You are on page 1of 11

Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

DOI 10.1007/s00213-009-1564-x

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Superstitious conditioning as a model of delusion formation


following chronic but not acute ketamine in humans
Tom P. Freeman & Celia J. A. Morgan &
Elissa Klaassen & Ravi K. Das & Ana Stefanovic &
Brigitta Brandner & H. Valerie Curran

Received: 28 February 2009 / Accepted: 30 April 2009 / Published online: 13 May 2009
# Springer-Verlag 2009

Abstract Keywords Dopamine . Glutamate . Schizophrenia .


Background Ketamine has previously been shown to Modeling . Drug abuse
induce delusion-like or referential beliefs, both acutely in
healthy volunteers and naturalistically among nonintoxi-
cated users of the drug. Delusions are theoretically under- Introduction
pinned by increased superstitious conditioning or the
erroneous reinforcement of random events. Acutely, the noncompetitive NMDA-receptor antagonist
Materials and methods Using a novel and objectively ketamine induces a range of cognitive impairments and
measured superstitious conditioning task, experiment 1 psychotic-like symptoms in healthy volunteers, including
assessed healthy volunteers before and during placebo (n= delusion-like and referential beliefs (Bowdle et al. 1998;
16), low-dose (n=15), and high-dose ketamine (n=16) under Lahti et al. 2001; Pomarol-Clotet et al. 2006). Recently, it
randomized and double-blind conditions. Experiment 2 used has been suggested that acute ketamine may provide a
the same task to compare ketamine users (n=18), polydrug pharmacological model of delusion formation (Corlett et al.
controls (n=19), and nondrug-using controls (n=17). 2007; Pomarol-Clotet et al. 2006). Corlett et al. (2006)
Results In experiment 1, ketamine produced dose-dependent employed an associative learning task aiming to link
psychotomimetic effects but did not cause changes in delusion formation to mismatches between actual and
superstitious conditioning. Experiment 2 found increased expected outcomes or “prediction error” signals on an
levels of superstitious conditioning among ketamine users associative learning task. Compared to placebo, a low dose
compared to polydrug and nondrug-using controls, respec- of ketamine increased right lateral prefrontal activation
tively, as evidenced by both objective task responses and responses to expected contingencies and decreased in
subjective beliefs following the task. response to mismatches; this was interpreted as a disruption
Conclusions Results indicate that chronic but not acute in prediction error signaling. Further, these frontal
exposure to ketamine may increase the propensity to adopt responses were predictive of clinician rated delusion-like
superstitious conditioning. These findings are discussed in referential beliefs on the Present State Examination.
terms of acute and chronic ketamine models of delusion- In chronic recreational users of ketamine, a range of
like belief formation in schizophrenia. cognitive impairments and psychotic-like symptoms have
been observed (Curran and Monaghan 2001; Curran and
Morgan 2000; Morgan et al. 2004). We have recently found
evidence of elevated delusional symptoms among recrea-
T. P. Freeman (*) : C. J. A. Morgan : E. Klaassen : R. K. Das : tional ketamine users when they were not intoxicated with
A. Stefanovic : B. Brandner : H. V. Curran the drug (Morgan et al. 2009; Uhlhaas et al. 2007). Morgan
Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, Clinical Health Psychology, et al. found an apparent dose–response effect whereby
University College London,
frequent users scored higher than infrequent, ex users, and
Gower St,
London WC1E 6BT, UK nonusers, respectively. Further in the frequent users,
e-mail: tom.freeman@ucl.ac.uk delusional symptoms correlated with the amount of ket-
564 Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

amine currently being used. This group also rated greater teers aged 18–35 years were recruited through an adver-
conviction in and more preoccupation with their delusions tisement and were paid for participation. All were native
than all other groups. English speakers with no history of (a) serious medical
Given that delusions have been observed in humans conditions, (b) personal or family mental health diagnosis,
following both a single dose and repeated exposure to and (c) substance misuse. The study was approved by the
ketamine, we decided to investigate the effects of both UCL/UCLH ethics committee, and all participants provided
acute and chronic ketamine on the same assessments of written informed consent.
delusional thinking. Further, as previous studies of All participants attended a screening session (screening
ketamine-induced delusion-like beliefs have used either questionnaires, blood pressure, height, and weight) prior to
clinician or participant ratings, we designed a novel, the experimental session. Of the 58 volunteers meeting the
objective task aimed at assessing delusion formation, based inclusion criteria, ten dropped out due to adverse effects at
on the process of superstitious conditioning. the high dose. Forty-eight volunteers successfully complet-
Superstitious conditioning is a form of associative ed the testing session; however, due to a computer error,
learning first documented by Skinner (1948). Skinner data were not analyzed for one participant in the low-dose
exposed pigeons to a food reward at regular response- ketamine group. During screening, participants were
independent intervals and under the principles of operant assessed on The National Adult Reading Test (NART;
conditioning, any behavior a pigeon happened to be Nelson 1982), a full version of the Brief Psychiatric Rating
engaged in at the time the reward was delivered would Scale (BPRS; Overall and Gorham 1962) and the shortened
become positively reinforced. Accordingly, Skinner ob- Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences
served the pigeons adopting a number of strange behaviors, questionnaire (O-LIFE; Mason et al. 2005). Self-reported
including head twisting and turning on the spot. These abstinence from recent drug use was verified with urinal-
behaviors are analogous to superstitious behavior in ysis. Participants were required (1) to abstain from the use
humans; if a gambler wears a pair of socks on the night of alcohol for 24 h prior to the testing session and (2) to fast
of a big win, these socks might be worn again in the belief for morning testing from midnight, or for afternoon testing
that they are “lucky” when in fact, they were randomly for >6 h beforehand.
contingent on reward, just like the movements shown by
Skinner’s pigeons. Similarly, delusions are characterized by Drug administration Two 20-gauge intravenous cannulae
beliefs that defy a cause–effect relationship and may lie were inserted into participants’ forearms before predrug
upon the same continuum as superstitious beliefs; as such testing (one for the infusion and the other for blood samples),
superstitious conditioning may provide an analog of and 1 l of Hartmann’s solution (Baxter, UK) was administered
delusion formation (Brugger et al. 1994). over 2 h. Inserting cannulae and administering fluids early
Superstitious conditioning is thought to underpin delu- helped with tolerability of ketamine. Ketamine administration
sion formation due to chaotic firing in the mesolimbic was via a Graseby intravenous infusion pump controlled by
dopamine system (King et al. 1984; Shaner 1999). the Stan-pump program (Schafer et al. 1990). The program
However, previous demonstrations of superstitious condi- used a bolus-elimination-transfer infusion scheme based on
tioning in humans have not addressed either delusions or the Clements pharmacological model (Clements and Nimmo
pharmacological models of psychopathology (Aeschleman 1981). Steady state of predicted plasma ketamine/placebo
et al. 2003; Bloom et al. 2007; Matute 1994; Ono 1987; concentration according to the model was achieved over a
Rudski 2001). The aim of this study was to assess period of 12 min.
superstitious conditioning following acute and chronic At the beginning of the study, participants received
exposure to ketamine objectively using a novel supersti- ketamine (100 or 200 ng/ml) or saline placebo (0.9% NaCl
tious conditioning task. solution). However, due to dropouts, the dosage was modified
to 75 and 150 ng/ml. This resulted in one participant on
100 ng/ml and two participants on 200 ng/ml being included
Method in the low- and high-dose groups in the final analysis,
respectively; the remaining participants were tested on 75/
Experiment 1: acute effects of ketamine 150 ng/ml. Peripheral venous blood samples were taken at 6
and 45 min after achieving the steady state to determine
Design and participants A randomized double-blind inde- ketamine concentration in the blood (all blood samples were
pendent group design was used to compare a low dose of from the 75/150 ng/ml groups). Plasma was obtained
ketamine (target plasma level—75 ng/ml) and a high dose immediately by centrifugation and stored at −80°C. Ketamine
of ketamine (target plasma level—150 ng/ml) with placebo levels were measured using gas chromatography (C3P
(saline). Groups were balanced for gender. Healthy volun- Analysis Lab, Plymouth, UK).
Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573 565

