You are on page 1of 28

Psychological Bulletin © 2009 American Psychological Association

2009, Vol. 135, No. 5, 679 –706 0033-2909/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016311

Psychosis as a Risk Factor for Violence to Others: A Meta-Analysis

Kevin S. Douglas Laura S. Guy


Simon Fraser University University of Massachusetts Medical School

Stephen D. Hart
Simon Fraser University

The potential association between psychosis and violence to others has long been debated. Past research
findings are mixed and appear to depend on numerous potential moderators. As such, the authors
conducted a quantitative review (meta-analysis) of research on the association between psychosis and
violence. A total of 885 effect sizes (odds ratios) were calculated or estimated from 204 studies on the
basis of 166 independent data sets. The central tendency (median) of the effect sizes indicated that
psychosis was significantly associated with a 49%– 68% increase in the odds of violence. However, there
was substantial dispersion among effect sizes. Moderation analyses indicated that the dispersion was
attributable in part to methodological factors, such as study design (e.g., community vs. institutional
samples), definition and measurement of psychosis (e.g., diagnostic vs. symptom-level measurement,
type of symptom), and comparison group (e.g., psychosis compared with externalizing vs. internalizing
vs. no mental disorder). The authors discuss these findings in light of potential causal models of the
association between psychosis and violence, the role of psychosis in violence risk assessment and
management, and recommendations for future research.

Keywords: psychosis, violence, risk assessment, mental illness, schizophrenia

People have long assumed that there is a link between major 1985). Some critics were cautious, concluding that the extant
mental illness (MMI) and violence. This belief is prevalent across research was an inadequate basis from which to draw any firm
cultures and has been quite stable over at least the past five conclusions (the “absence of proof” position; e.g., Monahan, 1981/
millennia (Monahan, 1992a). Surveys of community residents 1995). Others held more extreme views, arguing that, despite its
indicate that this idea is still widespread in contemporary Ameri- weaknesses, the evidence was sufficient to disprove a link between
can society (Link & Cullen, 1986; Link, Cullen, Frank, & mental disorder and violence (the “proof of absence” position; e.g.,
Wozniak, 1987; Pescosolido, Monahan, Link, Stueve, & Teplin, 1985).
Kikuzawa, 1999; Riskind & Wahl, 1992; Socall & Holtgraves, Whether there is a link between MMI and violence, and, more
1992). Starting in the 1960s and continuing into the mid-1980s, importantly, what the nuances of such a link may be, is important
critics began to question this assumption. The basis for the criti- for several reasons. First, persons with MMI are among the most
cisms was twofold: First, there was a general lack of empirical stigmatized groups in society (Corrigan & O’Shaughnessy, 2007;
evidence supporting the association and, second, the results of Corrigan & Penn, 1999). Moreover, persons who carry a psychi-
follow-up studies of psychiatric patients released into the commu- atric diagnosis may internalize such stigma and experience dimin-
nity were interpreted as evidence against an association (Bonta, ished self-esteem and self-efficacy (Watson, Corrigan, Larson, &
Law, & Hanson, 1998; Monahan, 1981/1995; Rabkin, 1979; Sells, 2007). A perception that persons with MMI are violent un-
Steadman & Halfon, 1971; Steadman & Keveles, 1972; Teplin, doubtedly contributes to this well-documented stigma (Phelan &
Link, 1998). Understanding the relationship between these constructs
could inform efforts to dispel myths (if there is not an association) or,
Kevin S. Douglas and Stephen D. Hart, Department of Psychology, conversely, could educate the public about the realities of MMI,
Simon Fraser University; Laura S. Guy, Department of Psychiatry, Uni- including the limits of its association with violence.
versity of Massachusetts Medical School. Second, various symptoms and syndromes of MMI, such as
Kevin S. Douglas gratefully acknowledges the support of the Michael delusions and hallucinations, are often used in violence risk as-
Smith Foundation for Health Research, Career Scholar Program. During sessments to determine whether persons’ constitutionally protected
the study, Kevin S. Douglas served as a Guest Professor of Applied liberties should be violated in the name of public safety. Perhaps
Criminology at Mid-Sweden University. Laura S. Guy gratefully acknowl- the clearest example of this is involuntary civil commitment,
edges the support of the Social Science and Humanities Research Council
where (depending on jurisdiction) laws dictate that a person may
of Canada, Doctoral Fellowship. We thank Ray Koopman for his statistical
consultation, and Sarah Spain for her assistance in conducting the study.
be detained against his or her will if, inter alia, a risk to others is
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kevin S. posed that is connected to a MMI.
Douglas, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, 8888 Uni- Third, research has demonstrated that violence perpetration is
versity Drive, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada, V5A 1S6. E-mail: correlated with other adverse outcomes for persons with MMI,
douglask@sfu.ca such as victimization and suicide/self-harm (Hillbrand, 2001;

679
680 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

Nicholls, Brink, Desmarais, Webster, & Martin, 2006). Although (Bjørkly, 2002a, 2002b; Bloom, 1989; Bradford, 1983; De Pauw &
causal statements cannot be made about this association, it high- Szulecka; Junginger, 1996; Krakowski, Volavka, & Brizer, 1986;
lights the need to understand the connection between MMI and Monahan, 1992b; Tardiff, 1984; Wessely, 1993). For example, in
violence in that the perpetration of violence could inform under- a Danish birth cohort of more than 358,000 people, Brennan,
standing of risk for the experience of victimization and suicide/ Mednick, and Hodgins (2000) reported that the risk for violent
self-harm. Similarly, violence to others ostensibly could disrupt offending for men with schizophrenia was 4.6 times higher than
treatment efforts, as well as lead to psychiatric deterioration and that of the general population, even after controlling for the po-
overall worse mental health and quality of life. In turn, these tentially confounding factor of low socioeconomic status (SES).
outcomes could elevate the risk for self-harm, self-neglect, victim- For women, the risk was 23.2 times higher. Similarly, a recent U.S.
ization, and alienation from familial and other supports. study of 1,410 patients with schizophrenia drawn from 57 mental
Fourth, violence to others poses a salient public health concern. health sites across 24 states (Swanson et al., 2006) found that
To the extent that MMI raises the risk for violence, persons in the positive symptoms of schizophrenia were associated with both
community will be victimized. Often, when persons with MMI are minor and serious violence, even after controlling for numerous
violent, the victims of violence are family members (Estroff, possible confounds and covariates.
Swanson, Lachicotte, Swartz, & Bolduc, 1998), who therefore However, others have rejected the view that violence has any
may bear a disproportionate risk of victimization and personal substantial association with MMI more generally, or with psycho-
suffering. sis more specifically. A large, multisite follow-up of patients
Finally, there are enormous potential costs associated with a discharged from psychiatric emergency units in three states found
potential link between MMI and violence. Financial costs associ- that MMI was typically unrelated, and sometimes slightly nega-
ated both with MMI/mental health treatment and with violence are tively related, to violence (Appelbaum, Robbins, & Monahan,
estimated to be in the hundreds of billions, or trillions, of dollars 2000; Monahan et al., 2001; Steadman et al., 1998). Some very
per year (Anderson, 1999; D. P. Rice, Kelman, & Miller, 1992). To specific symptoms, such as command hallucinations to perpetrate
the extent that violence is associated with MMI, there may be violence, predicted violence (Monahan et al., 2001), as did comor-
concomitant economic and personal costs due to increased violent bid MMI and substance use disorders (Steadman et al., 1998).
behavior (costs to members of the public, to the patient) and However, a fair summary of this study is that MMI, including
society at large (costs of hospitalization, incarceration). psychotic disorders, played a very small role in the violence of
Numerous narrative reviews of the literature on mental disorder patients.
and violence or crime have been written. Popular academic posi- Elbogen and Johnson (2009) analyzed a two-wave epidemio-
tion has shifted over the years. Prior to the 1990s, the conventional logical data set of 34,653 persons residing in the community in the
view held that there is no association (e.g., Teplin, 1985) or at least United States. The authors reported that MMI, measured at Time
no demonstrable association (Monahan, 1981/1995; Rabkin, 1979) 1, did not predict violence, measured between Times 1 and 2.
between mental disorder and violence. Starting in the 1990s, However, comorbid MMI and substance use disorders did predict
however, the conventional view evolved to one acknowledging later violence, more so than substance use disorders alone, sug-
that the association between mental disorder and violence was gesting an interaction between MMI and substance use disorders.
small but statistically robust and practically important (Monahan, Although the odds of future violence among those with schizo-
1992b; Mulvey, 1994). This position is aptly summarized by E. phrenia alone were about double the odds of future violence
Silver (2006): among persons without schizophrenia, this association was not
statistically significant, despite the large sample size, perhaps
The vast body of research conducted . . ., including studies of violence
attributable to extreme skew in cell frequencies.
committed by psychiatric inpatients before, during and after hospital-
ization, studies of rates of mental disorder among inmates incarcerated A lack of association has been reported in studies of mentally
for violent offenses, epidemiological and birth cohort studies measur- disordered offenders. For example, on the basis of studies of a
ing the onset and prevalence of mental disorder and violence cross- large cohort of forensic psychiatric patients, Quinsey, Harris, Rice,
sectionally and over the life course, and several comprehensive liter- and Cormier (2006) concluded the following:
ature reviews summarizing these studies, suggests that: Although
most people with major mental disorder do not engage in violence, the The presence of schizophrenia and psychotic symptoms exhibited
likelihood of committing violence is greater for people with a major around the time of the index offense or admission to hospital were
mental disorder than for those without. (p. 685) negatively related to risk. Psychosis, psychotic symptoms, and exac-
erbation of those symptoms have little value as indicators of the risk
Similarly, in a review of a cluster of family, twin, adoption, and of violence in offender populations. (p. 113)
epidemiological studies, Tehrani, Brennan, Hodgins, and Mednick
(1998) stated the following: The same conclusion was reached in a meta-analysis of variables
associated with recidivism among mentally disordered offenders.
Violent offending by the mentally ill and the offspring of the mentally Bonta et al. (1998) found that the average association between
ill tends to be recidivistic. These groups tend to commit a dispropor- psychosis and violence was small and negative (r ⫽ ⫺.04) across
tionately high percentage of the total number of violent crimes com- the 11 studies in their meta-analysis reporting on psychosis.
mitted by the total cohort. The absolute number of the violent crimes
Why have findings and opinions been so discrepant? One likely
committed by these individuals has some societal significance.
(p. S85)
explanation is methodological diversity. Researchers have exam-
ined the association between MMI and violence in many different
Of the various forms of MMI, most reviewers identified psy- samples, using many different research designs, defining and mea-
chotic disorders as having the clearest association with violence suring MMI and violence in many different ways. As such, one
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 681

cannot easily determine from a cursory reading of the literature behavior. The thought disturbances include delusions (false be-
why some studies find support for an association and others do not. liefs, often bizarre, held with conviction even in the face of
One can find studies from all types of settings that report positive disconfirmatory evidence) and impaired communication (disorga-
associations, including samples of criminal offenders (Etherington, nized, illogical, or incoherent speech). Perceptual disturbances
1993), forensic patients (Erb, Hodgins, Freese, Müller-Isberner, & include hallucinations (perception in the absence of an external
Jöckel, 2001), civil patients (Aarsland, Cummings, Yenner, & stimulus, such as hearing voices or seeing things that are not
Miller, 1996), and the general population (Brennan et al., 2000). there), derealization (a sense that the external world is not real),
Similarly, one can locate studies from these same settings that fail and depersonalization (a sense that one is no longer a real, auton-
to find a positive association between mental disorder and vio- omous human being). The behavioral disturbances include disrup-
lence: criminal offenders (Arboleda-Florez et al., 1995), forensic tions in activity level (extreme agitation or lethargy) and disorga-
patients (Harris, Rice, & Quinsey, 1993), civil patients (Appel- nized or purposeless behavior (odd gestures, gait, and posturing;
baum et al., 2000), and the general population (Elbogen & John- social withdrawal and poor hygiene; and strange habits, such as
son, 2009; Swartz & Lurigio, 2004). Given such discrepant find- eating cigarette butts or playing with feces). People with psychosis
ings across different populations, it is simply understandable why also frequently experience disturbances of mood (flat or blunted
different conclusions are reached by different commentators. emotions, silliness) and motivation (extreme apathy).
Another explanation is confounding factors. Understanding the Clearly, psychosis is a heterogeneous syndrome. But it is em-
link between MMI and violence is complicated by the fact that pirically robust: Certain symptoms co-occur much more frequently
mental illness is associated with a host of other variables, many of than would be expected by chance and wax or wane together over
which, in turn, increase the odds for violence (Elbogen & Johnson, time. Moreover, events or circumstances that tend to exacerbate or
2009). Some researchers have raised the sensible possibility that alleviate one symptom often influence several others at the same
the association between MMI and violence, should it be observed, time. In fact, factor analytic techniques suggest that the broad
could be attributable to confounding factors, such as young age, syndrome of psychosis appears to subsume at least three distinct
low SES, comorbid substance use, or personality disorder (Walsh, domains of symptomatology: positive, disorganized, and negative
Buchanan, & Fahy, 2002). Controlling or adjusting for potential (Ratakonda, Gorman, Yale, & Amador, 1998; see also Baxter,
confounding factors may result in a dramatic change in the inter- 1997, who provided a conceptual discussion of the three-domain
pretation of findings. This is a legitimate possibility. Some studies model as it relates to violence). The positive domain includes
have reported that, once other putative risk factors are controlled or symptoms that are pathological by their presence, such as delu-
adjusted for, the relationship between MMI and violence becomes sions and hallucinations. The negative domain includes symptoms
nonsignificant (Mojtabai, 2006). Yet, many studies— often of that are pathological by their absence, such as flat or blunted
good quality— have found the converse: MMI remains predictive affect, poverty of speech, apathy, and social withdrawal. Finally,
of violence in the presence of other risk factors that have been the disorganization domain includes symptoms that reflect impair-
entered into multivariate analyses (Swanson et al., 2006) or after ment in basic cognitive functioning, such as in odd or purposeless
adjusting for population parameters, such as age (Brennan et al., behavior, impaired communication, and inappropriate affect.
2000; Eronen, Hakola, & Tiihonen, 1996b), marital status, and Why might psychosis be associated with violence? There are at
SES (Brennan et al., 2000). Resolving this confounder issue will least three possible explanations. It is possible that psychosis is a
require as much theory as data in that some possible covariates cause of violence. Psychotic symptoms may provide a clear, if
could be consequences, rather than causes, of MMI. For instance, delusional, motivation for violence or interfere with the ability to
some research shows that MMI causes low SES, rather than the manage interpersonal conflict. An important point here is that
converse (e.g., the geographic drift hypothesis; Dembling, proof of causation requires demonstration that psychotic symp-
Rovnyak, Mackey, & Blank, 2002; see also Hudson, 2005). Hence, toms occur before (certain) acts of violence. Psychosis also may be
it is not clear whether such variables should be controlled for in a consequence of violence. Perhaps the stress of perpetrating
analyses of the association between mental illness and violence. violence triggers the onset of psychotic symptoms in people who
Despite these methodological and conceptual complications are so predisposed. Finally, psychosis may be a simple correlate of
within the literature, one theme that has emerged from past nar- violence. The association between the two may be statistical rather
rative reviews and research is that the key to understanding the than causal, the result of links with some third variable, such as
relationship between MMI and violence may lie with a more stressful life events, lack of social support, personality traits,
focused approach on its symptoms. In particular, commentators substance use, victimization, and so forth. If this explanation were
have suggested that psychotic disorders (e.g., schizophrenia) and true, we would expect to find no clear temporal or statistical
symptoms (e.g., delusions) might form the basis for the bridge association between psychosis and violence, at least after control-
between MMI and violence (Monahan, 1992b). We turn our at- ling for potential confounding factors. At the present time, there
tention to this issue. exists no clear, unambiguous evidence to rule out any of these
potential mechanisms.
Possible Conceptual Links Between Psychosis If psychosis is a cause of violence, there are three major roles it
and Violence may play. First, psychosis may play a role in focusing (organizing)
decision and behavior, giving individuals a clear motivation for
Psychosis is a syndrome found in mental disorders such as violence. Many people with psychosis report clear explanations for
schizophrenia, delusional disorders, bipolar mood disorder, and their behavior and commit acts that are complex, organized, and
some forms of severe depression. The syndrome comprises symp- goal-oriented, even if appearing illogical to outside observers. This
toms reflecting profound disturbances in thought, perception, and has been described aptly as the “principle of rationality within
682 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

irrationality” (Link & Stueve, 1994, p. 143). If psychotic symp- 2002), criminal offenders (Porporino & Motiuk, 1995), community
toms play a focusing role in violence, they are almost certain to be residents (Brennan et al., 2000; Elbogen & Johnson, 2009), or
symptoms from the positive domain (i.e., delusions or hallucina- some mixture of the foregoing (Ryan, Hart, Messick, Aaron, &
tions). Burnette, 2004). Comparisons can be made among persons with
Second, certain types of psychosis may play a role in destabi- different disorders (Tardiff, Marzuk, Leon, & Portera, 1997) or to
lizing (disorganizing) decisions and behavior, interfering with the individuals without a diagnosis of mental illness (Beaudoin, Hod-
ability of individuals to manage interpersonal conflicts. Here, gins, & Lavoie, 1993). Some studies have used a form of random
disturbances of thought, behavior, and affect (illogical speech, sampling, such as consecutive admissions (Klassen & O’Connor,
agitation, labile mood) may frustrate psychotic individuals or the 1988a), whereas others have not (Asnis, Kaplan, van Praag, &
people with whom they interact, increasing the likelihood that Sanderson, 1994). Some have used matching designs (Hodgins,
someone will become angry or make an impulsive decision to act 1992). Both prospective (Monahan et al., 2001) and postdictive
violently. Hiday (1995; see also Hiday, 1997) has posited a model designs (Modestin & Wuermle, 2005) commonly have been used,
in which violence may occur through “tense situations” elicited by and violence has sometimes counted only if it occurred in a
active psychotic symptoms. Baxter (1997) has hypothesized that hospital or prison (Krakowski & Czobor, 2004b) and in other cases
this type of psychosis can lead to “disorganized/impulsive” vio- has counted only if it occurred in the community (Swanson,
lence and crime. Borum, Swartz, & Hiday, 1999).
Third, psychosis may play a disinhibiting role in violence. MMI. In past research, mental illness often was considered a
Whereas positive symptoms motivate or excite behavior and dis- unitary, static construct and was inferred from an individual’s
organization symptoms destabilize behavior, negative symptoms (former) status as a psychiatric patient. The state of clinical prac-
interfere with goal-directed behavior. Because of their retarding tice with respect to diagnosis and record keeping often did not
influence on behavior, negative symptoms probably play a rather permit comprehensive and reliable assessments of mental disorder.
limited role in decisions to act violently. However, negative symp- This made it impossible to determine whether violence was dif-
toms may increase violence risk in specific circumstances. For ferentially associated with various forms of mental disorder (e.g.,
example, if individuals have comorbid substance use or personality Which is more strongly related to violence: schizophrenia or
disorders, symptoms or consequences of the comorbid disorder substance use disorder?) and whether the severity or course of the
may give rise to motivations for violence, and negative psychotic disorder had an impact (e.g., Is remission of symptoms associated
symptoms may result in a lack of inhibitions to act violently by with decreased likelihood of violence?). Monahan (1988; Mona-
(further) impairing one’s ability to experience empathy, remorse, han & Steadman, 1994) has described the problem of “impover-
or anxiety. Moreover, negative symptoms that result in depression ished” predictor variables in the field of violence risk assessment,
or suicidality may increase violence risk, as morbid thoughts of which is closely allied with the issue of the relationship between
self-harm may change or expand in focus to include others. These psychosis and violence. The problem is rooted in gross categori-
theoretical routes to violence underscore the need for an analysis at zations of complex phenomena, such as MMI. Finer gradations of
the level of symptom or at least that of symptom domain. conceptualization, to the symptom level if possible, are likely to
provide more meaningful data on the relationship between psy-
Methodological Challenges in the Study of Psychosis chosis and violence. Very broadband definitions, such as “any
and Violence mental disorder,” are apt to blur important distinctions between
specific psychotic syndromes and violence. Junginger (1996) made
The interest sparked by the psychosis–violence debate has led to a similar point in his narrative review, in which he described the
increased awareness of methodological weaknesses in the research concept of “psychotic action,” which refers to violence that stems
(see discussions by Junginger & McGuire, 2004; Krakowski et al., from and is consistent with the contents and themes of symptoms,
1986; Monahan, 1981/1995, 1988, 1992b; Monahan & Steadman, such as delusions and hallucinations.
1994; Mulvey, 1994; Mulvey, Blumstein, & Cohen, 1986;Walsh et In addition to the level of conceptualization, studies differ in
al., 2002; Wessely, 1993). At least four major areas of concern terms of how they measure psychosis: Some use only chart-based
have been identified: study design, conceptualization and measure- diagnoses (e.g., Quinsey et al., 2006), others use well-validated
ment of MMI, conceptualization and measurement of violence, research-based interview measures (e.g., Crocker et al., 2005), and
and the role of potential confounding factors. yet others simply rely on the diagnoses arrived at by clinicians in
Study design. In the past, many studies were based on rela- the course of their everyday duties. All such differences can impact
tively small samples of convenience comprising cohorts of civil or the observed association between violence and mental illness, if
forensic psychiatric patients who had undergone assessment, were only because of the systematically higher reliability of research-
treated for months or years, and then were followed up some time based, standardized interview procedures relative to unstructured
after their release into the community. Problems with such a design clinical diagnostic procedures (e.g., First, 2003).
include the lack of an appropriate comparison group (e.g., Was the Violence. There is no simple way to define or measure vio-
rate of violence among patients higher or lower than that among lence (e.g., Douglas & Ogloff, 2003; Monahan & Steadman, 1994;
nonpatients?) and an inability to control for interventions (e.g., Did Monahan et al., 2001; Mulvey, Shaw, & Lidz, 1994). In the past,
treatment or community support alter the likelihood of violence?). there was a tendency to conceptualize violence in dichotomous
Further, studies on the connection between mental illness and terms—that is, present versus absent— on the basis of reviews of
violence have been conducted in a variety of samples, including official criminal or psychiatric records. This practice may be
civil psychiatric patients (Monahan et al., 2001), forensic psychi- described as providing an impoverished criterion variable (Mona-
atric patients or “insanity acquittees” (Rosenfeld & Harmon, han, 1988; Monahan & Steadman, 1994) and possesses the same
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 683

