You are on page 1of 11

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283 www.elsevier.

com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Regional dissociations within the hippocampusmemory and anxiety


D.M. Bannermana, J.N.P. Rawlinsa,*, S.B. McHugha, R.M.J. Deacona, B.K. Yeeb, T. Bastb,1, W.-N. Zhangb, H.H.J. Pothuizenb, J. Feldonb
b

Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK Laboratory of Behavioural Neurobiology, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, Schorenstrasse 16, Schwerzenbach CH-8603, Switzerland

Abstract The amnestic effects of hippocampal lesions are well documented, leading to numerous memory-based theories of hippocampal function. It is debatable, however, whether any one of these theories can satisfactorily account for all the consequences of hippocampal damage: Hippocampal lesions also result in behavioural disinhibition and reduced anxiety. A growing number of studies now suggest that these diverse behavioural effects may be associated with different hippocampal subregions. There is evidence for at least two distinct functional domains, although recent neuroanatomical studies suggest this may be an underestimate. Selective lesion studies show that the hippocampus is functionally subdivided along the septotemporal axis into dorsal and ventral regions, each associated with a distinct set of behaviours. Dorsal hippocampus has a preferential role in certain forms of learning and memory, notably spatial learning, but ventral hippocampus may have a preferential role in brain processes associated with anxiety-related behaviours. The latters role in emotional processing is also distinct from that of the amygdala, which is associated specically with fear. Gray and McNaughtons theory can in principle incorporate these apparently distinct hippocampal functions, and provides a plausible unitary account for the multiple facets of hippocampal function. q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Hippocampus; Dorsal; Ventral; Amygdala; Spatial; Learning; Memory; Anxiety; Fear; Freezing; Startle

Contents 1. Preferentially dorsal hippocampus-dependent functions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2. Preferentially ventral hippocampus-dependent functions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3. Partial hippocampal lesion effects in freezing paradigms and the watermaze . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4. Partial hippocampal lesion effects on unconditioned ethologically based tests of anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5. The roles of the ventral hippocampus and amygdala in anxiety-related processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

274 275 276 277 277 279 280

The hippocampus has a long-established role in certain forms of memory [117]. However, it has become increasingly apparent that the hippocampus is not just concerned with the formation of memories, but is likely to play a more general role in information processing and the subsequent regulation of behaviour. Recently it has emerged that these various functions may be distributed through the hippocampus. This review outlines what some of these apparently very different functions may be,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 44-1865-271424; fax: 44-1865310447. E-mail address: nick.rawlins@psy.ox.ac.uk (J.N.P. Rawlins). 1 Present address: Department of Neuroscience, University of Edinburgh, 1 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, Scotland, UK. 0149-7634/$ - see front matter q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.004

and how far we can at present localise them to specic hippocampal subregions. We suggest (i) that the dorsal subregion, dened as 50% of hippocampal volume starting at the septal pole [10], and sometimes referred to as posterior hippocampus in primates, has a preferential role in spatial learning and memory, and (ii) that the ventral subregion, dened as 50% of hippocampal volume starting at the temporal pole, and sometimes referred to as anterior hippocampus in primates, may have a preferential role in anxiety-related behaviours. The latters role in emotional processing is also distinct from that of the amygdala, which we suggest is associated with fear rather than anxiety. We conclude that Gray and McNaughtons [56] theory can in principle

274

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

incorporate these apparently distinct hippocampal functions, and provides a plausible unitary account for the multiple facets of hippocampal function [82]. When Scoville and Milner reported that bilateral resection of the medial temporal lobe caused a profound anterograde amnesia in human patients, they initiated a new era of study into the neuropsychology of memory [84]. One focus of that study was the hippocampus, which had been substantially and bilaterally damaged in their most famous patient, H.M. [31,117]. A series of studies in animals sought to determine whether damage restricted to the hippocampus alone could induce a comparable amnesia. Somewhat puzzlingly, the consensus from these studies up until the 1960s was that by and large there was no evidence of a selective amnesia. Animals with large hippocampal lesions apparently learned a variety of new tasks quite normally. Their abnormality lay not in new task acquisition, but in an apparent tendency to persist in these learned responses once they were no longer appropriate. There was evidence of perseverative responding during extinction in a runway [64]; evidence of difculty in reversal learning [69]; and evidence of excessive responding in operant schedules even when increased lever-pressing led to decreased reinforcement [28]. These kinds of observations led to the formulation of theories of hippocampal function that emphasised its role in behavioural inhibition, an adaptive process whereby organisms suppress responses that are, for some reason, no longer appropriate. On this kind of view, the mechanisms for increasing the probability of positively reinforced responses are segregated from those in the behavioural inhibition system via which the probability of negatively reinforced responses is decreased. Behavioural inhibition under conditions of conict had been the focus of a series of studies on the effects of tranquillising drugs, initially investigating the effects of alcohol and barbiturate administration and subsequently including study of the benzodiazepine minor tranquillisers. Moderate doses of these agents appeared to resolve conict between approach and avoidance tendencies in favour of approach [12]. Thus the effects of minor tranquilliser administration, at least in some respects, paralleled those of hippocampal dysfunction. Grays theory of hippocampal function proposed that these parallels arose because the septo-hippocampal system was a core component of the behavioural inhibition system, and went further by identifying the hippocampal theta rhythm (at 7.7 Hz) as the neural substrate of frustrative non-reward [52,55]. The proposal by OKeefe and Nadel [97] that the hippocampus is the neural substrate of a cognitive map of space, coupled with the development of new spatial memory tasks for rodentsparticularly the radial arm and T-maze spatial working memory tasks, and the immensely inuential watermaze reference memory

task-changed the picture radically [85,86,98,110]. Lesion experiments and single unit recording studies concentrated increasingly on spatial paradigms. The effect sizes [29] of hippocampal lesion-induced changes in performance in these designs can be exceptionally large, as the cognitive map theory would suggest. There seemed to be little room for a hippocampal role in the behavioural inhibition of ongoing behaviour under conditions of fear or anxiety.

