You are on page 1of 12

Christian Homburg, Nicole Koschate, & Wayne D.

Hoyer

The Role of Cognition and Affect in


the Formation of Customer
Satisfaction: A Dynamic Perspective
Despite the strong recognition that customer satisfaction should be viewed from a dynamic perspective, iittie is
known about how the satisfaction judgment deveiops over time. Therefore, this study provides a dynamic analysis
of the simultaneous influence of cognition and affect in the satisfaction formation process. The results of an
experimental study based on a real consumption experience indicate that the impact of cognition on the satisfaction
evaluation increases and the influence of affect decreases over time. Moreover, these effects are attenuated with
inconsistent performance experiences. Finally, the study shows that the variance in customer satisfaction jointly
explained by cognition and affect increases as experience accumulates.

T
he topic of customer satisfaction/dissatisfaction has (1997, p. 319) states that affect "coexists alongside various
been of great interest to marketing and consumer cognitive judgments in producing satisfaction" and that it is
researchers for many years. This interest is driven in central to understanding customers' consumption experi-
part by the notion that customer satisfaction can have tong- ences. In addition, a key conclusion of Szymanski and
term benefits, including customer loyalty and increased Henard's (2001) meta-analysis is that disconfirmation and,
profitability (e.g., Anderson, Fomell, and Lehmann 1994; to a lesser extent, affect are strongly related to satisfaction.
Anderson, Fomell, and Rust 1997; Anderson and Sullivan However, only a few studies have investigated cognitive
1993; Bearden and Teel 1983; Bolton and Drew 1991; For- and affective antecedents of customer satisfaction simulta-
nell 1992; LaBarbera and Mazursky 1983; Oliver 1980; neously. For example, in studying affects that are provoked
Oliver and Swan 1989a, b; Rust, Moorman, and Dickson by service failures. Smith and Bolton (2002) find that feel-
2002; Rust and Zahorik 1993). Against this background, it ings predict satisfaction levels, after accounting for cogni-
is important to understand the mechanism that leads to cus- tive factors. Kempfs (1999) study finds that feelings (e.g.,
tomer satisfaction from both an academic and an applied arousal, pleasure) are particularly important antecedents of
perspective. product evaluations of hedonic products, whereas brand
Previous research has recognized that both cognition cognitions are not. The opposite is tme for functional prod-
and affect significantly predict satisfaction judgments. Cog- ucts. Oliver (1993) examines the role of disconfirmation
nition has been studied mainly in terms of the disconfirma- and affective experiences and finds that both are critical to
tion paradigm, which predicts satisfaction to be a function the modeling of satisfaction judgments.
of a comparison between expectations and performance Although previous studies identify the key roles of cog-
(e.g., Bearden and Teel 1983; LaBarbera and Mazursky nition and affect in influencing satisfaction, it is important
1983; Oliver 1980; Oliver and DeSarbo 1988). Other stud- to acknowledge that previous work has largely been static
ies have recognized that the affect experienced during the (i.e., cross-sectional) in nature. This represents a significant
acquisition and consumption of the product or service (e.g., research gap because it is well established that customer
joy, happiness, disgust) can also have a significant influence satisfaction is a dynamic phenomenon. Even the classic
on satisfaction judgments (e.g., Mano and Oliver 1993; confirmation/disconfirmation paradigm, which has guided
Westbrook 1987; Westbrook and Oliver 1991). Oliver much of the research in the area, recognizes that satisfaction
judgments can have an important influence on customer
expectations for subsequent consumption occasions (Oliver
Christian Homburg is Professor of Business Administration and Marketing 1980). The dynamic nature of customer satisfaction is also
and Chairman of the Department of Marketing (e-mail: homburg@bwi.uni- emphasized in the work by Bolton (1998), who proposes a
mannheim.de), and Nicole Koschate is Assistant Professor of Marketing dynamic model of cumulative satisfaction. In a similar vein,
(e-maii: nicoie.koschate@bwi.uni-mannheim.de). University of Mannheim, Bolton and Lemon (1999) stress the importance of a
Germany. Wayne D. Hoyer is James L. Bayiess/Wiliiam S. Farish Fund
dynamic perspective with their model of customer service
Chair for Free Enterprise and Chairman of the Department of Marketing,
McCombs School of Business, University of Texas at Austin (e-mail: usage. Despite this strong recognition that customer satis-
wayne.hoyer@mccombs.utexas.edu). The authors thank the Deutsche faction should be viewed from a dynamic perspective, the
Forschungsgemeinschaft, Sonderforschungsbereich 504, for financiai role of cognitive and affective influences has not been sys-
support. tematically studied in this manner.
The few studies that investigate the antecedents of cus-
To read or conttibute to teader and authot dialogue on this atticle, visit
http://www,matketingpowet,com/jmblog.
tomer satisfaction from a dynamic perspective focus on the
cognitive component of customer satisfaction. For example.

