, rather than achieving a fixed state wherein all problems are solved. Life-span theories also give explicit emphasis to continued growth and to facing new challenges to tasks at different periods of life. Thus, continued growth and self-realization are prominent themes in the aforementioned theories. In sum, the integration of mental health, clinical, and life-span developmental theories points to multiple converging aspects of positive psychological functioning. Conceptually, these criteria are distinct from prominent
Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

15

empirical indicators of well-being such as happiness and and pursuing goals, attempting to realize one's potential, life satisfaction (13, 141, most of which lack theoretical experiencing deep connections to others, managing surfoundation. There are, however, notable parallels between rounding demands and opportunities, exercising selfthe formulation of wellness emerging from the literature direction, and possessing positive self-regard. We posit and philosophical perspectives on the meaning of 'the that such features of human wellness are not culturegood life' [IS]. Diverse philosophical frameworks give bound, although their phenotypic forms (i.e., how puremphasis to the individual's potential to choose and then pose is expressed), and the relative emphasis given to difcarry out projects that are valuable; that is, to have active ferent components, may be quite varied. Life purpose in pursuits that give life dignity [16-181, akin to the above Africa, as we have described elsewhere [15], may be formulation of a purpose in life. Mastery is also evident in expressed with a focus on maintaining the social order, in philosophical descriptions of self-command, which in- contrast to more individualized pursuits in our own convolve decision-making and taking action. Ideas of self- text. Similarly, people everywhere have abiding needs for love, self-esteem, and self-respect are also evident in lists deep, meaningful connections to others, although external of criteria1 goods [16], showing parallels to self-accep manifestations of these social ties may vary from exprestance. Philosophical writing about benevolence, involving sions of obligation and responsibility to experiences of the concern and affection one feels for others [I 61, com- companionship, intimacy, and love. panionship [18], love [17], and deep personal relations [19] underscore the importance of positive relations with Empirical Translation others. The dimension of personal growth parallels Aristotelian conceptions of human excellence, human flourishScale Construction and Evaluation. Self-descriptive ing, and the realization of one's true potential [20]. Eudaimonistic accounts of ethics and the good life [2 11point, in items were generated to operationalize both high and low fact, to the imperative to know onself (one's daimon) and scorers for each of the six dimensions of psychological to choose to turn it, as completely as possible, from an well-being (table I). Extensive item analyses were conideal to an actuality. Finally, philosophers emphasize a ducted to refine the final measurement scales [23]. The focus on whole lives, that is, the importance of looking at 20-item parent scales demonstrate high internal consisall aspects of an individual's life through time - an argu- tency reliability (alpha range = 0.86-0.93) and temporal ment elaborated in a long succession of texts on the histo- reliability (test-retest coefficients range from 0.81 to 0.88). The scales also correlated modestly and positively ry of ethics [16]. Definitions of the good life that emphasize happiness, with existing measures of positive functioning (e.g. life pleasure, and satisfaction (more hedonistic and epicurean satisfaction, affect balance, self-esteem, internal control) views) have been criticized by philosophers, using well- and negatively with extant measures of negative functionknown historical figures who experienced pleasure in ing (e.g. depression, powerful others, control, chance contheir lives, but were so unjust, evil, or pointless in the pur- trol), thereby demonstrating convergent and discriminant suit of their lives as to preclude a description of being validity. Recent data from a national probability sample good. Alternatively, other historical figures led desolate [24], using a dramatically reduced version of the welllives in terms of personal well-being, but were nonetheless being scales, supported, via confirmatory factor analyses, profoundly noble, creative, courageous, self-sacrificial the hypothesized six-factor structure. Among the many descriptive questions that can be 1161. John Stuart Mill [22] extends the critique of happiness, arguing that it is not to be attained if made an end in asked about well-being, we have addressed whether proitself. It is rather a byproduct of other pursuits - 'Those files of positive functioning vary across the life course, only are happy who have their minds fixed on some object whether men and women differ in basic dimensions of other than their own happiness, on the happiness of oth- wellness, and whether well-being varies by social class staers, on the improvement of mankind, even on some art or tus and across cultures. Age Differences. To explore differences, we have asked pursuit, followed not as a means, but as itself in ideal end. Aiming thus at something else, they find happiness by the young, middle-aged and old adults to rate themselves on way [p. 1151. each of the dimensions of well-being [23,25,26]. Ratings Taken together, these many lines of philosophical and from the original validation study revealed a diverse patscientific inquiry converge in their depiction of the good tern of cross-sectional differences (see top half of fig. 1). and healthy life as one that involves processes of setting Certain aspects of well-being, such as environmental mas16

