You are on page 1of 12

Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning

Author(s): John Seely Brown, Allan Collins, Paul Duguid


Source: Educational Researcher, Vol. 18, No. 1 (Jan. - Feb., 1989), pp. 32-42
Published by: American Educational Research Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1176008 .
Accessed: 14/02/2011 18:02

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at .
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=aera. .

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Educational Research Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Educational Researcher.

http://www.jstor.org
Situated Cognition
and the Culture of Learning
JOHN SEELY BROWN ALLAN COLLINS PAUL DUGUID

he breachbetween cabulary has often been


learning and use, Manyteachingpracticesimplicitlyassumethatconceptual knowl- taught, is slow and generally
which is captured by unsuccessful. There is barely
the folk categories "know edge
can beabstracted fromthe situationsin whichit is learned
andused.Thisarticlearguesthatthisassumption limits
inevitably enough classroom time to
what" and "know how," the teach more than 100 to 200
effectivenessof suchpractices.Drawingon recentresearchin- words
may well be a product of the to cognitionas it is manifestin everyday activity,theauthorsargue much ofper year. Moreover,
structureand practicesof our that a the what is taught turns
education system. Many knowledgeis situated,beingin part productof activity, out to be almost useless in
context,and culture in which it is developedandused. They discuss
methods of didactic educa- how this view the fol-
tion assume a separationbe- of knowledgeaffectsour understanding of learn- practice. They give of students'
and they notethat conventionalschoolingtoo oftenignores lowing examples
tween knowing and doing, ing, the influenceof schoolcultureon what is learnedin school.As uses of vocabulary acquired
treating knowledge as an in- an alternativeto conventionalpractices,theyproposecognitive this way:
tegral, self-sufficient sub- apprenticeship (Collins,Brown,& Newman,in press),which Meandmyparentscorrelate,
stance, theoreticallyindepen- honorsthe situatednature
dent of the situations in of knowledge.Theyexaminetwo ex- because without them I
amples of mathematics instructionthatexhibitcertainkeyfeatures wouldn'tbe here.
which it is learned and used. this approachto teaching.
The primary concern of of I was meticulousaboutfall-
schools often seems to be the ing off the cliff.
transferof this substance, which com- as cognitive apprenticeship(Collins, Mrs. Morrowstimulatedthe soup.2
prises abstract,decontextualizedformal Brown, & Newman, in press) that em- Given the method, such mistakes
concepts. The activity and context in bed learningin activityand make delib- seem unavoidable. Teaching from dic-
which learning takes place are thus re- erate use of the social and physicalcon- tionaries assumes that definitions and
garded as merely ancillary to learn- text are more in line with the under- exemplarysentences are self-contained
ing-pedagogically useful, of course, standing of learning and cognition that "pieces" of knowledge. But words and
but fundamentally distinct and even is emerging from research. sentences are not islands, entire unto
neutralwith respect to what is learned. themselves. Language use would in-
Recent investigations of learning, Situated Knowledge and Learning volve an unremitting confrontation
however, challenge this separating of Miller and Gildea's (1987) work on with ambiguity, polysemy, nuance,
what is learned from how it is learned vocabulary teaching has shown how metaphor, and so forth were these not
and used.' The activityin which knowl- the assumption that knowing and do- resolved with the extralinguistichelp
edge is developed and deployed, it is ing can be separatedleads to a teaching that the context of an utterance pro-
now argued, is not separable from or method that ignores the way situations vides (Nunberg, 1978).
ancillaryto learningand cognition. Nor structurecognition. Theirwork has de- Prominent among the intricacies of
is it neutral.Rather,it is an integralpart scribed how children are taught words language that depend on extralinguistic
of what is learned. Situations might be from dictionary definitions and a few help are indexicalwords-words like I,
said to co-produceknowledge through exemplary sentences, and they have here, now, next, tomorrow,afterwards,
activity. Learning and cognition, it is compared this method with the way this. Indexicalterms are those that "in-
now possible to argue, are fundamen- vocabularyis normallylearned outside dex" or more plainly point to a part of
tally situated. school. the situation in which communication
In this paper, we try to explain in a People generally learn words in the is being conducted.3They are not mere-
deliberately speculative way, why ac- context of ordinary communication. ly context-sensitive;they arecompletely
tivity and situations are integral to This process is startlinglyfast and suc- context-dependent.Wordslike I or now,
cognition and learning, and how dif- cessful. Millerand Gildea note that by
ferent ideas of what is appropriate listening, talking, and reading, the
learning activityproduce very different average 17-year-old has learned vo- JOHNSEELYBROWNand PAULDUGUIDare
results. We suggest that, by ignoring cabulary at a rate of 5,000 words per at the Institutefor Researchon Learning,
the situated natureof cognition, educa- year (13 per day) for over 16 years. By 2550HanoverStreet,PaloAlto, California
tion defeats its own goal of providing contrast, learning words from abstract 94304. ALLANCOLLINSis at BBN Inc, 10
useable, robust knowledge. And con- definitions and sentences taken out of MoultonStreet,Cambridge, Massachusetts
versely, we argue that approaches such the context of normal use, the way vo- 02238.