© 2006, American Marketing Association Journal of t\/larketing


ISSN: 0022-2429 (print), 1547-7185 (eiectronic) 21 Voi. 70 (July 2006), 21-31
Mittal, Kumar, and Tsiros (1999) and Mittal, Katrichis, and addition to the theoretical interest of this topic, it has practi-
Kumar (2001) find that the weights given to an attribute in cal relevance because it aids managers in understanding the
determining satisfaction shift over time on the basis of their development of customer satisfacfion and what types of fac-
salience. Slotegraaf and Inman (2004) show how satisfac- tors are most crifical at different stages in the process.
tion with product attributes can decline over time (particu-
larly those that can be remedied and especially as customers
approach the end of their warranty period). Other dynamic Development of Hypotheses
studies in this area focus on carryover effects of previous According to Oliver (1997), a safisfaction evaluation is a
satisfaction judgments to subsequent satisfaction evalua- specific type of attitude (i.e., postconsumption evaluafion).
tions (Bolton and Drew 1991; Boulding et al. 1993; Mittal, Therefore, to develop our hypotheses on the customer safis-
Kumar, and Tsiros 1999). Bolton (1998) proposes a faction formation process, we draw on research on attitudes.
dynamic model in which the duration of customer relation- Research in this area has found that newly formed atfitudes
ship depends on an anchoring and adjustment process in are unstable and held with less certainty and therefore are
which cumulative satisfaction serves as the anchor for sub- more difficult to predict (Eagly and Chaiken 1993). Cer-
sequent experiences. Nevertheless, to the best of our knowl- tainty has been shown to increase as the quanfity of infor-
edge, there is no study that investigates the effects of cogni- mation about the product increases (Berscheid et al. 1976;
tion and affect simultaneously over time. Dover and Olson 1977; Farley, Katz, and Lehmann 1978).
Against this background, our study investigates cogni- Over repeated consumpfion experiences, customers gain
tion and affect simultaneously as drivers of customer satis- more information about the product, and therefore their
faction in a dynamic context. The fundamental proposition judgment certainty should increase (Fazio 1986; Smith and
of our study is that the role of cognition and affect may Swinyard 1983). Thus, with repeated experience, the cogni-
change over time. More specifically, we argue that affect tive and affective judgments underlying these attitudes
plays its strongest role at early stages of satisfaction devel- become associated with higher certainty (i.e., become
opment because of a lack of knowledge about the product stronger and more stable) and should be better able to pre-
or service. As experience accumulates, however, the impact dict the satisfaction evaluation (Chandrashekaran et al.
of cognition should increase. To study this proposition, we 2000). We hypothesize the following;
examine customer satisfaction formation, beginning from a
point at which customers have no prior experience with the Hj: The variance in customer satisfaction jointly explained by
product at all. In this respect, our study differs from previ- cognitive and affective factors increases as experience
accumulates.
ous work in this area, which essentially considers situations
in which customers have some prior experience with the As we mentioned previously, a key contribufion of this
product or service. In addifion, we examine this process in article is its examinafion of whether the impact of cognifion
the context of moderately complex, utilitarian products. and eiffect on the satisfaction judgment changes over time.
' In addition, from a dynamic perspective, the evaluation In particular, we are interested in the early stages of the sat-
process that occurs during consumption serves as an input isfacfion formafion process (i.e., the first consumption
into subsequent decision-making situations. In other words, experiences with the product or service). Whenever cus-
the nature of the feedback received has a strong influence tomers encounter a new product or service, there is typi-
on the satisfaction judgment, and this affects future deci- cally litfie information to draw on from memory (i.e., no
sions. Therefore, we build a case that the phenomenon affect- or cognition-based evaluations). Therefore, cus-
under study also depends on the level of consistency of the tomers are more likely to rely on their feelings because feel-
consumpfion experience (i.e.. Is it consistently positive, ings are often elicited immediately on exposure to a new
consistently negative, or inconsistent?). The importance of stimulus (Pham et al. 2001). In other words, affective
the concept of feedback consistency has been established in responses to stimuli are evoked much quicker than cogni-
related areas, such as job performance (Alvero, Bucklin, five responses. Consistent with this view, several studies
and Ausfin 2001; Stone and Stone 1985). To the best of our have examined a feelings-based inference called the "how-
knowledge, however, this concept has not been studied in do-I-feel-about-it" heurisfic, whereby customers can make
the customer satisfacfion literature. satisfaction or dissatisfacfion judgments based on the
Furthermore, previous research in social psychology valence of their feelings (Gorn, Goldberg, and Basu 1993;
and marketing has indicated that attitudes and judgments Pham 1998; Schwartz and Clore 1988).
become more stable over time after multiple experiences This notion is also related to strategies outlined in the
with the attitude object (Fazio 1986; Petty, Unnava, and affect infusion model (AIM; Forgas 1994, 1995), in which
Strathman 1991; Smith and Swinyard 1983). On the basis affect infusion is highly likely to occur. Affect infusion
of this literature, we argue that over fime and with repeated refers to the process in which affectively loaded information
consumption experiences, satisfaction judgments should influences and becomes part of the judgmental process,
also become more stable and that the ability of both cogni- entering the judge's constructive thought process and even-
tive and affecfive factors to predict customer safisfacfion tually coloring the judgmental outcome (Forgas 1995, p.
should increase. 39). The AIM is a cognitively oriented approach for
Thus, our research is in line with the frequent call for describing the role of affect in judgment processes and thus
longitudinal studies in markefing (e.g., Bolton 1998; fits well with the decision context of the current study. The
Foumier and Mick 1999; Szymanski and Henard 2001). In AIM suggests that it is the constructive, generative nature of

