You are on page 1of 216

Volume 91 July 2006

Number 1

Published monthly ISSN 0022-3514


by the
American Psychological
Association

Journal of
Personality
and
Social Psychology
ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION
Charles M. Judd, Editor
Dacher Keltner, Associate Editor
Anne Maass, Associate Editor
Bernd Wittenbrink, Associate Editor
Vincent Yzerbyt, Associate Editor
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES
John F. Dovidio, Editor
Daphne Blunt Bugental, Associate Editor
Jacques-Philippe Leyens, Associate Editor
Antony Manstead, Associate Editor
Cynthia L. Pickett, Associate Editor
Jeffry A. Simpson, Associate Editor
Scott Tindale, Associate Editor
Jacquie D. Vorauer, Associate Editor
PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
Charles S. Carver, Editor
Tim Kasser, Associate Editor
Mario Mikulincer, Associate Editor
Eva M. Pomerantz, Associate Editor
Richard W. Robins, Associate Editor
Gerard Saucier, Associate Editor
www.apa.org/journals/psp.html Thomas A. Widiger, Associate Editor
The Journal of Personality and Social Psychology publishes original APA will not replace undelivered copies resulting from address changes;
papers in all areas of personality and social psychology. It empha- journals will be forwarded only if subscribers notify the local post office
sizes empirical reports but may include specialized theoretical, in writing that they will guarantee periodicals forwarding postage.
methodological, and review papers. The journal is divided into three
independently edited sections: Electronic access: APA members who subscribe to this journal have
automatic access to a 3-year file of the journal in the PsycARTICLES姞
f ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION addresses those domains of social full-text database. See http://members.apa.org/access.
behavior in which cognition plays a major role, including the interface
of cognition with overt behavior, affect, and motivation. Among topics Reprints: Authors may order reprints of their articles from the
covered are the formation, change, and utilization of attitudes, attribu- printer when they receive proofs.
tions, and stereotypes, person memory, self-regulation, and the origins
and consequences of moods and emotions insofar as these interact Single Issues, Back Issues, and Back Volumes: For information
with cognition. Of interest also is the influence of cognition and its regarding back issues or back volumes write to Order Department,
various interfaces on significant social phenomena such as persuasion, American Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washing-
communication, prejudice, social development, and cultural trends. ton, DC 20002-4242.

f INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES focuses on psycho- Microform Editions: For information regarding microform editions,
logical and structural features of interaction in dyads and groups. write to University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, MI 48106.
Appropriate to this section are papers on the nature and dynamics of
interactions and social relationships, including interpersonal attraction, Copyright and Permission: Those who wish to reuse APA-
communication, emotion, and relationship development, and on group copyrighted material in a non-APA publication must secure from APA
and organizational processes such as social influence, group decision and the author of reproduced material written permission to reproduce
making and task performance, intergroup relations, and aggression, a journal article in full or journal text of more than 500 words. APA
prosocial behavior and other types of social behavior. normally grants permission contingent upon like permission of the
author, inclusion of the APA copyright notice on the first page of
f PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES publishes re-
reproduced material, and payment of a fee of $20 per page. Permission
search on all aspects of personality psychology. It includes studies of
from APA and fees are waived for those who wish to reproduce a single
individual differences and basic processes in behavior, emotions, cop-
table or figure from a journal for use in a print product, provided the
ing, health, motivation, and other phenomena that reflect personality.
author’s permission is obtained and full credit is given to APA as
Articles in areas such as personality structure, personality develop-
copyright holder and to the author through a complete citation. (Re-
ment, and personality assessment are also appropriate to this section
questers requiring written permission for commercial use of a single
of the journal, as are studies of the interplay of culture and personality table or figure will be assessed a $25 service fee.) Permission and fees
and manifestations of personality in everyday behavior. are waived for authors who wish to reproduce their own material for
Manuscripts: Submit manuscripts to the appropriate section editor personal use; fees only are waived for authors who wish to use more
according to the above definitions and according to the Instructions than a single table or figure of their own material commercially (but for
to Authors. Section editors reserve the right to redirect papers among use in edited books, fees are waived for the author only if serving as the
themselves as appropriate unless an author specifically requests oth- book editor). Permission and fees are waived for the photocopying of
erwise. Rejection by one section editor is considered rejection by all; isolated journal articles for nonprofit classroom or library reserve use by
therefore a manuscript rejected by one section editor should not be instructors and educational institutions. A permission fee may be
submitted to another. The opinions and statements published are the charged to the requester if students are charged for the material,
responsibility of the authors, and such opinions and statements do not multiple articles are copied, or large-scale copying is involved (e.g., for
necessarily represent the policies of APA or the views of the editors. course packs). Access services may use unedited abstracts without
Section editors’ addresses appear below: the permission of APA or the author. Libraries are permitted to photo-
copy beyond the limits of U.S. copyright law: (1) post-1977 articles,
provided the per-copy fee in the code for this journal (0022-3514/06/
ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION $12.00) is paid through the Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rose-
wood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923; (2) pre-1978 articles, provided that
Charles M. Judd, Editor the per-copy fee stated in the Publishers’ Fee List is paid through the
c/o Laurie Hawkins
Department of Psychology Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA
University of Colorado 01923. Address requests for reprint permission to the Permissions
UCB 345 Office, American Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE,
Boulder, CO 80309 Washington, DC 20002-4242.

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP APA Journal Staff: Susan J. A. Harris, Senior Director, Journals Pro-
PROCESSES gram; Greg Long, Production Account Manager; Julie Palmer-
Hoffman, Manuscript Editor; Jodi Ashcraft, Advertising Sales Manager.
John F. Dovidio, Editor
Department of Psychology
University of Connecticut
406 Babbidge Road
Storrs, CT 06269-1020 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (ISSN 0022-3514)
is published monthly in two volumes per year by the American
Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC
PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL 20002-4242. Subscriptions are available on a calendar year basis
DIFFERENCES only (January through December). The 2006 rates follow: Non-
member Individual: $421 Domestic, $464 Foreign, $491 Air Mail.
Charles S. Carver, Editor
ATTN: JPSP: PPID Institutional: $1,249 Domestic, $1,340 Foreign, $1,367 Air Mail.
Department of Psychology APA Member: $202. Write to Subscriptions Department, American
University of Miami Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC
P.O. Box 248185 20002-4242. Printed in the U.S.A. Periodicals postage paid at
Coral Gables, FL 33124-0751
Washington, DC, and at additional mailing offices. POSTMASTER:
Change of Address: Send change of address notice and a recent Send address changes to Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
mailing label to the attention of the Subscriptions Department, American chology, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242.
Psychological Association, 30 days prior to the actual change of address.

The paper in this journal meets or exceeds EPA guidelines for recycled paper. Since 1986, this journal has been printed on acid-free paper.
APA DICTIONARY HANDBOOK OF COUNSELING
OF PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHOTHERAPY WITH
Editor-in-Chief: Gary R. VandenBos LESBIAN, GAY, BISEXUAL,
2006. 1,008 pages. Hardcover. AND TRANSGENDER CLIENTS
List: $49.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $39.95 Second Edition
ISBN 1-59147-380-2
Edited by Kathleen J. Bieschke,
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-380-0
Ruperto M. Perez, and Kurt A. DeBord
Item # 4311007
2007. 464 pages. Hardcover.
List: $79.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
BECOMING CULTURALLY ISBN 1-59147-421-3
ORIENTED ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-421-0
Practical Advice for Psychologists Item # 4317113
and Educators
Nadya A. Fouad and Patricia Arredondo INTERVENING IN
2007. 208 pages. Hardcover.
List: $49.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $39.95
CHILDREN’S LIVES
An Ecological, Family-Centered
ISBN 1-59147-424-8
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-424-1 Approach to Mental Health Care
Item # 4317114 Thomas J. Dishion
and Elizabeth A. Stormshak
2007. 320 pages. Hardcover.
SPIRITUAL APPROACHES IN List: $69.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
THE TREATMENT OF WOMEN ISBN 1-59147-428-0
WITH EATING DISORDERS
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-428-9
P. Scott Richards, Randy K. Hardman,
Item # 4317115
and Michael E. Berrett
2007. 312 pages. Hardcover.
List: $59.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95 PREVENTING YOUTH
ISBN 1-59147-393-4 SUBSTANCE ABUSE
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-393-0 Science-Based Programs
Item # 4317103 for Children and Adolescents
Edited by Patrick Tolan, José Szapocznik,
GRADUATE STUDY and Soledad Sambrano
IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2007 2007. 264 pages. Hardcover.
2007. 832 pages. Paperback. List: $69.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
List: $24.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $21.95 ISBN 1-59147-307-1
ISBN 1-59147-423-X ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-307-7
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-423-4 Item # 4316058
Item # 4270090
SCIENTIFIC JURY SELECTION
DISORDERS OF THE SELF Joel D. Lieberman and Bruce D. Sales
A Personality-Guided Approach 2007. 264 pages. Hardcover.
Marshall L. Silverstein List: $79.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
2007. 320 pages. Hardcover. ISBN 1-59147-427-2
List: $69.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95 ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-427-2
ISBN 1-59147-430-2 Item # 4316081
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-430-2
Item # 4317116 PRIMATE PERSPECTIVES ON
BEHAVIOR AND COGNITION
CHILD DEVELOPMENT Edited by David A. Washburn
AND SOCIAL POLICY 2007. 368 pages. Hardcover.
Knowledge for Action List: $79.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
Edited by J. Lawrence Aber, Sandra J. Bishop- ISBN 1-59147-422-1
Josef, Stephanie M. Jones, Kathryn Taaffe ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-422-7
McLearn, and Deborah A. Phillips Item # 4318035
2007. 352 pages. Hardcover.
List: $79.95 • APA Member/Affiliate: $49.95
ISBN 1-59147-425-6
ISBN-13: 978-1-59147-425-8
Item # 4318036
AD0456

1-800-374-2721 • WWW.APA.ORG/BOOKS
Available

apa books
A Landmark Reference That Defines the Lexicon of Psychology
July 2006

APA Dictionary of Psychology


Editor-in-Chief: Gary R. VandenBos, PhD
The American Psychological Association is proud to announce the
publication of an invaluable addition to your reference shelf, one that
represents a major scholarly and editorial undertaking. With over 25,000
terms and definitions, the APA Dictionary of Psychology encompasses
all areas of research and application, and includes coverage of
concepts, processes, and therapies across all the major subdisciplines
of psychology. Ten years in the making and edited by a distinguished
editorial board of nearly 100 psychological scholars, researchers and
practitioners, the Dictionary is destined to become the most authoritative
reference of its kind.

Academicians, researchers, clinicians, undergraduates and graduate


students, and professionals in allied mental health, education, medicine,
and law, as well as academic and public libraries, will find the Dictionary
essential.

2006. Hardcover. 1,008 pages.


List: $49.95 | APA Member/Affiliate: $39.95
ISBN 1-59147-380-2 | Item # 4311007

The APA Dictionary of Psychology includes


• 25,000 entries offering clear and authoritative definitions

• Thousands of incisive cross-references directing the user to synonyms and


antonyms, acronyms and abbreviations, and related terms and concepts that
deepen the user’s understanding of related topics

• Balanced coverage of over 100 subject areas across the field of psychology
including clinical, experimental, neuropsychology, cognitive, personality
and social, developmental, health, psychopharmacology, methodology and
statistics, and many others

• Entries include nearly 8,000 terms from the APA’s Thesaurus of


Psychological Index Terms® which helps students and researchers refine
their APA database searches (such as the flagship PsycINFO® bibliographic
database’s 2+ million records)
800-374-2721
www.apa.org/books
• “A Guide to Use” and “Quick Guide to Format” that together explain
important stylistic and format features to help readers most effectively
use the Dictionary

• Each of four appendices gathers terms into a thematic summary listing,


covering (1) biographies; (2) institutions, associations and organizations;
(3) psychological therapies and interventions; and (4) psychological tests
and assessment instruments

AD0470
Journal of

Personality
and

Social Psychology
www.apa.org/journals/psp.html

July 2006
Copyright © 2006 by the
VOLUME 91 NUMBER 1 American Psychological Association

Attitudes and Social Cognition


1 Alone but Feeling No Pain: Effects of Social Exclusion on Physical Pain Tolerance and Pain Threshold,
Affective Forecasting, and Interpersonal Empathy
C. Nathan DeWall and Roy F. Baumeister

16 The Single Category Implicit Association Test as a Measure of Implicit Social Cognition
Andrew Karpinski and Ross B. Steinman

33 Thinking Within the Box: The Relational Processing Style Elicited by Counterfactual Mind-Sets
Laura J. Kray, Adam D. Galinsky, and Elaine M. Wong

49 Self-Regulatory Processes Defend Against the Threat of Death: Effects of Self-Control Depletion and
Trait Self-Control on Thoughts and Fears of Dying
Matthew T. Gailliot, Brandon J. Schmeichel, and Roy F. Baumeister

Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes


63 Peacocks, Picasso, and Parental Investment: The Effects of Romantic Motives on Creativity
Vladas Griskevicius, Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T. Kenrick

77 Navigating the Interdependence Dilemma: Attachment Goals and the Use of Communal Norms With
Potential Close Others
Jennifer A. Bartz and John E. Lydon

97 Intergroup Helping as Status Relations: Effects of Status Stability, Identification, and Type of Help on
Receptivity to High-Status Group’s Help
Arie Nadler and Samer Halabi

111 Information Quantity and Quality Affect the Realistic Accuracy of Personality Judgment
Tera D. Letzring, Shannon M. Wells, and David C. Funder

124 Supplication and Appeasement in Conflict and Negotiation: The Interpersonal Effects of Disappointment,
Worry, Guilt, and Regret
Gerben A. Van Kleef, Carsten K. W. De Dreu, and Antony S. R. Manstead

Personality Processes and Individual Differences


143 Optimism in Close Relationships: How Seeing Things in a Positive Light Makes Them So
Sanjay Srivastava, Kelly M. McGonigal, Jane M. Richards, Emily A. Butler, and James J. Gross

(contents continue)
154 Discrepancies Between Explicit and Implicit Self-Concepts: Consequences for Information Processing
Pablo Briñol, Richard E. Petty, and S. Christian Wheeler

171 Investigating the Dopaminergic Basis of Extraversion in Humans: A Multilevel Approach


Jan Wacker, Mira-Lynn Chavanon, and Gerhard Stemmler

188 Possible Selves and Academic Outcomes: How and When Possible Selves Impel Action
Daphna Oyserman, Deborah Bybee, and Kathy Terry

Other
15 American Psychological Association Subscription Claims Information
ii E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
96 Instructions to Authors
62 Subscription Order Form

ii
154 Discrepancies Between Explicit and Implicit Self-Concepts: Consequences for Information Processing
Pablo Briñol, Richard E. Petty, and S. Christian Wheeler

171 Investigating the Dopaminergic Basis of Extraversion in Humans: A Multilevel Approach


Jan Wacker, Mira-Lynn Chavanon, and Gerhard Stemmler

188 Possible Selves and Academic Outcomes: How and When Possible Selves Impel Action
Daphna Oyserman, Deborah Bybee, and Kathy Terry

Other
15 American Psychological Association Subscription Claims Information
ii E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
96 Instructions to Authors
62 Subscription Order Form

ii
ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION
CHARLES M. JUDD, Editor
University of Colorado at Boulder
ASSOCIATE EDITORS ALICE H. EAGLY NIRA LIBERMAN LINDA SKITKA
Northwestern University Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel University of Illinois at Chicago
DACHER KELTNER
University of California, Berkeley NICHOLAS EPLEY DIANE M. MACKIE JOHN SKOWRONSKI
University of Chicago University of California, Santa Barbara Northern Illinois University
ANNE MAASS
Università di Padova, Padova, Italy RUSSELL H. FAZIO NEIL MACRAE ELIOT R. SMITH
Ohio State University Dartmouth College Indiana University Bloomington
BERND WITTENBRINK
University of Chicago LISA FELDMAN BARRETT TONY MANSTEAD DIEDERIK STAPEL
Boston College Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales University of Groningen, Groningen,
VINCENT YZERBYT
Catholic University of Louvain, SUSAN T. FISKE THOMAS MUSSWEILER the Netherlands
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Princeton University Universität Köln, Cologne, Germany FRITZ STRACK
CONSULTING EDITORS BARBARA L. FREDRICKSON JAMES M. OLSON Universität Würzburg, Würzburg,
University of Michigan University of Western Ontario, Germany
ICEK AJZEN
WENDI GARDNER London, Ontario, Canada
University of Massachusetts ABRAHAM TESSER
Northwestern University BERNADETTE M. PARK University of Georgia
MAHZARIN BANAJI
DANIEL GILBERT University of Colorado at Boulder
Harvard University YAACOV TROPE
Harvard University RICHARD E. PETTY New York University
MONICA BIERNAT Ohio State University
University of Kansas THOMAS GILOVICH THERESA K. VESCIO
Cornell University NEAL J. ROESE Pennsylvania State University
IRENE V. BLAIR University of Illinois at Urbana–
University of Colorado at Boulder ANTHONY G. GREENWALD
University of Washington Champaign WILLIAM VON HIPPEL
GALEN V. BODENHAUSEN University of New South Wales,
DAVID L. HAMILTON MYRON ROTHBART Sydney, Australia
Northwestern University University of Oregon
University of California, Santa
MARKUS BRAUER Barbara DUANE T. WEGENER
LAURIE RUDMAN Purdue University
LAPSCO, Université Blaise Pascal Rutgers, The State University
EDWARD R. HIRT
Clermont-Ferrand, France of New Jersey DANIEL M. WEGNER
Indiana University Bloomington
MARILYNN B. BREWER Harvard University
TIFFANY ITO MARK SCHALLER
Ohio State University University of Colorado at Boulder University of British Columbia, DIRK WENTURA
JOHN T. CACIOPPO Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Saarland University, Saarbrücken,
YOSHIHISA KASHIMA
University of Chicago TONI SCHMADER Germany
University of Melbourne, Victoria,
OLIVIER CORNEILLE Australia University of Arizona DANIEL WIGBOLDUS
Catholic University of Louvain, KARLE CHRISTOPHE KLAUER NORBERT SCHWARZ Radboud University Nijmegen,
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Albrecht-Ludwigs-Universität University of Michigan Nijmegen, the Netherlands
PATRICIA DEVINE Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany TIMOTHY D. WILSON
GÜN R. SEMIN
University of Wisconsin—Madison ARIE W. KRUGLANSKI Free University, Amsterdam, the University of Virginia
University of Maryland Netherlands PIOTR WINKIELMEN
AP DIJKSTERHUIS
University of Amsterdam, ALAN LAMBERT JEFFREY W. SHERMAN University of California, San Diego
Amsterdam, the Netherlands Washington University in St. Louis University of California, Davis
MARK P. ZANNA
DAVID DUNNING JENNIFER LERNER STEVEN J. SHERMAN University of Waterloo, Waterloo,
Cornell University Carnegie Mellon University Indiana University Bloomington Ontario, Canada
ASSISTANT TO THE EDITOR—LAURIE HAWKINS

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES


JOHN F. DOVIDIO, Editor
University of Connecticut
ASSOCIATE EDITORS XIMENA ARRIAGA LORNE CAMPBELL KLAUS FIEDLER
DAPHNE BLUNT BUGENTAL Purdue University University of Western Ontario, University of Heidelberg,
University of California, Santa Barbara London, Ontario, Canada Heidelberg, Germany
WINTON W. T. AU
JACQUES-PHILIPPE LEYENS The Chinese University of Hong Kong, SERENA CHEN GARTH FLETCHER
Catholic University of Louvain, Shatin, Hong Kong University of California, Berkeley University of Canterbury,
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium Christchurch, New Zealand
MARK BALDWIN MARGARET CLARK
ANTONY MANSTEAD McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Yale University SHELLY GABLE
Cardiff University, Cardiff, Canada University of California, Los Angeles
CARSTEN DE DREU
United Kingdom University of Amsterdam, LOWELL GAERTNER
KIM BARTHOLOMEW
Amsterdam, the Netherlands University of Tennessee, Knoxville
CYNTHIA L. PICKETT Simon Fraser University, Burnaby,
University of California, Davis British Columbia, Canada STÉPHANIE DEMOULIN SAMUEL L. GAERTNER
Catholic University of Louvain University of Delaware
JEFFRY A. SIMPSON C. DANIEL BATSON
University of Minnesota, University of Kansas Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium, and ADAM GALINSKY
Twin Cities Campus Belgan National Fund for Scientific Northwestern University
B. ANNE BETTENCOURT Research, Brussels, Belgium
SCOTT TINDALE University of Missouri—Columbia PETER GLICK
Loyola University Chicago DAVID DESTENO Lawrence University
GERD BOHNER
Northeastern University STEPHANIE A. GOODWIN
JACQUIE D. VORAUER Universität Bielefeld, Bielefeld,
University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Germany STEVE DRIGOTAS Purdue University
Manitoba, Canada Johns Hopkins University MARTIE G. HASSELTON
NIALL BOLGER
CONSULTING EDITORS New York University ELISSA S. EPEL University of California, Los Angeles
DOMINIC ABRAMS University of California, San S. ALEXANDER HASLAM
NYLA R. BRANSCOMBE Francisco
University of Kent at Canterbury, University of Exeter, Exeter,
University of Kansas
Canterbury, England VICTORIA ESSES United Kingdom
CHRIS AGNEW JONATHON D. BROWN University of Western Ontario, VERLIN HINSZ
Purdue University University of Washington London, Ontario, Canada North Dakota State University
ARTHUR ARON RUPERT BROWN BEVERLY FEHR GORDON HODSON
State University of New York at The University of Kent at Canterbury, University of Winnipeg, Winnipeg, Brock University, St. Catherine’s,
Stony Brook Canterbury, England Manitoba, Canada Ontario, Canada
(editors continue)
MICHAEL A. HOGG LAURA J. KRAY SABINE OTTEN CHRISTINE SMITH
University of Queensland, University of California, Berkeley University of Gröningen, Grand Valley State University
Brisbane, Australia JAMES R. LARSON JR. Gröningen, the Netherlands HEATHER J. SMITH
ANDREA B. HOLLINGSHEAD University of Illinois at Chicago CRAIG D. PARKS Sonoma State University
University of Southern California COLIN WAYNE LEACH Washington State University
RUSSELL SPEARS
JOHN G. HOLMES University of Sussex, Sussex, United LOUIS A. PENNER Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales
University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Kingdom Wayne State University
CHARLES STANGOR
Ontario, Canada PAULA PIETROMONACO
JOHN LEVINE University of Maryland
RICK H. HOYLE University of Pittsburgh University of Massachusetts at
University of Kentucky GARY L. STASSER
Amherst
JOHN E. LYDON Miami University—Ohio
JOLANDA JETTEN McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, TOM POSTMES
University of Exeter, Exeter, University of Exeter, Exeter, United WALTER STEPHAN
Canada
United Kingdom Kingdom New Mexico State University
JON K. MANER
JAMES D. JOHNSON FELICIA PRATTO WILLIAM B. SWANN JR.
Florida State University
University of North Carolina at University of Connecticut University of Texas at Austin
Wilmington BRENDA MAJOR
HARRY T. REIS JANET SWIM
University of California, Santa
TATSUYA KAMEDA University of Rochester Pennsylvania State University
Barbara
Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan LEIGH L. THOMPSON
CRAIG MCGARTY W. STEVEN RHOLES
BENJAMIN R. KARNEY Texas A&M University Northwestern University
Australian National University,
RAND Corporation, Santa Monica, Canberra, Australia TOM TYLER
JENNIFER A. RICHESON
California New York University
WENDY BERRY MENDES Northwestern University
YOSHI KASHIMA Harvard University JEROEN VAES
MARK SCHALLER
University of Melbourne, Victoria, Australia University of Padova, Padova, Italy
RICHARD MORELAND University of British Columbia,
DEBORAH A. KASHY Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada KEES VAN DEN BOS
University of Pittsburgh
Michigan State University University of Utrecht, Utrecht,
BRIAN MULLEN DAVID A. SCHROEDER
KERRY KAWAKAMI University of Arkansas the Netherlands
University of Kent at Canterbury,
York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada PAUL A. M. VAN LANGE
Canterbury, England CONSTANTINE SEDIKIDES
JANICE R. KELLY University of Southampton, Free University, Amsterdam,
AMÉLIE MUMMENDEY
Purdue University Southampton, England Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Friedrich-Schiller-Universität, Jena,
DACHER KELTNER Jena, Germany PHILLIP R. SHAVER LAURIE R. WEINGART
University of California, Berkeley University of California, Davis Carnegie Mellon University
MARK MURAVEN
DAVID A. KENNY University at Albany, State University J. NICOLE SHELTON GWEN M. WITTENBAUM
University of Connecticut of New York Princeton University Michigan State University
DOUGLAS T. KENRICK SANDRA L. MURRAY MARGARET SHIH WENDY L. WOOD
Arizona State University State University of New York at Buffalo University of Michigan Texas A&M University
NORBERT L. KERR LISA A. NEFF STACEY SINCLAIR MICHAEL ZÁRATE
Michigan State University University of Toledo University of Virginia University of Texas at El Paso
ASSISTANT TO THE EDITOR—CHRISTINE KELLY

PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES


CHARLES S. CARVER, Editor
University of Miami
ASSOCIATE EDITORS GEORGE A. BONANNO EDDIE HARMON-JONES DANIEL W. RUSSELL
TIM KASSER Teachers College, Texas A&M University Iowa State University
Knox College Columbia University TODD HEATHERTON OLIVER C. SCHULTHEISS
MARIO MIKULINCER AVSHALOM CASPI Dartmouth College University of Michigan
Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel King’s College, London JUTTA HECKHAUSEN SUZANNE C. SEGERSTROM
EDWARD C. CHANG University of California, Irvine University of Kentucky
EVA M. POMERANTZ
University of Illinois at Urbana– University of Michigan KENNON M. SHELDON
STEVEN J. HEINE
Champaign SERENA CHEN University of British Columbia, University of Missouri—Columbia
University of California, Berkeley Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada C. R. SNYDER
RICHARD W. ROBINS
University of California, Davis A. TIMOTHY CHURCH RICHARD KOESTNER University of Kansas
Washington State University McGill University SANJAY SRIVASTAVA
GERARD SAUCIER University of Oregon
JAMES COAN Montreal, Quebec, Canada
University of Oregon
University of Wisconsin—Madison DAVID LUBINSKI TIMOTHY STRAUMAN
THOMAS A. WIDIGER Vanderbilt University Duke University
University of Kentucky M. LYNNE COOPER
University of Missouri—Columbia RICHARD E. LUCAS MICHAEL J. STRUBE
CONSULTING EDITORS Michigan State University Washington University
MICHAEL EID
STEPHAN A. AHADI JERRY SULS
University of Geneva, Geneva, ROBERT R. MCCRAE
American Institutes for Research, University of Iowa
Switzerland National Institute on Aging, Baltimore
Washington, DC
ANDREW J. ELLIOT WENDY BERRY MENDES WILLIAM B. SWANN JR.
JAMIE ARNDT University of Rochester Harvard University University of Texas at Austin
University of Missouri—Columbia HOWARD TENNEN
LISA FELDMAN BARRETT RODOLFO MENDOZA-DENTON
JENS B. ASENDORPF University of Connecticut Health
Boston College University of California, Berkeley
Humboldt-Universität Berlin Center
Berlin, Germany WILLIAM FLEESON DANIEL K. MROCZEK SUZANNE THOMPSON
Wake Forest University Fordham University Pomona College
MICHAEL C. ASHTON
R. CHRIS FRALEY STEPHEN A. PETRILL ROBERT J. VALLERAND
Brock University, St. Catherines,
University of Illinois at Chicago Pennsylvania State University Université du Québec à Montréal
Ontario, Canada
ANTONIO L. FREITAS RALPH L. PIEDMONT Montreal, Quebec, Canada
OZLEM AYDUK State University of New York at Loyola College in Maryland
University of California, Berkeley KATHLEEN D. VOHS
Stony Brook E. ASHBY PLANT University of Minnesota
ROY F. BAUMEISTER DAVID C. FUNDER Florida State University DAVID WATSON
Florida State University University of California, Riverside University of Iowa
BRENT ROBERTS
VERÓNICA BENET-MARTÍNEZ STEVEN W. GANGESTAD University of Illinois at BARBARA WOIKE
University of California, Riverside University of New Mexico Urbana–Champaign Columbia University
APRIL L. BLESKE-RECHEK CAROL L. GOHM MICHAEL D. ROBINSON REX A. WRIGHT
University of Wisconsin—Eau Claire University of Mississippi North Dakota State University University of Alabama at Birmingham
ASSISTANT TO THE EDITOR—JESSICA LILLESAND
ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION

Alone but Feeling No Pain: Effects of Social Exclusion on Physical Pain


Tolerance and Pain Threshold, Affective Forecasting,
and Interpersonal Empathy
C. Nathan DeWall and Roy F. Baumeister
Florida State University

Prior findings of emotional numbness (rather than distress) among socially excluded persons led the
authors to investigate whether exclusion causes a far-reaching insensitivity to both physical and
emotional pain. Experiments 1– 4 showed that receiving an ostensibly diagnostic forecast of a lonesome
future life reduced sensitivity to physical pain, as indicated by both (higher) thresholds and tolerance.
Exclusion also caused emotional insensitivity, as indicated by reductions in affective forecasting of joy
or woe over a future football outcome (Experiment 3), as well as lesser empathizing with another
person’s suffering from either romantic breakup (Experiment 4) or a broken leg (Experiment 5). The
insensitivities to pain and emotion were highly intercorrelated.

Keywords: social exclusion, rejection, emotion, affective forecasting, empathy

People depend heavily on others for much of their physical and Social exclusion poses a serious threat to a person’s physical
mental well-being. With no fangs, no claws, an extremely pro- and psychological well-being, as indicated by higher rates of
longed childhood phase of dependency and vulnerability, and other physical and mental illnesses among people who live alone as
physical weaknesses, human beings are not well suited to living in compared with people who have strong social networks (Argyle,
isolation from others. Given the importance of acquiring and 1987; Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Berntson, 2003; Kiecolt-Glaser et
maintaining membership in social groups, it is therefore hardly al., 1984; Lynch, 1979; Myers, 1992). These findings confirm that
surprising that people would react strongly to any threat of social social exclusion has potentially drastic and negative effects on
exclusion. Multiple laboratory studies of social exclusion have physical and psychological health, but they offer no explanation
found, however, that people respond to social exclusion in a for the finding that people respond to social exclusion with emo-
seemingly detached and emotionally indifferent manner. Socially tional numbness (e.g., Twenge et al., 2001). Indeed, they under-
excluded people often report emotional states that do not differ score the paradox in the fact that people would respond to a
significantly from participants in acceptance or control conditions potentially momentous event, such as social exclusion, with emo-
(Baumeister, Twenge, & Nuss, 2002; Gardner, Pickett, & Brewer, tional numbness and detachment.
2000; Twenge, Baumeister, Tice, & Stucke, 2001; Twenge & The first purpose of the current investigation was to resolve the
Campbell, 2003; Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002; Zadro, paradox as to why rejected people report emotional numbness, by
Williams, & Richardson, 2004). identifying a possible mechanism by which people respond to
social exclusion. We propose that certain interpersonal events,
such as social rejection, activate the body’s pain response system
and potentially alter how it registers physical and emotional pain
C. Nathan DeWall and Roy F. Baumeister, Department of Psychology, (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003). With regard to phys-
Florida State University. ical pain, social exclusion may disrupt the ability to respond to
We gratefully acknowledge support by National Institute of Mental physical pain in the same manner as people who have not expe-
Health Grant MH-65559. rienced social exclusion. This would lead to increases in both pain
The research reported in this article was part of C. Nathan DeWall’s threshold (i.e., sensitivity to pain) and pain tolerance (i.e., with-
master’s thesis at Florida State University under the direction of Roy F. standing greater pain). Hence, the first goal of this research (tested
Baumeister. We graciously thank committee members Thomas Joiner, Jon
in Experiments 1– 4) was to demonstrate that socially excluded
K. Maner, and Dianne M. Tice for their helpful comments and suggestions.
We also thank Carey Morewedge for helpful comments on an earlier
people become relatively numb to physical pain.
version of this article. The second goal of this work was to extend the idea of physical
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to C. numbness to emotional functioning. As suggested by MacDonald
Nathan DeWall, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, and Leary (2005), if the body uses the same system to respond to
Tallahassee, FL 32306-1270. E-mail: dewall@darwin.psy.fsu.edu physical injury and interpersonal injury, then physical pain and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 1–15
Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.1