32 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER
for instance, can only be interpretedin share several significant features with appropriately without understanding
the context of their use. Surprisingly, knowledge: They can only be fully un- the community or culture in which it is
all words can be seen as at least partially derstood through use, and using them used.
indexical (Barwise & Perry, 1983). entails both changing the user's view Conceptual tools similarlyreflect the
Experiencedreadersimplicitlyunder- of the world and adopting the belief cumulative wisdom of the culture in
stand that words are situated. They, system of the culturein which they are which they are used and the insights
therefore, ask for the rest of the used. and experience of individuals. Their
sentence or the context before commit- First, if knowledge is thought of as meaning is not invariantbut a product
ting themselves to an interpretationof tools, we can illustrate Whitehead's of negotiation within the community.
a word. And they go to dictionaries (1929)distinctionbetween the mere ac- Again, appropriateuse is not simply a
with situated examples of usage in quisition of inert concepts and the de- function of the abstractconcept alone.
mind. The situation as well as the dic- velopment of useful, robust knowl- It is a function of the culture and the
tionarysupports the interpretation.But edge. It is quite possible to acquire a activitiesin which the concept has been
the students who produced the sen- tool but to be unable to use it. Similar- developed. Just as carpentersand cab-
tences listed had no supportfroma nor- ly, it is common for students to acquire inet makers use chisels differently, so
mal communicative situation. In tasks algorithms, routines, and decontex- physicistsand engineersuse mathemat-
like theirs, dictionary definitions are tualizeddefinitionsthat they cannotuse ical formulae differently. Activity, con-
assumed to be self-sufficient. The ex- and that, therefore, lie inert. Unfor- cept, and culture are interdependent.
tralinguisticprops that would structure, tunately, this problemis not always ap- No one can be totallyunderstood with-
constrain, and ultimately allow inter- parent. Old-fashioned pocket knives, out the other two. Learning must in-
pretationin normalcommunicationare for example, have a device for remov- volve all three. Teachingmethods often
ignored. ing stones from horses' hooves. People try to impart abstracted concepts as
Learning from dictionaries, like any with this device may know its use and fixed, well-defined, independent en-
method that tries to teach abstractcon- be able to talk wisely about horses, tities that can be explored in proto-
cepts independently of authenticsitua- hooves, and stones. But they may typical examples and textbook exer-
tions, overlooks the way understand- never betray-or even recognize-that cises. But such exemplificationcannot
ing is developed through continued, they would not begin to know how to provide the important insights into
situated use. This development, which use this implement on a horse. Similar- either the culture or the authentic ac-
involves complex social negotiations, ly, students can often manipulate tivities of members of that culture that
does not crystallize into a categorical algorithms, routines, and definitions learners need.
definition. Because it is dependent on they have acquiredwith apparentcom- To talk about academic disciplines,
situations and negotiations, the mean- petence and yet not reveal, to their professions, or even manual trades as
ing of a word cannot, in principle, be teachersor themselves, that they would communities or cultures will perhaps
capturedby a definition,even when the have no idea what to do if they came seem strange. Yet communitiesof prac-
definition is supported by a couple of upon the domain equivalent of a limp- titioners are connected by more than
exemplary sentences. ing horse. their ostensible tasks. They are bound
All knowledge is, we believe, like lan- People who use tools actively rather by intricate, socially constructed webs
guage. Its constituent parts index the than just acquire them, by contrast, of belief, which are essential to under-
world and so are inextricablya product build an increasingly rich implicit standing what they do (Geertz, 1983).
of the activity and situations in which understanding of the world in which The activitiesof many communitiesare
they are produced. A concept, for ex- they use the tools and of the tools unfathomable, unless they are viewed
ample, will continually evolve with themselves. The understanding, both from within the culture. The culture
each new occasion of use, because new of the world and of the tool, continual- and the use of a tool act together to
situations, negotiations, and activities ly changes as a result of their interac- determinethe way practitionerssee the
inevitably recast it in a new, more tion. Learning and acting are interest- world; and the way the world appears
densely textured form. So a concept, ingly indistinct, learning being a con- to them determinesthe culture'sunder-
like the meaning of a word, is always tinuous, life-longprocessresultingfrom standing of the world and of the tools.
under construction.This would also ap- acting in situations. Unfortunately, students are too often
pear to be true of apparently well-de- Learning how to use a tool involves asked to use the tools of a discipline
fined, abstracttechnicalconcepts. Even far more than can be accounted for in without being able to adopt its culture.
these are not wholly definableand defy any set of explicit rules. The occasions To learnto use tools as practitionersuse
categorical description; part of their and conditionsfor use arise directlyout them, a student, like an apprentice,
meaning is always inherited from the of the context of activitiesof each com- must enter that community and its cul-
context of use. munity that uses the tool, framed by ture. Thus, in a significantway, learn-
the way members of that community ing is, we believe, a process of encul-
Learningand tools. To explorethe idea see the world. The community and its turation.
that concepts areboth situatedand pro- viewpoint, quite as much as the tool
gressively developed through activity, itself, determine how a tool is used. Learning and enculturation. Encul-
we should abandon any notion that Thus, carpenters and cabinet makers turatingmay, at first,appearto have lit-
they are abstract,self-containedentities. use chisels differently. Because tools tle to do with learning. But it is, in fact,
Instead, it may be more useful to con- and the way they are used reflect the what people do in learning to speak,
sider conceptualknowledge as, in some particularaccumulatedinsights of com- read, and write, or becoming school
ways, similar to a set of tools.4 Tools munities, it is not possible to use a tool children, office workers, researchers,

JANUARY-FEBRUARY 1989 33
and so on. From a very early age and appear informal, but it is nonetheless Archetypalschool activityis very dif-
throughout their lives, people, con- full-blooded, authenticactivitythat can ferent from what we have in mind
sciously or unconsciously, adopt the be deeply informative-in a way that when we talk of authentic activity, be-
behavior and belief systems of new textbook examples and declarativeex- cause it is very different from what
social groups. Given the chance to planations are not. authentic practitioners do. When
observeand practicein situthe behavior authenticactivitiesaretransferredto the
Authentic Activity
of membersof a culture,people pick up classroom, their context is inevitably
relevant jargon, imitate behavior, and Our case so far rests on an undefined transmuted; they become classroom
graduallystartto act in accordancewith distinction between authentic and tasks and part of the school culture.
its norms. These cultural practices are school activity.If we take learningto be Classroom procedures, as a result, are
often reconditeand extremelycomplex. a process of enculturation,it is possible then applied to what have become
Nonetheless, given the opportunity to to clarifythis distinctionand to explain classroom tasks. The system of learn-
observe and practice them, people why much school work is inauthentic ing and using (and, of course, testing)
adopt them with great success. Stu- and thus not fully productive of useful thereafterremains hermeticallysealed
dents, for instance, can quickly get an learning. within the self-confirmingcultureof the
implicit sense of what is suitable dic- The activitiesof a domain are framed school. Consequently, contrary to the
tion, what makes a relevant question, by its culture. Their meaning and pur- aim of schooling, success within this
what is legitimate or illegitimate pose are socially constructed through culture often has little bearing on per-
behavior in a particular activity. The negotiations among present and past formance elsewhere.
ease and success with which people do members. Activities thus cohere in a Math word problems, for instance,
this (as opposed to the intricacy of way that is, in theory, if not always in are generally encoded in a syntax and
describingwhat it entails) belie the im- practice, accessible to members who diction that is common only to other
mense importance of the process and move within the social framework. math problems. Thus the word prob-
obscuresthe factthat what they pick up These coherent, meaningful, and pur- lems of a textbookof 1478 are instantly
is a product of the ambient culture poseful activities are authentic,accord- recognizable today (Lave, 1988c). But
rather than of explicit teaching. ing to the definition of the term we use word problems are as foreign to
Too often the practices of contem- here. Authenticactivitiesthen, are most authentic math practice as Miller and
porary schooling deny students the simply defined as the ordinarypractices Gildea's example of dictionarylearning
chance to engage the relevant domain of the culture. is to the practices of readers and
culture, because that culture is not in This is not to say that authentic ac- writers. By participatingin such ersatz
evidence. Although studentsare shown tivity can only be pursued by experts. activitiesstudents are likely to miscon-
the tools of many academiccultures in Apprenticetailors(Lave, 1988a),for in- ceive entirelywhat practitionersactual-
the course of a school career, the per- stance, begin by ironing finished gar- ly do. As a result, students can easily
vasive cultures that they observe, in ments (which tacitlyteaches them a lot be introduced to a formalistic,intimi-
which they participate, and which about cutting and sewing). Ironing is dating view of math that encourages a
some enter quite effectively are the simple, valuable, and absolutely culture of math phobia ratherthan one
cultures of school life itself. These authentic. Students of Palincsar and of authentic math activity.
cultures can be unintentionally anti- Brown's (1984) reciprocal teaching of In the creationof classroomtasks, ap-
theticalto useful domain learning. The reading may read elementarytexts, but parently peripheralfeatures of authen-
ways schools use dictionaries,or math they develop authentic strategies that tic tasks-like the extralinguistic sup-
formulae,or historicalanalysis are very are recognized by all readers. The stu- ports involved in the interpretationof
different from the ways practitioners dents in Millerand Gildea's study, by communication-are often dismissed as
use them (Schoenfeld, in press). Thus, contrast, were given a strategy that is "noise" from which salientfeaturescan
students may pass exams (a distinctive a poor extrapolation of experienced be abstractedfor the purpose of teach-
part of school cultures) but still not be readers' situated use of dictionaries. ing. But the context of activityis an ex-
able to use a domain's conceptualtools School activity too often tends to be traordinarily complex network from
in authentic practice. hybrid, implicitly framed by one cul- which practitionersdraw essential sup-
This is not to suggest that all students ture, but explicitlyattributedto another. port. The source of such support is
of math or history must be expected to Classroom activity very much takes often only tacitlyrecognized by practi-
become professionalmathematiciansor place within the culture of schools, al- tioners, or even by teachers or de-
historians, but to claim that in order to though it is attributedto the culture of signersof simulations.Classroomtasks,
learn these subjects (and not just to readers, writers, mathematicians, his- therefore,can completelyfail to provide
learn about them) students need much torians, economists, geographers, and the contextual features that allow
more than abstractconcepts and self- so forth.Many of the activitiesstudents authentic activity. At the same time,
contained examples. They need to be undertake are simply not the activities students may come to rely, in impor-
exposed to the use of a domain's con- of practitioners and would not make tant but little noticed ways, on features
ceptual tools in authentic activity-to sense or be endorsed by the cultures to of the classroom context, in which the
teachers acting as practitionersand us- which they are attributed.This hybrid task is now embedded, that are wholly
ing these tools in wrestling with prob- activity, furthermore, limits students' absent from and alien to authentic ac-
lems of the world. Such activity can access to the importantstructuringand tivity. Thus, much of what is learned
tease out the way a mathematicianor supportingcues that arisefrom the con- in school may apply only to the ersatz
historianlooks at the world and solves text. What students do tends to be er- activity, if it was learned through such
emergent problems. The process may satz activity. activity.