22 / Journal of Marketing, July 2006


most judgments that is critical for affect infusion to occur Thus, we assume that the relative impact of affective factors
(Fiedler 1991). Constructive processing involves the devel- does not decrease as heavily as in the case of consistent
opment of a judgment or decision rule on the spot rather experiences.
than the reliance on a well-formed rule or judgment
H3: The suggested pattern (i.e., the impact of cognition
(Bettman, Luce, and Payne 1998). increases, whereas the impact of affect decreases) is more
Applied to the current context, for the first consumption pronounced in the case of consistent experiences than in
experience, customers should have little information stored the case of inconsistent experiences.
in their memory on which to base a judgment, and therefore
they should rely on affective inputs. As the number of con-
sumption experiences increases, however, customers A Longitudinal Experimental Study
acquire increasing amounts of information about the prod-
uct or service (i.e., through trial and other sources). When Overview
making a satisfaction judgment on subsequent occasions, To enhance the realism of the study, we placed the "cus-
customers can recall this prior infonnation to help them tomers" in an actual consumption situation. Specifically, we
make a satisfaction judgment. In support of this notion, asked them to evaluate a newly created CD-ROM tutorial,
studies have found that previous satisfaction judgments which could be used and purchased to provide academic
affect subsequent satisfaction evaluations (Bolton 1998; assistance in a difficult pricing class. We gave them three
Bolton and Drew 1991; Boulding et al. 1993; Mittal, sample chapters (trials) of the CD-ROM tutorial over time
Kumar, and Tsiros 1999). This notion is consistent with the in a computer-based format. After each trial, we asked them
direct access strategy of the AIM model, which involves the to solve a pricing problem related to the sample chapter,
strongly cued retrieval of a crystallized judgment from and then we gave them feedback on their performance. We
memory, and is a robust process more resistant to affect manipulated performance by varying the quality of the tuto-
infusion. rial and the feedback. After we provided the feedback, we
Thus, we expect that affect has a greater impact on the elicited responses to key measures.
satisfaction judgment in the earlier stages of the satisfaction
Sample
development process when judgments require a higher
degree of constructive processing. In support of this notion. Our sample consisted of 157 marketing students enrolled in
Smith and Bolton (2002) fmd that affect is more likely to a graduate-level pricing class at a large German university.
predict transaction-specific customer satisfaction than Because students represent the target market for the CD-
cumulative satisfaction (which would be based on several ROM study guide, this sample is particularly relevant and
consumption experiences). Thus, we offer the following appropriate. Participants were aware that previous students
hypothesis: had experienced difficulties in this pricing class.
H2: As experience accumulates, (a) the impact of affective Research Design
factors on customer satisfaction decreases, and (b) the
The research design consisted of an 8 (levels of perfor-
impact of cognitive factors on customer satisfaction
increases. mance) X 3 (trial) full factorial design. Performance was a
between-subjects factor, and trial was a within-subjects fac-
Further refining our theoretical reasoning, we propose tor. To manipulate performance, we gave participants a
that the previously mentioned pattem of processing is con- sample chapter and asked them to solve a related pricing
tingent on the consistency of the consumption experience problem. In the high-performance condition, the content of
over time. Thus, we investigate the impact of consistent ver- the CD-ROM sample chapter made it easy to understand
sus inconsistent performance experiences on the proposed and to solve the pricing problem. This was accomplished by
relationships in H2. A consistent experience would result providing the different steps necessary to complete the pric-
from performance feedback that is in the same direction for ing task successfully in a logical order. Furthermore, par-
each consumption occasion (i.e., all consumption experi- ticipants in this condition received positive feedback that
ences are positive, or all are negative). This leads to the the CD-ROM helped them solve the problem. Note that this
building of a more reliable and diagnostic knowledge base feedback was designed to ensure that participants focused
that can be used as a basis for evaluation on subsequent on the performance of the CD-ROM rather than on their
occasions. In support of this notion, studies have found that own performance.
as consistency of information increases, attitude certainty In the low-performance condition, the content of the
also increases (Heslin, Blake, and Rotton 1972; Kahneman CD-ROM chapter was not in logical order and therefore
and Tversky 1973). Therefore, we expect that customers was difficult to read. It also contained little information
switch in early stages from a more constructive processing related to the pricing problem. We also provided partici-
strategy to a direct access strategy. pants with negative feedback that the CD-ROM did not help
Conversely, inconsistent experiences would generate them solve the pricing problem. Again, the goal was to
conflicting information, which would result in less reliable focus participants on the performance with the CD-ROM
and less diagnostic information stored in memory. There- rather than on their own performance.
fore, customers would need to continue engaging in con- To examine the proposed dynamic relationships, we
structive information processing, which, as we mentioned manipulated perfonnance across three different trials. In
previously, involves the significant role of affective factors. each trial, we presented participants with a different chapter

Cognition and Affect in the Formation of Customer Satisfaction / 23


from the CD-ROM tutorial and then asked them to solve a tract participants from the evaluation situation, after which
related pricing problem. Note that the three sample chapters we measured customer satisfaction. After the first trial, par-
covered completely different pricing issues and did not ticipants completed an intervening task. This involved read-
build on one another. Thus, leaming effects, in the sense ing a newspaper article about a recent pricing problem in
that knowledge gained in previous chapters could be helpful practice and answering some open-ended questions about
for understanding subsequent chapters and pricing tasks, the content of the article.
were not present. In the high-performance condition, par- We conducted the second and third trials in a similar
ticipants received the high-performance manipulation on all manner. We told participants that they had the chance to get
three trials. In the low-performance condition, participants more experience with the CD-ROM study guide. They then
received the low-performance manipulation on all three tri- received another sample chapter of the CD-ROM, solved a
als. These two conditions represent consistent performance pricing task, and received feedback. After we measured the
experiences. We then manipulated intermediate levels of key variables, we distracted participants between the second
performance through high- and low-performance combina- and the third trial with an intervening task similar to the one
tions across the three trials. To represent inconsistent expe- described previously. After the third trial, participants com-
riences, we constructed two conditions with a performance pleted a final set of general questions and provided demo-
experience on each subsequent trial that was different from graphic and classification infonnation.
the most recent one (H h and - + - ) . We outline the differ-
ent conditions in Table 1. Measurement of Variables

Experimental Procedure The key variables of interest were cognition and affect. We
measured cognition in terms of disconfirmation, which is
Before the first trial, we gave participants the introductory the key cognitive component in the confirmation/disconfir-
section, which described the purpose of the CD-ROM and mation paradigm (Oliver 1980). We measured disconfirma-
content. We told them that a CD-ROM study guide had tion with the following item, which parallels previous
been developed to assist participants in solving difficult approaches in measuring disconfirmation (Fomell et al.
pricing problems in the course. Furthermore, we informed 1996): "The performance of the CD-ROM meets my expec-
them that they would have the chance to test the CD-ROM tations." We evaluated the item on a seven-point Likert-type
tutorial before deciding if they wanted to purchase it. We scale anchored by "I totally disagree" (1) and "I totally
then set up expectations about the CD-ROM tutorial and agree" (7).
held them constant across the experimental conditions. We
We measured affect with three items. These items
told participants that the CD-ROM tutorial contained 73
included elation, delight, and joy, which have also been
chapters, which would be similar to the ones they received
measured in other studies examining affect (e.g., Holbrook
in the testing phase but would cover different pricing topics.
and Batra 1987; Oliver 1993; Westbrook 1987; Westbrook
The introductory section of the CD-ROM tutorial informed
and Oliver 1991). We asked participants how intensively
participants that the purpose of the study guide was to help
they experienced these types of affect on a seven-point
course participants understand difficult material in the class,
Likert-type scale.
and it provided an overview of the content.
In addition, we assessed affect and cognition with an
Participants then received one sample chapter of the
open-ended, verbal protocol measures. We asked partici-
CD-ROM study guide (first trial), after which they solved a
pants to describe any feelings and/or thoughts they had
problem related to the material. We manipulated perfor-
while using the tutorial. Responses were then coded into
mance in the manner described previously. The measure-
affective and cognitive categories. Responses were coded as
ment of cognition and affect occurred immediately after
affective if the statements related to any type of affect or
participants received the feedback from the performance
feelings; they were coded as cognitive if they related to the
manipulation. We then asked intervening questions to dis-
content and functional performance of the tutorial.
Research assistants carried out the codings. In terms of
TABLE 1 coder reliability, the coders agreed in 96% of the cases, and
Overview of Experimental Conditions they resolved disagreements through discussions.
The key dependent variable was customer satisfaction,
Trial which we measured with the following three items: "All in
Experimental
Condition all, I am satisfied with the CD-ROM tutorial"; "The CD-
ROM tutorial compares to an ideal CD-ROM tutorial"; and
1 "Overall, how satisfied are you with the CD-ROM tuto-
2 rial?" We measured the items on an U-point Likert-type
3 scale. For the first two items, the scale ranged from
4
5 "strongly agree" to "strongly disagree," and for the third
6 item, it ranged from "very satisfied" to "very dissatisfied."
7 The intemal consistency of the satisfaction scale was excel-
8 lent across the three trials (first trial: Cronbach's a = .95;
Notes: + = high performance, and - = low performance. second trial: Cronbach's a - .97; and third trial: Cronbach's