1
2 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

interpersonal emotions may be linked—and just as the body goes acceptance (e.g., aggression, self-defeating behavior), but the de-
numb to pain, it may also become less sensitive to emotion. If the constructed state may offer rejected people a temporary reprieve
emotional system ceases to function normally in the aftermath of from feeling the intense pain or distress that can accompany threats
social exclusion, people might show abnormalities not only in their to belongingness. Although escaping from aversive emotional
emotional reactions to current events but also in their forecasts of states and high self-awareness may be beneficial in the short-term,
emotional responses to future events. They might also lose their such behaviors are characteristic of severe psychopathology in
empathy for the physical suffering of another person. Last, and clinical populations (Bancroft, Skirmshire, & Simkins, 1976; Haw-
ironically, rejected people might even lose their empathy for the ton, Cole, O’Grady, & Osborn, 1982) and of a variety of self-
suffering of someone else who has also suffered rejection. Exper- destructive behaviors in nonclinical samples (see Baumeister &
iments 3–5 tested these predictions. Scher, 1988, for a review). The experiments reported in this article
sought to link emotional unresponsiveness with insensitivity to
Social Exclusion and Emotional Responding: The Paradox physical pain, both of which could be natural defenses against
of Detachment and Numbness one’s own suffering and could contribute to the deconstructed
state.
A desire to form and maintain social bonds with others is a If people have normal or natural defenses that help them escape
fundamental and pervasive desire among humans (Baumeister & or minimize emotional distress, then social exclusion may tempo-
Leary, 1995). Insofar as belongingness is a basic need rather than rarily impair the ability to experience emotions in a normal fash-
a want, people experience considerable difficulties when their need ion. The theory proposed in the current article is that the emotion
to belong is unsatisfied or frustrated. A lack of stable relationships system temporarily ceases to function normally in response to
also has detrimental effects on one’s health (Cacioppo, Hawkley, social exclusion. The resulting emotional numbness could be
& Berntson, 2003; Hawkley, Burleson, Berntson, & Cacioppo, linked to an insensitivity to physical pain, as the next section will
2003; Lynch, 1979; see Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, explain.
1996, for a review).
One might assume and hope that excluded people would show Social and Physical Pain
adaptive responses of changing themselves so as to become more
socially attractive or to take better care of themselves as individ- There is reason to believe that the pain of social exclusion shares
uals. The opposite is often found, however. Rejected people ex- many of the same neural and psychological mechanisms as expe-
hibit decreased intellectual functioning (Baumeister et al., 2002), riences of physical pain. At a purely linguistic level, people fre-
become more aggressive toward others (Buckley, Winkel, & quently use words connoting physical pain when describing emo-
Leary, 2004; Twenge et al., 2001; Twenge & Campbell, 2003), are tional responses to distressing events. For example, people may
less willing to self-regulate (Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & report feeling “hurt” or “crushed” following the dissolution of a
Twenge, 2005), and engage in various self-defeating behaviors, meaningful relationship (Leary & Springer, 2000). MacDonald
such as risk taking and procrastination (Twenge et al., 2002). None and Leary (2005) have suggested that the similarity between
of these responses seem likely to foster interpersonal acceptance or descriptions of social and physical pain extends beyond mere
personal health and well-being. metaphor. Specifically, they proposed that social pain and physical
As already noted, socially excluded participants frequently re- pain operate via shared physiological mechanisms, including the
port emotional states that do not differ significantly from socially anterior cingulate cortex, periaqueductal gray (PAG) brain struc-
accepted and control participants (Baumeister et al., 2002; Gardner tures, and the opioid and oxytocin neuroendocrine systems (see
et al., 2000; Twenge et al., 2002; Zadro et al., 2004). Even more also Rossi, Pasternak, & Bodnar, 1993).
surprisingly, when differences in emotion are found, these differ- Decades ago, Panksepp and colleagues proposed a link between
ences do not mediate the behavioral consequences of social exclu- social and physical pain (Herman & Panksepp, 1978; Panksepp,
sion. Even researchers who find that social exclusion increases Herman, Conner, Bishop, & Scott, 1978; Panksepp, Vilberg, Bean,
emotional distress have not found that the distress mediates the Coy, & Kastin, 1978). They suggested that as evolution prepared
behavioral consequences of social exclusion (Buckley et al., 2004; animals for increasing social interaction, instead of creating en-
Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000). Given the fundamental nature tirely new systems to react to social events, such as being rejected
of the need to belong, one might reasonably expect that real, or excluded, it piggybacked these responses onto the existing
potential, or imagined social exclusion would result in severe systems that were hard-wired for responding to physical pain.
emotional reactions. It is therefore unclear why emotion plays such Social events might therefore activate the body’s pain response
a minor role in explaining behavioral responses to social exclusion. system and potentially alter how it would register physical pain.
One reason socially excluded people report feelings of emo- Recent neuroscience and functional MRI research has shown
tional numbness may be that exclusion leads to a defensive state of that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) functions as a neural
cognitive deconstruction. The deconstructed state has been in- alarm system to warn people that factors in their environment
voked to characterize presuicidal mental states (Baumeister, 1990), threaten their goals (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Eisenberger &
and it is marked by emotional numbness, an altered perception of Lieberman, 2004; Eisenberger et al., 2003; Kimbrell et al., 1999;
time, thoughts of meaninglessness, lethargy, and avoidance of E. E. Nelson & Panksepp, 1998). Eisenberger et al. (2003) showed
self-focused attention. Twenge, Catanese, and Baumeister (2003) that the ACC plays a prominent role in the detection of threats to
showed that socially excluded people exhibited each of these belongingness. In their functional MRI study, ostracized people
symptoms of cognitive deconstruction. Social exclusion leads to showed activation of the dorsal ACC. Ostracized people also
behaviors that could preclude the possibility of gaining future showed activation of the right ventral prefrontal cortex, which has
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 3

been previously linked to the regulation of physical pain distress beneficial in terms of immediately reducing a person’s suffer-
and negative affect (Hariri, Bookheimer, & Mazziotta, 2000; ing, in the same way that physical injuries often create an
Petrovic, Kalso, Petersson, & Ingvar, 2002). Thus, preliminary analgesia that saves him or her from feeling acute, ongoing pain
evidence suggests that Panksepp and colleagues (Herman & Pank- for as long as the injury lasts. If the neural systems responsible
sepp, 1978; Panksepp, Herman, et al., 1978; Panksepp, Vilberg, et for detecting and regulating physical and emotional pain have a
al., 1978) were correct in proposing that certain physiological common physiological basis, then social exclusion should also
processes responsible for detection and regulation of physical pain influence how people respond to physically painful stimuli.
were co-opted to sense and respond to emotionally painful events, Thus, excluded people should become numb to physical pain,
such as being rejected or excluded. and this physical numbness should be related to emotional
Additional research has shown that the PAG brain structures, insensitivity (see Figure 1).
which receive input from the body’s injury detection system (no- What would provide the best measures of emotional insensitiv-
ciceptive system) and ACC, are involved in the detection of ity? One method would be direct self-reports of emotion, which
physical pain and are implicated in animal bonding behavior (sure enough) tend to indicate no emotional reaction (e.g.,
(Craig & Dostrovsky, 1999). Panksepp (1998) demonstrated that Baumeister et al., 2005). Self-reported emotion may not provide
activation of the PAG elicited separation distress cries from rats, the best measure, however, because of social desirability biases
and further evidence showed that lesions to the PAG led to reduced and other factors that could influence responding. For example,
separation distress cries (Wiedenmayer, Goodwin, & Barr, 2000). excluded people might feel emotionally upset but refuse to admit
Administration of the neuropeptide oxytocin and opioids such as it. To avoid these methodological pitfalls of direct self-reports of
morphine have also been shown to reduce separation distress cries emotion, we measured two emotional phenomena in which people
in rats (Carden, Hernandez, & Hofer, 1996; Carden & Hofer, 1990; seemingly rely on their current state and emotional simulation
Insel & Winslow, 1991). These findings indicate that some phys- (imagining how oneself would feel) to make judgments about
iological systems respond to both physical pain and socially dis- events distant from the present. The first of these phenomena is
tressing events. affective forecasting; the other is empathy. If rejected people are
Most relevant to the current investigation, however, is evidence replete with emotion but reluctant to admit their distress out of
that separation from caregivers and isolation from conspecifics self-presentational concerns, they should be quite willing to predict
results in decreased sensitivity to physical pain, also known as strong future emotional reactions and to empathize with others’
analgesia. Several studies have shown that short-term isolation suffering. But if their emotional systems have really ceased to
produces reduced pain sensitivity in rat pups (Kehoe & Blass, function normally, excluded people should find it difficult to
1986a, 1986b; Naranjo & Fuentes, 1985; Spear, Enters, Aswad, & imagine having strong emotions in response to hypothetical future
Louzan, 1985), mice (Konecka & Sroczynska, 1990), cows events, and they likewise might not empathize with another per-
(Rushen, Boissy, Terlouw, & de Passillé, 1999), and chicks (Sufka son’s distress.
& Hughes, 1990; Sufka & Weed, 1994; see MacDonald & Leary, Affective forecasting refers to the ways in which people
2005, for a review). Thus, threats to belongingness appear to predict their future feelings (see Wilson & Gilbert, 2003, for a
activate neural mechanisms associated with physical pain and the review). Research has shown consistently that people overesti-
regulation of pain in some nonhuman species (E. E. Nelson & mate the duration and intensity of their emotional responses to
Panksepp, 1998; Panksepp, 1998). future events. People overestimate how happy they will be after
Previous research has shown that social and physical pain share winning a date on a simulated dating game (Wilson, Wheatley,
common physiological mechanisms in some animals, but research Kurtz, Dunn, & Gilbert, 2004), predict greater distress follow-
has not provided much evidence about whether a similar link exists ing a romantic breakup or denial for academic tenure than
in humans. MacDonald, Kingsbury, and Shaw (2005) showed that actually occurs (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley,
people high in rejection sensitivity responded to ostracism with 1998), and overrate what their quality of life will be if they
decreased sensitivity to physical pain (pain threshold) on the receive an important medical procedure (Jepson, Loewenstein,
cold-pressor task, but people low in rejection sensitivity did not & Ubel, 2001). Other research has shown that a person’s current
show the same pattern of responding. If threats to belongingness emotional state can bias his or her predictions of future emo-
activate basic neural mechanisms designed to regulate pain (Eisen- tional responses and other decision-making processes (Gilbert,
berger et al., 2003), one would expect all ostracized participants to Gill, & Wilson, 2002; Loewenstein, O’Donoghue, & Rabin, in
exhibit decreased sensitivity to physical pain, regardless of their press; L. D. Nelson & Morrison, 2005; Van Boven & Loewen-
propensity to anxiously expect and overreact to a threat to their stein, 2003).
sense of belonging (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Those prelimi-
nary findings were also mute about another important issue,
namely, what relationship increased pain threshold and tolerance Emotional
Insensitivity
may have to emotional responding, to which we turn in the next
section.
Social Exclusion
Physical
Effects of Increased Pain Threshold and Pain Tolerance Insensitivity
for Emotional Responding
If people respond to social exclusion with emotional numb- Figure 1. Proposed model illustrating linked consequences of physical
ness, this temporary shutdown of the emotion system might be and emotional insensitivity following social exclusion.
4 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

If social exclusion hampers the capacity for people to respond to status (no feedback). The third control group (misfortune control),
their own emotions, rejected people might show a different pattern added in Experiment 2, told people that their futures would be
of affective forecasting than other participants. Specifically, re- marred by accidents. The purpose of this third control group was
jected participants might report less happiness when forecasting to rule out the possibility that the effects of social exclusion were
their emotional reaction to a positive event and less unhappiness simply due to receiving an unpleasant forecast about the future.
when forecasting their emotional reaction to a negative event Hence, the misfortune control feedback predicted bad outcomes
compared with participants who did not experience rejection. that did not include social exclusion.
Another process that may illustrate the intimate link between We predicted that participants in the future alone condition
social exclusion and emotional responding is the ability of rejected would have higher pain thresholds and higher pain tolerance than
people to show empathy toward people in various forms of phys- participants in the three control conditions. The inclusion of both
ical and psychological distress. Previous research has shown that pain threshold and pain tolerance measures provided two separate
rejected people tend to behave less prosocially than nonrejected
chances to confirm or disconfirm our hypothesis that social exclu-
people (Twenge, Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & Bartels, 2006),
sion leads to numbness to physical pain. We had no a priori reason
and work by Batson and colleagues has demonstrated that empathy
to think that socially excluded participants would show increases
plays a prominent role in shaping prosocial behavior (Batson,
in pain threshold but not in pain tolerance (or vice versa). If
Klein, Highberger, & Shaw, 1995). It therefore seems plausible
that rejected participants might feel reduced empathy toward an- socially excluded participants showed increases in both pain
other person’s suffering. threshold and pain tolerance, however, that would provide con-
verging support for our hypothesis.

Present Research
Method
The present experiments tested the theory that social exclusion
activates the body’s pain system and sets of responses that may Participants. Thirty-three right-handed undergraduates (24 women)
reduce sensitivity to both physical and emotional pain. Our pre- participated in Experiment 1 for partial course credit. Experiment 2 had
diction had two parts. First, participants who experienced social 30 participants (24 women). Of the participants in Experiment 1, 67%
exclusion would show increased pain threshold and pain tolerance were White and 33% were a racial minority. Of the participants in
compared with socially accepted and control participants. Second, Experiment 2, 83% were White and 17% were a racial minority. In
this insensitivity to physical pain should be related to emotional these and subsequent studies, we restricted participation to people who
insensitivity. Participants who experience social exclusion should were nonsmokers and right-handed because smoking has been shown to
reduce pain sensitivity (Kanarek & Carrington, 2004; Pomerleau, Turk,
find it difficult to predict strong emotional responses to hypothet-
& Fertig, 1984) and because left limbs show greater pain sensitivity
ical future events, and they should exhibit decreased empathy with
than right limbs, regardless of hand of preference (Murray & Hagan,
others’ suffering, compared with participants who do not experi-
1973). Participants were also required to not have ingested any sugared
ence rejection. foods, alcoholic beverages, or pain medicine (including aspirin) for at
Thus, the current investigation sought to resolve the seeming least 8 hr prior to participation in the experiments (Kanarek & Car-
paradox as to why people respond to such a threatening experience rington, 2004; Mercer & Holder, 1997).
as rejection with emotional numbness. The first goal was to test the Materials and procedure. Participants arrived individually for a study
hypothesis that people become numb to physical pain in the ostensibly concerned with personality and physical sensitivity. After giving
aftermath of social exclusion. The second goal of this work was to informed consent, participants in Experiment 1 but not 2 completed mea-
demonstrate that physical numbness has implications for emo- sures of rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996) and global
tional responding, such as affective forecasting and interpersonal self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965).
empathy. Baseline measurements of pain threshold and pain tolerance (two trials
each) were then taken by using a pressure algometer (Type II; Somedic,
Solletuna, Sweden). For pain threshold, participants were instructed to say
Experiments 1 and 2: Not Feeling My Own Pain
“now” when they first felt pain due to the pressure increase. For pain
Experiments 1 and 2 tested the hypothesis that social exclusion tolerance, participants were instructed to say “stop” when the pain became
reduces sensitivity to physical pain. Social exclusion was manip- too uncomfortable to continue. At this point, the experimenter immediately
ulated by having participants complete a personality test and retracted the algometer. The digital display showed the value of pressure
applied at the moment the algometer was retracted. The algometer was
giving some participants bogus feedback stating that their person-
applied perpendicularly to the skin and lowered at a rate of approximately
ality profile enabled the researchers to predict that they would
5 kPA per second until pain threshold or tolerance was reached, as
most likely end up alone in life. This future alone condition has
indicated by participants’ verbal report. All pain measurements were taken
been used in past research to create a sense of social exclusion and
at the first dorsal interosseous muscle (i.e., behind the first knuckle of the
impending isolation, because people take it to mean that something index finger) of the participant’s dominant hand. The order of the pain
about their personalities will cause others to reject them (Twenge tolerance and threshold measurements was counterbalanced across partic-
et al., 2001). There were three control groups. One of these groups ipants. To prevent habituation, there was a 1.5-min interval between all
received personality feedback stating that they had a personality pain threshold and tolerance measurements (Orbach, Mikulincer, King,
type that would lead to a future filled with several meaningful and Cohen, & Stein, 1997).
lasting relationships (future belonging). The second control group Participants then completed a brief demographic questionnaire and the
was given no feedback regarding their personality or the implica- Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975). To bolster
tions their personality may have for their future belongingness the credibility of the cover story, we gave participants accurate feedback
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 5

regarding their extroversion score.1 Participants then received bogus feed- pared with future belonging and misfortune control participants,
back about the implications their extroversion score would have for their F(1, 27) ⫽ 21.35, p ⬍ .001.
future belongingness. Following a procedure developed by Twenge et al. A one-cell t test confirmed that the difference between baseline
(2001), we randomly assigned participants in Experiment 1 to one of three and post-feedback pain threshold measurement among future alone
social feedback conditions: future belongingness, future alone, and no-
participants was greater than zero in both Experiment 1, t(10) ⫽
feedback control. For Experiment 2, a misfortune control group replaced
the no-feedback control group.
⫺4.83, p ⫽ .001, and Experiment 2, t(9) ⫽ ⫺3.14, p ⫽ .01. In
In the future belongingness condition, participants were given their Experiment 1, future belonging participants showed a significant
correct score on extroversion and then told, as additional feedback, that decrease in pain threshold from baseline, t(10) ⫽ 4.16, p ⫽ .002,
they would have stable, rewarding relationships throughout life. In the though this effect did not replicate in Experiment 2 (t ⬍ 1, ns).
(crucial) future alone condition, participants were given their correct ex- There was no change from baseline in the no-feedback control
troversion score but then told that they would likely end up alone in life. condition in Experiments 1 and 2 (both ts ⬍1, ns). These findings
They were told they might have friends now, but these would drift away suggest that the increase in pain sensitivity was not merely relative
after college, and once they were past the age at which people are to participants in the other two conditions but was an absolute
constantly making new friends, they would spend more and more time
change from baseline measurements. The means and standard
alone. The experimenter said they might marry once or several times, but
these would be short-lived.
deviations are presented in Table 1.
The no-feedback control condition gave neither an extroversion score Pain tolerance. In both studies, socially excluded participants
nor a forecast about the future. The misfortune control condition included showed significantly higher levels of pain tolerance than all other
the extroversion score and a forecast that the participant would become participants. Results from an ANCOVA on the pain tolerance
increasingly accident prone in future years, with many injuries and hospital scores indicated that there was significant variation between the
stays. three experimental groups in Experiment 1, F(2, 30) ⫽ 20.36, p ⬍
Participants then completed the Brief Mood Introspection Scale (BMIS; .001, and Experiment 2, F(2, 27) ⫽ 9.66, p ⫽ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a
Mayer & Gaschke, 1988). After completing the BMIS, measurements of priori contrast confirmed that future alone participants had higher
pain threshold and tolerance (five trials each) were taken. When partici-
overall pain tolerance than both other conditions in Experiment 1,
pants had completed the pain threshold and tolerance measurements, they
F(1, 30) ⫽ 40.60, p ⬍ .001, and Experiment 2, F(1, 26) ⫽ 19.21,
were carefully and thoroughly debriefed. Care was taken to ensure that
participants recognized that the feedback they had received was based on p ⬍ .001.
random assignment and had nothing to do with them, their personality, or A one-cell t test confirmed that the difference between baseline
any form they had completed during the experimental session. The exper- and post-feedback pain tolerance measurement among future alone
imenter also apologized for being misleading regarding the personality participants was greater than zero in Experiment 1, t(10) ⫽ ⫺4.94,
feedback. When the experimenter was certain that participants understood p ⫽ .001, and again in Experiment 2, t(10) ⫽ ⫺2.91, p ⬍ .02,
the true purpose of the experiment and the bogus nature of the personality indicating that their tolerance was significantly higher after receiv-
feedback, participants were given partial course credit, were thanked for ing the feedback than it had been beforehand. There was no change
their time, and were dismissed. from baseline in the other conditions (both ts ⬍1, ns).
Mood and emotion, rejection sensitivity, and self-esteem. To
Results investigate the possible role of mood and emotion in shaping the
observed effects, we tested for fluctuations in mood valence and
We computed both the omnibus F statistic and performed a arousal as a result of personality feedback. For each experiment,
priori contrasts comparing socially excluded participants (i.e., fu- two one-way ANOVAs were carried out by using the Valence and
ture alone) with nonsocially excluded participants (i.e., future Arousal subscales of the emotion measure (BMIS) as dependent
belongingness, no-feedback control, and misfortune control). Pain measures. In Experiment 1, there was no significant variation
threshold and tolerance scores were compared by using baseline between the three social feedback groups in terms of their reported
pain threshold and pain tolerance scores as a covariate. There were mood valence, F(2, 30) ⫽ 2.21, p ⫽ .13. Pairwise comparisons
no main effects or interactions on pain threshold and tolerance (or revealed that future alone participants (M ⫽ 18.18, SD ⫽ 8.15)
any other dependent measure) involving participant gender or reported levels of emotional valence that did not differ signifi-
ethnicity in any other of the studies reported in this article. Hence, cantly from future belonging participants (M ⫽ 20.73, SD ⫽ 8.46;
these variables will not be discussed further. F ⬍ 1, ns). No-feedback control participants (M ⫽ 11.73, SD ⫽
Pain threshold. As predicted, socially excluded participants 13.54) reported more negative moods than future belonging par-
showed significantly higher pain thresholds than participants who ticipants (M ⫽ 20.73, SD ⫽ 8.46), F(1, 30) ⫽ 4.16, p ⫽ .05.
were led to anticipate a future filled with social acceptance or No-feedback control participants (M ⫽ 11.73, SD ⫽ 13.54) also
future misfortune or those who received no personality feedback. reported a somewhat less positive mood than future alone partic-
Results from an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on pain thresh- ipants (M ⫽ 18.18, SD ⫽ 8.15), although this difference did not
old scores in Experiment 1 (using baseline score as covariate) reach significance, F(1, 30) ⫽ 2.14, p ⫽ .15. There was also no
indicated that there was significant variation between the three significant variation between the three social feedback groups in
experimental groups, F(2, 30) ⫽ 17.85, p ⬍ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a terms of their reported mood arousal (F ⬍ 1, ns). Regression
priori contrast confirmed that future alone participants had higher analyses revealed that neither self-esteem nor social sensitivity
pain thresholds compared with future belongingness and no- significantly moderated the effects of condition on pain threshold
feedback control participants, F(1, 30) ⫽ 33.63, p ⬍ .001. For
Experiment 2, the ANCOVA yielded the following: F(2, 27) ⫽
10.84, p ⬍ .001; and the 2–1–1 contrast showed that future alone 1
It should be noted that individual differences on the Introversion–
participants had substantially higher pain threshold scores com- Extroversion Scale did not influence the results in any way.
6 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

Table 1
Pain Threshold and Pain Tolerance, Experiments 1– 4

Future Future Misfortune Other


alone belonging control control

Experiment and measure M SD M SD M SD M SD

1
Pain threshold 31.73 12.46 13.27 12.68 19.01 12.17
Pain tolerance 199.99 50.23 129.40 49.65 133.20 49.88
2
Pain threshold 45.94 32.05 10.55 31.46 14.41 31.89
Pain tolerance 388.22 113.44 276.32 107.68 271.94 114.82
3
Pain threshold 74.57 61.92 19.17 61.81 12.52 61.87
Pain tolerance 392.53 134.59 197.66 136.34 216.58 133.93
4
Pain threshold 62.51 22.11 14.35 21.05 10.24 22.22
Pain tolerance 496.84 120.97 236.05 120.81 244.47 109.89

Note. In all experiments, baseline pain threshold and pain tolerance scores were entered as covariates.
Covariate values for each experiment were as follows: Experiment 1, Pain threshold ⫽ 20.35; Experiment 1, Pain
tolerance ⫽ 163.83; Experiment 2, Pain threshold ⫽ 16.18; Experiment 2, Pain tolerance ⫽ 312.03; Experiment
3, Pain threshold ⫽ 23.02; Experiment 3, Pain tolerance ⫽ 269.02; Experiment 4, Pain threshold ⫽ 25.03;
Experiment 4, Pain tolerance ⫽ 314.37.

or tolerance ( ps ⬎ .20). In Experiment 2, there was no significant esis that people become less sensitive to physical pain as a
variation between the three groups in either mood valence or mood result of having their need to belong thwarted. Participants who
arousal (both Fs ⬍ 1, ns). The means and standard deviations are anticipated a lonely future showed greater tolerance and less
presented in Table 2. Thus, the observed increases in pain toler- sensitivity to physical pain than participants who experienced
ance and threshold among socially excluded participants were not social acceptance, received no personality feedback, or received
due to differences in mood valence and arousal or individual feedback forecasting future physical misfortunes. They also
differences in self-esteem and rejection sensitivity. showed significantly less sensitivity to physical pain than they
themselves had shown on the baseline measures. The estimated
Discussion effect sizes for the increases in pain threshold and pain toler-
Social exclusion produced increases in both pain threshold ance among socially excluded participants were quite large
and pain tolerance in both studies, consistent with the hypoth- (Cohen, 1977), exceeding a full standard deviation in each case.

Table 2
Self-Reports of Emotion by Experimental Condition, Experiments 1–5

Future Future Misfortune Other


alone belonging control control

Experiment and measure M SD M SD M SD M SD

1
Mood Valence 18.18 8.15 20.73 8.46 11.73 13.54
Mood Arousal 25.64 5.63 26.91 7.56 28.91 7.42
2
Mood Valence 17.80 13.40 22.90 12.97 15.30 13.37
Mood Arousal 26.60 5.50 24.90 7.48 27.10 6.15
3
Mood Valence 10.40 12.13 18.70 9.78 15.20 12.93
Mood Arousal 28.40 7.14 26.40 6.29 26.10 6.76
4
Mood Valence 19.50 10.71 16.90 11.23 11.45 10.51
Mood Arousal 27.40 5.87 22.90 120.81 24.64 4.41
5
Negative Affect 12.55 3.52 12.50 3.51 13.91 4.39
Positive Affect 22.98 5.74 24.76 5.23 23.32 4.64

Note. Values represent mean scores on either the BMIS Mood Valence or Mood Arousal subscales (Mayer &
Gaschke, 1988) or the PANAS Positive Affect and Negative Affect subscales (Watson et al., 1988). BMIS ⫽
Brief Mood Introspection Scale; PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 7

The results of Experiment 1 and 2 were not due to any differ- Florida State won this particular game. These two measures were highly
ences in reported mood valence or mood arousal. Future alone correlated (r ⫽ .50, p ⫽ .005) and were combined to create a Florida State
participants did not report moods that differed significantly from football “fan” index.
those reported by future belonging or no-feedback control partic- Last, participants completed five additional measurements of pain
threshold and tolerance. Participants were then debriefed, thanked for their
ipants. Neither rejection sensitivity nor trait self-esteem played a
time, given partial course credit, and dismissed.
moderating role in predicting the relationship between social ex-
clusion and physical pain threshold and pain tolerance. Thus, a
Results
future diagnostic forecast of social exclusion led to substantial
increases in pain threshold and pain tolerance compared with Pain threshold and pain tolerance: Replication. As in Exper-
socially accepted and control participants. iments 1 and 2, a future diagnostic forecast of social exclusion
produced significant increases in pain threshold compared with
participants who anticipated a future filled with meaningful rela-
Experiment 3: Not Feeling Future Pain
tionships or who received no feedback on their personality test. An
Experiment 3 tested whether social exclusion would have im- ANCOVA on the pain threshold scores, using baseline pain thresh-
plications for affective forecasting. Affective forecasting involves old scores as a covariate, revealed significant variation between the
simulating emotional reactions to possible future events. Exclusion three experimental groups, F(2, 27) ⫽ 9.07, p ⫽ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1
causes the emotion system to cease functioning properly, indeed a priori contrast also showed that future alone participants had
muting and numbing emotional reactions. It is therefore possible significantly higher pain threshold scores than future belonging
that hypothetical emotional reactions to the future (i.e., imagining and no-feedback control participants, F(2, 27) ⫽ 17.96, p ⬍ .001.
how one would feel) will also be numbed. Accordingly, rejected A one-cell t test confirmed that the difference between baseline
participants (who reported emotional states that did not differ from and post-feedback pain threshold measurement among future alone
participants in the other conditions in Experiments 1 and 2) should participants was greater than zero, t(9) ⫽ ⫺2.50, p ⫽ .03, whereas
make affective forecasts of relative emotional numbness compared there was no change from baseline in the other conditions (both ts
with accepted and control participants. That is, rejected partici- ⬍1, ns). Socially excluded participants also showed increased pain
pants should report less happiness when forecasting their emo- tolerance compared with participants who experienced social ac-
tional reaction to a positive event and less sadness and distress ceptance and participants who received no feedback on a person-
when forecasting their emotional reaction to a negative event. ality test. An ANCOVA on the pain tolerance scores revealed
Socially accepted and control participants, in contrast, should significant variation between the three experimental groups, F(2,
exhibit the previously documented impact bias, in which people 27) ⫽ 18.91, p ⬍ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast confirmed that
overestimate their happiness and sadness to possible future events future alone participants had significantly higher pain tolerance
(e.g., Wilson et al., 2004). The current study also tested whether scores than both future belonging and no-feedback control partic-
increases in pain threshold and pain tolerance were related to the ipants, F(2, 27) ⫽ 37.69, p ⬍ .001. A one-cell t test confirmed that
predicted unemotional affective forecasts among socially excluded the difference between baseline and post-feedback pain threshold
participants. measurement among future alone participants was greater than
zero, t(9) ⫽ ⫺2.93, p ⫽ .02. There was no change from baseline
in the other conditions (both ts ⬍ 1, ns). Thus, future alone
Method participants showed substantial and absolute increases in both pain
Participants. Thirty right-handed undergraduates (19 women) partici- threshold and pain tolerance. The means and standard deviations
pated for partial course credit. Of the participants, 77% were White and are presented in Table 1.
23% were a racial minority. The average age was 18.6 years. Affective forecasting. Replicating previous affective forecast-
Materials and procedure. Participants arrived at the laboratory indi- ing research, the results indicated that future belonging and no-
vidually for an experiment ostensibly concerned with the relationship feedback control participants predicted strong emotional responses
between personality, verbal and nonverbal behaviors, and physical sensi- to a Florida State victory and defeat. Most relevant to the current
tivity. After giving informed consent, participants completed baseline
investigation, however, was the finding that participants in the
measurements of pain threshold and pain tolerance (two trials each).
Participants then completed a personality test and were exposed to the
future alone condition predicted relatively neutral emotional reac-
same social exclusion manipulation used in Experiment 1. By random tions to both football outcomes. A mixed-model analysis of vari-
assignment, participants were told that they had a personality type in which ance (ANOVA) was conducted by using the personality feedback
they would likely end up alone (future alone) or that their personality type condition as a between-subjects factor and responses to the ques-
was diagnostic of a future filled with many rewarding and lasting relation- tions “Please predict what your overall happiness would be right
ships (future belonging). Another group was not given any feedback on after the game if Florida State beats Florida” and “Please predict
their personality test (no-feedback control). what your overall happiness would be right after the game if
After receiving their personality feedback, participants completed the Florida State loses to Florida” as two levels of a within-subjects
BMIS mood and affective forecasting measures. The latter were modeled factor. Results revealed a significant Personality Feedback ⫻
after previous research by Wilson and colleagues (Wilson, Wheatley,
Differential Predicted Happiness interaction, F(2, 27) ⫽ 16.22,
Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, 2000). Participants were reminded that the
Florida State football team was scheduled to compete against its closest
p ⬍ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast confirmed that future alone
rival in approximately 2 months and were asked to predict what their level participants (M ⫽ 4.70, SD ⫽ .99) predicted a significantly lower
of overall happiness would be directly after a victory and after a loss. degree of happiness in response to Florida State defeating the
Participants also reported to what degree they considered themselves a University of Florida compared with both future belonging (M ⫽
Florida State University football fan and to what extent they cared whether 6.60, SD ⫽ 0.69) and no-feedback control participants (M ⫽ 6.70,
8 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

SD ⫽ .67), F(2, 27) ⫽ 41.42, p ⬍ .001. In addition, a 2⫺1⫺1 a in Experiments 1 and 2, socially excluded participants exhibited
priori contrast confirmed that future alone participants (M ⫽ 3.50, increases in both pain threshold and pain tolerance compared with
SD ⫽ 0.71) predicted greater happiness in response to Florida accepted and control participants. Excluded participants also re-
State being beaten by the University of Florida than both future ported emotional states that did not differ from accepted and
belonging (M ⫽ 2.30, SD ⫽ 1.34) and no-feedback control (M ⫽ control participants. It is difficult to ascertain whether excluded
1.90, SD ⫽ 1.10) participants, F(2, 27) ⫽ 11.20, p ⫽ .002. Thus, participants truly felt no emotion or whether they were simply
future alone participants predicted less happiness to a Florida State refusing to admit that they were upset. If the latter were true,
victory and less sadness to a Florida State defeat compared with however, excluded participants should be willing to describe and
no-feedback control and future belonging participants. predict their future emotional reactions, such as being happy if
Supplementary analyses were conducted to test whether the their university were to win an important football game 2 months
exclusion, acceptance, and control groups differed in the degree to in the future. The findings of Experiment 3 provide evidence that
which they perceived themselves as Florida State football fans and social exclusion causes the emotion system to cease functioning
the degree to which they cared about the outcome of the upcoming normally. Socially excluded participants not only failed to report
football game. Results from an ANOVA revealed no significant much in the way of current emotion; they also predicted little in the
variation between the three groups on the fan index, F(2, 27) ⫽ way of a future emotional response to a major meaningful event.
2.29, p ⫽ .12. Pairwise comparisons revealed that future alone Excluded participants predicted less happiness when forecasting
participants (M ⫽ 10.30, SD ⫽ 2.63) had scores on the fan index their emotional reaction to a positive event and greater happiness
that were not significantly different from future belonging partic- when forecasting their emotional reaction to a negative event,
ipants (M ⫽ 8.90, SD ⫽ 3.45), F(1, 27) ⫽ 1.04, p ⫽ .32, and not compared with participants in the other conditions.
different from no-feedback control participants (M ⫽ 11.60, SD ⫽ It would have been plausible for exclusion to cause people to
2.27), F(1, 27) ⫽ 1.40, p ⫽ .25. Future belonging participants identify either more or less strongly with Florida State University
(M ⫽ 8.90, SD ⫽ 3.45) had scores on the fan index that were and its football team, but they did not. In any case, the failure of
marginally lower than those of the no-feedback participants (M ⫽ rejected people to predict much in the way of an emotional
11.60, SD ⫽ 2.27), F(1, 27) ⫽ 4.28, p ⬍ .06. Thus, the attenuated reaction to future football outcomes was not mediated by changes
forecasts (in the future alone condition) of affective reactions to in their identification with Florida State University or expressed
the future football game outcome were not due to participants concern with the outcome of the football game. After hearing that
feeling as less a fan or feeling less interested in the football game. they were likely to be alone later in their lives, they retained their
Mood and emotion. To assess difference in mood, we con- (relatively strong and positive) identification with their university
ducted two one-way ANOVAs by using the Mood Valence and (i.e., they acknowledged their identity as Florida State University
Mood Arousal subscales of the BMIS (Mayer & Gaschke, 1988) as football fans who cared about the outcome of an important game),
dependent measures. Both ANOVAs revealed no significant vari- but they did not think they would feel much emotion about the
ation between the three experimental groups (both Fs ⬍ 1.28, ps ⬎ outcome of its football game against a major rival. Thus, the
.30; see Table 2). These results suggest that increases in pain results of Experiment 3 demonstrated that the body responds to
threshold and pain tolerance following social exclusion were not social exclusion feedback with a kind of physical shock reaction
the result of differences in mood. that includes numbness and insensitivity to current physical pain
Link between physical and emotional insensitivity. To test the and possible future emotional events (cf. Campbell, Baumeister,
hypothesis that unemotional affective forecasts were related to Dhavale, & Tice, 2003).
increases in pain threshold and pain tolerance, we conducted a
regression analysis in which the measures of physical insensitivity Experiment 4: Not Feeling Someone Else’s Pain
predicted measures of emotional insensitivity. For ease of analysis,
we computed a differential predicted happiness index by subtract- If social exclusion causes the emotion system to cease function-
ing scores to the question “Please predict what your overall hap- ing normally, one should be less capable of empathy, and so one’s
piness would be right after the game if Florida State beats Florida” reaction to another person’s suffering should be muted. Partici-
from scores to the question “Please predict what your overall pants in Experiment 4 were asked to empathize with someone who
happiness would be right after the game if Florida State loses to was suffering over a recent romantic failure. The main predictions
Florida.” Pain threshold scores predicted differential predicted were that social exclusion would lead to less empathy toward the
happiness index scores, t(27) ⫽ ⫺2.36, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.32, p ⬍ .03. Pain stimulus person who had suffered the romantic rejection and that
tolerance scores were also a significant predictor of differential this emotional insensitivity would be related to increases in both
predicted happiness scores, t(27) ⫽ ⫺2.68, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.31, p ⫽ .01. pain threshold and pain tolerance.
These findings provide evidence in support of the hypothesis that
social exclusion has two linked consequences, namely, numbness Method
to physiological pain and emotional numbness to possible future
events. Participants. Thirty-one undergraduates (21 women) participated in
this experiment in exchange for partial course credit. Right-handedness
was stipulated, but one left-handed participant signed up and participated
Discussion anyway. Of the participants, 71% were White and 29% were a racial
minority. The average age was 18.5 years.
Experiment 3 provided support for the hypothesis that responses Materials and procedure. The materials and procedure for Experiment
to social exclusion operate via a shared psychological system that 4 were similar to Experiments 1–3. Participants arrived at the laboratory
has related consequences for both physical and emotional pain. As individually for an experiment ostensibly concerned with the relationship
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 9

between different aspects of personality, verbal and nonverbal perfor- experimental groups. An ANCOVA on the pain tolerance scores
mance, and physical sensitivity. After giving informed consent, partici- showed significant variation between the three experimental
pants completed baseline measurements of pain threshold and pain toler- groups, F(2, 28) ⫽ 45.25, p ⬍ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast
ance (two trials each) and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Eysenck
confirmed that future alone participants had significantly higher
& Eysenck, 1975). By random assignment, participants received future
alone feedback, future belonging feedback, or no feedback.
pain tolerance scores than participants who did not experience
After completing the BMIS mood measure, participants completed a social exclusion (i.e., future belonging and no-feedback control),
measure of empathy. The experimenter explained that there was another F(2, 28) ⫽ 89.99, p ⬍ .001. A one-cell t test confirmed that the
experiment being conducted in the laboratory in which 1 participant writes difference between baseline and post-feedback pain threshold
an essay about something going on in his or her life and another participant measurement among future alone participants was significantly
reads the essay and answers some questions about it. Participants were told greater than zero, t(10) ⫽ ⫺6.69, p ⬍ .001. There was no change
that the participant who was supposed to read and respond to the essay did from baseline in the other conditions (both ts ⬍1, ns). The means
not show up for the experiment. The experimenter explained that the
and standard deviations are presented in Table 1.
participant would read and respond to the essay. Participants were then
handed a manila folder that contained a handwritten essay (in male or Empathy. Socially excluded participants showed less empathy
female handwriting,2 to match the participant’s gender) and short ques- than other participants toward the romantically rejected stimulus
tionnaire. The content of the essay was adapted from Batson et al. (1995). person. An ANOVA on the five-item empathic emotion index
The essay read (in part): revealed significant variation between the three experimental
groups, F(2, 28) ⫽ 10.10, p ⬍ .001. A 2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast
Two days ago I broke up with my (girlfriend) boyfriend. We’ve been demonstrated that future alone participants reported feeling less
going together since our junior year in high school and have been
sympathy, compassion, warmth, tenderness, and softheartedness
really close, and it’s been great being at FSU together. I thought (s)he
felt the same, but things have changed. Now, (s)he wants to date other toward the participant who had recently experienced relationship
people. (S)He says (s)he still cares a lot about me, but (s)he doesn’t loss, compared with future belonging and no-feedback control
want to be tied down to just one person. I’ve been real down. It’s all participants, F(2, 28) ⫽ 19.67, p ⬍ .001. Pairwise comparisons
I think about. My friends all tell me that I’ll meet other (girls)guys and using the ANOVA mean-square error showed that future alone
they say that all I need is for something good to happen to cheer me participants (M ⫽ 32.80, SD ⫽ 7.66) had significantly lower
up. I guess they’re right, but so far that hasn’t happened. scores on the empathy index compared with future belonging
Participants reported how sympathetic, warm, compassionate, soft-
participants (M ⫽ 42.00, SD ⫽ 5.79), F(1, 28) ⫽ 12.37, p ⫽ .002,
hearted, and tender they felt toward the author of the essay. These adjec- d ⫽ 1.35. Future alone participants (M ⫽ 32.80, SD ⫽ 7.66) also
tives have been used in previous research to measure empathy (Batson, had lower scores on the empathy index compared with no-
1987, 1991; Batson et al., 1995). The internal reliability for the empathy- feedback control participants (M ⫽ 43.55, SD ⫽ 3.59), F(1, 28) ⫽
related adjectives was good (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .92), and therefore an 17.67, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 1.79. Future belonging participants did not
empathy index was created by summing responses to the five empathy differ in their empathy scores compared with no-feedback control
adjectives (sympathetic, warm, compassionate, softhearted, and tender). participants (F ⬍ 1, ns). Thus, future alone participants expressed
When finished, participants placed the essay and the questionnaire in an
less emotional responsiveness to another person who had experi-
envelope, sealed it, and handed it back to the experimenter. The experi-
menter then left the laboratory ostensibly to return the envelope to the enced exclusion compared with future belonging and no-feedback
experimenter for the other study. When the experimenter returned, the control participants.
participant completed additional measurements of pain threshold and pain Mood and emotion. Two one-way ANOVAs were conducting
tolerance (five trials each). Participants were then thoroughly debriefed, by using the Mood Valence and Mood Arousal subscales of the
thanked for their time, given partial course credit, and dismissed. BMIS as dependent measures. Both ANOVAs revealed no signif-
icant variation between the three experimental groups (both Fs ⬍
Results 1.79, ps ⬎ .20; see Table 2).
Link between physical and emotional insensitivity. To exam-
Pain threshold and pain tolerance: Replication. As in the ine whether the lack of empathic responding among socially ex-
previous three experiments, a future diagnostic forecast of social cluded participants was related to increased pain tolerance and
exclusion produced increases in pain threshold compared with pain threshold, a regression analysis was conducted in which pain
participants who anticipated a future filled with social acceptance threshold and pain tolerance scores predicted empathy scores.
or who received no feedback on a personality test. An ANCOVA Results revealed that pain threshold scores were a significant
on the pain threshold scores revealed significant variation between predictor of empathy scores, t(28) ⫽ ⫺3.90, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.59, p ⫽ .001.
the three experimental groups, F(2, 28) ⫽ 54.12, p ⬍ .001. A
Pain tolerance scores also predicted empathy scores, t(27) ⫽
2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast confirmed that future alone participants
⫺5.24, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.61, p ⬍ .001. Thus, the increase in tolerance and
had significantly higher pain threshold scores compared with fu-
threshold for physical pain following social exclusion was related
ture belonging and no-feedback control participants, F(2, 28) ⫽
to lack of empathic concern to others who had also experienced
108.15, p ⬍ .001. A one-cell t test confirmed that the difference
social exclusion.
between baseline and post-feedback pain threshold measurement
among future alone participants was significant, t(9) ⫽ ⫺5.99, p ⬍
.001, whereas there was no change from baseline in the other 2
The essay was handwritten by either a male or female research assis-
conditions (both ts ⬍1, ns). tant. No participants expressed suspicion about the gender of the author of
Socially excluded participants also demonstrated a significantly the essay. In fact, many participants commented during the debriefing that
higher tolerance for pain than participants assigned to the other the handwriting was indicative of either a male or female author.
10 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

