You are on page 1of 16

Developmental Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association

2005, Vol. 41, No. 3, 464 – 478 0012-1649/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.464

Narrative Development in Bilingual Kindergarteners: Can Arthur Help?


Yuuko Uchikoshi
Harvard University

This study examined the effects of the children’s TV program Arthur on the development of narrative
skills over an academic year for Spanish-speaking English-language learners. In October, February, and
June of their kindergarten year, children were asked to tell a story, in English, prompted by 3 pictures.
Before the 2nd and 3rd assessments, half of the 108 children were randomly assigned to view Arthur 3
times a week during school hours, and the other half, which formed the control group, viewed the
children’s program Between the Lions on the same schedule. Individual growth modeling analysis
showed that children who viewed Arthur had steeper growth trajectories than those who viewed Between
the Lions. Boys displayed better English narrative skills than girls but no difference in narrative growth
rate. The results suggest that certain educational TV programs can assist in some aspects of the language
development of bilingual children.

Keywords: narrative development, educational television, English language learners, kindergarten,


growth modeling

The number of children who have limited proficiency in English ability to mark the significance of events and their ability to
in U.S. schools has risen dramatically over the past 2 decades. represent informational context in expository discourse predicted
Moreover, 53% of these children are concentrated in kindergarten 8-year-olds’ reading comprehension skills. Dickinson and Tabors
through Grade 4; nationally, at least 1 in 12 kindergarteners comes (2001) found kindergarten narrative production scores to be cor-
from a home in which English is not the primary language (August related with fourth- and seventh-grade receptive vocabulary as
& Hakuta, 1997). In addition, 77% of the English-language learner well as reading comprehension. In addition, Mason, Stewart, Pe-
(ELL) children in the United States have Spanish as their first terman, and Dunning’s (1992) study showed that at-risk children
language (Office of English Language Acquisition, Language En- without narrative problems do much better on later reading
hancement, and Academic Achievement for Limited English Pro- achievement than do children with narrative delays.
ficient Students, n.d.). Despite a dramatic increase in the number Given that narratives have been identified as the critical link to
of young children learning English as a second language, there has later school success (Paul & Smith, 1993), narratives are often
been little systematic research on the development of their English demonstrated in classroom situations such as show-and-tell, even
narrative skills. by kindergarteners and first graders. Yet, although children from
Narrative skills have been pointed out as being strong predictors mainstream backgrounds enter school familiar with the type of
of later language and literacy achievement for monolingual En- narrative structure that is valued in schools (Heath, 1982), children
glish children (e.g., Dickinson & Tabors, 2001; Griffin, Hemphill, from other backgrounds, such as ELL children, do not. Research
Camp, & Wolf, 2004; Paul & Smith, 1993). Griffin et al. (2004) shows that narrative skills are associated with cultural background
showed that children’s oral narrative abilities at age 5 predicted and ethnic-group membership (Dart, 1992; Heath, 1982, 1986;
reading comprehension skills at age 8. In particular, 5-year-olds’ Minami & McCabe, 1991; Ninio, 1980; Shiro, 1995; Silva &
McCabe, 1996; Tannen, 1980; Wang & Leichtman, 2000). For
example, Latino children tend not to follow a linear model when
Yuuko Uchikoshi, Graduate School of Education, Harvard University. telling narratives (Rodino, Gimbert, Perez, Craddock-Willis, &
This research was supported by dissertation grants from the Interna-
McCabe, 1991, as cited in Silva & McCabe, 1996). They tend to
tional Reading Association and the Harvard University Graduate School of
Education.
deemphasize event sequencing yet emphasize description and
I am thankful to Catherine Snow, Terry Tivnan, Maria Carlo, and Lowry evaluation (Silva & McCabe, 1996). Latino children also tend to
Hemphill for their valuable comments on previous versions of this article. produce narratives focused on personal and family relationships
I also thank Carlo Santos, Frida Gomez, Kim Keith, Elisa Jazo, Kaytie rather than on events as monolingual English-speaking children do
Dowcett, Jill Jacobs, Jody Clarke, Jude Higdon, Patti Sullivan-Hall, Sara (Silva & McCabe, 1996).
Roberts, Robyn Viloria, and Elizabeth Willmott for their assistance with Thus, Spanish-speaking ELL children are at a disadvantage
data collection and Elisabeth Duursma for her assistance with coding from school entry as a result not only of their weak English skills
reliability. In addition, I am grateful to television station WGBH as well as
but also of their different narrative styles. Consequently, they are
the principals, teachers, staff, and children of the schools for their support
projected to have later academic problems. In order to prevent this,
and participation in this study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Yuuko interventions that can be conducted prior to school entry, particu-
Uchikoshi, who is now at the School of Education, University of Califor- larly for the growing Spanish-speaking ELL population, need to be
nia, Davis, 2059 Academic Surge, Davis, CA 95616. E-mail: yuchikoshi@ created and examined. More research is crucial to give these ELL
ucdavis.edu children an equal start in first grade.
464
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 465