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

Table 1. Definitions of theory-guided dimensions of well-being

SelJ-acceptance High scorer Possesses a positive attitude toward the self, acknowledges and accepts multiple aspects of self including good and bad qualities; feels positive about past life Low scorer Feels dissatisfied with self; is disappointed with what has occurred in past life; is troubled about certain personal qualities; wishes to be different than what he or she is Positive relations with others High scorer Has warm, satisfying, trusting relationships with others; is concerned about the welfare of others: capable of strong empathy, affection, and intimacy; understands give and take of human relationships Low scorer Has few close, trusting relationships with others; finds it difficult to be warm, open, and concerned about others; is isolated and frustrated in interpersonal relationships; not willing to make compromises to sustain important ties with others Autonomy High scorer Is self-determining and independent; able to resist social pressures to think and act in certain ways: regulates behavior from within; evaluates self by personal standards Low scorer Is concerned about the expectations and evaluations of others; relies on judgments of others to make important decisions; conforms to social pressures to think and act in certain ways Environmental mastery High scorer Has a sense of mastery and competence in managing the environment; controls complex array of external activities; makes effective use of surrounding opportunities; able to choose or create contexts suitable to personal needs and values Low scorer Has difficulty managing everyday affairs; feels unable to change or improve surrounding context; is unaware of surrounding opportunities; lacks sense of control over external world Purpose in life High scorer Has goals in life and a sense of directedness; feels there is meaning to present and past life; holds beliefs that give life purpose; has aims and objectives for living Low scorer Lacks a sense of meaning in life; has few goals or aims, lacks sense of direction; does not see purpose in past life; has no outlooks or beliefs that give life meaning Personal growth High scorer Has a feeling of continued development; sees self as growing and expanding; is open to new experiences; has sense of realizing his or her potential; sees improvement in self and behavior over time; is changing in ways that reflect more self-knowledge and effectiveness Low scorer Has a sense of personal stagnation; lack sense of improvement or expansion over time; feels bored and uninterested with life; feels unable to develop new attitudes or behaviors
-

80

Young

Midlife

Older

17 l81

9-

Young

Midlife

Older

Fig. 1. Significant age differences in the validation (a) and national (b) samples. Dimensions of well-being: = Positive relations; A = personal growth; A = purpose in life; = environmental mastery; 0 = autonomy.

tery and autonomy, showed incremental patterns with age, particularly from young adulthood to midlife. Other aspects, such as personal growth and purpose in life, showed decremental patterns, especially from midlife to old age. Still other aspects, notably positive relations with others and self-acceptance, showed no significant age differences across the three age periods and thus are not shown in the figure. Initial samples were based on community volunteers, and as such, lacked generalizability. Data from a probability sample [24] further underscore the consistency of the cross-sectional age patterns (see lower portion of fig. 1). Environmental mastery continued to show incremental patterns with age, purpose in life and personal growth showed declines with aging, and selfacceptance revealed no age differences. For self-ratings of
Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:1423

Psychological Well-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and Implications for Psychotherapy Research