problems as do impoverished predictor variables. Recently, re- Method


searchers have paid more attention to the nature of the violence
committed. Numerous studies on the topic, however, still decline Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis
to define violence (e.g., Alexander, Crouch, Halstead, & Piachaud,
Search procedure. Relevant studies were located in four ways.
2006). Others include within their definitions ostensibly nonvio-
First, we conducted searches of the PsycINFO, MEDLINE, Crim-
lent behavior, such as damaging property (e.g., Cooper, Browne,
inal Justice Abstracts, Google Scholar, National Criminal Justice
McClean, & King, 1983; Grassi, Peron, Marangoni, Zanchi, &
Reference Service (NCJRS), Sociological Abstracts, Humanities
Vanni, 2001) or self-harming behavior (e.g., Barlow, Grenyer, &
and Social Sciences Index Database, and Dissertation Abstracts
Ilkiw-Lavallle, 2000; Barnard, Robbins, Newman, & Carrera,
computerized literature databases. The stems of the following
1984).
identifier and subject words were used in separate and combined
Research has varied greatly in interpretations of exactly what
searches: mental illness/disorder, psychopathology, psychosis,
behaviors constitute violence. Some have limited the definition to
schizophrenia, hallucination, delusion, affective disorders, mood
physical acts that cause demonstrable harm to victims (Monahan et
disorders, violent/aggressive behavior, aggression, homicide, as-
al., 2001); others have studied only homicide (Schanda et al.,
sault, crime, and criminal. Second, we searched the reference lists
2004); and yet others have included less serious forms of behavior,
of major narrative review articles. Third, we examined the tables
such as minor physical acts (i.e., pushing) or verbal behavior (i.e.,
of contents of 65 journals. The journals selected comprised all
threats to harm someone; Troisi, Kustermann, Di Genio, & Sir-
sources in which studies included in the present meta-analysis
acusano, 2003). Moreover, the method of detecting violence has
were published, as well as several additional journals known to
ranged from relatively simple procedures, such as sole reliance on
publish high-quality research. Finally, we searched the reference
official state or federal recidivism records (Kaliski & Zabow,
lists of articles identified in the first three steps. We limited our
1995), to more comprehensive procedures involving official
search to articles published in the scientific literature before or
records, self-report, and reports from collateral informants (Mona-
during August 2006. Unpublished research was limited to disser-
han et al., 2001).
tations.
Confounding factors. As reported above, some studies have
Inclusion and exclusion criteria. Studies were coded for the
reported on the relationship between MMI and violence after
meta-analysis if they met two inclusion criteria. First, they needed
controlling for possible competing risk factors. A related issue
to present data on the association between psychosis and violent
stems from the particular diagnostic mix in a given sample. For
behavior. Any disorder characterized by psychosis was included
instance, as Quinsey et al. (2006) have argued, it could be that,
(e.g., any schizophrenia-spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, de-
when compared with the risk posed by persons with primary
pression with psychotic features) so long as the assessment was not
diagnoses of personality disorder or substance abuse/dependence,
based solely on a self-report measure of personality or psychopa-
MMI poses an inverse relative risk for violence. Evidence has
thology (e.g., Brief Symptom Inventory, Derogatis & Melisaratos,
accumulated to support both substance use (especially alcohol) and
1983; Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2, Butcher,
certain personality disorders (Cluster B; psychopathic personality)
Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989; Personality
as robust risk factors for violence (for substance use/alcohol, see
Assessment Inventory, Morey, 1991), as self-reports do not di-
Mulvey et al., 2006; Steadman et al., 1998; Swanson et al., 2002;
rectly assess the presence of psychotic symptoms (e.g., the studies
for psychopathic personality, see Grann, Långström, Tengström, &
of Parker, Morton, Lingefelt, & Johnson, 2005, and Valliant,
Kullgren, 1999; Monahan et al., 2001; Skeem & Mulvey, 2001).
Gristey, Pottier, & Kosmyna, 1999, were deemed not to meet this
Some studies have indeed reported that once such diagnoses are
inclusion criterion because the only assessment of psychosis com-
controlled, or when major mental illness is compared with these
prised MMPI scores). Studies that did not state explicitly that the
other diagnoses, its relative association with violence is decreased
mentally ill participants in their sample had some form of psycho-
(Tengström, Hodgins, Grann, Långström, & Kullgren, 2004). Yet,
sis were deemed not to meet this inclusion criterion (e.g., Lagos,
as with other areas of this literature, one can locate studies that fail
Perlmutter, & Saexinger, 1977, p. 1134, was not coded because
to demonstrate a reduced association (Stueve & Link, 1997).
participants were described only as having a “mental illness”). For
some studies, despite there being a focus on psychosis, we could
Purpose of the Meta-Analysis not parse out psychotic from nonpsychotic participants.
In an effort to strike a balance between our competing interests
Given the divergent findings on the association between of wanting to compare “uncontaminated” groups of people with
psychosis and violence reported within the body of research (sum- and without psychosis and wanting to include as many studies as
marized above), a quantitative synthesis of the literature seems possible for the sake of generalizability, we created the following
necessary to move the field forward. We decided to conduct a rule: Studies were deemed to have met this inclusion criterion if no
meta-analysis of the association between MMI and violence, fo- more than 5% of the persons in the “psychosis group” had or might
cusing more specifically on psychosis. We had two goals for this have had disorders without psychosis and/or if no more than 5% of
meta-analysis: first, to characterize, in general terms, the magni- the persons in the “non–psychosis group” had or might have had
tude of the association between psychosis and violence observed in some form of psychosis. Several high-quality studies (e.g., McNiel
research to date and, second, to identify moderating factors—such & Binder, 1989, 1994) were not included in the meta-analysis on
as study design and issues related to the conceptualization and this basis (i.e., more than 5% of the study’s “psychosis group” did
measurement of psychosis and violence (as discussed above)—that not have psychosis and/or more than 5% of the study’s non–
might moderate this association. psychosis group” had psychosis). The number of studies that were
684 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

included in the meta-analysis according to these criteria was 1998) were judged to overlap only minimally and were coded as
small.1 Further, in an effort to ensure that persons without psy- comprising separate data sets.4
chosis were not included within the “psychosis group,” we did not
code effect sizes on the basis of total scores from clinician- or Choice and Calculation of Effect Size
researcher-rated measures, such as the Brief Psychiatric Rating
Scale (BPRS; Overall, & Gorham, 1962) or the Positive and Odds ratios were chosen as the effect size because they are
Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, appropriate for characterizing the association between two natu-
1987). When psychosis was assessed with these types of measures, rally dichotomous variables (e.g., psychotic or not; violent or not)
only individual scales and items that assessed psychosis specifi- and are easily calculated from data presented in the form of 2 ⫻ 2
cally (e.g., “hallucinatory behavior”) were coded (e.g., scales that contingency tables. Of special relevance to the present investiga-
assessed “general psychopathology” were not coded). tion, odds ratios are directly comparable across studies with dif-
Violence was defined as any actual, attempted, or threatened ferent experimental designs (i.e., retrospective, cross-sectional,
harm to another person or persons (Webster, Douglas, Eaves, & and prospective), which typifies this literature. As Fleiss (1994)
Hart, 1997). Thus, studies in which episodes of self-harm or noted,
violence against objects formed part of the definition of violence
The odds ratio is not prone to the artifactual appearance of interaction
were not included in the meta-analysis. As such, many studies that
across studies due to the influence on other measures of association or
used the total score on structured measures of violence that record
effect of varying marginal frequencies or to constraints on one or the
several types of “violence” (such as the Overt Aggression Scale; other sample proportion. On the basis of this and all of its other
Yudofsky, Silver, Jackson, Endicott, & Williams, 1986) were not positive features, the odds ratio is recommended as the measure of
included because harm to property or self is part of the definition choice for measuring effect or association when the studies contrib-
of violence or aggression in these measures. We also followed the uting to the research synthesis are summarized by fourfold tables.
“5% rule” for violence such that only studies in which less than 5% (p. 259, emphasis added)
of the “nonviolent” group was violent and/or less than 5% of the
“violent” group was not violent were included. Very few studies As the majority of studies presented frequency data reducible to
were included on the basis of this rule.2 fourfold tables, we opted for the odds ratio as the effect size of
The second inclusion criterion was that the data needed to be choice. The use of odds ratios in meta-analyses also has been
presented in a manner that allowed us to calculate odds ratios or to discussed and recommended by Haddock, Rindskopf, and Shadish
estimate them from other statistics, such as Pearson’s r or Cohen’s (1998). Odds ratios are relatively easy to interpret, with the ob-
d. As such, we did not include studies or effect sizes in which the served values indicating the increased relative risk of one variable
association between psychosis and violence was described as (in this case, violence) associated with the presence of the other
being “not significant” or otherwise lacked sufficient information variable (in this case, psychosis). Odds ratios of 1 indicate no
to compute an effect size (i.e., that psychosis was significant but association between the two variables; those greater than 1 indicate
a positive association (i.e., increased risk), and those less than one
only a p value was reported). Although a few studies were deemed
not to meet this inclusion criterion on this basis (e.g., Walsh et al.,
2004), this strategy more typically led to the exclusion of a subset 1
Two studies were coded in which the “psychotic” group contained
of effect sizes from a study rather than exclusion of the entire (fewer than 5%) individuals without psychosis (Fido, Razik, Mizra, &
study. In addition, some studies did not meet this inclusion crite- El-Islam, 1992; Fulwiler, Grossman, Forbes, & Ruthazer, 1997), and one
rion because, although they reported the percentage of violent study was coded in which there were (fewer than 5%) persons with
persons who were psychotic or vice versa (i.e., the percentage of psychosis in the “nonpsychotic” group (Oulis, Lykouras, Dascalopoulou, &
nonviolent individuals who were not psychotic), there was no Psarros, 1996).
2
comparison group (e.g., Fazel & Grann, 2004) and, hence, esti- In one study (Nestor, Haycock, Doiron, Kelly, & Kelly, 1995), 2
mating an effect size for the effect of psychosis on violence participants in the nonviolent group had committed acts of violence; in one
study (Raja & Azzoni, 2005), self-harm was included in the definition of
literally was impossible. A detailed record pertaining to selection
violence; and in one study (Buckley et al., 2004), individuals who com-
of studies and effect sizes (as well as all transformational proce- mitted minor acts of violence against property might have been included in
dures used; see below) is available from the authors. the violent group.
Overlapping studies and data sets. When two or more dissem- 3
Three disseminations by researchers at the Penetanguishene mental
inations examined the same sample and no unique odds ratios for health center (Harris et al., 1993; M. E. Rice & Harris, 1992, 1995), and
psychosis and violence could be calculated, the more methodolog- four disseminations by Krakowski and colleagues (Krakowski, & Czobor,
ically sound dissemination was included (e.g., peer-reviewed pub- 1997, 2004a, 2004b; Krakowski, Czobor, & Chou, 1999) were judged to
lications were selected rather than the dissertations on which they have samples in which the majority of participants overlapped between
were based; studies that provided more descriptive information studies.
4
about relevant moderator variables were selected). Studies that Participants in these studies were selected via case linkage from a
crime database and a psychiatric registrar in the Australian state of Vic-
clearly presented data on the same sample were coded as being
toria. Wallace et al. (1998; N ⫽ 1,044) linked persons convicted between
from the same data set. Studies also were coded as representing the
1993 and 1995 to the psychiatric register, whereas Wallace et al. (2004;
same data set if, in the opinions of the authors, the majority of the N ⫽ 2,861) studied individuals with criminal records who were diagnosed
participants in the samples were judged to overlap. Such judgment with schizophrenia during a first admission to a psychiatric hospital in
was exercised for two groups of studies.3 In one case, two dis- 1975, 1980, 1985, 1990, and 1995 and compared them with community
seminations (Wallace, Mullen, & Burgess, 2004; Wallace et al., residents. Thus, some overlap in the two samples was possible.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 685

indicate a negative association (i.e., decreased risk). Epidemiolo- an approximation to the standard weighting practices presented by
gists usually consider odds ratios of 2.0 –2.5 and higher to repre- Hedges and Vevea’s (1998) Equation 13 or DerSimonian and
sent clinically or practically meaningful associations (Fleiss, Wil- Laird’s (1986) Equation 3 but one that avoids using n_i where
liams, & Dubro, 1986); odds ratios of 3.0 or greater are considered possible and gives more weight to studies with larger samples but
to be large (Haddock et al., 1998). down-weights them appropriately to the extent that there is evi-
Whenever possible, we calculated or estimated multiple odds dence of true between-studies variability (DerSimonian & Laird,
ratios from a single study so that we could determine whether 1986; Hedges & Vevea, 1998). We considered down-weighting
various aspects of study design were related systematically to the extreme sample sizes important in the current analyses because
strength of the association between psychosis and violence (see several studies had population-sized Ns (i.e., 100,000 – 400,000),
below). The majority of studies either presented odds ratios or and we were concerned about giving them undue influence in
provided the necessary information to calculate them directly. analyses.7
When a study presented both adjusted and unadjusted odds ratios,
we coded both but used the unadjusted value in the main analyses.5 Choice of Statistical Tests and Analyses
We opted for unadjusted effect sizes because the majority of
studies reported only such effects. We report the differences be- Our first goal was to characterize the association between psy-
tween adjusted and unadjusted effects in the Results section. When chosis and violence in overall terms across all studies, so the
a study only provided other types of effect size—such as r, d, or primary data analytic strategy was descriptive in nature. Because
␹2— odds ratios were estimated by converting the effect size odds ratios are not normally distributed, we used nonparametric
provided with formulas presented in Lipsey and Wilson (2001) and measures of central tendency and dispersion—the median (Mdn)
Rosenthal (1991). We converted rate ratios to odds ratios using the and interquartile range (IQR), respectively—to describe the distri-
formula detailed in Zocchetti, Consonni, and Bertazzi (1997).
Unstandardized beta coefficients from logistic regression analyses 5
Forty-seven studies presented only adjusted odds ratios, which there-
were converted to odds ratios with eb and were exponentiated if fore were used in the main analyses. An adjusted odds ratio takes into
necessary. The Appendix presents all transformational formulas account the influence of potential covariates (e.g., age) specified by the
used. researcher on the association between the two variables of interest that are
Most odds ratios (k ⫽ 446; 50.4%) were calculated directly from used to form the odds ratio (i.e., psychosis and violence).
frequency counts provided in the studies of the number of people 6
By using the natural logarithm of the odds ratios, ln(ORs), weights
who were violent or not violent, as a function of having or not were applied that were proportional to 1/(U ⫹ Vi), where U ⫽ the variance
having psychosis. The remaining odds ratios were reported in the of the true ln(ORs) in the universe of studies (i.e., tau squared, which
study (k ⫽ 158; 17.9%) or estimated from formulas given in the represents the random effects variance component) and Vi ⫽ the sampling
Appendix (k ⫽ 281; 31.7%). Seven of these latter studies reported variance of the ln(ORs) for study i. The lower bound, 16/ni, was substituted
for Vi, which is the smallest value that Vi can have. This lower bound
a mean rating or the mean number of violent acts committed by
occurs when the four cells are the same, n/4: v_min ⫽ 1/(n/4) ⫹ 1/(n/4) ⫹
people with and without psychosis. For those studies, a Cohen’s d
1/(n/4) ⫹ 1/(n/4) ⫽ 16/n. U was estimated as U ⫽ {[⌺(Li – L)2]/(k ⫺ 1)} ⫺
value was calculated and then converted to an odds ratio. Simi- {[16 ⫻ ⌺(1/ni)]/k}, where ni ⫽ the sample size for study i; Li ⫽ the
larly, when a study presented mean scores for violent and nonvi- ln(ORs) for study i; L ⫽ the unweighted average ln(ORs); and k ⫽ the
olent participants on scales or items of measures of psychosis, a number of studies. The weight for study i was proportional to wi ⫽ 1/(u ⫹
Cohen’s d value was calculated and then converted to an odds 16/ni) ⫽ ni /[(ni ⫻ (u ⫹ 16)].
ratio. 7
Because the inverse variance weighting method often is preferred, we
In total, then, 885 effect sizes were calculated or estimated from also calculated inverse variance weights for as many effect sizes as pos-
204 studies of 166 independent data sets. (Studies included in the sible so that we could compare weighting methods. We were able to
meta-analysis are marked with an asterisk in the Reference sec- calculate inverse variance weights for 79.5% of effect sizes. We compared
tion.) Coding of the odds ratios and moderator variables (described the mean weighted logged odds ratios for this 79.5% with the mean
below) were based on consensus ratings of the first two authors. weighted logged odds ratios using the method we ultimately used for
analyses, described in the text and Footnote 6. The inverse variance
weighting method produced somewhat larger logged odds ratios (M ⫽
Weighting .628, 95% CI ⫽ .468 –.788) compared with the method we adopted (M ⫽
.378, 95% CI ⫽ .294 –.463). The back-transformed raw odds ratios were
Because an effect size estimate obtained from a large sample is 1.87 and 1.46, respectively. We also compared the two methods using the
assumed to be a more precise estimate of the population of effect entire sample of 885 effect sizes (though weights for 181 effect sizes
sizes in comparison with one based on a relatively small sample, necessarily were computed with our original approach, as the inverse
effect sizes typically are weighted such that those supplied by variance was not calculable). Using the inverse variance weighting method,
larger samples make a relatively more substantial contribution. we obtained a mean log odds ratio of .637, with a 95% CI of .510 –.765.
Optimally, effect sizes should be weighted by their inverse vari- Using the method we adopted, we obtained a mean log odds ratio of .441
ances (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Because of the number of effect with a 95% CI of .363–.520. In back-transforming, we obtained odds ratios
of 1.89 and 1.56, respectively. The findings that the inverse variance
sizes that needed to be estimated, it was impossible to use the
weighting method produced larger logged odds ratios than did our pre-
inverse variance to weight each effect size. Therefore, we ferred method are consistent with our concern that the inverse weighting
weighted all 885 odds ratios using weights that were proportional method might give undue influence to extreme sample sizes (e.g.,
to the inverse of the sum of the variance of the true log odds ratios 100,000⫹), some of which were present amongst studies that also had
in the universe of studies and the sampling variance of the log odds large odds ratios (i.e., epidemiological studies; studies of community
ratio for each study.6 This procedure—a random effects model—is residents rather than persons in institutions).
686 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