Labov (1972) defined a narrative as at least two sequential In addition, when children pretend to read storybooks out loud,
independent clauses describing a single past event. Narratives are they use a discourse different from the one they use when they are
a form of decontextualized extended discourse. The ability to telling a personal narrative. That is, when they pretend to read,
produce a narrative demonstrates a child’s ability to sustain talk children use the language of storybooks (Purcell-Gates, 1988,
about the world beyond the here and now (Snow, Tabors, Nichol- 2001). They tend to use a lexicon that is more literary and varied
son, & Kurland, 1995). Furthermore, narratives are not streams of as well as to use the syntax found in written texts. For example,
unrelated words or sentences. They require the use of several narrators may include direct or indirect quotes as well as conjoined
utterances or turns that must be linked to build a coherent linguistic phrases.
structure (Snow et al., 1995). However, not all of these traits appear in the narratives of
Story structure (Chang, 2004; Labov, 1972; Peterson & Mc- children before the age of 6 (Peterson & McCabe, 1983). Narra-
Cabe, 1983), evaluation (Chang, 2004; Labov, 1972; Peterson & tives of 4- and 5-year-old monolingual children contain little
McCabe, 1983), temporality and reference (Chang, 2004; Labov, evaluation of story events (Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991; Eaton,
1972; Peterson & McCabe, 1983), and storybook language Collis, & Lewis, 1999; Peterson & McCabe, 1983). The most
(Purcell-Gates, 1988, 2001) are the key dimensions of narrative common narrative structures of 4-year-old monolingual English-
skills. Much of the past literature in this area has focused on the speaking children have been described as either leap-frog narra-
development of these dimensions in the narratives of English tives, in which children jump from one event to another and leave
monolingual children. out major events, or chronology narratives, in which they provide
The components of a well-formed story structure include an only a simple recounting of successive events (Peterson & Mc-
abstract (a summary of the narrative or a title), an introduction (a Cabe, 1983, pp. 43– 45). At age 5, the most common narrative
conventional opening to the narrative), an orientation (a descrip- patterns are the chronology narrative and the ending-at-the-high-
tion of the characters, setting, time, and activity to set the stage for point narrative, in which there is no resolution (Peterson & Mc-
the narrative), events (actions that advance the storyline, including Cabe, 1983, pp. 41– 45). At age 6, the classic narrative pattern
the problem), a resolution (a termination of complicating events), finally becomes dominant (Peterson & McCabe, 1983, pp. 36 – 41).
and a coda (an ending of the narrative; Chang, 2004; Labov, 1972; The classic narrative pattern has been defined as one in which the
Peterson & McCabe, 1983; Snow et al., 1995; Willenberg & Kang,
narrative is built around a high point. “The narrator builds up to a
2001).
high point, evaluatively dwells on it, and then resolves it” (Peter-
It is also important to focus on the evaluation component of
son & McCabe, 1983, p. 36). Yet the narrative patterns of ELL
narratives. When telling a story, the narrator not only tells the
children—in particular, those of Spanish-speaking ELL children—
listener what happens in the story but also talks about the meaning
and how their English narratives develop are still unexplored. Most
of those events. In other words, the narrator includes evaluation
studies of Spanish–English bilinguals’ narrative performance, such
devices to signal the point of the story from the narrator’s per-
as the one by Gutiérrez-Clellen (2002), have focused on cross-
spective (Chang, 2004; Labov, 1972; Peterson & McCabe, 1983).
language comparison, not on charting the developmental process
Evaluative devices include intensifiers (e.g., “all the other bears”),
involved.
adjectives (e.g., “the little bears”), negatives or defeats of expec-
Various environmental factors, such as parental responsiveness,
tations (e.g., “The bear could not reach the kite”), references to
emotional states or cognitions (e.g., “The bear was scared”), affect the development of children’s narrative skills (Peterson,
references to physical states (e.g., “The bear was hurt”), intentions Jesso, & McCabe, 1999). The types and number of parent– child
(e.g., “The bear wanted to get the kite”), causal markers (e.g., “The conversational exchanges during book reading shape children’s
bear fell because he slipped out of the tree”), and words with high narrative development (McCabe & Peterson, 1991; Peterson &
evaluative content (e.g., “The bear came crashing to the ground”; McCabe, 1992; E. Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1993). Some parents
Beck, Coker, Hemphill, & Bellinger, 2003; Peterson & McCabe, make detailed references to past events and ask multiple questions,
1983; Willenberg & Kang, 2001). Peterson and McCabe (1983) whereas others ask simple questions and frequently switch topics.
found 21 types of evaluation devices in the narratives of 4- to These differences in parental style have been shown to influence
9-year-old American children, with the older children using a both the quantity and quality of narratives produced by preschool
larger variety of evaluations than the younger children. children.
Temporality and reference are the two main elements in the Frequency of book reading is also associated with narrative
organization of a narrative. In order for narratives to be coherent development (Purcell-Gates, 2001). Books display narrative struc-
and follow a time line, linguistic temporal devices such as con- tures particularly clearly, and furthermore, some interventions use
nectives are used. Peterson and McCabe (1991) found that even book reading as a carrier for their interactions. Well-read-to mono-
31⁄2-year-old children used connectives such as and in their narra- lingual kindergarten children can learn linguistic registers that are
tives. In addition, how the narrator first introduces the story specific to social contexts and use more formal and booklike
characters in the narrative influences the organization of the story narrating language when pretending to read a storybook (Adams,
(Beck et al., 2003). Ideally, the character should be introduced in 1990; Heath, 1982; Ninio, 1980; Purcell-Gates, 1988, 1991, 1992,
a way that does not assume any prior knowledge on the part of the 1996, 2001; Purcell-Gates & Dahl, 1991; Scarborough & Dobrich,
listener. For example, a story that begins with an unspecified 1994; Scarborough, Dobrich, & Hager, 2001; Sulzby, 1985).
pronoun such as he would have an unclear beginning. A narrative Preschool-age children whose parents spend more time in narrative
that starts with a nonpresupposing introduction using an indefinite conversation, in asking more open-ended and context-eliciting
article and a noun, such as a bear, would be more coherent (Beck questions, and in encouraging longer narratives through book-
et al., 2003; Willenberg & Kang, 2001). reading sessions tend to produce more context-setting descriptions
466 UCHIKOSHI

about when and where the events in a narrative took place (Peter- In this study, my aim was to investigate whether the book-based
son et al., 1999). educational TV program Arthur would influence narrative devel-
However, research shows that Latino preschool children are less opment among ELL children from Spanish-speaking homes. I
likely to have frequent book-reading experiences than are White, attempted to answer the following question: Does viewing Arthur
non-Latino children (Nord, Lennon, Liu, & Chandler, 1999). have a beneficial effect on narrative skills so that ELL children can
Given the evidence from past research with monolingual children be better prepared for first grade? To control for effects of watch-
demonstrating that exposure to narratives through book reading ing educational TV, I used Between the Lions, another book-based
during the preschool years is positively associated with narrative educational TV show, as a control.2
skills in kindergarten and subsequent literacy outcomes, the impact Between the Lions is also a 30-min book-based educational
of mechanisms for supplementing Spanish-speaking ELL chil- program that is broadcast on PBS stations across the country and
dren’s access to experiences with narrative needs to be evaluated. targeted to kindergarten-age children. Between the Lions intro-
Educational TV programs, particularly shows based on books, duces a book in each episode, but instead of focusing explicitly on
could provide children with the desired extra exposure to language the narrative, the show puts more weight on text structure, indi-
and literacy environments as well as to English narrative conven- vidual words, and other print features (Linebarger, 2000). Phono-
tions.1 Past research suggests that educational TV programs, such logical awareness and reading fluency are heavily emphasized.
as Sesame Street and Between the Lions, can be a source of Each Between the Lions episode follows a “whole–part–whole”
language learning for monolingual English-speaking children (e.g., framework, adopted as the approach to literacy instruction. The
Linebarger, 2000; Rice, Huston, Truglio, & Wright, 1990; Rice & story line of each Between the Lions episode begins with a read-
Woodsmall, 1988; Van Evra, 1998). Children have acquired new aloud experience as the “whole,” in which portions of the text are
vocabulary and developed phonological awareness skills by view- displayed on the screen and words are highlighted as they are read
ing these shows. In addition, child language research shows that (Rath, 2000). Then the “parts” are emphasized to point the view-
the number of conversations and the variety of words that children ers’ attention to such topics as phonological awareness, letter–
hear affect the speed of their language and literacy growth (Tabors, sound correspondence, word meanings, punctuation, and other
Beals, & Weizman, 2001). Studies show that, on average, children conventions of written English (Rath, 2000). Most of the show is
between the ages of 3 and 5 years watch 19 –20 hr of TV per week centered on this “parts” part. At the end, the “whole” text is
revisited and the “parts” are reviewed (Rath, 2000).
(Huston, Wright, Rice, Kerkman, & St. Peters, 1990); it is impor-
Hence, Arthur focuses more on the narrative structure, whereas
tant to know whether, if these hours were concentrated on educa-
Between the Lions focuses more on phonics and story mechanics.
tional TV programs, it would assist in the English-language and
By showing a program that is educational but not entirely focused
literacy development of ELL children.
on narratives, such as Between the Lions, to the control group, I
Children from nonmainstream backgrounds might become more
could control for any effects that could be attributed to TV
accustomed to mainstream English narrative styles that are prev-
viewing.
alent in school settings by viewing educational TV programs that
Thus, in this study I examined the effect of viewing Arthur
are based on books and follow the classic narrative pattern. Like
versus Between the Lions on ELL children’s English narrative
book reading, TV viewing can be a routinized activity. Many
growth within the context of a standard kindergarten program. In
shows follow a routine format. Moreover, children can view reruns
addition, I investigated the patterns of growth in oral English
or videotaped versions of the same episode. In addition, research-
narrative skills in young ELL kindergarteners. Such research is
ers have found that with monolingual English-speaking children, needed to fully understand the narrative skills development of
parent– child interactions around TV programs can replicate as- Spanish-speaking ELL children and to prepare them for school
pects of interactions around books (Lemish & Rice, 1986). entry.
Yet there have been no studies investigating the impact of Moreover, in this study I used individual growth modeling
book-based educational programs on either monolingual or ELL techniques (Singer & Willett, 2003; Willett, 1994) to analyze the
children’s narrative development. A representative of a book- children’s narrative development. As individual growth modeling
based show is Arthur. Arthur is a 30-min educational program makes use of repeated waves of data and conceptualizes change as
appearing on Public Broadcasting Service (PBS) stations across a continuous process of development (Willett, 1994), it yields a
the country and targeted to audiences of preschool and kindergar- more accurate picture of change over time than do traditional
ten children. Arthur tells a story to the viewers in each episode and techniques, such as ordinary regression methods.
is popular with kindergarten-age children.
In Arthur, each episode presents two stories, each with a plot, a
1
conflict, and a resolution. As each story is based on a storybook, Educational TV refers to shows that have a core educational or infor-
each story follows the components of a well-formed story structure mational purpose. In 1990, the Federal Communications Commission
as defined by Labov (1972) and Peterson and McCabe (1983). The passed the Children’s TV Act, which required commercial broadcasters to
air programming that has a core educational and informational purpose
Arthur TV series exposes children to various stories with moral
targeted to children under age 16 (Hill-Scott, 2001). Such programs must
points of interest to them. The show is about growing up. The be regularly scheduled, weekly programs of at least 30 min and be aired
characters in Arthur learn to make thoughtful decisions and resolve between 7:00 a.m. and 10:00 p.m.
problems in each episode. The problems Arthur and his friends 2
Between the Lions focuses on phonics and story mechanics. How
face are similar to the ones the viewers may face at home and at Between the Lions viewing affects the development of ELL children’s
school. In addition, evaluation is emphasized through the charac- phonological awareness and story mechanics, with Arthur viewing as a
ters’ speech and actions. control, is not discussed in this article.
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 467