17

autonomy and positive relations with others, the results across studies varied between no differences and incremental age patterns. Longitudinal studies are needed to clarify whether these age patterns reflect developmental changes or cohort differences. Whatever the explanation, the recurring reports of lower self-ratings among older adults on purpose in life and personal growth warrants attention. These patterns point to possibly important psychological challenges of later life, and related arguments that contemporary social structures lag behind the added years of life many people now enjoy. That is, opportunities for continued growth and development and for meaningful experience may be limited for current cohorts of older persons. An alternative hypothesis is that older persons place less value on personal growth and purpose in life than younger age groups. We have also had respondents rate their ideals of well-being, and these data challenge the notion that the aged no longer idealize continued self-development or purposeful living. Sex Differences. Across multiple datasets, we have found that women of all ages consistently rate themselves higher on positive relations with others than do men, and that women tend to score higher than men on personal growth. The remaining four aspects of psychological wellbeing have consistently shown no significant differences between men and women [25]. These findings are particularly relevant in light of prior mental health research, which repeatedly documents a higher incidence of certain psychological problems such as depression among women [27]. When the positive end of the mental health spectrum is considered, however, women are shown to have greater psychological strength compared to men in certain aspects of well-being, and comparable profiles with regard to other dimensions. To miss these findings is to tell an incomplete story about the mental health of women. Class Differences. Whether socioeconomic status, typically defined in terms of education, income, and occupational standing, is linked with profiles of psychological well-being has been examined in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study of educational and occupational attainment. Based on a sample of midlife adults who have been followed since their senior year in high school, the research shows higher profiles of well-being for those with higher educational attainment [28], with differences particularly evident for the dimensions of purpose in life and personal growth (for both men and women). Education remains strongly linked with well-being, even after controlling for prior life history variables (e.g., high school IQ; parental education, income, and occupational status). Higher well18

being is also evident for those with higher occupational status [29]. Findings from The National Survey of Families and Households, a nationally representative US sample, also show large education differences on all six measures of well-being [30]. Viewed from the perspective of the growing scientific literature that links social class standing to health, these findings indicate that lower positions in the social order not only increase the likelihood of negative health outcomes, they also decrease the likelihood of positive well-being [311. We submit that the presence of these goods in life affords important protective factors in the face of stress, challenge, and adversity, and for those lacking such goods, creates vulnerabilities. Cultural Differences. How culture bears on fundamental conceptions of self, self-in-relation-to-others, and health is an increasingly prevalent theme in social scientific inquiry [28]. Much discussion involves contrasts between individualisticlindependent cultures with those that are more collectivisticlinterdependent. These ideas suggest that more self-oriented aspects of well-being, such as self-acceptance or autonomy, may have greater salience in Western cultural contexts, while others-oriented dimensions of wellbeing, such as positive relations with others, might be of greater significance in Eastern, interdependent cultures. To examine these issues, self-reported psychological wellbeing was investigated in a midlife sample of US adults and a sociodemographically comparable sample of adults from South Korea [33]. It was found that, on the whole, Americans were much more likely to see positive qualities in themselves than Koreans, a finding consistent with formulations of underlying cultural variation in self-presentation (i.e., tendencies toward modesty in the East, proclivities for honorific self-evaluation in the West). Despite these main effects of culture, analyses within cultures revealed that Koreans, as predicted, showed highest self-ratings on the measure of positive relations with others, and lowest ratings for self-acceptance and personal growth. Among US respondents, personal growth was rated highest, especially for women, while autonomy, contrary to the purported emphasis on self-determination in our own culture, was rated lowest. Sex differences were the same in both cultures - women rated themselves significantly higher than men on positive relations with others and personal growth. Supportive of cultural similarities in basic features of wellness, qualitative data revealed themes of selfhood (e.g., self-realization, self-knowledge, self-reliance) as well as connection to others (e.g., faithfulness, responsibility, kindness, trust) in both Korean and US responses.

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65: 14-23

Understanding Variations in Well-Being

The above sociodemographic profiles point to social structural variations in psychological well-being. Prior research on subjective well-being [14] suggests that these broad factors, even in combination, rarely account for more than 10% of the variance in reports of happiness and life satisfaction. We propose that understanding who does and does not possess high well-being requires closer examination of the actual substance of people's lives, that is, their life experiences. The findings from two avenues of empirical inquiry are briefly summarized. The first pertains to studies of particular life events or experiences and their effects on well-being. The second addresses more complex life history analyses, and how they have been linked with psychological vulnerability and resilience. Life Event/Experience Studies. In a series of studies, we have investigated life experiences, and individuals' interpretations of these experiences, as key influences on psychological well-being [34-401. The experiences range from the having and raising of children, to growing up with an alcoholic parent, to health problems and relocation experiences in later life. These experiences vary by their location in the life course, by the nature of the challenge or task posed, and by their typicality (whether the experience is shared by many or few). Our formulation of how experiences are interpreted draws extensively on social psychological theory. For example, we are interested in how people make sense of their life experiences by comparing themselves with others (social comparison processes), by evaluating the feedback they perceive from significant others (reflected appraisals), by their understanding of the causes of their experiences (attributional processes), and by the importance they attach to such experiences (psychological centrality). Specific hypotheses regarding the influence of these interpretive processes on well-being outcomes are detailed in each study. Collectively, these investigations demonstrate that life experiences and how they are interpreted provide useful avenues for understanding human variations in wellbeing. The research on midlife parenting shows, for example, that considerable variance in adults' environmental mastery, purpose in life, self-acceptance, and depression is accounted for by parents' perceptions of how their grown children have 'turned out' and how these children compare with themselves. In later life, we have found that the physical health problems of aging women, combined with their assessments of how they compare with other older women, explain substantial variation in reports of personal growth, positive relations with others, autonoPsychological Well-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and Implications for Psychotherapy Research