1. Preferentially dorsal hippocampus-dependent functions An opportunity to reconcile these accounts of hippocampal function has arrived with the demonstration that lesions of different hippocampal subregions have distinct and dissociable behavioural effects. A number of early behavioural experiments [60,92,121,119] coupled with anatomical studies demonstrating clear differences in afferent and efferent connectivity along the septotemporal extent of the hippocampus [118,123,133], had hinted at potential differences between dorsal and ventral hippocampus. These suspicions were conrmed with the demonstration by Moser and colleagues [90] that whereas aspiration dorsal hippocampal lesions resembled complete hippocampal lesions in severely disrupting spatial learning in the Morris watermaze, ventral hippocampal lesions of similar size were without effect. Importantly, in a follow up study, Moser and colleagues replicated these ndings but this time using bre-sparing, ibotenic acid lesions rather than aspiration lesions [91]. Using this approach they made bilateral, symmetrical hippocampal lesions, varying in size from 20 to 100% of the total hippocampal volume, extending from either the septal or temporal pole of the hippocampus. Consistent with their previous ndings, they found that although spatial learning was essentially intact with as little as 26% of the dorsal hippocampus still remaining, lesions of the septal pole sparing as much as 60% of the ventral hippocampus, resulted in a robust watermaze impairment. The neurotoxic lesion approach resulted in discrete and specic lesions to the intended area but preserved nerve bres and cerebrovasculature passing through the lesioned area [63]. Consequently, the resulting behavioural effects could be attributed, with some degree of condence, to differences in the functions performed by the different hippocampal subregions, and were unlikely to be due to differential effects on bres of passage coursing through either the septal or temporal pole of the hippocampus. Since the publication of this nding, a number of studies have now examined the effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal cytotoxic lesions on spatial learning, in various different learning paradigms, and in general they have revealed similar patterns of results [8 10,70,81]. In addition to impairing spatial reference memory acquisition

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

275

in the watermaze, dorsal hippocampal lesions have also been shown to impair spatial learning on appetitive maze tasks such as T-maze rewarded alternation [8,10,59] and performance on a 4/8 radial maze task [106], thus resembling complete hippocampal lesions. In contrast, ventral hippocampal lesions left performance in these tasks unaffected. These appetitive maze studies were important for two reasons. First, they demonstrated that dorsal but not ventral hippocampal lesions still impaired spatial learning under very different experimental conditions from those encountered in the watermaze, with very different sensorimotor and motivational demands (e.g. running vs. swimming; food reward vs. escape from water). Second, they showed that dorsal but not ventral hippocampal lesions impair spatial working as well as spatial reference memory [8,10,59,106]. Several other lines of evidence are also now available to support the conclusion from lesion studies that the dorsal hippocampus has a preferential role in spatial learning and memory. For example, there is now evidence accumulating to suggest that, in spatial memory tasks, acute microinfusions of drugs which temporarily interfere with the normal activity within localised hippocampal subregions yield similar patterns of results to those obtained from lesion studies (e.g. [89,128] McHugh et al., in preparation; Yee et al., in preparation). This is important because microinfusion studies offer fewer opportunities than lesion studies for compensatory changes because the effects of the manipulation are transient and short lasting (see Refs. [13,14], and also permit within subject comparisons. Further evidence from electrophysiological single unit recording studies in both rats [66] and primates [30], from c-fos activation studies [130], and from structural magnetic resonance imaging studies [77], also suggest a functional dissociation between dorsal (posterior in primates) and ventral (anterior in primates, see Refs. [133]) hippocampus, and a preferential role for dorsal hippocampus in spatial learning and memory. As has been highlighted previously [88], this is entirely consistent with the fact that the major input of visuo-spatial information to the hippocampus from primary sensory cortical areas, via association cortex, and perirhinal and entorhinal areas, is primarily to the dorsal two thirds of the hippocampus [3,24,25,39,40]. In contrast, other types of sensory input (such as olfactory input) are more evenly distributed along the dorsoventral extent of the hippocampus [88]. Thus, in types of hippocampus-dependent memory relying less on spatial, but more on other types of information, such as olfactory cues [22,23], the role of the dorsal hippocampus may be less dominant than in spatial memory. This remains to be examined. At this point it is important to stress that the distinction between dorsal and ventral hippocampal roles in spatial learning may be preferential rather than absolute in nature. That is to say, the ventral hippocampus may contribute to spatial learning at least under some conditions [38,44,45]. For

example, De Hoz and colleagues have recently shown that rats with excitotoxic dorsal hippocampal lesions can eventually acquire a spatial reference memory task in the Morris watermaze if given sufcient training [38]. Because animals with complete hippocampal lesions did not show this improvement in performance, these results suggest that there are conditions under which the ventral hippocampus can contribute effectively to spatial learning. As De Hoz and colleagues point out, this nding is consistent with the fact that ventral as well as dorsal hippocampus exhibits increased c-fos activation after spatial training (albeit proportionately less selectively relative to non-spatial training; [130]), and also with the observation that place cells do exist in the ventral hippocampus (albeit more infrequently and with less well dened place elds; [66,107]). Both of these ndings suggest that ventral hippocampus might support spatial learning, albeit generally less effectively than its dorsal counterpart. This functional gradient is consistent with the fact that many anatomical differences in connectivity along the septotemporal axis of the hippocampus are gradual rather than absolute, with a graded pattern of afferent and efferent projections to and from dorsal and ventral hippocampus, rather than marked demarcations [3]. As a consequence, the denitions of dorsal and ventral hippocampus may themselves need further renement. Moreover, the extensive connectivity between dorsal and ventral hippocampus [2] also predicts some degree of functional interdependence between the two subregions, however dened.

2. Preferentially ventral hippocampus-dependent functions Identication of clearly dorsal hippocampus-dependent functions (that are largely ventral hippocampus independent) raises the intriguing question as to whether the ventral hippocampus might likewise be preferentially involved in a particular subset of hippocampus-dependent behaviours [59,88]. As was outlined above, the preferential involvement of the dorsal hippocampus in spatial information processing is entirely consistent with its known anatomical connectivity-dorsal hippocampus receives most of the highly pre-processed visuo-spatial information from sensory modalities [88]. The identity of the anatomical connections to and from the ventral hippocampus must surely provide a major clue as to its function. The ventral subregion differs markedly from the dorsal subregion in its anatomical connections. It projects to the prefrontal cortex, whereas the dorsal hippocampus does not [11,49,65,131]. It is closely connected to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) and the amygdala [57,72,102,105,123,129], as well as other subcortical structures which are associated with the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis [3,62,118,123, 133]. Most of the amygdalar nuclei have some reciprocal projections with the hippocampal formation, although this is most pronounced for the basal and lateral nuclei [105].