Method average parental educational level being some secondary education. The
top 10% of all parents had received some higher education, whereas the
Participants bottom 10% had not completed primary education. The remaining 80% fell
somewhere in between, with the majority completing primary education.
A total of 108 children (47 girls and 61 boys) attending six public The English vocabulary levels of the target children were measured with
schools in a large urban district located on the East Coast participated in the the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Third Edition (PPVT–III; Dunn &
study. The average age of these children in October was 5 years 7 months. Dunn, 1997) in October before the intervention started. The results of the
The average age for the boys was 5 years 7 months; for the girls it was 5 test indicated that, on average, in October these native Spanish speakers
years 6 months. scored at the level of a monolingual English child 3 years 2 months of age
Spanish–English bilingual kindergarten classrooms were selected from (Dunn & Dunn, 1997).
these schools. In all classrooms, instruction occurred in both English and The Spanish vocabulary levels of the target children were also measured
Spanish. All kindergarten teachers were fluent in both English and Spanish. in October with the Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP;
All children were from primarily Spanish-speaking homes and lived in Dunn, Padilla, Lugo, & Dunn, 1986). The results of this test revealed that,
neighborhoods heavily populated by Spanish-speaking people. Participants on average, these native Spanish-speaking entering kindergarteners
were recruited through these kindergarten classrooms. School district de- achieved scores that would be expected from Spanish monolingual children
mographics and school data indicate that 80% or more of the participating residing in Mexico and ranging in age from 4 years 8 months to 5 years 0
students qualified for free lunch. months, according to the age norms of the test (Dunn et al., 1986). As the
To gain background information about the students, I sent home with average age of the children in this study was 5 years 7 months, these ELL
them a parental questionnaire in both English and Spanish (see Table 1 for children had vocabulary levels that were slightly lower than those of their
a summary of these data). The number of older siblings of the target child Spanish monolingual counterparts. (See Table 1 for a summary.)
ranged from 0 to 5, with the average child having 1.2 older siblings. Years Raw scores are reported for initial vocabulary levels in both Spanish and
living in the United States ranged from 3 months to 7 years, with the English for later use in growth modeling analysis. It should be noted that
average being 4.9 years. The majority of those whose parents responded there are fewer items on the TVIP than on the PPVT–III, which may
had been born in the United States; only 22% of these children had been explain the lower Spanish raw scores.
born outside of the United States. Although variation in the number of
children’s books in the home was large—from 0 to 300 books (SD ⫽ 35
books)—parents responded that on average there were 21, including both Design
English and Spanish books. The mode was 10 books. Concerning prekin-
dergarten experience, 56 of 108 parents responded that their target child On the basis of a stratified random sampling, half of the students in six
had gone to either prekindergarten or a Head Start program. A third (34 of classrooms (51 children) were assigned to watch Arthur during school
108 parents) said they took their target child to libraries on a frequent basis. hours, and the other half in the same six classrooms (57 children) were
In terms of home viewing, 73.4% of the children viewed Arthur at home, assigned to watch Between the Lions during school hours. In each class-
whereas 41.9% watched Between the Lions at home. room, the children were first grouped according to gender, and then they
Information about the mothers was also collected. The educational levels were rank ordered on the basis of their October English PPVT–III vocab-
of the mothers ranged from no education to professional degrees, the ulary scores. I then randomized assignment to the two viewing conditions,

Table 1
Background Information and Vocabulary Scores for All Children (N ⫽ 108) and for Children by Viewing Group and Gender

Total Arthur Between the Lions Boys Girls

Variable M SD n Range M SD n M SD n M SD n M SD n

Parental educationa 3.69 1.69 93 0–8 3.76 1.76 45 3.63 1.63 48 3.49 1.79 53 3.95 1.52 40
No. of older siblings 1.16 1.16 92 0–5 1.09 1.27 44 1.23 1.06 48 1.19 1.12 53 1.13 1.22 40
Years lived in the U.S. 4.89 1.80 99 0.3–7 4.87 1.85 48 4.90 1.78 51 5.02 1.69 57 4.71 1.96 42
No. of Spanish books at home 12.40 27.32 91 0–250 7.39 6.79 46 17.51 37.77 45 8.73 9.14 52 17.28 40.16 39
No. of English books at home 9.01 11.22 92 0–55 5.60 5.44 47 12.57 14.29 45 8.06 10.16 53 10.31 12.54 39
No. of total books at home 21.47 35.07 92 0–300 13.11 9.55 47 30.02 47.70 45 16.94 17.72 53 27.51 49.26 39

No. of yes n No. of yes n No. of yes n No. of yes n No. of yes n

Preschool experience (yes/no) 56 89 26 44 30 45 28 49 28 40


Library experience (yes/no) 34 92 13 47 21 45 21 52 13 40
Arthur home viewing (yes/no) 69 94 35 46 34 48 39 54 30 40
Between the Lions home viewing
(yes/no) 36 86 17 50 19 42 21 50 15 36

M SD n Range M SD n M SD n M SD n M SD n
Receptive vocabulary score
Initial English vocabulary 39.69 20.38 108 0–85 41.37 20.74 51 38.19 20.13 57 44.30 18.37 61 33.72 21.49 47
Initial Spanish vocabulary 32.05 13.29 108 2–69 32.65 12.49 51 31.51 14.06 57 31.28 13.24 61 33.04 13.44 47

Note. Background information was obtained from parental questionnaires, and vocabulary scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition
and the Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody.
a
Parental education was on a scale from 0 to 8.
468 UCHIKOSHI