Assessments Questionnaire phase To index subjective delusion-like


beliefs formed during the task, participants were given a
Superstitious conditioning task post-task questionnaire asking: “in the first part of the task—
Experimental phase: the experimental phase (see Fig. 1) where you were given points—did you notice any patterns?”
was a 108-trial choice task which employed a response- Participants were given the following answers to choose from:
independent reward contingency. Following a central “yes,” “no,” “not sure,” or “no, they were there but I didn’t
fixation cross shown for 1,000 ms, one of the three possible find them.” Participants who responded “yes” to this question
stimuli appeared to the left of the cross, and two (of the were asked to describe the nature of the “patterns” they had
same three possible) stimuli appeared above and below a noticed.
question mark, to the right of the cross. Participants chose,
through trial and error learning, the stimulus on the right
they believed formed the pair with the stimulus on the left Subjective effects (pre- and during infusion)
likely to result in the highest number of points. Pairs could
be formed using identical and nonidentical stimuli, in two The psychomimetic states inventory (PSI; Mason et al.
possible locations, giving a total of nine possible stimulus 2008) assesses state schizotypy induced by psychotomi-
pairs in each version of the task. Two versions of the task metic drugs. The PSI contains 48 alternative forced choice
were completed (pre- and post-infusion), each consisting of questions ranging in agreement from 0 (not at all) to 3
different stimuli (yellow, blue, and red stars in version 1, (strongly) and has subscales of delusory thinking, perceptual
and red triangles, circles, and squares in version 2). The distortions, cognitive disorganization, anhedonia, mania, and
order of completion of the two task versions was counter- paranoia. Participants were assessed on three key positive
balanced between participants. and four key negative items of the BPRS (see Krystal et al.
Following each response, feedback was provided, which 1994), yielding scores for positive symptoms, negative
showed the chosen stimulus pair and the amount of points symptoms, anxious depression, and activation.
awarded. In order to prevent excessive salience being
attributed to rewarded versus nonrewarded pairs and to Procedure After cannulation on the testing day, baseline
obtain a continuous measure of superstitious conditioning, blood pressure and heart rate measures were taken followed
every pair was rewarded equally over the course of the task, by pre-infusion tasks (PSI, key BPRS items, superstitious
ranging from 20 to 100 points in 20-point intervals. Each conditioning task) and then the start of the infusion. After
level of points awarded was accompanied by a distinct the target plasma concentration was theoretically achieved,
“ching” cash register sound: 20 points was accompanied by post-drug testing commenced (PSI, key BPRS items,
one “ching,” 40 points by two “chings,” and so on, with the superstitious conditioning task). In addition to the reported
amplitude of the sound also increasing in intensity according battery of tests, participants also completed two semantic
to the number of points rewarded. Responses were recorded priming tasks, the results of which are reported in
in terms of the frequency with which each stimulus pair was Stefanovic et al. (2009). After testing, participants were
chosen. Practice was given pre-infusion using a “demonstra- provided with refreshments and discharged when “street
tion version,” in which pairs were chosen from three ready.”
different stimuli (flower cartoons). Following five practice
trials with the demonstration version, participants began the
learning phase. Superstitious conditioning was indexed by Experiment 2: Chronic self-administered ketamine
the frequency of pair choice. Choices were ranked for
frequency and due to limited spread in the data, superstitious Design and participants An independent group design was
conditioning was computed as the frequency of most chosen used to compare (1) ketamine users who reported using
pair minus frequency of least chosen pair. Ranges were ketamine more than twice a month for at least 6 months, (2)
calculated for individual blocks and over all trials. polydrug controls, reporting regular drug use other than
ketamine (e.g., ecstasy, amphetamine, cocaine, cannabis) but
Response who were either ketamine naïve or who had not used ketamine
Fixation Stimuli Feedback more than three times in their life, (3) nondrug-using controls
who reported to have never used illicit drugs. Participants
were recruited using snowball sampling (Solowij et al. 1992).
+ + ? + Inclusion criteria were English as a first language and normal
1000 ms 1000 ms 20 Points! or corrected to normal sight. Exclusion criteria were a family
Fig. 1 Trial sequence of the experimental phase (version 1) in the or personal history of mental health problems or drug
superstitious conditioning task dependence. The study was approved by the UCL Graduate
566 Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

School ethics committee. All participants provided written onal contrasts compared (1) ketamine groups with placebo
informed consent. and (2) high-dose with low-dose ketamine (experiment 1);
(1) drug users with nondrug-using controls and (2) ket-
Assessments amine users with polydrug controls (experiment 2).