276

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

The strong connectivity between ventral hippocampus and both the hypothalamus and the amygdala, makes it tempting to propose a role for the ventral subregion in fear and/or anxiety, and thus potentially account for some of the hippocampal lesion effects on emotionality. One paradigm in which this possibility has been extensively examined is contextually conditioned freezing. Conditioned freezing in rats is a protective immobility response to conditioned stimuli associated with aversive unconditioned stimuli, such as a foot-shock, and is particularly expressed in a situation where escape is impossible [43]. A number of studies have shown that hippocampal lesions disrupt the acquisition and/or expression of contextually conditioned freezing, although the underlying basis for these effects has been debated at length in recent years [51,83]. There is evidence that ventral hippocampal lesions can be as effective as total hippocampal lesions in reducing the levels of freezing seen after the delivery of a mild foot-shock ([112] but see also Refs. [70]). The reduction in freezing levels was observed both to the tone that had signalled the delivery of the foot-shock and to the experimental context in which the shock had been delivered, and was evident immediately after the delivery of the rst foot-shock during the training phase of the experiment. A similar reduction in freezing levels was observed following ventral subiculum lesions [78]. In contrast, Richmond et al. [112] showed that cell loss limited to the dorsal hippocampus had no effect on freezing (see also Refs. [79]). These results led to the suggestion that the ventral, as opposed to the dorsal hippocampus, is preferentially involved in conditioned freezing. The generality of this conclusion is still debated. Possible reasons for discrepancies between different lesion studies, such as distal damage and compensatory processes, have been discussed at length recently [4,5,14,16,70]. Differences in behavioural testing procedures may also be potentially important, and so could themselves merit further attention. Indeed, it may be the case that conditioned freezing is not the optimum paradigm for delineating ventral hippocampal functions in anxiety-related behaviour: other paradigms are considered in a subsequent section. An alternative approach derives from the use of temporary pharmacological manipulation of dorsal and ventral hippocampus by intracerebral drug microinfusions. These experiments have been taken to indicate that both dorsal and ventral hippocampal subregions can play a role in specic memory processes underlying a normal freezing response in fear conditioning experiments (for review and discussion see Refs. [4,5,14,16,116]). If so, then disruption of these memory processes could account at least for a part of the freezing impairments seen after hippocampal lesions. It is of course important to be able to establish the exact spread of the drug for precise comparison with lesion studies. Moreover, when drugs are administered so as to manipulate acquisition or retrieval only, then the possibility of state-dependent memory impairment must be considered

alongside the possibility of specic memory impairment. Given these caveats, it is clearly important to reconcile the results from ablation and temporary activation or inactivation studies. Finally, it has recently been suggested that the ventral hippocampus may exert control over the expression of conditioned freezing by modulating dopaminergic transmission in the medial prefrontal cortex [99,103]. This proposal extends earlier reports that lesions of discrete prefrontal subelds can interfere with the expression of conditioned fear in rats. The ventral, but not the dorsal, hippocampus enjoys a direct projection to the medial prefrontal cortex and is in a unique position to inuence the prefrontal dopamine functions (for review see Ref. [124] and also Ref. [13, p. 334]). This may represent a route whereby the ventral regions of the hippocampus make a distinctive contribution to conditioned freezing.

3. Partial hippocampal lesion effects in freezing paradigms and the watermaze There is a widely held notion that spatial learning, as exemplied by tasks such as the Morris watermaze, and contextual fear conditioning are essentially different manifestations of the same basic underlying process [80,93,97,115,122]. Such a line of thought is tempting because there is an intuitive similarity between these spatial and contextual forms of learning in that both are likely to involve processing, perhaps integrating, complex stimuli which are often large [27], diffuse, multicue, polymodal, and relational [16]. However, there are differences in their sensitivities to partial hippocampal lesions, and certain pharmacological manipulations, consistent with differences in their underlying neural substrates within the hippocampal system [10,14,112]. When lesions are made before training, cytotoxic dorsal lesions appear to disrupt spatial learning more consistently and effectively than ventral ones. Conversely, cytotoxic dorsal lesions appear to disrupt contextually conditioned freezing less reliably than ventral ones. When lesions made after training are considered, there is an intriguing analogy between the differences in antero- and retrograde effects of partial hippocampal lesions on contextual fear-conditioning and in watermaze experiments. Contextual fear conditioning seems to be more susceptible to selective dorsal hippocampal lesions if they are made between training and testing, than if they are made before training [4,79]. An analogous result has been obtained for ventral hippocampal lesions and spatial learning in the watermaze [89]. As with contextual fear conditioning, memories acquired prior to the lesion were affected by a degree of hippocampal damage that was without effect on the acquisition of new memories. Partial hippocampal lesions restricted to the ventral 40% of the hippocampus disrupted the retrieval of a previously acquired spatial reference memory task in the watermaze,

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

277

but then had no effect on the acquisition of a second spatial task in a novel environment. The retrograde effects both of ventral hippocampal lesions on spatial learning in the watermaze and of dorsal hippocampal lesions on contextual freezing, coupled with the absence of equivalent anterograde effects, suggest that under normal circumstances these memories may be encoded and subsequently retrieved by widely distributed networks extending beyond a more crucialor even obligatezone into a substantial portion of the remainder of the hippocampus. However, acquisition of new memories and their subsequent retrieval can be achieved with more limited neuronal networks when partial lesions are made before learning. Under these circumstances different hippocampal subregions preferentially support the acquisition of spatial learning and of contextual freezing.

4. Partial hippocampal lesion effects on unconditioned ethologically based tests of anxiety The possibility that the ventral hippocampus may have a specialised role in fear or anxiety related behaviours has also recently been examined using a series of unconditioned anxiety tests [81]. This study had two goals. First, it extended the comparison of the effect of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions to examine the generality of the inferences derived from freezing studies. Second, a group of animals with cytotoxic lesions of the amygdalar complex (intended to include all the major amygdala nuclei) was included for direct comparison with the effects of hippocampal lesions to determine whether these two brain areas contribute differentially to fear and anxiety. Ventral hippocampal lesions had behavioural effects resembling those induced by benzodiazepines on these tasks, consistent with a reduction of anxiety (see Ref. [56], Appendix 1). In agreement with previous studies, rats with selective ventral hippocampal lesions (i) showed reduced hyponeophagia, i.e. they were quicker to eat in potentially anxiogenic, unfamiliar environments (see also Refs. [8,9], (ii) displayed increased social interaction (see also Ref. [8]), (iii) were quicker to cross from the black to the white compartment during a two compartment box test, and (iv) spent an increased proportion of time in a more anxiogenic section of the successive alleys apparatus, a modied version of the elevated plus maze (see also Refs. [8,70]). On each of these four very different behavioural tests, the ventral hippocampal lesions had effects that are consistent with an anxiolytic effect. Animals with ventral hippocampal damage differed from those with dorsal hippocampal lesions on these tests. The latter generally displayed little, if any, effect on tests of anxiety, relative to the sham operated controls [8,70,81]. Rats with dorsal hippocampal lesions did, however, display a marked spatial learning impairment in the Morris watermaze, unlike the ventral lesioned rats whose spatial learning performance was indistinguishable from the controls. This