24 / Journai of Marketing, July 2006


a = .96). Thus, for further analyses, we calculated the satis- sented by the different trials (e.g., trial 1 = time 1). The ran-
faction scores as the means of the satisfaction scale items. dom error associated with timej is represented by rj.
Finally, we collected several variables as possible In the testing of H2_ the coefficients of the two interac-
covariates. These included participants' age, gender, tion terms, b4 and bs, are of key interest. These interaction
income, degree to which the budget for studying material is terms are similar to slope-shift parameters (Mittal,
exhausted, perceived pressure to buy the CD-ROM tutorial, Katrichis, and Kumar 2001), which enable us to estimate
price consciousness, value consciousness, and self- the differential slopes over time as experience accumulates.
confidence. Analyses indicate that the fixed effects of these A significant, positive coefficient for the interaction term
covariates are not significant.' Thus, we dropped them from implies that the slope is higher for those who have more
further analysis. experience with the CD-ROM. In other words, the corre-
sponding factor has a larger weight for customers who have
more experience with the CD-ROM than for those who
Results have less experience with it. On the basis of our theoretical
Hi predicted that the variance in customer satisfaction reasoning, we expect the coefficient b4 to be positive.
jointly explained by cognitive and affective factors A negative coefficient for the interaction term implies
increases as experience accumulates. As participants gain that the slope is lower for those who have more experience
more experience with the CD-ROM tutorial across the three with the CD-ROM. The corresponding factor has a smaller
trials, the satisfaction judgment becomes more cumulative. weight for customers who have more experience with the
To test H|, we estimated the following regression model for CD-ROM than for those who have less experience with it.
each of the three trials: On the basis of our theoretical reasoning, we expect the
coefficient b^ to be negative. Furthermore, statistically non-
(1) CS = bo + b | X CO + b2 X AF + r.
significant coefficients imply that the weight of the corre-
where CS is customer satisfaction, CO is the cognitive pre- sponding factor is invariant to the experience a customer
dictor, AF is the affective predictor, and r is the random has with the CD-ROM. We estimated the model with the
error. maximum likelihood method using the procedure MIXED
We estimated the three regression models with the in SAS 8.02. The estimation results appear in Table 3.
maximum likelihood method using the procedure MIXED The results indicate that there is an overall statistically
in SAS 8.02.2 xhe results appear in Table 2. First, in each of significant, positive relationship between cognition and sat-
the three trials, there is a positive and statistically signifi- isfaction (bl = 22.380, p < .001) and between affect and sat-
cant relationship between the cognitive predictor and satis- isfaction (b2 = 8.490, p < .001). In addition, there is a sig-
faction, and there is a positive and statistically significant nificant, positive interaction between cognition and time
relation between the affective predictor and satisfaction. (b4 = 1.960, p < .05). This means that the impact of cogni-
Second, the results indicate that the R-square values sub- tive factors on the satisfaction judgment increases over
stantially increase from the first (.645) to the third (.731) time. Thus, there is support for H2a.
trial. These results support Hj. Furthermore, there is a significant, negative interaction
Our second analysis pertained to the changing role of between affect and time (bs = -2.330, p < .05), supporting
cognitive and affective factors over time. H2a predicted that the notion that the impact of affective factors on customer
the impact of affective factors decreases as experience accu- satisfaction decreases over time. Thus, there is also support
mulates, and H2b predicted that the role of cognitive factors for H2b. Taking these two findings together, the impact of
increases as experience accumulates. To conduct a test of cognitive factors on the satisfaction judgment increases
these hypotheses, we estimated the following regression over time, whereas the impact of affective factors decreases.
model with interaction effects: Further support for these hypotheses is provided by
examining the results for the verbal protocol measures. As
(2) i = bo + b, X COi + b2 X AFi + b3 x time;
we mentioned previously, open-ended responses were
b4 X COj X timej + bj x AFj x timej + q. coded into affective and cognitive categories. Overall, there
were more cognitive statements than affective ones. We
where CS; is customer satisfaction, COj is cognition, and
expected this because the product category is cognitive in
AFj is affect at timej. The different points in time are repre-
nature. More important, however, the pattem of responses
over the trials was consistent with the hypotheses. Specifi-
cally, the largest number of emotional statements was made
'The individual significance levels were as follows: age: p - in the first trial (23) and then decreased (14 in both the sec-
.485; gender: p = .140; income: p = .825; degree to which the bud- ond and the third trials). For cognitive statements related to
get for studying material is exhausted: p = .354; perceived pres- the content and functional performance, participants exhib-
sure to buy the CD-ROM tutorial: p = .563; price consciousness: ited an increasing trend. On the first trial, 74 such state-
p = .223; value consciousness: p = .295; and self-confidence: p = ments were made, followed by 89 in the second trial and 90
.103.
2We used full maximum likelihood estimation because the focus
in the third.
of the analysis is on deviance tests and not on the random part H3 proposed that an inconsistent performance experi-
parameters, for which the restricted maximum likelihood method ence would attenuate the increasing impact of cognitive fac-
is preferable (Singer and Willett 2003; Snijders and Bosker 1999). tors and the decreasing impact of affective factors over time