Self-ratings Participants were assessed on trait schizotypy


(shortened O-LIFE; Mason et al. 2005), and on the “spot
the word task” as a correlate of premorbid verbal IQ Results
(Baddeley et al. 1993).
Experiment 1: acute effects of ketamine
Superstitious conditioning task The task used was identical
to that described in experiment 1 (version 2). Demographics (Table 1) Groups did not differ in age,
gender, NART, O-LIFE, or BPRS scores.
Procedure Participants were tested in a quiet room at their
own homes. Written, witnessed informed consent was Plasma ketamine levels Mean ketamine plasma concen-
obtained before testing. A semistructured interview was trations were 39.37±23.89 ng/ml in the low dose and
carried out concerning participants’ psychiatric history and 115.08±54.72 ng/ml in the high-dose groups 6 min after
lifetime drug use in order to meet inclusion criteria, and the steady state was achieved; at 45 min, they were 54.64±
urinalysis was used to confirm that reports of recent drug 17.57 ng/ml (low dose) and 143.42±60.67 ng/ml (high
use were accurate. Participants were then given the “spot dose). Norketamine plasma concentrations at 6 min were
the word” task, followed by the superstitious conditioning 4.2±6.32 ng/ml (low dose) and 12.17±9.85 ng/ml (high
task (training, learning phase, and questionnaire phase). dose); at 45 min, they were 22.24±8.99 ng/ml (low dose)
Finally, they completed the O-LIFE. Participants also and 50.5±32.62 (high dose).
completed two semantic priming tasks, reported elsewhere
(Stefanovic et al. 2009). Superstitious conditioning task

Statistical analyses All analyses were performed using Data from the superstitious conditioning task were missing
Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPPS Version for one participant in the low-dose ketamine group;
11.5). Group differences in the experimental phase of the therefore, analysis of this task was completed on n=15 in
superstitious conditioning tasks were examined using one- this group and n=16 in the other two groups.
way ANOVAs (Kruskall–Wallis tests where data were
nonparametric) and RMANOVAs. If transformations did Experimental phase (Table 2) The experimental phase of
not yield homogenous variance, RMANOVAs were con- the task was examined in terms of the frequency with which
ducted using nonparametric data. Bonferroni post hoc tests each of the nine pairs was chosen in each of the three
and Bonferroni-corrected Mann–Whitney U tests were used blocks of trials. The range between the most and least
to explore simple effects where a significant effect of group frequently chosen pairs was used to index superstitious
was revealed. Chi-squared tests were also used to explore conditioning, with a high range indicating a high level of
dichotomous data. Bonferroni-corrected Pearson correla- superstitious conditioning. A 3×3×2×2 RMANOVA was
tional analyses were conducted between ketamine use, trait performed with Block (1, 2, and 3 ) and Time (pre- and
schizotypy, and superstitious conditioning. Planned orthog- post-infusion) as within subjects factors and Group (place-

Table 1 Group means (SD) of


participants’ demographic data Placebo Low dose High dose Group main effect
in experiment 1
Age 24.71 (4.47) 23.10 (3.18) 25.65 (4.29) NS
Gender (N male/female) 8:8 8:8 8:8 NS
NART 113.38 (5.24) 112.19 (4.13) 115.06 (4.42) NS
BPRS 27.56 (2.39) 28.50 (3.01) 26.69 (1.92) NS
O-LIFE (total) 10.25 (5.15) 7.38 (5.14) 9.00 (6.43) NS
Unusual experiences 2.31 (2.09) 1.06 (1.69) 2.31 (2.18) NS
Cognitive disorganization 4.19 (2.51) 3.12 (2.47) 3.13 (2.83) NS
Impulsive nonconformity 2.63 (2.00) 2.06 (1.53) 2.69 (1.85) NS
Introvertive anhedonia 1.13 (1.41) 1.13 (1.26) 0.88 (0.89) NS
Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573 567

Table 2 Group means (SD) in


levels of superstitious Group Superstitious conditioning
conditioning on the
experimental phase across Block 1 Block 2 Block 3 All trials
individual blocks and all trials in
experiments 1 and 2 Experiment 1
Placebo Pre 5.50 (1.21) 5.38 (1.09) 4.88 (1.67) 11.19 (3.31)
Post 4.81 (1.11) 5.44 (1.03) 5.63 (1.41) 11.06 (3.40)
Low dose Pre 5.60 (1.18) 5.73 (1.71) 5.67 (1.40) 12.80 (4.70)
Post 5.27 (1.03) 5.67 (1.50) 5.53 (1.00) 12.00 (3.89)
High dose Pre 5.19 (1.33) 5.00 (1.67) 4.94 (1.34) 10.31 (3.65)
Post 5.88 (5.26) 5.25 (1.61) 5.50 (1.26) 12.06 (3.49)
Experiment 2
Ketamine users 5.56 (1.29) 6.72 (1.78) 6.00 (1.61) 18.28 (3.36)
Polydrug controls 5.37 (1.50) 5.47 (1.47) 4.89 (1.49) 15.74 (2.94)
Nondrug-using controls 3.94 (2.75) 5.82 (2.00) 5.06 (2.05) 11.53 (5.42)