Discussion Materials and procedure. Participants arrived at a large classroom in


groups of 10 –20 for a study ostensibly concerned with how people under-
Experiment 4 provided further evidence that the emotion system stand each other. After giving informed consent, participants completed a
temporarily ceases to function normally following social exclu- short questionnaire packet that contained instructions for an autobiograph-
sion. As compared with socially accepted and no-feedback con- ical narrative, a mood questionnaire, and additional materials aimed at
trols, socially excluded participants showed very little empathy measuring empathic responding. By random assignment, participants were
assigned to one of three autobiographical narrative conditions: social
toward another person who had suffered a recent romantic
rejection, social acceptance, and control.
breakup. In fact, 1 participant in the future alone condition com- The instructions for the social rejection narrative read as follows:
mented “tough shit” upon reading about the other participant’s
romantic woes—a comment that clearly indicates an uncharitable On the next pages, you will write an essay about a time when you
and unsympathetic attitude toward another person’s heartbreak. experienced rejection or exclusion by others. Think of a time when
The increases in pain threshold and pain tolerance among so- you felt that others did not want to be in your company and when you
cially excluded participants were related to their lack of empathy did not feel a strong sense of belongingness with another person or
toward the other participant. This supports the broad view that group. Nearly everyone has experienced such an experience more than
once; please choose an especially important and memorable event.
social exclusion causes both physical numbness and emotional
insensitivity to others. The instructions urged participants to describe the rejection experience in
as thorough detail as possible and to provide the “full story.” Other
participants wrote about a childhood experience of social acceptance. The
Experiment 5: I Can’t Feel Another Person’s Physical instructions for the social acceptance narrative mirrored the social rejection
Pain instructions and replaced only words that dealt directly with the experience
of rejection or acceptance. Participants assigned to the control condition
One possible boundary condition for Experiment 4 is that it were instructed to write a detailed essay about one of the things he or she
explored the effects of social exclusion on empathy toward some- did the previous day.
one else who had recently suffered romantic rejection. Although After participants completed writing their narrative, participants com-
the essay used in Experiment 4 has been used in previous research pleted the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, &
to evoke general empathy (Batson et al., 1995), the lack of em- Tellegen, 1988). Participants then read a short vignette that depicted a
pathic responding among socially excluded participants may have college student (of unknown gender) and was meant to evoke empathic
been due to the author of the essay having also experienced a form responding from participants. The vignette read (in part):
of social exclusion. Excluded participants may have refused to
Two days ago I broke my leg playing intramural sports. I’ve been
empathize with another rejected person because the other person’s playing on the same intramural team for the past three years and I’m
rejection may have reminded them of their own recent social upset that my season has been cut short. I’m experiencing pain
exclusion. Therefore, Study 5 measured empathy toward someone because of my injury. I’m also having a tough time getting around
who had suffered a broken leg. campus, as there are lots of hills and stairs that make it hard to use my
Exclusion was manipulated by having participants complete a crutches on. The parking people won’t let me get a handicapped
vivid recall task in which they completed a brief autobiographical permit because they said my injury was only temporary. I’ve been real
narrative recalling a time they experienced social rejection, social down. It’s all I think about.
acceptance, or an unrelated event. This was meant to prime par- Participants reported how sympathetic, warm, compassionate, soft-
ticipants with thoughts of their relevant experience, which would hearted, and tender they felt toward the author of the essay. The internal
in turn influence responding on a questionnaire designed to mea- reliability of these five items was good (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .86), and
sure empathy. Previous research has shown that real and imagined therefore an empathy index was created by summing responses to these
events activate many of the same neural and psychological pro- five items. When participants had finished the empathy rating task, they
cesses (Kosslyn et al., 1999; McGuire, Shah, & Murray, 1993), were debriefed, given partial course credit, thanked for their time, and
and other research has shown that the exclusion vivid recall task dismissed.
evokes responses similar to those found in manipulations of im-
mediate rejection (Gardner et al., 2000; Pickett, Gardner, & Results
Knowles, 2004). It was predicted that the current manipulation of
rejection would produce the same emotional numbness as in ex- Empathy. Participants who wrote about a time they experi-
cluded participants in Experiments 1– 4. The main prediction of enced social rejection showed significantly less empathy toward
Experiment 5 was that socially rejected participants would express the author compared with participants in the other two conditions.
less empathy toward the author of the essay compared with ac- An ANOVA on the five-item empathic emotion index revealed
cepted and control participants. significant variation between the three experimental groups, F(2,
122) ⫽ 6.72, p ⫽ .002. A 2⫺1⫺1 a priori contrast confirmed that
socially rejected participants felt significantly less sympathy,
Method warmth, compassion, tenderness, and softheartedness toward the
author of the essay compared with both socially accepted and
Participants. One-hundred twenty-five participants (84 women) par-
ticipated in this study in exchange for partial course credit. Results from 9
control participants, F(1, 122) ⫽ 12.79, p ⫽ .001. Pairwise com-
participants were discarded from all analyses because of incomplete ques- parisons using the ANOVA mean-square error showed that re-
tionnaire packets (thus, there were originally 134 participants). Of the jected participants (M ⫽ 26.33, SD ⫽ 9.42) empathized with the
participants, 74% were White and 26% were a racial minority. The average essay author significantly less than did accepted participants (M ⫽
age was 18.5 years. 31.82, SD ⫽ 8.65), F(1, 122) ⫽ 7.53, p ⫽ .007, d ⫽ .61. In
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 11

addition, rejected participants (M ⫽ 26.33, SD ⫽ 9.42) showed participants who experienced social acceptance, received a nega-
less empathic concern toward the essay author compared with tively valenced future diagnostic forecast, or received no feedback
control participants (M ⫽ 32.96, SD ⫽ 8.43), F(1, 122) ⫽ 12.21, on their personality test.
p ⫽ .001, d ⫽ .74. Socially accepted and control participants did The second goal of the current investigation was to examine
not differ significantly in terms of how much they empathized with whether insensitivity to physical pain might be linked to emotional
the essay author (F ⬍ 1, ns). Thus, rejected participants were quite numbness following social exclusion. In all five experiments,
unsympathetic toward a member of their peer group who had excluded participants reported emotional states that did not differ
suffered a physically painful injury and was having difficulty significantly from socially accepted and control participants. Ad-
adjusting to the changes brought about by the injury. These results mittedly, these results suffer from the uncertainties that accompany
suggest that rejection brings about a lack of empathic responding the use of self-reports of emotional responding, especially the idea
toward others, regardless of whether the person has experienced that excluded participants might be reluctant to admit that they are
rejection or another traumatic and physically painful event. upset over the experimental feedback. But excluded participants
Mood and emotion. Two one-way ANOVAs were conducting should be willing and able to predict future emotional reactions or
by using the Positive Affect and Negative Affect subscales of the empathize with others. The results of Experiments 3–5 showed that
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule as dependent measures. excluded participants failed to make the standard predictions about
There was no significant variation between the three experimental their emotional reactions to future events and failed to empathize
groups in terms of reported positive affect, F(2, 122) ⫽ 1.31, p ⫽ with others’ suffering. Social exclusion feedback caused people to
.27, or negative affect, F(2, 122) ⫽ 1.89, p ⫽ .16 (see Table 2). predict less happiness over a future football victory and less
sadness over a possible defeat. Social exclusion also led partici-
Discussion pants to become less empathic toward another person who had just
suffered a relationship loss, which as a form of social exclusion
Experiment 5 provided evidence that rejected participants ex- might have elicited high empathy from participants who them-
pressed less empathy than accepted and control participants toward selves had also experienced exclusion. And recalling a past expe-
a person suffering physical pain and inconvenience from a broken rience of social exclusion made people less empathic toward a
leg. These findings provide converging evidence that the emotion student who was suffering over a broken leg (which was unrelated
system temporarily ceases to function normally following social to social exclusion). These findings suggest that social exclusion
exclusion, which leads to emotional numbness toward another produces a sweeping shutdown of the emotional system.
person in physical pain. Thus, the excluded people’s lack of Thus, the findings from the current experiments indicate that the
empathy found in Experiment 4 appears to be part of a broader body responds to social exclusion in much the same manner as it
pattern rather than being tied to the special case of refusing to responds to physical injury. Exclusion may well produce a bio-
empathize with someone else who has suffered social exclusion. chemical reaction that leads to temporary numbness to physical
pain. This physical numbness is also linked to emotional respond-
General Discussion ing, which is demonstrated in emotional insensitivity.
The findings of Experiment 3 make a novel contribution to the
Social exclusion threatens a fundamental human motivation for affective forecasting literature by identifying a moderating vari-
strong and stable relationships. Such a threat strikes at the core of able to the previously documented pattern of people overestimat-
human psychological and physical well-being. Social exclusion ing the impact of future emotional events on their lives (e.g.,
can have destructive effects on mental and physical well-being, but Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). Exclusion temporarily disrupted the
socially excluded people frequently report relatively numb emo- capacity for the emotion system to function normally, and this led
tional states. This paradox leaves open the mystery of why socially excluded participants to predict a significantly less emotional
excluded people often respond to such a disturbing event as social response to both future positive and negative events than partici-
exclusion in an emotionally numb manner. One possible explana- pants who had experienced social acceptance or than control
tion for this anomalous emotional responding following social participants. This finding is potentially intriguing in that socially
exclusion is that the emotion system temporarily ceases function- excluded people made affective forecasts that were likely more
ing normally, leaving a person momentarily unable to respond to realistic than participants in the other conditions (D. T. Gilbert,
emotional events in a customary fashion. This should lead to personal communication, December 16, 2004), though we did not
numbness to physically painful stimuli and emotional numbness to collect data on participants’ actual emotional responses to the
current and possible future events. game’s outcome. Regardless of accuracy, our finding that de-
Across four experiments, socially excluded participants consis- creased sensitivity to pain was closely linked to affective forecasts
tently showed both decreased sensitivity and increased tolerance to of indifference provides further reason to think that the physical
physical pain, compared with socially accepted and control par- pain and social emotion systems may share a common physiolog-
ticipants and compared with their own baseline responses. The ical basis (or at least are intimately linked).
estimated effect size of the difference in pain threshold and pain In sum, the results of the current studies provide evidence of an
tolerance between socially excluded participants and participants immediate physical numbing effect in the aftermath of social
in the other conditions was consistently large, exceeding the cri- exclusion, which is related to the degree of emotional numbness
teria typically used to describe large effects (Cohen, 1977). Thus, that socially excluded people exhibit. These findings help to re-
participants responded to the social exclusion feedback by growing solve the paradox in the rejection literature by showing that one
increasingly numb to physical pain stimuli, and these increases in possible reason people report relative emotional numbness to
pain threshold and pain tolerance were large in comparison to social exclusion is that they experience a physiological reaction
12 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

that creates numbness to physically and emotionally painful stim- ps ⬎ .24). Similar results emerged in Experiment 4. After control-
uli. It is not likely that these physical numbing effects are long ling for baseline pain sensitivity measurement and social exclusion
lasting. Instead, physical numbness likely functions as an imme- condition, we found that the relationship between Time 2 pain
diate buffer that wards off potentially threatening aspects of one’s threshold and tolerance scores and empathy scores was nonsignif-
current surroundings. The possible long-term effects of repeated or icant (␤s ⫽ ⫺.18 and ⫺.16, respectively; both ps ⬎ .14). Thus,
chronic social rejection remain to be explored. these findings indicate that the changes induced by condition in
pain sensitivity were directly related to the differences in emo-
Limitations and Alternative Explanations tional sensitivity as a result of the social exclusion manipulation
and that within each condition there were not any strong and
The five experiments reported in this article provide consistent consistent relationships between the physical and emotional sen-
evidence that social exclusion produces increases in pain threshold sitivity measures after controlling for baseline measurements and
and pain tolerance and that this physical numbness to pain is social exclusion condition.
related to emotional insensitivity. Despite the consistency of these A final explanation is that increased pain tolerance and pain
effects, however, several alternative explanations warrant further threshold among socially excluded participants were due to in-
consideration. A first possibility is that social exclusion simply creased self-regulation instead of a temporary shutdown in the
constitutes a form of bad news. From this perspective, receiving emotion system. Pain tolerance is often used as a measure of
any negatively valenced feedback should temporarily cause the self-regulation (Hilgard et al., 1974), and participants may have
emotion system to cease functioning normally, resulting in numb- responded to social exclusion with increased self-regulation as a
ness to painful physical stimuli. The results of our misfortune means of improving their moods. The results of Experiments 1–5
control condition in Experiment 2 provide evidence that is incon- provided consistent and converging evidence that socially ex-
sistent with this perspective. Participants assigned to that condition cluded participants did not report moods that were significantly
received a negatively valenced future diagnostic forecast that was different from socially accepted or control participants, which
unrelated to their future belongingness status, namely, that they casts doubt on the likelihood that excluded participants were trying
would become accident prone later in life. Despite the relative to improve their moods. The present data cannot easily rule out the
negativity of this diagnostic forecast, misfortune control partici- idea that reduced sensitivity and increased tolerance to pain re-
pants responded to painful stimuli in a manner that was most flects increased self-regulation in response to social exclusion. Past
similar to participants who experienced social acceptance or who work has clearly shown the opposite pattern, however, namely,
received no personality feedback. that social exclusion leads to poor self-regulation (Baumeister et
A second possibility is that the methods used for assessing pain al., 2005; DeWall, Baumeister, & Vohs, 2004). If the current
threshold assessed something other than pain threshold, such as a results could be explained in terms of effective self-regulation,
general ability to report tactile stimulation. If the pain threshold excluded participants would have also probably shown high levels
measures were tapping something other than physical numbness to of empathy toward others’ suffering. This was, however, not the
pain, the pain threshold scores in the current study should not be case.
associated with measures of tolerance for physical pain. Against
this line of reasoning, Time 2 pain threshold scores (controlling for Concluding Remarks
baseline measurement) were strongly correlated with pain toler-
ance scores (controlling for baseline measurement). The correla- The purpose of the current investigation was to resolve the
tions between pain threshold and pain tolerance were consistently seeming paradox that people respond to social exclusion with
large and were reduced significantly when controlling for social emotional numbness. The need to belong is a fundamental and
exclusion condition. The correlations were as follows: Experiment pervasive human motivation, and the behavioral consequences of
1, r(29) ⫽ .43, p ⬍ .02; controlling for exclusion condition, social exclusion are often quite large. It was therefore a mystery as
r(30) ⫽ ⫺.20, p ⫽ .30; Experiment 2, r(29) ⫽ .82, p ⬍ .001; to why people fail to show strong emotional responses in reaction
controlling for exclusion condition, r(27) ⫽ .67, p ⬍ .001; Exper- to such a seemingly momentous event as social exclusion.
iment 3, r(26) ⫽ .46, p ⫽ .01; controlling for exclusion condition, The apparent solution to the mystery, as suggested by the
r(25) ⫽ ⫺.06, p ⫽ .77; Experiment 4, r(27) ⫽ .85, p ⬍ .001; present findings, is that the body responds to social exclusion in
controlling for exclusion condition, r(26) ⫽ .22, p ⫽ .27. Thus, it much the same way (and possibly by means of the same physio-
appears that our measures of pain threshold were measuring one logical mechanisms) as it responds to painful physical injury.
component of physical pain sensation as a result of the social Physical sensation and emotion reactivity become dulled and in-
exclusion manipulation, namely, sensitivity to physical pain. sensitive. This may temporarily reduce suffering and enable the
Another possible limitation in the current studies is whether individual to begin to cope without feeling acutely distressed each
there is any relationship between chronic levels of pain sensitivity moment.
and emotional sensitivity not induced by the social exclusion Reduced suffering and potentially better coping would of course
manipulation. To examine this possibility, we predicted Time 2 be adaptive responses, but past work has suggested that the im-
pain sensitivity scores from the emotional sensitivity measures in mediate behavioral effects of rejection are often far from beneficial
Experiments 3 and 4, while simultaneously controlling for social (e.g., Baumeister et al., 2005; Twenge et al., 2001, 2002, 2003),
exclusion condition and baseline pain sensitivity scores. In Exper- even extending to behaviors that would seemingly reduce chances
iment 3, results revealed no significant relationship between Time of gaining future acceptance. From the perspective of the present
2 pain threshold and tolerance scores and scores on the differential findings, one possible implication is that people use their emo-
predicted happiness index (␤s ⫽ .08 and .15, respectively; both tional faculties to help understand their world. Emotional numb-
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 13

ness may therefore impair the person’s ability to relate to others Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different
and function effectively in a complex social world. The present contributions of the human amygdala ventromedial prefrontal cortex to
results showed that excluded people were seemingly unable to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5473–5481.
furnish the typical (and presumably socially adaptive) patterns of Buckley, K. E., Winkel, R. E., & Leary, M. R. (2004). Reactions to
affective forecasting and empathy. Most likely there are other acceptance and rejection: Effects of level and sequence of relational
evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 14 –28.
cognitive tasks that social animals routinely perform with the aid
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional
of their emotion systems and that, accordingly, may abruptly be influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4,
lost when social exclusion temporarily causes the emotional sys- 215–222.
tem to cease functioning normally. Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Berntson, G. G. (2003). The anatomy
There was no sign that people realize how much they are of loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 71–74.
affected by rejection. When asked to forecast their future reactions, Campbell, W. K., Baumeister, R. F., Dhavale, D., & Tice, D. M. (2003).
our excluded participants did not say they were uncertain as to how Responding to major threats to self-esteem: A preliminary, narrative
they would feel. Rather, they engaged in self-reflection and told us study of ego-shock. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22,
they would not have a strong emotional response to whether their 79 –96.
football team won or lost. Recent work has increasingly shown that Carden, S. E., Hernandez, N., & Hofer, M. A. (1996). The isolation and
some emotional capabilities are essential for good decision making companion comfort responses of 7- and 3-day-old rat pups are modu-
lated by drugs active at the opioid receptor. Behavioral Neuroscience,
and other adaptive behavioral patterns (e.g., Bechara, Damasio,
110, 324 –330.
Damasio, & Lee, 1999; Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch,
Carden, S. E., & Hofer, M. A. (1990). Socially mediated reduction of
2001). If normal emotional functioning is essential for effective isolation distress in rat pups is blocked by naltrexone but not by Ro
decision making and behaviors, then when the emotion system 15–1788. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 457– 463.
ceases to function normally, the person may be given faulty Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
information with which to make decisions and interact with others, New York: Academic Press.
which may have potentially costly results. Craig, A. D., & Dostrovsky, J. O. (1999). Medulla to thalamus. In P. Wall
The emerging image of the socially rejected person is thus one & R. Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of pain (pp. 183–214). New York:
of someone who is emotionally and hence socially impaired— but Churchill Livingstone.
may not realize it. Without a functioning emotion system, the DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Recovering from
person may not anticipate how his or her actions will affect others rejection: Undoing the self-regulation deficits stemming from social
or even the person’s own future outcomes. Without recognizing exclusion. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications for rejection sensitivity
this impairment, the person may even act on those faulty intuitions.
for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
In that way, the short-term relief brought by the emotional numb-
70, 1327–1343.
ness caused by social exclusion may also bring destructive social Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: The
consequences. neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294 –300.
References Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003, October
Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness. London: Methuen. 10). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science,
Bancroft, J., Skirmshire, A., & Simkins, S. (1976). The reasons people give 302, 290 –292.
for taking overdoses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 538 –548. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck
Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Personality Questionnaire. San Diego, CA: EDITS.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20, Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclusion
pp. 65–122). New York: Academic Press. and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social psycholog- social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 486 – 496.
ical answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The future is now:
Batson, C. D., Klein, T. R., Highberger, L., & Shaw, L. L. (1995). Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organization Behavior and
Immorality from empathy-induced altruism: When compassion and jus- Human Decision Processes, 88, 430 – 444.
tice conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1042– Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T.
1054. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective fore-
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from the self. Psychological casting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617– 638.
Review, 97, 90 –113. Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating
Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciaracco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (2005). emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic
Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and system. Neuroreport, 11, 43– 48.
Social Psychology, 88, 589 – 604. Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychoso-
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psycho- cial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social
logical Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Psychology, 85, 105–120.
Baumeister, R. F., & Scher, S. J. (1988). Self-defeating behavior patterns Hawton, K., Cole, D., O’Grady, J., & Osborn, M. (1982). Motivational
among normal individuals: Review and analysis of common self- aspects of deliberate self-poisoning in adolescents. British Journal of
destructive tendencies. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 3–22. Psychiatry, 141, 286 –291.
Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of social Herman, B. H., & Panksepp, J. (1978). Effects of morphine and naloxone
exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces intelli- on separation distress and approach attachment: Evidence for opiate
gent thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 817– mediation of social affect. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 9,
827. 213–220.
14 DEWALL AND BAUMEISTER

Hilgard, E. R., Ruch, J. C., Lange, A. F., Lenox, J. R., Morgan, A. H., & hands and feet. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
Sachs, L. B. (1974). The psychophysics of cold pressor pain and its 84, 639 – 643.
modification through hypnotic suggestion. American Journal of Psy- Myers, D. (1992). The pursuit of happiness. New York: Morrow.
chology, 87, 17–31. Naranjo, J. R., & Fuentes, J. A. (1985). Association between hypoalgesia
Insel, T. R., & Winslow, J. T. (1991). Central administration of oxytocin and hypertension in rats after short-term isolation. Neuropharmacology,
modulates the infant rat’s response to social isolation. European Journal 24, 167–171.
of Pharmacology, 203, 149 –152. Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant–mother
Jepson, C., Loewenstein, G., & Ubel, P. (2001). Actual versus estimated attachment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine.
differences in quality of life before and after renal transplant. Unpub- Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 437– 452.
lished manuscript, Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carne- Nelson, L. D., & Morrison, E. L. (2005). The symptoms of resource
gie Mellon University. scarcity: Judgments of food and finances influence preferences for
Kanarek, R. B., & Carrington, C. (2004). Sucrose consumption enhances potential partners. Psychological Science, 16, 167–173.
the analgesic effects of cigarette smoking in male and female smokers. Orbach, I., Mikulincer, M., King, R., Cohen, D., & Stein, D. (1997).
Psychopharmacology, 173, 57– 63. Thresholds and tolerance of physical pain in suicidal and nonsuicidal
Kehoe, P., & Blass, E. M. (1986a). Behaviorally functional opioid systems patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 646 – 652.
in infant rats: II. Evidence for pharmacological, physiological, and Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and
psychological mediation of pain and stress. Behavioral Neuroscience, animal emotions. London: Oxford University Press.
100, 624 – 630. Panksepp, J., Herman, B. H., Conner, R., Bishop, P., & Scott, J. P. (1978).
Kehoe, P., & Blass, E. M. (1986b). Opioid-mediation of separation distress The biology of social attachments: Opiates alleviate separation distress.
in 10-day-old rats: Reversal of stress with maternal stimuli. Develop- Biological Psychiatry, 13, 607– 618.
mental Psychobiology, 19, 385–398. Panksepp, J., Vilberg, T., Bean, N. J., Coy, D. H., & Kastin, A. J. (1978).
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Ricker, D., George, J., Messick, G., Speicher, C. E., Reduction of distress vocalization in chicks by opiate-like peptides.
Garner, W., & Glaser, R. (1984). Urinary cortisol levels, cellular immu- Brain Research Bulletin, 3, 663– 667.
nocompetency, and loneliness in psychiatric inpatients. Psychosomatic Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K. M., & Ingvar, M. (2002). Placebo and
Medicine, 46, 15–24. opioid analgesia—imaging a shared neural network. Neuroimage, 16,
Kimbrell, T. A., George, M. S., Parekh, P. I., Ketter, T. A., Podell, D. M., 1142–1150.
Danielson, A. L., et al. (1999). Regional brain activity during transient Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The
self-induced anxiety and anger in healthy adults. Biological Psychiatry, need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and
46, 454 – 465. Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107.
Konecka, A. M., & Sroczynska, I. (1990). Stressors and pain sensitivity in Pomerleau, O. F., Turk, D. C., & Fertig, J. B. (1984). The effects of
CFW mice: Role of opioid peptides. Archives Internationales de Physi- cigarette smoking on pain and anxiety. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 265–271.
ologie et de Biochimie, 98, 245–252. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
Kosslyn, S. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Felician, O., Camposano, S., Keenan, NJ: Princeton University Press.
J. P., Thompson, W. L., et al. (1999, May 7). The role of area 17 in Rossi, G. C., Pasternak, G. W., & Bodnar, R. J. (1993). Synergistic
visual imagery: Convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science, 284, brainstem interactions for morphine analgesia. Brain Research, 624,
167–170. 171–180.
Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. A. (2000). Hurt feelings: The neglected Rushen, J., Boissy, A., Terlouw, E. M., & de Passillé, A. M. (1999). Opioid
emotion. In R. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in peptides and behavioral and physiological responses of dairy cows to
interpersonal transgression (pp. 151–175). Washington, DC: American social isolation in unfamiliar surroundings. Journal of Animal Science,
Psychological Association. 77, 2918 –2924.
Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (in press). Projection bias Spear, L. P., Enters, E. K., Aswad, M. A., & Louzan, M. (1985). Drug and
in predicting future utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics. environmentally induced manipulations of the opiate and serotonergic
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk systems alter nociception in neonatal rat pups. Behavioral and Neural
as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286. Biology, 44, 1–22.
Lynch, J. J. (1979). The broken heart: The medical consequences of Sufka, K. J., & Hughes, R. A. (1990). Dose and temporal parameters of
loneliness. New York: Basic Books. morphine-induced hyperalgesia in domestic fowl. Physiology & Behav-
MacDonald, G., Kingsbury, R., & Shaw, S. (2005). Adding insult to injury: ior, 47, 385–387.
Social pain theory and response to social exclusion. In K. Williams, J. Sufka, K. J., & Weed, N. C. (1994). Construct validation of behavioral
Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social indices of isolation stress and inflammatory nociception in young do-
exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 77–90). New York: Psychology mestic fowl. Physiology & Behavior, 55, 741–746.
Press. Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., &
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? Bartels, J. M. (2006). Social exclusion reduces prosocial behavior.
The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bul- Unpublished manuscript.
letin, 131, 202–223. Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If
Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta- you can’t join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive
experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1058 –1069.
102–111. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). “Isn’t it fun to get the respect
McGuire, P. K., Shah, G. M. S., & Murray, R. M. (1993). Increased blood that we’re going to deserve?” Narcissism, social rejection, and aggres-
flow in Broca’s area during auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. sion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 261–272.
Lancet, 342, 703–706. Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Social
Mercer, M. E., & Holder, M. D. (1997). Antinociceptive effects of palat- exclusion causes self-defeating behavior. Journal of Personality and
able sweet ingesta on human responsivity to pressure pain. Physiology & Social Psychology, 83, 606 – 615.
Behavior, 61, 311–318. Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Social
Murray, F. S., & Hagan, B. C. (1973). Pain threshold and tolerance of exclusion and the deconstructed state: Time perception, meaningless-
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 15

ness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. Journal of Person- Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P.
ality and Social Psychology, 85, 409 – 423. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp.
Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The 345– 411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
relationship between social support and physiological processes: A Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Kurtz, J., Dunn, E., & Gilbert, D. T. (2004).
review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for When to fire: Anticipatory versus post-event reconstrual of uncontrol-
health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488 –531. lable events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 340 –351.
Van Boven, L., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Social projection of transient Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D.
drive states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1159 – (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting.
1168. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821– 836.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali- Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go?
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of
Wiedenmayer, C. P., Goodwin, G. A., & Barr, G. A. (2000). The effect of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 560 –567.
periaqueductal gray lesions on responses to age-specific threats in infant
rats. Developmental Brain Research, 120, 191–198.
Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). CyberOstracism: Received February 24, 2005
Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Revision received August 15, 2005
Social Psychology, 79, 748 –762. Accepted August 25, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 111–123 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.111

Information Quantity and Quality Affect the Realistic Accuracy


of Personality Judgment

Tera D. Letzring Shannon M. Wells


University of California, Riverside Sanford Systems, Incorporated

David C. Funder
University of California, Riverside

Triads of unacquainted college students interacted in 1 of 5 experimental conditions that manipulated


information quantity (amount of information) and information quality (relevance of information to
personality), and they then made judgments of each others’ personalities. To determine accuracy, the
authors compared the ratings of each judge to a broad-based accuracy criterion composed of personality
ratings from 3 types of knowledgeable informants (the self, real-life acquaintances, and clinician-
interviewers). Results supported the hypothesis that information quantity and quality would be positively
related to objective knowledge about the targets and realistic accuracy. Interjudge consensus and
self– other agreement followed a similar pattern. These findings are consistent with expectations based
on models of the process of accurate judgment (D. C. Funder, 1995, 1999) and consensus (D. A. Kenny,
1994).