Cognition and Affect in the Formation of Customer Satisfaction / 25


885
ooo

ooo

ai
o r^
^ CO CO
1-^ u i CO
CD 1 -
•*
I
N
CO
cd

CO O3 1-

If) ••5

ooo
ooo
ooo (0
u
LU
"5
ooo 0)
p
00
§ .2
t n cn CO
CJ) CO 1 -
cd oo iri
in T-
I I
N
cd
CC
To
M
10
to O
(0
O tv
2 (A >*
ps. CQ
I "^ v^
(^ o CO
q h- CO (A CO
u cd (0
M< UJ
UJ ^
m (0

o o
88

ooo a>
• * •>- CO
CJ) in q
cd cri (d cd
in -^ N

CO CO CO
cd
I
w
UJ

£
UJ

o
8
1 0)
d) CQ

26 / Journai of iVIarketing, July 2006


(compared with a consistent performance experience). To decrease of affect over time in predicting customer satisfac-
test this hypothesis, we created two subgroups from the data tion. Thus, inconsistent performance experiences attenuate
set. The first group had consistent satisfaction/performance these effects. These results support H3.
experiences (+ + + or ), whereas the second group had
completely inconsistent performance experiences (+ - + or
- + -)•
Discussion
We then ran the regression model with interaction The goal of this study was to examine the joint effects of
effects described previously for each group separately. As cognitive and affective factors on satisfaction judgments in
we show in Table 4, for the consistent performance experi- a dynamic setting. The results of our study show that
ences, the impact of cognition increases slightly, and the together, cognitive and affective factors explain the variance
impact of affect decreases over time, as is evidenced by the in satisfaction judgments well and that the strength of asso-
results for the interaction effects (b4 = .119, p < .06; h^ = ciation increases over time. More important, we found that
-.156, p < .05). In the case of inconsistent performance as the number of experiences increases over time, the influ-
experiences, however, neither of these interaction terms is ence of cognitive factors increases, whereas the influence of
significant (see Table 4). In other words, in the case of affective factors decreases. However, this effect attenuates
inconsistent performance experiences, there is neither a sig- with inconsistent consumption experiences. These findings
nificant increase of cognition over time nor a significant have several theoretical and managerial implications.

TABLE 3
Regression Results with Interaction Effects
- 2 log-likelihood 1566.9

Solution for Fixed Effects


Parameter Effect Estimate t-Value P
bo Intercept 6.072 103.200 .00
Cognition .773 22.380 .00
Affect .275 8.490 .00
Time -.248 -3.440 .00
Cognition x time .083 1.960 .05
Affect X time -.091 -2.330 .02

Solution for Residual Effect


Parameter Estimate Z Value P
1.630 15.350 .00

TABLE 4
Regression Results for Consistent and Inconsistent Performance Experience

Consistent Performance Inconsistent Performance


Experience Experience
- 2 log-likelihood 449.5 422.8

Solution for Fixed Effects Solution for Fixed Effects


Parameter Effect Estimate t-Value P Estimate t-Value P
bo Intercept 6.095 59.660 .00 6.268 48.320 .00
Cognition .742 14.610 .00 .833 10.450 .00
b2 Affect .335 6.600 .00 .249 8.490 .00
b3 Time -.197 -1.590 .12 -.119 3.220 .45
b4 Cognition x time .119 1.870 .06 .048 -.750 .62
bs Affect X time -.156 -2.500 .01 .111 1.200 .23

Solution for Residual Effect Solution for Residual Effect


Parameter Estimate Z Value P Estimate Z Value P
O2 1.419 15.350 .00 1.9844 15.350 .00