bo, low dose, and high dose) and Order (version 1 first or Contrasts showed greater changes in the ketamine groups
version 2 first) as between subjects factors. No main effects compared to placebo (t45 =−4.49, p<0.001), and in the high-
were found for Block, Time, Group, or Order, and no dose compared to low-dose ketamine group (t45 =−4.28, p<
significant interactions emerged. 0.001). Negative symptoms: Analysis of negative symptoms
revealed a Group×Time interaction and main effects of
Questionnaire phase (Table 3) Due to low expected frequen- Group and Time. Subjects receiving placebo were at floor
cies, responses were collapsed into non-superstitious responses level at both pre-infusion and during the infusion. Contrasts
(“no” and “not sure”) and superstitious responses (“no, there again showed greater changes in the ketamine groups
were patterns but I didn’t find them” and “Yes”). Chi-squared compared to placebo (t45 =−2.53, p<0.05), and in the high-
tests showed no significant Group or Time effects. dose compared to the low-dose group (t45 =−2.51, p<0.05).
Anxious depression: There was a Group×Time interaction
and a main effect of Time. Contrasts between placebo and
Subjective effects (Table 4) ketamine groups revealed no significant ketamine effect.
Activation: No significant effects or interactions were found.
BPRS Neither ketamine nor norketamine plasma levels correlated
Positive symptoms: A 3×2-RMANOVA revealed a Group× with increased BPRS scores in the high-dose group.
Time interaction and main effects of Group and Time.
PSI
Table 3 Number of participants showing superstitious conditioning Perceptual distortions: A 3×2-RMANOVA revealed a
according to post-task responses for experiments 1 and 2 Group×Time interaction and a main effect of Time. Contrasts
Group Questionnaire response (n) revealed a significantly higher increase in scores during the
infusion in the ketamine groups than in placebo (t45 =−2.4,
Superstitious Non-superstitious p<0.05). Cognitive Disorganisation: There was a significant
Group×Time interaction and a main effect of Time. Contrasts
Experiment 1
showed a significantly higher increase in scores during the
Placebo Pre 13 3
infusion in both ketamine groups compared to the placebo
Post 13 3
group (t45 =−2.82, p<0.01). Delusory Thinking: There was a
Low dose Pre 11 4
main effect of time, indicating more distortion pre-infusion
Post 10 5
than during the infusion. No effects were found for
High dose Pre 12 4
Anhedonia or Mania, and scores on Paranoia were at a floor
Post 13 3
level. Neither ketamine nor norketamine plasma levels
Experiment 2
correlated with increased PSI scores in the ketamine groups.
Ketamine users 15 4
Polydrug controls 8 11 Correlations Within the two ketamine groups, Pearson
Non-drug using 4 13 correlations were conducted. In order to reduce the chance
controls
of type I error, these were conducted between degree of
568 Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

Table 4 Group means (SD) for subjective effects pre- and during the infusion

Subjective effect Group and time Group × Group Time


time
Placebo Low dose High dose

Pre During Pre During Pre During F (2,45) F (2,45) F (1,45)

BPRS
Positive 4.63 (0.72) 4.19 (0.54) 4.69 (0.87) 4.94 (0.77) 4.44 (0.63) 6.38 (1.36) 19.25*** 9.51*** 13.15***
symptoms
Negative 3.00 (0.00) 3.00 (0.00) 3.25 (0.58) 3.63 (1.03) 3.13 (0.34) 4.50 (1.86) 6.33** 5.21** 12.78***
symptoms
Anxious 7.69 (1.45) 6.44 (0.63) 8.31 (1.96) 6.81 (0.91) 7.06 (1.44) 7.75 (1.29) 8.43*** NS 8.34**
depression
Activation 3.13 (0.34) 3.25 (0.45) 3.06 (0.25) 3.06 (0.25) 3.31 (0.60) 3.31 (0.60) NS NS NS
PSI
Perceptual 1.00 (1.90) 1.31 (2.02) 0.56 (0.96) 2.44 (2.48) 1.25 (2.41) 3.94 (3.11) 3.25* NS 17.68***
distortions
Cognitive 6.50 (4.66) 5.50 (4.78) 4.13 (3.01) 6.25 (4.30) 5.13 (4.47) 9.44 (5.13) 4.78* NS 6.61*
disorganization
Delusory thinking 2.25 (2.44) 0.94 (1.34) 1.38 (2.00) 1.31 (2.24) 2.13 (2.19) 1.19 (1.68) NS NS 9.93**
Anhedonia 4.69 (2.52) 5.13 (2.71) 4.56 (3.01) 4.63 (2.47) 4.12 (2.16) 5.63 (3.91) NS NS NS
Mania 3.31 (1.54) 3.63 (1.63) 3.00 (1.32) 3.00 (1.71) 3.63 (2.94) 3.44 (1.71) NS NS NS
Paranoia 1.44 (1.46) 0.63 (1.31) 0.69 (1.01) 1.19 (2.29) 0.94 (0.77) 0.56 (0.73) NS NS NS

*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001

superstitious conditioning (range in frequency of pair choice) regular use of illicit drugs. The only significant difference
across all blocks both pre-infusion and during the infusion and was that ketamine users reported using cannabis for more
the delusory thinking and paranoia subscales of the PSI. With years than polydrug controls (F1,36 =4.59, p<0.05).
the alpha level Bonferroni-adjusted to 0.0125, only one All ketamine users reported regular use of ketamine,
significant correlation emerged, and this was between pre- administering a mean of 0.99 (±0.85) grams per session, for
infusion performance on the superstitious conditioning task 3.30 (±2.92) years, 17.80 (±11.05) days per month, and last
and paranoia during the infusion (r=−0.458, p=0.01). There used the drug 3.74 (±5.10) days before testing. Seven
was also a trend for a correlation between level of polydrug controls reported to have tried ketamine between
superstitious conditioning pre-infusion and delusory thinking 28 days and a year prior to testing (mean 111.60 days, ±124),
post-drug (r=−0.408, p=0.023). but none had used it more than three times in their life. A
number of participants reported to have tried other drugs but
did not use them regularly.
Experiment 2: Chronic self-administered ketamine
Superstitious conditioning task Data were missing for one
Demographics (Table 5) Groups did not differ in age, gender, participant from the ketamine group so data from the
or spot-the-word scores. A one-way ANOVA found group superstitious conditioning task were analyzed with 18
differences in unusual experiences, cognitive disorganization, ketamine users.
impulsive nonconformity, and introvertive anhedonia.
Bonferroni-corrected contrasts showed that unusual experi- Experimental phase (Table 2) As in experiment 1, the
ences was significantly higher among polydrug controls range in frequency of pair choice was used to index
compared to healthy controls (p<0.01), cognitive disorgani- superstitious conditioning in each of the three blocks of
zation was higher among ketamine users compared to the experimental phase. A 3×3-RMANOVA with Block
healthy controls (p<0.001), and introvertive anhedonia was as a within subjects factor and Group as a between
higher among healthy controls compared to both ketamine subjects factor revealed a main effect of Group (F2,51 =
users (p<0.001) and polydrug controls (p<0.05). 4.334, p<0.05) and a main effect of Block (F2,102 =5.706,
p<0.01) but no significant interaction. Planned compar-
Drug use (Table 6) Urinalysis results were consistent with isons showed that drug users (ketamine users and poly-
recently reported drug use. Healthy controls reported no drug controls) showed greater levels of superstitious
Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573 569