matching the children on their vocabulary scores, which yielded viewing (Beck et al., 2003; Snow et al., 1995; Willenberg & Kang, 2001): abstract,
groups with similar composition in gender and initial English vocabulary introduction, orientation, character delineation, problem, resolution, and
skills. Receptive vocabulary scores were chosen as a basis for stratification coda. If the narrative contained all seven elements, the child was given a
because children’s understanding of the shows would be most influenced total of 7 points. The definitions of the elements, as well as examples from
by their English vocabulary. the actual data, are listed in the Appendix.
Both groups watched one 30-min episode three times a week in a Events coding. Narrative events relate the actions of the story charac-
classroom at school from October to the beginning of May, for a total of ters and move the plot forward. Five categories of events from Willenberg
54 episodes. A frequency of three episodes a week was chosen because of and Kang (2001) were included in this section: bears playing, bears flying
the importance of repetition in interventions (Galdwell, 2000), children’s kite, bear climbed tree or bear attempts to get kite, bear fell or jumped, bear
liking for repetition and familiar events (Galdwell, 2000), and the feasi- hurt or dead.
bility and practicality of children viewing educational TV during school Evaluation coding. Evaluative devices in narrative are used to signal
hours. Owing to time constraints and to keep the intervention consistent the point of the story from the narrator’s perspective. Eight types of
among classrooms, teachers and researchers only showed the videos and evaluation from Willenberg and Kang (2001) were included: intensifiers,
did not do follow-up activities based on the episodes with the children. adjectives, negatives or defeats of expectations, references to emotional
All kindergarteners were assessed with the narrative tests described in states or cognitions, references to physical states, intentions, causal mark-
the following section at three time points throughout the school year: ers, and words with high evaluative content. See the Appendix for
October (before watching any episodes in the classrooms), February (after examples.
watching 27 episodes in the classrooms), and late May or early June (after Temporality and reference. Temporality and reference (e.g., use of
watching an additional 27 episodes in the classrooms).3 The total testing connectives, first mention of story characters) were included from Willen-
time for each individual session was 30 – 45 min. Children were also berg and Kang (2001) as presented in the Appendix.
assessed on English as well as Spanish receptive vocabulary knowledge. Storybook language. The elements of storybook language examined
came from previous studies concerning pretend book reading (Beck et al.,
2003; Purcell-Gates, 2001; Snow et al., 1995). As most instances of
Measures storybook language contain syntactic constructions that are typically found
To measure narrative skills, I asked children to tell a “Bear Story” in only in older children (Labov, 1968, cited in Peterson & McCabe, 1983),
English, prompted by three slides of pictures that depicted a family of only three elements of storybook language from Willenberg and Kang
teddy bears in an adventure involving a flyaway kite and a baby bear (2001) were examined in this study: direct or indirect quotes, -ly adverbs,
falling from a tree (from the SHELL [School-Home Early Language and and conjoined noun/verb or adverbial phrases (see the Appendix for
Literacy] test battery in Snow et al., 1995). The child was allowed to look examples).
at the slides as long as needed but was asked to put them away before The points for each of these five dimensions of the child’s narrative were
telling the story. However, when the child appeared hesitant to give a added up to form a combined narrative measure score. For reliability of
narrative, the child was allowed to see the “Bear Story” pictures as he or coding, 20% of the narratives were randomly selected and independently
she related the story. scored by a trained researcher. The Cohen’s kappa statistic, which corrects
Trained assessors transcribed the narratives dictated by the children on for chance agreement (Bakeman & Gottman, 1997), was .82.
the spot. The children’s narratives were written down so that the assessor
could read the story back to the child and confirm that the story had been
correctly recorded. If the child began to describe the picture instead of
Statistical Analysis
providing a narrative, the assessor prompted the child with questions such
First, a descriptive analysis was conducted on the total number of words,
as “What is happening in the story?” or “What happened next?” If the child
the mean clause length, and the combined narrative measure. Then, to
appeared to be taking no definite course in the narrative or if the child
examine the difference in the level and rate of change on the combined
ended the narrative abruptly, the assessors were instructed to ask, “Is that
narrative measure among individuals, I made use of individual growth
the end of the story?” or “How does the story end?”
modeling.

Total Number of Words and Mean Clause Length


Results
Because past research showed an increase in the length of stories as well
as the syntactic complexity of utterances as children got older (Eaton et al., Descriptive Results
1999; Karmiloff-Smith, 1985; Purcell-Gates, 2001), I calculated the total
number of words and the mean clause length in each story. After separating In Table 2, the means and standard deviations for the total
each story into clauses, I calculated the mean clause length by dividing the number of words, the mean clause length, and the combined
number of clauses by the total number of words.
narrative measure are presented for all children, and by viewing
group and gender. The average for each of these three measures
Combined Narrative Measure increased as the children approached the end of their kindergarten
year. Individual variation was high for all three variables. The
Each story was then coded along the following five dimensions: story
structure, number of main events, evaluation, temporality and reference,
average total number of words increased by 43% from slightly
and storybook language. This coding scheme was adapted from Willenberg over 20 words to 37 words, a growth of two thirds of a standard
and Kang (2001), whose coding checklists were based on previous Bear deviation. The average mean clause length went from 4 words per
Story checklists from Snow et al. (1995) and Beck et al. (2003). Each clause to 5 words per clause, an increase of nearly one half of a
category was coded for the presence or absence of each feature listed standard deviation. The average combined narrative measure score
regardless of how frequently it occurred in the narrative.
Story structure coding. Seven major types of narrative elements were
3
categorized on the basis of an adaptation of high-point structure (Peterson Children were tested on a variety of literacy measures, but only
& McCabe, 1983) and previous coding checklists used for the “Bear Story” narrative production is discussed here.
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 469

Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for Narrative Variables for All Children (N ⫽ 108) and for Children by Viewing Group and Gender

Total Arthur Between the Lions Boys Girls

Narrative measure M SD n Range M SD n M SD n M SD n M SD n

Total no. of words


October 21.91 21.80 99 0–96 21.92 19.10 49 21.90 22.54 50 25.85 21.74 55 16.98 18.64 44
February 26.18 20.15 108 0–108 28.29 20.42 51 24.28 19.89 57 29.13 19.63 61 22.34 20.36 47
May/June 37.33 32.54 102 0–253 42.66 39.44 47 32.78 24.69 55 37.79 23.47 58 36.73 41.92 44
Mean clause length
October 4.41 2.54 99 0–11 4.72 2.55 49 4.11 2.52 50 4.63 2.05 55 4.04 3.10 44
February 4.70 2.28 108 0–14 5.35 2.18 51 4.12 2.23 57 5.00 1.72 61 4.31 2.82 47
May/June 5.05 1.90 102 0–8.43 5.26 1.67 47 4.87 2.08 55 5.51 1.35 58 4.44 2.33 44
Combined narrative measure
October 4.90 3.76 99 0–15 5.00 3.50 49 4.82 4.02 50 6.12 3.86 55 3.52 3.15 44
February 6.32 3.77 108 0–15 7.04 3.56 51 5.68 3.87 57 7.21 3.64 61 5.17 3.66 47
May/June 8.07 4.14 102 0–20 9.13 4.35 47 7.16 3.75 55 8.93 3.58 58 6.93 4.57 44

also increased from 5 points to 8 points, roughly equivalent to Singer & Willett, 2003). Second, it allows for the spacing of waves
three quarters of a standard deviation. of data to vary across individuals (Littell et al., 1996; Singer &
These three measures (total number of words, mean clause Willett, 2003). In this data set, narrative measurements were taken
length, and the combined narrative measure) were moderately to at slightly different times. For some children, the time between
highly correlated with each other, with correlations of about .60 at assessments was 3 months, whereas for others it was closer to 4
each of the three time points (see Table 3). Children who produced months. Third, individual growth modeling can analyze data sets
elaborate narratives tended to incorporate a lot of detail into their with varying numbers of waves of data (Littell et al., 1996; Singer
narrative clauses. The total number of words as well as the number & Willett, 2003). That is, unlike other approaches, individual
of other features increased in the children’s narratives. growth modeling includes all participants in the estimation regard-
Initial English vocabulary scores tended to be moderately cor- less of missing data. The majority of children in this study had a
related with the narrative measures at all three time points, with the narrative score at all three time points, yet some had a narrative
median correlation of English vocabulary with the narrative mea- score at only two time points.
sures being just over .50. Conversely, initial Spanish vocabulary To arrive at a final model that best predicted English narrative
scores were not correlated with English narrative scores on the
development, I built a taxonomy of theoretically motivated indi-
“Bear Story.”
vidual growth models. Time was denoted in number of months
rather than assessment occasions, because assessments were car-
Individual Growth Modeling: Effect of Arthur? ried out with some variation in exact timing among individuals. As
To examine differences in the level and rate of change among most participants had three data points each, a linear model was
individuals, I used individual growth modeling to analyze the used (Singer & Willett, 2003; Willett, Singer, & Martin, 1998).
combined narrative measure. Individual growth modeling was the In the first stage, I fit an unconditional means model that
appropriate analysis tool for this data set for several reasons. First, included no predictors. This model describes variation in the
it is designed for exploring longitudinal data on individuals over outcomes (Singer & Willett, 2003). I then fit an unconditional
time (Littell, Milliken, Stroup, & Wolfinger, 1996; Singer, 1998; growth model, in which I examined within-person change by

Table 3
Correlation Matrix for Vocabulary and Narrative Measures (N ⫽ 108)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Spanish initial vocabulary — .07 .09 ⫺.09 ⫺.26** .06 ⫺.02 ⫺.25* .03 ⫺.02 ⫺.16
2. English initial vocabulary — .53** .33** .24* .51** .45** .44** .67** .57** .54**
3. Total no. of words in October — .39** .13 .61** .32** .31** .77** .57** .38**
4. Total no. of words in February — .31** .36** .51** .35** .34** .77** .48**
5. Total no. of words in May/June — .02 .18† .54** .07 .32** .63**
6. Mean clause length in October — .54** .30** .68** .53** .30**
7. Mean clause length in February — .50** .42** .58** .45**
8. Mean clause length in May/June — .37** .55** .60**
9. Combined narrative measure in October — .61** .47**
10. Combined narrative measure in February — .69**
11. Combined narrative measure in May/June —

† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.