Cognition and Affect in the Formation of Customer Satisfaction / 27


Implications nature of experiences can influence satisfaction judgments
as well as the importance of managing customer experi-
Several implications follow from our study. Most important,
ences. If customers have inconsistent experiences, their
our study contributes to the existing literature on customer
evaluations of the product/service will be less crystallized
satisfaction. First, we illustrate the importance of examining
and less stable. Thus, in an effort to improve customer satis-
customer satisfaction from a dynamic perspective. We show
faction and loyalty, it is critical to ensure that customer
that the antecedents leading to customer satisfaction vary
experiences are positive and consistent.
over time. As we mentioned previously, it is well recog-
nized that both cognition and affect have important influ- Fifth, the findings provide further support for research
ences on the satisfaction judgment. However, the key find- on attitude strength in an alternative context (Jaccard et al.
ing of this study points to the differential roles of these 1995; Jacoby et al. 2002; Petty and Krosnick 1995). That is,
factors as experience accumulates. To the best of our cognitive and affective factors were better able to predict
knowledge, this is the first study to examine the role of cog- satisfaction evaluations as the number of trials increased,
nition and affect simultaneously in a dynamic setting. Thus, particularly in the case of consistent experiences. This is in
we provide a deeper integrative understanding of how cog- line with the notion that newly formed attitudes are less sta-
nition and affect combine to form the customer satisfaction ble and, therefore, less predictive of information processing
judgment. Our findings show that these judgments are and behavior (Eagly and Chaiken 1993). As knowledge and
based on different factors, depending on the amount of experience accumulates, the relationship between the cogni-
experience that has been accumulated over time. Thus, cap- tive and affective factors and satisfaction is strengthened.
turing satisfaction judgments at only one time may not pro- Sixth, our study also makes a methodological contribu-
vide a full picture of the underlying process. tion by developing a procedure for studying these dynamic
effects in a tightly controlled, yet realistic setting. This
Second, most studies in this area examine in a cross- methodology could be employed to examine judgment for-
sectional manner the satisfaction judgment process after mation processes in various other product/service contexts
customers already have some experience or knowledge with as well.
the product or service. In the current study, we investigated Finally, on a general level, our study suggests that it is
the customer satisfaction formation process beginning with insightful to investigate marketing phenomena from a
the very first consumption experience. This provides a dynamic perspective. As our study shows, judgment pat-
deeper insight into how the satisfaction judgment crystal- terns change over time.
lizes over time. In this context, we found that affective fac-
tors have their strongest role in the early stages. The cogni- Limitations and Avenues for Further Research
tive component maintains its importance and increases its
Several limitations of this study are worth mentioning. In
role over time.
tum, these provide avenues for further research. First, the
Third, we examine the role of cognition and affect in study was based on a highly utilitarian product (i.e., a CD-
influencing satisfaction judgment processes. Our findings ROM tutorial). In general, the consumption of utilitarian
suggest that affect is particularly important in the early goods is highly cognitively driven and accomplishes a func-
stages of the judgment formation process in which cus- tional or practical task (Strahilevitz and Myers 1998). This
tomers have little knowledge or experience related to the may partially explain why the role of cognition (affect)
product. In other words, when a stimulus is new and unfa- became stronger (weaker) over trials. In other words, as
miliar, feelings appear to play a critical role in the construc- customers gained experience in evaluating the utilitarian
tion of satisfaction judgments, consistent with the how-do- benefits of the product, these cognitive factors gained
I-feel-about-it heuristic (Gorn, Goldberg, and Basu 1993; importance in infiuencing satisfaction. However, it is not
Pham 1998; Schwartz and Clore 1988). As experience accu- clear whether the same pattern would occur for highly
mulates, however, the role of cognition becomes more hedonic products/services. The consumption of hedonic
prominent. That is, a direct access strategy (Forgas 1994, products/services is characterized by highly affective and
1995) can be employed in which evaluations from previous sensory experiences of sensual and/or aesthetic pleasure
trials (based on a more detailed evaluation of product attrib- (Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000; Hirschman and Holbrook
utes) can be recalled from memory. 1982; Okada 2005). Thus, in such cases, the impact of
Fourth, we provide insights into how the inconsistency affect might not decline over time. It is possible that a direct
versus consistency of performance feedback can also affect access strategy will still be employed as experience accu-
the customer satisfaction formation process. Specifically, mulates (i.e., a recall of a previous crystallized evaluation
when experiences are inconsistent, the related judgments from memory), but this prior evaluation would be based
are less stable and predictable. In this case, the changing more on affective than on cognitive factors. In support of
patterns of affect and cognition do not emerge. An implica- this view, Kempf (1999) finds that for hedonic products in
tion of our findings is that affective factors are particularly particular, affective responses to the trial are powerful
important and relevant when the judgments are based on lit- antecedents of consumers' evaluations of a trial experience
tle information (i.e., they are newly formed) or when infor- and, subsequently, brand attitude. In addition, it might be
the case that the relative importance of cognitive versus
mation is inconsistent. In these cases, the cognitive infor-
affective drivers of customer satisfaction varies across prod-
mation has unstable predictive validity, so customers are
uct categories. It is plausible to assume that the relative
less likely to rely on this information. This finding empha-
importance of cognitive factors is higher for utilitarian
sizes the importance of identifying how the pattern or

28 / Journal of iVIariceting, July 2006


products than for hedonic ones. These speculations, which and affect in the formation of customer satisfaction might
are beyond the scope of this article, show that a more further differentiate between different types of emotions.
detailed investigation of affective and cognitive drivers of
customer satisfaction that accounts for different product Implications for Marketing Practice
categories is a fruitful avenue for further research. Our fmdings also have several important implications for
Second, in addition to the product category type, cus- marketing managers. Our study helps managers understand
tomers' product involvement could also play an important customer satisfaction in a more thorough way. It sheds light
role in this context. Product involvement can be defmed as on the formation process of customer satisfaction, and man-
the perceived personal relevance of a product and a cus- agers leam that customer satisfaction has a more stochastic
tomer's inherent interest in a product (Wilkie 1994; character in the early stages. This gives managers the
Zaichkowsky 1985). This study investigated the proposed opportunity to influence the satisfaction judgment to a
relationships in a high-involvement context. In these situa- greater extent in the early stages because the satisfaction
tions, customers usually employ a more analytical has not been crystallized.
information-processing strategy and exhibit increased cog- Furthermore, it is common in practice for managers to
nitive elaborations (Petty, Cacioppo, and Schumann 1983; consider customer satisfaction in a logical, rational manner
Wright 1973). Thus, they are likely to base their decisions (i.e., if the product or service performs well, satisfaction
more on cognitive than on affective factors. Conversely, in will be higher). The results of our study point out that affec-
low-involvement situations, information processing is usu- tive factors can play a critical role as well, particularly in
ally more holistic, and thus decisions might be influenced the early stages of the satisfaction formation process. Thus,
more by affective factors. Apart from these effects, it might achieving customer satisfaction involves not merely discon-
be interesting to investigate whether and how these poten- firmation judgments but a subjective, aifective component
tial effects of customers' product involvement interact with as well. This point is particularly important when compa-
the product category type (hedonic and utilitarian). Thus, nies are in the early stages of establishing a relationship
studying the relevance of involvement in the context of with a customer. Thus, for new relationships or new prod-
affective and cognitive drivers of customer satisfaction is ucts, managers must pay close attention to affective aspects
another interesting area for further research. and be careful to manage them effectively.
Moreover, our study provides insights into managing
Third, in our empirical study, we measured only the customer satisfaction. For example, it shows how important
prevalence of positive affect and did not consider negative it is to manage affect when customers have inconsistent
affect. Because it has been shown that positive and negative experiences over time. Thus, if companies are trying to
affect can have asymmetric effects on customers' judgments recover from a situation in which their products/services
and choices (Mittal, Ross, and Baldasare 1998), a promis- have suffered from inconsistent performance, it appears that
ing avenue for further research might be to investigate posi- it is important to manage the affective factors surrounding
tive and negative affect simultaneously. Prior research also the satisfaction in addition to product/service quality con-
demonstrates that different types of (negative) emotions can siderations. In addition, providing customers with consis-
have opposing moderating effects (Garg, Inman, and Mittal tent, positive experiences is critical to the development of
2005). Thus, additional research on the role of cognition stable satisfaction judgments.