Table 5 Group means (SD) of participant demographic data in experiment 2

Ketamine users Polydrug controls Nondrug-using controls Group main effect

Age 21.00 (2.24) 20.95 (1.03) 21.23 (4.66) NS


Gender (N male/female) 16:3 10:9 10:7 NS
Spot the word 49.00 (3.11) 46.47 (9.20) 49.24 (4.52) NS
O-LIFE (total) 16.42 (4.19) 17.16 (6.53) 13.53 (5.94) NS
Unusual experiences 3.47 (1.95) 5.00 (3.09) 2.35 (2.59) F(2,52) =4.759*
Cognitive disorganization 6.84 (2.48) 5.00 (2.76) 3.12 (2.23) F(2,52) =9.864***
Impulsive nonconformity 4.32 (1.70) 4.58 (2.12) 3.82 (2.04) NS
Introvertive anhedonia 1.79 (1.87) 2.58 (1.92) 4.25 (1.95) F(2,52) =7.783***

*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001

conditioning—as indexed by a greater range in frequency Discussion


of pair choice—than healthy controls (p<0.01) and that
ketamine users showed higher levels of superstitious The main findings of this experiment were firstly that
conditioning than polydrug controls (p<0.05). Increased ketamine users and, to a lesser extent, polydrug controls
levels of superstitious conditioning among ketamine showed increased levels of superstitious conditioning
users compared to polydrug controls were still reliable compared to nondrug-using controls. This was evidenced
when years of cannabis use was entered as a covariate. both by their task responses in the objective learning phase
Years of cannabis use did not reach significance as a of the task and in their subjective responses about perceived
covariate, and no significant interactions emerged. Post patterns following the task. Secondly, although acutely
hoc analysis of Block with the alpha level adjusted to ketamine induced a range of psychotomimetic effects in
0.017 indicated that superstitious conditioning was healthy volunteers, it did not produce changes in supersti-
greater in block 2 than block 1 (p<0.01). A trend was tious conditioning compared with placebo.
also found for lower superstitious conditioning in block 3
compared to block 2 (p=0.064).
Table 6 Numbers of users and means (SD) for extent of drug use in
Questionnaire phase (Table 3) As in experiment 1, due to ketamine and polydrug control groups
low expected frequencies, answers were collapsed
Drug Ketamine users Polydrug controls
between non-superstitious responses (“no” and “not
sure”) and superstitious responses (“no, there were Cannabis
patterns but I didn’t find them” and “yes”). There were Number of users 19 19
significant group differences in the frequency of Last use (days) 125.00 (2.95) 77.10 (257.66)
superstitious and non-superstitious responses (χ2(2)= Amount per session (joints) 2.92 (3.04) 2.50 (1.80)
11.592, p< 0.01) with ketamine users reporting more Days used per month 14.53 (11.41) 17.39 (11.22)
patterns than polydrug controls (p<0.05) and polydrug Years used 4.45 (1.98) 2.81 (2.88)*
controls reporting more patterns than nondrug-using Ecstasy
controls (p<0.05). Number of users 19 17
Last use (days) 124.26 (250.70) 24.38 (23.92)
Correlations Within the ketamine group, Pearson corre- Amount per session (mg) 382.00 (252.65) 279.41 (196.10)
lations were conducted between degree of superstitious Days used per month 2.32 (2.26) 1.12 (1.53)
conditioning (range in frequency of pair choice) across Years used 2.70 (2.63) 1.28 (1.24)
all blocks, cognitive disorganization, introvertive anhe- Cocaine
donia, last use of ketamine, amount of ketamine used Number of users 16 17
per session, days per month of ketamine use, and days Last use (days) 117.94 (213.57) 159.00 (267.54)
since last use of ketamine. With the alpha level
Amount per session (mg) 387.50 (457.62) 217.68 (257.33)
Bonferroni adjusted to 0.00625 only one significant
Days used per month 0.25 (0.45) 0.77 (1.11)
correlation emerged and this was between cognitive
Years used 1.06 (3.34) 0.50 (0.97)
disorganization and days since last use of ketamine
(r=−0.646, p=0.003). *p<0.05
570 Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

Acute ketamine and superstitious conditioning levels of superstitious conditioning compared to healthy
controls. Increased levels of superstitious conditioning
Acutely, ketamine produced a number of psychotomimetic among ketamine users compared to polydrug controls, and
effects compared to placebo, as evidenced by dose- among all drug users compared to healthy controls, were
dependent increases in positive symptoms, negative symp- evidenced by a greater range in the frequency of pair
toms and anxious depression on the BPRS, and increased choices in the experimental phase and a greater number of
perceptual distortions and cognitive disorganization on the self reported “patterns” observed during the task. Experi-
PSI. These findings replicate similar observations following ment 2 provides the first evidence that repeated use of
acute ketamine on the BPRS (Krystal et al. 1994; Malhotra ketamine is associated with changes in superstitious
et al. 1996; Newcomer et al. 1999). conditioning. Taken together with the incidence of delu-
No group differences were found for delusory thinking and sions in ketamine users observed in previous studies
across the groups, scores on this subscale were higher pre- (Morgan et al. 2009; Uhlhaas et al. 2007), these findings
infusion than during the infusion. In addition, paranoia was at may provide preliminary support for the theory that
a floor level in all three groups, and no differences in superstitious conditioning underpins delusion-like belief
superstitious conditioning were observed, according to both formation (King et al. 1984; Shaner 1999).
the stimuli chosen in the experimental phase and post-task Participants did not differ in premorbid verbal IQ, age, or
reports in the questionnaire phase. These results differ from gender, and the two drug-using groups were broadly
those of Corlett et al. (2006), who found that a low dose of matched for other drug use other than ketamine. However,
ketamine-disrupted prediction error signaling, visible as the groups showed differences in trait schizotypy. Both
changes in right lateral prefrontal cortical activation. Howev- ketamine users and polydrug controls exhibited high levels
er, the behavioral data collected in the current study may have of positive schizotypy and low levels of negative schizo-
not been sensitive enough to detect equivalent changes in typy compared to nondrug-using controls, consistent with
superstitious conditioning, and since Corlett and colleagues previous findings among recreational drug users (Nunn et
did not index delusion-like beliefs at the initial low dose of al. 2001). Polydrug controls showed higher levels of
ketamine, it is difficult to compare these findings with ours. unusual experiences compared to ketamine users, which
Despite the absence of differences between the groups in was unexpected. Ketamine users scored more highly on
either symptoms or superstitious conditioning, we did find a cognitive disorganization, an analog of thought disorder in
relationship between ketamine-evoked symptoms and base- subclinical populations. This factor of the O-LIFE was
line levels of superstitious conditioning. Within the ketamine highly correlated with the recency of ketamine use, and
groups in experiment 1, pre-infusion superstitious condition- these findings concur with previous observations of thought
ing (range in frequency of pair choice) correlated negatively disorder among ketamine users (Curran and Morgan 2000;
with scores on the paranoia subscale of the PSI during the Morgan et al. 2009).
infusion, and a trend was found for a negative correlation
between pre-infusion superstitious conditioning and post-drug The superstitious conditioning task and delusion formation
delusory thinking. Intriguingly, this may parallel findings of
Corlett et al. (2006), who showed that increased activation in Pre-infusion levels of superstitious conditioning in experi-
the rPFC to prediction error violation during placebo was ment 1 were related to subsequent ketamine-induced
predictive of post-drug increases in related symptoms on the symptoms; however, in experiment 2, psychotic-like symp-
PSE. One of the interpretations the authors proposed was that toms in the ketamine group did not correlate with levels of
individuals who are more sensitive to prediction errors may be superstitious conditioning. This leaves some ambiguity as
more vulnerable to the perturbation of this system by to whether the superstitious conditioning task is indeed
ketamine. This explanation may hold true for experiment 1 tapping the early processes of delusion formation. To assess
in that individuals who are more sensitive to prediction errors, delusions fully in ketamine users, future studies should
evident here as lower superstitious conditioning pre drug, may employ an additional measure such as the Peters Delusion
be more susceptible to perturbation of this system by Inventory (Peters et al. 1999).
ketamine, observed in this study as greater ketamine-induced Ketamine users in experiment 2 reported using the drug
paranoia and delusory thinking. less (1.0 g/session for 3.3 years) than in Morgan et al. (2009),
in which frequent users reported to have used 3.8 g/session
Chronic self-administered ketamine and superstitious for 6.1 years, and infrequent users reported to have used
conditioning 1.3 g/session for 3.7 years. The observed group differences
in superstitious conditioning given relatively light ketamine
The main findings of experiment 2 were that ketamine users use in experiment 2 may be in line with the theory that
and, to a lesser extent, polydrug controls showed increased superstitious beliefs lie on a continuum with delusions
Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573 571