bution of odds ratios, although we report the mean and the coded for whether the disorder was an internalizing, externalizing,
weighted mean as well. Possible values of odds ratios range from organic/cognitive, or other disorder.
0 to ⬁, with 1 representing chance levels of association (negative Definition and measurement of psychosis. We coded four vari-
relationships are indicated by values between 0 and 1, and positive ables related to the definition and measurement of psychosis. The
relationships are represented by values between 1 and ⬁). We nature of the psychotic group was coded as type of psychosis. We
then performed parametric analyses to examine the distribution differentiated among studies that examined schizophrenia, affec-
of the natural logarithms of the odds ratios (log odds), tive psychoses, mixed or unspecified psychotic disorders, and
ESLOR ⫽ ln共OR兲. Log odds are distributed more normally than presence versus absence of psychotic symptoms. We parsed type of
their corresponding raw odds ratios. The theoretical range of log psychotic symptoms into the following categories: positive, nega-
odds is ⫺⬁ to ⬁, with 0 indicating chance association (negative tive, disorganized, and other or unspecified. We also coded
relationship ⬍0 and positive relationship ⬎0). Results were then whether positive symptoms comprised hallucinations and delu-
converted back into odds ratios via the inverse natural log function, sions versus other positive symptoms, such as grandiosity and
OR ⫽ eESLOR. To ensure that the overall findings were represen- suspiciousness. Symptoms that were of the threat/control-override
tative, we repeated the descriptive analyses after controlling for type also were coded. A threat/control-override psychotic symp-
multiple effect sizes per study and per data set. The latter controls tom is one that involves either a threatening component, such as
were accomplished by replacing the multiple effect sizes per study delusions of persecution in which a person believes that others
or data set with a single value, namely, the median of effect sizes wish to harm him or her, or the delusional belief that outside forces
within that study or data set.8,9 For studies in which odds ratios control one’s mind, such as thought insertion or withdrawal (Link
were coded both for scales on a measure (e.g., BPRS positive
& Stueve, 1994). Purpose of assessment reflected whether diag-
symptoms scale) and for the individual items that comprised the
noses were made for clinical, research, or other/unspecified pur-
scale, only the odds ratio for the scale was included among the
poses. Although studies typically had not examined whether there
values considered in computing the median for the overall odds
were active symptoms of psychosis at the time of violence perpe-
ratio estimate (i.e., the “one per study or data set” odds ratio).
tration, we were able to code two variables that approximated this
We also aggregated weighted effect sizes using a meta-analysis
temporal relation. Timing of assessment was coded according to
macro for SPSS written by David B. Wilson (see Lipsey & Wilson,
whether psychosis was assessed before, at the time of, or after the
2001; available online at http://mason.gmu.edu/⬃dwilsonb/
ma.html). The macro calculates upper and lower 95% confidence occurrence of violence; in some studies, psychosis was assessed
intervals (CIs) and performs a homogeneity test with the Q statis- before and after the violence, and in other studies the timing was
tic. The Q statistic (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) is used to identify data unspecified.
sets that produce significant heterogeneity and thus could be Definition and measurement of violence. The final four vari-
construed as outliers. It is distributed as a chi-square with k ⫺ 1 ables pertained to the manner in which studies defined and mea-
degrees of freedom, where k is the number of data sets.
8
Of course, several analytic choices are available in meta-analysis. The
Moderators single odds ratio for each data set could have been computed in at least two
ways, both of which take into account the non-normal distribution of odds
Our goal in terms of moderators was to identify variables ratios. In one approach, the median value of a data set’s raw odds ratios
relating to method, disorder, violence, and demographics that could be taken and subsequently transformed into a log odds ratio. In the
might moderate the association between psychosis and violence, as other approach, each raw odds ratio in a data set first could be transformed
described in the introduction. We examined three categories of into a log odds ratio, and then the mean of those values could be computed.
methodological factors: study design, definition and measurement Calculations using both approaches were completed in determining the
of psychosis, and definition and measurement of violence. single independent effect size for each data set. Both approaches yielded
similar findings (r ⫽ .97, p ⬍ .01, across the 166 pairs of effect sizes). For
Study design. Study design comprised eight variables related
all analyses reported, we used the first of the two strategies described here.
to the basic design of the studies. Sample setting was coded 9
As described in more detail below, one of the moderator variables
according to whether studies included civil psychiatric patients,
coded was whether people with psychosis were compared with individuals
forensic psychiatric patients, correctional offenders, community without a mental illness or with individuals with another mental illness. For
residents, or a combination of participants from two or more of the studies in which odds ratios could be calculated for both comparison
four settings. The method used to select participants from within a groups, it typically was the case that more effect sizes could be calculated
given setting was coded as sampling procedure. This variable had for comparisons with other mental illness (e.g., separate effects could be
four levels, including random sampling, matching, nonrandom computed for psychosis versus depression, psychosis versus substance
sampling, and unspecified sampling procedure. The distinction abuse). To determine whether the computation of the overall association
between retrospective or cross-sectional, archival (or pseudo-) between psychosis and violence might have been biased in either direction
prospective, and true prospective studies was coded as method. by not considering this variable, we took an alternative approach to
Sample size was coded continuously. The gender of participants in computing a single effect size for each data set. Specifically, we also
computed the median of effect sizes within a study or data set by taking the
the sample was coded as men only, women only, or both men and
average of two values: (a) the median of effect sizes based on comparisons
women. Country of data collection was coded dichotomously as between persons with psychosis and with persons without a mental illness
United States or any other country. The nature of the comparison and (b) the median of effect sizes based on comparisons between persons
group was coded as no mental disorder or other mental disorder. with psychosis and with another mental illness. Results obtained with
Finally, for those studies in which the comparison group was this alternative approach yielded different values for 9 of 166 data sets
another mental disorder, mental disorder comparison group was (mean difference among the 9 pairs of odds ratios was 1.29).
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 687

sured violence. Setting of violence was coded according to whether that psychosis was reliably and significantly associated with an
studies examined violence that occurred in an institution, in the approximately 49% to 68% increase in the odds of violence
community, or both. Method of assessing violence had three levels: relative to the odds of violence in the absence of psychosis. This
official records (including direct observation), self-report (includ- may be interpreted as a small, though reliable effect, correspond-
ing interviews), or multiple methods (i.e., both official records and ing to a standardized mean difference score (d) of .24 to .32 (see
self-reports). Severity of violence contrasted studies that examined Haddock et al., 1998, p. 349).
severe violence (homicide, attempted homicide, sexual violence, Dispersion. As Tables 1 and 2 also indicate, there was con-
or assaults resulting in serious physical injury), less severe vio- siderable dispersion of odds ratios around the central tendency.
lence (assaults not resulting in serious physical injury and threats The IQR for all 885 odds ratios revealed that at least 25% of all of
of violence), and any or unspecified acts of violence. Finally, we the effect sizes were negative in magnitude (ⱕ0.73), indicating a
also coded violence comparison group, which detailed whether negative association between psychosis and violence. On the other
participants who committed violence were compared with partic- hand, about 25% of the odds ratios were large in magnitude
ipants who engaged in criminal/antisocial but nonviolent acts (e.g., (ⱖ3.30), including about 14% that were very large (ⱖ5.00). A
theft or fraud) or in no detected criminal/aggressive behavior at all. similar pattern emerged when controlling for multiple effects per
Using a nonparametric test of the differences between medians study and per data set.
(␹2), we determined whether the magnitude of observed (un- The distribution of log odds closely approximated a normal
weighted) odds ratios varied significantly as a function of the curve. We evaluated the heterogeneity of the 166 independent
methodological factors described above. We also aggregated effect sizes (one effect size per sample) by calculating Q, follow-
weighted effect sizes using the SPSS macro written by David B.
ing Shadish and Haddock (1994, p. 266). Q (169.17) was not
Wilson, a meta-analytic analogue to the one-way analysis of vari-
significant. Although these findings suggest that the distribution of
ance (ANOVA), to investigate each moderator variable.
effect sizes is homogeneous, the observed variability did, in fact,
exceed expectation but merely not to such a degree as to be
Results statistically significant at p ⬍ .05. More specifically, because the
Association Between Psychosis and Violence expected value of a chi-square is equal to its degrees of freedom,
and the observed Q value of 169.17 is larger than its 166 associated
Central tendency. Table 1 summarizes the central tendency degrees of freedom, the observed variability exceeds what would
(mean, median) and distribution (standard deviation, 95% confi- be expected by chance (see Lipsey & Wilson, p. 135). These
dence interval, interquartile range, standard error) of unweighted analyses indicate that, notwithstanding the nonsignificance of Q,
raw and log odds ratios across (a) all 885 effect sizes, (b) one effect there was considerable variability with respect to the magnitude of
size per study, and (c) one effect size per data set. The raw and effect sizes. This supports the search for factors that potentially
logged odds ratios for the analyses evaluating one effect size per moderate the psychosis–violence association. This is also consis-
study were statistically significant, indicating a very small proba- tent with our observation that 25% of the effect sizes were nega-
bility that we would have observed these effect sizes if the popu- tive, indicating an inverse association between psychosis and vi-
lation effect sizes had been zero ( p ⬍ .0000001).
olence, and 25% were positive and large (ⱖ3), indicating a strong
The mean raw odds ratios were highly negatively skewed in part
association between psychosis and violence.
because of the natural distribution of odds ratios. That is, very high
odds ratios can influence the mean more so than can very low odds
ratios. As such, although we present them for descriptive purposes, Potentially Moderating Variables Between Psychosis
we consider the median raw odds and both the mean and median and Violence
log odds ratios to be more accurate depictions of central tendency.
Analyses of the (natural) log-transformed odds ratios indicated Publication bias. Meta-analysts have acknowledged that there
that their distribution was quasi-normal, as the stem-and-leaf plot may exist a publication bias that favors positive findings. This
in Table 2 indicates. Back-transformation of the mean and median means that the effect sizes reported in the published scientific
log odds ratio in Table 1 yielded values ranging from 1.49 to 1.68, literature may systematically overestimate the true magnitude of
which is highly consistent with the reported median raw odds the effect. This is often referred to as the “file drawer” problem, as
ratios. The consistency of findings across these analyses indicates numerous studies with null results— especially studies based on

Table 1
Association Between Psychosis and Violence: Distribution of Nonweighted Effect Sizes (Odds Ratios and Natural Log Odds)

Odds ratio Natural log odds

Effect size k M 95% CI SD Mdn Q1 to Q3 M (⫾SE) 95% CI SD Mdn Q1 to Q3

All effect sizes 885 3.50 2.53 to 4.46 14.59 1.53 0.73 to 3.33 .42 (.04) .34 to .50 1.15 0.43 ⫺0.32 to 1.20
One effect size per study 204 2.62 2.20 to 3.03 2.99 1.69 0.81 to 3.36 .51 (.07) .37 to .64 0.97 0.52 ⫺0.21 to 1.21
One effect size per data set 166 2.17 1.85 to 2.50 2.11 1.66 0.79 to 3.09 .40 (.07) .26 to .54 0.91 0.51 ⫺0.24 to 1.13

Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; Q1 ⫽ first quartile; Q3 ⫽ third quartile.


688 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

Table 2
Association Between Psychosis and Violence: Stem-and-Leaf
Plot of Distribution of Effect Sizes (Natural Log Odds)

Frequency Stem Leaf

5 Extremes ⬍⫺2.8
9 ⫺2.0 4&
28 ⫺1.0 556789
54 ⫺1.0 000112223334
87 ⫺0.0 555556666777778888899
130 ⫺0.0 000000111111222222223333334444
151 0.0 0000001111111122222222223333334444444
150 0.0 5555555566666666677777777788888999999
126 1.0 0000111111112222222333333344444
80 1.0 5555666667788889999
38 2.0 001112234
12 2.0 57&
11 3.0 03& Figure 1. Trim-and-fill funnel plot. s.e. ⫽ standard error.
4 Extremes ⬎3.7

Note. N ⫽ 885 effect sizes; stem width ⫽ 1.00; each leaf ⫽ 4 cases;
& ⫽ fractional leaf.
represents the odds ratio. This is not meaningfully different than
the unadjusted ln(OR) of .43. On the basis of these results, we do
not believe that publication bias was operative in the current study.
small samples—may go unpublished and be relegated to the file
drawer. Moderating Effects: Study Design
Logically, bias in favor of positive findings is unlikely to plague
this area of research as much as it might affect other areas of For all moderator analyses, we tested the difference among
research because many, if not most, studies have investigated levels of moderators using weighted log odds and the Q statistic,
numerous possible predictors of violence. Hence, if psychosis was which provides an ANOVA analog. However, the odds ratios
not significant, but other predictors were, the paper, in theory, presented in Tables 3–17 are raw odds (as opposed to log odds), to
would not be more likely to be rejected for publication than studies facilitate interpretation. We note that nonparametric significance
finding a positive effect for psychosis. In addition, on the basis of tests (median tests) of the raw odds ratios produced the same
our narrative review at the outset of this article, we were much less findings, in terms of which moderators were significant.
interested in the overall effect size than we were in moderated Setting. As Table 3 indicates, most studies were of civil psy-
effect sizes, which promise to be much more meaningful. chiatric patients. The difference among the effect sizes across the
To help minimize any potential publication bias, we searched four settings was statistically significant, with the highest odds
and included unpublished research in the form of dissertations. We ratios obtained in community (i.e., general population) samples.
located only six dissertations that were not subsequently published Odds ratios were significantly greater than chance among civil
as journal articles. The mean log odds for effect sizes estimated psychiatric, correctional, and community samples, indicating that
from unpublished dissertations was 0.90, as compared with 1.68 persons with psychosis in those samples were more likely to be
for published articles. Although the odds ratio for dissertations was violent than persons without psychosis in those samples.
smaller, the difference was not statistically significant, ␹2(1,
166) ⫽ 0.69, p ⫽ .41.
We also investigated possible publication bias by conducting Table 3
trim-and-fill analyses (Duval & Tweedie, 2000). This procedure is Moderating Effect of Setting
a quantitative form of the more traditional funnel graph, a scatter- Median odds
plot of the association between sample size and effect size. The Setting k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
“filled” trim-and-fill funnel graph is presented in Figure 1. The
figure includes a reference line for the overall mean log odds effect Civil psychiatricⴱⴱ 86 1.69b 0.88 to 2.80
Forensic psychiatric 38 0.91c 0.43 to 3.07
size as well as 95% confidence intervals. Visual inspection does Correctionalⴱ 21 1.27b,c 0.89 to 2.87
not reveal evidence of publication bias. Communityⴱⴱ 15 3.46a 2.21 to 5.50
We used the metatrim procedure in Stata 10 (StataCorp LP, Mixed 9 1.30b,c 0.58 to 2.30
2007) with the linear estimator (the most stable estimator). This
procedure indicated that there were 0 effect sizes to trim and that Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
the adjusted effect size was identical to the raw effect size. Using QB(4) ⫽ 16.70, p ⫽ .0022. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts
the run estimator, we obtained a “number to trim” result of 2, differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-
meaning that there may be two missing studies with large, negative hand column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log
effect sizes (see funnel plot, which shows the imputed effects odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
Tau squared∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .41 for civil psychiatric; u ⫽
surrounded by large squares). The adjusted effect size obtained 1.01 for forensic psychiatric; u ⫽ .46 for correctional; u ⫽ .67 for
with a random effects model and the run estimator was ln(OR) ⫽ community; and u ⫽ 1.12 for mixed.
0.427, z ⫽ 10.35, p ⬍ .001, 95% CI ⫽ 0.347– 0.508, where OR ⴱ
p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 689