Keywords: personality judgment, judgment accuracy, realistic accuracy, information quantity, informa-
tion quality

Every day, people make personality judgments of other people. more accurate judgments of personality. The effect of information
The person making the judgments (the judge) has known some quantity has also been referred to as the acquaintanceship effect
targets for several years and has seen them in many different (Colvin & Funder, 1991; Kenny, 1991), because as people are
contexts (e.g., a sibling or a spouse), whereas other targets are acquainted longer they are assumed to acquire more information
people the judge has just met and has seen only in a relatively about each other. The concept of information quality is more
limited context (e.g., a new coworker, a bank teller). Not surpris- complex, but at its core refers to the likelihood that even when
ingly, these judgments vary in how accurate they are, perhaps information quantity is held constant, different contexts of ac-
because they are made on the basis of varying degrees of acquain- quaintanceship might vary in the degree to which personality-
tance and information that vary in how relevant they are to relevant information becomes available in them. For example, a
personality. context in which everybody acts the same would yield only low-
These two aspects of the information available to a judge are quality information for personality judgment because information
referred to as information quantity, the sheer amount of informa- about individual differences in personality would not be revealed,
tion that is available, and information quality, the degree to which whereas a context in which behavior is relatively free to vary
the available information is relevant to personality (Blackman & would yield higher quality information because people would be
Funder, 1998; Funder, 1999). The concept of information quantity more likely to behave in a way that reflects their personalities
is straightforward in that it assumes that judges who have access to (Snyder & Ickes, 1985). It is also possible that different kinds of
more information about the personality of the target will make information—for example, thoughts and feelings as opposed to
hobbies and activities (Anderson, 1984)—are differentially infor-
mative about personality. When more personality-relevant infor-
Tera D. Letzring and David C. Funder, Department of Psychology, mation is available, the accuracy of judgments of individual dif-
University of California, Riverside; Shannon M. Wells, Sanford Systems, ferences in personality is expected to be higher. Information
Incorporated, Lake Elsinore, California (now Key Data Systems). quantity and quality are two aspects of good information, one of
This article is based, in part, on the doctoral dissertation by Shannon M. four factors proposed to be related to judgment accuracy (Funder,
Wells titled “The Effect of Information Quantity and Quality on the 1999).
Accuracy of Personality Judgments.” Some of these findings were also It is difficult to examine information quantity and quality inde-
presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social
pendently of each other in natural settings of acquaintanceship,
Psychology as part of a symposium titled “The Accuracy of Interpersonal
Judgment.” Data gathering for this article was supported by National
because people are likely to emit a broader range of cues to
Institute of Mental Health Grant MH42427 to David C. Funder. personality and share more personality-relevant information with
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tera D. acquaintances they have known longer. For example, according to
Letzring, who is now at the Department of Psychology, Idaho State social penetration theory (Altman & Taylor, 1973), people share
University, Pocatello, ID 83209. E-mail: tera.letzring@gmail.com information about a larger number of the aspects of their person-
111
112 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

ality and share more personal information within these aspects as Consensus
a relationship progresses, which suggests that information in-
creases in both quantity and quality along with acquaintance. Consensus can be measured more directly than realistic accu-
In the current study, the quantity and quality of the information racy and is more commonly studied. Consensus is the level of
available to judges were experimentally and independently manip- agreement between the personality judgments rendered by two or
ulated so that the effects of these two factors on the accuracy of more people about another person. Consensus can be used as an
personality judgment could be examined, in order to achieve two indicator of accuracy, but it is important to keep in mind that
goals. The first goal was to further examine the effects of infor- consensus is not necessarily related to what the person is actually
mation quantity on judgment accuracy by (a) testing for the ac- like, because many people could agree about the personality of
quaintanceship effect in a new context and across different levels someone else, and all could be wrong, as is the case when inac-
of information quantity and by (b) examining the effects of infor- curate stereotypes are used to make judgments.
mation quantity on a relatively new construct, realistic accuracy, in
addition to the more traditional constructs of consensus and self– Self–Other Agreement
other agreement. The second goal of the present research was to
Self– other agreement can also be directly measured and has
directly examine the effects of experimentally manipulated infor-
been used frequently in previous research (Ambady, Hallahan, &
mation quality on realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other
Rosenthal, 1995; Blackman & Funder, 1998). It refers to the level
agreement, which is a more distinctive contribution, as this is one
of agreement between judgments made about another person and
of the first empirical studies to examine these effects.
that person’s self-judgments. The self-judgments are often implic-
itly assumed to reflect reality, and the judgments made by others
Indicators of Judgment Accuracy are assumed to be accurate to the degree that they are related to the
self-judgments. However, self– other agreement does not guaran-
Three indicators of judgment accuracy were assessed: realistic tee highly accurate judgments, because the self-rating will not
accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement. adequately represent one’s real personality if people are unwilling
or unable to provide accurate judgments of themselves (Hofstee,
1994; Kolar, Funder, & Colvin, 1996). Despite this complication
Realistic Accuracy of using self– other agreement to measure accuracy, many inves-
tigations refer to self– other agreement simply as accuracy (Am-
Realistic accuracy refers to a hypothetical construct representing
bady et al., 1995; Bernieri, Zuckerman, Koestner, & Rosenthal,
the level of agreement between a personality judgment and what a
1994; Blackman & Funder, 1998). However, in the current article
target is really like (Funder, 1995). This construct cannot be
the term accuracy is used more specifically to describe realistic
directly measured by any single personality or behavior rating, as
accuracy so that realistic accuracy can be differentiated from
any single rating is highly uncertain as an indicator of what a
consensus and self– other agreement.
person is really like. Instead, the ideal of realistic accuracy can be
We report findings concerning all three indicators of judgment
approached to the degree that multiple methods of measurement
accuracy for two reasons. First, realistic accuracy, although related
are used and combined to form a broad-based accuracy criterion
to consensus and self– other agreement, is far from synonymous
for each target of judgment. The assumption is that this broad-
with either of these operationalizations and could potentially yield
based criterion is closer to what the target is really like than is any
different results. Second, consensus and self– other agreement
single rating, because random errors in the ratings should cancel
have been used in many prior studies (e.g., Anderson, 1984;
each other out as more ratings are combined. In the current study,
Blackman & Funder, 1998; Kenny, Albright, Malloy, & Kashy,
we formed an accuracy criterion by using a composite rating based
1994), and our presentation of these constructs allows comparison
on judgments from real-life acquaintances, psychologists who
with the existing literature.
interviewed the targets, and the self.
The difficulty of the criterion problem has led many researchers
to shy away from studying the accuracy of judgments of real Methods of Personality Judgment
people and to instead turn their attention to models of the cognitive
Realistic Accuracy Model
processing of artificial stimuli (Funder, 1987, 1995). As research
progresses, it behooves investigators to begin to respond to the The Realistic Accuracy Model (RAM; Funder, 1995, 1999)
criterion problem instead of continuing to bypass it. The ultimate describes an interpersonal and cognitive process that results in
goal should be an appropriate, broad-based accuracy criterion, accurate personality judgment when all four of its stages are
which would ideally derive from multiple modes of assessment, successfully completed. First, the target of the judgment must
including the target’s self-perspective, ratings from knowledgeable display cues or behaviors that are relevant to the characteristic
informants, clinical judgments, behavioral measures, life outcome being judged in such a way and in contexts that are available to the
data, and perhaps even biological information such as hormone judge. Then, the judge must detect these cues and correctly use
levels and functional MRI images (Funder, 1995). We propose that them to make a judgment. RAM assumes that the four stages
research that seeks to go beyond single operationalizations of the combine in a multiplicative fashion so that a failure at any of the
accuracy of personality judgment should refer to the outcome as four stages will make accuracy impossible. For example, cues can
realistic accuracy in order to distinguish this construct from other only be detected if they are available.
constructs that measure some other singular form of agreement or Four factors are thought to influence the judgment process by
accuracy in personality judgment. affecting one or more stages of RAM (Funder, 1995): the good
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 113

judge (Allport, 1937; Kolar, 1995; Taft, 1955; Vernon, 1933; Vogt has received less attention, although the available evidence sug-
& Colvin, 2003), good target (Colvin, 1993), good trait (Borkenau gests that not all information is created equal and that some kinds
& Liebler, 1993; Funder & Dobroth, 1987; John & Robins, 1993; of information are more likely to be related to judgment accuracy
Norman & Goldberg, 1966), and good information (Blackman & than others (e.g., Anderson, 1984).
Funder, 1998; Funder & Colvin, 1988; Funder, Kolar, & Black-
man, 1995). Two aspects of good information, information quan- Information Quantity
tity and information quality, are the focus of the current article. A fair amount of research has examined the relationships be-
A prediction regarding the relations between information quan- tween information quantity and two aspects of personality judg-
tity and quality and all three indicators of accuracy can be gener- ment, consensus and self– other agreement. To our knowledge,
ated on the basis of RAM. Even though RAM is a model for research has not previously been published on the relationship
accuracy, it can also be used to make predictions about consensus between information quantity and realistic accuracy. Past findings
if one assumes that personality is something real, in which case on consensus and self– other agreement have been mixed.
two accurate judgments of personality must agree with each other, In between-subjects designs, in which different participants are
and therefore consensus can be expected to be high at sufficiently involved at each level of acquaintance, findings concerning con-
high levels of accuracy. A linear increase between information and sensus have both supported and not supported the acquaintance-
realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement was used ship effect. Blackman and Funder (1998) experimentally manipu-
as the prediction based on RAM. This is a conservative prediction lated level of acquaintance by having some people observe a target
because it is not possible to determine the degree to which the interacting for 5 or 10 min and other people observe a target for 25
manipulated levels of information quantity and quality are equally or 30 min, and they found that consensus did not increase with
spaced and, therefore, whether the increases in accuracy should acquaintance. However, researchers have found that consensus is
also be equally spaced. higher among real-life acquaintances than among relative strang-
ers—that is, people who viewed dyadic interactions for either 5
Weighted-Average Model min (Funder & Colvin, 1988) or 25 or 30 min (Blackman &
Funder, 1998). Furthermore, a meta-analysis suggests that for
The Weighted-Average Model (WAM; Kenny, 1991, 1994) agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to
offers a detailed description of the basis of consensus in person- experience, consensus is greater among long-term acquaintances
ality judgment. The model predicts that consensus will not increase than among people who were unacquainted or had interacted only
with acquaintanceship when judges see completely overlapping once (Kenny et al., 1994). In general, higher levels of consensus
behaviors of the target and interpret what they see in exactly the have been associated with higher acquaintance in studies with
same way (Kenny, 1991). However, when these assumptions are between-subjects designs in which there was a relatively large
replaced by the more realistic expectations that different judges difference in the amount of available information between the high
will interpret behaviors in different ways and will not detect and low acquaintance groups, but not when there was a relatively
exactly the same behaviors of the target, WAM predicts that small difference in acquaintance.
consensus will increase rapidly at low levels of acquaintance and Between-subjects designs have generally found support for the
remain about the same across higher levels of acquaintance. This acquaintanceship effect when self– other agreement is the out-
reasoning led us to predict that consensus would increase between come. For example, self– other agreement was higher among peo-
low and medium levels of information quantity and quality but ple who watched videotaped unstructured dyadic interactions for
would be the same between medium and high levels of information 25 to 30 min versus 5 or 10 min (Blackman & Funder, 1998),
quantity and quality. people who had been roommates for more than 10 months versus
The prediction based on WAM was tested only for consensus less than 10 months (Bernieri at al., 1994), people who indicated
because WAM is explicitly formulated as a model of the process they knew the target of judgment extremely well versus not at all
of consensus, and it has been suggested that the relations between (Paunonen, 1989), and real-life acquaintances versus people who
different indicators of accuracy and acquaintanceship are not nec- had watched 5-min unstructured dyadic interactions (Funder et al.,
essarily the same (Kenny, 1991). For example, Kenny (1991) 1995). In between-subjects designs, information quantity appears
pointed out that accuracy can increase with acquaintance even as to have a positive linear relationship with self– other agreement
consensus stays the same, as is the case when all judges become across all levels of acquaintance. However, a complication com-
more accurate but continue to agree with each other to the same mon to between-subjects designs of consensus and self– other
degree. agreement is that real-life acquaintances have self-selected to
It was not feasible to determine whether the prediction based on know a person for longer, which introduces the possibility that
RAM was superior to the prediction based on WAM, as the two confounds such as similarity to the target and liking of the target
predictions were highly correlated.1 However, it was still interest- influence accuracy in addition to length of acquaintance. This is
ing to determine how well the obtained data fit each prediction. why, in the current study, we have experimentally manipulated
information quantity.
Past Findings and Rationale for Current Experiment In within-subjects research designs in which the same partici-
pants are involved at each level of acquaintance, previous findings
Information quantity has received a fair amount of research
attention, and although the findings have been mixed, the main
conclusion has been that, all other things being equal, to know With contrast weights of –1, 0, ⫹1 for RAM and –2, ⫹1, ⫹1 for
1

someone longer is to know him or her better. Information quality WAM, the correlation between the predictions was .87.
114 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

suggest that as acquaintance increases, consensus generally re- and Funder’s experiment were exposed to targets via videotaped
mains constant, whereas self– other agreement generally increases. two-person interactions, whereas participants in the current study
Consensus has been shown to stay about the same as acquaintance actually interacted with each other in three-person groups before
increases across a variety of samples, situations, and lengths of making personality judgments. This method may have different
time, including previously unacquainted students who participated implications for the way judges detect and use information when
in 30-min group interactions on 4 consecutive days (Park & Judd, making personality judgments. For example, judges watching vid-
1989), students who participated in weekly 20-min group meetings eotapes have no need to be concerned with how they are perceived
for 7 weeks (Paulhus & Bruce, 1992), and students living near or what they will say next, whereas judges who are involved in an
each other in a dorm from 2 to 33 weeks after the start of a interaction are likely to be concerned with these and other issues.
semester (Park, Kraus, & Ryan, 1997). Furthermore, a meta- More important, many researchers, including Blackman and
analysis suggested that consensus did not increase among people Funder, have used self– other agreement as a proxy for accuracy,
who interacted for between 8 min and 2 hr in both laboratory and whereas in the current study, we went beyond self– other agree-
naturalistic settings (Kenny et al., 1994). ment and examined a construct closer to the ideal of realistic
On the other hand, an examination of self– other agreement accuracy by using a broad-based accuracy criterion.
provided evidence that self– other agreement generally increased
among students who participated in weekly 20-min group meet- Information Quality
ings for 7 weeks (Paulhus & Bruce, 1992). Also, findings by
Borkenau and colleagues (Borkenau, Mauer, Riemann, Spinath, & Although a considerable amount of research has focused on the
Angleitner, 2004) suggest that acquaintance and accuracy are relationship between information quantity and personality judg-
positively related when accuracy is operationalized as the level of ment, surprisingly little research has focused on an arguably even
agreement with ratings from the target, acquaintances of the target, more important aspect of information—information quality, or the
experimenters, and a confederate who interacted with the target; personality relevance of the information that becomes available
and when acquaintance is operationalized as the number of behav- within a given period of time. Intuitively, it seems apparent that
ioral episodes observed and rated by different judges that were not all kinds of information contribute equally to the achievement
included in a composite judgment. of an accurate judgment, as it is possible to be acquainted with
One possible explanation for the inconsistent findings concern- someone for only a short time but to know him or her very well,
ing the acquaintanceship effect is that consensus was used as the or to be acquainted with someone for years and still know little
outcome, which is not necessarily the appropriate way to approach about that person. From a research perspective, the first step in
this issue if one wishes to determine the extent to which judgments examining information quality is to determine the extent to which
become more accurate with increased acquaintance. It is possible it is related to indicators of accuracy, including realistic accuracy,
that the ratings of the judges in the studies mentioned previously consensus, and self– other agreement, which was one goal of the
(Kenny et al., 1994; Park & Judd, 1989; Park et al., 1997; Paulhus current study.
& Bruce, 1992) did become more accurate across time, but this Anderson’s (1984) groundbreaking study supported the idea that
change might not be reflected in consensus if the ratings continued information quality is related to self– other agreement. Anderson
to have the same level of similarity with each other (Blackman & found that observing an interview in which questions about
Funder, 1998). thoughts and feelings were discussed yielded higher self– other
Another possible limitation of past research is that researchers agreement of personality ratings than did observing an interview in
have simply assumed that people who have known each other which hobbies and activities were discussed. One conclusion that
longer have also acquired more information about each other, can be based on this finding is that thoughts and feelings are higher
which is not necessarily true. One way to determine whether quality information than hobbies and activities, and this is why the
people in longer relationships have actually acquired more judges who learned about thoughts and feelings were able to agree
personality-relevant information is to directly measure the amount more closely with the target’s self-judgments of personality than
of objective information they know about the target. In the current were the judges who learned about hobbies and activities.
study, this concern was addressed by asking participants a series of Another aspect that might influence the quality of the informa-
factual questions about their interaction partners that sampled from tion that is likely to be revealed is the situation’s “strength”
the kinds of information people might learn in an initial interac- (Snyder & Ickes, 1985). “Strong” situations limit the range of
tion. This sample of relatively objective questions was used as a behavior that people display because they include explicit rules or
manipulation check to determine whether participants who interact evoke implicit norms to which people generally adhere. At the
for a longer period of time acquire more information about each other extreme, “weak” situations allow for considerable behavioral
other. If the manipulation is successful, then people who interact variation because there are few rules or norms for typical behavior.
longer will have acquired more of this information, concomitant to Therefore, in comparison with a strong situation, a weak situation
picking up more information relevant to personality, and therefore should allow for more behavioral variation, the availability of
we expected to find a moderate positive relation between infor- higher quality individuating information, the acquisition of more
mation acquisition and realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– objective information, and greater judgment accuracy.
other agreement. The present study was designed to begin the investigation of
The information quantity analyses are a conceptual replication what occurs when people interact in situations with different levels
of a study by Blackman and Funder (1998). The current project of information quality. A strong situation was created experimen-
also went beyond Blackman and Funder’s study and other previous tally by giving participants the specific task of answering a long set
research in several important ways. First, participants in Blackman of trivia questions. Two weaker situations were created by telling
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 115

participants to talk about whatever they would like or to get to relevant information and to achieve higher levels of realistic ac-
know one another as well as possible. We predicted that more curacy, consensus, and self– other agreement than participants who
personality-relevant information would be revealed in the two were simply instructed to talk about whatever they would like.
weak situations than in the strong situation and that realistic Therefore, on the basis of RAM, we predicted a linear relationship
accuracy, self– other agreement, and consensus would be higher in between information quality and all three indictors of accuracy.
the two weak situations. We also predicted that more personality- We could also make a prediction based on WAM. Even though the
relevant information would be available in the weak situation in model does not directly consider information quality, we have
which participants were told to get to know each other than in the defined information quality as the availability of more personality-
weak situation in which participants were told to talk about what- relevant information with time held constant, so that the acquain-
ever they would like, because the participants would be more tanceship parameter of WAM should increase with information
likely to reveal or ask about personality-relevant information as quality. Therefore, on the basis of WAM, we predicted that con-
they attempted to get to know each other than when they were sensus would increase between the low- and medium-quality con-
simply trying to pass the time by talking about whatever they liked. ditions and remain about the same between the medium- and
high-quality conditions.
Research Questions
With the theoretical basis of both the RAM (Funder, 1995) and
Method
the WAM (Kenny, 1991) in mind, in the current study we exam- The current study was a between-subjects experimental design in which
ined how quantity and quality of available information affect each participant interacted in one of five conditions. The first independent
realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement. variable, information quantity, had three levels: low (minimal information
condition), medium (short unstructured condition), and high (long unstruc-
Hypothesis 1: Participants who interact for longer periods of tured condition). The second independent variable, information quality,
time will achieve higher levels of realistic accuracy, consen- also had three levels: low (trivia condition), medium (short unstructured
sus, and self– other agreement than people who interact for condition), and high (get to know condition). The effects of each indepen-
shorter periods of time. dent variable were examined for three dependent variables: realistic accu-
racy (agreement between personality judgments and a broad-based accu-
To examine this issue, we compared personality judgments fol- racy criterion), self– other agreement (agreement between judgments and
the target’s self-ratings), and consensus (agreement between two judges
lowing three-person unstructured interactions of three lengths
about a target).
(minimal interaction, 50 min, and 3 hr). On the basis of RAM
(Funder, 1995, 1999), we expected that all three indicators of
personality judgment—realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– Participants
other agreement—would increase linearly across these experimen- A total of 506 undergraduate students participated in the Riverside
tal conditions. On the basis of WAM (Kenny, 1991), we expected Accuracy Project—Phase 2 (RAP–II) and were paid $10 per hour for their
consensus to increase between the low- and medium-quantity time. A core group of 180 target participants (90 men and 90 women) were
conditions and to stay about the same between the medium- and recruited via announcements made in psychology classes and fliers placed
high-quantity conditions. Thus, the predicted relationships be- on bulletin boards advertising “research on personality.” These participants
tween information quantity and consensus differ slightly depend- were asked to recruit two close acquaintances (for a total of 326 acquain-
ing on which model is used, and both predictions were tested. tance informants) to provide personality descriptions of themselves and the
target participants. The ethnic breakdown of the target participants was
Hypothesis 2: Participants who interact in situations in which 38% Asian American, 20% Hispanic, 14% Caucasian, 12% African Amer-
more personality-relevant information is likely to be available ican, and 16% other or not specified. There were three groups of each
possible gender composition (female–female–female [FFF], male–male–
will achieve higher levels of realistic accuracy, consensus,
male [MMM], female–female–male [FFM], and female–male–male
and self– other agreement than participants who interact in [FMM]) within each experimental condition, and all of the gender com-
situations in which less personality-relevant information is positions were analyzed together because of the balanced nature of the
likely to be available. design and because the number of participants within each gender compo-
sition would be too small to find reliable results if analyzed separately.
In one of the first experimental investigations to directly examine This is the second article to come out of the extensive RAP–II data set,
this issue, we compared the accuracy of personality judgments and the analyses do not overlap with the previous project, which focused
made following one of three 50-min interactions that vary in the on the constructs of ego-control and ego-resiliency (see Letzring, Block, &
amount of personality-relevant information likely to be available Funder, 2005), or with future planned projects.
in them. The low-quality condition was a strong situation that
allowed for little behavioral variation and was expected to elicit Materials
the least amount of personality-relevant information and therefore
the lowest levels of realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other California Adult Q (CAQ) set. The CAQ (Block, 1961, as modified for
use by nonprofessionals by Bem & Funder, 1978) consists of 100 carefully
agreement. The medium- and high-quality conditions were both
formulated descriptive statements about personality (e.g., is critical/skep-
relatively weak situations that were expected to elicit more behav- tical, is personally charming, is cheerful). In the Q-item rating, each item
ioral variation and therefore higher levels of all indicators of is responded to using a 9-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (extremely
accuracy than the strong situation. Between the two weak situa- uncharacteristic) to 9 (extremely characteristic). Participants rated them-
tions, participants who were given the objective to get to know selves and their group interaction partners using the Q-item rating, and
each other were expected to share a broader range of personality- acquaintance informants also rated the target participants using the Q-item
116 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

rating. Participants rated both group interaction partners at the same time participants were acquainted with their interaction partners prior to the
for each item of the CAQ, which implies that for each item the interaction experimental group interaction.
partners were directly compared with each other at the time of the judg- Experimental conditions. Target participants interacted in the labora-
ment. We expected this procedure to help the judges make individuating tory in one of five experimental conditions designed to vary in the quantity
and thoughtful ratings for each item and both targets. Clinicians rated the and quality of interpersonal information likely to become available in them.
target participants using the traditional Q-sort method, in which the 100 Each of the groups consisted of three previously unacquainted target
items are placed into a forced-choice, quasi-normal distribution so that participants in one of four possible gender compositions: all female (FFF),
each category (1 through 9) contains a predetermined number of items. all male (MMM), 2 females and 1 male (FFM), and 1 female and 2 males
Information fact sheet. An information fact sheet was constructed for (FMM). An equal number of the four gender compositions was assigned to
RAP–II that consisted of open-ended questions that were designed to each experimental condition. A total sample size of 180 allowed for a
sample from the wide array of information that could become available balanced design with 3 groups of each gender composition per condition,
during the experimental interactions. The information fact sheet was used for a total of 12 groups per condition.2
as a manipulation check to determine whether participants in higher quan- In all conditions, participants were seated in a room at a round table and
tity and quality interactions actually acquired more information about their given verbal instructions. At the conclusion of the interaction, participants
interaction partners than did participants in lower quantity or quality rated their two partners using three measures, which included the rating
interactions. Questions included information about age, place of birth, form of the CAQ and the information fact sheet.
political affiliation, future goals, family makeup, and so forth. No attempt Minimal information condition. Participants rated the other two par-
was made to assess the information that participants picked up in any ticipants in the room immediately following a short set of instructions
comprehensive fashion, or even to focus specifically on personality- explaining that they were studying first impressions and to do the best they
relevant information (because techniques for either goal are presently could describing their first impressions of the other two people in the room.
unavailable). Rather, we aimed simply to obtain a small sample of the They were asked not to speak to each other but were in each others’
information that one person might learn about another during a casual physical presence for as long as it took to complete the ratings (less than
interaction, with the assumption that scores on this measure would corre- 1 hr). This situation provided an empirical estimate of the baseline of
late with the amount of information, including personality-relevant infor- realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement and was used as
mation, acquired overall. the low-level condition for information quantity.
The information fact sheet was completed by the target participants Trivia quiz condition. Participants were presented with a packet con-
about their group interaction partners immediately following the ratings of taining 380 trivia-type questions, each of which had a single correct
personality and was completed on a later occasion by each participant answer. They were informed that they would have 50 min to work jointly
about him- or herself. Each question was scored correct (1 point) if the through the packet of questions and arrive unanimously at what they
answer was equivalent to the answer given by the self; half (.5 points) if the believed to be the correct answers. This context was designed to be a strong
answer was similar, but not equivalent, to the answer given by the self; or situation that would leave relatively little room for extraneous commentary
incorrect (0 points) if the answer was different from the answer given by or the disclosure of personality-relevant information, as the group would
the self or if there was no response. Each information fact sheet was scored spend the entire interaction time responding to the trivia questions. This
by two independent judges, and, as the decisions about the correctness of condition was used as the low-level condition for information quality.
an answer were relatively objective, any discrepancies in scoring were Short unstructured condition. Participants were told that they could
reconciled by a third judge. The reliability of the 19 items was high enough talk about anything they liked for the next 50 min. No attempt was made
to justify using the sum as a score representing information acquisition to direct or suggest what should be done over the course of the interaction.
(Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .77). This context provided a weak situation in that participants were able to do
and say whatever they liked and to exhibit individual differences in social
behavior, which should have included at least some personality-relevant
Procedures information on which judgments could be based. This condition was used
Overview. Target participants came to the lab on three separate occa- as the mid-level condition for both information quantity and quality.
sions and also completed three packets of self-report questionnaires outside Long unstructured condition. Participants were told that they could
of the lab, only some of which were used in the current analyses. During talk about anything they liked for the next 3 hr, and as in the short
the first session, participants interacted in three-person groups in one of unstructured condition, no attempt was made to direct or suggest what
five experimental conditions and then made personality judgments of their should be done over the course of the interaction. The experimenter
two interaction partners. During a subsequent session, each participant was returned to the room halfway through the interaction to give participants a
interviewed by a clinical psychologist who described the personality of the short break, at which time snacks were provided and a restroom break was
participant after the conclusion of the interview. Additionally, acquain- offered. As in the short unstructured condition, this context provided a
tance informants completed questionnaires regarding the target participant weak situation with very little structure and should have included at least
with whom they were acquainted. some personality-relevant information on which judgments could be based.
Presession. At the presession, participants were given the first take- This condition was used as the high-level condition for information
home packet of questionnaires and were scheduled into one of the five quantity.
experimental conditions. The times of the conditions were established in Get to know (GTK) condition. Participants were told that their task for
advance, and the participants chose a session that fit with their schedules. the next 50 min was to get to know each other as well as possible and to
Although technically this procedure was only quasi-random, it did assign learn as much as they could about what type of person each of them was.
participants to conditions independently of any aspect of the experimental
design, and we have no reason to believe that participants in the various
2
conditions differed from each other in any way that might have affected the On completion of data collection, it was found that 2 target participants
outcome of the study. Additionally, each participant was shown a folder were inadvertently included in the study twice. For both participants, the
containing pictures of the other participants in the same group to determine second condition that they were in was different from the first. Analyses
whether he or she had ever seen the other participants. If the participant were done with these groups included and excluded. The differences
recognized another participant in the same group, the participant was between these two sets of findings were within rounding error. To maintain
assigned to a different group. This procedure ensured that none of the a fully balanced design, we report results from the data of all 60 groups.
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 117

This context provided a weak situation that was expected to yield a wide
range of behaviors relevant to personality, and the objective of getting to
know each other was expected to guide participants to probe for and to
remember information related to personality. This condition was used as
the high-level condition for information quality.
Life history interview. Participants were individually interviewed by
one of four professionally trained (i.e., MSW, MA in counseling, or PhD
in clinical psychology) interviewers who had experience with a college
population. With consent of the participants, all interviews were video-
taped. The clinicians conducted a 1-hr semistructured life history interview
adapted from a protocol used for many years by the Institute of Personality
Assessment and Research (Craik et al., 2002). The protocol used in the
current study was adapted to better apply to college students and sought to
capture a broad range of personality-relevant information without explic-
itly asking about sensitive topics and risky behaviors. Each interview
started with the clinician asking the participant, “Tell me something about
yourself,” and then covered a broad range of topics, including college and
academic experiences, future plans, interpersonal relationships, and child-
Figure 1. Computation of profile realistic accuracy scores. Note that
hood and family history. In conclusion, each participant was asked to
profile correlations were computed for each of the three judges in each
describe “a defining event in [their] life that had a significant impact on or
experimental group, and these scores were Z transformed and averaged to
changed [their] life in some way.”3 Following the interview, the clinician
compute scores for each group; the scores of all 12 groups in each type of
completed a Q-sort description of the target participant. Some of the
experimental interaction were averaged to determine the accuracy for the
interviews were later viewed by a second clinician, and in these cases the
corresponding interaction type. CAQ ⫽ California Adult Q.
second clinician also described the participant’s personality using the
CAQ.4 When two ratings of the participant were available, a composite
score was computed for each item and used in subsequent analyses. The
average interclinician profile agreement was r ⫽ .50 (SD ⫽ 0.17),5 and the the judge and an approximation of what the target is really like. This
coefficient of internal consistency based on a composite of the two raters procedure yielded six scores for each group (two scores for each of three
was .67. judges). These scores could be transformed using Fisher’s r-to-z transfor-
Acquaintance–informant ratings. Participants were asked to provide mation and averaged so that each group had a single realistic accuracy
the names and contact information of the two people who knew them best score (see Figure 1).
at the university. Acquaintances came into the lab and provided descrip- Computation of consensus. To examine consensus, we correlated the
tions of the target participant by whom they had been identified, using the ratings provided by the two interaction partners of each target with each
rating format of the CAQ. The average profile agreement for the 100 items other, resulting in a profile correlation that represented the degree of
as rated by two acquaintances was r ⫽ .40 (SD ⫽ 0.19).6 The average similarity in the partner’s ratings of that target. This analysis yielded three
coefficient of internal consistency, or the dependability of the profile based consensus scores for each group (one for each target), which could be Z
on a composite of the two acquaintances, was .57. transformed and averaged so that each group had one consensus score.
Computation of self– other agreement. Finally, to examine self– other
Analyses agreement, we correlated the ratings of each interaction partner with the
partner’s self-ratings, resulting in a profile correlation that represented the
Realistic accuracy criterion. Many researchers have used self– other relation between the ratings of the judge and the target’s self-ratings. As in
agreement as a proxy for accuracy, but in the current project we strived to realistic accuracy, this procedure yielded six self– other agreement scores
go beyond this simple operationalization by comparing judgments to a
broader based criterion composed of ratings by three types of knowledge-
able informants: the self, two acquaintances nominated by the target, and 3
The full protocol for the clinical interview is available on the Riverside
a professionally trained clinician-interviewer. The 100 items of the CAQ Accuracy Project’s Web site at http://www.rap.ucr.edu/interview.htm.
were rated by each type of rater, and these ratings were used to compute a 4
When the participant scheduled for a live interview was unable to keep
composite score for each item. First, when ratings from two acquaintances the appointment, clinicians were given the option to observe and rate a
or two clinicians were available, these ratings were averaged.7 Then, a randomly chosen, previously recorded interview. In this manner, 47 inter-
simple average for each item was computed across the three ratings from
views were observed and rated by a second clinician.
the self, acquaintances, and clinician. The construct of realistic accuracy is 5
This number can be compared with 30 random pairings of ratings by
very new, so there is not an established level of reliability for determining
clinicians who described different targets, which resulted in an average
the adequacy of the accuracy composite. However, on the basis of the
profile correlation of .29.
magnitudes of correlations that are reported in the self– other agreement 6
literature (Bernieri et al., 1994; Funder, 1980), the mean alpha reliability of This number can be compared with 30 random pairings of ratings by
the accuracy composite seemed high enough to justify the use of these acquaintances who described different targets, which resulted in an average
average ratings (M ⫽ 0.42, SD ⫽ 0.13, range ⫽ 0.08 – 0.72).8 profile correlation of .24.
7
Computation of realistic accuracy. Profile analysis allows for an ex- When ratings from only one acquaintance or clinician were available,
amination of judgments in regard to a target’s overall personality by the use then there was no need to compute an average before averaging the ratings
of the entire set of 100 CAQ items in a single analysis. The computation with the other sources.
8
of a profile correlation simply involves correlating two sets of ratings for On the basis of correlations among types of raters, the accuracy
the same target across the 100 items of the CAQ. First, to examine realistic criterion is appropriate to use for computing profile correlations: self–
accuracy, we correlated the ratings by each judge of each interaction acquaintance (M ⫽ 0.48, SD ⫽ 0.17), self– clinician (M ⫽ 0.39, SD ⫽
partner with the accuracy composite for that interaction partner, resulting 0.14), acquaintance– clinician (M ⫽ 0.37, SD ⫽ 0.19); Spearman–Brown
in a profile correlation that represented the relation between the ratings of for three raters ⫽ .68.
118 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

per group, and these scores could be Z transformed and averaged so that Post hoc t tests. Contrast analyses are informative regarding how well
each group had a single self– other agreement score. the obtained data fit an a priori prediction about how the data should be
Components of accuracy scores. Cronbach (1955) was one of the first ordered, but they do not provide information regarding which pairs of
to point out that profile correlations may be confounded with several other groups are reliably different from each other. Therefore, post hoc
elements besides differential accuracy, including elevation, differential independent-samples t tests were used to determine which pairs of exper-
elevation, and stereotype accuracy, which result from the use of response imental conditions had reliably different levels of realistic accuracy, con-
sets or a reliance on stereotypes. The effects of response sets were not of sensus, and self– other agreement. No adjustments were made to the p
concern for between-groups comparisons in the current experiment be- values associated with these tests, so the conservative reader may wish to
cause participants were randomly assigned to one of five experimental adjust the p values to account for the number of comparisons in each
conditions, and therefore, participants with various response sets were analysis.
equally distributed among the conditions. The effects of stereotype accu-
racy also were not of concern because stereotype accuracy should have Results
equivalent effects on the magnitude of the profile correlations across all
conditions, and we were interested in the differences between conditions The focus of our analyses was on realistic accuracy, as this
and not in the absolute magnitude of accuracy. For this reason, we used the project was one of the first to explore the construct and the first, to
raw profile correlations in our cross-group comparisons, as did Blackman our knowledge, to examine the relationships between realistic
and Funder (1998) when they conducted similar analyses. accuracy and experimentally manipulated information quantity
However, it is true that as a result of stereotype accuracy, the baseline and quality. We also provided evidence concerning our predictions
correlation between two sets of ratings was not expected to be zero, as for consensus and self– other agreement. Before examining the
some items of the CAQ are generally rated higher than others, regardless effects of information quantity and quality on judgment accuracy,
of the target (Blackman & Funder, 1998). To determine baseline profile we first determined whether participants in longer and higher
correlations within the current data, we computed the correlations among quality interactions did indeed acquire more information (on the
30 random pairs of ratings. For realistic accuracy, the profile correlations basis of a sample of information possibly available in such inter-
were computed between the ratings of one target and the accuracy com- actions), as a manipulation check. Then, we used contrast analysis
posite for a different target, which resulted in a baseline profile correlation
(Rosenthal et al., 2000) to examine how well the changes in
of .36. For consensus, the profile correlations were computed between two
accuracy across experimental conditions fit the predictions based
interaction partners of different targets, which resulted in a baseline profile
correlation of .25. For self– other agreement, the profile correlations were
on RAM for realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agree-
computed between the ratings of one target and the self-ratings of a ment and based on WAM for consensus only. Finally, the degree
different target, which resulted in a baseline profile correlation of .28. to which information acquisition was related to realistic accuracy,
These baseline correlations may at first seem somewhat high, but when one consensus, and self– other agreement, regardless of the type of
realizes that all participants in the sample were college students and that, group the participants interacted in, was examined.
in general, college students are similar to each other in many ways, it There are several possible ways to analyze these data. We used
makes sense that even random pairings resulted in profile correlations of traditional statistical procedures to examine differences at the
moderate magnitude. Profile correlations greater than these baseline cor- group level by conducting a between-groups analysis to determine
relations indicate differential accuracy (Blackman & Funder, 1998). how well the obtained data fit the predictions. Our predictions
Contrast analysis. Contrast analysis was used to test focused questions were concerned with properties of the context, not of the individ-
by assigning weights to experimental conditions that reflect a prediction ual, and a group-level analysis reflects this orientation. Although
concerning the structure of the data (Rosenthal, Rosnow, & Rubin, 2000). none of the basic conclusions of this study change when data are
Once contrast weights were assigned to each condition, it was possible to
examined at different levels of analysis, an advantage of analyzing
determine the extent to which the data were in line with the prediction.
data at the level of the group is that it allowed us to perform all
When a t test was computed for a contrast analysis,9 the degrees of freedom
analyses in a consistent manner (at the same level of analysis).
were the number of participants minus the number of groups.10 One-tailed
p values that are associated with the tcontrast are reported. It was also
possible to determine how much variance in the group means was ac- Information Acquisition
counted for by the prediction by correlating the group means with the
To determine whether participants in interactions of greater
contrast weights and squaring the obtained correlation, which resulted in an
r2alerting. A large r2alerting provides support for the prediction. Furthermore,
quantity and quality acquired more information about each other,
it was possible to test whether the leftover variance, or the variance
not accounted for by the prediction, was significant by computing a 9
The formula used to compute tcontrast was
tnoncontrast.11 A nonsignificant tnoncontrast also lends support to the a priori
prediction because little variance is left over that is not accounted for by the
prediction.
On the basis of RAM, we predicted that there would be linear relation-
冑 2
ralerting ⫻ SSbetween
MSwithin
.