(Brugger et al. 1994). However, future research comparing superstitious conditioning or delusion-like symptoms were
heavy and light ketamine users or patient groups with observed following acute ketamine in the current study.
varying levels of delusional symptoms is needed to clarify These apparent discrepancies may be due to different rating
this matter. The superstitious conditioning task we scales used in different studies, which Pomarol-Clotet et al.
employed is language-independent, easy to administer, and (2006) attempted to overcome using clinical interviews
yields objective behavioral results. It could have a number based on the Present State Examination. However, clinical
of practical applications if validated, for example, the interviews were beyond the scope of the current study.
assessment of the early onset of delusions, a stage of the Further, it should be noted that previous studies using
disorder that is difficult to trace since the symptoms prior steady-state infusions have aimed for higher target plasma
to their formation are difficult to conceptualize (Jorgensen ketamine levels of 200 ng/ml (Corlett et al. 2006; Pomarol-
1998). Clotet et al. 2006) compared to 150 ng/ml in the current
Differences in performance observed on this task might study.
alternatively be understood in terms of the exploitation Based on the limited measures of psychopathology used
versus exploration dilemma (for a review, see Cohen et al. in this study, the findings from experiments 1 and 2 suggest
2007). During our task, participants had to choose whether that chronic ketamine may provide a better model of one of
to exploit previous reward contingent choices by choosing the processes theoretically involved in delusion formation
the same stimulus pairs repeatedly or to explore other pairs than acute ketamine. The findings of experiment 2 are
that may or may not have a higher reward contingency. supportive of previous evidence of self-reported delusions
Over the course of the task, a dominant pattern of in recreational ketamine users and of both human and an
exploitation rather than exploration would be reflected by animal data suggesting that chronic exposure to NMDA
stereotyped responding to certain stimuli and increased receptor antagonists is better able to model some aspects of
levels of superstitious conditioning. Similarly, schizophren- psychosis than acute exposure (Jentsch and Roth 1999;
ic patients show “strategic stiffness” or the impaired Morgan and Curran 2006; Mouri et al. 2007). Furthermore,
integration of choice outcomes over long periods of time it has been shown that repeated exposure to phencyclidine
during decision-making tasks (Kim et al. 2007). In common can cause dysregulation of dopamine afferents to the
with other theories of delusion formation (Corlett et al. nucleus accumbens of rats (Jentsch et al. 1998), a
2007; Kapur 2003), we believe that a failure to adapt to pathophysiology thought to underlie delusion formation in
changes in reward contingencies in one’s environment will superstitious conditioning theories (King et al. 1984;
create a world of unpredictable circumstances, and delu- Shaner 1999) and others (reviewed by Corlett et al. 2007).
sional beliefs are, thus, constructed in order to rationalize Despite previous findings of delusion-like beliefs fol-
these bizarre yet seemingly important events. lowing acute ketamine and the clear advantages of acute
challenge studies over cross-sectional drug-user studies (for
Superstitious conditioning among polydrug controls example high control, within-subject manipulations, and the
use of healthy volunteers) a temporary pharmacological
Polydrug controls showed higher levels of superstitious manipulation may be limited in its contextual validity of
conditioning than healthy controls. This is not surprising delusions in psychosis. Repeated exposure to ketamine, or
given that they reported regular use of other psychotomi- other psychotomimetic drugs, may provide a better model
metic drugs. Cannabis is considered an independent risk of delusion formation since the nature of regular drug use
factor in the development of psychosis (Moore et al. 2007), can precipitate a slow onset of psychotomimetic change, as
and elevated schizotypy and delusions have previously is the case in the emergence of psychosis. This possibility is
been reported by recreational cannabis users (Nunn et al. supported by recent findings that ketamine users showed a
2001). Meanwhile cocaine-induced psychosis (CIP) is well profile of affective, perceptual, attentional, and cognitive
established, and cocaine-dependent users can suffer from disturbances that was remarkably similar to that of
transient delusions (Cubells et al. 2005). On the other hand, prodromal individuals who later transitioned to psychosis
chronic MDMA use is not thought to be associated with (Morgan et al. submitted).
increased levels of delusions (Montoya et al. 2002).
Limitations and future research
Acute and chronic ketamine as models of delusions
This study has a number of limitations. In experiment 1, a
Acute ketamine has previously been shown to induce high dropout rate meant that the planned target plasma
delusion-like and referential beliefs among healty volun- levels could not be used, and this might have reduced the
teers (Bowdle et al. 1998; Lahti et al. 2001; Pomarol-Clotet psychotomimetic effects of acute ketamine. Initially, we
et al. 2006; Corlett et al. 2006). However no changes in aimed for 200 ng/ml plasma levels of ketamine in the high-
572 Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573