therefore provides a clear double dissociation of the effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions on spatial learning and a range of ethologically based tests of anxiety. A double dissociation was also observed for the dorsal and ventral lesion groups between the anxiety tests and spontaneous locomotor activity as measured in photocell activity cages [8,81]. This is important because lesioninduced hyperactivity is a potential confound of all of these anxiety tests, as well as of conditioned freezing tests [51]. This double dissociation suggests that a simple account based on a general hyperactivity in lesioned animals cannot adequately explain the pattern of results obtained here. Note that we and others have also observed other effects of ventral hippocampal lesions which are consistent with reduced anxiety and which do not rely on measures of activity. For example, there is good evidence that high levels of anxiety are associated with increased defaecation [20,21,53,54]. We have observed reduced levels of defaecation in ventral hippocampal lesioned rats, both in a brightly illuminated open eld test and at certain stages of contextual fear conditioning and its subsequent extinction, although importantly there were no differences between sham and lesioned animals in defaecation levels in the home cage [9]. A similar observation was made by Kjelstrup and colleagues who reported that both complete and ventral hippocampal lesions reduced defaecation scores relative to sham and dorsal hippocampal lesioned animals following connement in a brightly lit white chamber [70]. In addition, they also found that ventral lesioned animals showed a reduced increase in plasma corticosterone concentrations 20 min after the connement, as compared to controls (see also Ref. [128]; but note also Ref. [58]). Both of these results suggest that the effects of ventral hippocampal lesions extend beyond a mere disinhibition of motor activity under anxiogenic conditions, indicating a more central role for this brain area in anxiety.

5. The roles of the ventral hippocampus and amygdala in anxiety-related processes Direct comparison between the effects of amygdala lesions and ventral hippocampal lesions revealed a number of important and notable distinctions. Amygdala lesions had no effect on the successive alleys test, a result which is consistent with several previous demonstrations that these lesions, in contrast to hippocampal damage, have no effect on performance on the elevated plus maze [37,70,120, 125 127]. Amygdala lesions also had no signicant effect on social interaction (to the extent that there was any change it was in the opposite direction to rats with ventral hippocampal lesions: their levels of social interaction were slightly lower than those observed in sham operated animals). On the hyponeophagia tests, the amygdala lesioned animals took signicantly longer than the controls to begin eating, an effect which was once again

278

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

in the opposite direction to that seen with ventral hippocampal lesions. Indeed the only test on which the amygdala and ventral hippocampal lesions resulted in comparable effects was the black/white two-compartment box test. These results clearly demonstrate that the effects of ventral hippocampal lesions are, at least in some respects, distinct from those of amygdala lesions. These observations are important, rstly because they demonstrate that the behavioural effects of ventral hippocampal lesions are not simply a consequence of either direct or indirect effects on the amygdala (e.g. unintended incidental damage or deafferentation), and secondly because they clearly indicate that the ventral hippocampus and the amygdala contribute differentially to the processing of fearful or anxiogenic stimuli, despite having similar effects on some measures of conditioned freezing [61,104,112]. Several additional lines of evidence support the idea that the amygdala, unlike the hippocampus, is not crucial for normal performance during anxiety tests such as the elevated plus maze. In addition to the clear absence of amygdalar lesion effects on this test under normal conditions, they also fail to block the anxiolytic effects of systemic benzodiazepine treatment on this apparatus if a single test is conducted ([127]; see also Ref. [47]). It is also worth noting that, in general, infusion of benzodiazepines directly into the amygdala has no effect on plus maze performance ([50,100]; but see also Refs. [101]), although it does produce apparently anxiolytic effects in at least some test paradigms [94], and there is a high density of benzodiazepine binding sites in the amygdala [95,96]. There is, however, a clear role for the hippocampal formation during elevated plus maze testing. In addition to the recent demonstrations of ventral hippocampal lesion effects on plus maze performance [8,70,81], there are several reports of anxiolytic effects of both complete hippocampal and septal lesions on this task [36,37,125,126]. The idea that the hippocampus and amygdala contribute differentially to mechanisms underlying fear and anxiety is not novel [56]. While it is apparent that anxiety and fear are intimately related and overlapping, it is now widely accepted that they should not be considered as a unitary construct [35,46,56,109]. According to Davis and colleagues, fear is a phasic response to explicit, often conditioned, aversive cues, while anxiety is a more tonic response to rather diffuse, often unconditioned, aversive cues or situations [35]. Gray and McNaughton [56] similarly propose that fear is associated with (more phasic) active avoidance or escape responses and removal from a dangerous situation. Anxiety, on the other hand, may result from diffuse aversive stimuli; is associated with conict and uncertainty (e.g. whether or not an animal should enter/ approach a potentially dangerous situation); and results when there is competition between concurrently available goals. They further propose that fear may often be viewed as a logical precursor of anxiety, and that there is a hierarchical

organization with anxiety systems providing an inhibitory control over fear systems. A further line of evidence that also indicates separate roles for the hippocampus and amygdala in fear or anxietyrelated behaviours derives from studies of potentiated startle. The acoustic startle reex is a rapid contraction of the skeletal muscles elicited by a sudden auditory stimulus and is assumed to be an unconditioned protective reaction. The startle reex is mediated by a brain-stem circuit [34,134], but modulated by forebrain sites [71]. For example, the BNST and the amygdala, which are both closely linked to the ventral hippocampus [105,123], have access to the startle circuit and exert control over it [35,71]. Importantly, emotional states exert a strong inuence on startle reactivity [71,73]: enhanced startle reactivity is a diagnostic criterion for anxiety disorders in humans and is used as an animal model of anxiety [35,114]. According to the concept of emotional priming, the startle response as a protective reaction to an aversive and threatening stimulus may be enhanced by negative and attenuated by positive emotional states [73]. For example, in the conditioned fearpotentiated startle paradigm rats are rst trained to be fearful of a light CS predicting footshock. Conditioned fear to the light is then indexed by the elevation of startle response in the presence of the light relative to that in its absence. The fear-potentiated startle effect is blocked by amygdala lesions [33,34,35]. Hippocampal lesions do not reliably affect basal startle magnitude (e.g. [67,74 76,108, for review, see Ref. [13]). Localized microinfusions of a wide range of drugs (e.g. NMDA, tetrodotoxin, muscimol, picrotoxin) into either the dorsal or ventral subregions all tended to attenuate the startle response [15,17,132,136,137], with the exception of dorsal hippocampal infusion of the non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist, MK-801, which potentiated basal startle reactivity [6,7,135]. Thus no regionally or neuropharmacologically specic hippocampal role has been identied in basal startle responding, at least when startle reactivity is evaluated in an emotionally relatively neutral situation. A rather different picture emerges when startle is elicited in an anxious state, induced using a pharmacological manipulation in the form of intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) microinfusion of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH). Intraventricular CRH infusion elicits a syndrome that resembles the effects of stress or anxiety [41]. This encompasses physiological, endocrinological and behavioural changes, including an increase in startle amplitude. There is now increasing evidence that the hippocampal formation in combination with the BNST represents the neural system that underlies CRH-enhanced startle [75,76]. For example, transection of the mbria with a knife cut resulted in an almost complete blockade of CRH-enhanced startle [75]. The same study showed that electrolytic dorsal hippocampal lesions did not affect CRH-enhanced startle, suggesting that if the hippocampus is involved then it is