ships among information quantity and quality and realistic accuracy, con- The formula used to compute rcontrast was
sensus, and self– other agreement, for which the appropriate contrast
weights are ⫺1 (low information), 0 (medium information), and ⫹1 (high
information). On the basis of WAM, we predicted a slightly different 冑 t2
2
t
contrast

contrast⫹ df
.
relationship between information quantity and quality and consensus, 10
In the current analyses, there were 12 participants in each of the 3
which reflects an increase at low levels of acquaintance and then a leveling groups, so there were [(12)(3) – 3] ⫽ 33 degrees of freedom for each
off at higher levels of acquaintance, for which the appropriate contrast contrast analysis.
weights are ⫺2 (low information), ⫹1 (medium information), and ⫹1
(high information). 11
tnoncontrast ⫽ 冑SSbetween ⫺ SScontrast
MSwithin
.
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 119

we compared the scores achieved on the information fact sheet


across experimental conditions. This can be considered a manip-
ulation check that determines the degree to which participants in
longer and higher quality interactions actually learned more about
each other.
Information quantity. A good fit was found between amount
of acquired information and a linear increase across the three
levels of information quantity: minimal information (M ⫽ 2.80,
SD ⫽ 1.10); short unstructured (M ⫽ 5.17, SD ⫽ 1.09); long
unstructured (M ⫽ 7.00, SD ⫽ 1.58); and rcontrast ⫽ .81,
tcontrast(33) ⫽ 8.02, p ⬍ . 0001 (see Figure 2). Furthermore, on the
basis of independent-samples t tests, we found that the scores of all
groups differed significantly from each other: minimal information
Figure 3. Mean information acquisition scores for experimental condi-
and short unstructured, t(22) ⫽ 5.29, p ⬍ .0001; minimal infor- tions used in the information quality analyses. GTK ⫽ get to know
mation and long unstructured, t(22) ⫽ 7.54, p ⬍ .0001; and short condition.
unstructured and long unstructured, t(22) ⫽ 3.29, p ⫽ .003.
Information quality. A good fit was also found between the
amount of acquired information and a linear increase across the profile correlations were transformed using Fisher’s r-to-z trans-
three levels of information quality: trivia (M ⫽ 1.65, SD ⫽ 1.04); formation, and then contrast analyses (Rosenthal et al., 2000) were
short unstructured (M ⫽ 5.17, SD ⫽ 1.09); GTK (M ⫽ 5.87, SD ⫽ used to determine the degree to which the profile correlations
1.57); and rcontrast ⫽ .82, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 8.22, p ⬍ .0001 (see Figure followed the predictions based on RAM and WAM.
3). Participants in both the short unstructured and GTK conditions As predicted, we found a good fit between realistic accuracy
acquired reliably more information than did participants in the scores and the prediction of a positive linear relationship with
trivia condition, t(22) ⫽ 8.08, p ⬍ .0001, and t(22) ⫽ 7.75, p ⬍ information quantity: minimal information (mean r ⫽ .29, SD ⫽
.0001, respectively, although information acquisition did not differ 0.20); short unstructured (mean r ⫽ .45, SD ⫽ 0.14); long un-
reliably between the short unstructured and GTK conditions, structured (mean r ⫽ .47, SD ⫽ 0.09); and rcontrast ⫽ .53,
t(22) ⫽ 1.26, p ⫽ .22. tcontrast(33) ⫽ 3.56, p ⫽ .000058 (see Figure 4). The linear pre-
These findings concerning a small sample of the available diction accounted for 86% of the variance in the group means, and
information imply that participants in longer and higher quality the variance not accounted for by the prediction did not reach the
interactions acquired more information about each other. The next .05 level of significance, tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 1.41, p ⫽ .08. When the
analyses addressed the question of whether personality judgments groups were compared with each other, realistic accuracy was
were also more accurate in the longer and higher quality reliably higher in both the short unstructured and long unstructured
interactions. conditions than in the minimal information condition, t(22) ⫽
2.57, p ⫽ .02, and t(22) ⫽ 3.46, p ⫽ .002, respectively, but
Personality Judgment accuracy in the long unstructured condition was not reliably higher
than accuracy in the short unstructured condition, t(22) ⫽ 0.74,
Information quantity. To test the prediction that judges would p ⫽ .47.
be more accurate after interacting for longer periods of time, we Information quantity also had a good fit to the prediction of a
examined personality judgments at three levels of information positive linear relationship with consensus: minimal information
quantity: low (minimal information–no interaction), medium (mean r ⫽ .25, SD ⫽ 0.18); short unstructured (mean r ⫽ .41,
(short unstructured–50-min interaction), and high (long SD ⫽ 0.20); long unstructured (mean r ⫽ .44, SD ⫽ 0.17); and
unstructured–3-hr interaction). First, profile correlations were rcontrast ⫽ .44, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 2.83, p ⫽ .004. The linear prediction
computed for each group, as described in the Analyses section. The accounted for 86% of the variance in the group means, and the

Figure 4. Mean realistic accuracy scores for experimental conditions


Figure 2. Mean information acquisition scores for experimental condi- used in the information quantity analyses. Note that the random baseline
tions used in the information quantity analyses. profile correlation was .36.
120 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

variance not accounted for by the prediction was not significant, (mean r ⫽ .45, SD ⫽ 0.12); GTK (mean r ⫽ .49, SD ⫽ 0.16); and
tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 1.14, p ⫽ .13. Judges in the short unstructured rcontrast ⫽ .43, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 2.71, p ⫽ .0053 (see Figure 5). The
and long unstructured conditions had reliably higher consensus linear prediction accounted for 97% of the variance in the group
than judges in the minimal information condition, t(22) ⫽ 2.33, means, and the variance not accounted for by the prediction was
p ⫽ .03, and t(22) ⫽ 2.98, p ⫽ .007, respectively, although not significant, tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 0.50, p ⫽ .31. Realistic accuracy
consensus did not differ for judges in the short unstructured and was reliably higher in the GTK condition than in the trivia condi-
long unstructured conditions, t(22) ⫽ 0.42, p ⫽ .68. tion, t(22) ⫽ 2.76, p ⫽ .01, and the difference between the trivia
The consensus data were also in line with the prediction based and short unstructured conditions approached the conventional
on WAM, in which an increase was predicted between the low- level of significance, t(22) ⫽ 1.94, p ⫽ .07. However, the GTK
and medium-quantity conditions but not between the medium- and condition did not differ reliably from the short unstructured con-
high-quantity conditions, rcontrast ⫽ .48, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 3.04, p ⫽ dition, t(22) ⫽ 0.88, p ⫽ .39.
.0023. The nonlinear prediction accounted for 98% of the variance A good fit to the prediction of a linear increase was also found
in the group means, and the variance not accounted for by the with consensus: trivia (mean r ⫽ .29, SD ⫽ 0.17); short unstruc-
prediction was not significant, tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 1.18, p ⫽ .12. tured (mean r ⫽ .41, SD ⫽ 0.20); GTK (mean r ⫽ .41, SD ⫽
Finally, information quantity had a good fit to the prediction of 0.10); and rcontrast ⫽ .33, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 1.99, p ⫽ .028. The linear
a positive linear increase with self– other agreement: minimal prediction accounted for 74% of the variance in the group means,
information (mean r ⫽ .21, SD ⫽ 0.19); short unstructured (mean and the variance not accounted for by the prediction was not
r ⫽ .36, SD ⫽ 0.12); long unstructured (mean r ⫽ .39, SD ⫽ 0.12); significant, tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 1.19, p ⫽ .12. Judges in the GTK
rcontrast ⫽ .49, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 3.24, p ⫽ .0014. The linear prediction condition had reliably higher consensus than judges in the trivia
accounted for 86% of the variance in the group means, and the condition, t(22) ⫽ 2.34, p ⫽ .03; the difference between consensus
variance not accounted for by the prediction was not significant, in the short unstructured and trivia conditions approached signif-
tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 1.29, p ⫽ .10. Judges in the short unstructured icance, t(22) ⫽ 1.78, p ⫽ .09; and consensus did not differ for
and long unstructured conditions had reliably higher self– other judges in the short unstructured and GTK conditions, t(22) ⫽ 0.04,
agreement than judges in the minimal information condition, p ⫽ .97.
t(22) ⫽ 2.58, p ⫽ .02, and t(22) ⫽ 3.03, p ⫽ .006, respectively, The consensus data were also in line with the prediction based
although self– other agreement did not differ for judges in the short on WAM, rcontrast ⫽ .37, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 2.31, p ⫽ .014. The
unstructured and long unstructured conditions, t(22) ⫽ 0.72, p ⫽ nonlinear prediction accounted for 99% of the variance in the
.55. group means, and the variance not accounted for by the prediction
Note that for all indicators of accuracy, the mean profile corre- was not significant, tnoncontrast(33) ⫽ 0.001, p ⫽ .50.
lations for the minimal information condition were approximately Finally, a good fit to the prediction of a linear increase was
equal to or smaller than the random baseline correlations of .36 for found with self– other agreement: trivia (mean r ⫽ .27, SD ⫽
realistic accuracy, t(11) ⫽ 0.31, p ⫽ .31; .25 for consensus, 0.09); short unstructured (mean r ⫽ .36, SD ⫽ 0.12); GTK (mean
t(11) ⫽ 0.11, p ⫽ .91; and .28 for self– other agreement, t(11) ⫽ r ⫽ .38, SD ⫽ 0.16); rcontrast ⫽ .39, tcontrast(33) ⫽ 2.40, p ⫽ .011.
1.29, p ⫽ .22; but the mean profile correlations were all higher The linear prediction accounted for 91% of the variance in the
than the random baseline correlations for the short unstructured group means, and the variance not accounted for by the prediction
condition: realistic accuracy, t(11) ⫽ 2.94, p ⫽ .01; consensus, was not significant, tnoncontrast ⫽ 0.75, p ⫽ .23. Judges in the short
t(11) ⫽ 3.22, p ⫽ .008; and self– other agreement, t(11) ⫽ 2.76, unstructured and GTK conditions had reliably higher self– other
p ⫽ .02; and for the long unstructured condition: realistic accu- agreement than judges in the trivia condition, t(22) ⫽ 2.16, p ⫽
racy, t(11) ⫽ 6.31, p ⬍ .0001; consensus, t(11) ⫽ 4.44, p ⫽ .001; .04, and t(22) ⫽ 2.33, p ⫽ .03, respectively, although self– other
and self– other agreement, t(11) ⫽ 3.53, p ⫽ .005. This pattern agreement did not differ for judges in the short unstructured and
suggests that even though realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– GTK conditions, t(22) ⫽ 0.49, p ⫽ .63.
other agreement can reach moderate strengths without any inter- Note that again, the mean profile correlations for the trivia
action, this strength is at or below chance levels. It is only when condition were approximately equal to or smaller than the random
people are allowed to interact that differential realistic accuracy, baseline correlations of .36 for realistic accuracy, t(11) ⫽ 0.48,
consensus, and self– other agreement are achieved.
Information quality. Recall that information quality refers to
the relevance of information to personality. To test our prediction
that judgments would be more accurate when participants inter-
acted in higher quality situations, we had participants interact in
three experimental conditions in which information quantity was
held constant while information quality was manipulated. We
again used contrast weights reflecting the prediction of a linear
relationship between information quality and realistic accuracy,
consensus, and self– other agreement to test the prediction based
on RAM and a nonlinear relationship to test the prediction for
consensus based on WAM.
As predicted, we found that realistic accuracy had a good fit to Figure 5. Mean realistic accuracy scores for experimental conditions
the prediction of a positive linear relationship with information used in the information quality analyses. Note that the random baseline
quality: trivia (mean r ⫽ .36, SD ⫽ 0.12); short unstructured profile correlation was .36. GTK ⫽ get to know condition.
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 121

p ⫽ .64; .25 for consensus, t(11) ⫽ 1.09, p ⫽ .30; and .28 for findings that self– other agreement increases with acquaintance
self– other agreement, t(11) ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ .98, but the mean profile (Bernieri et al., 1994; Blackman & Funder, 1998; Funder et al.,
correlations were all higher than the random baseline correlations 1995; Paulhus & Bruce, 1992; Paunonen, 1989) and provides one
for the short unstructured condition: realistic accuracy, t(11) ⫽ more piece of evidence in favor of the existence of a positive
2.94, p ⫽ .01; consensus, t(11) ⫽ 3.22, p ⫽ .008; and self– other relationship between level of acquaintance and self– other
agreement, t(11) ⫽ 2.76, p ⫽ .02; and for the GTK condition: agreement.
realistic accuracy, t(11) ⫽ 3.84, p ⫽ .003; consensus, t(11) ⫽ 6.45, In addition to confirming past findings, the current research
p ⬍ .0001; and self– other agreement, t(11) ⫽ 2.73, p ⫽ .02. This takes an additional step in the examination of the acquaintanceship
pattern suggests that not just any context of interaction or obser- effect by using a broad-based accuracy criterion based on the
vation will afford differential judgment but that differential real- ratings of several people who knew the target well. Such a criterion
istic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement are only allows for a more precise test of the acquaintanceship effect than
achieved in the conditions that allow for the exchange of was possible in past work. In line with our prediction, realistic
moderate- to high-quality information. accuracy was positively related to information quantity.
Both RAM and WAM can be used to make predictions con-
Relation Between Information Acquisition and Accuracy cerning consensus. RAM predicts that realistic accuracy will in-
crease across all levels of acquaintance, and because people who
We could also examine the degree to which information acqui- are highly accurate about something real must also be in agree-
sition is related to realistic accuracy, self– other agreement, and ment, RAM predicts that consensus will also increase with ac-
consensus across all groups. RAM predicts that people who ac- quaintance (Funder, 1995). WAM is specifically aimed at explain-
quire more information that is relevant to personality will make ing consensus, and our prediction was that consensus would
more accurate judgments of personality, and it is reasonable to increase at low levels of acquaintance and then stay about the same
assume that participants who acquired more of the sample of even as acquaintance continued to increase (Kenny, 1991). These
information asked about on the information fact sheet also ac- predictions are fairly similar, and we found support for both
quired more personality-relevant information, broadly speaking. If predictions. It is not possible to determine if one prediction is
this is true, then there should be a positive relationship between superior to the other, for a couple of reasons. First, the predictions
scores on the information fact sheet and realistic accuracy, self– themselves are highly similar, and so any data should fit both
other agreement, and consensus, regardless of the type of experi- predictions to about the same degree. Second, it was not possible
mental condition in which the participants interacted.12 This pre- to determine whether the experimentally manipulated difference
diction can be tested by correlating the average information fact between the low- and medium-information quantity conditions
sheet score of each group with the average realistic accuracy, was equal to the difference between the medium and high condi-
consensus, and self– other agreement scores of each group across tions and, therefore, whether a linear or nonlinear prediction was
all five experimental conditions. As predicted, moderate positive more appropriate. However, it is obvious from the means of the
relationships were found between information acquisition and re- conditions that the larger increase was between the low and me-
alistic accuracy (r ⫽ .31, p ⫽ .02), self– other agreement (r ⫽ .30, dium groups, but it is not possible to determine whether the
p ⫽ .02), and consensus (r ⫽ .30, p ⫽ .02), indicating that findings support the prediction based on WAM better than the
regardless of information quantity or quality, groups that acquired prediction based on RAM.
more of the objective information that was sampled with the Overall, these findings support the existence of the acquain-
information fact sheet also judged their partners with higher aver- tanceship effect and the role of information quantity in accurate
age levels of accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement. judgment. This interpretation was bolstered by the additional find-
ing that participants learned more facts about each other in the
Discussion longer interactions and therefore had a larger quantity of informa-
tion available to them when making judgments of personality.
The preceding analyses provide tests of the relationships be-
tween two aspects of good information and the accuracy of per- Information Quality
sonality judgment. Experimentally manipulated information quan-
tity, or length of acquaintance, was positively related to realistic To our knowledge, the current article provides the first direct
accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement. Additionally, in- evidence of the positive relationships between information quality
formation quality was manipulated while information quantity was and realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement. In-
held constant, and support was found for the prediction that real- formation quality was manipulated by varying situational strength
istic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement are higher in and instructions that provided an objective for the interaction. For
situations in which personality-relevant information is more likely
to be available. 12
Some astute readers may wonder why we did not perform a media-
tional analysis with amount of information acquired as a mediator of the
Information Quantity relations of information quantity and quality with accuracy. We did not
compute these analyses because the information fact sheet was designed to
RAM predicts that accuracy will increase in relation to infor- be a manipulation check and is only a sample of some relatively objective
mation quantity (Funder, 1995), but previous research has only information that is likely to become available during an initial interaction,
tested this prediction using self– other agreement, which is a lim- and it is not a comprehensive measure of the amount of personality-
ited definition of accuracy. The current research confirms past relevant information acquired.
122 LETZRING, WELLS, AND FUNDER

the low-quality interaction, participants interacted in a strong sit- The present results involving realistic accuracy, consensus, and
uation with specific instructions to complete a packet of trivia-type self– other agreement were in general quite similar to each other.
questions. We expected participants in this condition to exhibit This finding could be interpreted as evidence that realistic accu-
little behavioral variation and to not share information about racy cannot be meaningfully distinguished from the simpler and
thoughts and feelings, and therefore only limited amounts of more traditional operationalizations or that constructing multifac-
personality-relevant information would become available. For the eted criteria is not worth the time and effort. We caution against
medium- and high-quality interactions, participants interacted in both conclusions. First, realistic accuracy is a hypothetical con-
relatively unstructured situations that allowed for a good deal of struct that is in principle quite different from any specific criterion
behavioral variation and larger amounts of personality-relevant for or measurement of it, and on theoretical grounds it is important
information to become available. The difference in these two to be clear about the distinction. Second, it is easy to imagine
conditions was the instructions informing the participants about circumstances in which self– other agreement, consensus, and re-
the objective of the interaction. Participants in the medium-quality alistic accuracy would diverge (such as when an absentminded job
interaction were not provided with an objective, whereas partici- applicant tries to appear conscientious), and at our present state of
pants in the high-quality interaction were given the specific ob- knowledge it is wise to be alert to this possibility and to develop
jective of getting to know each other as well as possible. Partici- tools to detect such differentiation between constructs when it
pants in this latter condition were expected to reveal the most happens.
personality-relevant information and to pay the best attention to Even though the contrast analyses suggest that there are mod-
the interaction partners in an attempt to get to know them well, erate relationships between information quantity and quality and
which should have increased accuracy by affecting the availability indicators of accuracy, this effect was largely driven by differences
and detection stages of RAM. Results confirmed these expecta- between the low conditions (minimal information and trivia) and
tions, with realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement the medium and high conditions (short unstructured, GTK, and
all increasing, along with factual knowledge, across the three long unstructured), with the medium and high conditions being
conditions of information quality. However, the present data do only slightly different, albeit consistently in the predicted direc-
not allow us to determine whether these effects should be attrib- tion. The pattern of findings would have been even more convinc-
uted to what cognitive psychologists would call the encoding stage ing had there been a larger difference in realistic accuracy between
or the decoding stage. It may be that more relevant information the medium- and high-level conditions. We propose that the
was available to be detected (encoding), that the detected infor- present evidence supports the role of quantity and quality in
mation was remembered and utilized better (decoding), or both. accuracy but that our highest conditions may not have increased
As noted in the Results section, even though the data showed a acquaintance enough or provided enough motivation to show a
good fit to a linear prediction, most of the increase in the indicators substantial increase in indicators of accuracy beyond the medium
of accuracy was between the low- and medium-quality conditions, levels. Future research should experiment further with techniques
and the medium- and high-quality conditions were quite similar. for providing participants large amounts of information or infor-
One explanation for this finding is that people naturally try to get mation that is highly relevant to personality.
to know each other in an initial interaction, so even though we Another reasonable next step would be to increase the external
explicitly instructed participants in the high-quality condition to validity of our conclusions through a project in which interactions
get to know each other and simply told participants in the medium- take place in real social contexts (vs. in the lab, as was the case in
quality condition to talk about whatever they liked, they may have the current experiment). There are many situations in which pre-
had similar interactions in which they learned approximately the viously unacquainted groups of people interact and get to know
same amounts of personality-relevant information that could be each other over a period of time, such as 1st-year freshmen in
used when making judgments of personality. If this is the case, college dorms, participants in volunteer corps who live and/or
then information quality may not have been very different between work together (i.e., Peace Corps, Jesuit Volunteer Corps), and
our medium- and high-quality conditions, which would explain older adults who move into retirement communities. Information
why realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– other agreement were quantity could be examined in such situations by obtaining judg-
also not very different in these conditions. ments about others in the judge’s living community at several
stages of acquaintance. Information quality could be examined by
Limitations and Future Directions obtaining information about the quality of interactions from both
the target and the judge, perhaps by asking for reports of types of
As in any study, our experimental design has limitations that topics most often discussed and amount of time spent talking
may affect interpretation. First, the construct of realistic accuracy versus engaging in other activities. Such a project is likely to be
is a philosophical ideal, and any measurement of it must neces- informative about how information quantity and quality are related
sarily be an approximation. We attempted to approach realistic to realistic accuracy in situations and contexts outside of the
accuracy by having several knowledgeable people, including ex- laboratory, as well as to how these two aspects of information are
pert judges, describe the personalities of the target participants. related to each other and personality judgment.
This procedure was not likely to tell us exactly what the target is A third direction for future research is to determine the actual
like, but it was likely to bring us closer to reality than would behavioral events during an interaction that are related to the achieved
ratings provided by any single informant, even the self. In future levels of realistic accuracy, self– other agreement, and consensus.
research, we would hope to see criteria for realistic accuracy Behavioral coding of the experimental interactions might increase our
expanded even further to include direct behavioral measurements, understanding of why information quantity and quality are related to
important life outcomes, and perhaps even biological markers. accuracy, self– other agreement, and consensus.
INFORMATION AND PERSONALITY JUDGMENT 123

Conclusion Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: Acquain-
tanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judgment. Journal
The current findings support several aspects of RAM, including of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 149 –158.
the implications of information for the relevance, availability, and Funder, D. C., & Dobroth, K. M. (1987). Differences between traits:
detection stages that make accurate judgment possible, along with Properties associated with interjudge agreement. Journal of Personality
specific predictions of WAM concerning consensus. Our main and Social Psychology, 52, 409 – 418.
findings are that judgments of personality are more likely to Funder, D. C., Kolar, D. C., & Blackman, M. C. (1995). Agreement among
achieve higher levels of realistic accuracy, consensus, and self– judges of personality: Interpersonal relations, similarity, and acquain-
other agreement when judges have interacted with targets for tanceship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 656 – 672.
longer periods of time or in situations that allow for or encourage Hofstee, W. K. B. (1994). Who should own the definition of personality?
European Journal of Personality, 8, 149 –162.
people to reveal personality-relevant information.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of interjudge agreement
on personality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluative-
References ness, and the unique perspective of the self. Journal of Personality, 41,
Allport, G. W. (1937). The ability to judge people. In G. W. Allport, 521–551.
Personality: A psychological interpretation (pp. 499 –522). New York: Kenny, D. A. (1991). A general model of consensus and accuracy in
Holt. interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 98, 155–163.
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception. New York: Guilford Press.
interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kenny, D. A., Albright, L., Malloy, T. E., & Kashy, D. A. (1994).
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). On judging and being Consensus in interpersonal perception: Acquaintance and the Big Five.
judged accurately in zero-acquaintance situations. Journal of Personal- Psychological Bulletin, 116, 245–258.
ity and Social Psychology, 69, 518 –529. Kolar, D. W. (1995). Individual differences in the ability to accurately
Anderson, S. M. (1984). Self-knowledge and social inference: II. The judge the personality characteristics of others. Unpublished doctoral
diagnosticity of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Per- dissertation, University of California, Riverside.
sonality and Social Psychology, 46, 294 –307. Kolar, D. W., Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1996). Comparing the
Bem, D. J., & Funder, D. C. (1978). Predicting more of the people more of accuracy of personality judgments by the self and knowledgeable others.
the time: Assessing the personality of situations. Psychological Review, Journal of Personality, 64, 311–337.
85, 485–501. Letzring, T. D., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (2005). Ego-control and
Bernieri, F. J., Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Rosenthal, R. (1994). ego-resiliency: Generalization of self-report scales based on personality
Measuring person perception accuracy: Another look at self– other descriptions from acquaintances, clinicians, and the self. Journal of
agreement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 367–378. Research in Personality, 39, 395– 422.
Blackman, M. C., & Funder, D. C. (1998). The effect of information on Norman, W. T., & Goldberg, L. R. (1966). Raters, ratees, and randomness
consensus and accuracy in personality judgment. Journal of Experimen- in personality structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
tal Social Psychology, 34, 164 –181. 4, 681– 691.
Block, J. (1961). The Q-sort method in personality assessment and psy- Park, B., & Judd, J. M. (1989). Agreement on initial impressions: Differ-
chiatric research. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. ences due to perceivers, trait dimensions, and target behaviors. Journal
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Consensus and self– other agreement of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 493–505.
for trait inferences from minimal information. Journal of Personality, Park, B., Kraus, S., & Ryan, C. S. (1997). Longitudinal changes in
61, 477– 496. consensus as a function of acquaintance and agreement in liking. Journal
Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 604 – 616.
(2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Paulhus, D. L., & Bruce, M. N. (1992). The effect of acquaintanceship on
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599 – 614. the validity of personality impressions: A longitudinal study. Journal of
Colvin, R. C. (1993). “Judgeable” people: Personality, behavior, and Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 816 – 824.
competing explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Paunonen, S. V. (1989). Consensus in personality judgment: Moderating
64, 861– 873. effects of target–rater acquaintanceship and behavior observability.
Colvin, R. C., & Funder, D. C. (1991). Predicting personality and behavior: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 823– 833.
A boundary on the acquaintanceship effect. Journal of Personality and Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Contrasts and effect
Social Psychology, 60, 884 – 894. sizes in behavioral research: A correlational approach. New York:
Craik, K. H., Ware, A. P., Kamp, J., O’Reilly, C., III, Staw, B., & Zedeck, Cambridge University Press.
S. (2002). Explorations of construct validity in a combined managerial Snyder, M., & Ickes, W. (1985). Personality and social behavior. In G.
and personality assessment programme. Journal of Occupational and Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed.,
Organizational Psychology, 75, 171–193. Vol. 2, pp. 883–947). New York: Random House.
Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on “understanding of Taft, R. (1955). The ability to judge people. Psychological Bulletin, 52,
others” and “assumed similarity.” Psychological Bulletin, 52, 177–193. 1–23.
Funder, D. C. (1980). On seeing ourselves as others see us: Self– other Vernon, P. E. (1933). Some characteristics of the good judge of personal-
agreement and discrepancy in personality ratings. Journal of Personal- ity. Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 42–57.
ity, 48, 473– 493. Vogt, D. S., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Interpersonal orientation and the
Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of accuracy of personality judgments. Journal of Personality, 71, 267–295.
social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.
Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic
approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652– 670. Received August 26, 2004
Funder, D. C. (1999). Personality judgment: A realistic approach to person Revision received July 22, 2005
perception. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Accepted December 14, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 124 –142 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.124

Supplication and Appeasement in Conflict and Negotiation:


The Interpersonal Effects of Disappointment, Worry, Guilt, and Regret

Gerben A. Van Kleef and Carsten K. W. De Dreu Antony S. R. Manstead


University of Amsterdam Cardiff University

This study examined the social effects of emotions related to supplication and appeasement in conflict
and negotiation. In a computer-simulated negotiation, participants in Experiment 1 were confronted with
a disappointed or worried opponent (supplication), with a guilty or regretful opponent (appeasement), or
with a nonemotional opponent (control). Compared with controls, participants conceded more when the
other experienced supplication emotions and conceded less when the other experienced appeasement
emotions (especially guilt). Experiment 2 replicated the effects of disappointment and guilt and showed
that they are moderated by the perceiver’s dispositional trust: Negotiators high in trust conceded more to
a disappointed counterpart than to a happy one, but those with low trust were unaffected. In Experiment
3, trust was manipulated through information about the other’s personality (cooperative vs. competitive),
and a similar moderation was obtained.

Keywords: conflict, negotiation, emotion, supplication, appeasement

Social interactions can produce conflict at all levels of society. duration (Barry, 1999; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996), and intentional—
One of the most common and constructive ways of resolving such directed at an object, person, or event (Frijda, 1993). In this article,
conflicts and conducting social and economic exchange is by we use the term emotion in the sense intended above, whereas
means of negotiation. Negotiation can be defined as a discussion affect is used as a superordinate construct that encompasses both
between two or more parties aimed at resolving a perceived moods and emotions (Barry & Oliver, 1996).
divergence of interests (Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993). People may Research on emotion in conflict and negotiation can be roughly
negotiate with a car dealer when buying a new car, work groups divided into two categories: studies of intrapersonal effects and
may negotiate the allocation of organizational resources, and par- studies of interpersonal effects. In the 1980s and 1990s, research-
ents may negotiate with their children about how to spend the ers focused mostly on the intrapersonal effects of moods and
holidays. Emotions are inherent to negotiation and social conflict emotions, investigating the influence of a negotiator’s emotional
(Davidson & Greenhalgh, 1999) and are crucial to understanding state on his or her own cognitions and behavior. For example,
how individuals behave within bargaining situations (Barry, 1999). positive affect has been shown to increase concession making
So far, empirical research on emotion in conflict and negotiation (Baron, 1990), stimulate creative problem solving (Isen, Daubman,
has focused almost exclusively on the effects of anger and happi- & Nowicki, 1987), increase joint gains (Allred, Mallozzi, Matsui,
ness. In this article, we focus on the social effects of emotions & Raia, 1997; Carnevale & Isen, 1986), increase preferences for
related to supplication (e.g., disappointment, worry) and appease- cooperation (Baron, Fortin, Frei, Hauver, & Shack, 1990), reduce
ment (e.g., guilt, regret) in negotiation, examining the ways in the use of contentious tactics (Carnevale & Isen, 1986), and
which negotiators respond to their opponent’s emotions.
increase the use of cooperative negotiation strategies (Forgas,
1998). Conversely, negative affect has been shown to decrease
Emotions in Conflict and Negotiation initial offers (Baron et al., 1990), decrease joint gains (Allred et al.,
There are multiple definitions of emotion, most of which point 1997), promote the rejection of ultimatum offers (Pillutla & Mur-
to three distinct features of emotion: physiological reactions, ac- nighan, 1996), increase the use of competitive strategies (Forgas,
tion tendencies, and subjective experience (Lazarus, 1991). Emo- 1998), and decrease the desire to work together in the future
tions differ from moods in that they are discrete (Russell & (Allred et al., 1997).
Feldman Barrett, 1999), of relatively high intensity and short Recently, scientific interest in the role of affect in conflict and
negotiation has shifted away from the intrapersonal effects of
moods and emotions. Recognizing that negotiation is a social
phenomenon—negotiators’ emotions influence not only them-
Gerben A. Van Kleef and Carsten K. W. De Dreu, Department of selves, but also their counterparts—several scholars have empha-
Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; sized the importance of the interpersonal or social effects of
Antony S. R. Manstead, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff,
emotions in negotiations (Adler, Rosen, & Silverstein, 1998;
United Kingdom.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerben Barry, Fulmer, & Van Kleef, 2004; Davidson & Greenhalgh, 1999;
A. Van Kleef, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Morris & Keltner, 2000; Thompson, Medvec, Seiden, & Kopel-
Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, the Netherlands. E-mail: man, 2001; Van Kleef, De Dreu, & Manstead, 2004a, b). The basic
g.a.vankleef@uva.nl premise is that emotions have important social functions and
124
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 125

consequences (Frijda & Mesquita, 1994; Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Specifically, we focused on the interpersonal effects of disappoint-
Oatley & Jenkins, 1992; Parkinson, 1996). Most notable is that ment, worry, guilt, and regret on demands and concessions in
emotions convey information (Carver & Scheier, 1990), for in- negotiations. Below we review research pertaining to these emo-
stance about how one feels about things (Ekman, 1993), about tions, and we build on this research to advance a number of
one’s social intentions (Fridlund, 1994), and about one’s orienta- hypotheses regarding the interpersonal effects of these emotions
tion toward other people (Knutson, 1996). In this way, one’s on negotiation behavior.
emotions may influence not only one’s own behavior, but also the
behavior of others (Levenson, 1994). When One Feels One Has Taken Too Much:
In line with this social functions perspective, recent research has Appeasement
demonstrated the pervasive interpersonal effects of anger and
happiness in negotiations. In a computer-mediated negotiation task Emotions such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and interper-
with a simulated opponent, Van Kleef et al. (2004a) provided sonal regret serve an appeasement function (Baumeister, Stillwell,
participants with information about the opponent’s emotional state & Heatherton, 1994; Keltner & Buswell, 1997). Of these emotions,
(angry, happy, or no emotion) at three time points during the guilt is perhaps the most extensively researched. It entails a feeling
negotiation. They found that participants with an angry opponent of self-reproach resulting from the belief that one has done some-
placed lower demands and made larger concessions than did thing wrong (R. H. Smith, Webster, Parrott, & Eyre, 2002).
participants with a nonemotional opponent, whereas participants Baumeister et al. (1994) noted that “from an interpersonal per-
with a happy opponent placed higher demands and made smaller spective, the prototypical cause of guilt would be the infliction of
concessions. Sinaceur and Tiedens (in press) examined the effects harm, loss, or distress on a relationship partner” (p. 245). The
of anger and happiness using a different paradigm and obtained experience of guilt is typically rooted in an interpersonal context
similar results. In face-to-face negotiations, they instructed one and often (although not necessarily) arises as a result of a per-
negotiator in each dyad to show either anger or happiness. In ceived transgression, in particular when the individual feels that he
keeping with the results obtained by Van Kleef et al. (2004a), or she has violated some expectation or norm (Leith & Baumeister,
Sinaceur and Tiedens found that participants conceded more to an 1998; H. B. Lewis, 1971; Tangney, 1990, 1995, 1999).
angry than to a happy counterpart. So results from research using Guilt is closely linked with reactions such as regret, self-
different procedures (i.e., computer mediated and face to face) reproach, repentance, and remorse (R. H. Smith et al., 2002) and
point to the social impact of anger and happiness on negotiation tends to produce outwardly focused behaviors aimed at reducing
behavior. the damage caused by one’s behavior (Barrett, 1995; Tangney,
Given the pervasive interpersonal effects of anger and happiness 1995). Guilt is associated with perspective-taking, interpersonal
on negotiation behavior, it is worth considering whether other sensitivity, and improved relationship outcomes (Baumeister et al.,
emotions have the potential to affect negotiation behavior. Accord- 1994; Leith & Baumeister, 1998). People experiencing guilt tend
ing to the social functions perspective, emotions convey informa- to engage in behaviors aimed at repairing the social relationship
tion that is likely to influence other people’s behavior. For exam- (Baumeister et al., 1994; M. Lewis, 2000). Research has shown
ple, Van Kleef et al. (2004a) demonstrated that negotiators that transgressions and concomitant guilt increase subsequent
concede more to angry counterparts than to happy ones because helping, compliance, and cooperation on the part of the transgres-
anger signals high limits, whereas happiness signals low limits. sor (Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Freedman, Wallington, & Bless,
Obviously, emotions can also convey other important information, 1967; Ketelaar & Au, 2003). Other research has found that guilt
the strategic implications and interpretation of which are likely to motivates people to apologize and to make reparations or amends
depend in part on observers’ appraisals of the cause of the emotion. (Friedman, 1985; Hoffman, 1982; H. B. Lewis, 1971). Further-
For instance, in a negotiation the distribution of resources can be more, when the transgression has an interpersonal character, guilt
perceived as fair or unfair (Bazerman, Curhan, Moore, & Valley, motivates people to compensate the victim (Berscheid & Walster,
2000; Hegtvedt & Killian, 1999). Research on distributive justice 1967; Wallace & Sadalla, 1966). Thus, if the transgressor displays
has shown that fair distributions give rise to positive emotions, guilt, the victim may see this as an implicit commitment to rectify
whereas unfair distributions give rise to negative emotions (De the transgression by making amends and as a promise of better
Dreu, Lualhati, & McCusker, 1994; Loewenstein, Thompson, & treatment in the future (Baumeister et al., 1994; Manstead, 1991).
Bazerman, 1989). Individuals who receive what they expected In an interpersonal context, social transgressions can also cause
tend to experience happiness or satisfaction (Hegtvedt, 1990; Mes- feelings of regret (Zeelenberg et al., 1998). When the regret is
sick & Sentis, 1979; Sprecher, 1992). By contrast, individuals who interpersonal in nature—that is, when one regrets a behavior that
feel that they are getting too much or too little are likely to has inflicted harm on another person (rather than on oneself)—it
experience negative emotions. Emotional reactions that are likely shares a number of characteristics with guilt (Berndsen, van der
to occur in individuals who feel underrewarded include disappoint- Pligt, Doosje, & Manstead, 2004; Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz,
ment, sadness, depression, anger, and resentment (Hegtvedt & 1994). Because regret is an aversive state, people are motivated to
Killian, 1999), whereas those who feel overrewarded can be ex- avoid it and, once they experience it, to take action to undo it
pected to experience guilt (Hegtvedt & Killian, 1999; Homans, (Zeelenberg & Beattie, 1997; Zeelenberg, van Dijk, Manstead, &
1974) or regret (van Dijk & Zeelenberg, 2002; Zeelenberg, van der van der Pligt, 2000). Gilovich and Medvec (1994, 1995) refer to
Pligt, & Manstead, 1998). this undoing as “behavioral repair work” or “ameliorative behav-
The purpose of the present research was to investigate the ior.” In the case of interpersonal regret, this repair work typically
interpersonal effects of emotions that may arise as a result of the takes the form of apologizing to the person who has been affected
appraisal that one has taken too much or received too little. by the transgression (Steiner, 2000; Zeelenberg et al., 1998).
126 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