dose group and 100 ng/ml in the low-dose group. However, reinforcers elicit striatal activation in response to prediction
due to a high dropout rate, these were reduced to 150 and error (Jensen et al. 2007). A superstitious conditioning task
75 ng/ml, respectively. Nevertheless, increased BPRS and might, therefore, be more sensitive to the formation of
PSI scores post-infusion reflect the successful induction of delusional beliefs if it incorporated positive and negative
psychotomimetic symptoms among both the low- and high- reinforcement schedules. Finally, differential financial
dose groups compared to placebo. reward based on task responses has been incorporated into
Experiment 2 relied on naturalistic sampling of drug salience attribution tasks used to index delusion-like beliefs
users and, hence, may be confounded by other drug use, (Murray et al. 2008; Roiser et al. 2009). Future research
problems in verifying drug use, and pre-existing differences should, therefore, aim to determine whether superstitious
(Curran 2000). The observed group differences in trait conditioning is more readily adopted when money—rather
schizotypy are suggestive of pre-existing psychosis- than “points”— is utilized as a noncontingent reinforcer.
proneness, which might have accounted for group differ-
ences in superstitious conditioning. This possibility cannot Summary of findings
be ruled out since a longitudinal or prospective design was
not used. Because ketamine users were matched with In summary, the main findings of this study were that
polydrug controls for other reported drug use, it is likely repeated exposure to ketamine and, to a lesser extent,
that the heightened levels of superstitious conditioning seen polydrug use increased the propensity to adopt superstitious
among ketamine users can be explained predominantly by conditioning, while acute ketamine did not induce changes
the chronic effects of ketamine. However, the interactive in delusory thinking or superstitious conditioning among
effects of ketamine with other drugs cannot be ruled out. healthy volunteers. This study, therefore, replicated previ-
Further research should assess superstitious conditioning in ous findings of delusions in ketamine users using a novel,
cannabis- and cocaine-only groups in order to further objective measure and subjective post-experimental reports.
explore this issue. Findings of increased superstitious conditioning among
The questionnaire phase of the superstitious conditioning polydrug users are in line with the psychotomimetic
task may be limited in a number of ways. Firstly, since this profiles of cannabis and cocaine. If clinically validated,
measure was included following completion of the task, it our novel task assessing superstitious conditioning may
reflects a retrospective overview of task responses over all have important practical applications.
three blocks. In experiment 2, superstitious conditioning
increased between blocks 1 and 2 and appeared to decrease
by block 3. This suggests that perceived reward magnitude References
changed during the course of the task. Including a
subjective measure of self-reported patterns following each
block of trials may have given a more accurate indicator of Aeschleman SR, Rosen CC, Williams MR (2003) The effect of non-
superstitious beliefs during different stages of the task. contingent negative and positive reinforcement operations on
the acquisition of superstitious behaviors. Behav Processes
Another limitation is that participants did not rate their 61:37–45
confidence in their responses. Evaluation apprehension may Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I (1993) The spot the word test:
have led some participants to report patterns which they a robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision.
were unsure of, and crucially, the experimental conditions Br J Clin Psychol 32:55–65
Bloom CM, Venard J, Harden M, Seetharaman S (2007) Non-
of experiments 1 and 2 may have accounted for differences contingent positive and negative reinforcement schedules of
in these responses. Speculatively, the experimental con- superstitious behaviors. Behav Processes 75:8–13
ditions of experiment 1, in which participants were assessed Bowdle TA, Radant AD, Cowley DS, Kharasch ED, Strassman RJ,
under blind procedures in hospital and were connected to Roy-Byrne PP (1998) Psychedelic effects of ketamine in healthy
volunteers: relationship to steady-state plasma concentrations.
various heart rate and blood pressure monitors as well as Anesthesiology 88:82–88
the intravenous infusion, may have encouraged participants Brugger P, Dowdy MA, Graves RE (1994) From superstitious
to be more suggestible that they had seen patterns at this behavior to delusional thinking: the role of the hippocampus in
stage of the task. In comparison, participants in experiment misattributions of causality. Med Hypotheses 43:397–402
Clements JA, Nimmo WS (1981) Pharmacokinetics and analgesic
2 were tested in the familiar environment of their own effect of ketamine in man. Br J Anaesth 53:27–30
homes and may have felt less pressure to respond in a Cohen JD, McClure SM, Yu AJ (2007) Should I stay or should I go?
particular way. How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation
It has been noted that negative reinforcement schedules and exploration. Phil Trans R Soc B 362:933–942
Corlett PR, Honey GD, Aitken MR, Dickinson A, Shanks DR,
can lead people to believe they have more control over a Absalom AR, Lee M, Pomarol-Clotet E, Murray GK, McKenna
response-independent contingency (Aeschleman et al. PJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET, Fletcher PC (2006) Frontal
2003; Bloom et al. 2007) and both positive and negative responses during learning predict vulnerability to the psychoto-
Psychopharmacology (2009) 206:563–573 573