470 UCHIKOSHI

fitting growth trajectories for each child over time. The growth Comparing the variance components in Model 2 (unconditional
trajectories of each individual child varied. Some children showed growth model) with those of Model 1 shows that 48.6% (from 8.38
steady growth, whereas others showed no growth. In light of the to 4.31) of the within-person variation in the combined narrative
variation across children, it is important to understand the general measure was systematically associated with linear time. Further-
growth patterns in ELL children’s ability to narrate stories. Hence, more, as there was nonzero variability in both true initial status
I looked at between-person variation and added predictors to ( p ⬍ .01) and true rate of change ( p ⬍ .01), Model 2 suggested
investigate whether they affected individual changes in the com- adding more predictors into the model to explain heterogeneity in
bined narrative measure. I first added the predictor show to inves- each parameter. Preschool experience, number of older siblings,
tigate whether individual changes in the combined narrative mea- total number of children’s books in the home, and library experi-
sure (CNM) were related to viewing of Arthur. ence were not significant when included as predictors, and they
Combining the within-person and between-person models were not included in subsequent models. Model 9 was chosen as
yielded the following model: the final model and is interpreted in the following sections.
Effect of show. As Model 9 in Table 4 shows, the estimated
CNM ti ⫽ 关 ␤ 00 ⫹ ␤ 01 SHOW i ⫹ ␤ 10 TIME ti coefficient for show was not statistically significant, indicating that
⫹ ␤ 11 SHOW i TIME ti兴 ⫹ 关u0i ⫹ u1i TIME ti ⫹ rti兴. the two groups did not significantly differ from each other at the
start of kindergarten, after I controlled for the other variables in the
The parameters in the above model represent the effect of show on model. However, the estimated coefficient for the Show ⫻ Time
the initial level of the CNM (␤01) and the effect of show on the interaction was positive and significant after the other variables in
rate of change in the CNM (␤11). Because I was comparing models the model were controlled. This finding indicates that viewing
that differ in their fixed effects but not their variance components, Arthur during class hours improved the narrative outcome at a
I used full maximum-likelihood estimation (see Singer, 1998). faster pace than did viewing Between the Lions, after I controlled
As a general modeling strategy, I first evaluated the above for initial show differences, classroom differences, gender, home
full-model equation for significance. SHOW was kept in the model viewings, initial English vocabulary, and parental education, as
even if it was not significant, as it was a key predictor. The CLASS shown in Figure 1. The standard deviation for the combined
variable was kept in the model to control for classroom difference. narrative measure pooled across all occasions was 4.08 points.
Indicators of home viewing—that is, watching Arthur at home and Thus, the coefficient of .26 for the interaction of show (Arthur) and
watching Between the Lions at home—were also kept in the model time corresponds to an effect size of slightly over one twentieth of
to control for home viewing. Past research has used mother’s a standard deviation per month, or an effect of slightly over one
education to control for socioeconomic status (SES). Although half of a standard deviation for the entire school year.
parental education was not significant, it was included in the final Classroom differences. The estimated coefficients for most
model to control for SES. Subsequent analyses investigated classrooms tended to be around 0, indicating that most classrooms
whether other variables such as gender, prekindergarten experi- had similar estimated average initial levels of the combined nar-
ence, home educational TV viewing, number of older siblings, rative measure. Moreover, differences in growth in the combined
number of years the child had been in the United States, the child’s narrative measure among classrooms were also not significant.
initial vocabulary levels in Spanish and English, the number of Home viewing. Home viewing was kept in the model to con-
children’s books in the home, and library exposure4 were signif- trol for extra viewings of Arthur or Between the Lions. Model 9
icant variables. indicates that home viewing of neither show was significant.
Gender. After I controlled for show, class, home viewing,
Individual Growth Modeling Results initial English vocabulary, and parental education, gender had a
significant effect on the estimated average initial level of the
After fitting a baseline unconditional means model (Model 1) combined narrative measure. However, the interaction between
and baseline unconditional growth model (Model 2) for the com- gender and time was not significant. Boys began with a 1.40-point
bined narrative measure, I built a taxonomy of theoretically mo- (effect size of roughly one third of a standard deviation) advantage
tivated individual growth models, as shown in Table 4. The pre- on the combined narrative measure at the start of kindergarten, and
dictors that were not statistically significant were kept out of the this difference remained throughout the school year, as shown in
model. Control variables (show, Arthur home viewing, Between Figure 1.
the Lions home viewing, class, parental education) were kept in the Initial English vocabulary. After I controlled for the other
model even if they were not significant. variables in the model, initial English vocabulary had a significant
The variance components in Model 1 (unconditional means effect on the estimated average initial level of the combined
model) indicate that the average child’s English combined narra- narrative measure. Every 20-point (one standard deviation) in-
tive measure varied over time and that the children differed from crease in initial English vocabulary was associated with a 2.2-point
each other. Using the results of this model, I calculated the intra- (effect size of slightly over one half of a standard deviation)
class correlation coefficient to be .50. The intraclass correlation increase in the combined narrative measure. That is, children who
coefficient is the intercept divided by the sum of the intercept and started kindergarten with higher initial English vocabulary scores
the residual (8.53/[8.53 ⫹ 8.38] in this case), and it “describes the also started with higher initial combined narrative measures. How-
proportion of the total outcome variation that lies between people”
(Singer & Willett, 2003, p. 96). Thus, this correlation indicated
that half of the total variation in the combined narrative measure 4
Library exposure indicates whether or not the child was taken to the
was attributable to differences among children. library on a frequent basis.
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 471

Table 4
Estimates of Fixed and Random Effects From a Series of Fitted Individual Growth Models in Which Show, Class, Arthur Home
Viewing, Between the Lions (BTL) Home Viewing, Gender, Initial English Vocabulary Scores, and Library Experience Predict the
Average Combined Narrative Measure at the Start of Kindergarten and Rate of Change in the Combined Narrative Measure During
the Kindergarten Year for All Children (N ⫽ 108)

Model 1: Model 2: Model 4: Model 6: Model 8: Model 9:


Unconditional Unconditional Model 3: Show ⫻ Model 5: Home Model 7: English Parental
Effect means model growth model Show Time Class TV Gender vocabulary education

Fixed effects
Intercept
␤00 6.34** 4.37** ⫺0.74 4.27** 4.46** 2.87** 1.51† ⫺0.73 ⫺0.78
SE 0.33 0.38 0.87 0.52 0.78 0.90 0.90 0.77 1.02
Time (in months)
␤10 0.46** 0.32** 0.36** 0.36** 0.36** 0.36** 0.35** 0.33**
SE 0.05 0.08 0.07 0.07 0.07 0.07 0.07 0.07
Show: Arthur
␤01 ⫺0.03 0.21 0.22 0.11 0.16 ⫺0.09 ⫺0.25
SE 0.63 0.76 0.74 0.73 0.69 0.58 0.61
Class 1
␤02 ⫺1.08 ⫺0.93 ⫺0.52 ⫺0.41 ⫺0.37
SE 1.10 1.04 0.97 0.77 0.81
Class 2
␤02 ⫺0.90 ⫺0.30 ⫺0.29 0.19 0.42
SE 1.04 1.01 0.94 0.74 0.78
Class 3
␤02 1.17 0.97 0.81 ⫺0.94 ⫺0.51
SE 0.97 0.92 0.86 0.72 0.76
Class 4
␤02 ⫺1.78† ⫺1.41 ⫺1.19 ⫺0.89 ⫺1.15
SE 0.98 0.94 0.88 0.69 0.74
Class 5
␤02 0.77 1.38 1.46† 0.57 1.08
SE 0.91 0.92 0.86 0.68 0.77
Home TV: Arthur
␤03 1.62* 1.69** 0.76 0.46
SE 0.68 0.63 0.51 0.55
Home TV: BTL
␤04 1.05 0.96 0.47 0.45
SE 0.63 0.59 0.47 0.50
Gender: Boys
␤05 2.19** 1.31** 1.40**
SE 0.53 0.44 0.47
Initial English vocabulary
␤06 0.10** 0.09**
SE 0.01 0.01
Parental education
␤07 0.12
SE 0.14