34 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER
Activities of students, practitioners, features of JPF, practitioner, and setting. At no time did the Weight
and just plain folks. The idea that most putative student behavior. Watchercheckhis procedure against
school activity exists in a culture of its This Table is intended mainly to a paperand pencilalgorithm,which
own is central to understanding many make apparentthat, in our terms, there would have produced3/4cup x 2/3cup
of the difficultiesof learning in school. is a great similaritybetween JPFs'and =
? cup. Instead,thecoincidence
of
Jean Lave's ethnographic studies of practitioners'activity. Both have their the problem,setting,and enactment
learning and everyday activity (1988b) activities situated in the cultures in was the means by which checking
reveal how different schooling is from which they work, within which they tookplace. (p. 165)
the activities and culture that give negotiate meanings and construct un- The dieter's solution path was ex-
meaning and purpose to what students derstanding. The issues and problems
learn elsewhere. Lave focuses on the that they face arise out of, are defined tremelyexpedient and drew on the sort
behavior of JPFs(just plain folks) and of inventiveness that characterizesthe
by, and are resolved within the con-
records that the ways they learn are straintsof the activitythey are pursuing. activity of both JPFsand practitioners.
It reflectedthe natureof the activity,the
quite distinct from what students are Lave's work (1988b)provides a good
resources available, and the sort of
asked to do. example of a JPFengaged in authentic resolution requiredin a way that prob-
Threecategoriesprimarilyconcernus activity using the context in which an lem solving that relies on abstracted
here: JPFs,students, and practitioners. issue emerged to help find a resolution.
Put most simply, when JPFsaspire to The example comes from a study of a knowledge cannot.
learn a particularset of practices, they Weight Watchersclass, whose partici- This inventive resolution depended
have two apparent options. First, they pants were preparing their carefully on the dieter seeing the problem in the
can enculturate through apprentice- regulated meals under instruction. particular context, which itself was
embedded in ongoing activity.And this
ship. Becoming an apprentice doesn't In thiscasetheyweretofix a serv-
involve a qualitativechange from what again is characteristicof both JPFsand
ing of cottagecheese,supposingthe
JPFs normally do. People enculturate amount laid out for the meal was experts. The dieter's position gave him
into differentcommunities all the time. privilegedaccess to the solutionpath he
three-quarters of the two-thirdscup chose. (This probably accounts for the
The apprentices'behaviorand the JPFs' the programallowed. The problem
behaviorcan thus be thought of as pret- solverin this examplebeganthe task certaintyhe expressedbeforebeginning
his calculation.)He was thus able to see
ty much the same.5 mutteringthat he had takena cal-
The second, and now more conven- the problemand its resolutionin terms
culus coursein college. . . Then after
of the measuring cup, cutting board,
tional, option is to enter a school as a a pausehe suddenlyannouncedthat
and knife. Activity-tool-culture
student. Schools, however, do seem to he had "gotit!" Fromthenon he ap-
demand a qualitative change in be- pearedcertainhe was correct,even (cooking-kitchen utensils-dieting)
havior. What the student is expected to moved in step throughout this pro-
beforecarryingout theprocedure. He
cedure because of the way the problem
do and what a JPFdoes are significantly filleda measuring-cup two-thirds full
different.The student enters the school was seen and the task was performed.
of cottagecheese,dumpedit out on The whole micro-routine simply be-
culture while ostensibly being taught thecuttingboard,pattedit intoa cir-
came one more step on the road to a
something else. And the general cle,marked a crosson it, scoopedaway meal.6Knowing and doing were inter-
strategies for intuitive reasoning, re- one quadrant,and servedthe rest.
locked and inseparable.
solving issues, and negotiating mean- Thus, "takethree-quarters of two-
ing that people develop through every- thirdsofa cupof cottagecheese"was This sort of problemsolving is carried
out in conjunction with the environ-
day activityare superseded by the pre- not just the problemstatementbut
cise, well-defined problems, formal also the solutionto the problemand ment and is quite distinctfrom the pro-
definitions, and symbol manipulation theprocedure for solvingit. Theset- cessing solely inside heads that many
of much school activity. ting was partof the calculatingpro- teachingpracticesimplicitlyendorse. By
We try to represent this discontinui- cessand the solutionwas simplythe off-loadingpartof the cognitivetask on-
to the environment, the dieter auto-
ty in Table 1, which compares salient problemstatement,enactedwith the
maticallyused his environmentto help
solve the problem.His actionswere not
in any way exceptional; they resemble
TABLE1. many ordinary working practices.
JPF, Practitioner, and Student Activity Scribner (1984) records, for instance,
how complex calculations can be per-
JPFs Students Practitioners formed by practitionersusing their en-
vironment directly. In the case she
reasoning with: causal stories laws causal models
studied, dairy loaders used the con-
acting on: situations symbols conceptual situations figurationof cratesthey were fillingand
resolving: emergent problems well-defined ill-defined emptying almost like an elaborate
and dilemmas problems problems abacus. Nor are such problem solving
producing: negotiable meaning fixed meaning negotiable strategies limited to the physical or
& socially & immutable meaning socialenvironment.This sort of reliance
constructed concepts & socially on situations can be seen in the work
understanding constructed
of physicists, who see "through" for-
understanding
mulae by envisioning a physical situa-