my, depression, and anxiety. Importantly, this work shows that older women in poor physical health who engage in positive social comparison processes, have comparable psychological well-being to that of women in good physical health. Our longitudinal research on community relocation among aging women shows how changes in central features of the self-concept can enhance well-being during a life transition. Thus, multiple experiences and diverse avenues for interpreting them point to gains or losses in psychological well-being through time. Life History Agenda: Pathways to Vulnerability and Resilience. The above works focus on particular life events or experiences and how they influence well-being. Individual's lives are, however, a composite of many events and experiences. To capture this richer, more complex set of antecedents, we are currently conducting life history research with the members of the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS). We approach the life histories from the perspective of the limited differences theory [41], which emphasizes the events to which individuals are exposed over their lives and their reactions to these events. Over short time intervals, events and reactions to them produce small, or limited, differences in outcome measures. Such small differences tend to accumulate, however, and over the life course, these cumulative effects can substantially influence mental health. We elaborate and extend these conceptual principles in the WLS [42]. Key hypotheses are that: (a) adversity and its cumulation over time have negative metal health consequences; (b) advantage and its cumulation over time have positive mental health consequences; (c) reactions to adversity and advantage can heighten or reduce the impact of life experiences; (d) position in social hierarchies through time has consequences for mental health, and (e) social relationships can heighten or reduce the impact of life experiencesand enduring conditions. The life history data to which we apply these principles consist of 36 years of information regarding the respondents' family background, intelligence, adolescent aspirations and resources, education and training, job characteristics, marriage and parenting, social support and social participation, health and health-related behaviors, social comparisons, and acute events. Using the above conceptual guides, we examine the life histories of four specific mental health groups that emerge from the cross classification of positive and negative mental health indicators. The depressed/unwell are those with prior episode@)of major depression who also lack high psychological well-being (across the six dimensions). The healthy are those with high levels of well-being and who
Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

19

have no history of depression. The resilient are individuals with a prior history of depression, but who report high current well-being. The vulnerable are those with no history of depression and who have low levels of well-being. Our methodology begins with detailed biographies of individuals and progresses toward a formulation of generic life histories within each of these four groups (as well as separately for men and women) [43]. The central objective of the life history analyses is to understand how a given outcome (e.g., depression, resilience) came about; that is, what particular experiences of adversity, advantage, social position, and so on, are associated with each of the four mental health groups. Analyses to date have characterized the life histories of vulnerable men and resilient women [44]. Vulnerable men are distinguished from other men in the mental health typology by their low starting resources (family background) and persistently high work stress. These cumulative adversity factors are not offset by the presence of significant advantage experiences. With regard to social orderings, these men tend to lack post high school education, are in consistently low-status jobs, and have low perceived success about their own lives. Finally, they tend to have little closeness with a spouse or confidante and have experienced conflictual relationships (some times because of drinking). Such profiles of negative cumulative experience provide a partial explanation for the men's lack of positive well-being in midlife. We propose that the absence of positive 'goods' in these men's lives creates in them a vulnerability to possible future adversities, particularly those associated with aging processes. We will test such ideas as their mental and physical health is tracked through the transition to old age. Resilient women have lives that also include adversity, typically growing up with an alcoholic parent or experiencing early family deaths, but these women possess important factors of advantage (high IQ, high grades in high school, good physical health). With regard to social orderings, many have experienced upward mobility in the workplace and tend to have supervisory work status. Their social relationships are also frequently characterized by closeness with a spouse or confidante as well as high participation in social organizations. We propose that the presence of these positive work and relational experiences have contributed to their high profiles of life purpose, mastery, growth, and quality connections to others. Further, we predict that such features of well-being will offer important protective resources as these women confront the vicissitudes of growing old.