Show ⫻ Time
␤11 0.22* 0.22* 0.22* 0.22* 0.23* 0.26*
SE 0.11 0.11 0.11 0.11 0.11 0.11

Random effects (variance components)


Intercept
Estimate 8.53** 10.74** 10.96** 10.79** 10.03** 9.30** 7.77** 4.07** 3.69**
SE 1.61 2.22 1.50 2.22 2.14 2.05 1.85 1.39 1.45
Slope
Estimate 0.11** 0.11* 0.11** 0.11** 0.11** 0.11** 0.11* 0.09*
SE 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.05
Residual
Estimate 8.38** 4.31** 4.42** 4.28** 4.29** 4.31** 4.29** 4.28** 4.28**
SE 0.84 0.60 0.66 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.64
Proportional reduction in
variance from Model 2:
Intercept N/A N/A 6.6% 13.4% 27.7% 62.10% 65.6%
Slope N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A 18.2%
Akaike’s information criterion 1,691.8 1,611.1 1,338.8 1,607.9 1,606.1 1,598.5 1,584.7 1,535.8 1,346.5

† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.


472 UCHIKOSHI

Figure 1. Average fitted growth trajectories, after controlling for parental education, that describe the effect of
show and gender on the change in the combined narrative measure for English-language learner children in an
average class with average initial vocabulary scores of 40 points who watch both Arthur and Between the Lions
(BTL) at home (N ⫽ 108).

ever, initial English vocabulary was not associated with rate of change. The average growth trajectories (see Figure 1) show a
growth on the narrative measures. gradual increase in narrative skills. In this section, the stories
Parental education. Parental education was included in the provided by two boys are presented in order to demonstrate the
model to control for SES. Model 9 indicates that parental educa- average growth pattern. These two boys, Miguel and Raymond,
tion was not significant. were in the Arthur group. Miguel started the school year with
minimal English abilities. Raymond was relatively fluent and
Five Narrative Measures comfortable in speaking English.
The fitted growth trajectories for each individual narrative mea- At Time 1, in October, for many children, their lack of English
sure (story structure, events, evaluations, temporality and refer- vocabulary kept them from creating long, detailed narratives.
ence, and storybook language) were further examined using the Many children with limited English, such as Miguel, tended to stay
growth modeling perspective. The estimated coefficients for the silent or to produce narratives in Spanish. These Spanish stories
interaction between show and time were positive and significant were not used in the evaluation, because the objective of this study
for story structure (␤11 ⫽ .09; p ⫽ .04) and evaluation (␤11 ⫽ .09; was to measure English narrative growth. Children with greater
p ⫽ .03), after class, home viewing, gender, initial English recep- English abilities, such as Raymond, were able to connect several
tive vocabulary, and parental education were controlled. This sentences together to make a narrative, and their narratives in-
finding indicates that viewing Arthur during class hours improved cluded more events to move the plot forward. Under Peterson and
the narrative outcome for story structure and evaluation at a faster McCabe’s (1983) categorization, many of these narratives would
pace than did viewing Between the Lions. be classified as chronological narratives, in which the child con-
The interaction between show and time was not significant nects sentences with and and then, or leap-frog narratives, in
either for events or for temporality and reference. For the story- which the child jumps from one event to the next.
book language measure, many of the children scored 0 in October
Excerpt 1: Miguel at Time 1
and stayed at 0. The low occurrence of storybook language sug-
[Said nothing]
gests that it might be difficult for kindergarten-age ELL children to
provide such language. Excerpt 2: Raymond at Time 1
In addition, gender was significant for story structure, events, Last morning the bear was getting kite out of tree. And it was stuck.
Then last morning the kite fell down. Last morning he went to take out
and temporality and reference. That is, boys started kindergarten
the kite. Then it fell. Then it broke. Now it pull pull harder.
with higher scores in these three areas than did girls, and these
differences remained constant throughout kindergarten.
At Time 2, in February, children began to include more evalu-
Figure 2 shows the average growth trajectories describing the
ative words in their stories. For example, Miguel said, “One [bear]
effect of show on the change in story structure, evaluation, events,
was tired.” He described the bears as being little. In addition, the
and temporality and reference for ELL boys who watched Arthur
words bear and kite were mentioned more in the stories.
during the intervention period. On average, the most gains were
seen in the story structure measure. Excerpt 3: Miguel at Time 2
The three little bear was walking. One was tired. And one was in the
Qualitative Results tree. That is it.
Growth trajectories for each individual child varied. Some chil- Excerpt 4: Raymond at Time 2
dren displayed steady development, whereas others exhibited little One day a bear was running a kite. And it was a sunny day. Then the
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 473

Figure 2. Average fitted growth trajectories, after controlling for parental education, that describe the effect of
show on the change in story structure, evaluation, events, and temporality and reference for English-language
learner boys who watched Arthur during the intervention period, who were in an average class with average
initial vocabulary scores of 40 points, and who watched both Arthur and Between the Lions at home (N ⫽ 108).

kite was stuck in the tree. Then the bear was climbing to the tree. Then viewed Between the Lions during class hours. The study was
he was trying to get it. Then the kite ripped. designed so that half of the children in each classroom watched
one of the two shows. Because of time constraints, it was not
At Time 3, in May or June, many of the advanced ELL children
possible for the teacher or the researchers to follow up with
provided more events and advanced story structures. Some ad-
exercises to reinforce learning from the educational TV shows. Yet
vanced ELL children used direct speech, painting a more vivid
even though no reinforcement followed the viewing sessions,
verbal picture of their narratives. Some children started their
growth in narrative skill was greater for children who watched
narratives with titles, such as Miguel and his title “The Book.” He
Arthur, after any initial show differences, classroom differences,
then continued by beginning his story with a traditional beginning
gender, home viewings, initial English vocabulary, and parental
such as “Once upon a time” and ended his story with a formal
education were controlled for, as shown in Figure 1.
ending such as “The End.” According to Peterson and McCabe’s
Each half-hour Arthur episode presented two stories, each with
(1983) categorization, some of the stories would be classified as
ending-at-the-high-point narratives, in which the child builds up to a plot, a conflict, and a resolution. Hence, children who viewed
the high point and then abruptly ends the story. For example, Arthur had a lot of exposure to mainstream storytelling techniques.
Miguel ended his story at a climactic moment with “he fall off.” This effect was also demonstrated in the separate growth modeling
Some children even produced stories of the classic pattern. It is analysis for story structure, in which the interaction between show
interesting to note that in May, Raymond gave the same story as he (Arthur) and time was significant.
did in February yet he added a resolution and coda. The interaction between Arthur and time was also significant in
the separate growth modeling analysis for evaluation. Coviewing
Excerpt 5: Miguel at Time 3 in a classroom with peers may have assisted in comprehension and
The Book. understanding of the meaning of the events. Researcher observa-
Once upon a time, they was flying kites. The kite then hit the tree. tions and teacher reports indicated that there appeared to be more
Then a bear try to get it. Now he fall off. And the end.
conversations concerning plot, storyline, and characters after the
Excerpt 6: Raymond at Time 3 viewing sessions among the children who viewed Arthur than
One day a bear was running a kite. And it was a sunny day. Then the among those who viewed Between the Lions. Arthur may have had
kite was stuck in the tree. Then the bear was climbing to the tree. Then
more memorable characters and impressionable storylines than
he was trying to get it. Then the kite ripped. So they got the kite out
Between the Lions, which focused more on phonics and rhythm.
of the tree. And it was so much fun. That was the end.
Children may have understood the emotions of the Arthur charac-
ters through these casual discussions with classmates. Conversa-
Discussion
tions among the Arthur viewers may have reinforced some vocab-
The results from this year-long study show intervention effects; ulary items, expanding the children’s lexicons.
children who viewed Arthur during class hours had steeper trajec- Although each episode of Between the Lions begins with a story
tories on the combined narrative measure than did those who that the Lion family reads together, the program spends consider-
474 UCHIKOSHI