REFERENCES
Alvero, Alicia M., Barbara R. Bucklin, and John Austin (2001), bution, and Attraction," Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
"An Objective Review of the Effectiveness and Essential Char- chology, 34 (3), 978-89.
acteristics of Performance Feedback in Organizational Set- Bettman, James R., Mary Frances Luce, and John W. Payne
tings," Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 21 (1998), "Constructive Consumer Choice Processes," Journal of
(0,3-29. Consumer Research, 25 (3), 187-217.
Anderson, Eugene W., Claes Fomell, and Donald R. Lehmann Bolton, Ruth N. (1998), "A Dynamic Model of the Duration of the
(1994), "Customer Satisfaction, Market Share, and Profitabil- Customer's Relationship with a Continuous Service Provider:
ity: Findings from Sweden," Journal of Marketing, 58 (July), The Role of Satisfaction," Marketing Science, 17 (1), 45-65.
53-66.
and James H. Drew (1991), "A Longitudinal Analysis of
, , and Roland T. Rust (1997), "Customer Satisfac-
the Impact of Service Changes on Customer Attitudes," Jour-
tion, Productivity, and Profitability: Differences Between
Goods and Services," Marketing Science, 16 (2), 129-45. nal of Marketing, 55 (January), 1-9.
- and Mary W. Sullivan (1993), "The Antecedents and Con- and Katherine N. Lemon (1999), "A Dynamic Model of
sequences of Customer Satisfaction for Firms," Marketing Sci- Customers' Usage of Services: Usage as an Antecedent and
ence, 12 (2), 125-43. Consequence of Satisfaction," Journal of Marketing Research,
Bearden, William O. and Jesse E. Teel (1983), "Selected Determi- 36 (May), 171-86.
nants of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaint Reports," Jour- Boulding, William, Ajay Kalra, Richard Staelin, and Valarie A.
nal of Marketing Research, 20 (February), 21-28. Zeithaml (1993), "A Dynamic Process Model of Service Qual-
Berscheid, Ellen, William Graziano, Thomas Monson, and Mar- ity: From Expectations to Behavioral Intentions," Journal of
shall Dermer (1976), "Outcome Dependency: Attention, Attri- Marketing Research, 30 (February), 7-27.

Cognition and Affect in the Formation of Customer Satisfaction / 29


Chandrashekaran, Murali, Kevin McNeilly, Frederick A. Russ, and cessing Models of Attitude Formation," Journal of Consumer
Detelina Marinova (2000), "From Uncertain Intentions to Psychology, 12(1), 21-34.
Actual Behavior: A Threshold Model of Whether and When Kahneman, Daniel and A. Tversky (1973), "On the Psychology of
Salespeople Quit," Journal of Marketing Research, 37 Prediction," Psychological Review, 80 (4), 237-51.
(November), 463-79. Kempf, DeAnna S. (1999), "Attitude Formation from Product
Dhar, Ravi and Klaus Wertenbroch (2000), "Consumer Choice Trial: Distinct Roles of Cognition and Affect for Hedonic and
Between Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods," Journal of Market- Functional Products," Psychology & Marketing, 16 (1), 35-50.
ing Research, 37 (February), 60-71. LaBarbera, Pricilla A. and David Mazursky (1983), "A Longitudi-
Dover, Philip A. and Jerry C. Olson (1977), "Dynamic Changes in nal Assessment of Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction: The
an Expectancy-Value Attitude Model as a Function of Multiple Dynamic Aspect of the Cognitive Process," Journal of Market-
Exposures to Product Information," in Contemporary Market- ing Research, 20 (November), 393^04.
ing Thought: Proceedings of the 1977 AMA Conference, Bar- Mano, Haim and Richard L. Oliver (1993), "Assessing the Dimen-
nett A. Greenberg and Danny N. Bellenger, eds. Chicago: sionality and Structure of the Consumption Experience:
American Marketing Association, 455-60. Evaluation, Feeling, and Satisfaction," Journal of Consumer
Eagly, Alice H. and Shelly Chaiken (1993), The Psychology of Research, 20 (3), 451-67.
Attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Mittal, Vikas, Jerome M. Katrichis, and Pankaj Kumar (2001),
Farley, John U., Jerrold P. Katz, and Donald R. Lehmann (1978), "Attribute Perfonnance and Customer Satisfaction over Time:
"Impact of Different Comparison Sets on Evaluation of a New Evidence from Two Field Studies," Journal of Services Market-
Subcompact Car Brand," Journal of Consumer Research, 5 ing, 15 (5), 343-56.
(September), 138-42. -, Pankaj Kumar, and Michael Tsiros (1999), "Attribute-
Fazio, Russel H. (1986), "How Do Attitudes Guide Behavior?" in Level Performance, Satisfaction, and Behavioral Intentions
Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social over Time: A Consumption-System Approach," Journal of
Behavior, Richard M. Sorrentino and Edward T. Higgins, eds. Marketing, 63 (April), 88-101.
Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, 204-243. -, William T. Ross, and Patrick M. Baldasare (1998), "The
Fiedler, Klaus (1991), "On the Task, the Measures and the Mood: Asymmetric Impact of Negative and Positive Attribute-Level
Research on Affect and Social Cognition," in Emotion and Performance on Overall Satisfaction and Repurchase Inten-
Social Judgments, Joseph P. Forgas, ed. Elmsford, NY: Perga- tions," Journal of Marketing, 62 (January), 33—47.
mon Press, 83-104. Okada, Erica M. (2005), "Justification Effects on Consumer
Forgas, Joseph P. (1994), "The Role of Emotion in Social Judg- Choice of Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods," Journal of Market-
ments: An Introductory Review and an Affect Infusion Model ing Research, 42 (February), 43-53.
(AIM)," European Journal of Social Psychology, 24 (1), 1-24. Oliver, Richard L. (1980), "A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents
(1995), "Mood and Judgment: The Affect Infusion Model and Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions," Journal of Mar-
(AIM)," Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39-66. keting Research, 17 (November), 460-69.
Fomell, Claes (1992), "A National Customer Satisfaction Barome- (1993), "Cognitive, Affective, and Attribute Bases of Sat-
ter: The Swedish Experience," Journal of Marketing, 56 (Janu- isfaction Response," Journal of Consumer Research, 20
ary), 6-21. (December), 418-30.
———, Michael D. Johnson, Eugene W. Anderson, Jaesung Cha, (1997), Satisfaction: A Behavioral Perspective on the
and Barbara Everitt Bryant (1996), "The American Customer Consumer, Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Satisfaction Index: Nature, Purpose, and Findings," Journal of and Wayne S. DeSarbo (1988), "Response Determinants
Marketing, 60 (October), 7-18. in Satisfaction Judgments," Journal of Consumer Research, 14
Foumier, Susan and David G. Mick (1999), "Rediscovering Satis- (March), 495-507.
faction," Journal of Marketing, 63 (October), 5-23. and John E. Swan (1989a), "Consumer Perceptions of
Garg, Nitika, J. Jeffrey Inman, and Vikas Mittal (2005), "Inciden- Interpersonal Equity and Satisfaction in Transactions: A Field
tal and Task-Related Affect: A Re-Inquiry and Extension of the Survey Approach," Journal of Marketing, 53 (April), 21-35.
• Influence of Affect on Choice," Journal of Consumer Research, and (1989b), "Equity and Disconfirmation Percep-
32 (June), 154-59. tions as Influences on Merchant and Product Satisfaction,"
Gom, Gerald J., Marvin E. Goldberg, and Kunal Basu (1993), Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (December), 372-83.
"Mood, Awareness, and Product Evaluation," Journal of Con- Petty, Richard E., John T. Cacioppo, and David T. Schumann
sumer Psychology, 2 (3), 237-56. (1983), "Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effec-
Heslin, Richard, Brian Blake, and James Rotton (1972), "Informa- tiveness: The Moderating Effect of Involvement," Journal of
tion Search as a Function of Stimulus Uncertainty and the Consumer Research, 10 (September), 135—46.
Importance of the Response," Journal of Personality and and Jon A. Krosnick (1995), Attitude Strength:
Social Psychology, 23 (3), 333-39. Antecedents and Consequences, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. and Morris B. Holbrook (1982), "Hedo- baum Associates.
nic Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods, and Proposi- -, H. Rao Unnava, and Alan J. Strathman (1991), "Theories
tions," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101. of Attitude Change," in Handbook of Consumer Behavior,
Holbrook, Morris B. and Rajeev Batra (1987), "Assessing the Role Thomas S. Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian, eds. Engle-
of Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Advertis- wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 241-80.
ing," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (December), 404-420. Pham, Michel Tuan (1998), "Representativeness, Relevance, and
Jaccard, James, Carmen M. Radecki, Tracey E. Wilson, and Patri- the Use of Feelings in Decision Making," Journal of Consumer
cia J. Dittus (1995), "Methods for Identifying Consequential Research, 25 (September), 144-60.
Beliefs: Implications for Understanding Attitude Strength," in , Joel B. Cohen, John W. Pracejus, and G. David Hughes
Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences, Richard E. (2001), "Affect Monitoring and the Primacy of Feelings in
Petty and Jon A. Krosnick, eds. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl- Judgment," Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (September),
baum Associates, 337-59. 167-88.
Jacoby, Jacob, Maureen Morrin, James Jaccard, Zeynep Gurhan, Rust, Roland T., Christine Moorman, and Peter R. Dickson (2002),
Alfred Kuss, and Durairaj Maheswaran (2002), "Mapping Atti- "Getting Retum on Quality: Revenue Expansion, Cost Reduc-
tude Formation as a Function of Information Input: Online Pro- tion, or Both?" Journal of Marketing, 65 (October), 7-24.