Sampling procedure. Most studies used some form of random Table 5


sampling (see Table 4), although a fair number did not specify Moderating Effect of Study Design
their sampling procedure. Odds ratios differed significantly as a
function of sampling procedure, ranging from a low of 1.49 for Median odds
Design k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
simple random sampling to a high of 5.50 for matching (mostly
epidemiological studies), a procedure designed to control for pos- ⴱⴱⴱ
Retrospective 123 1.88a 0.81 to 3.10
sible confounding factors. Odds ratios were significant at each Archival (pseudo-)prospective 18 1.55a 0.62 to 3.89
level of the moderator except for nonrandom sampling, although True prospectiveⴱⴱ 37 1.49a 0.83 to 2.96
this effect was based on only seven studies.
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
Design. Design was not a statistically significant moderating quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
factor, and odds ratios were homogeneous across types of design QB(2) ⫽ 1.06, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ
(see Table 5). Odds ratios were significant only for retrospective significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand
and true prospective designs, although the number of pseudopro- column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
spective studies (k ⫽ 18) was small and, hence, power was limited Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .71 for retrospective; u ⫽
within this level of the variable. 1.36 for archival (pseudo-)prospective; and u ⫽ .54 for true prospective.
ⴱⴱ
Sample size. We tested whether sample size influenced ob- p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
served effect sizes by treating it as a continuous variable in a
meta-analytic analog to regression analysis (MetaReg macro in
with internalizing disorders (nonpsychotic mood disorders, anxiety
SPSS; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). The model was not significant
disorders) than when compared with externalizing disorders (Clus-
(Q ⫽ 0.88, ns). The unstandardized beta for sample size was 0.00.
ter B personality disorders, substance-related disorders). In fact,
Thus, sample size appears not to have influenced effect size, a
the effect size for psychosis was not significantly different from
finding that is consistent with our trim-and-fill analyses reported
chance when the comparison group comprised externalizing dis-
above.
orders.
Gender. Gender did not moderate effect size. Studies that used
We also tested the relationship between psychosis and violence
only men, only women, or both men and women as participants did
when the comparison group was limited to personality disorder
not produce effect sizes that differed significantly from one an-
(k ⫽ 34). For approximately half of the 34 data sets for which we
other. Effect sizes were significantly different from chance at each
could parse out this subgroup, the nature of the personality disor-
level of this moderator (see Table 6).
der was not specified; for those in which it was specified, the group
Country of data collection. Effect sizes did not differ between
consisted of antisocial or borderline personality disorders or psy-
U.S. and non-U.S. samples and were significantly different from
chopathic personality disorder. The median odds ratio for this
chance at both levels of this moderator (see Table 7).
subset of analyses was 0.59, and the mean log odds was ⫺0.38
Comparison group. Effect size varied significantly as a func-
(SD ⫽ 1.00), which was a statistically significant inverse effect
tion of the nature of the comparison group (see Table 8). As might
(Z ⫽ ⫺2.06, p ⬍ .05). That is, psychosis was associated with
be expected, odds ratios were higher when individuals with psy-
significantly lower odds for violence than were these personality
chosis were compared with persons without mental disorder
disorders.
(Mdn ⫽ 3.68) than when compared with individuals with other
In addition, we tested the impact of substance-related disorders
mental disorders (Mdn ⫽ 1.51). However, odds ratios were sig-
on the psychosis–violence association. The cleanest comparisons
nificantly greater than chance at both levels of the moderator.
we could make were from studies that divided their samples into
We further broke down the nature of the comparison group, as
participants with psychosis who either did or did not have a
shown in Table 9. Psychosis had a stronger effect when compared
comorbid substance-related disorder. We located 12 independent
data sets that permitted this comparison. The mean log odds for
Table 4
Moderating Effect of Sampling Procedure
Table 6
Median odds Moderating Effect of Gender
Sampling procedure k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
Median odds
ⴱⴱ
Random 125 1.49b 0.65 to 2.64 Gender group k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
Nonrandom 7 1.84b 1.40 to 3.10
Matched to populationⴱⴱ 17 5.50a 3.33 to 7.85 Menⴱⴱ 56 1.37a 0.76 to 3.22
Not specifiedⴱⴱ 20 1.69b 0.91 to 2.94 Womenⴱⴱ 21 1.73a 0.92 to 4.98
Men and womenⴱⴱⴱ 109 1.76a 0.79 to 2.83
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
QB(3) ⫽ 64.99, p ⫽ .0001. Median odds ratios that do not share super- quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
scripts differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the QB(2) ⫽ 2.00, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share superscripts differ
left-hand column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand
log odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds
z-test. Tau squared∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .62 for random; u ⫽ ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
.71 for nonrandom; u ⫽ .40 for matched to population; and u ⫽ .14 for not Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .87 for men; u ⫽ 1.43 for
specified. women; and u ⫽ .71 for men and women.
ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001. p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
690 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

Table 7 Table 9
Moderating Effect of Country of Data Collection Moderating Effect of Type of Mental Disorder Comparison
Group
Median odds
Country k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3) Comparison Median odds
ⴱⴱⴱ
group k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
United States 82 1.72a 0.81 to 2.75
Not United Statesⴱⴱⴱ 83 1.56a 0.73 to 3.47 Internalizingⴱ
27 2.15a 0.76 to 4.17
Externalizing 47 0.85b 0.41 to 2.00
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first Organic 16 1.41a,b 0.96 to 2.62
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall Otherⴱⴱⴱ 123 1.69a 0.96 to 2.80
QB(1) ⫽ 0.35, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ
significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log odds quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. QB (3) ⫽ 14.90, p ⫽ .0019. Median odds ratios that do not share super-
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .57 for United States, and u ⫽ scripts differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the
.76 for not United States. left column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001. ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test:
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ 1.29 for internalizing; u ⫽ .79
for externalizing; u ⫽ .46 for organic; and u ⫽ .47 for other.
noncomorbid psychosis was 1.54 (SD ⫽ 0.55), and that for co- ⴱ
p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
morbid psychosis was 2.37 (SD ⫽ 1.16). The difference between
these effect sizes was large (Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.97). The raw odds
ratios associated with the log odds are 4.67 and 10.70, indicating violence association (see Table 10). That is, studies that coded at
that these 12 studies had odds ratios that were larger than the the level of the symptom had significantly higher effect sizes than
average odds across all studies. those that either failed to specify how they defined psychosis or
Finally, we tested the difference between adjusted and unad- combined different types of psychotic disorders into one category.
justed effect sizes reported in studies, with the rationale that The effect sizes for both the symptom and the schizophrenia level
adjusted effect sizes control for various other potential risk factors of analysis were significantly greater than chance.
for violence. We did so with some caution, as it is not clear which We looked in more detail at the potential impact of type of
variables, in fact, should be controlled statistically (Dembling et symptom on the strength of the association between psychosis and
al., 2002; Hudson, 2005), in that some covariates, such as SES, violence. As shown in Table 11, the overall moderating effect of
might actually themselves be consequences of mental illness rather type of symptom (positive, negative, disorganized, other/
than truly competing causes of violence. Further, different studies unspecified) showed a trend toward significance. Pairwise com-
controlled for different factors. Nonetheless, we located 59 pairs of parisons revealed that positive symptoms were associated with a
adjusted and unadjusted odds ratios. The mean (SD) unadjusted larger odds ratio (2.32) than were negative symptoms (1.32).
odds ratio was 5.11 (4.95), and the mean adjusted odds ratio was However, effect sizes at all levels of this moderator were signifi-
3.22 (3.06), which represented a significant difference, t(58) ⫽ cantly greater than chance.
3.98, p ⬍ .001. Median odds were 3.50 and 2.21, respectively. The We evaluated whether more specific categories of positive
standardized difference between means was moderate (d ⫽ 0.47). symptoms were associated with violence. Of the data sets, 37
permitted a test of whether hallucinations and/or delusions were
Moderating Effects: Definition and Measurement associated with violence, 25 permitted a test of other positive
of Psychosis symptoms (e.g., bizarre behavior, excitement, suspiciousness, non-

Type of psychosis. The manner in which psychosis was con-


ceptualized in studies significantly moderated the psychosis– Table 10
Moderating Effect of Type of Psychosis
Table 8 Median odds
Moderating Effect of Type of Comparison Group Type of psychosis k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
ⴱⴱⴱ
Median odds Schizophrenia 68 1.71a,b 0.61 to 3.83
Comparison group k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3) Affective psychoses 22 1.36a,b 0.66 to 3.27
Mixed/unspecified psychoses 62 1.25a 0.62 to 2.85
Other mental disorderⴱⴱⴱ 149 1.51a 0.73 to 2.59 Psychotic symptomsⴱⴱⴱ 77 2.07b 1.29 to 3.17
No mental disorderⴱⴱⴱ 29 3.68b 0.98 to 6.20
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall QB(3) ⫽ 9.28, p ⫽ .026. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts
QB(1) ⫽ 13.24, p ⫽ .0003. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-
differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left- hand column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log
hand column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ 1.10 for schizophrenia; u ⫽
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .55 for other mental disorder, .64 for affective psychoses; u ⫽ .98 for mixed/unspecified psychoses; and
and u ⫽ 1.13 for no mental disorder. u ⫽ .26 for psychotic symptoms.
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001. p ⬍ .001.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 691

Table 11 Timing of assessment. Overall, timing of assessment ap-


Moderating Effect of Type of Psychotic Symptom proached significance as a moderating factor (Table 13). The only
significant pairwise difference occurred between assessments of
Type of psychotic Median odds psychosis made before the measurement of violence and studies
symptom k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
that did not specify the relative timing of assessment. It is notable
ⴱⴱⴱ that the largest effect size was that for studies in which the
Positive 62 2.32a 1.23 to 3.46
Negativeⴱ 22 1.32b 0.87 to 2.05 assessment of psychosis occurred at approximately the same time
Disorganizedⴱ 20 1.85a,b 1.20 to 3.06 as the violent incident. It likely did not differ from the other
Other/unspecifiedⴱⴱ 22 1.78a,b 1.23 to 3.42
categories because there were few studies (k ⫽ 10) that fell into
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first this category. When all effect sizes were used, this odds ratio was
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall significantly greater than those in the “before” or “after” catego-
QB(3) ⫽ 6.88, p ⫽ .076. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts ries.
differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left
column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. Moderating Effects: Definition and Measurement
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .43 for positive psychotic of Violence
symptoms; u ⫽ .25 for negative psychotic symptoms; u ⫽ .46 for disor-
ganized psychotic symptoms; and u ⫽ .51 for other/unspecified psychotic Setting of violence. Setting was not a significant moderator.
symptoms. As Table 14 indicates, the strength of association between psy-

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
chosis and violence did not differ as a function of whether violence
was measured within an institution (i.e., psychiatric hospital) or in
the community, and effect sizes were significantly greater than
delusional or nonhallucinatory paranoia) in relation to violence, 16
chance at both levels of this moderator.
allowed us to assess the relationship between some type of para-
Method of violence detection. The method of violence detec-
noid symptom and violence, and 18 enabled us to examine threat/
tion did not significantly moderate effect sizes (see Table 15), and
control-override symptoms in relation to violence.
there were no pairwise differences between levels. Effects were
The median odds ratio for hallucinations/delusions was 2.31,
significant when either official records or self-report was used to
with a mean log odds of .66 (SD ⫽ .91), which was significantly
measure violence, but not when a combination of these methods
different from chance (Z ⫽ 4.32, p ⬍ .0001). For other positive
was used.
symptoms, the median was 2.37, and the mean log odds was 0.87
Severity of violence. Severity of violence was not a significant
(SD ⫽ .61), Z ⫽ 6.75, p ⬍ .0001. These types of symptoms did not
moderator (Table 16). Psychosis was related to violence whether it
differ from one another. Paranoid symptoms showed a trend to-
was severe, moderate, or not specified.
ward significance: median odds ⫽ 1.11; mean log odds ⫽ 0.30
Violence comparison group. As shown in Table 17, the
(SD ⫽ 0.73), Z ⫽ 1.81, p ⫽ .07. Threat/control-override symptoms
psychosis–violence relationship was stronger when the compari-
were significantly different than chance: median odds ⫽ 1.92;
son of violent individuals was made to individuals without anti-
mean log odds ⫽ 0.68 (SD ⫽ 0.98), Z ⫽ 3.09, p ⫽ .002.
social behaviors, as opposed to nonviolent but still antisocial/
Method of assessment. As shown in Table 12, whether eval-
criminal incidents. When the comparison was to nonviolent,
uations of psychosis occurred in the context of clinical practice,
though antisocial conduct, the odds ratio was not different from
took place in the context of research, or were unspecified did not
chance.
moderate the association between psychosis and violence. In the
vast majority of studies, either the purpose of evaluations was
specified or it could be inferred. Odds ratios were significant for
Table 13
both of these types of evaluation.
Moderating Effect of Timing of Assessment

Median odds
Table 12 Timing of diagnosis k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
Moderating Effect of Purpose of Assessment
Assessment made before
Diagnostic Median odds violence 51 1.30a 0.62 to 2.40
method k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3) Assessment made at violenceⴱ 10 2.51a,b 0.78 to 4.43
Assessment made after
Clinicalⴱⴱ 79 1.51a 0.62 to 3.09 violenceⴱⴱⴱ 84 1.57a,b 0.76 to 2.30
Researchⴱⴱⴱ 87 1.88a 0.94 to 3.15 Unspecifiedⴱⴱⴱ 36 2.22b 1.01 to 3.91
Unspecified/other 6 1.27a 0.91 to 2.27
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall QB(3) ⫽ 6.89, p ⫽ .076. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts
QB(2) ⫽ 1.87, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left
significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds
column marked with designated symbols have weighted mean log odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .72 for assessment made
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ 1.08 for clinical; u ⫽ .43 for before violence; u ⫽ .64 for assessment made at violence; u ⫽ .59 for
research; and u ⫽ .36 for unspecified/other. assessment made after violence; and u ⫽ .77 for unspecified.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. ⴱ
p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
692 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

Table 14 Table 16
Moderating Effect of Setting in Which Violence Occurred Moderating Effect of Severity of Violence

Setting in which violence Median odds Median odds


occurred k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3) Severity of violence k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
Institutional 47 1.84a 1.00 to 3.09 Severe 52 1.79a 0.64 to 3.54
Communityⴱⴱⴱ 117 1.59a 0.73 to 3.08 Moderateⴱⴱⴱ 72 1.86a 0.88 to 2.97
Unspecified/otherⴱⴱⴱ 67 1.45a 0.73 to 2.83
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes in analysis; IQR ⫽ interquartile range;
QB(1) ⫽ 0.36, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ Q1 ⫽ first quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile).
significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand Overall QB(2) ⫽ 0.79, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts
column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log odds differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. hand column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .57 for institutional, and u ⫽ odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
.69 for community. Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .96 for severe; u ⫽ .54 for
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001. moderate; and u ⫽ .79 for unspecified/other.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.

To gain a better sense of how the strength of association be-


tween psychosis and violence varied as a function of the number of psychosis and violence themselves. Across 885 effect sizes from
significant moderators present, we plotted the mean log odds ratios 204 studies of 166 independent data sets, psychosis was reliably
as a function of the number of moderators present in a given study. associated with a 49%– 68% increased likelihood of violence. We
The result is shown in Figure 2 and illustrates that with no observed substantial variability as a function of the moderators we
moderators present, log odds ratios were below chance levels. tested. In fact, perhaps of more importance than the overall effect
With one moderator present, log odds ratios were small and lower size estimates were the moderator analyses, which shed light on
than the sample mean. With two to four moderators present, the the discrepant findings and professional opinions discussed in the
log odds ratios were larger than the sample mean. Translating from introduction. That is, depending on the number of moderators
the log odds shown in the data labels in Figure 2, we obtained raw present in a given study, odds ratios ranged from a meaningful
odds ratios corresponding to the number of moderators as follows: inverse association to a large positive association. Given this range
no moderators, OR ⫽ 0.81; one moderator, OR ⫽ 1.09; two in effect sizes, the range in conclusions about psychosis and
moderators, OR ⫽ 1.83; three moderators, OR ⫽ 3.66; four mod- violence makes complete sense.
erators, OR ⫽ 4.56. These data illustrate quite clearly why such
divergent conclusions have been drawn about the association be- The Status of Psychosis as a Risk Factor for Violence
tween psychosis and violence. Putting aside the issue of moderation for the moment, one might
ask: How does psychosis compare with other risk factors for
Discussion violence in terms of strength of association? It is difficult to
The findings of our meta-analysis provided strong support for interpret the effect size for the psychosis–violence association in
the view that psychosis and violence are associated with one relation to other risk factors because of a lack of appropriate
another, albeit with a small overall effect size that varies consid- comparison data. Only some of the studies included in our meta-
erably and in important ways across study features and aspects of analysis permitted us to examine systematically the effect size
associated with other diagnoses of mental disorder, demographic
characteristics, or criminal history variables. Furthermore, there
Table 15 are no other meta-analyses of which we are aware that have
Moderating Effect of Method of Detecting Violence

Median Table 17
odds IQR Moderating Effect of Violence Comparison Group
Method of detecting violence k ratio (Q1 to Q3)
Median odds
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
Official records (including observation) 114 1.66a 0.79 to 3.09 Comparison group k ratio IQR (Q1 to Q3)
Self-reports (including interview)ⴱⴱⴱⴱ 38 2.02a 1.25 to 3.02
Mixed/not specified 18 0.80a 0.60 to 1.78 Nonviolent antisocial incident 42 1.07a 0.64 to 1.88
No incidentⴱⴱⴱ 119 1.92b 0.80 to 3.15
Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall Note. k ⫽ number of effect sizes; IQR ⫽ interquartile range; Q1 ⫽ first
QB(2) ⫽ 4.18, ns. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts differ quartile (25th percentile); Q3 ⫽ third quartile (75th percentile). Overall
significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-hand QB(1) ⫽ 8.13, p ⫽ .0043. Median odds ratios that do not share subscripts
column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log odds differ significantly from one another at p ⱕ .05. Moderators in the left-
ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test. hand column marked with the designated symbol have weighted mean log
Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .66 for official records odds ratios that differed significantly from chance, as indicated by a z-test.
(including observation); u ⫽ .72 for self-reports (including interview); and Tau-square∧ for each level is as follows: u ⫽ .55 for nonviolent antisocial
u ⫽ .50 for mixed/not specified. incident, and u ⫽ .72 for no incident.
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001. p ⬍ .001.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 693

Figure 2. Effect of number of significant moderators on size of log odds ratio.

summarized the effect size of a violence risk factor across multiple larger than, numerous other risk factors, though tends to be smaller
settings. Some meta-analyses have examined a range of risk fac- than externalizing disorders, such as antisocial personality or psy-
tors in a specific setting (e.g., mentally disordered offenders, as per chopathy, or early-onset criminal behavior. Our finding that psy-
Bonta et al., 1998) or a specific risk factor in a few settings (e.g., chosis is associated with violence is consistent with that of previ-
psychopathy in correctional and forensic psychiatric settings, as ous narrative reviews (e.g., Bradford, 1983; De Pauw & Szulecka,
per Guy, Edens, Anthony, & Douglas, 2005; Hemphill, Hare, & 1988; Junginger, 1996; McNiel, 1994; Monahan, 1992b; Mulvey,
Wong, 1998; Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996). 1994; Taylor, 1995; Taylor et al., 1994; Wessely, 1993). However,
With this caveat in mind, it appears that the average effect size some readers may be surprised by our finding in light of the
for psychosis in the present meta-analysis (r ⫽ .12–.16, as derived meta-analysis of factors associated with recidivism among men-
from transformation from OR to r) is comparable to numerous tally disordered offenders described above (Bonta et al., 1998).
individual risk factors studied in the Bonta et al. (1998) meta- Bonta et al. reported that the association between psychosis and
analysis (most of which were estimated from fewer than 10 stud- violence was heterogeneous across studies, with the overall asso-
ies). For example, Bonta et al. obtained mean effect sizes (Zr) for ciation being small and negative in direction (Zr ⫽ ⫺.04). How-
the following predictors: history of violence (.16), marital status ever, it is important to recognize that Bonta et al.’s review was
(.13), weapon use (.12), nonviolent criminal history (.13), institu- limited to a small number of effect sizes dealing with psychosis
tional adjustment (.14), and hospital admissions (.17). Antisocial and violence (k ⫽ 11) from a specific type of study (longitudinal
personality (.18) and juvenile delinquency (.20) were only slightly studies of mentally disordered offenders released from correctional
more strongly associated with violence in Bonta et al. than was institutions).
psychosis in the current study. Several risk factors had smaller
effect sizes in Bonta et al. than psychosis had in the present study: The Importance of Moderators
substance use (.08), offense seriousness (.06), mood disorder (.01),
violent index offense (⫺.04), race (.09), intelligence (⫺.02), and Perhaps the most important finding to emerge from the present
education (⫺.02). meta-analysis is the extent to which the strength of association
The effect size for psychosis appears meaningfully smaller than between psychosis and violence differed as a function of moder-
that for psychopathy according to several meta-analyses that re- ator variables. Indeed, if one generalized from a certain group of
ported mean correlations typically in the .25⫺.30 range (Hemphill studies, the conclusion would be that psychosis is inversely related
et al., 1998; Salekin et al., 1996; Walters, 2003). Some meta- to violence. With another set of studies, the conclusion would be
analyses (Guy et al., 2005) have reported smaller effect sizes (rs ⫽ that psychosis is a very strong correlate of violence. To some
.11–.16) between psychopathy and violence in particular settings, extent, this systematic variation in strength of association, depicted
such as within prisons. clearly in Figure 2, should provide guidance for researchers in
In general, then, psychosis appears to be intermediate in com- terms of framing hypotheses about the anticipated relationship
parison with other putative risk factors. It is comparable to, or between psychosis and violence in any given study, depending on
694 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