By extrapolating these findings to the domain of conflict and pointment; Bell, 1985; Loomes & Sugden, 1986; see Zeelenberg et
negotiation, we can advance the following general predictions. al., 2000, for an overview). Timmers, Fischer, and Manstead
First, a negotiator who deals with an opponent who appears to (1998) reported evidence suggesting that under particular circum-
experience emotions of appeasement (e.g., guilt or regret) is likely stances, people may deliberately express disappointment to change
to expect the opponent to make up for his or her previous “trans- the behavior of a target person in a direction that would be
gression” (e.g., tough demands) by making a concession. Follow- beneficial for the expresser. We argue that, at the interpersonal
ing this line of reasoning, a negotiator faced with an opponent who level, disappointment is likely to have effects similar to those of
seems to be guilty or regretful can be expected to stand firm and other distress-related emotions (e.g., fear, worry, sadness), which
wait for the other to make a concession. Second, emotions of have generally been shown to facilitate prosocial behavior aimed
appeasement are associated with interpersonal sensitivity and the at easing the other’s pain (Barnett, King, & Howard, 1979; Batson,
willingness to appreciate another person’s perspective, and they 1987; Eisenberg, Fabes, Miller, et al., 1989; Fabes, Eisenberg,
share a number of relationship-repairing qualities (Baumeister et Karbon, Troyer, & Switzer, 1994; Morris & Keltner, 2000).
al., 1994; Leith & Baumeister, 1998; Steiner, 2000). Guilt and On the basis of the above considerations, it can be predicted that
regret can therefore be expected to signal an outward focus and a a negotiator who is confronted with an opponent who appears to
concern for the other and to have a beneficial effect on the experience emotions of supplication (e.g., disappointment or
interpersonal relationship. worry) will try to relieve the other’s pain by making concessions.
Further, emotions of distress are associated with self-focus and
When One Feels One Has Received Too Little: egoistic motivations (Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, & Miller, 1989).
Supplication By reverse analogy with previous research suggesting that ap-
peasement emotions enhance interpersonal relationships because
Emotions such as sadness, disappointment, fear, and worry they signal interpersonal sensitivity and concern (Baumeister et al.,
serve a supplication function (M. S. Clark, Pataki, & Carver, 1994; Leith & Baumeister, 1998), we argue that supplication
1996). These emotions communicate dependency and a need for emotions lead to less positive impressions because they signal an
support (Eisenberg, 2000; Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 2001), and inward focus and preoccupation with the self.
they evoke empathy and helping behavior (M. S. Clark et al., 1996;
Eisenberg, 2000; Hill, Weary, & Williams, 1986; Parrott, 1993). Experiment 1
Sadness, for example, communicates to the self and to others that
one is in need of help (Tomkins, 1963). Expressions of sadness The purpose of Experiment 1 was to investigate the interper-
have been demonstrated to increase perceptions of neediness and sonal effects of emotions related to supplication and appeasement
dependency (M. A. Clark & Taraban, 1991) and to evoke helping in negotiations. We predicted that participants with an opponent
behavior in both children (Barnett, Howard, Melton, & Dino, who experiences disappointment or worry (i.e., emotions of sup-
1982) and adults (M. S. Clark, Ouellette, Powell, & Milberg, 1987; plication) would make smaller demands than would participants
Yee & Greenberg, 1998). In a similar vein, crying serves a help- with a nonemotional opponent (Hypothesis 1a), whereas negotia-
seeking function (Labott, Martin, Eason, & Berkey, 1991). For tors with an opponent who experiences guilt or regret (i.e., emo-
example, Cornelius (1984) demonstrated that (involuntary) crying tions of appeasement) would make larger demands (Hypothesis
was an effective means of eliciting a positive and desired change 1b). We also expected that participants with a guilty or regretful
in other people’s behavior, in this case a shift from conflict to opponent would perceive the opponent as more interpersonally
support. sensitive than would participants with a disappointed or worried
Similar effects on helping behavior have been observed for opponent (Hypothesis 2a) and that they would develop a more
expressions of worry and fear. Like sadness, worry and fear favorable impression of the opponent (Hypothesis 2b). Finally, we
communicate a need for assistance, and they elicit sympathetic and explored whether the effects of the opponent’s emotion on partic-
supportive responses in others (Eisenberg, 2000; Kennedy-Moore ipants’ impressions of the opponent are mediated by their percep-
& Watson, 2001). For example, a study of reactions to crime tion of the opponent’s interpersonal sensitivity (Hypothesis 2c).
victims by Yee and Greenberg (1998) revealed that fear on the part
of the victims influences observers’ appraisals of need and in- Method
creases the inclination to help, especially if the observer and the
victim are in a communal rather than an exchange relationship. By
Participants and Experimental Design
the same token, employees who display fear are likely to evoke A total of 84 male and female undergraduate students at the University
helpful, supportive responses from coworkers (Côté, 2005). of Amsterdam participated either in partial fulfillment of a course require-
In contrast to the interpersonal effects of sadness and, to a lesser ment or for monetary compensation (EUR 7, approximately US $8). The
degree, fear and worry, the interpersonal effects of disappointment experimental design included the opponent’s emotion (disappointment vs.
have not received much research attention. Carver and Scheier worry vs. guilt vs. regret vs. no emotion) as the independent variable and
demand level as the main dependent variable. Participants were randomly
(1990) suggested that disappointment signals that progress toward
assigned to the conditions, and the experimenters were blind to this
a goal is below expectations. In other words, disappointment arises
assignment.
when a desired outcome is not achieved (Bell, 1985; Frijda, 1986;
van Dijk & van der Pligt, 1997), and as such, it is highly relevant
to negotiation. Research on the effects of disappointment is sparse,
Procedure
and it has focused predominantly on intrapersonal consequences For each session, 6 to 8 participants were invited to the laboratory. On
(e.g., engaging in behaviors aimed at minimizing future disap- arrival, participants were welcomed to the experiment and seated in sep-
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 127

arate cubicles in front of a computer. From that point onward all instruc- Manipulation of the opponent’s emotion. We chose to manipulate the
tions, questionnaires, and experimental tasks were presented on the com- opponent’s emotion in the context of a computer-mediated negotiation in
puter screen. To facilitate the manipulation of the opponent’s emotion (see which parties could not see each other and communicated via computers
below), we led participants to believe that the purpose of the study was to (see De Dreu & Van Kleef, 2004; De Dreu & Van Lange, 1995; Hilty &
find out how knowledge about one’s opponent’s intentions affects negoti- Carnevale, 1993). Participants were led to believe that the purpose of the
ation processes in a situation in which the negotiating parties cannot see study was to find out how knowledge about one’s opponent’s intentions
each other. They were then told that they would engage in a computer- affects negotiation processes and outcomes. After the first, third, and fifth
mediated negotiation with another participant (whose behavior was in fact negotiation rounds, participants received information about “the intentions
simulated by the computer). of the buyer,” which contained the manipulation of the buyer’s emotion.
Negotiation task. The negotiation task was one used previously by Van Participants had to wait for about a minute and a half while the buyer was
Kleef et al. (2004a, b; see also De Dreu & Van Lange, 1995; Hilty & supposedly asked to reveal what he or she intended to offer in the next
Carnevale, 1993). The task captures the main characteristics of real-life round, and why. After this short wait, participants received what appeared
negotiation (i.e., multiple issues differing in utility to the negotiator, to be the buyer’s answer. This was presented in a separate box in a
information about one’s own payoffs only, and the typical offer– different font and contained some minor typing errors in order to
counteroffer sequence). In the current version, participants learned that enhance experimental realism. The buyer’s intentions were held constant
they would be assigned the role of buyer or seller of a consignment of across conditions and contained the buyer’s intended offer for the next
mobile phones and that their objective was to negotiate the price, the round. That is, after Round 1 the buyer wrote, “I think I will offer 8 –7–7,”
warranty period, and the duration of the service contract of the phones. which would indeed be the buyer’s next offer. The buyer’s intention
They were then presented with a payoff chart (see Appendix A) that information also contained an emotional statement that constituted the
showed which outcomes were most favorable to them, and they learned experimental manipulation.
that their objective was to earn as many points as possible. Level 9 on price The emotion statements were pretested in a pilot study involving 64
($110) yielded 0 points, and Level 1 ($150) yielded 400 points (i.e., psychology students, none of whom participated in the main study. We
increments of 50 points per level). For warranty period, Level 9 (9 months) tested seven statements designed to reflect disappointment, six statements
yielded 0 points, and Level 1 (1 month) yielded 120 points (i.e., increments designed to reflect worry, seven statements designed to reflect interper-
sonal regret, and seven statements designed to reflect guilt. The statements
of 15 points per level). Finally, for duration of service contract, Level 9 (9
were pretested using a within-participants design. All participants rated a
months) yielded 0 points, and Level 1 (1 month) yielded 240 points (i.e.,
selection of 13 or 14 out of the total of 27 statements, the order of
increments of 30 points per level). Participants were told, “You can see that
statements being randomized across participants. The statements were
the best deal for you is 1–1–1, for a total outcome of 760 points (400 ⫹
distributed in such a way that each was rated by half of the participants. For
120 ⫹ 240).” The corresponding payoff table for the other party was not
each statement, participants were asked to indicate on a 7-point scale how
displayed, and participants were told only that it differed from their own.
comprehensible they found it (1 ⫽ very incomprehensible, 7 ⫽ very
To enhance involvement in the negotiation task, we informed partici-
comprehensible) and to what extent they felt it reflected disappointment,
pants that points would be converted to lottery tickets at the end of the
worry, regret, and guilt (1 ⫽ not at all, 7 ⫽ to a great extent). We then
experiment and that the more points earned, the more lottery tickets one
selected the statements that had the highest scores on the emotion they
would obtain and the greater would be one’s chance of winning a EUR 50
were supposed to reflect and the lowest scores on the emotions that they
(approximately US $64) prize. To emphasize the mixed-motive nature of
were not supposed to reflect, provided that they did not differ with respect
the negotiation, we told participants that only those who reached an
to their comprehensibility. We selected three statements for each emotion.
agreement would participate in the lottery. Thus, there were incentives both
All selected statements were rated higher on the emotion they were
to earn as many points as possible and to reach an agreement.
supposed to express than on the emotions they were not supposed to
After a short pause during which the computer supposedly assigned express according to paired-samples t tests (5.09 ⬍ ts ⬍ 18.29, all ps ⬍
buyer and seller roles to the participants, all participants were assigned the .01). Further, one-sample t tests showed that there was a significant effect
role of seller. They were told that the buyer (i.e., the opponent) would make of all statements on the rating of the corresponding emotion (13.80 ⬍ ts ⬍
the first offer and that the negotiation would continue until an agreement 18.32, all ps ⬍ .01). Finally, paired-samples t tests revealed that the
was reached or time ran out. Just before the negotiation started, participants statements did not differ with respect to comprehensibility (all ts ⬍ 1.12,
learned that an additional goal of the study was to examine the effects of ns).
having versus not having information about the opposing negotiator’s After the first negotiation round, participants in the disappointed oppo-
intentions. They read that the computer had randomly determined that they nent condition received the following information: “I am pretty disap-
would receive information about the intentions of the opponent and that the pointed about this,” followed by the intention statement “I think I will offer
opponent would not receive information about their intentions. 8 –7–7,” which was the same for all conditions. In the worried opponent
After these instructions, the negotiation started, and the buyer (i.e., the condition, participants read “This worries me quite a lot”; in the guilty
computer) made a first offer. Over the negotiation rounds the buyer opponent condition, participants read “I feel guilty for not having conceded
proposed the following levels of agreement (for price–warranty–service): more”; in the regret condition, participants read “I am sorry that I haven’t
8 –7– 8 (Round 1), 8 –7–7 (Round 2), 8 – 6 –7 (Round 3), 7– 6 –7 (Round 4), conceded more.” In the control condition participants received only the
7– 6 – 6 (Round 5), and 6 – 6 – 6 (Round 6). Past research has shown that this intention statement. After the third and fifth negotiation rounds, partici-
preprogrammed strategy has face validity and is seen as intermediate in pants in the experimental conditions again received an emotional statement
cooperativeness and competitiveness (De Dreu & Van Lange, 1995). A and an intention, whereas those in the control condition simply received the
demand by the participant was accepted if it equaled or exceeded the offer intention. Appendix B lists all statements used in the experiment.
the computer was about to make in the next round. If no agreement was Dependent measures. The main dependent variable was participants’
reached by the sixth round, the negotiation was interrupted (see De Dreu & level of demand in Rounds 1 to 6. In addition, participants completed a
Van Lange, 1995). Following the procedure of a study by Tripp and postnegotiation questionnaire that contained manipulation checks and
Sondak (1992), we excluded from the sample participants who reached items designed to measure impressions of the opponent. To check the
agreement before Round 6 (n ⫽ 8) to allow for repeated-measures analy- adequacy of the emotion manipulation, we asked participants to indicate on
ses. (However, retaining these participants yielded a similar pattern of a 7-point scale how disappointed, worried, guilty, and regretful they
results.) thought their opponent had been during the negotiation. Perceptions of the
128 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

opponent’s disappointment were measured by three items (e.g., “The buyer intended emotion than for the other emotions (2.27 ⬍ ts ⬍ 11.73,
appeared to be disappointed during the negotiation,” 1 ⫽ totally disagree all ps ⬍ .05).
to 7 ⫽ totally agree), which were combined into a single index of
perceived disappointment (␣ ⫽ .83). Perception of the opponent’s worry
was measured using five items (e.g., “The buyer appeared to be worried
Demand Level
during the negotiation”), which were averaged into an index of perceived Demand level in Rounds 1 to 6 was analyzed using a mixed-
worry (␣ ⫽ .88). Perception of guilt was measured by three items (e.g., model ANOVA with the opponent’s emotion (disappointment vs.
“The buyer appeared to feel guilty during the negotiation”), which were
worry vs. guilt vs. regret vs. no emotion) as a between-participants
combined into an index of perceived guilt (␣ ⫽ .93). Finally, perceptions
variable and negotiation round (1 to 6) as a repeated-measures
of the opponent’s regret were measured by three items (e.g., “The buyer
appeared to feel sorry during the negotiation”), which were averaged into variable. Unsurprisingly, this analysis revealed a main effect of
a scale of perceived regret (␣ ⫽ .91). round, F(5, 355) ⫽ 286.28, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .80), indicating
Impression of the opponent was assessed with seven items (“The buyer that demands declined from Round 1 (M ⫽ 677, SD ⫽ 63) to
strikes me as a sympathetic person”; “During the negotiation, the buyer Round 6 (M ⫽ 484, SD ⫽ 93). More important, there was a
made a hostile impression,” reverse scored; “The buyer made a cooperative significant effect of the opponent’s emotion on average demands,
impression”; “The buyer made a friendly impression”; “The buyer made a F(4, 71) ⫽ 7.76, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .30), indicating that
competitive impression,” reverse scored; “The buyer made a stubborn participants’ demands were influenced by their adversaries’ emo-
impression,” reverse scored; “I have developed a positive impression of the tions (disappointment: M ⫽ 533, SD ⫽ 59; worry: M ⫽ 513, SD ⫽
buyer”; 1 ⫽ totally disagree to 7 ⫽ totally agree). The seven items were
76; guilt: M ⫽ 633, SD ⫽ 79; regret: M ⫽ 592, SD ⫽ 46; control:
combined into a measure of impression of the opponent (␣ ⫽ .79). We also
M ⫽ 569, SD ⫽ 56). Finally, the main effects of the opponent’s
included three items measuring participants’ perceptions of the opponent’s
interpersonal sensitivity and perspective taking (“During the negotiation, emotion and negotiation round were qualified by a significant
the buyer was self-centered,” reverse scored; “During the negotiation, the two-way interaction, F(20, 355) ⫽ 5.21, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .23).
buyer took my interests into consideration”; “During the negotiation, the As can be seen in Figure 1, the influence of the opponent’s
buyer appeared to be preoccupied with him- or herself,” reverse scored). emotion became more apparent as the negotiation progressed. In
These three items were averaged into an index of the opponent’s interper- Round 1, there were no differences among any of the conditions.
sonal sensitivity (␣ ⫽ .82). This is hardly surprising, given that the emotion manipulation
began only after this round. From the second round onward,
Results however, the different conditions started to diverge, the effect
becoming stronger after each consecutive round. Therefore, we
Treatment of the Data decided to use demands in Round 6 for our specific hypothesis
tests.
The offers made by participants in each round were transformed Hypotheses 1a and 1b were tested with planned comparisons.
into an index revealing the negotiator’s total level of demand for The means and standard deviations as well as the specific contrasts
each negotiation round (i.e., the number of points demanded in that
round, summed across the three negotiation issues of price, war-
ranty, and service; see Appendix A).

Manipulation Check
A 5 (opponent’s emotion: disappointment, worry, guilt, regret,
no emotion) ⫻ 4 (perception of the opponent’s emotion: disap-
pointment, worry, guilt, regret) analysis of variance (ANOVA)
with repeated measures on the last factor revealed a significant
interaction between the opponent’s emotion and participants’ per-
ceptions of the opponent’s emotion, F(12, 213) ⫽ 31.77, p ⬍ .01
(partial ␩2 ⫽ .59). Post hoc tests showed that participants in the
disappointed opponent condition rated the opponent as more dis-
appointed (M ⫽ 5.96, SD ⫽ 1.25) than did participants in all the
other conditions (2.84 ⬍ Ms ⬍ 4.95, 0.96 ⬍ SDs ⬍ 1.29).
Similarly, participants with a worried opponent rated the opponent
as more worried (M ⫽ 5.94, SD ⫽ 0.85) than did participants in the
other conditions (2.54 ⬍ Ms ⬍ 4.79, 0.83 ⬍ SDs ⬍ 1.68), and
participants with a guilty opponent rated the opponent as more
guilty (M ⫽ 5.85, SD ⫽ 1.39) than did those in the other conditions
(1.85 ⬍ Ms ⬍ 4.39, 0.82 ⬍ SDs ⬍ 1.85). Finally, participants with
a regretful opponent rated the other as more regretful (M ⫽ 5.03,
SD ⫽ 1.60) than did participants in all the other conditions (1.98 ⬍
Ms ⬍ 2.77, 0.68 ⬍ SDs ⬍ 1.04) except the guilty opponent
condition (M ⫽ 4.81, SD ⫽ 1.72). Furthermore, paired-samples t Figure 1. Demand level as a function of the opponent’s emotion and
tests showed that ratings within each condition were higher for the negotiation round in Experiment 1.
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 129

that were computed to test these hypotheses are shown in Table 1. (Hypothesis 2a), planned comparisons revealed that participants
Four contrasts were computed. First, we tested whether partici- with a guilty or regretful opponent judged the opponent to be more
pants made smaller demands to opponents who experienced sup- interpersonally sensitive (M ⫽ 4.31, SD ⫽ 1.36 and M ⫽ 4.33,
plication emotions (i.e., disappointment and worry) than to non- SD ⫽ 1.39, respectively) than did participants with a worried or
emotional opponents (Contrast 1). Second, we examined whether disappointed opponent (M ⫽ 3.15, SD ⫽ 1.05 and M ⫽ 2.69, SD ⫽
participants responded differentially to disappointed versus wor- 1.14, respectively), t(71) ⫽ 4.37, p ⬍ .002 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .21).
ried counterparts in terms of their demands (Contrast 2). Third, we Participants with a nonemotional opponent occupied an interme-
examined whether participants made larger demands to opponents diate position (M ⫽ 3.86, SD ⫽ 1.15) that differed from the
engaging in appeasement (i.e., guilt or regret) than to nonemo- disappointment and worry conditions, t(71) ⫽ 2.76, p ⬍ .01
tional opponents (Contrast 3). Fourth, we tested whether partici- (partial ␩2 ⫽ .15), but not from the guilt and regret conditions,
pants responded differentially to guilty versus regretful counter- t(71) ⫽ 1.22, ns.
parts (Contrast 4). For Hypotheses 1a and 1b to be supported, We also found a significant effect of emotion on participants’
Contrasts 1 and 3 should be significant (participants should make impressions of the opponent, F(4, 71) ⫽ 4.87, p ⬍ .002 (partial
lower demands to opponents showing signs of supplication and ␩2 ⫽ .22). Consistent with Hypothesis 2b, planned comparisons
higher demands to opponents showing signs of appeasement). showed that participants developed more favorable impressions of
Contrasts 2 and 4 speak to the question of whether the two opponents who experienced guilt or regret (M ⫽ 4.48, SD ⫽ 0.99
supplication emotions (disappointment and worry) have similar and M ⫽ 4.24, SD ⫽ 0.87, respectively) than of opponents who
effects on demands, and whether the two appeasement emotions experienced disappointment or worry (M ⫽ 3.19, SD ⫽ 1.07 and
(guilt and regret) have similar effects on demands. The latter two M ⫽ 3.78, SD ⫽ 0.75, respectively), t(71) ⫽ 3.64, p ⬍ .01 (partial
contrasts were computed for exploratory purposes. ␩2 ⫽ .16). Again, participants with a nonemotional opponent
In accordance with Hypothesis 1a, Contrast 1 was significant. occupied an intermediate position (M ⫽ 4.02, SD ⫽ 0.79), differ-
Participants made smaller demands to a disappointed or worried
ing from the disappointment and worry conditions, t(71) ⫽ 2.14,
opponent than to a nonemotional opponent, t(71) ⫽ 2.89, p ⬍ .01
p ⬍ .04 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .09), but not from the guilt and regret
(partial ␩2 ⫽ .15). Contrast 2 was nonsignificant, indicating that
conditions, t(71) ⫽ 1.28, ns.
participants did not make differential demands to disappointed
To test the idea that participants developed more favorable
versus worried counterparts, t(71) ⬍ 1, ns. In support of Hypoth-
impressions of opponents who engaged in appeasement than of
esis 1b, Contrast 3 was significant—participants made larger de-
opponents who engaged in supplication because they perceived the
mands to a guilty or regretful opponent than to a nonemotional
former to be more interpersonally sensitive than the latter (Hy-
one, t(71) ⫽ 2.10, p ⬍ .05 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .08). Unexpectedly,
pothesis 2c), we conducted mediated regression analyses (see
Contrast 4 was also significant, indicating that participants made
Baron & Kenny, 1986). To this end, the opponent’s emotion was
smaller demands to regretful than to guilty counterparts, t(71) ⫽
dummy coded (0 for worry and disappointment and 1 for guilt and
2.02, p ⬍ .05 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .07). These findings clearly support the
prediction that negotiators concede more to opponents who expe- regret). This dummy variable had a significant effect on partici-
rience supplication emotions than to opponents who experience pants’ impressions of the opponent (the dependent variable; ␤ ⫽
appeasement emotions. Although the supplication emotions disap- .45, p ⬍ .01) and on participants’ judgments of the opponent’s
pointment and worry had similar effects on behavior, the effects of interpersonal sensitivity (the mediator; ␤ ⫽ .51, p ⬍ .01). When
the appeasement emotions guilt and regret differed. both variables were simultaneously entered into the regression, a
significant effect of interpersonal sensitivity on impression
emerged (␤ ⫽ .69, p ⬍ .01), and the originally significant effect of
Ratings of Interpersonal Sensitivity and Impression of the
the dummy variable on impression was reduced to nonsignificance
Opponent
(␤ ⫽ .10, ns). The reduction of the direct path from the opponent’s
An ANOVA showed that participants’ ratings of the opponent’s emotion to participants’ impressions of the opponent was signifi-
interpersonal sensitivity were influenced by the opponent’s emo- cant according to a Sobel test (Z ⫽ 3.74, p ⬍ .01; see Kenny,
tion, F(4, 71) ⫽ 5.62, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .24). As predicted Kashy, & Bolger, 1998, and for an updated version of the formula,

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations (in Parentheses), and Contrasts Computed to Test Hypothesis 1
(Experiment 1)

Condition

Demand and contrast Disappointment Worry Guilt Regret No emotion

Demand in Round 6 435 (63) 422 (85) 573 (100) 527 (59) 492 (70)
Contrast 1* 1 1 0 0 ⫺2
Contrast 2 1 ⫺1 0 0 0
Contrast 3* 0 0 1 1 ⫺2
Contrast 4* 0 0 1 ⫺1 0

* p ⬍ .05.
130 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

see Kenny’s Web site at http://users.rcn.com/dakenny/mediate. ulation of Experiment 1 with a “clean” manipulation that did not
htm). These findings suggest that the effect of the opponent’s include a justification for why the guilt was experienced. Both
emotion on impressions is indeed mediated by judgments of in- guilt conditions were compared with a control condition. We
terpersonal sensitivity, although it should be noted that both con- expected that participants in both guilt conditions would make
structs were measured at the same time. smaller concessions in the course of the negotiation than would
participants in the control condition and that the two guilt condi-
Discussion tions would not differ from each other. Extending the line of
argument presented earlier, we further predicted that participants
The results of Experiment 1 are largely supportive of our pre- in both guilt conditions would expect larger concessions from the
dictions. In line with Hypothesis 1, participants whose opponents opponent than would participants in the control condition (Hypoth-
experienced emotions of supplication (i.e., disappointment or esis 3).
worry) made smaller demands than did participants whose oppo-
nents experienced emotions of appeasement (i.e., guilt or regret). Method
Negotiators who are faced with an opponent who experiences
emotions of appeasement appear to anticipate behavioral repair in Participants and Design
the form of concessions and stand firm. Conversely, negotiators
who deal with a counterpart who experiences emotions of suppli- Eighty-four undergraduate students at the University of Amsterdam
were assigned to one of three conditions: guilt with implicit intention to
cation respond to the other’s discomfort by making concessions.
concede, guilt without implicit intention to concede, or no emotion. In
Thus, supplication and appeasement emotions have unique and return for participation, they received course credit or EUR 5 (approxi-
theoretically meaningful effects on negotiation behavior. Further, mately $6).
and consistent with Hypotheses 2a to 2c, participants whose op-
ponents showed signs of appeasement developed a more positive
impression of the opponent than did those whose opponents en- Procedure
gaged in supplication, because the former were perceived as more The procedure was similar to the one used in Experiment 1, except for
interpersonally sensitive than the latter. This finding is in line with the manipulation of guilt. In the guilt with intention condition, we used the
the presumed relationship-enhancing functions of expressing guilt same guilt statements as in Experiment 1. Thus, after the first negotiation
and regret. round, participants received the following statement from the opponent
One finding of Experiment 1 was unanticipated. On the basis of (see Appendix B): “I feel guilty for not having conceded more, I think I
the literature reviewed in the introduction, we expected guilt and will offer 8 –7–7.” In the guilt without intention condition, we omitted the
explanatory part. Thus, after the first round, participants simply read “I feel
regret to have similar effects on demands. However, the present
guilty, I think I will offer 8 –7–7.” Participants who reached agreement
findings suggest that guilt has a stronger effect on demands. A
before Round 6 (n ⫽ 7) were excluded from the analyses (although
possible explanation could be that expressions of regret are more retaining these participants yielded similar results).
ambiguous. Expressing regret for not having conceded more could
indicate regret for hurting someone else or regret for not having
been more strategic and self-interested. Expressions of guilt are Dependent Measures
more unequivocal in that they necessarily imply that one feels bad In addition to recording demands, we included a brief questionnaire to
about one’s behavior vis-à-vis someone else. measure participants’ expectations of the opponent’s future concessions
Another issue concerns the emotional statements that were used and to check the emotion manipulation. After Round 6, participants read
to manipulate guilt and regret. In previous work (Van Kleef et al., that the negotiation would be temporarily interrupted for some questions
2004a, b), we used rather straightforward expressions to manipu- and that the negotiation would resume later. Expectations regarding the
late anger and happiness (e.g., “This offer makes me really angry/ opponent’s future concessions were then measured by three items (“I
happy”). Although similar expressions could easily be used to expect that the opponent will make large concessions in the next rounds”,
“I expect that the opponent will take a cooperative stance”, “I expect that
express disappointment and worry (e.g., “I am very disap-
the opponent will be conciliatory”; 1 ⫽ totally disagree to 9 ⫽ totally
pointed”), we felt that expressions of guilt and regret would appear
agree; ␣ ⫽ .85). The manipulation of guilt was checked using the same
implausible unless they were accompanied by some kind of justi- scale as in Experiment 1 (␣ ⫽ .89).
fication. We therefore decided to add a brief explanation to the first
guilt and regret statements for why the opponent felt guilty or
regretful (e.g., “I feel guilty for not having conceded more”). Results
However, in addition to making the emotion statement more cred- Manipulation Check
ible, this also inadvertently introduced an implicit intention to
concede. Despite the fact that only the first of three guilt and regret An ANOVA revealed a significant effect of the opponent’s
statements was accompanied by such an implicit intention, we emotion on participants’ perceptions of the opponent’s emotion,
cannot be sure that guilt and regret were driving the effects. This F(2, 74) ⫽ 37.99, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .51). Planned comparisons
issue is dealt with in Experiment 1b. showed that participants in the guilt with intention (M ⫽ 6.36,
SD ⫽ 1.77) and guilt without intention (M ⫽ 6.26, SD ⫽ 2.01)
Experiment 1b conditions rated the other as more guilty than did participants in
the no-emotion condition (M ⫽ 3.02, SD ⫽ 1.18), t(74) ⫽ 8.72,
The goal of Experiment 1b was to identify the unique interper- p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .51). The two guilt conditions did not differ
sonal effects of guilt on demands by contrasting the guilt manip- from each other, t(74) ⫽ .21, ns.
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 131