- JANUARY-FEBRUARY 1989 35
tion, which then provides support for thus implicitly devalues not just indi- theirreference(see, for instance,Rubin,
inferencesand approximations(deKleer vidual heuristics,which may be fragile, 1980, on the differencebetween speech
& Brown, 1984). Hutchins' (in press) but the whole process of inventive and writing).
study of intricate collaborative naval problemsolving. Lave (1988c)describes Perhaps the best way to discover the
navigation records the way people dis- how some students feel it necessary to importance and efficiency of indexical
tribute the burden across the environ- disguise effective strategies so that terms and their embedding context is
ment and the group as well. The result- teachers believe the problems have to imagine discourse without them.
ing cognitive activity can then only be been solved in the approved way. Authors of a collaborativework such as
explained in relation to its context. this one will recognize the problem if
"[W]hen the context of cognition is ig- Structuringactivity. Authenticactivity, they have ever discussed the paper
nored," Hutchins observes, "it is im- as we have argued, is important for over the phone. "What you say here"
possible to see the contributionof struc- learners,becauseit is the only way they is not a very useful remark.Herein this
ture in the environment, in artifacts, gain access to the standpoint that en- setting needs an elaborate description
and in other people to the organization ables practitionersto act meaningfully (such as "page 3, second full para-
of mental processes. and purposefully. It is activity that graph, fifth sentence," beginning...)
Instead of taking problemsout of the shapes or hones their tools. How and and can often lead to conversations at
context of their creationand providing why remain to be explained. Activity cross purposes. The problem gets
them with an extraneous framework, also provides experience, which is harderin conferencecalls when yoube-
JPFsseem particularlyadept at solving plainly important for subsequent ac- comes as ambiguous as hereis unclear.
them within the frameworkof the con- tion. Here, we try to explain some of The contents of a shared environment
text that produced them. This allows the products of activity in terms of make a central contributionto conver-
JPFsto share the burdens of both de- idiosyncratic"indexicalized"represen- sation.
fining and solving the problemwith the tations. When the immediacy of indexical
task environment as they respond in Representationsarising out of activi- terms is replaced by descriptions, the
"real time." The adequacy of the solu- ty cannot easily (or perhaps at all) be natureof discoursechanges and under-
tion they reach becomes apparent in replaced by descriptions. Plans, as standing becomes much more proble-
relationto the role it must play in allow- Suchman argues (1987), are distinct matic. Indexical terms are virtually
ing activity to continue. The problem, from situated actions. Most people will transparent.They draw little or no at-
the solution, and the cognitioninvolved agree that a picture of a complex tention to themselves. They do not
in getting between the two cannot be machine in a manual is distinctly dif- necessarily add significantlyto the dif-
isolated from the context in which they ferent from how the machine actually ficulty of understanding a proposition
are embedded. looks. (In an intriguing way you need in which they occur, but simply point
Even though students are expectedto the machineto understandthe manual, to the subject under discussion, which
behave differently, they inevitably do as much as the manual to understand then provides essentialstructurefor the
behave like the JPFsthey are and solve the machine.)The perceptionsresulting discourse.Descriptions,by comparison,
most of their problems in their own from actions are a central feature in are at best translucent and at worst
situated way. Schoenfeld (in press) de- both learning and activity. How a per- opaque, intruding emphatically be-
scribes mathematics students using son perceives activity may be deter- tween speakers and their subjects.The
well-known but unacknowledgedstrat- mined by tools and their appropriated audience has first to focus on the de-
egies, such as the position of a problem use. Whatthey perceive,however, con- scriptionsand try to interpretthem and
in a particularsection of the book (e.g., tributesto how they act and learn. Dif- find what they might refer to. Only
the firstquestionsat the end of chapters ferent activitiesproducedifferentindex- then can the proposition in which they
are always simple ones, and the last icalized representationsnot equivalent, are embedded be understood. (How-
usually demand concepts from earlier universal ones. And, thus, the activity ever elaborate, a description does not
chapters) or the occurrence of a par- that led to those representationsplays merely replacethe indexicalword.) The
ticularword in the problem(e.g., "left" a central role in learning. more elaboratethe description is in an
signals a subtractionproblem), to find Representationsare, we suggest, in- attempt to be unambiguous, the more
solutions quickly and efficiently. Such dexicalized rather in the way that lan- opaque it is in danger of becoming.
ploys indicatehow thoroughlylearners guage is. Thatis to say, they are depen- And in some circumstances,the index-
really are situated, and how they al- dent on context. In face-to-faceconver- ical term simply cannot be replaced
ways lean on whatever context is avail- sations, people can interpret indexical (Perry, 1979).
able for help. Within the practices of expressions (containing such words as Knowledge, we suggest, similarlyin-
schooling this can obviously be very ef- I, you,here,now,that,etc.), because they dexes the situation in which it arises
fective. But the school situation is ex- have access to the indexed features of and is used. The embedding circum-
tremely specialized. Viewed from out- the situation, though people rarely stances efficiently provide essential
side, where problems do not come in notice the significanceof the surround- parts of its structureand meaning. So
textbooks, a dependency on such ings to theirunderstanding.The impor- knowledge, which comes coded by and
school-based cues makes the learning tance of the surroundingsbecomes ap- connected to the activity and environ-
extremely fragile. parent, however, when they try to hold ment in which it is developed, is spread
Furthermore,though schooling seeks similar conversations at a distance. across its component parts, some of
to encourage problem solving, it disre- Then indexical expressions become which are in the mind and some in the
gards most of the inventive heuristics problematic until ways are found to world much as the finalpictureon a jig-
that students bring to the classroom. It secure their interpretationby situating saw is spread across its component

36 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER -
pieces. in a way similar to that evident-and students with the opportunity to enter
As Hutchins (in press), Pea (1988), evidently successful-in craft appren- the culture of mathematicalpractice.
and others point out, the structure of ticeship. In this section, we examine Schoenfeld'sstudentsbringproblems
cognition is widely distributed across briefly two examples of mathematics to class that he and they investigate
the environment, both social and teaching in an attemptto illustratehow mathematically.His students can wit-
physical. And we suggest that the en- some of the characteristicsof learning ness and participate in spontaneous
vironment, therefore, contributes im- that we have discussed can be honored mathematicalthinking and see mathe-
portantly to indexical representations in the classroom. We use examples matics as a sense-making pursuit. This
people form in activity.These represen- from mathematicsin part because that approach is distinctive because, before
tations, in turn, contributeto future ac- is where some of the most innovative graduate school, few students get the
tivity. Indexical representations de- work in teaching can be found. But we opportunity to see their teachers en-
veloped through engagement in a task firmly believe that this sort of teaching gaged in mathematicalpractice,yet the
may greatlyincreasethe efficiencywith is not just possible in mathematics. students are expected to understand
which subsequent tasks can be done,
if part of the environment that struc-
tures the representations remains in-
variant. This is evident in the ability to
perform tasks that cannot be described
or remembered in the absence of the
situation. Recurringfeatures of the en-
vironment may thus afford recurrent
sequences of actions. Memory and sub-
sequent actions, as knots in handker-
chiefs and other aidesmemoiresreveal,
are not context-independentprocesses.
Routines (Agre, 1985) may well be a
product of this sort of indexicalization.
Thus, authenticactivitybecomes a cen-
tral component of learning.
One of the key points of the concept
of indexicality is that it indicates that
knowledge, and not just learning, is
situated.A corollaryof this is that learn- I
Ij
ing methods that are embedded in c
authenticsituationsare not merely use- s
x
ful; they are essential. (e
L
Ij

Learning Through Cognitive I


I
Apprenticeship cL
(e
u
We have been working toward a con- I

ception of human learning and reason- -,


x
ing that, we feel, it is important for I
school practicesto honor. Though there s
u
are many innovative teachers, schools,
and programs that act otherwise, pre-
valent school practices assume, more
often than not, that knowledge is indi-
vidual and self-structured,that schools
are neutral with respect to what is The tentmakers and the apprentice
learned, that concepts are abstract,rela-
tively fixed, and unaffected by the ac-
tivity through which they are acquired Schoenfeld's teaching of problem solv- the nature of that practice.
and used, and that JPFbehaviorshould ing. Schoenfeld's teaching of problem In one case (Schoenfeld, in press), he
be discouraged. solving (1985, in press) deliberatelyat- and his class faced the problem of the
Cognitive apprenticeship (Collins, tempts to generate mathematicalprac- magicsquare(see Figure1). Though the
Brown, & Newman, in press), whose tice and to show college students how problem is relatively straightforward,
mechanisms we have, to some extent, to think mathematically about the the collaborativework involved in solv-
been trying to elucidate, embraces world, how to see the world through ing it and, importantly,in analyzingthe
methods that stand in contradistinction mathematicians'eyes, and, thus, how solution helped reveal to the class the
to these practices. Cognitive appren- to use the mathematician's tools. His way mathematicianslook at problems.
ticeship methods try to enculturate approachgoes well beyond simply giv- The class worked collectively through
students into authentic practices ing students problem-solvingstrategies. a number of strategies, which, on re-
through activity and social interaction Much more importantly, it provides flection, they recognized as more gen-