the characteristics of the study (i.e., nature of comparison groups, Concerning the first point, we note that at an individual assess-
level of measurement). ment level, decisions must be made about a specific person, not
First, several potential moderators were not significant. As such, about groups of people. The presence of some comparison group
a reasonable inference is that the psychosis–violence relationship is of only peripheral relevance to a given individual’s risk. The
is relatively robust across levels of these moderators, including evidence from this meta-analysis suggests that psychosis be con-
gender, country, and severity of violence. sidered a possible risk factor at an individual level whenever it is
Design-related moderators. Of the study design or sampling present. For instance, upon release from a correctional institution,
variables, both sample type and sampling procedure moderated the mutually exclusive groups of persons with psychosis may be at
psychosis–violence relationship. In particular, studies with com- lower risk than groups of persons with primary externalizing
munity samples and studies using epidemiological designs pro- diagnoses. However, when released to the general population
duced larger effect sizes than did studies conducted in other (largely comprising people without any externalizing disorders),
settings and those using other designs. However, psychosis was individuals with psychosis may be considered to be at higher risk
still a significant predictor of violence within correctional and civil relative to the majority of people in the population. Viewed in this
psychiatric settings and in studies where random sampling was way, individuals with either psychosis or externalizing disorders
used. Study design was not a significant moderator, with both would be at elevated risk compared with their nondisordered
retrospective and prospective designs producing significant and community counterparts. Those with externalizing disorders
comparable effects. would just be at higher risk than those with psychotic disorders.
Another one of the stronger moderators came from the study Second, if analysis is at the level of the sample or the popula-
design category of potential moderators: the nature of the compar- tion, then the composition of the reference group clearly matters in
ison group. That is, to whom are people with psychosis being terms of hypothesis generation. Researchers should expect effect
compared? If the comparison was to individuals with other— sizes that range from negative (comparison with personality dis-
nonpsychotic—mental illnesses, the odds of violence were slightly order) to small (comparison with nonpsychotic mental disorder
higher for persons with psychosis (OR ⫽ 1.51). However, when broadly) to large (comparison with internalizing disorders or no
the comparison group consisted of individuals who had not been disorder at all), depending on the nature of the comparison group.
identified as having mental disorders, the odds ratio was substan- This could be an important issue in the context of a particular
tially higher, and large (OR ⫽ 3.68). Splitting up “other mental study. For instance, if a researcher were interested in predicting
illness” further, we found that comparing psychosis with internal- violence within a prison therapeutic community setting in which
izing disorders produced a meaningful effect size (OR ⫽ 2.15), most participants had antisocial personality disorder, it would be
whereas comparing it with externalizing disorders did not (OR ⫽ reasonable to predict that psychosis would have a minimal predic-
0.85). tive effect. If, however, a researcher were predicting violence
Although we were not able to conduct exactly the same type of within a community residential setting consisting of numerous
analyses with substance use disorders as we could for externalizing individuals with internalizing disorders, then psychosis might be
disorders more broadly, our findings indicated that comorbid psy- expected to play a more important predictive role.
chosis and substance-related diagnoses produced substantially Psychosis-related moderators. Turning to the measurement of
larger effect sizes than did psychosis alone (d ⫽ .97). This obser- psychosis as a potential moderator, we observe several noteworthy
vation is consistent with leading studies (Elbogen & Johnson, findings. The psychosis–violence association was significant when
2009; Monahan et al., 2001) pointing to the crucial role of sub- psychosis was measured as a diagnosis of schizophrenia or mea-
stance use problems in understanding risk for violence. sured at the level of the symptom. It was not significant for
There are three important points to consider with respect to the affective psychoses or when researchers either failed to specify
comparison group issue. First, these analyses shed some light on how they measured it or aggregated across all types of psychosis.
the relative ranking of categories of mental illness vis-à-vis their These findings argue against the practice in research of lumping
relationship to violence. Notably, externalizing disorders are likely individuals with psychoses into a single, gross category, a point
more strongly related to violence than are psychotic disorders. This that has been made cogently in the past (e.g., Monahan & Stead-
assertion is consistent with several meta-analyses showing mean man, 1994). At the level of the symptom, positive symptoms were
correlational effect sizes between psychopathy and violence in the significantly more strongly related to violence than were negative
.25 to .30 range (i.e., Hemphill et al., 1998; Salekin et al., 1996; symptoms, providing some support for causal models that empha-
Walters, 2003), although there are exceptions in which smaller size these types of symptoms (i.e., “psychotic action”; Junginger,
effect sizes have been reported (Guy et al., 2005). 1996). However, we also note that symptoms of disorganization
Second, the findings suggest that, compared with persons with- (and other/unspecified) symptoms predicted violence. It is impos-
out mental illness (or, presumably, any externalizing disorders), sible to comment on the other/unspecified category in that it is
psychosis is, in fact, a strong risk factor for violence, increasing the possible that it included positive symptoms. The positive finding
odds of violence three- to fourfold. This issue of comparison group for symptoms of disorganization would suggest that researchers
has important implications for the role that psychosis might play in should investigate whether such symptoms might play a role in
violence risk assessment. It is tempting to interpret these findings violence, perhaps by disrupting executive functioning, by compro-
such that psychosis should be considered a noncorrelate in samples mising the ability to remain composed, or by leading to tense or
consisting of many highly externalizing people. This is true, how- stressful interpersonal encounters (see Hiday, 1997, 2006). We
ever, only under two conditions: (a) that only group-level behavior also note that most studies did not report the effects of different
is of interest and (b) that there is no possibility that another types of symptoms while controlling for the others, which would
reference group would be of relevance for decisions. be an important step in future research.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 695

Much has been made in the psychosis–violence literature about encourage researchers to always report the time between diagnosis
the role of different types of positive symptoms: hallucinations, and violence.
delusions, paranoid symptoms, threat/control-override symptoms, Second, it is artificial to presume that studies capture, in any
and so forth. Our findings revealed few differences among these in systematic way, all of the violence that a person has perpetrated
terms of their relation to violence: Hallucinations and delusions, and either the first break of psychosis or the complex waxing and
threat/control-override symptoms, and other positive symptoms waning of psychotic symptoms across time. As such, most studies
were all associated with violence, and symptoms with some sort of represent some snapshot of the timing of psychosis and violence
paranoid element approached significance. Whereas some past vis-à-vis one another, a view that could change if the snapshot
studies (e.g., Link, Andrews, & Cullen, 1992; Link & Stueve, were taken at a different point in a person’s life or included a larger
1994) have reported that threat/control-override symptoms predict time frame. As such, one cannot presume that if psychosis were
violence more strongly than other symptoms, other studies have diagnosed after violence had occurred (say, for instance, that
failed to replicate this. For example, Appelbaum et al. (2000) psychosis were diagnosed in an individual in a psychiatric hospital
provided a detailed analysis of the association between threat/ and that it was clear from arrest records or the person’s self-report
control-override symptoms and violence and reported that the that she or he had been violent in the past), psychosis did not exist
association did not hold up when patients’ reports of putatively prior to the violence. Rather, the only conclusions that could be
delusional experiences were confirmed for true delusional status, made under such circumstances would be either that (a) psychosis
although subthreshold (non-delusional) perceptions similar in na- was not diagnosed or measured at that time or (b) studies failed to
ture to threat/control-override delusions were associated with past report or measure age of first onset of psychosis. Similarly, if
violence. Few studies in this meta-analysis provided such a de- psychosis were diagnosed today and a person was not detected as
tailed level of analysis. As such, our findings cannot address such having been violent in a 6-month follow-up period, it would not
fine, though important, distinctions. However, we argue that it is mean that psychosis and violence had not co-occurred for that
unlikely that a median odds ratio of 2.32 for positive symptoms is individual at some point or that these variables would not co-occur
generally artifactual across five dozen samples. The precise con- at some point in the future. In such a case, it could only be said that
tours and boundaries of the symptom–violence association will violence was not observed to occur in the 6 months after psychosis
was measured.
need to be addressed in further primary studies, drawing on inves-
Studies on mental illness and violence rarely document either
tigations such as Appelbaum et al. (2000) and models of
onset or course of illness (waxing and waning of symptoms) or
psychosis–violence such as those proposed by Hiday (1997); Jung-
violence across a person’s life, or whether these are systematically
inger (1996), or Taylor and colleagues (Taylor, 1998, 2006; Taylor
related to one another. Such a design is exactly what is needed,
& Hodgins, 1994).
however, to make firm conclusions about exactly how psychosis
Finally, with respect to the timing of the psychosis determina-
and violence may or may not be related. On the basis of the
tion vis-à-vis the occurrence of violence, the largest effect was for
preceding issues, we argue that because (a) symptom-level mea-
the small number of studies (k ⫽ 10) where diagnosis and violence
surement produced the largest odds and (b) close timing between
occurred at approximately the same time. This potential moderator
assessment of psychosis and measurement of violence produced
is muddied by the fact that 36 studies failed to specify the timing
the largest odds, there is some support for the view that risk for
of diagnosis vis-à-vis violence. In theory, if psychosis is a causal violence is particularly elevated when the course of psychosis is
risk factor, then its presence must precede the outcome of violence. active.
However, when psychosis was assessed prior to the measurement It is also important to note that the correlation between psycho-
of violence, it produced a small and nonsignificant odds ratio of sis and violence could, in part, stem from other forms of possible
1.30. Before concluding that this means that psychosis cannot, relationships between them. That is, a person may become psy-
therefore, be considered a causal risk factor for violence, we point chotic in the stressful context of being violent or in a transaction
out two very important limits to this general timing variable. that involves both violence and victimization. In such a scenario,
First, in addition to information about whether the diagnosis of it would not be appropriate to describe psychosis as a cause of that
psychosis occurred before or after violence, it is important to know particular violent act. A complicating factor to such a scenario,
how close in time the two occurred, particularly if it is active however, is that it is unlikely that psychosis would develop in
symptoms that are most crucial in the psychosis–violence associ- response to violence-related stress without there being an under-
ation. Although for most studies we were able to code whether lying psychotic disorder present, such as schizophrenia. In such a
diagnosis preceded or followed the occurrence of violence, we case, then, a psychotic disorder precedes the violence, which in
were not able to systematically determine how far apart diagnosis turn leads to decompensation involving onset of florid symptoms.
and violence were. That is, an assessment of psychosis in, say, Moreover, it is unlikely that acting violently would cause a disor-
1990, may have little relevance for a person’s behavior in 1997, der itself, such as schizophrenia, but more likely that it could
being separated by 7 years. This idea is consistent with calls in the exacerbate some of the symptoms of the disorder. We were not
literature to focus on the dynamic, changing nature of risk factors able to meta-analyze the literature at such a fine level, although we
and on the consideration that certain risk factors that are prone to again call for researchers to clearly indicate specific details about
change, such as psychotic symptoms, may only be of relevance to the timing and onset not only of active symptoms but also of the
violence when they are in some active or elevated state (see course of the disorder more generally, as well as how these
Douglas & Skeem, 2005; Skeem & Mulvey, 2002). As such, the variables relate to the occurrence of violent behavior. It is certainly
increased odds ratio associated with assessments of psychosis and possible that for some individuals, or for the same person at
violence that occurred close in time is meaningful. However, we different times, violence may cause psychotic decompensation. At
696 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

other times or for other people, psychosis may cause violence. The interrater reliability of, as well as the risk associated with, indi-
two possibilities are not mutually exclusive. vidual symptoms of psychosis. In addition, future research should
Violence-related moderators. In terms of violence-related control for the effects of comorbid mental disorders within both
moderators, strongest support was found for the outcome compar- psychotic and nonpsychotic samples statistically, by selection, or
ison group. That is, psychosis was not predictive of violence when by matching. Such control would enhance the precision of the
the outcome comparison was other antisocial (though nonviolent) estimate of risk for violence associated with psychosis and also
behavior, as opposed to no known antisocial behavior. Given the may identify important cofactors, such as substance use or person-
relatively high correlation between violent and nonviolent antiso- ality disorder. Finally, future research should examine which psy-
cial behavior, however, we expect that this effect would be ob- chotic disorders or symptoms are active versus in remission at the
served for other risk factors as well. Results also indicated that the time of violence, and, if possible, track both the onset and course
psychosis–violence relationship was not moderated by the severity of psychosis and violence. It may be that the association between
of violence, with severe and moderate violence producing compa- psychosis and violence is strongest when the symptoms are active
rable odds ratios. This finding should indicate that, to the extent (Krakowski et al., 1986; Monahan, 1988, 1992b; Monahan &
that psychosis is a risk factor for violence, it is not limited to trivial Steadman, 1994), a view partially supported by the present anal-
acts of violence. ysis. The general point here is that better measurement of psycho-
The method for assessing violence (i.e., reliance on single vs. sis should allow strong tests of competing causal models of its
multiple methods) was not a significant moderator. Studies that association with violence.
relied on official records, observation, or self/other reports pro- Researchers also need to pay more attention to the measurement,
duced significant effects. Studies that incorporated multiple meth- conceptualization, and context of violence. Although relying
ods, or failed to specify the measurement approach for violence, solely on records or self-reports is sometimes necessary, it often
did not. There were relatively few (k ⫽ 18) of the latter type of precludes measurement of other features of violence, such as
study. This finding was not expected, given that multiple methods motivation, targets, or location. It is probable that some types of
of measurement typically capture a greater amount of violence violence (predatory, instrumental violence against strangers) are
(Steadman et al., 1998). However, it may be the case that the not frequently committed by persons with psychosis but that other
official records and observation methods in the current studies types may be (reactive violence against family members or cohab-
were confounded by other variables (such as retrospective design) itants following stressful or tense encounters). Indeed, research has
or were based on cases with known outcomes (i.e., all violent identified that family members are commonly at risk of acts of
offenders in a prison sample were “detected” to be violent accord- violence by persons with psychosis (Estroff et al., 1998).
ing to official records). As such, the value of multiple methods of Longitudinal research would be well-suited to identifying the
measurement is likely greater in prospective studies in which possible transactions between elements of psychosis and violence
participants are tracked in the community after release from an across time, an area that little research has been able to address. It
institution, and there is a very real risk of failing to detect violence would be particularly interesting to track the first onset of psycho-
by sole reliance on single measurement procedures, such as formal sis and violence. In addition, additional studies of the possible
recidivism records. mediating role of third variables are necessary. Psychosis might be
associated with violence because it is related to other risk factors
Implications for Future Research (Elbogen & Johnson, 2009), such as unemployment or housing
problems. From one perspective, this would reduce psychosis to a
Posing the question, “Are individuals with psychosis more nuisance variable if these other variables were considered caus-
likely to be violent than individuals without psychosis?” is sort of ative of both psychosis and violence. However, if mental illness
like asking whether 10-year olds are tall. Compared with toddlers, itself plays a causal role in housing problems or unemployment
they certainly are. Compared with adults, they are decidedly short. (Dembling et al., 2002; Hudson, 2005), then it is not clear, theo-
And so it is with psychosis: Compared with individuals with no retically, that these variables should be controlled for. Rather, it
mental disorders, people with psychosis seem to be at a substan- could be the case that violence is but one of a host of adverse
tially elevated risk for violence. Compared with individuals with outcomes associated with mental illness, many of which are asso-
externalizing psychopathology, psychosis does not appear to fur- ciated with basic problems in living (employment, housing, rela-
ther elevate the risk for violence. Whereas we did find a statisti- tionships, education, health). We expect that many of these vari-
cally significant, though small, overall relationship between psy- ables could influence onset of illness as well as transact with
chosis and violence, we argue that the general question should be course of illness and violence over time. A simple A3 B concep-
abandoned in favor of the more complex and sophisticated ques- tualization of the issue is unlikely to capture it adequately.
tion, “What particular symptoms of psychosis, under which situ-
ational circumstances, and in combination with which personal or Implications for Clinical Practice: Evaluating Risk
situational factors, are associated with increased or decreased risk of Violence
of various kinds of violence?”
With respect to psychosis, future research should focus on The findings reported herein have a number of implications for
specific diagnostic groups (formed according to standardized di- violence risk assessment. First, psychosis should be evaluated in
agnostic criteria) and on specific clusters of psychotic symptoms all violence risk assessments. At first, this seems inconsistent with
(assessed with standardized measures). It would be helpful if, in findings that psychosis was not associated with violence under
the future, researchers provided more detail concerning their as- certain conditions (e.g., comparison with externalizing psychopa-
sessments of psychosis, including analyses of the prevalence and thology, within forensic psychiatric samples). However, we base
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 697

this argument on two premises: (a) There was an overall associa- were out of reach. For example, it was not possible to test partic-
tion between psychosis and violence, and this was particularly ular theories about why psychosis might elevate the odds for
strong when the comparison group consisted of individuals with- violence. Such theories come in various forms and are crucial for
out mental disorder, and (b) as we outlined above, all of our moving the field forward. Relatively focused theoretical models
findings, and those of the original studies, are based on groups. were developed specifically to explain the relationship between
Further, some studies (Elbogen & Johnson, 2009) have reported psychosis (or MMI) and violence. Hiday (1997, 2006), for exam-
that close to half of persons with MMI have a co-occurring ple, posited that the features of major mental illness (odd behavior,
substance-related disorder, and comorbidity elevates the risk of difficulty handling stimulation) can lead to tense interpersonal
violence substantially (see also Monahan et al., 2001). As such, it situations in which the probability of violence is increased. Jung-
is important to know whether a person with either an MMI or a inger (1996) developed a theoretical framework to explain when
substance-related disorder has a co-occurring disorder. psychosis might lead to violence. In particular, violence in the
Clinical assessment of risk for violence inherently is about form of “psychotic action” is more likely when the themes and
making individual decisions. In our view, the evidence from the
content of the delusions and hallucinations are aggressively laden
current meta-analysis is sufficient to conclude that psychosis may
and, hence, consistent with acting violently.
elevate a person’s risk compared with the case in which psychosis
Other theoretical explanations of the link between psychosis and
is absent in that individual. At the very least, the current meta-
violence draw from classic psychological theories, such as psy-
analysis supports treating the violence relevance of psychosis in an
chodynamic theory (Mizen, 2003). For instance, some scholars
individual as a hypothesis to be disconfirmed through an assess-
ment of its relevance to a person’s history of violence and likeli- have tested the role that defense mechanisms such as projection
hood for future violence. In essence, evaluators should be asking might play in violence (Porcerelli, Cogan, Kamoo, & Leitman,
themselves what potential role psychosis might play vis-à-vis 2003). Such theoretical approaches—narrow or broad— have great
violence for the given individual whom they are evaluating. potential merit in furthering understanding of any link between
Further, the issue of “compared with what or whom?” can be psychosis and violence. For example, in principle, it should be
factored into an individual risk assessment. That is, an evaluator possible to measure whether a particular psychotic symptom, such
might conclude that risk is lower for his or her patient with as paranoid delusions, was acting as a defense mechanism for an
psychosis compared with persons with externalizing disorders but individual. However, the existing empirical literature on psychosis
that it is still higher than it would be if psychosis were absent. The and violence did not permit testing such theoretical models in this
evaluator might also conclude, depending on the context in which meta-analysis, because most studies do not actually measure
the patient might be residing in the future, that the patient likely is whether such psychotic symptoms are indeed manifestations of
at lower risk for violence than is his or her externalizing counter- these (or other) theoretical postulates. Similarly, very few of the
parts but that he or she is at higher risk than individuals without included studies measured the themes of symptoms and whether
any mental disorders. Note that this does not mean that clinicians such themes were consistent or inconsistent with violence. Had
need to perform exhaustive evaluations of psychosis in every case; they done so, we could have tested Junginger’s (1996) model of
rather, a brief screen for psychosis could be conducted, with a psychotic action. There are simply too few empirical studies that
more comprehensive assessment limited to cases in which the incorporate measurement of psychosis and violence that also in-
results of the screen are positive. clude tests of these theories. Again, we encourage such steps in
The second implication is that the presence of psychosis should future research.
be considered neither necessary nor sufficient for a determination Moreover, it was not possible to determine the appropriate role
of high risk. This conclusion is based on the finding that the overall of covariates, such as SES or employment (i.e., are such variables
magnitude of the psychosis–violence association was found to be causes or correlates of mental illness?), or to evaluate the impor-
small; most violent individuals are not psychotic, and most psy- tance of timing between psychosis and violence across the life
chotic individuals are not violent. This means that clinicians con-
span. These specific and complicated aspects of the psychosis–
ducting a risk assessment should consider the role that psychosis
violence association are best suited for future in-depth, individual
may have played in past violence (according to the individual’s
studies.
history) and could play in future violence (e.g., as an organizing or
Finally, any meta-analysis is limited by the nature and quality of
a disorganizing factor). Only in cases in which the psychosis seems
the research on which it is based. In our view, the research
logically linked to the occurrence of future violence should clini-
cians consider it an important risk factor. Of course, other cofac- literature on psychosis and violence is sufficiently large to permit
tors may also be present that increase (e.g., substance abuse, general conclusions about the overall association, as well as about
psychopathy) or decrease (e.g., social support, stable housing) the some of the specific aspects of the association. However, we also
individual’s risk for violence. As such, psychosis should never be note— echoing the comments of others (e.g., Krakowski et al.,
the sole factor that is considered in determining a person’s risk– or 1986)—that researchers need to include more detail in their pub-
lack thereof—for violence. lished findings that would facilitate reviews, whether narrative or
quantitative. That is, for some moderators, there were a large
number of studies in which important methodological features
Limitations
were not specified. Despite this, we were able to code enough
First, although we were able to evaluate some interesting and studies in sufficient detail to permit a meaningful evaluation of the
important issues, such as the composition of the comparison group, overall psychosis–violence relationship and the impact of putative
types of symptoms, and the impact of comorbidity, many questions moderators.
698 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

References psychopathology in persons with severe or profound mental retardation.