able time directing viewers’ attention to phonological sensitivity, lary was found in this study. English vocabulary was predictive of
the alphabetic principle, letter–sound correspondence, word mean- initial levels of English narrative skills. An investigation of the
ings, punctuation, and other conventions of written English (Rath, children’s word choice from the narratives may be useful for
2000). Thus children’s attention may be diverted from the narra- further understanding the relationship between second-language
tive structure of the extended discourse. After the viewings, the English vocabulary and narrative abilities.
researchers and teachers noted that the children sang the songs and I have demonstrated the positive impact of Arthur on children’s
chanted the rhymes from the show, but they did not discuss the narrative skills. It would also be of value to explore further the
contents of the show, including plot and character development, degree to which characteristics of the language used in Arthur,
like the Arthur groups did.5 such as syntactic complexity and lexical repetition, contributed to
This study also suggests that children can develop mainstream this effect. Research has shown that TV programs designed for
narrative styles prior to first grade. Past research shows that children use characteristics of child-directed speech (Hoff-
narrative styles are influenced by early socialization experiences Ginsberg, 1986; Snow, 1984; Wells, 1985), but a more targeted
and may differ depending on cultural background (Dart, 1992; analysis of what ELL children actually understand after one view-
Heath, 1982, 1986; Minami & McCabe, 1991; Ninio, 1980; Shiro, ing of the various episodes would be informative.
1995; Silva & McCabe, 1996; Tannen, 1980; Wang & Leichtman, Further, this study focused on only one bilingual population; it
2000) as well as SES (Burger & Miller, 1999; Heath, 1982; Shiro, would be of value to replicate the study with other groups of young
2003; Whitehurst et al., 1994). Children from nonmainstream bilinguals, particularly as limited research shows that ELL children
cultural backgrounds or with lower SES enter school with different are likely to begin schooling in English less prepared for literacy
narrative styles than those valued in schools (Dickinson & Mc- instruction than children from middle-class English-speaking
Cabe, 1991; Heath, 1982; Minami & McCabe, 1991). The com- homes (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). In addition, these children
bined narrative measure examined features typical of school norms were followed only for their kindergarten year; although consid-
(such as having a title, an introduction, and a closing as part of the erable other data suggest that narrative skills in kindergarten relate
story structure) and of the stories shown in Arthur. This study to later narrative and literacy skills, a longer term longitudinal
shows that routine and attentive viewing of Arthur can assist study collecting data on those later outcomes would be of great
nonmainstream bilingual children to develop English narrative value.
styles that match the English-speaking school norms faster, even
prior to formal literacy instruction.
It is interesting that there was a gender effect; boys started with Conclusion
and maintained higher English narrative scores than girls. This This study showed how ELL children progressed in their nar-
gender gap was also seen in the children’s initial English vocab- rative skills as a function of exposure to two educational TV shows
ulary scores. Yet for Spanish vocabulary, there was no gender that are easily available on PBS to all children. For the population
difference. of children in this study, viewing Arthur assisted in the develop-
Past research with monolingual English children has shown that, ment of their extended discourse. Further studies should examine
on average, girls have higher literacy skills than boys (Bornstein, the influence of other children’s TV shows on ELL children’s
Haynes, & Painter, 1998; Gambell & Hunter, 1999; Karmiloff & literacy outcomes. A more detailed understanding of ELL chil-
Karmiloff-Smith, 2001). Although only a few studies have looked dren’s literacy processes as well as of the effects of educational TV
at gender differences in second-language acquisition, mainly in the is important to the design of better language education practices,
areas of reading, these studies have provided contradictory results. assessments, and interventions.
Medrano (1986) found no significant gender differences in English
reading among sixth-grade Mexican American students whose first
language was Spanish. Yet L. Reese, Garnier, Gallimore, and
5
Goldenberg (2000) showed that second-generation seventh-grade Intervention effects on phonological awareness skills were seen for the
Latina students outscored their male counterparts in English read- Between the Lions viewers. This finding will be discussed in a separate
ing achievement tests. article.
Although further research is necessary to verify the patterns
identified here, these results are suggestive of several hypotheses. References
Boys and girls may have different exposure histories; for example,
girls may be more likely to be taken care of by Spanish-speaking Adams, M. J. (1990). Learning to read: Thinking and learning about print.
relatives and may have fewer interactions with their English en- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
vironments than boys. Gender effects may also depend on the age August, D., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.). (1997). Improving schooling for
of acquisition, the age of testing, as well as on the skill being language-minority children: A research agenda. Washington, DC: Na-
assessed. In addition, differences in cultural values may play a tional Academy Press.
role. Parents may emphasize academic language attainment more Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. (1997). Observing interaction (2nd ed.).
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
to boys than to girls. Future research should involve a more
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide
in-depth study of children’s home environments. evaluative comments—further explanations of children’s narrative com-
Furthermore, and similar to past findings by Tabors, Roach, and petencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689 –710.
Snow (2001) that kindergarten assessments of oral narrative pro- Beck, S., Coker, D., Hemphill, L., & Bellinger, D. (2003). Literacy skills
duction and receptive vocabulary were moderately correlated, a of children with a history of early corrective heart surgery. In C. M.
relationship between English narrative skills and English vocabu- Fairbanks, J. Worthy, B. Maloch, J. Hoffman, & D. Schallert (Eds.),
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 475