Demand Level tive expectations of the intentions or behaviors of another” (Rous-


seau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998, p. 395). Among other things,
As in Experiment 1, an ANOVA revealed a significant main level of trust has been shown to affect the exchange of information
effect of round, F(5, 370) ⫽ 128.76, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .76), regarding preferences and priorities in negotiations (Pruitt & Kim-
indicating that demands declined from Round 1 (M ⫽ 641, SD ⫽ mel, 1977), the willingness to cooperate (Yamagishi, 1986), the
72) to Round 6 (M ⫽ 489, SD ⫽ 100). More important, we found attainment of integrative solutions (De Dreu, Giebels, & Van de
a significant effect of the opponent’s emotion on average demands, Vliert, 1998; Kimmel, Pruitt, Magenau, Konar-Goldband, &
F(2, 74) ⫽ 4.53, p ⬍ .015 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .08), indicating that Carnevale, 1980), and the desire for future interaction (Naquin &
participants’ demands were influenced by the opponent’s emotion Paulson, 2003; for a review, see Ross & LaCroix, 1996).
(guilt with intention: M ⫽ 575, SD ⫽ 78; guilt without intention: Aside from its specific effects in the negotiation arena, trust has
M ⫽ 574, SD ⫽ 88; control: M ⫽ 521, SD ⫽ 66). These main a more generalized influence on the trustworthiness individuals
effects were qualified by a significant interaction between emotion ascribe to others and on their willingness to accept information as
and negotiation round, F(10, 370) ⫽ 4.64, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ sincere and accurate (Parks, Henager, & Scamahorn, 1996; Rotter,
.14). As in Experiment 1, the influence of the opponent’s emotion 1980). This has important consequences for negotiators’ reactions
became more apparent as the negotiation progressed. In Round 1, to their opponent’s emotions. Although the effects of trust on
there were no differences among any of the conditions. From the responses to other people’s emotions have not been the explicit
third round onward, the different conditions started to diverge, the focus of research attention, some indirect evidence exists for the
effect becoming stronger after each round. Planned comparisons of moderating role of trust. For instance, research has shown that the
demands in Round 6 revealed that both guilt conditions (guilt with interpersonal consequences of expressions of distress depend on
intention: M ⫽ 524, SD ⫽ 88; guilt without intention: M ⫽ 536, how receivers interpret such expressions (Kennedy-Moore &
SD ⫽ 107) differed significantly from the control condition (M ⫽ Watson, 2001). Sometimes recipients respond to these expressions
430, SD ⫽ 70), t(74) ⫽ 4.93, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .19), and that in less than helpful ways because they misread the expresser’s
they did not differ significantly from each other, t(74) ⫽ .45, ns. intent (L. F. Clark, 1993). When observers perceive that the
expression is untruthful or manipulative, they are less likely to
Expectations of Opponent’s Future Concessions respond positively. On the other hand, when they view the expres-
sions as trustworthy or at least benign, they are more likely to
An ANOVA yielded an effect of emotion on participants’ ex-
respond with social helping (Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 2001). In
pectations of the opponent’s future concessions, F(2, 74) ⫽ 6.35,
other words, individuals’ reactions to others’ emotions are likely to
p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .14). Planned comparisons revealed that
be moderated by interpersonal trust.
participants in both guilt conditions expected more cooperation
Variations in trust may be rooted in individual differences or
from the opponent (guilt with intention: M ⫽ 5.45, SD ⫽ 1.19;
stem from characteristics of the situation (Yamagishi, 1986). The
guilt without intention: M ⫽ 5.10, SD ⫽ 1.28) than did participants
present experiment is concerned with the potential moderating role
in the control condition (M ⫽ 4.28, SD ⫽ 1.31), t(74) ⫽ 3.45, p ⬍
of individual differences in interpersonal trust. The aforemen-
.01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .06). Again, the guilt conditions did not differ
tioned research suggests that a negotiator’s decision to adapt his or
from each other, t(74) ⫽ .82, ns.
her demands to the opponent’s emotions will be influenced by the
focal negotiator’s level of interpersonal trust. Negotiators with
Discussion high levels of trust can be expected to adapt their demands to the
These results show that the interpersonal effects of guilt as opponent’s emotional state, whereas those with low trust may not
observed in Experiment 1 also obtain when no justification for the respond differentially to the other’s emotions. To test this idea, we
experienced guilt is provided or, in other words, no implicit manipulated the opponent’s emotion (guilt vs. disappointment)
intention to concede is mentioned. In line with our theoretical and measured participants’ level of dispositional trust. We pre-
model, participants in both guilt conditions expected the opponent dicted that individuals high in trust would make higher demands to
to make large concessions, supporting Hypothesis 3. Furthermore, a guilty opponent (i.e., an opponent engaging in appeasement) than
participants in both guilt conditions made smaller concessions than to a disappointed opponent (i.e., an opponent engaging in suppli-
did participants in the control condition, providing additional sup- cation). By contrast, we predicted that individuals low in disposi-
port for Hypothesis 1b. tional trust would not respond differentially to their counterpart’s
emotions (Hypothesis 4). We further hypothesized that, compared
with high trust participants, low trust participants would be more
Experiment 2
suspicious about the opponent’s emotion and more likely to dis-
The results thus far support the central proposition of the present count the other’s emotions rather than incorporate them into their
research: In a negotiation, emotions of appeasement elicit high negotiation strategy (Hypothesis 5).
demands from one’s adversary, whereas emotions of supplication The second objective of Experiment 2 was to shed more light on
elicit low demands. The objective of Experiment 2 was twofold. the process underlying the effects of supplication and appeasement
The first objective was to replicate and extend the findings of emotions on demands. According to the social functions perspec-
Experiment 1 by investigating the potential moderating role of tive that was outlined in the introduction, appeasement emotions
interpersonal trust. Trust plays an important role in negotiations, such as guilt signal that behavioral repair can be anticipated,
and it is essential to the resolution of mixed-motive conflict whereas supplication emotions such as disappointment signal that
(Lindskold, 1978). It can be defined as “a psychological state the other needs help. It seems reasonable to assume that such
comprising the intention to accept vulnerability based upon posi- considerations would affect participants’ negotiation objectives
132 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

and strategy. In this context it is useful to consider the role of the ceeded to the negotiation task, which was identical to the one used in
negotiator’s goal (Kelley, Beckman, & Fischer, 1967; Pruitt & Experiment 1.
Carnevale, 1993; Siegel & Fouraker, 1960; Zetik & Stuhlmacher, Dependent measures. In addition to recording demands, we measured
2002). Among other things, higher goals tend to produce larger participants’ goals, their interpretation of the other’s emotions, suspicion
regarding the trustworthiness of the opponent and his or her emotions, and
demands and greater resistance to concession making (Holmes,
discounting of the other’s emotions. We used the same manipulation
Throop, & Strickland, 1971; Kelley et al., 1967; D. L. Smith,
checks as in Experiment 1 (disappointment: ␣ ⫽ .85; guilt: ␣ ⫽ .88).
Pruitt, & Carnevale, 1982; Yukl, 1974a, b). A negotiator who Participants’ goals were measured with six items, two for each issue
expects to receive compensation in the form of large concessions (“On which level of [price/warranty/service] do you strive to reach an
(e.g., in the case of a guilty opponent) can be expected to adopt agreement?” and “On which level of [price/warranty/service] do you hope
higher goals than a negotiator who does not anticipate large to reach an agreement?”). Responses could range from 1 (indicating an
concessions (e.g., in the case of a disappointed opponent). Thus, it extremely low goal) to 9 (indicating an extremely high goal; see Appendix
may be that negotiators concede more to a disappointed opponent A). The items were averaged into a single index of participant’s goal (␣ ⫽
than to a guilty one because the other’s guilt raises their goals, .79).
whereas disappointment lowers them. To examine this possibility, Interpretation of the opponent’s emotion was assessed by two semantic
differentials, both of which were introduced by the question, “How do you
we measured participants’ goals and hypothesized that participants
interpret the emotions the buyer expressed during the negotiation?” The
with a guilty opponent would adopt higher goals than would those
two items participants responded to were “The buyer has received too
with a disappointed opponent (Hypothesis 6). We further predicted much/the buyer has received too little” and “The buyer has offered me too
that negotiators’ tendency to make higher demands to guilty op- much/the buyer has offered me too little” (both measured on a 7-point
ponents than to disappointed ones would be mediated by the scale). Because the correlation between the items was modest (r ⫽ ⫺.39),
ambitiousness of their goals (Hypothesis 7). Finally, we were we analyzed them separately.
interested to see whether guilt and disappointment are indeed Suspicion was measured with three items (“During the negotiation I was
interpreted to mean that someone has received too much versus too suspicious”; “I distrusted the information I received from the buyer”; “The
little, as suggested by the literature reviewed in the introduction. information I received from the buyer made me suspicious”; 1 ⫽ totally
This issue was addressed in an exploratory fashion. disagree, 7 ⫽ totally agree), which were combined into a single index
(␣ ⫽ .79). Discounting of the opponent’s emotion was also measured by
three items (“During the negotiation I did not take the information about
Method the buyer into account”; “During the negotiation, I paid serious attention to
the information I received about the buyer,” reverse scored; “I ignored the
Participants and Experimental Design information I received from the buyer”; 1 ⫽ totally disagree, 7 ⫽ totally
agree), which were also averaged into a single index (␣ ⫽ .67).
Participants were 154 undergraduate students at the University of Am-
sterdam. They took part in the experiment for course credit or for monetary
Results
compensation (EUR 7, approximately U.S.$8). The experimental design
included the opponent’s emotion (disappointment vs. guilt) and the partic- Manipulation Check
ipant’s dispositional trust (high vs. low) as the independent variables and
demand level as the main dependent variable. Participants were randomly A 2 (emotion of the opponent: disappointment vs. guilt) ⫻ 2
assigned to the conditions with the use of a double-blind procedure. (dispositional trust: high vs. low) ⫻ 2 (perception of the oppo-
nent’s emotion: disappointed vs. guilty) ANOVA with repeated
Procedure measures on the last factor revealed a significant interaction be-
tween the opponent’s emotion and participants’ perceptions of the
The procedure was identical to the one used in Experiment 1, with one opponent’s emotion, F(1, 144) ⫽ 300.54, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽
major exception. In addition to manipulating the opponent’s emotion, we .67). Participants with a disappointed opponent rated the opponent
also measured the participant’s level of dispositional trust (see below). As as more disappointed (M ⫽ 5.25, SD ⫽ 1.42) than did those with
in Experiment 1, participants who reached agreement before Round 6 (n ⫽ a guilty opponent (M ⫽ 3.29, SD ⫽ 1.16). Similarly, participants
6) were excluded from the analyses (although retaining these participants
with a guilty opponent rated the other as more guilty (M ⫽ 4.56,
yielded a similar pattern of results).
SD ⫽ 1.54) than did those with a disappointed opponent (M ⫽
Assessment of trust. Trust was assessed with Yamagishi’s (1986) trust
scale, which comprises the following five items: “Most people tell a lie if 2.21, SD ⫽ 0.76). Paired-sample t tests showed that participants in
they can benefit from doing so”, “Those devoted to unselfish causes are the disappointed-opponent condition rated the opponent as more
often exploited by others”, “Many people do not cooperate because they disappointed than guilty (M ⫽ 5.25 vs. M ⫽ 2.21), t(72) ⫽ 15.84,
pursue only their own interests”, “Most people are basically honest”, and p ⬍ .01, and that participants in the guilty-opponent condition
“There will be more people who will not work if the social security system rated the opponent as more guilty than disappointed (M ⫽ 4.56 vs.
is developed further” (1 ⫽ totally disagree to 7 ⫽ totally agree). The M ⫽ 3.29), t(74) ⫽ 7.79, p ⬍ .01. There were no effects of trust.
internal consistency of the scale was good (␣ ⫽ .77). Following past
research (Parks & Hulbert, 1995), a median split (Mdn ⫽ 4.63) was
performed to classify participants as high or low in trust, yielding 35 to 40 Demand Level
participants per condition. Ratings on the trust scale did not differ across
emotion conditions (disappointment: M ⫽ 4.68, SD ⫽ 0.61; guilt: M ⫽ Demands in Rounds 1 to 6 were submitted to a 2 (opponent’s
4.65, SD ⫽ 0.63), F(1, 146) ⬍ 1, ns. After the assessment of trust, emotion: disappointment vs. guilt) ⫻ 2 (participant’s trust: high vs.
participants completed a 10-min filler task (consisting of a number of low) mixed-model ANOVA with the opponent’s emotion and the
unrelated scales) to reduce the likelihood of carryover effects from the trust participant’s trust as between-participants variables and demands
assessment to the negotiation task. After the filler task, participants pro- in Rounds 1 to 6 as a repeated-measures variable. As in Experi-
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 133

ment 1, we first describe lower order effects, and we then turn to emotion. Participants who negotiated with a guilty opponent in-
the hypothesized interaction. The anticipated effect of round was terpreted the other’s emotion as signifying that the other had
significant, F(5, 720) ⫽ 413.82, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .74), obtained too much (M ⫽ 3.71, SD ⫽ 0.97), whereas those who
showing that demands declined from Round 1 (M ⫽ 643, SD ⫽ dealt with a disappointed opponent took the other’s emotion to
77) to Round 6 (M ⫽ 466, SD ⫽ 91). Results further revealed a mean that the other had received too little (M ⫽ 4.47, SD ⫽ 1.17),
main effect of the opponent’s emotion on average demand, F(1, F(1, 144) ⫽ 19.33, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .11). Conversely,
144) ⫽ 10.30, p ⬍ .002 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .07), indicating that partic- participants with a guilty opponent interpreted the other’s emotion
ipants’ demands were influenced by their adversaries’ emotions as a signal that the other had offered too little (M ⫽ 4.88, SD ⫽
(disappointment: M ⫽ 521, SD ⫽ 75; guilt: M ⫽ 560, SD ⫽ 84). 1.09), whereas those with a disappointed partner were more likely
These effects were qualified by an Emotion ⫻ Round interaction, to believe that the other’s emotion revealed that he or she had
F(5, 720) ⫽ 8.48, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .06), which showed that offered too much (M ⫽ 4.49, SD ⫽ 1.04), F(1, 144) ⫽ 5.28, p ⬍
the effect of the opponent’s emotion on participants’ demands .025 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .04). There was no significant effect of trust and
increased from Round 1 (disappointment: M ⫽ 641, SD ⫽ 77; no interaction.
guilt: M ⫽ 646, SD ⫽ 78) to Round 6 (disappointment: M ⫽ 439,
SD ⫽ 80; guilt: M ⫽ 491, SD ⫽ 93).
In line with Hypothesis 4, there was a significant interaction Suspicion and Discounting of the Opponent’s Emotions
between opponent’s emotion and participant’s trust, F(1, 144) ⫽ According to Hypothesis 5, low trusters would be more suspi-
7.67, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .05). As predicted, simple effects cious and more likely than high trusters to discount their counter-
analysis revealed that high trust participants made higher demands part’s emotion. In line with this prediction, an ANOVA showed a
to a guilty opponent (M ⫽ 586, SD ⫽ 66) than to a disappointed tendency for participants low in trust to be more suspicious (M ⫽
one (M ⫽ 510, SD ⫽ 66), F(1, 144) ⫽ 16.96, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ 3.92, SD ⫽ 1.20) regarding the opponent’s emotions than were
.26), whereas those low in trust did not respond differentially to participants high in trust (M ⫽ 3.50, SD ⫽ 1.31), F(1, 144) ⫽ 2.79,
their counterpart’s emotions (guilt: M ⫽ 537, SD ⫽ 92; disap- p ⬍ .10 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .02). Low trusters were also more likely to
pointment: M ⫽ 531, SD ⫽ 82), F(1, 144) ⬍ 1, ns. This interaction discount the opponent’s emotion than were high trusters (M ⫽
was qualified by a significant three-way interaction between emo- 3.14, SD ⫽ 1.23 vs. M ⫽ 2.71, SD ⫽ 0.99), F(1, 144) ⫽ 5.14, p ⬍
tion, trust, and round, F(5, 720) ⫽ 2.59, p ⬍ .03 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .08), .025 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .04). There were no effects of emotion and no
indicating that the interactive effects of emotion and trust became interaction (Fs ⬍ 1, ns).
more apparent as the negotiation progressed (see Figure 2). Simple
effects analysis revealed a significant Emotion ⫻ Round interac-
tion for high trusters, F(5, 720) ⫽ 8.07, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .16), Participants’ Goals
but not for low trusters, F(5, 720) ⫽ 1.74, ns. As can be seen from
Figure 2, participants with high levels of trust made larger con- In line with Hypothesis 6, participants’ goals were influenced by
cessions in the course of the negotiation when the opponent the opponent’s emotion. Participants who dealt with a guilty op-
experienced disappointment than when the opponent experienced ponent reported higher goals (M ⫽ 5.04, SD ⫽ 0.79) than did those
guilt, whereas participants with low levels of trust were not re- who dealt with a disappointed opponent (M ⫽ 5.38, SD ⫽ 0.79),
sponsive to the opponent’s emotional state. F(1, 144) ⫽ 7.73, p ⬍ .01, partial ␩2 ⫽ .05 (recall that low
numbers correspond to high goals; see Appendix A). This effect
was moderated by the participant’s level of trust, F(1, 144) ⫽ 4.63,
Interpretation of the Opponent’s Emotions
p ⬍ .04 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .03). Means and standard deviations per-
ANOVAs yielded significant main effects of emotion on the two taining to this interaction are shown in Table 2. Simple effects
items tapping the participant’s interpretation of the opponent’s analyses revealed that high trusters adapted their goals to the

Figure 2. Demand level as a function of the opponent’s emotion, participants’ trust, and negotiation round in
Experiment 2.
134 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

Table 2
Participants’ Goals and Average Demands as a Function of the Opponent’s Emotion and the
Participant’s Trust (Experiment 2)

Participant’s trust

Low High

Condition Disappointment Guilt Disappointment Guilt

Goal 4.88a (0.86) 4.81a (0.70) 5.07a (0.67) 4.45b (0.82)


Demand 531a (82) 537a (92) 510a (66) 586b (66)

Note. Participants’ goals ranged from 1 (indicating a high goal) to 9 (indicating a low goal). Demand refers to
participants’ average demands in Rounds 1 to 6. Standard deviations are in parentheses. Means within the low
and high trust conditions with a different subscript differ at p ⬍ .05 according to simple-effects analyses.

opponent’s emotion, F(1, 144) ⫽ 11.54, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .15), sheds some light on the processes underlying the effects of guilt
whereas low trusters did not, F(1, 144) ⬍ 1, ns. and disappointment on demands. A mediation analysis revealed
that negotiators with high levels of trust made smaller demands to
Mediation Analysis a disappointed opponent than to a guilty one because the other’s
disappointment led them to lower their goals, whereas the other’s
To investigate whether the interactive effects of the opponent’s guilt led them to raise their goals. Finally, results pertaining to
emotion and the participant’s trust on demands were mediated by participants’ interpretation of their counterparts’ emotions are
the participant’s goals (as predicted in Hypothesis 7), we per- compatible with the framework that was outlined in the introduc-
formed mediated regression analyses, following Baron and Ken- tion: Guilt is interpreted to mean that the other has claimed too
ny’s (1986) procedure. In Step 1 we entered emotion, trust, and much, whereas disappointment is taken as a signal that the other
their interaction as the independent variables and demands as the has received too little.
dependent variable. This produced a significant main effect of The fact that trust was measured rather than manipulated might
emotion (␤ ⫽ .24, p ⬍ .003) and an interaction between emotion be regarded as a limitation of Experiment 2. Although the results
and trust (␤ ⫽ .22, p ⬍ .01). In Step 2, we used the same are consistent with our predictions, we cannot rule out the possi-
independent variables to predict the participant’s goals. This, too, bility that they are caused by some unknown third variable that
yielded a significant main effect of emotion (␤ ⫽ .21, p ⬍ .01) and was not taken into account. Another shortcoming of Experiment 2
a significant interaction (␤ ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .04). In Step 3, we is that there was no nonemotional control condition. Therefore, we
simultaneously entered emotion and goals to predict demands. cannot draw firm conclusions as to whether disappointment led
This yielded a significant effect of goals on demands (␤ ⫽ .61, p ⬍ participants to place lower demands, whether guilt led them to
.01) and reduced the formerly significant Emotion ⫻ Trust inter- place higher demands, or both. Although Experiment 1 demon-
action to nonsignificance (␤ ⫽ .11, ns). A Sobel test indicated that strated that guilt and disappointment elicited higher and lower
the reduction of the direct path from the Emotion ⫻ Trust inter- demands, respectively, compared with a nonemotional control
action to demands was significant (Z ⫽ 2.10, p ⬍ .04; see Kenny condition, we felt that a replication of this effect would increase
et al., 1998). In support of Hypothesis 7, these results show that the confidence in its robustness. We therefore conducted a third ex-
interactive effect of the opponent’s emotion and the participant’s periment in which trust was manipulated (through expectations
trust on demands is fully mediated by the participant’s goals. about the opponent’s personality) and a control condition was
included.
Discussion
The results of Experiment 2 corroborate our hypotheses. As Experiment 3
predicted, negotiators’ reactions to their opponent’s emotions are
moderated by trust. Negotiators with high levels of dispositional Experiment 2 showed that individual differences in dispositional
trust responded with high demands to an opponent who appeared trust moderate the interpersonal effects of supplication and ap-
to experience guilt and with low demands to an opponent who peasement emotions in negotiations. As noted earlier, variations in
appeared to experience disappointment. By contrast, negotiators trust may also arise from features of the situation and/or the other
with low trust did not respond differentially to the opponent’s party (Yamagishi, 1986). Among other things, trust depends on a
emotions. These findings replicate and extend those of Experiment negotiator’s expectations about the other’s cooperation or compe-
1 by showing that negotiators are more likely to act on their tition. Indeed, trust has been defined as the expectation that the
opponent’s emotions to the extent that they trust the opponent and other will cooperate (Pruitt & Kimmel, 1977). Because trusting
see his or her emotions as trustworthy and reliable. We further another person is more risky to the extent that he or she can be
found that individuals with low trust were more suspicious regard- expected to take advantage of and exploit the trust (Ross &
ing the trustworthiness of the other’s emotions and were more LaCroix, 1996), people exhibit greater levels of trust in relation to
likely to discount them rather than take them into account when others whom they expect to be cooperative than others whom they
designing their negotiation strategy. Furthermore, Experiment 2 expect to be competitive (De Cremer, Snyder, & Dewitte, 2001;
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 135

De Dreu et al., 1998; Deutsch, 1960; Kee & Knox, 1970; Loomis, verse scored). These items were averaged into a composite index of
1959; Pruitt & Kimmel, 1977; Rempel, Holmes, & Zanna, 1985; judgment of the opponent’s cooperativeness versus competitiveness, rang-
for an overview of bases of trust in negotiation, see Ross & ing from 1 ⫽ cooperative to 7 ⫽ competitive (␣ ⫽ .90). Additionally, we
LaCroix, 1996). On the basis of this research and the results of assessed participants’ perceptions of the opponent’s trustworthiness using
three items (“The buyer is very trustworthy–not very trustworthy,” reverse
Experiment 2, we predicted that negotiators would make higher
scored; “The buyer is very unreliable–very reliable”; “The buyer is very
demands to a guilty opponent than to a disappointed one, but only
honest–very dishonest”). These items were combined into an index of the
if they believed that the opponent was cooperative. If the opponent opponent’s trustworthiness, ranging from 1 ⫽ not very trustworthy to 7 ⫽
was expected to be competitive, the trustworthiness of his or her very trustworthy (␣ ⫽ .84).
emotions should be decreased, and hence negotiators should not
adapt their demands to the other’s emotion (Hypothesis 8).
Results
Method Manipulation Checks
Participants and Experimental Design Opponent’s emotion. As in the previous experiments, an
ANOVA showed the predicted interaction between the opponent’s
Ninety-four male and female undergraduate students at the University of
Amsterdam participated for course credit or monetary compensation (EUR emotion and the participants’ perception of the opponent’s emo-
7, approximately US $8). The experimental design included the opponent’s tion, F(2, 86) ⫽ 168.30, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .80). Participants in
emotion (disappointment vs. guilt vs. no emotion) and the opponent’s the disappointed opponent condition rated the opponent as signif-
personality (cooperative vs. competitive) as the independent variables and icantly more disappointed (M ⫽ 6.00, SD ⫽ 0.93) than did
demand level as the main dependent variable. Participants were randomly participants in the guilty or nonemotional opponent conditions
assigned to conditions, and the experimenters were blind to this (M ⫽ 3.91, SD ⫽ 1.16; M ⫽ 2.81, SD ⫽ 1.00). Similarly,
assignment. participants with a guilty opponent rated the opponent as more
guilty (M ⫽ 5.67, SD ⫽ 0.85) than did participants in the other two
Procedure conditions (disappointment: M ⫽ 2.22, SD ⫽ 1.10; no emotion:
M ⫽ 2.57, SD ⫽ 0.89). Furthermore, paired-samples t tests showed
The procedure was the same as in Experiment 2, except that trust was
manipulated rather than measured. As in the preceding experiments, par-
that ratings within the emotion conditions were higher for the
ticipants who reached agreement before Round 6 (n ⫽ 2) were excluded intended emotion than for the other emotion (8.13 ⬍ ts ⬍ 15.82;
from the analyses (although including these participants in the analyses both ps ⬍ .01). The ratings did not differ in the control condition.
yielded a similar pattern of findings). Opponent’s cooperation/competition and trustworthiness. An
Manipulation of trust. Trust was manipulated by varying participants’ ANOVA revealed a significant effect of the manipulation of the
expectations regarding the opponent’s cooperation versus competition with opponent’s orientation on participants’ judgments of the oppo-
a procedure that has been used successfully in past research (see De Dreu nent’s cooperativeness versus competitiveness. Participants in the
& Van Kleef, 2004; Steinel & De Dreu, 2004; Van Kleef & De Dreu, cooperative opponent conditions rated the opponent as signifi-
2002). At the beginning of the experiment, participants were asked to cantly more cooperative (M ⫽ 3.67, SD ⫽ 1.08) than did those in
complete a (fake) “Personality Test.” This questionnaire was described as
the competitive opponent conditions (M ⫽ 4.77, SD ⫽ 1.29), F(1,
measuring collaboration skills and contained 20 items having to do with
cooperation in daily life (e.g., “In the bus, I vacate my seat for older
86) ⫽ 19.63, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .19). There was no main effect
people”; “I enjoy working with other people”; “Love and respect are more of the opponent’s emotion and no interaction. We also obtained a
important than status and money”; “Winning is everything”; “I like situa- significant main effect of the opponent’s orientation on percep-
tions in which it is me against someone else”). Participants were asked to tions of the opponent’s trustworthiness, F(1, 86) ⫽ 18.69, p ⬍ .01
indicate their agreement on 5-point Likert scales (1 ⫽ strongly disagree to (partial ␩2 ⫽ .18). Participants who learned that the opponent had
5 ⫽ strongly agree). a cooperative orientation judged the other as more trustworthy
After completion of this personality test, participants received the same (M ⫽ 5.14, SD ⫽ 0.98) than did those who learned that the other
instructions as in Experiments 1 and 2. They then learned that on the basis had a competitive orientation (M ⫽ 4.17, SD ⫽ 1.23).
of the personality test, each participant had been classified as either
“cooperative” or “competitive” and that some participants would receive
this information about their opponent, whereas others would not. Next, Demand Level
participants read that the computer had selected them to receive their
opponent’s personality information, but that the opponent would not re- Demands were submitted to a 3 (opponent’s emotion: disap-
ceive their information. In the cooperative opponent condition, the out- pointment vs. guilt vs. no emotion) ⫻ 2 (opponent’s personality:
come of the opponent’s personality test was presented on the screen with cooperative vs. competitive) mixed-model ANOVA with the op-
answers allegedly given by the opponent suggesting that he or she was very ponent’s emotion and personality as between-participants vari-
cooperative. These answers were accompanied by a “general test result,” ables and demands in Rounds 1 to 6 as a repeated-measures
which indicated that the other could best be classified as cooperative. In the variable. A main effect of round indicated that participants’ de-
competitive opponent condition, participants were shown answers suggest- mands declined over time (Round 1: M ⫽ 643, SD ⫽ 89; Round
ing that the opponent was very competitive, and the general test result
6: M ⫽ 457, SD ⫽ 103), F(5, 430) ⫽ 143.22, p ⬍ .01 (partial ␩2 ⫽
showed that the other could best be classified as competitive.
Dependent measures. The emotion check was the same as in Experi- .63). Furthermore, we found a main effect of emotion on average
ment 2. To check participants’ expectations regarding the opponent’s demands, F(2, 86) ⫽ 4.41, p ⬍ .02 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .10). Planned
cooperative versus competitive orientation, we asked participants to rate comparison results were consistent with those of the previous
the opponent’s personality on five 7-point semantic differential scales (e.g., experiments, indicating that participants with a disappointed op-
“cooperative– competitive,” “compliant– bossy,” “dominant– docile,” re- ponent made lower demands (M ⫽ 505, SD ⫽ 88) than did
136 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

participants with a guilty opponent (M ⫽ 556, SD ⫽ 94), t(89) ⫽ (i.e., low trust) did not respond differentially to the other’s emo-
1.88, p ⬍ .05 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .05). Participants with a nonemotional tions. Thus, the results of this experiment are consistent with the
opponent occupied an intermediate position (M ⫽ 541, SD ⫽ 70) findings reported in Experiment 2, and this suggests that trust is an
that did not differ significantly from the other two conditions (ts ⬍ important prerequisite for the interpersonal effects of disappoint-
1.50, ns). ment and guilt to obtain. Further, the inclusion of a nonemotional
We also found a significant interaction between the opponent’s control condition allows for specific conclusions regarding the
emotion and the opponent’s orientation, F(2, 86) ⫽ 4.74, p ⬍ .01 respective effects of disappointment and guilt. In line with the
(partial ␩2 ⫽ .10). In line with our expectations, simple effects results of Experiment 1, the present data show that participants
analysis revealed a significant effect of the opponent’s emotion in who expected cooperation made significantly smaller demands to
the cooperative opponent condition, indicating that average de- a disappointed opponent than to a nonemotional one, whereas they
mands were lower for participants who negotiated with a disap- made significantly larger demands to a guilty counterpart.
pointed opponent (M ⫽ 475, SD ⫽ 52) than for those who dealt
with a guilty or nonemotional opponent (M ⫽ 603, SD ⫽ 81 and General Discussion
M ⫽ 558, SD ⫽ 77, respectively), F(2, 86) ⫽ 7.73, p ⬍ .01 (partial
␩2 ⫽ .37). In the competitive opponent condition, by contrast, The results of the present experiments support our predictions.
there was no effect of the opponent’s emotion on demands (523 ⬍ Negotiators whose opponents appeared to experience emotions of
M ⬍ 525), F(2, 86) ⫽ 0.01, ns. appeasement (i.e., guilt or regret) developed a positive impression
Finally, we found a significant three-way interaction between of their opponents, but they were nonconciliatory in the level of
opponent’s emotion, opponent’s orientation, and negotiation their demands. By contrast, participants whose opponents experi-
round, F(10, 430) ⫽ 3.84, p ⬍ .025, partial ␩2 ⫽ .08 (see Figure enced emotions of supplication (i.e., disappointment or worry)
3). Results of simple effects analysis were consistent with Hypoth- rated their opponents less positively, but they made larger conces-
esis 8, revealing a significant Emotion ⫻ Round interaction in the sions in the course of the negotiation (Experiment 1). Experiment
cooperative opponent condition, F(10, 430) ⫽ 5.62, p ⬍ .01 1b showed that negotiators with a guilty opponent expected to
(partial ␩2 ⫽ .22), but not in the competitive opponent condition, receive larger concessions from the other than did those with a
F(10, 430) ⫽ .31, ns. Planned comparisons of demands in Round nonemotional opponent. Also in line with our theoretical frame-
6 revealed that participants with a cooperative opponent made work were results showing that the interpersonal effects of guilt
higher demands when the opponent appeared to feel guilty than and disappointment on demands were mediated by the focal ne-
when the opponent expressed no emotion (M ⫽ 553, SD ⫽ 130 vs. gotiator’s goals. Negotiators with a disappointed opponent lowered
M ⫽ 472, SD ⫽ 69), t(39) ⫽ 2.24, p ⬍ .03 (partial ␩2 ⫽ .06), and their goals and made smaller demands, whereas those with a guilty
that they made lower demands when the other appeared to be opponent raised their goals and made larger demands. The data
disappointed (M ⫽ 393, SD ⫽ 59), t(39) ⫽ 3.06, p ⬍ .01 (partial further showed that participants with a guilty opponent believed
␩2 ⫽ .08). There were no differences in the competitive opponent that the other had claimed too much and offered too little, whereas
condition (439 ⬍ Ms ⬍ 449), ts ⬍ 1.0, ns. those with a disappointed counterpart believed that the other had
received too little and offered too much. This, too, is consistent
with our theoretical framework. Apparently, emotions of suppli-
Discussion
cation signal that one is in need of compensation, which may lead
The results of Experiment 3 replicate those of Experiment 2 others to lower their goals and make concessions. Conversely,
using a situational manipulation of trust. Participants who thought emotions of appeasement appear to signal that one is willing to
that the opponent had a cooperative orientation (i.e., high trust) compensate one’s counterpart, which may lead others to increase
conceded more to a disappointed opponent than to a nonemotional their goals and stand firm.
one, and they tended to concede less to a guilty opponent. By The findings of Experiments 1 and 2 point to the pervasive
contrast, participants who believed the other to be competitive effects of emotions related to supplication and appeasement on

Figure 3. Demand level as a function of the opponent’s emotion, the opponent’s orientation, and negotiation
round in Experiment 3.
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 137

negotiation behavior and impressions, and the results of these functions and consequences (Frijda & Mesquita, 1994; Keltner &
experiments shed some light on the processes underlying these Haidt, 1999; Morris & Keltner, 2000). This conclusion points to
effects. In addition, the present research has identified an impor- the need for more research on the effects of discrete emotions
tant moderator of the interpersonal effects of appeasement and rather than nonspecific positive versus negative affect.
supplication emotions on demands and concessions: interpersonal Third, the present findings contribute to a more thorough un-
trust. Experiment 2 showed that only negotiators with high levels derstanding of the social consequences of expressing guilt, regret,
of trust adapted their goals and demands to their opponent’s disappointment, and worry. Although all of these emotions have
emotion. Participants with low trust reported more suspicion re- been the subject of research, most of this research has focused on
garding the opponent’s emotions, were more likely to discount the intrapersonal effects of these emotions on the individual’s
them, and did not adapt their goals and demands to the other’s cognitions and behavior. For example, research on guilt has ad-
emotion. In Experiment 3, trust was manipulated by varying par- dressed the question of how the experience of guilt influences the
ticipants’ expectations regarding the opponent’s cooperative ver- guilty party’s behavior and thereby his or her relationship to the
sus competitive orientation, and a similar effect was found. Par- interaction partner. Thus it has been shown that guilt motivates
ticipants who expected a cooperative opponent had high trust and people to make apologies and amends and to compensate the other
made larger concessions to a disappointed opponent and smaller for one’s transgression (see Baumeister et al., 1994, for an over-
concessions to a guilty opponent. By contrast, participants who view of this research). Although this research contains an inter-
thought that the opponent had a competitive orientation had low personal component (guilt may affect interpersonal relations), the
trust and did not respond differentially to the opponent’s disap- major focus has been on the impact of guilt on the individual’s
pointment versus guilt in terms of their demands and concessions. own motivations and behavior. Our findings move beyond the
Together, the results of these experiments strongly support the intrapersonal effects of guilt by showing that interaction partners
idea that emotions of appeasement and supplication have the anticipate compensation from the guilty party by making high
potential to influence negotiation behavior at the interpersonal demands and small concessions. In a similar vein, previous re-
level. This conclusion has interesting implications for research on search on regret has established that regret instigates a desire to
conflict and negotiation and for our understanding of the social undo one’s actions (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994, 1995; Zeelenberg
consequences of emotions. Below we discuss these implications as
et al., 2000). The present research suggests that regret may not
well as address some of the strengths and limitations of our
only affect one’s own behavior but also that of others, although the
approach. We conclude by outlining some avenues for future
effects of regret appear to be weaker than those of guilt. A possible
research.
explanation for this lies in the ambiguous nature of regret. An
opponent who regrets his or her behavior may do so because he or
Implications and Contributions she has harmed the other or because he or she should have been
more strategic and self-interested.
In exploring the interpersonal effects of appeasement and sup-
The present findings also increase our understanding of the
plication emotions in negotiations, the present work makes a
interpersonal effects of worry and disappointment. As is the case
number of important contributions. First, prior research on emo-
with guilt and regret, most previous research on disappointment
tions in negotiation has focused almost exclusively on the role of
general positive versus negative affect or, in a few cases, discrete has adopted an intrapersonal approach, investigating for instance
emotions such as anger, happiness, and compassion (Allred et al., how the experience of disappointment motivates people to mini-
1997; Van Kleef et al., 2004a, b). The present research extends this mize future disappointment (see Zeelenberg et al., 2000, for an
line of inquiry by examining the effects of guilt, regret, disappoint- overview). Our research shows that disappointment can also in-
ment, and worry. Our findings demonstrate that these emotions, fluence behavior at the interpersonal level—it appears to be effec-
like anger and happiness, can have a powerful impact on negoti- tive in eliciting concessions. The same holds for expressions of
ation behavior. This underscores the recent acknowledgment of the worry. These findings are consistent with prior research on
importance of considering affective phenomena in conflict and distress-related emotions such as sadness, which have been shown
negotiation and indicates that scholarly attention should not remain to facilitate prosocial behavior (Barnett et al., 1979; Batson, 1987;
limited to the role of anger and happiness. Eisenberg, Fabes, Miller, et al., 1989; Fabes et al., 1994; Morris &
Second, the current research indicates that the role of emotion in Keltner, 2000). Apparently, various emotions related to distress
negotiation cannot be understood by merely classifying emotions and supplication have broadly comparable effects on others’
as positive or negative. Previous research has explored the inter- behavior.
personal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations, demon- Altogether, it appears that discrete emotions have distinct and
strating that negotiators concede more to angry opponents than to predictable effects in negotiations, which can be conceptualized
happy ones (Van Kleef et al., 2004a, b). Although it is tempting to and understood in terms of the information they provide. For
explain this finding in terms of a positivity–negativity dimension, example, guilt (and to a lesser degree regret) informs the adversary
the present results suggest that it would be unwise to do so. The that one has taken too much, and it signals that one is willing to
emotions that were investigated in this research— guilt, regret, compensate for this. Disappointment and worry, on the other hand,
worry, and disappointment—are all negative in valence, yet they inform the other that one has received less than expected and
have quite different effects on behavior: The effects of guilt and signal that one is in need of compensation. Because the informa-
regret were opposite to those of worry and disappointment. A more tion conveyed by these and other emotions is similar across situ-
fruitful approach, then, is to adopt a social–functional perspective ations, we believe that parallel effects are to be expected in other
on emotion, which assumes that emotions have distinct social domains of social interaction.
138 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