Research in Developmental Disabilities, 19, 423– 438.
References marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis. *Binder, R. L., & McNiel, D. E. (1986). Victims and families of violent
psychiatric patients. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
*Aarsland, D., Cummings, J. L., Yenner, G., & Miller, B. (1996). Rela-
the Law, 14, 131–139.
tionship of aggressive behavior to other neuropsychiatric symptoms in
*Björkdahl, A., Olsson, D., & Palmstierna, T. (2006). Nurses’ short-term
patients with Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
prediction of violence in acute psychiatric intensive care. Acta Psychi-
243–247.
atrica Scandinavica, 113, 224 –229.
*Abu-Akel, A., & Abushua’leh, K. (2004). ‘Theory of mind’ in violent and
Bjørkly, S. (2002a). Psychotic symptoms and violence toward others—A
nonviolent patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Re-
literature review of some preliminary findings: Part 1. Delusions. Ag-
search, 69, 45–53.
gression and Violent Behavior, 7, 617– 631.
*Abushua’leh, K., & Abu-Akel, A. (2006). Association of psychopathic
Bjørkly, S. (2002b). Psychotic symptoms and violence toward others—A
traits and symptomatology with violence in patients with schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Research, 143, 205–211. literature review of some preliminary findings: Part 2. Hallucinations.
Alexander, R. T., Crouch, K., Halstead, S., & Piachaud, J. (2006). Long- Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7, 605– 615.
term outcome from a medium secure service for people with intellectual Bloom, J. D. (1989). The character of danger in psychiatric practice: Are
disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 305–315. the mentally ill dangerous? Bulletin of the American Academy of Psy-
Anderson, D. A. (1999). The aggregate burden of crime. Journal of Law chiatry and the Law, 17, 241–255.
and Economics, 42, 611– 642. Bonta, J., Law, M., & Hanson, R. K. (1998). The prediction of criminal and
*Appelbaum, P. S., Robbins, P. C., & Monahan, J. (2000). Violence and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: A meta-
delusions: Data from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 123–142.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 566 –572. Bradford, J. M. W. (1983). The forensic psychiatric aspects of schizophre-
*Arango, C., Bombı́n, I., González-Salvador, T., Garcı́a-Cabeza, I., & nia. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 8, 96 –103.
Bobes, J. (2005). Randomised clinical trial comparing oral versus depot *Brennan, P. A., Mednick, S. A., & Hodgins, S. (2000). Major mental
formulations of zuclopenthixol in patients with schizophrenia and pre- disorders and criminal violence in a Danish birth cohort. Archives of
vious violence. European Psychiatry, 21, 34 – 40. General Psychiatry, 57, 494 –500.
*Arango, C., Calcedo Barba, A., González-Salvador, T., & Calcedo *Brownstone, D. Y., & Swaminath, R. S. (1989). Violent behaviour and
Ordóñez, A. (1999). Violence in inpatients with schizophrenia: A pro- psychiatric diagnosis in female offenders. Canadian Journal of Psychi-
spective study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 493–503. atry, 34, 190 –194.
*Arboleda-Florez, J., Love, E. J., Fick, G., O’Brien, K., Hashman, K., & *Buckley, P. F., Hrouda, D. R., Friedman, L., Noffsinger, S. G., Resnick,
Aderibigbe, Y. (1995). An epidemiological study of mental illness in a P. J., & Camlin-Shingler, K. (2004). Insight and its relationship to
remanded population. International Medical Journal, 2, 113–126. violent behavior in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of
*Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Murray, R., Poulton, R., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, Psychiatry, 161, 1712–1714.
T. E. (2003). Childhood origins of violent behaviour in adults with Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A., & Kaem-
schizophreniform disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 520 –525. mer, B. (1989). Manual for administration and scoring MMPI-2. Min-
*Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2002). The targets neapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
of violence committed by young offenders with alcohol dependence, *Calcedo-Barba, A. L., & Calcedo Ordóñez, A. (1994). Violence and
marijuana dependence and schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: Findings paranoid schizophrenia. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry,
from a birth cohort. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, 155– 17, 253–263.
168. *Carrithers, V. C. (1994). A regression/correctional analysis study of
*Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, P. J., & Silva, P. A. (2000). Mental violence, six demographics, and three mental illness symptoms. Unpub-
disorders and violence in a total birth cohort: Results from the Dunedin lished doctoral dissertation, University of Louisville.
study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 979 –986. *Cheung, P., Schweitzer, I., Crowley, K., & Tuckwell, V. (1997). Aggres-
*Asnis, G. M., Kaplan, M. L., van Praag, H. M., & Sanderson, W. C. sive behavior in schizophrenia: The role of psychopathology. Australian
(1994). Homicidal behaviors among psychiatric outpatients. Hospital and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 62– 67.
and Community Psychiatry, 45, 127–132. *Chuang, H. T., Williams, R., & Dalby, J. T. (1987). Criminal behaviour
*Barkataki, I., Kumari, V., Das, M., Hill, M., Morris, R., O’Connell, P., et among schizophrenics. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 255–258.
al. (2005). A neuropsychological investigation into violence and mental *Cirincione, C., Steadman, H. J., Robbins, P. C., & Monahan, J. (1992).
illness. Schizophrenia Research, 74, 1–13. Schizophrenia as a contingent risk factor for criminal violence. Interna-
Barlow, K., Grenyer, B., & Ilkiw-Lavallle, O. (2000). Prevalence and tional Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 15, 347–358.
precipitants of aggression in psychiatric inpatients. Australian and New *Clare, P., Bailey, S., & Clark, A. (2000). Relationship between psychotic
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 967–974. disorders in adolescence and criminally violent behaviour: A retrospec-
Barnard, G. W., Robbins, L., Newman, G., & Carrera, F. (1984). A study tive examination. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 275–279.
of violence within a forensic treatment facility. Bulletin of the American *Coid, J., Yang, M., Roberts, A., Ullrich, S., Moran, P., Bebbington, P., et
Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 12, 339 –348. al. (2006). Violence and psychiatric morbidity in the national household
*Bassiony, M. M., Steinberg, M. S., Warren, A., Rosenblatt, A., Baker, population of Britain: Public health implications. British Journal of
A. S., & Lyketsos, C. G. (2000). Delusions and hallucinations in Alz- Psychiatry, 189, 12–19.
heimer’s disease: Prevalence and clinical correlates. International Jour- *Convit, A., Isay, D., Otis, D., & Volavka, J. (1990). Characteristics of
nal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 99 –107. repeatedly assaultive psychiatric inpatients. Hospital and Community
Baxter, R. (1997). Violence in schizophrenia and the syndrome of disor- Psychiatry, 41, 1112–1115.
ganisation. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 7, 131–139. Cooper, S. J., Browne, F. W., McClean, K. J., & King, D. J. (1983).
*Beaudoin, M. N., Hodgins, S., & Lavoie, F. (1993). Homicide, schizo- Aggressive behaviour in a psychiatric observation ward. Acta Psychiat-
phrenia and substance abuse or dependency. Canadian Journal of Psy- rica Scandinavica, 68, 386 –393.
chiatry, 38, 541–546. Corrigan, P. W., & O’Shaughnessy, J. R. (2007). Changing mental illness
*Bihm, E. M., Poindexter, A. R., & Warren, E. R. (1998). Aggression and stigma as it exists in the real world. Australian Psychologist, 42, 90 –97.
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 699

Corrigan, P. W., & Penn, D. L. (1999). Lessons from social psychology on mental illness: A study of arrest rates in a rural state. American Journal
discrediting psychiatric stigma. American Psychologist, 54, 765–776. of Psychiatry, 134, 80 – 83.
*Côté, G., Lesage, A., Chawky, N., & Loyer, M. (1997). Clinical speci- Duval, S. D., & Tweedie, R. T. (2000). A nonparametric “trim and fill”
ficity of prison inmates with severe mental disorders: A case-control method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of
study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 571–577. the American Statistical Association, 95, 89 –98.
*Craig, T. J. (1982). An epidemiologic study of problems associated with *Edwards, J. G., Jones, D., Reid, W. H., & Chu, C. C. (1988). Physical
violence among psychiatric inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, assaults in a psychiatric unit of a general hospital. American Journal of
139, 1262–1266. Psychiatry, 145, 1568 –1571.
*Crocker, A. G., Mueser, K. T., Drake, R. E., Clark, R. E., McHugo, G. J., *Ehmann, T. S., Smith, G. N., Yamamoto, A., McCarthy, N., Ross, D., Au,
Ackerson, T. H., et al. (2005). Antisocial personality, psychopathy, and T., et al. (2001). Violence in treatment resistant psychotic inpatients. The
violence in persons with dual disorders: A longitudinal analysis. Crim- Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 716 –721.
inal Justice and Behavior, 32, 452– 476. Elbogen, E. B., & Johnson, S. C. (2009). The intricate link between
*Daffern, M., Howells, K., Ogloff, J. R. P., & Lee, J. (2005). Individual violence and mental disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic
characteristics predisposing patients to aggression in a forensic psychi- survey on alcohol and related condition. Archives of General Psychiatry,
atric hospital. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 16, 729 – 66, 152–161.
746. *Elbogen, E. B., Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., & Van Dorn, R. (2005).
*Dalal, B., Larkin, E., Leese, M., & Taylor, P. J. (1999). Clozapine Family representative payeeship and violence risk in severe mental
treatment of long-standing schizophrenia and serious violence: A two- illness. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 563–574.
year follow-up study of the first 50 patients treated with clozapine in *Erb, M., Hodgins, S., Freese, R., Müller-Isberner, R., & Jöckel, D. (2001).
Rampton high security hospital. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, Homicide and schizophrenia: Maybe treatment does have a preventive
9, 168 –178. effect. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 11, 6 –26.
*Danielson, K. K., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). *Eronen, M. (1995). Mental disorders and homicidal behavior in female
Comorbidity between abuse of an adult and DSM-III-R mental disor- subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1216 –1218.
ders: Evidence from an epidemiological study. American Journal of *Eronen, M., Hakola, P., & Tiihonen, J. (1996a). Factors associated with
Psychiatry, 155, 131–133. homicide recidivism in a 13-year sample of homicide offenders in
De Pauw, K. W., & Szulecka, K. (1988). Dangerous delusions: Violence
Finland. Psychiatric Services, 47, 403– 406.
and the misidentification syndromes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152,
*Eronen, M., Hakola, P., & Tiihonen, J. (1996b). Mental disorders and
91–96.
homicidal behavior in Finland. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53,
*Dean, K., Walsh, E., Moran, P., Tyrer, P., Creed, F., Byford, S., et al.
497–501.
(2006). Violence in women with psychosis in the community: A pro-
*Eronen, M., Tiihonen, J., & Hakola, P. (1996c). Schizophrenia and
spective study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 264 –270.
homicidal behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 83– 89.
*Dell, S., Robertson, G., James, K., & Grounds, A. (1993). Remands and
Estroff, S. E., Swanson, J. W., Lachicotte, W. S., Swartz, M., & Bolduc, M.
psychiatric assessments in Holloway Prison. I: The psychotic popula-
(1998). Risk reconsidered: Targets of violence in the social networks of
tion. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 634 – 640.
people with serious psychiatric disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychi-
Dembling, B. P., Rovnyak, V., Mackey, S., & Blank, M. (2002). Effects of
atric Epidemiology, 33, S95–S101.
geographic migration on SMI prevalence estimates. Mental Health Ser-
*Estroff, S. E., & Zimmer, C. (1994). Social networks, social support, and
vices Research, 4, 7–12.
violence among persons with severe, persistent mental illness. In J.
*Denman, N. (1996). Relationships among diagnoses and features of
Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder:
violence in adult males. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Kansas. Developments in risk assessment (pp. 259 –295). Chicago: University of
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: Chicago Press.
An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13, 595– 605. *Etherington, R. (1993). Diagnostic and personality differences of juvenile
DerSimonian, R., & Laird, N. (1986). Meta-analysis in clinical trials. sex offenders, non–sex offenders and non-offenders. Unpublished doc-
Controlled Clinical Trials, 7, 177–188. toral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Los
*Desai, R. A., Lam, J., & Rosenheck, R. A. (2000). Childhood risk factors Angeles, Alliant University.
for criminal justice involvement in a sample of homeless people with Fazel, S., & Grann, M. (2004). Psychiatric morbidity among homicide
serious mental illness. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188, offenders: A Swedish population study. American Journal of Psychiatry,
324 –332. 161, 2129 –2131.
*Dhossche, D. M. (1999). Aggression and recent substance abuse: Absence *Fazel, S., & Grann, M. (2006). The population impact of severe mental
of association in psychiatric emergency room patients. Comprehensive illness on violent crime. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1397–
Psychiatry, 40, 343–346. 1403.
*Dickerson, F., Ringel, N., Parente, F., & Boronow, J. (1994). Seclusion *Fido, A. A., Razik, M. A., Mizra, I., & El-Islam, M. F. (1992). Psychiatric
and restraint, assaultiveness, and patient performance in a token econ- disorders in prisoners referred for assessment: A preliminary study.
omy. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 168 –170. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 37, 100 –103.
Douglas, K. S., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2003). Violence by psychiatric patients: First, M. B. (Ed.). (2003). Standardized evaluation in clinical practice.
The impact of archival measurement source on violence prevalence and Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
risk assessment accuracy. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48, 734 –740. *Flannery, R. B., Fisher, W., Walker, A. P. M., Littlewood, K. B., &
Douglas, K. S., & Skeem, J. L. (2005). Violence risk assessment: Getting Spillane, M. J. (2001). Nonviolent psychiatric inpatients and subsequent
specific about being dynamic. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, assaults on community patients and staff. Psychiatric Quarterly, 72,
347–383. 19 –27.
*Doyal, M., Dolan, M., & McGovern, J. (2002). The validity of North Fleiss, J. L. (1994). Measures of effect size for categorical data. In H.
American risk assessment tools in predicting in-patient violent behav- Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp.
iour in England. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 7, 141–154. 245–260). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
*Durbin, J. R., Pasewark, R. A., & Albers, D. (1977). Criminality and Fleiss, J. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Dubro. A. F. (1986). The logistic
700 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