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283

279

likely to be the ventral and not the dorsal hippocampus that is important (so long as the mbria itself was left unaffected by the dorsal lesions). Indeed, Lee and Davis concluded from this study that the effects of mbria-fornical transection on CRH-enhanced startle were likely to be due to the disruption of a well documented bre pathway from the ventral hippocampus to the BNST [3,26,32,123], which in turn then projects to the neural circuitry underlying the startle reex. In a subsequent study they demonstrated that the primary site of action of CRH is likely to be the BNST. Interestingly, although cytotoxic lesions of the BNST completely blocked CRH-enhanced startle, they had no effect on the acquisition of the classical fear-potentiated startle response to an explicit visual CS. In contrast, cytotoxic lesions of either the central or basolateral nuclei of the amygdala, which did block fear potentiated startle, had no effect on CRHenhanced startle. This double dissociation between the effects of BNST and amygdala lesions on the two variants of the startle paradigm further support the idea of distinct components of fear and anxiety with different underlying neural substrates. The key question for the present review concerns the role of the ventral hippocampus in CRH-enhanced startle. At present, this remains unclear. The effects of ventral hippocampal lesions on the startle response after CRH infusion were suggestive but inconclusive [76]. Inspection of the data (see Ref. [76]; Fig. 1A) shows virtually no increase in the mean startle amplitude following i.c.v. CRH infusion into ventral hippocampal lesioned rats, in contrast to the dramatic change observed in the sham operated animals (a 150 200% increase). There was nevertheless a statistically signicant effect of the drug in the animals with ventral lesions, albeit smaller than that observed in the control group. Closer inspection of the data revealed a striking dichotomy in terms of the drug-induced startle enhancement shown by this group of lesioned animals. Further studies are therefore required to clarify the role of the ventral hippocampus in CRH-enhanced startle. Nevertheless, the work of Lee and Davis [75,76] is suggestive of the possibility that projections from the ventral hippocampus to the BNST may be an important neural pathway in a brain system underlying anxiety [3,26,32,123], that is distinct from an amygdala dependent system associated with fear.

6. Conclusions The results we have reviewed support the idea that fear and anxiety are distinct, emotional entities with separate underlying neural substrates. Furthermore, they support Gray and McNaughtons [56] distinction between the contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala to emotional behaviour, suggesting differential roles for these structures in anxiety and fear respectively. The two brain

systems are of course likely to be highly interactive. These distinctions, and their respective places within a hierarchical defence system, are described at length in the Neuropsychology of Anxiety ([56]; see also Refs. [18,19,75,76]). Theories of hippocampal function have typically treated the structure as a whole; to the extent that they have differentiated between different components of the structure, they have tended to focus on the different cell subelds (dentate gyrus, CA3 and CA1). We now suggest that the ventral hippocampus makes its own distinctive contribution to the control of behaviour in certain anxiogenic situations. The evidence is consistent with a ventral hippocampal role in anxiety as distinct from fear, which more plausibly depends on amygdalar function. Such a view could coexist with views that allocate a specialised role to the dorsal region of the hippocampus in spatial learning, while at the same time specifying at least one way in which the role of the hippocampus extends beyond the purely spatial domain and into the anxiety related behaviours that were at the core of Grays behavioural inhibition theory [55,56]. Furthermore, it seems increasingly likely that even the mnemonic functions of the hippocampus are not restricted to the spatial domain. The hippocampal memory system may provide the means for encoding both the spatial and temporal contexts (the where and the when) associated with a particular event, thus capturing the key properties associated with episodic memory in humans [42,87]. There are several recent demonstrations of hippocampal involvement in the temporal sequencing of events [1,48,68]. A major challenge that now remains is to identify a common psychological algorithm or operation that might underlie both an episodic-like memory function and a role in anxiety. Gray and McNaughtons theory can in principle incorporate these apparently very different hippocampal roles, thus providing a plausible unitary account of hippocampal function ([56]; see also Ref. [82], this issue). They suggest that the algorithm performed by the hippocampus is to compare different potential response alternatives and then to select the optimal response. In the case of episodic-like memory tasks, this will involve using conditional information provided by either spatial or temporal cues, or both, to select the appropriate learned response. In the case of unconditioned tests of anxiety, this may involve selecting between conicting approach and avoid responses, whether it be a novel foodstuff in a novel environment, or the open arms of an elevated plus maze. Theories of hippocampal function need to identify the different domains within which the hippocampus plays a key role. We believe at least two such domains have already been identied, and further analysis may well identify more, as suggested elsewhere [102,113]. Although these domains apparently depend preferentially on different hippocampal subregions, we would surely want a truly unifying theory of hippocampal function to explain its role within those domains by reference to a consistent physiological algorithm performed by the intra-hippocampal circuitry.