While not described here, we emphasize that our life history analyses elaborate within-group variability (e.g., different routes to resilience) as well as between-group differences (e.g., how life histories of the healthy and vulnerable differ from those of the resilient). The larger aim is to utilize the tensions created by working simultaneously with thickly described, finely nuanced details of individual lives and thinner, less textured summaries of groups of lives. Other dialectics are embodied in the general a p proach as well, such as our efforts to work inductively up from the life history data and deductively down from the guiding principles, and back and forth between qualitative and quantitative data sources. These combined strategies, we believe, advance understanding of the diverse human pathways to psychological well-being, depression, resilience, and vulnerability.

Implications for Psychotherapy Research

What are the implications of the above program of research on largely healthy community and national Samples for the study and treatment of mental disorders in clinical populations? Does the study of wellness offer anything of merit to those attempting to alleviate psychological distress and dysfunction? We propose four avenues of linkage. A first point pertains to the evaluation of treatments, be they pharmacological or psychotherapeutic, for mental problems. To determine the effectiveness of such interventions, researchers often assess quality of life. Current and evolving indicators of quality of life [45,46] give emphasis to patients' self-reported problems or complaints across multiple domains (physical, cognitive, affective, social, economic). Much less attention is paid to their self-reported strengths or gains. Understanding the presence of the positive in patients' lives provides a more complete picture of their overall functioning,and thereby, a more comprehensive analysis of treatment effectiveness. Interventions that bring the person out of negative functioning into a neutral position are one form of success. Facilitating progressions toward restoration of the positive (e.g., possessing mastery, self-regard, purpose, quality relations) is quite another. Second, knowledge of psychological well-being provides insight and direction regarding the factors that influence the occurrence of mental disorders. Etiologically speaking, most formulations include both genetic and enviromental influences. The latter are frequently construed in terms of life stresses in the workplace, the home, the neighborhood, or community. An underlying assump

20

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

*
&&w

Future Studies of Well-Being: tion is that the presence of such adversity is part of what The Connection of Mind and Body contributes to the incidence of depression, anxiety, and other disorders. Recognition of the positive in human A final implication of the study of well-being is that experience suggests another etiological route: that the absence ofgoods in people's lives may also affect why they knowledge of positive mental health poses new directions become despondent about their lives. Viewed in this light, for age-old quandaries about how the mind and body are the explanatory task is broadened to encompass how the connected. Existing knowledge of physical health, like presence of the negative as well as the absence of the posi- mental health, is dramatically skewed toward the negative, focusing almost entirely on indicators of disease, tive work together to influence mental health. Such a dual emphasis has further implications for chronic conditions, functional impairment, and general understanding recovery from psychological dysfunction dysfunction. Building on theoretical and empirical work [47]. Feelings of mastery and self-regard, the possession of summarized herein, we reconfigure the meaning of posiquality relations with others, the capacity to set goals for tive human health and suggest new scientific agendas living, and a sense of self-realization may play critical regarding the physiological substrates of human flourishroles in the process of regaining mental health. Again, the ing [15]. The challenge in such future inquiries is to map message is that the route to recovery is not exclusively the biochemical and neurophysiological underpinnings of about alleviating the negative, but about engendering the positive mental states; that is, what occurs in the body positive. It may be the appearance (or reappearance) of when the mind is actively engaged in meaningful pursuits, such salubrious goods in life that provides a critical impe- quality relationships, and salubrious self-regard. tus in the capacity to rebound from distress. Recognition Animal and human literature on stress demonstrates of these pluses may also define important protective fac- extensive links of mental perceptions and behavioral tors in the face of future challenges and adversity. responses to stress with physiological and biochemical Our life history studies give emphasis to understanding processes, and ultimately neuroendocrine, immune and the cumulation of adversity and advantage experiences autonomic systems tied to disease outcomes [42]. McEwfrom the distant past. Psychotherapy, particularly in the en and Stellar [48] propose, for example, that a high 'allopsychoanalytic tradition, emphasizes the need to examine static load' (i.e., the strain on multiple organs and tissues critical experiences from early childhood, although the resulting from repeated fluctuations in physiological refocus is primarily on negative, traumatic events. We pro- sponse to perceived threat) leads to organ-system breakpose that there is much to be gained in therapeutic con- down, compromised immune response, elevated cortisol texts from investigating lives of the mentally healthy and and insulin secretion, and thereby,to disease. The impact the resilient. Our data show that among the healthy are of stress on biological function is not, however, explained individuals who have experienced considerable adversity in strictly physical terms, but involves emotions, over the course of their lives. Understanding the factors thoughts, and an array of psychological factors. Mood that protect them from becoming physically or mentally states associated with depression illustrate this linkage via ill provides critical preventive knowledge. Such factors, we dysregulation of the pituitary-adrenal system and chroninote, are not restricted to early life events, nor are they cally elevated levels of glucocorticoids in the blood [49]. limited to experiences in the family sphere (i.e., adult Generally missing in this prior literature is the positive experiences, including those in work and community con- counterpoint in mindlbody research, although there are texts are also relevant). The lives of the resilient convey glimmers in the direction of beneficial connections. For further important information about factors involved in example, while overshadowed by the pervasive focus on the recovery from depression. In sum, life history data negative emotions and immune responsiveness, there is a about the healthy, the distressed, and those who traverse growing body of work on positive emotions and immune both realms over the course of their lives enriches think- modulation [50]. Included are studies that demonstrate ing about relevant influences and targets in the treatment links between positive feelings and spontaneous remisof mental illness. sions of cancers, intensive daily meditation and regression of metastatic sarcomas, positive life outlooks and wound healing, quality social relationships and increased survival from cancer. Such investigations underscore the power of positive beliefs, emotions, and relationships in recovery from physical health challenges.