regression analysis of psychiatric data. Journal of Psychiatric Research, correlations, causes and controls. International Journal of Law and
20, 145–209. Psychiatry, 29, 316 –331.
*Fresán, A., Apiquian, R., de la Fuente-Sandoval, C., Löyzaga, C., Garcı́a- *Hill, C. D., Neumann, C. S., & Rogers, R. (2004). Confirmatory factor
Anaya, M., Meyenberg, N., et al. (2005). Violent behavior in schizo- analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version in offenders
phrenic patients: Relationship with clinical symptoms. Aggressive Be- with Axis I disorders. Psychological Assessment, 16, 90 –95.
havior, 31, 511–520. Hillbrand, M. (2001). Homicide-suicide and other forms of co-occurring
*Fulwiler, C., Grossman, H., Forbes, C., & Ruthazer, R. (1997). Early- aggression against self and against others. Professional Psychology:
onset substance abuse and community violence by outpatients with Research and Practice, 32, 626 – 635.
chronic mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 48, 1181–1185. *Hodelet, N. (2001). Psychosis and offending in British Columbia: Char-
*Gardner, W., Lidz, C. W., Mulvey, E. P., & Shaw, E. C. (1996). A acteristics of a secure hospital population. Criminal Behaviour and
comparison of actuarial methods for identifying repetitively violent Mental Health, 11, 163–172.
patients with mental illnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 35– 48. *Hodgins, S. (1992). Mental disorder, intellectual deficiency, and crime:
*Gosden, N. P., Kramp, P., Gabrielsen, G., Andersen, T. F., & Sestoft, D. Evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49,
(2005). Violence of young criminals predicts schizophrenia: A 9-year 476 – 483.
register-based followup of 15- to 19-year-old criminals. Schizophrenia *Hodgins, S., Hiscoke, U. L., & Freese, R. (2003). The antecedents of
Bulletin, 31, 759 –768. aggressive behavior among men with schizophrenia: A prospective
Grann, M., Långström, N., Tengström, A., & Kullgren, G. (1999). Psy- investigation of patients in community treatment. Behavioral Sciences &
chopathy (PCL-R) predicts violent recidivism among criminal offenders the Law, 21, 523–546.
with personality disorders in Sweden. Law and Human Behavior, 23, *Holcomb, W. R., & Ahr, P. R. (1988). Arrest rates among young adult
205–217. psychiatric patients treated in inpatient and outpatient settings. Hospital
Grassi, L., Peron, L., Marangoni, C., Zanchi, P., & Vanni, A. (2001). and Community Psychiatry, 39, 52–57.
Characteristics of violent behavior in acute psychiatric in-patients: A *Hoptman, M. J., Yates, K. F., Patalinjug, M. B., Wack, R. C., & Convit,
5-year Italian study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104, 273–279. A. (1999). Clinical prediction of assaultive behavior among male psy-
*Greenberg, W. M., Shah, P. J., & Seide, M. (1993). Recidivism on an chiatric patients at a maximum-security forensic facility. Psychiatric
acute psychiatric forensic service. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, Services, 50, 1461–1466.
44, 583–585. Hudson, C. G. (2005). Socioeconomic status and mental illness: Tests of
Guy, L. S., Edens, J. F., Anthony, C., & Douglas, K. S. (2005). Does the social causation and selection hypotheses. American Journal of
psychopathy predict institutional misconduct among adults? A meta- Orthopsychiatry, 75, 3–18.
analytic investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, *Inada, T., Minagawa, F., Iwashita, S., & Tokui, T. (1995). Mentally
73, 1056 –1064. disordered criminal offenders: Five years’ data from the Tokyo district
Haddock, C. K., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, W. R. (1998). Using odds ratios public prosecutor’s office. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry,
as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: A primer on 18, 221–230.
methods and issues. Psychological Methods, 3, 339 –353. *Jamieson, L., & Taylor, P. J. (2004). A re-conviction study of special
*Hafner, H., & Boker, W. (1982). Crimes of violence by mentally disor- (high security) hospital patients. British Journal of Criminology, 44,
dered offenders (H. Marshall, Trans.). Cambridge, United Kingdom: 783– 802.
Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1973) *Janofsky, J. S., Spears, S., & Neubauer, D. N. (1988). Psychiatrists’
*Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, C. A. (2002). Prospective repli- accuracy in predicting violent behavior on an inpatient unit. Hospital
cation of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide in predicting violent recid- and Community Psychiatry, 39, 1090 –1094.
ivism among forensic patients. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 377–394. Junginger, J. (1996). Psychosis and violence: The case for a content
*Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1993). Violent recidivism of analysis of psychotic experience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 91–103.
mentally disordered offenders: The development of a statistical predic- Junginger, J., & McGuire, L. (2004). Psychotic motivation and the paradox
tion instrument. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 20, 315–335. of current research on serious mental illness and rates of violence.
*Harris, G. T., & Varney, G. W. (1986). A ten-year study of assaults and Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30, 21–30.
assaulters on a maximum security psychiatric unit. Journal of Interper- *Kaliski, S. Z. (2002). A comparison of risk factors for habitual violence
sonal Violence, 1, 173–191. in pre-trial subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 106(Suppl. 412),
*Harwood, D. G., Ownby, R. L., Barker, W. W., & Duara, R. (1998). The 58 – 61.
behavioral pathology in Alzheimer’s disease scale (BEHAVE-AD): Fac- *Kaliski, S. Z., & Zabow, T. (1995). Violence, sensation seeking, and
tor structure among community-dwelling Alzheimer’s disease patients. impulsivity in schizophrenics found unfit to stand trial. Bulletin of the
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 793– 800. American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 23, 147–155.
Hasselblad, V., & Hedges, L. V. (1995). Meta-analysis of screening and Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The Positive and Negative
diagnostic tests. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 167–178. Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13,
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 261–276.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. *Kay, S. R., Wolkenfeld, F., & Murrill, L. M. (1988). Profiles of aggres-
Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in sion among psychiatric patients II: Covariates and predictors. Journal of
meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3, 486 –504. Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 547–557.
Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and *Kessler, R. C., Molnar, B. E., Feurer, I. D., & Appelbaum, M. (2001).
recidivism: A review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139 – Patterns and mental health predictors of domestic violence in the United
170. States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. International
Hiday, V. A. (1995). The social context of mental illness and violence. Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 24, 487–508.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 36, 122–137. *Kienlen, K. K., Birmingham, D. L., Solberg, K. B., O’Regan, J. T., &
Hiday, V. A. (1997). Understanding the connection between mental illness Meloy, J. R. (1997). A comparative study of psychotic and nonpsychotic
and violence. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 20, 399 – stalking. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 25,
417. 317–334.
Hiday, V. A. (2006). Putting community risk in perspective: A look at *Kjelsberg, E. (2004). Gender and disorder specific criminal career profiles
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 701

in former adolescent psychiatric in-patients. Journal of Youth and Ad- *Lindqvist, P., & Allebeck, P. (1990). Schizophrenia and crime: A longi-
olescence, 33, 261–269. tudinal follow-up of 644 schizophrenics in Stockholm. British Journal of
*Klassen, D., & O’Connor, W. A. (1988a). Crime, inpatient admissions, Psychiatry, 157, 345–350.
and violence among male mental patients. International Journal of Law *Link, B. G., Andrews, H., & Cullen, F. T. (1992). The violent and illegal
and Psychiatry, 11, 305–312. behavior of mental patients reconsidered. American Sociological Re-
*Klassen, D., & O’Connor, W. A. (1988b). Predicting violence in schizo- view, 57, 275–292.
phrenic and non-schizophrenic patients: A prospective study. Journal of Link, B. G., & Cullen, F. T. (1986). Contact with the mentally ill and
Community Psychology, 16, 217–227. perceptions of how dangerous they are. Journal of Health and Social
*Klassen, D., & O’Connor, W. A. (1989). Assessing the risk of violence in Behavior, 27, 289 –303.
released mental patients: A cross-validation study. Psychological As- Link, B. G., Cullen, F. T., Frank, J., & Wozniak, J. F. (1987). The social
sessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 75– 81. rejection of former mental patients: Understanding why labels matter.
*Krakowski, M. I., & Czobor, P. (1997). Violence in psychiatric patients: American Journal of Sociology, 96, 1461–1500.
The role of psychosis, frontal lobe impairment, and ward turmoil. *Link, B. G., Monahan, J., Stueve, A., & Cullen, F. T. (1999). Real in their
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38, 230 –236. consequences: A sociological approach to understanding the association
*Krakowski, M. I., & Czobor, P. (2004a). Gender differences in violent between psychotic symptoms and violence. American Sociological Re-
behaviors: Relationship to clinical symptoms and psychosocial factors. view, 64, 316 –322.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 459 – 465. *Link, B. G., & Stueve, A. (1994). Psychotic symptoms and the violent/
*Krakowski, M. I., & Czobor, P. (2004b). Suicide and violence in patients illegal behavior of mental patients compared to community controls. In
with major psychiatric disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 10, J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder:
233–238. Developments in risk assessment (pp. 137–159). Chicago: University of
*Krakowski, M. I., Czobor, P., & Chou, J. C. (1999). Course of violence Chicago Press.
in patients with schizophrenia: Relationship to clinical symptoms. *Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Phelan, J. (1998). Psychotic symptoms and
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 505–517. violent behaviors: Probing the components of “threat/control-override”
*Krakowski, M. I., Jaeger, J., & Volavka, J. (1988). Violence and psycho- symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, S55–
pathology: A longitudinal study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29, 174 – S60.
181. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand
Krakowski, M. I., Volavka, J., & Brizer, D. (1986). Psychopathology and Oaks, CA: Sage.
violence: A review of the literature. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, *Lowenstein, M., Binder, R. L., & McNiel, D. E. (1990). The relationship
131–148. between admission symptoms and hospital assaults. Hospital and Com-
*Kravitz, H. M., Cavanaugh, J. L., & Rigsbee, S. S. (2002). A cross- munity Psychiatry, 41, 311–313.
sectional study of psychosocial and criminal factors associated with *Mamak, M. (1997). Violence and major mental illness. Unpublished
arrest in mentally ill female detainees. Journal of the America Academy doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
of Psychiatry and the Law, 30, 380 –390. *Margari, F., Matarazzo, R., Casacchia, M., Roncone, R., Dieci, M.,
*Kumari, V., Das, M., Hodgins, S., Zachariah, E., Barkataki, I., Howlett, Safran, S., et al. (2005). Italian validation of MOAS and NOSIE: A
M., et al. (2005). Association between violent behaviour and impaired useful package for psychiatric assessment and monitoring of aggressive
prepulse inhibition of the startle response in antisocial personality dis- behaviours. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research,
order and schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Research, 158, 159 –166. 14, 109 –118.
*Kunik, M. E., Snow-Turek, A. L., Iqbal, N., Molinari, V. A., Orengo, McNiel, D. E. (1994). Hallucinations and violence. In J. Monahan & H. J.
C. A., Workman, R. H., et al. (1999). Contribution of psychosis and Steadman (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder: Developments in risk
depression to behavioral disturbances in geropsychiatric inpatients with assessment (pp. 183–202). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
dementia. Journals of Gerontology: Series A, Biological Sciences and McNiel, D. E., & Binder, R. L. (1989). Relationship between preadmission
Medical Sciences, 54, 157–161. threats and later violent behavior by acute psychiatric inpatients. Hos-
*Lafayette, J. M., Frankle, W. G., Pollock, A., Dyer, K., & Goff, D. C. pital and Community Psychiatry, 40, 605– 608.
(2003). Clinical characteristics, cognitive functioning, and criminal his- McNiel, D. E., & Binder, R. L. (1994). The relationship between acute
tories of outpatients with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 54, 1635– psychiatric symptoms, diagnosis, and short-term risk of violence. Hos-
1640. pital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 133–137.
Lagos, J. M., Perlmutter, K., & Saexinger, H. (1977). Fear of the mentally *McNiel, D. E., Eisner, J. P., & Binder, R. L. (2000). The relationship
ill: Empirical support for the common man’s response. American Jour- between command hallucinations and violence. Psychiatric Services, 51,
nal of Psychiatry, 134, 1134 –1137. 1288 –1292.
*Lamb, H. R., Shaner, R., Elliott, D. M., DeCuir, W. J., & Foltz, J. T. *Milton, J., Amin, S., Singh, S. P., Harrison, G., Jones, P., Croudace, T.,
(1995). Outcome for psychiatric emergency patients seen by an outreach et al. (2001). Aggressive incidents in first-episode psychosis. British
police-mental health team. Psychiatric Services, 46, 1267–1271. Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 433– 440.
*Lamb, R., & Grant, R. W. (1982). The mentally ill in an urban county jail. Mizen, R. (2003). A contribution towards an analytic theory of violence.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 17–22. Journal of Analytic Psychology, 48, 285–305.
*Landau, S. F. (1996). Possession of firearms, psychiatric hospitalization *Modestin, J., & Ammann, R. (1995). Mental disorders and criminal
and violent criminal behaviour: The Israeli experience. Studies on Crime behaviour. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 667– 675.
& Crime Prevention, 5, 166 –181. *Modestin, J., & Ammann, R. (1996). Mental disorder and criminality:
*Leonard, R., Tinetti, M. E., Allore, H. G., & Drickamer, M. A. (2006). Male schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 69 – 82.
Potentially modifiable resident characteristics that are associated with *Modestin, J., & Wuermle, O. (2005). Criminality in men with major
physical or verbal aggression among nursing home residents with de- mental disorder with and without comorbid substance abuse. Psychiatry
mentia. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166, 1295–1300. and Clinical Neurosciences, 59, 25–29.
*Lidz, C. W., Mulvey, E. P., & Gardner, W. (1993). The accuracy of *Moffit, T. E., Brammer, G. L., Caspi, A., Fawcett, J. P., Raleigh, M.,
predictions of violence to others. Journal of the American Medical Yuwiler, A., et al. (1998). Whole blood serotonin relates to violence in
Association, 269, 1007–1011. an epidemiological study. Biological Psychiatry, 15, 446 – 457.
702 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

*Mojtabai, R. (2006). Psychotic-like experiences and interpersonal vio- Predictors of serious and violent offending by adjudicated male adoles-
lence in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epi- cents. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 407– 418.
demiology, 41, 183–190. *Pearson, M., Wilmot, E., & Padi, M. (1986). A study of violent behaviour
Monahan, J. (1988). Risk assessment of violence among the mentally among in-patients in a psychiatric hospital. British Journal of Psychia-
disordered: Generating useful knowledge. International Journal of Law try, 149, 232–235.
and Psychiatry, 11, 249 –257. Pescosolido, B. A., Monahan, J., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Kikuzawa, S.
Monahan, J. (1992a). “A terror to their neighbors”: Beliefs about mental (1999). The public’s view of the competence, dangerousness, and need
disorder and violence in historical and cultural perspective. Bulletin of for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. American
the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 20, 191–195. Journal of Public Health, 89, 1339 –1345.
Monahan, J. (1992b). Mental disorder and violent behavior. American Phelan, J., & Link, B. (1998). The growing belief that people with mental
Psychologist, 47, 511–521. illness are violent: The role of the dangerousness criterion for civil
Monahan, J. (1995). Predicting violent behavior: An assessment of clinical commitment. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 7–12.
techniques. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. (Original work published 1981) *Philipse, M. W. G., Koeter, M. W. J., van der Staak, C. P. F., & van den
Monahan, J., & Steadman, H. J. (1994). Toward a rejuvenation of risk Brink, W. (2006). Static and dynamic patient characteristics: A prospec-
assessment research. In J. Monahan & H. J. Steadman (Eds.), Violence tive study in a Dutch forensic psychiatric sample. Law and Human
and mental disorder: Developments in risk assessment (pp. 1–17). Behavior, 30, 309 –327.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. *Phillips, M. R., Wolf, A. S., & Coons, D. J. (1988). Psychiatry and the
Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Appelbaum, P. S., Robbins, P. C., criminal justice system: Testing the myths. American Journal of Psy-
Mulvey, E. P., et al. (2001). Rethinking risk assessment: The MacArthur chiatry, 145, 605– 610.
study of mental disorder and violence. New York: Oxford University *Phillips, P., & Nasr, S. J. (1983). Seclusion and restraint and prediction of
Press. violence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 229 –232.
Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality Assessment Inventory: Professional man- Porcerelli, J. H., Cogan, R., Kamoo, R., & Leitman, S. (2003). Defense
ual. Tampa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. mechanisms and self-reported violence toward partners and strangers.
*Mullen, P. E., Burgess, P., Wallace, C., Palmer, S., & Ruschena, D. Journal of Personality Assessment, 82, 317–320.
(2000). Community care and criminal offending in schizophrenia. The *Porporino, F. J., & Motiuk, L. L. (1995). The prison careers of mentally
Lancet, 355, 614 – 617. disordered offenders. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 18,
Mulvey, E. P. (1994). Assessing the evidence of a link between mental 29 – 44.
illness and violence. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 663– 668. *Quanbeck, C., Stone, D. C., Scott, C. L., McDermott, B. E., Altshuler,
Mulvey, E. P., Blumstein, A., & Cohen, J. (1986). Reframing the research L. L., & Frye, M. A. (2004). Clinical and legal correlates of inmates with
question of mental patient criminality. International Journal of Law and bipolar disorder at time of criminal arrest. Journal of Clinical Psychia-
Psychiatry, 9, 57– 65. try, 65, 198 –203.
Mulvey, E. P., Odgers, C. O., Skeem, J., Gardner, W., Schubert, C., & Quinsey, V. L., Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, C. A. (2006). Violent
Lidz, C. (2006). Substance use and community violence: A test of the offenders: Appraising and managing risk (2nd ed.). Washington, DC:
relation at the daily level. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- American Psychological Association.
ogy, 74, 743–754. *Rabinowitz, J., & Mark, M. (1999). Risk factors for violence among
Mulvey, E. P., Shaw, E., & Lidz, C. W. (1994). Why use multiple sources long-stay psychiatric patients: National study. Acta Psychiatrica Scan-
in research on patient violence in the community? Criminal Behaviour dinavica, 99, 341–347.
and Mental Health, 4, 253–258. Rabkin, J. G. (1979). Criminal behavior of discharged mental patients: A
*Myers, K. M., & Dunner, D. L. (1984). Self and other directed violence critical appraisal of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1–27.
on a closed acute-care ward. Psychiatric Quarterly, 56, 178 –188. *Raja, M., & Azzoni, A. (2005). Hostility and violence of acute psychiatric
*Naples, M., & Steadman, H. J. (2003). Can persons with co-occurring inpatients. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health, 1:11.
disorders and violent charges be successfully diverted? International (doi:10.1186/1745-0179-1-11)
Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 2, 137–143. *Räsänen, P., Tiihonen, J., Isohanni, M., Rantakallio, P., Lehtonen, J., &
*Nestor, P. G., Haycock, J., Doiron, S., Kelly, J., & Kelly, D. (1995). Moring, J. (1998). Schizophrenia, alcohol abuse, and violent behavior: A
Lethal violence and psychosis: A clinical profile. Bulletin of the Amer- 26-year followup study of an unselected birth cohort. Schizophrenia
ican Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 23, 331–341. Bulletin, 24, 437– 441.
Nicholls, T. L., Brink, J., Desmarais, S. L., Webster, C. D., & Martin, M. Ratakonda, S., Gorman, J. M., Yale, S. A., & Amador, X. F. (1998).
(2006). The Short-Term Assessment of Risk and Treatability (START): Characterization of psychotic conditions: Use of the domains of psycho-
A Prospective Validation Study in a Forensic Psychiatric Sample. As- pathology model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 75– 81.
sessment, 13, 313–327. *Repo, E., Virkkunen, M., Rawlings, R., & Linnoila, M. (1997). Criminal
*Nijman, H., Merckelbach, H., Evers, C., Palmstierna, T., & à Campo, J. and psychiatric histories of Finnish arsonists. Acta Psychiatrica Scan-
(2002). Prediction of aggression on a locked psychiatric admissions dinavica, 95, 318 –323.
ward. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 390 –395. Rice, D. P., Kelman, S., & Miller, L. S. (1992). The economic burden of
*Noble, P., & Rodger, S. (1989). Violence by psychiatric in-patients. mental illness. Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 43, 1227–1232.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 384 –390. *Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1992). A comparison of criminal recidivism
*Oulis, P., Lykouras, L., Dascalopoulou, E., & Psarros, C. (1996). Ag- among schizophrenic and nonschizophrenic offenders. International
gression among psychiatric inpatients in Greece. Psychopathology, 29, Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 15, 397– 408.
174 –180. *Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Psychopathy, schizophrenia, alcohol
Overall, J. E., & Gorham, D. R. (1962). The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. abuse, and violent recidivism. International Journal of Law and Psy-
Psychological Reports, 10, 799 – 812. chiatry, 18, 333–342.
*Paradis, C. M., Broner, N., Maher, L. M., & O’Rourke, T. (2000). Riskind, J. H., & Wahl, O. (1992). Moving makes it worse: The role of
Mentally ill elderly jail detainees: Psychiatric, psychosocial and legal rapid movement in fear of psychiatric patients. Journal of Social and
factors. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 31, 77– 86. Clinical Psychology, 11, 349 –364.
Parker, J. S., Morton, T. L., Lingefelt, M. E., & Johnson, K. S. (2005). *Ritsner, M., Modai, I., & Gibel, A. (2003). Decreased platelet peripheral-
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 703