280

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283 [17] Bast T, Zhang WN, Heidbreder C, Feldon J. Hyperactivity and disruption of prepulse inhibition induced by N-methyl-D -aspartate stimulation of the ventral hippocampus and the effects of pretreatment with haloperidol and clozapine. Neuroscience 2001;103:32535. [18] Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. An ethoexperimental analysis of defense, fear and anxiety. In: McNaughton N, Andrews G, editors. Anxiety. Dunedin: Otago University Press; 1990. p. 12433. [19] Blanchard RJ, Yudko EB, Rodgers RJ, Blanchard DC. Defense system psychopharmacology: an ethological approach to the pharmacology of fear and anxiety. Behav Brain Res 1993;58: 155 65. [20] Broadhurst PL. Applications of biometrical genetics to the inheritance of behaviour. In: Eysenck HJ, editor. Experiments in personality. Psychogenetics and psychopharmacology, vol. 1. London: Routledge Kegan Paul; 1960. p. 1 102. [21] Broadhurst PL. The Maudsley reactive and non-reactive strains of rats: a survey. Behav Genet 1975;5:299319. [22] Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H. Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus stimulus association. Hippocampus 1995;5:546 56. [23] Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H. Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 1996;379:255 7. [24] Burwell RD, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: interconnectivity and connections with the entorhinal cortex. J Comp Neurol 1998;391:293 321. [25] Burwell RD, Amaral DG. Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal, and entorhinal cortices of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1998; 398:179205. [26] Canteras N, Swanson L. Projections of the ventral subiculum to the amygdala, septum, and hypothalamus: a PHAL anterograde tracttracing study in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1992;324:180 94. [27] Cassaday HJ, Rawlins JNP. The hippocampus, objects, and their contexts. Behav Neurosci 1997;111:1228 44. [28] Clark CVH, Isaacson RL. Effect of bilateral hippocampal ablation on DRL performance. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1965;59:13740. [29] Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [30] Colombo M, Fernandez T, Nakamura K, Gross CG. Functional differentiation along the anterior posterior axis of the hippocampus in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1998;80:1002 5. [31] Corkin S, Amaral DG, Gonzalez RG, Johnson KA, Hyman BT. H.M.s medial temporal lobe lesion: ndings from magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997;17:396479. [32] Cullinan WE, Herman JP, Watson SJ. Ventral subicular interaction with the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus: evidence for a relay in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. J Comp Neurol 1993;332: 1 20. [33] Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear-potentiated startle: implications for animal models of anxiety. Trends Pharmacol 1992; 13:3541. [34] Davis M, Gendelman DS, Tischler MD, Gendelman PM. A primary acoustic startle circuit: lesions and stimulation studies. J Neurosci 1982;2:781805. [35] Davis M, Shi C. The extended amygdala: are the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis differentially involved in fear versus anxiety? Ann NY Acad Sci 1999;877: 281 91. [36] Deacon RMJ, Bannerman DM, Rawlins JNP. Anxiolytic effects of cytotoxic hippocampal lesions in rats. Behav Neurosci 2002;116: 494 7. [37] Decker MW, Curzon P, Brioni JD. Inuence of separate and combined septal and amygdala lesions on memory, acoustic startle, anxiety and locomotor activity in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1995; 64:15668. [38] De Hoz L, Knox J, Morris RGM. Longitudinal axis of the hippocampus: both septal and temporal poles of the hippocampus

This would enable the consequences of subeld-specic interventions to be predicted for each domain. Genetargeted manipulations that modify specic forms of synaptic plasticity have already been shown to result in striking dissociations within the domain of spatial learning and memory [111]. A unifying theory should also allow us to dene the kinds of dissociations that these manipulations will bring about in other aspects of hippocampus-dependent information processing.

References
[1] Agster KL, Fortin NJ, Eichenbaum H. The hippocampus and disambiguation of overlapping sequences. J Neurosci 2002;22: 5760 8. [2] Amaral DG, Witter MP. The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience 1989;31:57191. [3] Amaral DG, Witter MP. Hippocampal formation. In: Paxinos G, editor. The rat nervous system. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1995. p. 44393. [4] Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS. Hippocampus and contextual fear conditioning: recent controversies and advances. Hippocampus 2001;11:8 17. [5] Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS, et al. The hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning: reply to Bast et al. Hippocampus 2002;12:5615. [6] Bakshie VP, Geyer MA. Multiple limbic regions mediate the disruption of prepulse inhibition produced in rats by the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist dizocilpine. J Neurosci 1998;18: 8394 401. [7] Bakshie VP, Geyer MA. Alpha-1-adrenergic receptors mediate sensorimotor gating decits produced by intracerebral dizocilpine administration in rats. Neuroscience 1999;92:113 21. [8] Bannerman DM, Deacon RMJ, Offen S, Friswell J, Grubb M, Rawlins JNP. A double dissociation of function within the hippocampus: spatial memory and hyponeophagia. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:884901. [9] Bannerman DM, Grubb M, Deacon RMJ, Yee BK, Feldon J, Rawlins JNP. Ventral hippocampal lesions affect anxiety but not spatial learning. Behav Brain Res 2002;139:197213. [10] Bannerman DM, Yee BK, Good MA, Heupel MJ, Iversen SD, Rawlins JNP. Double dissociation of function within the hippocampus: a comparison of dorsal, ventral and complete hippocampal cytotoxic lesions. Behav Neurosci 1999;113: 1170 88. [11] Barbas H, Blatt GJ. Topographically specic hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey. Hippocampus 1995;5:511 33. [12] Barry H, Wagner AR, Miller NE. Effects of alcohol and amobarbital on performance inhibited by experimental extinction. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1962;55:464 8. [13] Bast T, Feldon J. Hippocampal modulation of sensorimotor processes. Prog Neurobiol 2003;70:319 45. [14] Bast T, Zhang W-N, Feldon J. Hippocampus and classical fear conditioning. Hippocampus 2001;11:82831. [15] Bast T, Zhang W-N, Feldon J. Hyperactivity, decreased startle reactivity, and disrupted prepulse inhibition following disinhibition of the rat ventral hippocampus by the GABA (A) receptor antagonist picrotoxin. Psychopharmacology 2001;156:225 33. [16] Bast T, Zhang W-N, Feldon J. Dorsal hippocampus and classical fear conditioning: the effects of local NMDA-receptor blockade and stimulation. Hippocampus 2003;13:657 75.

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283 support water maze spatial learning depending on the training protocol. Hippocampus 2003;13:587 603. Dolorfo CL, Amaral DG. Entorhinal cortex of the rat: topographic organization of the cells of origin of the perforant path projection to the dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 1998;398:25 48. Dolorfo CL, Amaral DG. Entorhinal cortex of the rat: organization of intrinsic connections. J Comp Neurol 1998;398:49 82. Dunn AJ, Berridge CW. Physiological and behavioral responses to corticotropin-releasing factor administration: is CRF a mediator of anxiety or stress responses? Brain Res Rev 1990; 15:71100. Eichenbaum H, Fortin N. Episodic memory and the hippocampus: its about time. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2003;12:53 7. Fanselow MS. What is conditioned fear? Trends Neurosci 1984;7: 4602. Ferbinteanu J, McDonald RJ. Dorsal and ventral hippocampus: same or different? Psychobiology 2000;28:31424. Ferbinteanu J, Ray C, McDonald RJ. Both dorsal and ventral hippocampus contribute to spatial learning in Long-Evans rats. Neurosci Lett 2003;345:131 5. File SE. Behavioral detection of anxiolytic action. In: Elliot JM, Heal J, Marsden CA, editors. Experimental approaches to anxiety and depression. London: Wiley; 1992. p. 25 44. File SE, Gonzalez LE, Gallant R. Role of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala in the formation of phobia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1998;19:397 405. Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nature Neurosci 2002;5:45862. Goldman-Rakic PS, Selemon LD, Schwartz ML. Dual pathways connecting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with the hippocampal formation and the parahippocampal cortex in the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 1984;12:71943. Gonzalez LE, Andrews N, File SE. 5-HT1A and benzodiazepine receptors in the basolateral amygdala modulate anxiety in the social interaction test, but not in the elevated plus-maze. Brain Res 1996; 732:14553. Good M, Honey RC. Dissociable effects of selective lesions to hippocampal subsystems on exploratory behaviour, contextual learning and spatial learning. Behav Neurosci 1997;111:487 93. Gray JA. Sodium amobarbital and effects of frustrative nonreward. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1970;69:5564. Gray JA. The psychology of fear and stress. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson; 1971. Gray JA. Emotionality in male and female rodents: a reply to Archer. Br J Psychol 1979;70:43540. Gray JA. The neuropsychology of anxiety, 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1982. Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. Henke PG. Hippocampal pathway to the amygdala and the stress ulcer development. Brain Res Bull 1990;25:691 5. Herman JP, Dolgas CM, Carlson SL. Ventral subiculum regulates hypothalamo-pituitaryadrenocortical and behavioural responses to cognitive stressors. Neuroscience 1998;86:44959. Hock Jr BJ, Bunsey MD. Differential effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions. J Neurosci 1998;18:702732. Hughes KR. Dorsal and ventral hippocampus lesions and maze learning: inuence of preoperative environment. Can J Psychol 1965;19:325 32. Iwata J, LeDoux JE, Meeley MP, Arneric S, Reis DJ. Intrinsic neurones in the amygdaloid eld projected to by the medial geniculate body mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli. Brain Res 1986;383:195214. Jacobsen L, Sapolsky R. The role of the hippocampus in feedback regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenocortical axis. Endocrine Rev 1991;12:11834.