Psychological Well-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and Implications for Psychotherapy Research

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

21

Positive experience, we assert, plays further, perhaps more important roles in protecting and enhancing the integrity of the organism. Optimal human functioning, embodied by individuals who lead lives of purpose, possess deeply felt and meaningful ties to others, exercise mastery over their environments, engage and realize their true capacities, has, in short, its own biological underpinnings. These are the least understood of the mindhody connections, but again there are hopeful signs on the horizon. Research on the transformation from rural to urban industrial society indicates that a purpose in life, stable social relations and social position, and individual motivation provide protection against excessive neuroendocrine response [5 11. While such radical social changes disrupt familiar environments and pose new demands, those with positive social and psychological assets are buffered against emotional upheavals and ensuing neuroendocrine disturbances involving release of catecholamines, or corticoids, or both, that may eventuate in cardiovascular and coronary heart disease. The benefits of social assets also appear in church societies such as monastic orders and other religious groups showing a lower incidence of coronary heart disease. The stable, orderly life of these societies successfully blunts potentially harmful psychosocial stimuli, thereby maintaining the intensity of neuroendocrine responses below levels that would lead to pathophysiologic change [52]. Meditation and prayer activities play further physiologically protective roles by leading to reduced oxygen consumption, heart rate, and blood pressure, all consequences of reduced activity of the sympathetic nervous systems. Interestingly, the incidence of coronary heart disease is much lower in cloistered Benedictine Brothers than in Benedictine Priests who eat the same food at refectory tables (i.e., dietary control) [53]. The Priests' parochial and teaching duties in a competitive environment were seen to produce Type A persons in Type A environments. Evolutionary perspectives emphasize the preprogrammed 'biogrammar' that governs flexible behavior used to attain desired goals [54]. This biogrammar is seen as a vital but prerational element of behavior dependent on subcortical structures. The limbic and striatal regions of the brain are the locus for neuronal complexes mediating the emotions and behavior critical for self- and species preservation [55]. These complexes are the result of genetically determined wiring and result in the organism engaging in territorial and attachment behavior appropriate for the occasion. Illustrating such substrates, neuroendocrine changes associated with the mother's care of her young
22