JANUARY-FEBRUARY 1989 37
working forwards from an initial solu- in their community. The students' pro-
FIGURE1 tion; using systematic generating pro- cedureparallelsthe storyproblemsthey
The Magic Square Problem cedures; having more than one way to had created.Eventuallythey find ways
solve a problem. Schoenfeld is con- to shortenthe process, and they usually
Can you place the digits 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8,
9 in the box below, so that the sum of the digits sistently careful to emphasize that all arriveat the standard algorithm,justi-
along each row, each column, and each
such strategiesare illustratedin action, fying theirfindingswith the storiesthey
diagonal is the same? The completed box is developed by the class, not declaredby created earlier.
called a magic square. the teacher. In his classes, the belief Through this method, students de-
system is instilledin the only way it can velop a composite understanding of
be, through practice in which the stu- four different kinds of mathematical
dents actively take part. knowledge: (a) intuitiveknowledge,the
kind of short cuts people invent when
Lampert's teaching of multiplication. doing multiplication problems in
Lampert (1986) also involves her stu- authentic settings; (b) computational
dents in mathematical exploration, knowledge,the basic algorithmsthat are
which she tries to make continuous usually taught; (c) concreteknowledge,
with their everyday knowledge. She the kind of concrete models of the
has devised methods for teaching algorithm associated with the stories
mathematics to fourth grade students the students created; and (d) principled
Note: From Schoenfeld, in press. that lead from students' implicitunder- knowledge,the principles such as asso-
standing of the world beyond the class- ciativityand commutativitythat under-
room, through activity and social con- lie the algorithmic manipulations of
eral and more powerful mathematical struction in the culture, to the sort of numbers. Lamperttries to inculcatean
ideas. In discussing whether 9 can go robust learning that direct teaching of inseparable understanding of these
in the center of the square, they de- algorithms usually fails to achieve. kinds of knowledge and the connec-
She startsteachingmultiplication,for tions between them, and thus to bridge
veloped the ideas of "focusing on key
points that give leverage," and "ex- example, in the context of coin prob- the huge gap that emerges from much
lems, because in the community of conventionalteachingbetween concep-
ploiting extreme cases." Although fourth grade students, there is usually
Schoenfeld may appear to be teaching tual knowledge and problem solving
a strong, implicit, shared understand- activity-between, as we characterized
strategy ratherthan subject matter, he
was, more fundamentally, building ing of coins. Next, the students create them at the beginning, knowing and
with his class a mathematical belief stories for multiplication problems, doing.
system around his own and the class's drawing on their implicitknowledge to This approachfostersproceduresthat
intuitive responses to the problem. delineatedifferentexamplesof multipli- are characteristicof cognitive appren-
As an indication that Schoenfeld's cation. Then, Lampert helps them ticeship:
class was working in the culture of toward the abstract algorithm that * By beginningwith a taskembedded in a
mathematics, not in the culture of everyone learns for multidigitmultipli- it shows the students the
cation, in the context of the coin prob- familiaractivity,
schooling, he did not have the students lems and stories the community has legitimacyof theirimplicitknowledgeand
stop at what, in culture of school prac- created.Thus, the method presents the
its availabilityas scaffoldingin apparently
tice, would mark the end: an answer. unfamiliartasks.
Arewe done?In mostmathematics algorithm as one more useful strategy * By pointingto differentdecompositions,
to help them resolve community prob-
classestheansweris "yes." Earlyin it stressesthat heuristicsare not absolute,
lems.
the semester,my students all say but assessedwith respectto a particular
The firstphase of teachingstartswith
"yes," expectingme to go on to task-and that even algorithmscan be as-
anotherproblem.My answer,how- simple coin problems, such as "using sessedin this way.
only nickels and pennies, make 82 * By allowingstudentsto generatetheir
ever,is a resounding"no." In most cents." With such problems, Lampert
classes,so-called"problems" areex- ownsolutionpaths,it helpsmakethemcon-
helps her students exploretheirimplicit scious, creativemembersof the cultureof
ercises;you are done when you've
shownthatyou'vemasteredthe rele- knowledge. Then, in the second phase, mathematicians. And, in
the students create stories for multipli- problem-solving
vanttechnique bygettingtheanswer. enculturating throughthisactivity,theyac-
cation problems (see Figure 2). They
(Schoenfeld,in press) performa series of decompositionsand
quiresomeof the culture'stools-a shared
His class's goal, by contrast, was to vocabulary andthe meansto discuss,reflect
discover that there is no one, magical-
understand the mathematicalnature of upon, evaluate,and validatecommunity
ly "right" decomposition decreed by
magic square, and it was in part by do- proceduresin a collaborative process.
authority, just more and less useful
ing this that the belief system was ex- decompositions whose useis judged in Schoenfeld's approach differs prin-
emplified.The class exploredother pos- the context of the problem to be solved cipallyin its strong emphasis on expos-
sible magicsquaresand discoveredgen- and the interests of the problem ing students to the authentic ways of
eral principles (e.g., an algebraicform solvers. thinking of a cultureand its conceptual
for describing the squares). It also led The third phase of instruction viewpoint, as much as to its subject
to some further generalizable mathe- gradually introduces students to the matter.
maticalstrategiesthat are less common- standard algorithm, now that such an Figure 3 shows how, in the terms of
ly seen in classroom practice, such as algorithmhas a meaning and a purpose cognitiveapprenticeship,we can repre-