281

[39]

[40] [41]

[42] [43] [44] [45]

[46]

[47]

[48]

[49]

[50]

[51]

[52] [53] [54] [55] [56] [57] [58]

[59] [60]

[61]

[62]

[63] Jarrard LE. On the use of ibotenic acid to lesion selectively different components of the hippocampal formation. J Neurosci Meth 1989; 29:2519. [64] Jarrard LE, Isaacson RL. Runway response perseveration in the hippocampectomised rat: determined by extinction variables. Nature 1965;207:10910. [65] Jay TM, Witter MP. Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 1991;313:57486. [66] Jung MW, Wiener SI, McNaughton BL. Comparison of spatial ring characteristics of units in dorsal and ventral hippocampus of the rat. J Neurosci 1994;14:734756. [67] Kemble ED, Ison JR. Limbic lesions and the inhibition of startle reactions in the rat by conditions of preliminary stimulation. Physiol Behav 1971;7:9258. [68] Kesner RP, Gilbert PE, Barua LA. The role of the hippocampus in memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:286 90. [69] Kimble DP, Kimble RJ. Hippocampectomy and response perseveration in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1965;60:474 6. [70] Kjelstrup KG, Tuvnes FA, Steffenach H-A, Murison R, Moser EI, Moser M-B. Reduced fear expression after lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:10825 30. [71] Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol 1999;59: 107 28. [72] Krettek JE, Price JL. Projections from the amygdaloid complex and adjacent olfactory structures to the entorhinal cortex and to the subiculum in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol 1977;172:72352. [73] Lang PJ. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. Am Psychol 1995;50:37285. [74] Leaton RN. Habituation of startle response, lick suppression, and exploratory behavior in rats with hippocampal lesions. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1981;95:813 26. [75] Lee Y, Davis M. Role of the septum in the excitatory effect of corticotropin-releasing hormone on the acoustic startle reex. J Neurosci 1997;17:642433. [76] Lee Y, Davis M. Role of the hippocampus, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and the amygdala in the excitatory effect of corticotropin-releasing hormone on the acoustic startle reex. J Neurosci 1997;17:643446. [77] Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97: 4398 403. [78] Maren S. Neurotoxic or electrolytic lesions of the ventral subiculum produce decits in the acquisition and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:28390. [79] Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res 1997;88:26174. [80] Maren S, Anagnostaras SG, Fanselow MS. The startled seahorse: is the hippocampus necessary for contextual fear conditioning? Trends Cogn Sci 1998;2:3942. [81] McHugh SB, Deacon RMJ, Rawlins JNP, Bannerman DM. Amygdala and ventral hippocampal lesions contribute differentially to mechanisms of fear and anxiety. Behav Neurosci 2004;118: 63 78. [82] McNaughton N, Corr P; 2004 [this issue] doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.005. [83] McNish KA, Gewirtz JC, Davis M. Evidence of contextual fear after lesions of the hippocampus: a disruption of freezing but not fearpotentiated startle. J Neurosci 1997;17:9353 60. [84] Milner B, Squire LR, Kandel ER. Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron 1998;20:44568. [85] Mitchell SJ, Rawlins JNP, Steward O, Olton DS. Medial septal area lesions disrupt theta rhythm and cholinergic staining in medial