shift abruptly when patterns of the attachment bond are disrupted. In man, attachment behavior persists for years after the young have become self-sufficient and with modifications by symbolism motivates a lifetime of socialized behavior [56]. Positive psychological functioning, exemplified by the acquisition of mastery over the environment, possession of quality relations with others, utilization of innate capabilities, etc., represents elaborated versions of complex adaptive behaviors requisite to survival. These latter refinements, like their evolutionary predecessors (e.g., territorial protection, caring for the young), have behavioral components, mental components, and underlying physiological profiles. Modem day science provides unprecedented opportunities thus to map these multiple facets of healthy, adaptive functioning. Viewed from the vantage points of psychiatry and psychotherapy, well-established practice treats mental and behavioral problems with pharmacological agents intended to alter the chemistry of the brain. Mindhody connections, in short, are far from novel in the clinical context. What is innovative is the call to shift the treatment focus to that of fostering positive human experience, which sets in motion its own biochemistry. Quality ties to others, feelings of purpose and selfrealization engender unique mindlbody spirals, but unlike those in the realm of stress, these move toward protection and enhancement of the organism.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development and a grant from the National Institute on Aging (lROlAG08979) (C.D.R.) and the MacArthur Foundation planning initiative and Socioeconomic Status and Health (B.S.).