52nd yearbook of the National Reading Conference. Oak Creek, WI: integrated model of early reading development (Tech. Rep. No. 566).
National Reading Conference. Champaign, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study
vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Lan- of parental styles in eliciting oral personal narratives and developing
guage, 25, 367–393. narrative skill. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narra-
Burger, L. K., & Miller, P. J. (1999). Early talk about the past revisited: tive structure (pp. 217–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Affect in working-class and middle-class children’s co-narrations. Jour- Medrano, M. F. (1986). Evaluating the long-term effects of a bilingual
nal of Child Language, 26, 133–162. education program: A study of Mexican American students. Journal of
Chang, C. (2004). Telling stories of experiences: Narrative development of Educational Equity and Leadership, 6, 129 –138.
young Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 83–104. Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1991). Haiku as a discourse regulation device:
Dart, S. (1992). Narrative style in the two languages of a bilingual child. A stanza analysis of Japanese children’s personal narratives. Language
Journal of Child Language, 19, 367–387. in Society, 20, 577–599.
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (1991). The acquisition and development Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother–infant dyads belonging
of language: A social interactionist account of language and literacy to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development, 51, 587–590.
development. In J. F. Kavanagh (Ed.), The language continuum from Nord, C. W., Lennon, J., Liu, B., & Chandler, K. (1999). Home literacy
infancy to literacy (pp. 1– 40). Parkton, MD: York Press. activities and signs of children’s emerging literacy, 1993 and 1999
Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. (Eds.). (2001). Beginning literacy with (NCES 2000 – 026rev). Washington DC: U. S. Department of Education,
language. Baltimore: Brookes. Office of Educational Research and Improvement, National Center for
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd Education Statistics. Retrieved November 2, 2003, from http://nces.ed
ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. .gov/pubs2000/2000026.pdf
Dunn, L. M., Padilla, E. R., Lugo, D. E., & Dunn, L. M. (1986). Test de Office of English Language Acquisition, Language Enhancement, and
Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. Circle Pines, MN: American Guid- Academic Achievement for Limited English Proficient Students. (n.d.).
ance Service. United States rate of LEP student growth. Retrieved May 1, 2002, from
Eaton, J. H., Collis, G. M., & Lewis, V. A. (1999). Evaluative explanations http://www.ncbe.gwu.edu/ncbepubs/reports/state-data/2000/usar.pdf
Paul, R., & Smith, R. L. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds with
in children’s narratives of a video sequence without dialogue. Journal of
normal, impaired, and late developing language. Journal of Speech &
Child Language, 26, 699 –720.
Hearing Research, 36, 592–598.
Galdwell, M. (2000). The tipping point: How little things can make a big
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in
difference. Boston: Little, Brown and Company.
preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26,
Gambell, T. J., & Hunter, D. M. (1999). Rethinking gender differences in
49 – 67.
literacy. Canadian Journal of Education, 24, 1–16.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics:
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral
Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum Press.
discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language,
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1991). Linking children’s connective use and
24, 123–147.
narrative macrostructure. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Develop-
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2002). Narratives in two languages: Assessing
ing narrative structure (pp. 29 –53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
performance of bilingual children. Linguistics and Education, 13, 175–
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1992). Parental styles of narrative elicitation:
197.
Effect on children’s narrative structure and content. First Language, 12,
Heath, S. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home
299 –321.
and school. Language in Society, 2, 49 –76.
Purcell-Gates, V. (1988). Lexical and syntactic knowledge of written
Heath, S. B. (1986). Taking a cross-cultural look at narratives. Topics of narrative held by well-read-to kindergarteners and second graders. Re-
Language Disorders, 7, 84 –94. search in the Teaching of English, 22, 128 –160.
Hill-Scott, K. (2001). Industry standards and practices. In D. G. Singer & Purcell-Gates, V. (1991). Ability of well-read-to kindergarteners to decon-
J. L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of children and the media (pp. 605– 620). texualize/recontextualize experience into written-narrative register. Lan-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. guage and Education, 5, 177–188.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in maternal speech: Their Purcell-Gates, V. (1992). Roots of response. Journal of Narrative and Life
relation to the child’s development of syntax. Developmental Psychol- History, 2, 151–161.
ogy, 22, 155–163. Purcell-Gates, V. (1996). Stories, coupons, and the TV guide: Relation-
Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., Rice, M. L., Kerkman, D., & St. Peters, M. ships between home literacy experiences and emergent literacy knowl-
(1990). Early television viewing. Developmental Psychology, 26, 409 – edge. Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 406 – 428.
420. Purcell-Gates, V. (2001). Emergent literacy is emerging knowledge of
Karmiloff, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Pathways to language: written, not oral, language. New Directions for Child and Adolescent
From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Development, 92, 7–22.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a Purcell-Gates, V., & Dahl, K. L. (1991). Low-SES children’s success and
developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 61– failures at early literacy learning in skills-based classrooms. Journal of
85. Reading Behavior, 23, 1–34.
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English Rath, L. (2000, March). Between the Lions: Using television to promote
vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. literacy. Book Links, pp. 41– 45.
Lemish, D., & Rice, M. (1986). Television as a talking picture book: A Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother– child conversations
prop for language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 13, 251–274. about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive
Linebarger, D. L. (2000). Summative evaluation of “Between the Lions”: Development, 8, 403– 430.
A final report to WGBH Educational Foundation. Boston: WGBH. Reese, L., Garnier, H., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2000). Longitu-
Littell, R. C., Milliken, G. A., Stroup, W. W., & Wolfinger, R. D. (1996). dinal analysis of the antecedents of emergent Spanish literacy and
SAS system for mixed models. Cary, NC: SAS Institute. middle-school English reading achievement of Spanish-speaking stu-
Mason, J., Stewart, J., Peterman, C., & Dunning, D. (1992). Toward an dents. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 633– 662.
476 UCHIKOSHI

Rice, M. L., Huston, A. C., Truglio, R., & Wright., J. C. (1990). Words books: A developmental study. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 458 –
from “Sesame Street”: Learning vocabulary while viewing. Develop- 481.
mental Psychology, 26, 421– 428. Tabors, P., Beals, D., & Weizman, Z. (2001). “You know what oxygen
Rice, M. L., & Woodsmall, L. (1988). Lessons from television: Children’s is?”: Learning new words at home. In D. Dickinson & P. Tabors (Eds.),
word learning when viewing. Child Development, 59, 420 – 429. Beginning literacy with language (pp. 93–110). Baltimore: Brookes.
Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to Tabors, P., Roach, K., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Home language and literacy
preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245–302. environment: Final results. In D. Dickinson & P. Tabors (Eds.), Begin-
Scarborough, H. S., Dobrich, W., & Hager. M. (2001). Preschool literacy ning literacy with language (pp. 111–138). Baltimore: Brookes.
experience and later reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabil- Tannen, D. (1980). A comparative analysis of oral narrative strategies:
ities, 24, 508 –511. Athenian Greek and American English. In W. Chafe (Ed.), The Pear
Shiro, M. (1995). Venezuelan preschoolers’ oral narrative abilities: Focus stories: Cognitive, cultural and linguistic aspects of narrative produc-
on research. Language Arts, 72, 528 –537. tion (pp. 51– 87). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Shiro, M. (2003). Genre and evaluation in narrative development. Journal Van Evra, J. (1998). Television and child development. Mahwah, NJ:
of Child Language, 30, 165–195. Erlbaum.
Silva, M. J., & McCabe, A. (1996). Vignettes of the continuous and family
Wang, Q., & Leichtman, M. D. (2000). Same beginnings, different stories:
ties: Some Latino American traditions. In A. McCabe (Ed.), Chameleon
A comparison of American and Chinese children’s narratives. Child
readers: Teaching children to appreciate all kinds of good stories. New
Development, 71, 1329 –1346.
York: McGraw-Hill.
Wells, G. (1985). Language development in the pre-school years: Lan-
Singer, J. (1998). Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models,
guage at home and at school (Vol. 2). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educa-
University Press.
tional and Behavioral Statistics, 24, 323–355.
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, S. A., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M.,
Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care
Press. and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psy-
Snow, C. E. (1984). Parent– child interactions and the development of chology, 30, 679 – 689.
communicative ability. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & J. Pickar (Eds.), Com- Willenberg, I., & Kang, J. (2001). Bear Story coding manual. Unpublished
municative competence: Acquisition and intervention (pp. 69 –108). manuscript, Harvard University.
Baltimore: University Park Press. Willett, J. (1994). Measuring change more effectively by modeling indi-
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading vidual growth over time. In T. Husen & T. N. Postlethwaite (Eds.), The
difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy international encyclopedia of education (2nd ed., pp. 671– 678). Oxford,
Press. England: Pergamon Press.
Snow, C. E., Tabors, P., Nicholson, P., & Kurland, B. (1995). SHELL: Oral Willett, J., Singer, J. D., & Martin, N. C. (1998). The design and analysis
language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first-grade chil- of longitudinal studies of development and psychopathology in context:
dren. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10, 37– 48. Statistical models and methodological recommendations. Development
Sulzby, E. (1985). Children’s emergent abilities to read favorite story- and Psychopathology, 10, 395– 426.
NARRATIVE DEVELOPMENT IN BILINGUAL KINDERGARTENERS 477

Appendix

Coding of the Five Dimensions in the Combined Narrative Measure

Each Bear Story was coded along five dimensions: story structure “Bear Story Coding Manual” (an unpublished manuscript by I. Willen-
features, events, evaluation, temporality and reference, and story- berg and J. Kang, 2001, Harvard University) with the permission of its
book language. The following coding scheme was adapted from the authors:

(Appendix continues)
Received February 10, 2004
Revision received November 17, 2004
Accepted January 14, 2005 䡲
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

You might also like