38 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER
sent the progress of the students from
embedded activityto generalprinciples FIGURE2
of the culture.In this sequence, appren- Story Problems for Teaching Multiplication
ticeship and coaching in a domain be-
gin by providing modeling in situ and T: Can anyone give me a story that could go with this
scaffolding for students to get started multiplication... 12 x 4?
in an authenticactivity.As the students S1: Therewere 12 jars, and each had 4 butterfliesin it.
T: And if I did thismultiplication
andfoundthe answer,
gain more self-confidence and control, what would I know aboutthose jarsand butterflies?
they move into a more autonomous
phase of collaborativelearning, where I S1: You'dknowyou hadthatmanybutterfliesaltogether.
ST: Okay, here are the jars. [Drawsa pictureto represent
they begin to participateconsciously in thejarsof butterflies-seediagram.]The starsin them
the culture. The social network within x will stand for butterflies.Now, it will be easier for
the culture helps them develop its lan- us to counthow manybutterfliestherearealtogether,
xX if we think of the jarsin groups. And as usual, the
guage and the belief systems and pro- mathematician'sfavoritenumberfor thinkingabout
motes the process of enculturation.Col-
laboration also leads to articulationof groups is?
S2: 10
strategies,which can then be discussed T: Eachof these 10 jarshas 4 butterfliesin it. [Drawsa
and reflected on. This, in turn, fosters
looparound10 jars.]...
generalizing,grounded in the students'
situated understanding. From here, T: Suppose I erase my circleand go backto looking at the 12 jarsagain altogether.Is there
students can use theirfledgling concep- any other way I could group them to make it easier for us to count all the butterflies?
tual knowledge in activity, seeing that S6: You could do 6 and 6.
activity in a new light, which in turn T: Now, how many do I have in this group?
leads to the furtherdevelopment of the S7: 24
conceptual knowledge. T: How did you figure that out?
In languagelearning,for instance,the S7: 8 and 8 and 8. [Heputsthe6 jarstogetherinto3 pairs,intuitivelyfindinga groupingthatmade
original frail understanding of a word thefiguringeasierfor him.]
is developed and extended through
T: That's 3 x 8. It's also 6 x 4. Now, how
subsequent use and social negotiation,
many are in this group?
though each use is obviously situated. •
S6: 24. It's the same. They both have 6 jars.
Miller and Gildea (1978) describe two
T: And now how manyaretherealtogether?
stages of this process. The first, in xx xx S8: 24 and 24 is 48.
which people learnthe word and assign
T: Do we get the samenumberof butterflies
it a semantic category (e.g., the word as before?Why?
olive is first assigned to the general
S8: Yeah,becausewe have the samenumber
category of color words), is quickly of jars and they still have 4 butterflies in
done. The second, in which distinctions each.
within this semantic category (e.g., be-
Note:FromLampert,1986.
tween olive and other colors) are ex-
plored as the word occurs again and
again, is a far more gradual process,
which "may never be completely fin- work at and membershipin their trade. worlds(see Burton, Brown, & Fischer,
ished" (p. 95). This second phase of Throughthis process, apprenticesenter 1984) can be replaced by increasing
word learning corresponds to the de- the culture of practice. So the term ap- complex enculturating environments.
velopment through activity of all con- prenticeshiphelps to emphasize the cen- Cognitiveemphasizes that apprentice-
ceptual knowledge. The threadbare tralityof activityin learningand knowl- ship techniques actuallyreach well be-
concepts that initiallydevelop out of ac-edge and highlights the inherently yond the physical skills usually asso-
tivity are graduallygiven textureas they context-dependent, situated, and en- ciated with apprenticeshipto the kinds
are deployed in different situations. culturatingnature of learning. And ap- of cognitive skills more normally asso-
prenticeship also suggests the paradigm ciated with conventional schooling.
Apprenticeship and Cognition of situated modeling, coaching, and This extension is not as incompatible
The development of concepts out of fading (Collins, Brown, & Newman, in with traditional apprenticeship as it
and through continuing authentic ac- press), whereby teachers or coaches may at first seem. The physical skills
tivity is the approach of cognitive ap- promote learning, first by making ex- usually associated with apprenticeship
prenticeship-a term closely allied to plicittheirtacitknowledge or by model- embody important cognitive skills, if
our image of knowledge as a tool. Cog- ing their strategies for students in our argument for the inseparabilityof
nitive apprenticeshipsupports learning authentic activity. Then, teachers and knowing and doing is correct.Certain-
in a domain by enabling students to ac- colleagues support students' attempts ly many professions with generally
quire, develop, and use cognitive tools at doing the task. And finally they em- acknowledged cognitive content, such
in authentic domain activity. Similarly, power the students to continue inde- as law, medicine, architecture,and bus-
craftapprenticeshipenables apprentices pendently. The progressive process of iness, have nonetheless traditionally
to acquire and develop the tools and learning and enculturation perhaps been learned through apprenticeship.
skills of their craft through authentic argues that IncreasinglyComplexMicro- Moreover, advanced graduate stu-

JANUARY-FEBRUARY 1989 39 -
McCloskey,Caramazza,& Green, 1980;
FIGURE3 White,1983)thatstudentshavemanymis-
conceptions aboutqualitativephenomena in
Students' Progress from Embedded Activity to Generality
physics.Teachers rarelyhavetheopportuni-
Sapprenticeship collaboration -reflection ty to hearenoughof what studentsthink
to recognize whentheinformation thatis of-
WORLD/ GENERALITY feredbackbystudentsis onlya surfaceretell-
ACTIVITY [
ing for schoolpurposes(the handingback
articulation of an uncomprehended tool,as we described
?coaching multiple
practice it at thebeginning)thatmaymaskdeepmis-
conceptionsaboutthe physicalworldand
problem solvingstrategies.Groupshowever,
dents in the humanities, the social lective wisdom of the community. canbeefficientin drawingout, confronting
sciences, and the physical sciences ac- The role of narrativesand conversa- anddiscussingbothmisconceptions and in-
quire their extremely refined research tions is perhaps more complex than effectivestrategies.
skills through the apprenticeshipsthey might first appear. An intriguing role * Providing collaborative work skills.
serve with senior researchers.It is then in learning is played by "legitimate Studentswhoaretaughtindividually rather
that they, like all apprentices, must peripheral participation," where peo- thancollaboratively canfail to developskills
recognize and resolve the ill-defined ple who are not taking part directly in neededfor collaborative work.In the col-
problems that issue out of authentic ac- a particularactivity learn a great deal laborative conditions
of theworkplace, know-
tivity, in contrastto the well-definedex- from their legitimate position on the ing how to learnand workcollaboratively
ercises that are typically given to them periphery(Lave& Wenger, in prepara- is increasinglyimportant.If peoplearego-
in text books and on exams throughout tion). It is a mistake to think that im- ing to learnand workin conjunctionwith
their earlierschooling. It is at this stage, portant discourse in learning is always others,theymustbegiven the situatedop-
in short, that students no longerbehave direct and declarative. This peripheral portunityto developthoseskills.
as students, but as practitioners, and participation is particularlyimportant In looking at Schoenfeld's and
develop theirconceptualunderstanding for people entering the culture. They Lampert'steaching, in noting what we
through social interactionand collabor- need to observe how practitioners at believe are important features of their
ation in the culture of the domain, not various levels behave and talk to get a methods, and in stressingsocialinterac-
of the school. sense of how expertise is manifest in tion and collaborativelearning, we are
In essence, cognitive apprenticeship conversation and other activities. trying to show how teaching through
attemptsto promotelearningwithin the a form of apprenticeship can accom-
nexus of activity, tool, and culture that Cognitive apprenticeshipand collabor- modate the new view of knowledge
we have described.Learning,both out- ative learning. If, as we propose, learn- and learning we have been outlining.
side and inside school, advances ing is a process of enculturatingthat is The increasing role of the teacher as a
through collaborativesocial interaction supported in part through social inter- masterto apprentices,and the teachers'
and the social construction of knowl- action and the circulationof narrative, use of authentic domain activity as a
edge. Resnick has pointed out (1988) groups of practitionersare particularly major part of teaching will perhaps,
that throughoutmost of their lives peo- important, for it is only within groups once and for all, dismiss George Ber-
ple learn and work collaboratively,not that social interactionand conversation nard Shaw's scurrilous criticism of
individually, as they are asked to do in can take place. Salientfeaturesof group teachers, "He who can, does. He who
many schools. Lampert's and Schoen- learning include: cannot, teaches." His comment may
feld's work, Scardamalia,Bereiter,and * Collectiveproblemsolving. Groupsare then be replacedwith AlexanderPope's
Steinbach's teaching of writing (1984), notjust a convenientway toaccumulate the hopeful "Let such teach others who
and Palincsarand Brown's (1984)work individualknowledgeof their members. themselves excell."
with reciprocalteaching of reading all Theygive risesynergistically to insightsand
Conclusion-Toward an Epistemology
employ some form of social interaction, solutionsthatwouldnotcomeaboutwithout
social construction of knowledge, and them (Schoenfeld,in preparation). of Situated Cognition
collaboration. * Displaying multiple roles. Successful Much research investigating situated
Withina culture,ideas are exchanged executionof mostindividualtasksrequires features of cognition remains to be
and modified and belief systems de- studentsto understandthe manydifferent done. It is, however, already possible
veloped and appropriatedthroughcon- rolesneededfor carryingout any cognitive to begin serious reappraisal of the
versationand narratives,so these must task. Gettingone personto be ableto play assumptionsabout learningthat under-
be promoted, not inhibited. Though all the rolesentailedby authenticactivity lie current classroom practice (see, for
they are often anathema to traditional and to reflectproductivelyuponhis or her example Resnick,1988;Shanker,1988).
schooling, they are an essential compo- performance is oneof the monumental tasks One of the particularlydifficultchal-
nent of social interactionand, thus, of of education.Thegroup,however,permits lenges for research,(which exceptional
learning. They provide access to much differentrolesto bedisplayedandengenders teachers may solve independently) is
of the distributed knowledge and reflectivenarrativesand discussionsabout determining what should be made ex-
elaborate support of the social matrix the aptnessof those roles. plicit in teaching and what should be
(Orr, 1987). So learning environments * Confrontingineffectivestrategies and left implicit.A common strategyin try-
must allow narrativesto circulateand misconceptions. Weknowfroman exten- ing to overcome difficult pedagogic
"war stories" to be added to the col- sive literature(diSessa,1982, 1983, 1986; problems is to make as much as possi-