282

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283 entorhinal cortex and produce impaired radial arm maze behavior in rats. J Neurosci 1982;2:292302. Morris RGM, Garrud P, Rawlins JNP, OKeefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 1982;297:681 3. Morris RGM, Moser EI, Riedel G, Martin SJ, Sandin J, Day M, OCarroll C. Elements of a neurobiological theory of the hippocampus: the role of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 2003;358:773 86. Moser MB, Moser EI. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 1998;8:608 19. Moser MB, Moser EI. Distributed encoding and retrieval of spatial memory in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1998;18:753542. Moser EI, Moser MB, Andersen P. Spatial learning impairment parallels the magnitude of dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is hardly present following ventral lesions. J Neurosci 1993;13:391625. Moser M-B, Moser EI, Forrest E, Andersen P, Morris RGM. Spatial learning with a minislab in the dorsal hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995;92:9697 701. Nadel L. Dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions and behavior. Physiol Behav 1968;3:891900. Nadel L, Willner J. Context and conditioning: a place for space. Physiol Psychol 1980;8:218 28. Nagy J, Zambo K, Decsi L. Anti-anxiety action of diazepam after intra-amygdaloid application in the rat. Neuropharmacology 1979; 18:5736. Niehoff DL, Kuhar MJ. Benzodiazepine receptors: localisation in rat amygdala. J Neurosci 1983;3:2091 7. Niehoff DL, Whitehouse PJ. Multiple benzodiazepine receptors: autoradiographic localization in normal human amygdala. Brain Res 1983;276:23745. OKeefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1978. Olton DS, Becker JT, Handelmann E. Hippocampus, space and memory. Behav Brain Sci 1979;2:313 66. Peleg D, Pezze MA, Ferger B, Murphy CA, Feldon J, Bast T. In: Kehr J, Fuxe K, editors. Effect of ventral hippocampal NMDA stimulation on dopamine transmission in medial prefrontral cortex: in vivo microdialysis study in freely moving rats. 2003. Pesold C, Treit D. The septum and amygdala differentially mediate the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Brain Res 1994;638: 295301. Pesold C, Treit D. The central and basolateral amygdala differentially mediate the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Brain Res 1995;671:21321. Petrovich GD, Canteras NS, Swanson LW. Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res Rev 2001;38:247 89. Pezze M, Bast T, Feldon J. Signicance of dopamine transmission in the rat medial prefrontal cortex for conditioned fear. Cereb Cortex 2003;13:37180. Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 1992;106:274 85. Pitkanen A, Pikkarainen M, Nurminen N, Ylinen A. Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the hippocampal formation, perirhinal cortex, and postrhinal cortex in rat. A review. Ann NY Acad Sci 2000;911:36991. Pothuizen HHJ, Zhang W-N, Jongen-Relo AL, Feldon J, Yee BK. Dissociation of function between the dorsal and ventral hippocampus in spatial learning abilities of the rat: a within-subject, within-task comparison of reference and working spatial memory. Eur J Neurosci 2004;19:705 12. Poucet B, Thinus-Blanc C, Muller RU. Place cells in the ventral hippocampus of rats. Neuroreport 1994;5:2045 8. Pouzet B, Feldon J, Veenman CL, Yee BK, Richmond M, Rawlins JNP, Weiner I. The effects of hippocampal and mbria-fornix lesions on prepulse inhibition. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:968 81. [109] Ramos A, Berton O, Mormede P, Chauloff F. A multiple-test study of anxiety-related behaviors in six inbred rat strains. Behav Brain Res 1997;85:57 69. [110] Rawlins JNP, Olton DS. The septo-hippocampal system and cognitive mapping. Behav Brain Res 1982;5:33158. [111] Reisel D, Bannerman DM, Schmitt WB, Deacon RMJ, Flint J, Borchardt T, Seeburg PH, Rawlins JNP. The spatial memory decit in GluR1 decient mice. Nature Neurosci 2002;5:86873. [112] Richmond MA, Yee BK, Pouzet B, Veenman L, Rawlins JNP, Feldon J, Bannerman DM. Dissociating context and space within the hippocampus: effects of complete, dorsal and ventral excitotoxic lesions on conditioned freezing and spatial learning. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:1189203. [113] Risold PY, Swanson LW. Structural evidence for functional domains in the rat hippocampus. Science 1996;272:14846. [114] Rodgers RJ. Animal models of anxiety: where next? Behav Pharmacol 1997;8:47796. [115] Rudy JW, Sutherland RJ. Congural association theory and the hippocampal formation: an appraisal and reconguration. Hippocampus 1995;5:375 89. [116] Sanders MJ, Wiltgen BJ, Fanselow MS. The place of the hippocampus in fear conditioning. Eur J Pharmacol 2003;463: 217 23. [117] Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20:11 21. [118] Siegel A, Tassoni JP. Differential efferent projections from the ventral and dorsal hippocampus of the cat. Brain Behav Evol 1971;4: 185 200. [119] Sinnamon HM, Feniere S, Kootz J. Rat hippocampus and memory for places of changing signicance. J Comp Physiol 1978;92: 142 55. [120] Sommer W, Moller C, Wiklund L, Thorsell A, Rimondini R, Nissbrandt H, Heilig M. Local 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine lesions of rat amygdala: release of punished drinking, unaffected plus-maze behavior and ethanol consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;24:43040. [121] Stevens R, Cowey A. Effects of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions on spontaneous alternation, learned alternation, and probably learning in rats. Brain Res 1973;52:203 24. [122] Sutherland RJ, Rudy JW. Congural association theory: the role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory and amnesia. Psychobiology 1989;17:12944. [123] Swanson LW, Cowan WM. An autoradiographic study of the organization of the efferent connections of the hippocampal formation in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1977;172:4984. [124] Thierry AM, Gioanni Y, Degenetais E, Glowinski J. Hippocampoprefrontal cortex pathway: anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus 2000;10:4119. [125] Treit D, Menard J. Dissociations among the anxiolytic effects of septal, hippocampal and amygdaloid lesions. Behav Neurosci 1997; 111:6538. [126] Treit D, Pesold C, Rotzinger S. Dissociating the anti-fear effects of septal and amygdala lesions using two pharmacologically validated models of rat anxiety. Behav Neurosci 1993;107:770 85. [127] Treit D, Pesold C, Rotzinger S. Noninteractive effects of diazepam and amygdaloid lesions in two animal models of anxiety. Behav Neurosci 1993;107:1099105. [128] Tuvnes FA, Steffenach H-A, Murison R, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Selective hippocampal lesions do not increase adrenocortical activity. J Neurosci 2003;23:434554. [129] Van Groen T, Wyss JM. Extrinsic projections from area CA1 of the rat hippocampus: olfactory, cortical, subcortical, and bilateral hippocampal formation projections. J Comp Neurol 1990;302: 515 28. [130] Vann SD, Brown MW, Erichsen JT, Aggleton JP. Fos imaging reveals differential patterns of hippocampal and parahippocampal

[86] [87]

[88] [89] [90]

[91]

[92] [93] [94]

[95] [96]

[97] [98] [99]

[100]

[101]

[102]

[103]

[104]

[105]

[106]

[107] [108]

D.M. Bannerman et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 273283 subeld activation in rats in response to different spatial memory tests. J Neurosci 2000;20:2711 8. [131] Verwer RW, Meijer RJ, Van Uum HF, Witter MP. Collateral projections from the rat hippocampal formation to the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex. Hippocampus 1997;7:397402. [132] Wan FJ, Caine SB, Swerdlow NR. The ventral subiculum modulation of prepulse inhibition is not mediated via dopamine D2 or nucleus accumbens non-NMDA glutamate receptor activity. Eur J Pharmacol 1996;314:918. [133] Witter MP. A survey of the anatomy of the hippocampal formation, with emphasis on the septotemporal organization of its intrinsic and extrinsic connections. In: Schwarcz R, Ben-Ar Y, editors. Excitatory amino acids and epilepsy (Advances in Experimental and Medical Biology, 203). New York: Plenum Press; 1986. p. 6782.

283

[134] Yeomans JS, Frankland PW. The acoustic startle reex: neurons and connections. Brain Res Rev 1996;21:30114. [135] Zhang WN, Bast T, Feldon J. Microinfusion of the non-competitive N-methyl-D -aspartate receptor antagonist MK-801 (dizocilpine) into the dorsal hippocampus of wistar rats does not affect latent inhibition and prepulse inhibition, but increases startle reaction and locomotor activity. Neuroscience 2000;101:58999. [136] Zhang WN, Bast T, Feldon J. Effects of hippocampal N-methyl-D aspartate infusion on locomotor activity and prepulse inhibition: differences between the dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Behav Neurosci 2002;116:72 84. [137] Zhang WN, Bast T, Feldon J. Prepulse inhibition in rats with temporary inhibition/inactivation of ventral or dorsal hippocampus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2002;73:92940.

You might also like