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

.....................................................................................................................................................
References
1 Maslow A: Toward a Psychology of Being, ed 2. New York, Van Nostrand, 1968. 2 Rogers CR: On Becoming a Person. Boston, Houghton Mimin, 196I. 3 Jung CG: Modem Man in Search of a Soul. New York, Harcourt, Brace, &World, 1933. 4 Von Franz ML: The process of individuation; in CG Jung (ed): Man and His symbols. New York, Doubleday, 1964. 5 AUport GW: Pattern and Growth in Personality. New York, Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1961. 6 Erikson E: Identity and the life cycle. Psychol Issues 1959;1:18-164. 7 Buhler C: The curve of life as studied in biographies. J Appl Psychol 1935;19:405-409. 8 Buhler C, Massarik F (eds): The Course of Human Life. New York, Springer 1968. 9 Neugarten BL: The awareness of middle age; in Neugarten BL (ed): Middle Age and Aging Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1968, pp 93-98. 10 Neugarten BL: Personality change in late life: A develo~mentd oers~ective: in Eisdorfer C. ~awton MP (eds): The ~ s ~ c h o l o g y ~ d u l t of Development and Aging. Washington, American Psychological Association, 1973, pp 31 1335. 11 Jahoda M: Current Concepts of Positive Mental Health. New York, Basic Books, 1958. 12 Ryff CD: Beyond Ponce de Leon and life satisfaction: New directions in quest of successful aging. Int J Behav Dev 1989;12:35-55. 13 Bradburn NM:The Structure of Psychological Well-Being. Chicago, Aldine, 1969. 14 Diener E: Subjective well-being. Psychol Bull 1984;95:542-575. I5 Ryff CD, Singer B: The Contours of Positive Human Health; unpubl manuscript, Univetsity of Wisconsin, Madison. 16 Becker LC: Good lives: Prolegomena. Soc Phi10s Policy 1992;9:15-37. 17 Russell B: The Conquest of Happiness (1930). New York, Liveright, 1958. 18 Summer LW: Two theories of the good. Soc Philos Policy 1992;9:1-14. 19 Griffin J: Well-Being, Oxford, Clarendon, 1986. 20 Waterman AS: Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J Pen Soc Psychol1993;64:678-691. 21 Norton D: Personal Destinies. Princeton, Princeton University Press, 1976. 22 Mill JS: Autobiography (1873). London, Penguin, 1989. 23 Ryff CD: Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 1989;57:10691081. 24 Ryff CD, Keyes CLM: The structure of psyche logical well-being revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol 1995;69:7 19-727. 25 Ryff C D Psychological well-being in adult life. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1995;4:99-104. 26 Ryff CD: Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of shifting horizons. Psychol Aging 1991;6:286-295. 27 Strickland BR: Women and depression. Curr Dir Psycho1 Sci 1992;1:132-135. 28 Ryff CD, Magee WJ: Opportunity, achievement, and well-being: A midlife perspective. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Meetings, August, New York City, 1995. 29 Carr D: The impact of men's unfulfilled career aspirations on midlife psychological well-being. Paper presented at the American Sociological Association Meetings, August, Washington, 1995. 30 Bumpass LL, Aquilino WS: A social map of midlife: Family and work over the middle life course. Vero Beach, MacArthur Research Network on Successful Midlife Development, 1995. 31 Marmot M, Ryff CD, Bumpass LL, Shipley M, Marks NF:Social Inequalities in Health: Next Questions and Converging Evidence; unpubl manuscript, University College London Medical School, London. 32 Markus HR, Kitayama S: Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev 1991;98:224-253. 33 RyffCD, Lee YH, KC: Through the Lens of Na Culture: Psychological Well-Being at Midlife; unpubl manuscript, University of Wisconsin, Madison. 34 Heidrich SM, RyfT CD: Physical and mental health in later life: The self-system as mediator. Psychol Aging 1993;8:327-338. 35 Heidrich SM, Ryff CD: The role of social comparison processes in the psychological adaptation of elderly adults. J Gerontol 1993;48: P127-P136. 36 RytTCD, Essex MJ: The interpretation of life experience and well-being: The sample case of relocation. Psychol Aging 1992;7:507-517. 37 RyffCD, Lee YH,Essex MI, Schmutte PS: My children and me: Mid-life evaluationsof grown children and of self. Psychol Aging 1994;9: 195-205. 38 Tweed S, Ryff CD: Adult children of alcoholics: Profiles of wellness and distress. J Stud Alcohol 1991;52:133-141. 39 Smider NA, Essex MJ, Ryff CD: Adaptation to community relocation: The interactive influence of psychological resources and contextual factors. Psychol Aging, in press. 40 Kling KC, Ryff CD, Essex MJ: Adaptive changes in the self-concept during a life transi% tion. Paper presented at the Gerontological . ciety ofAmerica Meetings, November, Atlanta, 1994. 41 Cole JR, Singer B: A theory of limited differences: Explaining the productivity puzzle in science; in Zuckerman H, Cole JR, Bruer JT (eds): The Outer Circle: Women in the Scientific Community. New York, Norton, 1991, pp 277-339. 42 Singer B, Ryff CD: Social orderinglhealth linkages: Pathways and dostasis. Paper presented at Conference on Racial and Ethnic Differences in Health in Late Life, Committee on Population, National Academicy of Sciences, December, Washington, 1994. 43 Carr D, RyffCD, Singer B, Magee W: Bringing the 'life' back into life course research: A 'person-centered' approach to studying the life course. Paper Presented at Meetings of the American Sociological Association, Washington, 1995. 44 Magee W, Singer B, Ryff CD, Carr D: Pathways to vulnerability and resilience at midlife. Paper presented at the Meetings of the American Sociological Association, Washington, 1995. 45 Bech P: Rating Scales for Psychopathology, Health Status, and Quality of Life. London, Springer, 1993. 46 Bech P: Measurement of psychological distress and well-being. Psychother Psychosom 1990; 5477-89. 47 Fava GA: The concept of recovery in affective disorders. Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:213. 48 McEwen BS, Stellar E: Stress and the individual. Arch Intern Med 1993;153:2093-2101. 49 Maier SF, Watkins LR, Reshner M: Psyche neuroimmunology: The interface between b e havior, brain, and immunity. Am Psychol 1994;49:1004-1017. 50 Melnechuk T: Emotions, brain, immunity, and health: A review; in Clynes M, Panksepp (eds): Emotions and Psychopathology. New York, Plenum, 1988, pp 181-247. 51 Henry JP, Stephens PM: Stress, Health, and the Social Environment: A sociobiology A p proach to Medicine. New York, Springer, 1977. 52 McEwen B, Schmeck H: The Hostage Brain. New York, Rockefeller University Press, 1994. 53 Caffrey CB: Behavior Patterns and Personality Characteristics as Related to Prevalence Rates for Coronary Heart Diseases in Trappist and Benedictine Monks; PhD Diss (Clinical Psychology), Catholic University of America, Washington, 1966. 54 Tiger L, Fox R: The Imperial Animal. London, Secker & Warburg, 1972. 55 MacLean PD: Sensory and perceptive factors in emotional functions of the triune brain; in Grenell RG, Gabay S (eds): Biological Foundations of Psychiatry, vol 1. New York, Raven Press, 1976, pp 177-198. 56 Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss, vol 1. London, Hogarth, 1970.

Psychological Well-Being: Meaning, Measurement, and Implications for PsychotherapyResearch

Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:14-23

23

You might also like