40 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER
ble explicit. Thus, we have ended up deeply indebted to her groundbreaking work. ing researchinto a theoryof computation
with wholly inappropriatemethods of 2The dictionary definitions that the students and semanticsbuilton notionsof situated-
used in writing these sentences are as follows:
teaching. Whatever the domain, expli- Correlate-be related one to the other; ness, embeddedness, and embodiedness; of
cation often lifts implicit and possibly meticulous-very careful; stimulate-stir up.
SusanStucky'simportantnew idealization
even nonconceptual constraints They were given these definitions with little of mind in terms of "radical"efficiency
(Cussins, 1988) out of the embedding or no contextual help, so it would be unfair ratherthanrationality;andalsoof thework
world and tries to make them explicit to regard the students as foolish for using the on indexicalityof Philip Agre and David
words as they did.
or conceptual. These now take a place Chapman.
3In the linguistics literature, the term deixis
in our ontology and become something is often used instead of indexicality. See, for Thisresearchwassupported in partbythe
more to learn about ratherthan simply example, J. Fillmore, Santa Cruz Lectures. PersonnelandTrainingResearch Programs,
4This image is, of course, not original. For
something useful in learning. But in- the way it is developed here, we are particular-
PsychologicalSciencesDivision, Officeof
dexical representations gain their effi- Naval Research under Contract NO.
ly indebted to Richard Burton, who explored
ciency by leaving much of the context it during a symposium on education organized N00014-C-85-0026. ContractAuthority
underrepresented or implicit. Future by the Secretary of Education of Kentucky and Number,NR 667-540.
Identification
work into situated cognition, from to D. N. Perkins' book Knowledgeas Design An extendedversionof thisarticlewillap-
(1986).
which educationalpracticeswill benefit,
sThe JPF must, of course, have access to a
pearas IRLreportNo. 88-0008 (available
must, among other things, try to frame culture and become what Lave and Wenger fromtheInstituteforResearch on Learning)
a convincingaccountof the relationship (in preparation) call a "legitimate peripheral andas BBNResearch Report 6886 (available
between explicit knowledge and im- participant." And, of course, an apprentice from Bolt Beranek& NewmanInc.).
plicit understanding. usually has to do a great deal of work. We are
not trying to suggest that anything magical oc-
We have described here only a frag- curs in the process of enculturation. (Medical Agre, P. (1985). Routines. MIT AI Memo.
ment of an agenda for a fully developed interns testify to how hard it can be.) But the Barwise, K. J., & Perry, J. (1983). Situationsand
theory of situated cognition. There re- process, we stress, is not qualitatively different attitudes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
mains major theoretical work to shift from what people do all the time in adopting Burton, R., Brown, J. S., & Fischer, G. (1984).
the traditionalfocus of education. For the behavior and belief systems of their peers. Skiing as a model of instruction. In B.
6To get some sense of how foreign this is to Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everydaycognition:
centuries, the epistemology that has school tasks, it might be useful to imagine the Its developmentin socialcontext.(pp. 139-150)
guided educationalpracticehas concen- impropriety of a student's being given this Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.
trated primarily on conceptual repre- problem and asked "Does the dieter have a Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E.
sentation and made its relation to ob- measuring cup, cutting board, and knife at (in press). Cognitive apprenticeship:
hand?" Though word problems are meant to Teaching the craft of reading, writing and
jects in the world problematic by as- ground theory in activity, the things that struc- mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Know-
suming that, cognitively,representation ture activity are denied to the problem solvers. ing, learning,and instruction:Essaysin honor
is prior to all else. A theory of situated Textbooks ask students to solve supposedly of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
"real-life" questions about people who do Cussins, A. (1988). The connectionistconstruc-
cognitionsuggests that activityand per- tion of concepts.(SSL Research Report). Palo
very unreal things, such as driving at constant
ception are importantlyand epistemol- speeds in straight lines or filling leaking Alto, CA: Xerox Palo Alto Research Center.
ogically prior-at a nonconceptual troughs with leaking buckets. Students are deKleer, J., & Brown, J. S. (1984). A qualita-
level-to conceptualization and that it usually not allowed to indulge in real-life tive physics based on confluences. Artificial
is on them that more attentionneeds to speculation. Their everyday inventiveness is IntelligenceJournal, 24, 1-3.
be focused. An epistemology that be- constrained by prescribing and proscribing diSessa, A. (1982). Unlearning Aristotelian
ways in which the solution must be found. The physics: A study of knowledge-based
gins with activity and perception, ubiquitous Mr. Smith might, after all, wisely learning. Cognitive Science, 6, 37-75.
which are first and foremostembedded repair the hole in his bucket or fill the trough diSessa, A. (1983). Phenomenology and the
in the world, may simply bypass the with a hose. Sitting down and calculating how evolution of intuition. In D. Gentner and
classical problem of reference-of many journeys it will take with a leaking A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models. (pp.
bucket is probably the very last thing he would 15-33) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
mediating conceptual representations. do. (See also Lave, 1988c.) diSessa, A. (1986). Knowledge in pieces. In G.
In conclusion, the unheraldedimpor- Forman & P. Pufall (Eds.), Constructivism
tance of activity and enculturation to in the computerage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
learning suggests that much common Editor'sNote: In an effortto encouragein- Engestrom, Y. (1987). Learningby expanding.
educational practiceis the victim of an Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit Oy.
formeddiscussionanddebateon the themes Fillmore, J. (1974). Santa Cruzlectureson deixis.
inadequate epistemology. A new of thisarticle,the newERwillpublisha set Bloomington, IN: Indiana University
epistemology might hold the key to a of commentaries in the May 1989 issue. Linguistics Club.
dramaticimprovement in learning and Geertz, C. (1983). Localknowledge.New York:
Basic Books.
a completely new perspective on edu-
Hutchins, E. (in press). Learning to navigate.
cation. Acknowledgement: Manyof theideasin this In S. Chalkin and J. Lave (Eds.), Situated
paperemerged fromgroupdiscussionsat the Learning.
Institutefor Researchon Learning.Weare Lampert, M. (1986). Knowing, doing, and
'All work in this area is to a greater or lesser teaching multiplication. Cognition and In-
especiallygratefulto JamesGreeno,Jean
degree, built upon research of activity theorists struction, 3, 305-342.
such as Vygotsky, Leontiev, and others. For Lave,SusanNewman,Roy Pea, and John Lave, J. (1977). Tailor-made experiments and
examples of recent work, see for instance, Rheinfrank,who read earlierdrafts and evaluating the intellectual consequences of
Rogoff and Lave, 1984; Scribner, 1984; Hut- commented on themwithgreatcare.Weare apprenticeship training. The Quarterly
chins, in press; Engestrom, 1987; Lave and also gratefulto RichardBurton, William Newsletter of the Institute for Comparative
Wenger, in preparation; and in particular Human Development, 1, 1-3.
Lave, 1977, 1988a, 1988b, 1988c, in prepara- Clancey,and Alan Schoenfeldfor helpful Lave, J. (1988a). The culture of acquisitionand
tion. Anyone familiar with Jean Lave's work andinsightfulcontributions.Moregeneral- the practice of understanding. (IRL report
on learning, apprenticeship, and everyday ly, we would like to acknowledgethe in- 88-00087). Palo Alto, CA: Institute for Re-
cognition will realize at once that we are fluenceof BrianCantwellSmith'spioneer- search on Learning.