type benzodiazepine receptors in persistently violent schizophrenia pa- Violence Risk Assessment Study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
tients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 549 –556. Psychology, 69, 358 –374.
*Robbins, Monahan, J., & Silver, E. (2003). Mental disorder, violence, and Skeem, J. L., & Mulvey, E. P. (2002). Monitoring the violence potential of
gender. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 561–571. mentally disordered offenders being treated in the community. In A.
*Robertson, G. (1988). Arrest patterns among mentally disordered offend- Buchanan (Ed.), Care of the mentally disordered offender in the com-
ers. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 313–316. munity (pp. 111–142). New York: Oxford Press.
*Rogers, P., Watt, A., Gray, N. S., MacCulloch, M., & Gournay, K. (2002). Socall, D. W., & Holtgraves, T. (1992). Attitudes toward the mentally ill:
Content of command hallucinations predicts self-harm but not violence The effects of labels and beliefs. Sociological Quarterly, 33, 435– 445.
in a medium secure unit. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psy- *Sosowsky, L. (1978). Crime and violence among mental patients recon-
chology, 13, 251–262. sidered in view of the new legal relationship between the state and the
*Rosenfeld, B., & Harmon, R. (2002). Factors associated with violence in mentally ill. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 33– 42.
stalking and obsessional harassment cases. Criminal Justice and Behav- StataCorp. (2007). Stata 10 [Software program]. College Station, TX:
ior, 29, 671– 691. Author.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Rev. Steadman, H. J., & Halfon, A. (1971). The Baxstrom patients: Back-
ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. grounds and outcomes. Seminars in Psychiatry, 3, 376 –386.
*Rossberg, J. I., & Friis, S. (2003). Staff members’ emotional reactions to Steadman, H. J., & Keveles, G. (1972). The community adjustment and
aggressive and suicidal behavior of inpatients. Psychiatric Serves, 54, criminal activity of the Baxstrom patients: 1966 –1970. American Jour-
1388 –1394. nal of Psychiatry, 129, 80 – 86.
*Rossi, A. M., Jacobs, M., Monteleone, M., Olsen, R., Surber, R. W., Steadman, H. J., Mulvey, E. P., Monahan, J., Robbins, P. C., Appelbaum,
Winkler, E. L., & Wommack, A. (1986). Characteristics of psychiatric P. S., Grisso, T., et al. (1998). Violence by people discharged from acute
patients who engage in assaultive or other fear-inducing behaviors. psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 154 –160. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 393– 401.
*Russo. G., Salomone, L., & Della Villa, L. (2003). The characteristics of *Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P. S., Robbins,
criminal and noncriminal mentally disordered patients. International P. C., Mulvey, E. P., et al. (2000). A classification tree approach to the
Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 26, 417– 435. development of actuarial violence risk assessment tools. Law and Hu-
*Ryan, E. P., Hart, V. S., Messick, D. L., Aaron, J., & Burnette, M. (2004). man Behavior, 24, 83–100.
A prospective study of assault against staff by youths in a state psychi- *Steels, M., Roney, G., Larkin, E., Jones, P., Coudace, T., & Duggan, C.
atric hospital. Psychiatric Services, 55, 665– 670. (1998). Discharged from special hospital under restrictions: A compar-
Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1996). A review and ison of the fates of psychopaths and the mentally ill. Criminal Behaviour
meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and Psychopathy-Checklist- and Mental Health, 8, 39 –55.
Revised: Predictive validity of dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: *Straznickas, K. A., McNiel, D. E., & Binder, R. L. (1993). Violence
Science and Practice, 3, 203–215. toward family caregivers by mentally ill relatives. Hospital and Com-
*Schanda, H., Knecht, G., Schreinzer, D., Stompe, T., Ortwein-Swoboda, munity Psychiatry, 44, 385–387.
G., & Waldhoer, T. (2004). Homicide and major mental disorders: A *Stueve, A., & Link, B. G. (1997). Violence and psychiatric disorders:
25-year study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110, 98 –107. Results from an epidemiological study of young adults in Israel. Psy-
*Schneider, L. S., Katz, I. R., & Park, S., Napolitano, J., Martinez, R. A., chiatric Quarterly, 68, 327–342.
& Azen, S. P. (2003). Psychosis of Alzheimer disease: Validity of the *Swanson, J. W. (1994). Mental disorder, substance abuse, and community
construct and response to Risperidone. American Journal of Geriatric violence: An epidemiological approach. In J. Monahan & H. J. Stead-
Psychiatry, 11, 414 – 425. man (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder: Developments in risk assess-
*Schwartz-Watts, D., & Morgan, D. W. (1998). Violent versus nonviolent ment (pp. 101–136). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
stalkers. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, *Swanson, J. W., Borum, R., Swartz, M. S., & Hiday, V. (1999). Violent
26, 241–245. behavior proceeding hospitalization among persons with severe mental
*Sendi, I. B., & Blomgren, P. G. (1975). A comparative study of predictive illness. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 185–204.
criteria in the predisposition of homicidal adolescents. American Journal *Swanson, J. W., Borum, R., Swartz, M. S., & Monahan, J. (1996).
of Psychiatry, 132, 423– 427. Psychotic symptoms and disorders and the risk of violent behavior in the
*Shader, R. I., Jackson, A. H., Harmatz, J. S., & Appelbaum, P. S. (1977). community. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 6, 309 –329.
Patterns of violent behavior among schizophrenic inpatients. Diseases of *Swanson, J. W., Holzer, C. E., III, Ganju, V. K., & Jono, R. T. (1990).
the Nervous System, 38, 13–16. Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: Evidence from the
Shadish, W. R., & Haddock, C. K. (1994). Combining estimates of effect epidemiologic catchment area surveys. Hospital and Community Psy-
size. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research chiatry, 41, 761–770.
synthesis (pp. 261–281). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. *Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Borum, R., Hiday, V. A., Wagner, H. R.,
*Shore, D., Filson, C. R., Johnson, W. E., Rae, D. S., Muehrer, P., Kelley, & Burns, B. J. (2000). Involuntary out-patient commitment and reduc-
D. J., et al. (1989). Murder and assault arrests of white house cases: tion of violent behaviour in persons with severe mental illness. British
Clinical and demographic correlates of violence subsequent to civil Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 324 –331.
commitment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 645– 651. *Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., & Elbogen, E. B. (2004). Effectiveness of
Silver, E. (2006). Understanding the relationship between mental disorder atypical antipsychotic medications in reducing violent behavior among
and violence: The need for a criminological perspective. Law and persons with schizophrenia in community-based treatment. Schizophre-
Human Behavior, 30, 685–706. nia Bulletin, 30, 3–20.
*Silver, H., Goodman, C., Knoll, G., Isakov, V., & Modai, I. (2005). Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Essock, S. M., Osher, F. C., Wagner, H. R.,
Schizophrenia patients with a history of severe violence differ from Goodman, L. A., et al. (2002). The social– environmental context of
nonviolent schizophrenia patients in perception of emotions but not violent behavior in persons treated for severe mental illness. American
cognitive function. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 300 –308. Journal of Public Health, 92, 1523–1531.
Skeem, J. L., & Mulvey, E. P. (2001). Psychopathy and community *Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., van Dorn, R. A., Elbogen, E. B., Wagner,
violence among civil psychiatric patients: Results from the MacArthur H. R., Rosenheck, R. A., et al. (2006). A national study of violent
704 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, *Tengström, A., Hodgins, S., Müller-Isberner, R., Jöckel, D., Freese, R.,
63, 490 – 499. Özokyay, K., et al. (2006). Predicting violent and antisocial behavior in
*Swartz, J. A., & Lurigio, A. A. (2004). Psychiatric diagnosis, substance hospital using HCR-20: The effect of diagnoses on predictive accuracy.
use and dependence, and arrests among former recipients of supplemen- International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 5, 39 –53.
tal security income for drug abuse and alcoholism. Journal of Offender *Tennent, G., Loucas, K., Fenton, G., & Fenwick, P. (1974). Male admis-
Rehabilitation, 39, 19 –38. sions to Broadmoor hospital. British Journal of Psychiatry, 125, 44 –50.
*Swartz, M. S., Swanson, J. W., Hiday, V. A., Borum, R., Wagner, R., & *Tennent, G., & Way, C. (1984). The English Special Hospital—A 12–17
Burns, B. J. (1998). Violence and severe mental illness: The effects of year follow-up study: A comparison of violent and non-violent re-
substance abuse and nonadherence to medication. American Journal of offenders and non-offenders. Medicine, Science, and the Law, 24,
Psychiatry, 155, 226 –231. 81–91.
*Swett, C., & Mills, T. (1997). Use of the NOSIE to predict assaults among *Teplin, L. A. (1985). The criminality of the mentally ill: A dangerous
acute psychiatric patients. Psychiatric Services, 48, 1177–1180. misconception. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 593–599.
*Tam, E., Engelsmann, F., & Fugere, R. (1996). Patterns of violent *Teplin, L. A. (1994). Psychiatric and substance abuse disorders among
incidents by patients in a general hospital psychiatric facility. Psychiat- male urban jail detainees. American Journal of Public Health, 84,
ric Services, 47, 86 – 88. 290 –293.
*Tanke, E. D., & Yesavage, J. A. (1985). Characteristics of assaultive *Teplin, L. A., Abram, K. M., & McClelland, G. M. (1994). Does psy-
patients who do and do not provide visible cues of potential violence. chiatric disorder predict violent crime among released jail detainees? A
American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1409 –1413. six-year longitudinal study. American Psychologist, 49, 335–342.
*Tardiff, K. (1981). Assault in hospitals and placement in the community. *Teplin, L. A., Abram, K. M., & McClelland, G. M. (1996). Prevalence of
Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 9, 33–39. psychiatric disorders among incarcerated women. I: Pretrial jail detain-
*Tardiff, K. (1984). Characteristics of assaultive patients in private hospi- ees. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 505–512.
tals. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1232–1235. *Thomas, S., Leese, M., Walsh, E., McCrone, P., Moran, P., Burns, T., et
*Tardiff, K., & Koenigsberg, H. W. (1985). Assaultive behavior among al. (2005). A comparison of statistical models in predicting violence in
psychiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 960 –963. psychotic illness. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 296 –303.
*Tardiff, K., Marzuk, P. M., Leon, A. C., & Portera, L. (1997). A pro- *Tiihonen, J., Eronen, M., & Hakola, P. (1993). Criminality associated
spective study of violence by psychiatric patients after hospital dis-
with mental disorders and intellectual deficiency. Archives of General
charge. Psychiatric Services, 48, 678 – 681.
Psychiatry, 50, 917–918.
*Tardiff, K., Marzuk, P. M., Leon, A. C., Portera, L., & Weiner, C. (1997).
*Tiihonen, J., Isohanni, M., Räsänen, P., Koiranen, M., & Moring, J.
Violence by patients admitted to a private psychiatric hospital. American
(1997). Specific major mental disorders and criminality: A 26-year
Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 88 –93.
prospective study of the 1966 Northern Finland birth cohort. American
*Tardiff, K., & Sweillam, A. (1980). Assault, suicide, and mental illness.
Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 840 – 845.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 164 –169.
*Troisi, A., Kustermann, S., Di Genio, M., & Siracusano, A. (2003).
*Tardiff, K., & Sweillam, A. (1982). Assaultive behavior among chronic
Hostility during admission interview as a short-term predictor of aggres-
inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 212–215.
sion in acute psychiatric male inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
Taylor, P. J. (1995). Schizophrenia and the risk of violence. In S. R. Hirsch
64, 1460 –1464.
& D. R. Weinberger (Eds.), Schizophrenia (pp. 163–183). Oxford,
*Valdiserri, E. V., Carroll, K. R., & Hartl, A. J. (1986). A study of offenses
United Kingdom: Blackwell.
committed by psychotic inmates in a county jail. Hospital and Commu-
Taylor, P. J. (1998). When symptoms of psychosis drive serious violence.
nity Psychiatry, 37, 163–166.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, S47–S54.
Taylor, P. J. (2006). Delusional disorder and delusions: Is there a risk of Valliant, P. M., Gristey, C., Pottier, D., & Kosmyna, R. (1999). Risk
violence in social interactions about the core symptom? Behavioral factors in violent and nonviolent offenders. Psychological Reports, 85,
Sciences and the Law, 24, 313–331. 675– 680.
Taylor, P. J., Garety, P., Buchanan, A., Reed, A., Wessely, S., Ray, K., *Ventura, L., Cassel, C., Jacoby, J., & Huang, B. (1998). Case manage-
Dunn, G., & Grubin, D. (1994). Delusions and violence. In J. Monahan ment and recidivism of mentally ill persons released from jail. Psychi-
& H. J. Steadman (Eds.), Violence and mental disorder: Developments atric Services, 49, 1330 –1337.
in risk assessment (pp. 161–182). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. *Verma, S., Poon, L. Y., Subramaniam, M., & Chong, S. A. (2005).
*Taylor, P. J., & Gunn, J. (1984). Violence and psychosis: I. Risk of Aggression in Asian patients with first-episode psychosis. International
violence among psychotic men. British Medical Journal, 288, 1945– Journal of Social Psychiatry, 51, 365–371.
1949. *Villeneuve, D. B. (1994). Predictors of general and violent recidivism
Taylor, P. J., & Hodgins, S. (1994). Violence and psychosis: Critical among mentally disordered federal incarcerates. Unpublished doctoral
timings. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 4, 267–289. dissertation, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada.
*Taylor, P. J., Leese, M., Williams, D., Butwell, M., Daly, R., & Larkin, *Volavka, J., Laska, E., Baker, S., Meisner, M., Czobor, P., & Krivelevich,
E. (1998). Mental disorder and violence: A special (high security) I. (1997). History of violent behaviour and schizophrenia in different
hospital study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 218 –226. cultures: Analyses based on the WHO study on determinants of outcome
*Teasdale, B., Silver, E., & Monahan, J. (2006). Gender, threat/control- of severe mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 9 –14.
override delusions and violence. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 649 – *Vreugdenhil, C., Vermeiren, R., Wouters, L., Doreleijers, T. A. H., van
658. den Brink, W., & Vreugdenhil, C. (2004). Psychotic symptoms among
Tehrani, J. A., Brennan., P. A., Hodgins, S., & Mednick, S. A. (1998). male adolescent detainees in the Netherlands. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
Mental illness and criminal violence. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric 30, 73– 86.
Epidemiology, 33, S81–S85. *Wallace, C., Mullen, P. E., & Burgess, P. (2004). Criminal offending in
*Tengström, A., Hodgins, S., Grann, M., Långström, N., & Kullgren, G. schizophrenia over a 25-year period marked by deinstitutionalization
(2004). Schizophrenia and criminal offending: The role of psychopathy and increasing prevalence of comorbid substance use disorders. Ameri-
and substance use disorders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 367– can Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 716 –727.
391. *Wallace, C., Mullen, P. E., Burgess, P., Palmer, S., Ruschena, D., &
PSYCHOSIS AND VIOLENCE 705

Browne, C. (1998). Serious criminal offending and mental disorder: *Wessely, S. C., Castle, D., Douglas, A. J., & Taylor, P. J. (1994). The
Case linkage study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 477– 484. criminal careers of incident cases of schizophrenia. Psychological Med-
Walsh, E., Buchanan, A., & Fahy, T. (2002). Violence and schizophrenia: icine, 24, 483–502.
Examining the evidence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 490 – 495. *Yesavage, J. A. (1983a). Bipolar illness: Correlates of dangerous inpatient
Walsh, E., Gilvarry, C., Samele, C., Harvey, K., Manley, C., Tattan, T., et behaviour. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 554 –557.
al. (2004). Predicting violence in schizophrenia: A prospective study. *Yesavage, J. A. (1983b). Inpatient violence and the schizophrenic patient.
Schizophrenia Research, 67, 247–252. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 353–357.
Walters, G. D. (2003). Predicting institutional adjustment and recidivism *Yesavage, J. A. (1984). Correlates of dangerous behavior by schizophren-
with the psychopathy checklist factor scores: A meta-analysis. Law and ics in hospital. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 18, 225–231.
Human Behavior, 27, 541–558. *Yesavage, J. A., Benezech, M., Larrieu-Arguille, R., Bourgeois, M.,
Watson, A. C., Corrigan, P., Larson, J. E., & Sells, M. (2007). Self-stigma Tanke, E., Rager, P., et al. (1986). Recidivism of the criminally insane
in people with mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 1312–1318. in France: A 22-year follow-up. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 47,
Webster, C. D., Douglas, K. S., Eaves, D., & Hart, S. D. (1997). HCR-20: 465– 466.
Assessing risk for violence, Version 2. Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada: *Yesavage, J. A., Werner, P. D., Becker, J., Holman, C., & Mills, M.
Mental Health, Law, & Policy Institute, Simon Fraser University. (1981). Inpatient evaluation of aggression in psychiatric patients. Jour-
*Weibe, D. J. (1998). Mental health factors in the adjustment of youth nal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169, 299 –302.
detention center residents. Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Yudofsky, S. C., Silver, J. M., Jackson, W., Endicott, J., & Williams, D.
Columbia, Canada. (1986). The Overt Aggression Scale for the objective rating of verbal
*Werner, P. D., Rose, T. L., & Yesavage, J. A. (1983). Reliability, and physical aggression American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 35–39.
accuracy, and decision-making strategy in clinical predictions of immi- *Zitrin, A., Hardesty, A. S., Burdock, E. I., & Drossman, A. K. (1976).
nent dangerousness. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, Crime and violence among mental patients. American Journal of Psy-
815– 825. chiatry, 133, 142–149.
Wessely, S. (1993). Violence and psychosis. In C. Thompson & P. Cowen Zocchetti, C., Consonni, D., & Bertazzi, P. A. (1997). Relationship be-
(Eds.), Violence: Basic and clinical science (pp. 120 –134). Oxford, tween prevalence rate ratios and odds ratios in cross-sectional studies.
England: Butterworth-Heinemann. International Journal of Epidemiology, 26, 220 –223.

(Appendix follows)
706 DOUGLAS, GUY, AND HART

Appendix

Transformational Formulas

Estimating an Odds Ratio From Frequencies Provided in 3. r, which subsequently was converted to d, was estimated from
an Article (Hasselblad & Hedges, 1995) a chi-square value (␹ 2 ) with df ⫽ 1 and total sample size (N)
(Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 201), as follows:
Status Psychosis No psychosis
|r| ⫽ 冑␹ 2/N
Violent a b 4. d was estimated from an independent t test (t) and total
Not violent c d
sample size (N) (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 198), as follows:
Note. OR ⫽ ad/bc. When a twofold table contained one or more empty
cells, 0.5 was added to each cell of the table to allow the calculation of an
d⫽ 冑2t/N 共assumes n1 ⫽ n2)
OR (Fleiss, 1994). 5. d was estimated from an independent t test (t) and the sample
size for each group (n) (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 198), as
Estimating an Odds Ratio From a Standardized Mean follows:
Difference (d) Effect Size (Hasselblad & Hedges, 1995;
d⫽ 冑t 共n1 ⫹ n2兲/共n1n2兲
see also Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 198)
6. d was estimated with an F ratio (F) from a one-way analysis
␲d/冑 3
OR ⫽ e of variance and total sample size (N) as follows (Lipsey & Wilson,
2001, p. 199):
1. If d needed to be estimated, formulas presented in Table B10
by Lipsey and Wilson (2001, pp. 198 –200) were used depending
|d| ⫽ 冑z F/N 共assumes n 1 ⫽ n 2兲
on the information presented in the article. Most commonly, stan-
Estimating an Odds Ratio From a Rate Ratio (Zocchetti,
dardized mean difference effect sizes were estimated with each of
the two means (X), the standard deviation (s), and the sample size
Consonni, & Bertazzi, 1997)
(n) for each group (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 198), as follows: POR ⫽ PRR关共1 – PrE ⫹ 共PRR ⫻ PrE 兲 – Pr D 兲/共1 ⫺ PrE 兲
d ⫽ 共X1 ⫺ X2 兲/冑spooled ⫹ 共PRR ⫻ PrE 兲 – 共PRR ⫻ PrD 兲兴,
where PrD ⫽ prevalence of psychosis, PrE ⫽ prevalence of the
spooled ⫽ 冑共n1 ⫺ 1兲s21 ⫹ 共n2 ⫺ 1兲s22/关共n1 ⫹ n2兲 ⫺ 2兴 exposure, POR ⫽ prevalence odds ratio, and PRR ⫽ prevalence
2. d was estimated from r (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 199), as rate ratio.
follows:
Received February 25, 2008

d ⫽ 2r/ 冉冑 冊1 ⫺ r2
Revision received April 9, 2009
Accepted April 20, 2009 䡲

You might also like