30 / Journal of Marketing, July 2006


and Anthony J. Zahorik (1993), "Customer Satisfaction, Stone, Dianna L. and Eugene F. Stone (1985), "The Effects of
Customer Retention, and Market Share," Journal of Retailing, Feedback Consistency and Feedback Favorability on Self-
69 (2), 193-215. Perceived Task Competence and Perceived Feedback Accu-
Schwartz, Nobert and Gerald L. Clore (1988), "How Do I Eeel racy," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
About It? Informative Functions of Affective States," in Affect, cesses, 36 (2), 167-85.
Cognition, and Social Behavior, Klaus Eielder and Joseph For- Strahilevitz, Michal and John Myers (1998), "Donations to Char-
gas, eds. Toronto: Hogrefe Intemational, 44-62. ity as Purchase Incentives: How Well They Work May Depend
Singer, Judith D. and John B. Willett (2003), Applied Longitudinal on What You Are Trying to Sell," Journal of Consumer
Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence, New Research, 24 (2), 434-46.
York: Oxford University Press. Szymanski, David M. and David Henard (2001), "Customer Satis-
Slotegraaf, Rebecca J. and J. Jeffrey Inman (2004), "Longitudinal faction: A Meta-Analysis of the Empirical Evidence," Journal
Shifts in the Drivers of Satisfaction with Product Quality: The of the Academy of Marketing Science, 29 (1), 16-35.
Role of Attribute Resolvability," Journal of Marketing Westbrook, Robert A. (1987), "Product/Consumption-Based
Research, 41 (August), 269-80. Affective Responses and Postpurchase Processes," Journal of
Smith, Amy K. and Ruth N. Bolton (2002), "The Effect of Cus- Marketing Research, 24 (August), 258-70.
tomers' Emotional Responses to Service Failures on Their and Richard L. Oliver (1991), "The Dimensionality of
Recovery Effort Evaluations and Satisfaction Judgments," Consumption Emotion Patterns and Consumer Satisfaction,"
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30 (I), 5-23. Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (June), 84—91.
Smith, Robert E. and William R. Swinyard (1983), Wilkie, William L. (1994), Consumer Behavior, 3d ed. New York:
"Attitude-Behavior Consistency: The Impact of Product Trial John Wiley & Sons.
Versus Advertising," Journal of Marketing Research, 20 Wright, Peter L. (1973), "The Cognitive Processes Mediating
(August), 257-67. Acceptance of Advertising," Journal of Marketing Research,
Snijders, Tom A.B. and Roel J. Bosker (1999), Multilevel Analy- 10 (February), 53-62.
sis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Model- Zaichkowsky, Judith L. (1985), "Measuring the Involvement Con-
ing, London: Sage Publications. struct," Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (December),
341-52.

Cognition and Affect in the Formation of Customer Satisfaction / 31

You might also like