- JANUARY-FEBRUARY 1989 41 -
Lave, J. (1988b). Cognitionin practice.Boston, 257, (3), 94-99. Perkins, D. N. (1986). Knowledgeas design.
MA: Cambridge. Nunberg, G. (1978). Thepragmaticsof reference. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lave, J. (1988c). Wordproblems:A microcosmof Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Lin- Perry, J. (1979). The problem of the essential
theories of learning. Paper presented at guistics Club. indexical. Nous, 13, 3-21.
AERA annual conference, New Orleans, Orr, J. (1987). Talkingaboutmachines.(SSL Re- Resnick, L. (1988). Learning in school and out.
LA. port). Palo Alto, CA: Xerox Palo Alto Re- EducationalResearcher,16(9), 13-20.
Lave, J. (in preparation). Tailored learning: search Center. Rogoff, B., & Lave, J. (Eds.). (1984). Everyday
Educationand everydaypracticeamongcrafts- Palincsar, A. S. (1986). Metacognitive strategy cognition: Its developmentin social context.
men in West Africa. instruction. Exceptional Children, 53, Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (in preparation). 118-124. Rubin, A. (1980). A theoretical taxonomy of
Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). the differences between oral and written
participation. Reciprocal teaching of comprehension- language. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, &
McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., & Green, B. fostering and monitoring activities. Cogni- W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoreticalissues in
(1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence tion and Instruction, 1, 117-175. readingcomprehension(pp. 411-438). Hills-
of external forces: Naive beliefs about the Pea, R. D. (1988). Distributed intelligence in dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
motion of objects. Science,210, 1139-1141. learningand reasoningprocesses.Paper pre- Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., & Steinbach, R.
Miller, G. A., & Gildea, P. M. (1987). How sented at the meeting of the Cognitive (1984). Teachability of reflective processes
children learn words. ScientificAmerican, Science Society, Montreal. in written composition. Cognitive Science,
8, 173-190.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematicalproblem
solving. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (in press). On mathematics
Educational Testing Service as sense-making: An informal attack on the
unfortunate divorce of formal and informal
1989-90 Fellowship Programs mathematics. In D. N. Perkins, J. Segal, &
J. Voss (Eds.), Informalreasoningand educa-
? William H. Angoff, Director tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Educational Testing Service invites applications for the ETS Postdoctoral Fellowship Schoenfeld, A. H. (in preparation). Ideasin the
air. (IRLreport 88-0011). Palo Alto, CA: In-
Program and the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) Visiting Scholar stitute for Research on Learning.
Program. Scribner, S. (1984). Studying working intelli-
Programs gence. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Every-
ETS Postdoctoral Fellowship Program day cognition:Its developmentin socialcontext
Up to four participants will conduct research for 1 year (September 1, 1989, through (pp. 9-40). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
July 31, 1990) at ETS in Princeton, NJ, in association with ETS senior staff in one versity Press.
of the following areas: psychometrics, cognitive psychology, educational psychology, Shanker, A. (1988). Exploring the missing con-
statistics, higher education, technology, occupational/vocational testing, minority nection. New YorkTimes, E7.
issues, testing issues, or policy studies. Stipend: $24,000. Some relocation expenses Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situatedactions.
will be included, as will a small allowance for an accompanying family. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Goals of the Program: To provide research opportunities for recent awardees of White, B. (1983). Sources of difficulty in un-
the doctorate and to increase the number of women and minority professionals work- derstanding Newtonian dynamics. Cogni-
ing in the areas specified above. tive Science, 7, 41-65.
Who Should Apply: The program is open to any individual who holds a doctorate Whitehead, A. N. (1929). Theaims of education.
in a relevant discipline and provides evidence of prior research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) Visiting Scholar
Program
One or two participants, using the NAEP data base, will conduct their own studies
(normally from September 1, 1989, through July 31, 1990) at ETS in Princeton, NJ, Continuedfrom page 10
with access to senior NAEP and ETS research staff. Studies should pertain to educa-
tional policy or measurement issues associated with black, Hispanic, or other minori- (Eds.), Job training for youth. Columbus,
OH: The Ohio State University, The Na-
ty students. Stipend: set in relation to compensation at home institution. Some reloca- tional Center for Research in Vocational
tion expenses will be included, as will a small allowance for an accompanying family. Education.
Goals of the Program: To provide research opportunities for recent awardees of National Federation of Independent Business.
the doctorate and to increase the number of women and minority professionals work-
(1987). [Survey of a stratified random sam-
ing in the areas specified. ple.] Unpublished raw data.
Who Should Apply: The program is open to any individual who holds a doctorate A. C. Nielsen Company. (1987). Unpublished
in a relevant discipline and provides evidence of prior research. raw data.
How to Apply (There is no special application form.) Organization of Economic Cooperation and
Applicants should submit: Development. (1986). Living conditions in
OECD countries: A compendium of social
"* A resume of educational and job history, honors, awards, etc. indicators. SocialPolicyStudiesNo. 3. Paris,
"* A detailed description of research interests and experience, plus a description of France: Author.
the nature of the research the applicant would be interested in pursuing during Sizer, T. R. (1984). Horace's compromise:The
the fellowship year (letter of about three pages). dilemmaof the Americanhigh school. Boston,
"* Names, addresses, and telephone numbers of three individuals who are willing to MA: Houghton Mifflin.
provide recommendations for the candidate. Slavin, R. (1983). When does cooperative
"* Transcripts-undergraduate and graduate.
learning increase student achievement?
An applicant should specify the program for which he or she is applying. PsychologicalBulletin, 99, 429-445.
Stevenson, H., Lee, S., & Stigler, J. W. (1986,
Applications for 1989-90 must be received by ETS on or before
February). Mathematics achievement of
February 1, 1989. All applicants will be notified by April 30, 1989. Chinese, Japanese & American children.
Contact: Direct required materials and inquiries to: Margaret B. Lamb, ETS, Mail Science, 231, 693-699.
Stop 30-B, Princeton, NJ 08541-0001; telephone (609) 734-1124. Taubman, P., & Wales, T. (1975). Education
as an investment and a screening device.
In F. T. Juster (Ed.), Education,income,and
human behavior.New York: McGraw-Hill.

42 EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHER -

You might also like