genic effects of ketamine: linking cognition, brain activity, and psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic
psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:611–621 review. Lancet 370:319–328
Corlett PR, Honey GD, Fletcher PC (2007) From prediction error to Morgan CJ, Curran HV (2006) Acute and chronic effects of ketamine
psychosis: ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. J upon human memory: a review. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
Psychopharmacol 21:238–252 188:408–424
Cubells JF, Feinn R, Pearson D, Burda J, Tang Y, Farrer LA, Gelernter Morgan CJA, Monaghan L, Curran HV (2004) Beyond the K-
J, Kranzler HR (2005) Rating the severity and character of hole: a 3-year longitudinal investigation of the cognitive and
transient cocaine-induced delusions and hallucinations with a subjective effects of ketamine in recreational users who have
new instrument, the Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms substantially reduced their use of the drug. Addiction
for Cocaine-Induced Psychosis (SAPS-CIP). Drug Alcohol 99:1450–1461
Depend 80:23–33 Morgan CJ, Muetzelfeldt L, Curran HV (2009) Ketamine use,
Curran HV (2000) Is MDMA (‘Ecstasy’) neurotoxic in humans? An cognition and psychological wellbeing: a comparison of frequent,
overview of evidence and of methodological problems in infrequent and ex-users with polydrug and non-using controls.
research. Neuropsychobiology 42:34–41 Addiction 104:77–87
Curran HV, Monaghan L (2001) In and out of the K-hole: a Mouri A, Noda Y, Enomoto T, Nabeshima T (2007) Phencyclidine
comparison of the acute and residual effects of ketamine in animal models of schizophrenia: approaches from abnormality of
frequent and infrequent ketamine users. Addiction 96:749–760 glutamatergic neurotransmission and neurodevelopment. Neuro-
Curran HV, Morgan CJA (2000) Cognitive, dissociative and psycho- chem Int 51:173–184
togenic effects of ketamine on recreational users on the night of Murray GK, Corlett PR, Clark L, Pessiglione M, Blackwell AD,
drug use and 3 days later. Addiction 95:575–590 Honey G, Jones PB, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Fletcher PC
Jensen J, Smith AJ, Willeit M, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Vitcu I, (2008) Substantia nigra/ventral tegmental reward prediction
Kapur S (2007) Separate brain regions code for salience vs. error disruption in psychosis. Mol Psychiatry 13:239267–
valence during reward prediction in humans. Hum Brain Mapp 239276
28:294–302 Nelson HE (1982) National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual.
Jentsch JD, Roth RH (1999) The neuropsychopharmacology of NFER, Nelson, Windsor
phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the Newcomer JW, Farber NB, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Selke G,
dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacol- Melson AK, Hershey T, Craft S, Olney JW (1999)
ogy 30:201–225 Ketamine-Induced NMDA receptor hypofunction as a model
Jentsch JD, Taylor JR, Roth RH (1998) Subchronic phencyclidine of memory impairment and psychosis. Neuropsychopharma-
administration increases mesolimbic dopaminergic system cology 20:106–118
responsivity and augments stress- and psychostimulant-induced Nunn JA, Rizza F, Peters ER (2001) The incidence of schizotypy among
hyperlocomotion. Neuropsychopharmacology 19:105–113 cannabis and alcohol users. J Nerv Ment Dis 189:741–748
Jorgensen P (1998) Schizophrenic delusions: the detection of warning Ono K (1987) Superstitious behavior in humans. J Exp Anal Behav
signals. Schizophr Res 32:17–22 47:261–271
Kapur S (2003) Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework Overall JE, Gorham DR (1962) The brief psychiatric rating scale.
linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizo- Psychol Rep 10:799–812
phrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160:13–23 Peters ER, Joseph SA, Garety PA (1999) Measurement of delusional
Kim H, Lee D, Shin YM, Chey J (2007) Impaired strategic decision ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et
making in schizophrenia. Brain Res 1180:90–100 al. Delusions Inventory). Schizophr Bull 25:553–576
King R, Barchas JD, Huberman BA (1984) Chaotic behavior in Pomarol-Clotet E, Honey GD, Murray GK, Corlett PR, Absalom AR,
dopamine neurodynamics. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 81:1244– Lee M, McKenna PJ, Bullmore ET, Fletcher PC (2006)
1247 Psychological effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Phe-
Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner nomenological study. Br J Psychiatry 189:173–179
JD, Heninger GR, Bowers MB, Charney DS (1994) Subanes- Roiser JP, Stephan KE, den Ouden HE, Barnes TR, Friston KJ, Joyce
thetic effects of the non-competitive NMDA-antagonist, ket- EM (2009) Do patients with schizophrenia exhibit aberrant
amine, in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:199–214 salience? Psychol Med 39:199–209
Lahti AC, Weiler MA, Tamara Michaelidis BA, Parwani A, Tamminga Rudski J (2001) Competition, superstition and illusion of control. Curr
CA (2001) Effects of ketamine in normal and schizophrenic Psychol 20:68–84
volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 25:455–467 Schafer SL, Vervel JR, Aziz N, Scott JC (1990) Pharmacokinetics of
Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Weingartner H, Sirocco K, Missar CD, fentanyl administered by computer controlled infusion pump.
Pickar D, Breier A (1996) NMDA receptor function and human Anaesthesiology 73:1091–1102
cognition: the effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Neuro- Shaner A (1999) Delusions, superstitious conditioning and chaotic
psychopharmacology 14:301–307 dopamine neurodynamics. Med Hypotheses 52:119–123
Mason O, Linney Y, Claridge G (2005) Short scales for measuring Skinner BF (1948) Superstition in the pigeon. J Exp Psychol
schizotypy. Schizophr Res 78:293–296 38:168–172
Mason O, Morgan CJA, Stefanovic A, Curran HV (2008) The Solowij N, Hall W, Lee N (1992) Recreational MDMA use in Sydney:
Psychotomimetic States Inventory: A new measure of psychotic- a profile of "Ecstasy" users and their experience with the drug. Br
type experiences. Schizophr Res Psychol Med 19:1–6 J Addict 87:1161–1172
Matute H (1994) Learned helplessness and superstitious behavior as Stefanovic A, Brandner B, Klassen E, Cregg R, Nagaratnam M,
opposite effects of uncontrollable reinforcement in humans. Bromley L, Das RK, Rossell SL, Morgan C, Curran HV (2009)
Learn Motiv 25:216–232 Acute and chronic effects of ketamine on semantic priming:
Montoya AG, Sorrentino R, Lukas SE, Price BH (2002) Long-term modelling schizophrenia? J Clin Psychopharmacol 29(2):124–133
neuropsychiatric consequences of "ecstasy" (MDMA): a review. Uhlhaas PJ, Millard I, Muetzelfeldt L, Curran HV, Morgan CJ (2007)
Harv Rev Psychiatry 10:212–220 Perceptual organization in ketamine users: preliminary evidence
Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, Barnes TR, Jones PB, of deficits on night of drug use but not 3 days later. J
Burke M, Lewis G (2007) Cannabis use and risk of Psychopharmacol 21:347–352

You might also like