The identification of trust as a moderator of the interpersonal carefully controlled manipulation of the opponent’s emotion. As a
effects of supplication and appeasement emotions on negotiation result, some caution is needed when generalizing the results. At the
behavior constitutes a fourth contribution of the present work. Two very least, our findings pertain to computer-mediated negotiations.
different operationalizations of trust were used. In Experiment 2 Given the pervasiveness of negotiation as a form of social inter-
we measured individual differences in trust; in Experiment 3 we action and the increasing popularity of information technologies as
manipulated trust by providing information about the opponent’s a communication medium, the question of how individuals react to
personality. Although both methods have been shown in prior each other’s emotions in computer-mediated communication is
research to constitute valid operationalizations of trust (De Dreu & itself of great theoretical and practical importance (McGrath &
Van Kleef, 2004; Steinel & De Dreu, 2004; Yamagishi, 1986), Hollingshead, 1994; McKersie & Fonstad, 1997; Moore,
they differ in a number of respects. Measures of dispositional trust Kurtzberg, Thompson, & Morris, 1999). However, considering
tap directly into the generalized tendency of individuals to trust that our paradigm has previously yielded results that have also
others, to believe in their honesty, and to accept what they say and been found in face-to-face settings (Sinaceur & Tiedens, in press)
do as true and righteous (or not). By contrast, the method that was we have no reason to suspect that our findings are restricted to the
used in Experiment 3 varied levels of state trust by manipulating domain of computer-mediated interaction. Future research could
characteristics of the opponent, rather than assessing a character- shed more light on this issue by investigating the extent to which
istic of the focal individual. Despite these differences, the two the interpersonal effects of emotions relating to supplication and
methods similarly affected participants’ perceptions of the trust- appeasement generalize across settings.
worthiness of the opponent and of his or her emotional expres- Another issue concerns the “cognitive” nature of the emotion
sions, and both meaningfully moderated the interpersonal effects manipulation that was used in the present experiments. The fact
of the other’s emotions. that we used verbal manipulations of emotion raises the question
Previous research has demonstrated the important role of infor- of whether our findings generalize to settings in which emotions
mation processing motivation in determining the interpersonal are communicated in a different manner (e.g., nonverbally). One
effects of anger and happiness on demands and concessions (Van could argue that the effects would be different if people were
Kleef et al., 2004b). Negotiators concede more to angry opponents presented with behavioral rather than cognitive emotion cues. This
than to happy ones, but only if they are sufficiently motivated to possibility cannot be ruled out on the basis of the present data.
consider the emotions of the other and to think about their impli- However, previous research on anger and happiness in negotia-
cations for their own goal attainment. The present research shows tions has documented similar effects regardless of whether a verbal
that trust has a similar moderating effect on the interpersonal (Van Kleef et al., 2004a, b) or nonverbal (Sinaceur & Tiedens, in
effects of supplication and appeasement emotions. Negotiators press) manipulation was used. We therefore have no reason to
tend to give in when the opponent experiences emotions of sup- doubt the generalizability of our findings. However, future re-
plication, but they stand firm when the opponent experiences search is needed to explore this issue in greater depth.
emotions of appeasement. However, this holds only for negotiators A final issue concerns the way in which the information about
with high levels of trust; those with low trust do not adapt their the opponent’s emotions and personality was presented. We told
demands to the other’s emotion. participants that the computer had randomly determined that they
The current findings point to an interesting dilemma facing would receive information about the intentions or personality of
negotiators who anticipate future interaction. Expressions of dis- their counterpart, but that the other would not receive such infor-
appointment or worry (supplication) can elicit concessions from mation about them. A possible downside of this procedure is that
others, but they may also contribute to a negative impression. By it created an asymmetric situation in that participants believed that
contrast, expressions of guilt and regret (appeasement) may serve they had more information about the opponent than vice versa. For
to engender a more positive impression, but they may lead others one thing, knowledge of this fact may have given participants a
to stand firm and resist concession making. Thus, on the one hand, sense of power over the opponent. Given that previous research
negotiators may be motivated to express guilt or regret strategi- has found that high-power negotiators are less susceptible to their
cally to make a good impression and to induce or maintain a counterpart’s emotions than are low-power negotiators (Sinaceur
positive interpersonal relationship, but this would be at the expense & Tiedens, in press; Van Kleef, De Dreu, Pietroni, & Manstead, in
of their personal negotiation outcomes. On the other hand, they press), this asymmetry is likely to have contributed to a more
may choose to display disappointment or worry to get their oppo- conservative test of our hypotheses. However, we cannot rule out
nents to comply with their wishes, thereby inadvertently spoiling the possibility that the informational asymmetry may have influ-
the interpersonal climate. enced our findings in one way or another. Future research might
explore possible differences in responses to the other’s emotions as
Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research a function of the distribution of information.
After a decade of research on the intrapersonal effects of moods
There are some limitations to our findings. First, there was no and, occasionally, of emotions on the negotiator’s cognition and
face-to-face interaction. The primary purpose of this research was behavior, there has been a recent upsurge of interest in the social
to enhance our knowledge and understanding of the interpersonal effects of discrete emotions in negotiations. This research has
effects of discrete emotions in negotiation by generating and begun to document the interpersonal effects of emotions relevant
testing new hypotheses about the effects of supplication and ap- to negotiation and conflict resolution. Although the results are
peasement emotions. In doing this, we made an explicit decision to promising, many questions remain unanswered. How does the
maintain as much experimental control as possible, and we chose expectation of future interaction with the same partner influence
to use a computer-mediated negotiation paradigm to permit a the interpersonal effects of different emotions on negotiation be-
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 139

havior? Are there other variables, besides trust and information settled: The role of emotion in negotiation. In M. J. Gelfand & J. M.
processing, that moderate these effects? What happens when more Brett (Eds.), The handbook of negotiation and culture (pp. 71–94).
than one negotiator expresses certain emotions? What factors Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
determine whose emotions will have the bigger impact? How long Barry, B., & Oliver, R. L. (1996). Affect in dyadic negotiation: A model
can a negotiator effectively continue to use anger or disappoint- and propositions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
cesses, 67, 127–143.
ment as a means of eliciting concessions? Do the effects of these
Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L.
emotions on demands wear off or even reverse in the long run? Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20,
The investigation of these and other questions will allow the study pp. 65–122). New York: Academic Press.
of emotion in social conflict to continue to advance. Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An
interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.
Conclusion Bazerman, M. H., Curhan, J. R., Moore, D. A., & Valley, K. L. (2000).
Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 279 –314.
The present research investigated the interpersonal effects of Bell, D. E. (1985). Disappointment in decision making under uncertainty.
guilt, regret, disappointment, and worry in negotiations. The re- Operations Research, 33, 1–27.
sults showed that negotiators tend to make larger concessions to Berndsen, M., van der Pligt, J., Doosje, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004).
opponents who experience disappointment or worry (supplication Guilt and regret: The determining role of interpersonal and intrapersonal
emotions) than to nonemotional opponents, whereas they make harm. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 55–70.
smaller concessions to adversaries who experience guilt or regret Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1967). When does a harm-doer compensate
(appeasement emotions). This effect was shown to be mediated by a victim? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 435– 441.
Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compliance.
the focal negotiator’s goals (negotiators with a guilty opponent
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 232–239.
adopted higher goals than did those with a disappointed opponent)
Carnevale, P. J., & Isen, A. M. (1986). The influence of positive affect and
and moderated by interpersonal trust—negotiators with high levels visual access on the discovery of integrative solutions in bilateral nego-
of trust adapted their demands to their counterpart’s emotion, but tiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37,
those with low trust did not. These findings point to the pervasive 1–13.
interpersonal effects of emotions in negotiations, and they stress Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive
the need for more research on the role of emotion in conflict and and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97,
negotiation. Such research promises to enhance our understanding 19 –35.
of the negotiation process, of the factors that facilitate or hinder Clark, L. F. (1993). Stress and the cognitive– conversational benefits of
constructive conflict resolution, and of the social consequences of social interaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 25–55.
emotions in general. Clark, M. A., & Taraban, C. B. (1991). Reactions to and willingness to
express emotion in two types of relationships. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 27, 324 –336.
References Clark, M. S., Ouellette, R., Powell, M. C., & Milberg, S. (1987). Recipi-
Adler, R. S., Rosen, B., & Silverstein, E. M. (1998). Emotions in negoti- ent’s mood, relationship type, and helping. Journal of Personality and
ation: How to manage fear and anger. Negotiation Journal, 14, 161–180. Social Psychology, 53, 94 –103.
Allred, K. G., Mallozzi, J. S., Matsui, F., & Raia, C. P. (1997). The Clark, M. S., Pataki, S. P., & Carver, V. H. (1996). Some thoughts and
influence of anger and compassion on negotiation performance. Orga- findings on self-presentation of emotions in relationships. In G. J. O.
nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 175–187. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relation-
Barnett, M. A., Howard, J. A., Melton, E. M., & Dino, G. A. (1982). Effect ships: A social psychological approach (pp. 247–274). Mahwah, NJ:
of inducing sadness about self or other on helping behavior in high- and Erlbaum.
low-empathic children. Child Development, 53, 920 –923. Cornelius, R. R. (1984). A rule model of emotional expression. In C. Z.
Barnett, M. A., King, L. W., & Howard, J. A. (1979). Inducing affect about Malatesta & C. E. Izard (Eds.), Emotion in adult development (pp.
self or other: Effects on generosity in children. Developmental Psychol- 213–233). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
ogy, 15, 164 –167. Côté, S. (2005). A social interaction model of the effects of emotion
Baron, R. A. (1990). Environmentally induced positive affect: Its impact regulation on work strain. Academy of Management Review, 30, 509 –
on self-efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. Journal of 530.
Applied Social Psychology, 20, 368 –384. Davidson, M. N., & Greenhalgh, L. (1999). The role of emotion in
Baron, R. A., Fortin, S. P., Frei, R. L., Hauver, L. A., & Shack, M. L. negotiation: The impact of anger and race. Research on Negotiation in
(1990). Reducing organizational conflict: The role of socially-induced Organizations, 7, 3–26.
positive affect. International Journal of Conflict Management, 1, 133– De Cremer, D., Snyder, M., & Dewitte, S. (2001). ‘The less I trust, the less
152. I contribute (or not?)’: The effects of trust, accountability and self-
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable monitoring in social dilemmas. European Journal of Social Psychology,
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and 31, 93–107.
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, De Dreu, C. K. W., Giebels, E., & Van de Vliert, E. (1998). Social motives
51, 1173–1182. and trust in integrative negotiation: The disruptive effects of punitive
Barrett, K. C. (1995). A functionalist approach to shame and guilt. In J. P. capability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 408 – 422.
Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychol- De Dreu, C. K. W., Lualhati, J. C., & McCusker, C. (1994). Effects of
ogy of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 25– 63). New York: gain/loss frames on satisfaction with self/other outcome-differences.
Guilford Press. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 497–510.
Barry, B. (1999). The tactical use of emotion in negotiation. Research on De Dreu, C. K. W., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2004). The influence of power on
Negotiation in Organizations, 7, 93–121. the information search, impression formation, and demands in negotia-
Barry, B., Fulmer, I. S., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2004). I laughed, I cried, I tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 303–319.
140 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

De Dreu, C. K. W., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). The impact of social facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social
value orientations on negotiator cognition and behavior. Personality and Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1178 –1188. Kee, H. W., & Knox, R. E. (1970). Conceptual and methodological
Deutsch, M. (1960). The effect of motivational orientation upon trust and considerations in the study of trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict
suspicion. Human Relations, 13, 123–139. Resolution, 14, 357–366.
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Kelley, H. H., Beckman, L. L., & Fischer, C. S. (1967). Negotiating the
Review of Psychology, 51, 665– 697. division of a reward under incomplete information. Journal of Experi-
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Mathy, R. M., Shell, R., mental Social Psychology, 3, 361–398.
& Reno, R. R. (1989). The relations of sympathy and personal distress Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and
to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250 –270.
Social Psychology, 57, 55– 66. Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., & Miller, P. (1989). Sympathy of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–521.
and personal distress: Development, gender differences, and interrela- Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (2001). How and when does emo-
tions of indexes. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Empathy and related emotional tional expression help? Review of General Psychology, 5, 187–212.
responses (pp. 107–126). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in so-
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, cial psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & A. Lindzey (Eds.),
48, 384 –392. The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 233–265). Boston:
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., Troyer, D., & Switzer, G. (1994). McGraw-Hill.
The relations of children’s emotion regulation to their vicarious emo- Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the
tional responses and comforting behaviors. Child Development, 65, behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining
1678 –1693. games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in
Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 429 – 453.
on negotiator cognition and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Kimmel, M. J., Pruitt, D. G., Magenau, J. M., Konar-Goldband, E., &
Psychology, 74, 565–577. Carnevale, P. J. D. (1980). Effects of trust, aspiration, and gender on
Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S., & Bless, E. (1967). Compliance without negotiation tactics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38,
pressure: The effect of guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
9 –22.
ogy, 7, 117–124.
Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San
trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165–182.
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Labott, S. M., Martin, R. B., Eason, P. S., & Berkey, E. Y. (1991). Social
Friedman, M. (1985). Toward a reconceptualization of guilt. Contempo-
reactions to the expression of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 5,
rary Psychoanalysis, 21, 501–547.
397– 417.
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
University Press.
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and
& J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381– 403). New
narratives of interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at
York: Guilford Press.
perspective taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of
1–37.
emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. S. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture:
Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotion: A functional view. In P. Ekman
Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51– 87). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association. & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions
Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1994). The temporal pattern to the (pp. 123–126). New York: Oxford University Press.
experience of regret. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International
357–365. Universities Press.
Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame,
when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379 –395. and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
Hegtvedt, K. A. (1990). The effects of relationship structure on emotional emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623– 636). New York: Guilford Press.
responses to inequity. Social Psychology Quarterly, 53, 214 –228. Lindskold, S. (1978). Trust development, the GRIT proposal, and the
Hegtvedt, K. A., & Killian, C. (1999). Fairness and emotions: Reactions to effects of conciliatory acts on conflict and cooperation. Psychological
the process and outcomes of negotiations. Social Forces, 78, 269 –302. Bulletin, 85, 772–793.
Hill, M. G., Weary, G., & Williams, J. (1986). Depression: A self- Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social
presentation formulation. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Per-
self (pp. 213–240). New York: Springer-Verlag. sonality and Social Psychology, 57, 426 – 441.
Hilty, J. A., & Carnevale, P. J. (1993). Black-hat/white-hat strategy in Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1986). Disappointment and dynamic inconsis-
bilateral negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision tency in choice under uncertainty. Review of Economic Studies, 53,
Processes, 55, 444 – 469. 271–282.
Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy Loomis, J. L. (1959). Communication, the development of trust, and
and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior cooperative behavior. Human Relations, 12, 305–315.
(pp. 281–313). New York: Academic Press. Manstead, A. S. R. (1991). Emotion in social life. Cognition and Emotion,
Holmes, J. G., Throop, W. F., & Strickland, L. H. (1971). The effects of 5, 353–362.
prenegotiation expectations on the distributive bargaining process. Jour- McGrath, J. E., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1994). Groups interacting with
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 582–599. technology: Ideas, evidence, issues, and an agenda. Thousand Oaks,
Homans, G. C. (1974). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. Oxford, CA: Sage.
England: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. McKersie, R. B., & Fonstad, N. O. (1997). Teaching negotiation theory and
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect skills over the Internet. Negotiation Journal, 13, 363–368.
SUPPLICATION AND APPEASEMENT IN NEGOTIATIONS 141

Messick, D. M., & Sentis, K. P. (1979). Fairness and preference. Journal response to inequity in close relations. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55,
of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 418 – 434. 57– 69.
Moore, D. A., Kurtzberg, T. R., Thompson, L. L., & Morris, M. W. (1999). Steinel, W., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2004). Social motives and strategic
Long and short routes to success in electronically mediated negotiations: misrepresentation in social decision making. Journal of Personality and
Group affiliations and good vibrations. Organizational Behavior and Social Psychology, 86, 419 – 434.
Human Decision Processes, 77, 22– 43. Steiner, C. (2000). Apology: The transactional analysis of a fundamental
Morris, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2000). How emotions work: An analysis of exchange. Transactional Analysis Journal, 30, 145–149.
the social functions of emotional expression in negotiations. Research in Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to
Organizational Behavior, 22, 1–50. shame and guilt: Development of the self-conscious affect and attribu-
Naquin, C. E., & Paulson, G. D. (2003). Online bargaining and interper- tion inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 102–
sonal trust. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 113–120. 111.
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1992). Human emotions: Functions and Tangney, J. P. (1995). Shame and guilt in interpersonal relations. In J. P.
dysfunctions. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 55– 85. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychol-
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Cambridge, ogy of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 114 –139). New
MA: Blackwell. York: Guilford Press.
Parkinson, B. (1996). Emotions are social. British Journal of Psychology, Tangney, J. P. (1999). The self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embar-
87, 663– 683. rassment, and pride. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of
Parks, C. D., Henager, R. F., & Scamahorn, S. D. (1996). Trust and cognition and emotion (pp. 541–568). New York: Wiley.
messages of intent in social dilemmas. Journal of Conflict Resolution, Thompson, L., Medvec, V. H., Seiden, V., & Kopelman, S. (2001). Poker
40, 134 –151. face, smiley face, and rant ‘n’ rave: Myths and realities about emotion in
Parks, C. D., & Hulbert, L. G. (1995). High and low trusters’ responses to negotiation. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook
fear in a payoff matrix. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 39, 718 –730. of social psychology: Group processes (pp. 139 –163). Malden, MA:
Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good Blackwell.
moods and for maintaining bad moods. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Timmers, M., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Gender
differences in motives for regulating emotions. Personality and Social
Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278 –305). Engle-
Psychology Bulletin, 24, 974 –985.
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 2: The negative
Pillutla, M. M., & Murnighan, J. K. (1996). Unfairness, anger, and spite:
affects. New York: Springer.
Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organizational Behavior and
Tripp, T. M., & Sondak, H. (1992). An evaluation of dependent variables
Human Decision Processes, 68, 208 –224.
in experimental negotiation studies: Impasse rates and Pareto efficiency.
Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale, P. J. (1993). Negotiation in social conflict.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 51, 273–295.
Buckingham, England: Open University Press.
van Dijk, W. W., & van der Pligt, J. (1997). The impact of probability and
Pruitt, D. G., & Kimmel, M. J. (1977). Twenty years of experimental
magnitude of outcome on disappointment and elation. Organizational
gaming: Critique, synthesis, and suggestions for the future. Annual
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 277–284.
Review of Psychology, 28, 363–392.
van Dijk, W. W., & Zeelenberg, M. (2002). Investigating the appraisal
Rempel, J. K., Holmes, J. G., & Zanna, M. P. (1985). Trust in close
patterns of regret and disappointment. Motivation and Emotion, 26,
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 95–112.
321–331.
Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behav-
Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2002). Social value orientation
iors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality
and impression formation: A test of two competing hypotheses about
and Social Psychology, 67, 206 –221. information search in negotiation. International Journal of Conflict
Ross, W., & LaCroix, J. (1996). Multiple meanings of trust in negotiation Management, 13, 59 –77.
theory and research: A literature review and integrative model. Interna- Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004a). The
tional Journal of Conflict Management, 7, 314 –360. interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of
Rotter, J. B. (1980). Interpersonal trust, trustworthiness, and gullibility. Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 57–76.
American Psychologist, 35, 1–7. Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004b). The
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so interpersonal effects of emotions in negotiations: A motivated informa-
different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Man- tion processing approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
agement Review, 23, 393– 404. 87, 510 –528.
Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Pietroni, D., & Manstead, A. S. R.
emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the (in press). Power and emotion in negotiation: Power moderates the
elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805– 819. interpersonal effects of anger and happiness on concession making.
Siegel, S., & Fouraker, L. E. (1960). Bargaining and group decision European Journal of Social Psychology.
making: Experiments in bilateral monopoly. New York: McGraw-Hill. Wallace, J., & Sadalla, E. (1966). Behavioral consequences of transgres-
Sinaceur, M., & Tiedens, L. Z. (in press). Get mad and get more than even: sion: I. The effects of social recognition. Journal of Experimental
When and why anger expression is effective in negotiations. Journal of Research in Personality, 1, 187–194.
Experimental Social Psychology. Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public
Smith, D. L., Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale, P. J. (1982). Matching and good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 110 –116.
mismatching: The effect of own limit, other’s toughness, and time Yee, J. L., & Greenberg, M. S. (1998). Reactions to crime victims: Effects
pressure on concession rate in negotiation. Journal of Personality and of victims’ emotional state and type of friendship. Journal of Social and
Social Psychology, 42, 876 – 883. Clinical Psychology, 17, 209 –226.
Smith, R. H., Webster, J. M., Parrott, W. G., & Eyre, H. L. (2002). The role Yukl, G. A. (1974a). Effects of situational variables and opponent conces-
of public exposure in moral and nonmoral shame and guilt. Journal of sions on a bargainer’s perception, aspirations, and concessions. Journal
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 138 –159. of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 227–236.
Sprecher, S. (1992). How men and women expect to feel and behave in Yukl, G. A. (1974b). Effects of the opponent’s initial offer, concession
142 VAN KLEEF, DE DREU, AND MANSTEAD

magnitude, and concession frequency on bargaining behavior. Journal of ing actions or inactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 323–335. 1113–1119.
Zeelenberg, M., & Beattie, J. (1997). Consequences of regret aver- Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., Manstead, A. S. R., & van der Pligt, J.
sion 2: Additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision (2000). On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: The psychol-
making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, ogy of regret and disappointment. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 521–541.
72, 63–78. Zetik, D. C., & Stuhlmacher, A. F. (2002). Goal setting and negotiation
Zeelenberg, M., van der Pligt, J., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Undoing performance: A meta-analysis. Group Processes and Intergroup Rela-
regret on Dutch television: Apologizing for interpersonal regrets involv- tions, 5, 35–52.

Appendix A Appendix B

Participants’ Payoff Chart Statements Used for the Manipulation of the Opponent’s
Emotion
Price of phones Warranty period Service contract
Opponent’s emotion Statement
Level Price Payoff Warranty Payoff Service Payoff
After Round 1
1 $150 400 1 month 120 1 month 240
2 $145 350 2 months 105 2 months 210 Disappointment I am pretty disappointed about this, I think I will
3 $140 300 3 months 90 3 months 180 offer 8–7–7
4 $135 250 4 months 75 4 months 150 Worry This worries me quite a lot, I think I will offer
5 $130 200 5 months 60 5 months 120 8–7–7
6 $125 150 6 months 45 6 months 90 Guilt I feel guilty for not having conceded more, I
7 $120 100 7 months 30 7 months 60 think I will offer 8–7–7
8 $115 50 8 months 15 8 months 30 Regret I feel sorry that I haven’t conceded more, I think
9 $110 0 9 months 0 9 months 0 I will offer 8–7–7
No emotion I think I will offer 8–7–7
Note. Prices in Euros were converted to U.S. dollars and rounded to the
nearest U.S.$5. After Round 3
Disappointment This is going awry, I am very disappointed. I am
going to offer 7–6–7
Worry This is going awry, I am very worried. I am
going to offer 7–6–7
Guilt This is going awry, I feel pretty guilty. I am
going to offer 7–6–7
Regret This is going awry, I regret it. I am going to
offer 7–6–7
No emotion I am going to offer 7–6–7

After Round 5
Disappointment I am going to offer 6–6–6, because I am really
disappointed
Worry I am going to offer 6–6–6, because I am really
worried
Guilt I am going to offer 6–6–6, because I feel very
guilty
Regret I am going to offer 6–6–6, because I am very
sorry
No emotion I am going to offer 6–6–6

Note. Statements were pretested and have been translated from Dutch.
The typing errors deliberately included in the original versions are not
shown.

Received April 17, 2004


Revision received May 31, 2005
Accepted December 14, 2005 䡲
PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES

Optimism in Close Relationships: How Seeing Things


in a Positive Light Makes Them So

Sanjay Srivastava Kelly M. McGonigal


University of Oregon Stanford University

Jane M. Richards Emily A. Butler


University of Texas at Austin University of Arizona

James J. Gross
Stanford University

Does expecting positive outcomes— especially in important life domains such as relationships—make
these positive outcomes more likely? In a longitudinal study of dating couples, the authors tested whether
optimists (who have a cognitive disposition to expect positive outcomes) and their romantic partners are
more satisfied in their relationships, and if so, whether this is due to optimists perceiving greater support
from their partners. In cross-sectional analyses, both optimists and their partners indicated greater
relationship satisfaction, an effect that was mediated by optimists’ greater perceived support. When the
couples engaged in a conflict conversation, optimists and their partners saw each other as engaging more
constructively during the conflict, which in turn led both partners to feel that the conflict was better
resolved 1 week later. In a 1-year follow-up, men’s optimism predicted relationship status. Effects of
optimism were mediated by the optimists’ perceived support, which appears to promote a variety of
beneficial processes in romantic relationships.

Keywords: optimism, relationship satisfaction, perceived support, close relationships

I have heard of reasons manifold affected by the cognitive dispositions of the individuals involved.
Why Love needs be blind, As Coleridge might say, lovers’ eyes are in their minds.
But this the best of all I hold— In this article, we present an investigation of the consequences
His eyes are in his mind.
of one particular cognitive disposition, namely optimism, within
—Samuel Taylor Coleridge (1811)
romantic relationships. Is optimism associated with happier and
Individuals’ perceptions of the social world are more than just longer lasting romantic relationships? To answer this question, our
objective reports of an external reality. Social perceptions are research was designed to test two related hypotheses. First, we
shaped in the mind of the perceiver, a fact that can have very real tested the hypothesis that optimists and their partners would have
consequences for social life. Romantic relationships, in particular, relationships that are more satisfying, are characterized by better
have long been observed by poets and writers to be substantially conflict resolution, and are longer lasting.1 Second, we tested the
hypothesis that the reason why optimists have better relationship
outcomes is that they perceive their partners as more supportive.
Sanjay Srivastava, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon; We tested these hypotheses in cross-sectional analyses of couples’
Kelly M. McGonigal and James J. Gross, Department of Psychology,
Stanford University; Jane M. Richards, Department of Psychology, Uni-
1
versity of Texas at Austin; Emily A. Butler, Department of Psychology, Optimism and pessimism can be conceptualized several different ways:
University of Arizona. as opposite poles of a single dimension, as two distinct dimensions, or as
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01 discrete categories. In this article we treat optimism both conceptually and
M58147 awarded to James J. Gross. Kelly M. McGonigal was supported empirically as a single, bipolar dimension, an approach that was supported
by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship. by analyses of the data. To avoid cumbersome language, we have used the
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sanjay term optimists in this article as a shorthand, meaning in effect, “individuals
Srivastava, Department of Psychology, 1227 University of Oregon, Eu- who score higher in optimism, relative to those who score lower.” It is not
gene, OR 97403-1227. E-mail: sanjay@uoregon.edu our intention to suggest that optimists are a discrete category.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 143–153
Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.143

143
144 SRIVASTAVA, MCGONIGAL, RICHARDS, BUTLER, AND GROSS

reports about their relationships, in analyses of how couples re- workers (Dougall, Hyman, Hayward, McFeeley, & Baum, 2001),
sponded to a conflict conversation, and in a 1-year follow-up of bereaved men (Park & Folkman, 1997), and college students
relationship dissolution. (Sarason, Levine, Basham, & Sarason, 1983). In a longitudinal
investigation, Brissette, Scheier, and Carver (2002) investigated
Optimism, Perceived Support, and Social Functioning the relationship between optimism and perceived social support. In
a sample of incoming college students, optimism was associated
Optimism is defined as the cognitive disposition to expect fa- with concurrent reports of perceived support and number of close
vorable outcomes (Scheier & Carver, 1985). A substantial body of friendships at the beginning of college and with increases in
research has linked optimism to effective coping and to positive perceived support over the course of the semester. The increases in
mental and physical health outcomes (e.g., Scheier, Carver, & perceived support mediated the effect of optimism on depression,
Bridges, 2001; Taylor, Kemeny, Reed, Bower, & Gruenewald, though not the effect of optimism on stress. Brissette and col-
2000). A smaller number of studies have also shown that optimism leagues’ findings are important and suggestive, but they were not
leads to better social functioning. For example, optimism is asso- able to examine relational outcomes such as relationship satisfac-
ciated with lower social alienation (Scheier & Carver, 1985) and tion or conflict resolution; their study also did not examine the
with longer lasting friendships (Geers, Reilley, & Dember, 1998). effects of an individual’s optimism on relationship partners.
In romantic relationships, two prior studies have suggested that
optimism about a particular relationship predicts greater satisfac-
tion in that relationship and reduced likelihood of relationship The Present Study
dissolution (Helgeson, 1994; Murray & Holmes, 1997), although
the mechanisms explaining such a relation were not directly tested. The available evidence suggests that optimism is associated
Why might optimists have more positive experiences in rela- with positive outcomes in relationships in general, possibly as a
tionships? As a cognitive disposition, optimism should influence result of processes that promote and maintain perceived support.
how individuals attend to and interpret others’ behaviors and Our particular interest was in examining these processes in the
intentions. We propose that within a close relationship, this cog- context of close relationships. Optimism and perceived support are
nitive disposition may manifest itself as perceived support, the often studied in terms of their consequences for social life in
belief that one’s partner is able and willing to provide support if general; an examination of close relationships offers several dis-
necessary (Murray & Holmes, 1997). Perceived support could in tinct opportunities to complement this research. For researchers
turn have a number of benefits: It has been shown to lead indi- who study close relationships, studying optimism and perceived
viduals to feel that their relationship facilitates their personal and support can potentially provide insights into the cognitive pro-
collective goals (Brunstein, Dangelmayer, & Schultheiss, 1996; cesses that maintain security and closeness between partners. For
Kaplan & Maddux, 2002), and it may buffer against stress and researchers who study optimism, close relationships are an impor-
negative affect in relationships (Dehle, Larsen, & Landers, 2001). tant life domain for which optimism may have meaningful
This latter effect may be particularly important in close consequences.
relationships. Studies of perceived support also suggest that there may be
Research on perceived support indicates that, like optimism, it is important processes taking place in the context of dyadic relation-
moderately stable over time (Sarason, Sarason, & Shearin, 1986), ships that could be missed in broad-bandwidth studies of social
and it appears to be something more than simply a direct reflection
life. Although individuals do differ in their general tendency to
of others’ actual supportive behaviors (Barrera, 1986; Belsher &
perceive all others as supportive, perceived support also draws
Costello, 1991; Newcomb, 1990). Yet despite the agreement
substantially on relationship-specific perceptions (Lakey et al.,
among many researchers that perceived support is influenced by
1996). That is, individuals form distinct judgments about the
personality variables, Lakey, McCabe, Fisicaro, and Drew (1996)
supportiveness of other individuals, above and beyond their broad
wrote that “surprisingly, there has been very little research on the
judgments about others in general. Although much research on
personality factors that predict the development of perceived sup-
social support has focused broadly on social networks, this finding
port” (p. 1278).
Among personality factors that might promote perceived sup- suggests that it is also important to examine the consequences of
port, optimism seems to be a likely candidate. Perceived support is perceived support in the context of specific relationships.
associated with positive biases in evaluating and remembering In developing our questions and hypotheses, we organized our
supportive behaviors in specific interactions and relationships investigation around two guiding questions. First, what conse-
(Lakey et al., 1996; Lakey & Cassady, 1990; Pierce, Sarason, & quences, if any, does optimism have for satisfaction in close
Sarason, 1992). Furthermore, the proposed mechanisms of per- relationships, both for the optimist and for the optimist’s partner?
ceived support—positive affect, coping self-efficacy, and adaptive Second, does perceived support explain the relation between op-
coping—are all robustly associated with optimism (Chang, 2001; timism and relationship satisfaction?
Cozarelli, 1993; Scheier et al., 2001). Optimists are better liked by Because of the complexity of the research design, we present the
others, which may reinforce their expectations about how others findings in three parts (see Table 1). Part 1 examines the cross-
will treat them (Carver, Kus, & Scheier, 1994). In relationships, we sectional relations among both partners’ optimism, perceived sup-
expect that optimists would be more likely to perceive others’ port, and relationship satisfaction at Time 1. Part 2 reports a closer
behaviors as supportive and to respond accordingly. examination of how the couples reacted to conflict (Time 2) and
A few studies have offered some evidence directly linking how well they felt the conflict was resolved 1 week later (Time 3).
optimism to perceived support. Associations between optimism Part 3 examines an objective outcome, relationship maintenance
and perceived support have been found among air crash rescue versus dissolution, 1 year later (Time 4).
OPTIMISM IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 145

Table 1
Overview of Design, Procedures, and Measures

Timepoint (time since Time 1) Procedure Major measure

Part 1

Time 1 Questionnaire packet Optimism, perceived support,


relationship satisfaction

Part 2

Time 2 (1 week) Laboratory-based conflict Positive conflict engagement


conversation
Time 3 (2 weeks) Post-conflict follow-up Conflict resolution

Part 3

Time 4 (1 year) One-year follow-up Relationship status

Part 1: Optimism and Perceived Support At least one member of each couple was an undergraduate recruited
in a Dating Relationship from one of three northern California universities. Couples were exclusive
and had been dating for at least 6 months at the start of the study, with a
In Part 1, we examined partners’ reports regarding their dating median relationship length of 16 months; 12% of couples were cohabiting.
relationship in general. We hypothesized that optimism would be Participant ages ranged from 18 to 25 years, with a mean age of 20.4 years.
associated with greater relationship satisfaction. Furthermore, we The ethnic and racial composition of this sample was 2.1% African
expected not only that optimists themselves would report greater American, 23.8% Asian, 56.3% Caucasian, 14.6% Latino/Hispanic, 0.8%
Native American, and 2.5% other. Participants were paid $15/hr for their
relationship satisfaction than would pessimists but also that the
participation.
partners of optimists would report greater relationship satisfaction
than the partners of pessimists. Such an effect would indicate that Measures
the positive relational consequences of optimism are not just “in
the head” of the optimists. We further hypothesized that the effects Optimism. The Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985) is
of optimism would be mediated by optimists’ tendency to perceive an eight-item self-report measure of general outcome expectancies. Sample
items include “In uncertain times, I usually expect the best” and the
their partners as supportive in the relationship.
reverse-coded item “If something can go wrong for me, it will.” Responses
To rule out possible confounds, we conducted several additional
range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). We rescaled scores
analyses. One possible confounding variable was partner invest- of all individual difference measures to percent of maximum possible
ment: Perhaps optimists attract more supportive partners, in which (POMP) metric, which sets the theoretical range of a scale from 0 to 100.
case an effect of optimism on perceived support could simply POMP scoring is a linear transformation of raw scores and thus does not
reflect an accurate appraisal rather than a perceptual disposition. affect standardized analyses, but it can aid in interpretation of raw scores
Thus, we also obtained reports from each partner of offered sup- by putting them on an intuitive metric (Cohen, Cohen, Aiken, & West,
port in the relationship to use as control variables. If optimists have 1999). Actual scores on the LOT, in POMP metric, ranged from 22 to 100;
a global tendency to see their partners as supportive, that relation means and standard deviations for the LOT and other major variables are
should be independent of the actual amount of support offered by reported in Table 2. Alpha reliability coefficients were .80 for men and .86
for women, and factor analysis indicated a unidimensional structure. All of
their partners.
our data analyses controlled for possible confounding due to partner
Finally, some studies have suggested that optimism may be
similarity on optimism. However, it is worth noting that the correlation
correlated with the personality traits of Neuroticism, Extraversion, between partners’ optimism was r ⫽ .12, p ⫽ .22. In other words, there was
or self-esteem (see Scheier et al., 2001). Thus, we conducted not a strong or reliable tendency for optimists to be partnered with other
additional control analyses to ensure that the effects of optimism optimists.
were independent of these other dimensions of individual differ- Perceived support. To assess perceived support in the dating relation-
ences, as well as the individuals’ ages, the length of the relation- ship, we used the Maintenance Questionnaire (MQ; Stafford & Canary,
ship, and whether the partners were living together. 1991). Participants rated 24 statements concerning their partner’s behaviors
on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The MQ has
five subscales that cover a broad range of supportive behaviors: (a) posi-
Method tivity (e.g., “Does not criticize me”), (b) openness (e.g., “Encourages me to
Procedure and Participants
2
We examined data from a study of dating couples assessed at multiple We compared the 108 couples included in this report with the 12
time points over a 1-year period (see Table 1). For the analyses presented couples who did not return at Times 2 or 3. Analyses indicated no
in this article as Parts 1 and 2, we included couples from the original differences on optimism for the men or women of these couples (rs ⬍ .07,
sample who completed all measures at Times 1, 2, and 3 (but not neces- ps ⬎ .52); however, the men in the 108 included couples were somewhat
sarily Time 4); this left us with 108 couples (N ⫽ 216) for the present higher in perceived support (r ⫽ .26, p ⫽ .004) and higher in relationship
report.2 In Part 1 we analyze data from Time 1, when participants com- satisfaction (r ⫽ .19, p ⫽ .04). The women in these couples did not differ
pleted measures of personality, social support, and the dating relationship. significantly on those dimensions (rs ⬍ .13, ps ⬎ .16).
146 SRIVASTAVA, MCGONIGAL, RICHARDS, BUTLER, AND GROSS

Table 2
Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations for Main Measures from Parts 1 and 2

Variable 1. MLOT 2. MMQ 3. MCSS 4. MCONV 5. MRES 6. FLOT 7. FMQ 8. FCSS 9. FCONV 10. FRES

1. MLOT —
2. MMQ .36 —
3. MCSS .32 .68 —
4. MCONV .19 .44 .49 —
5. MRES .21 .42 .40 .46 —
6. FLOT .12 .21 .26 .23 .26 —
7. FMQ .09 .41 .50 .51 .33 .26 —
8. FCSS .15 .29 .36 .30 .29 .27 .62 —
9. FCONV .16 .46 .41 .71 .49 .30 .49 .31 —
10. FRES .23 .25 .26 .40 .48 .32 .23 .32 .43 —
M 66.2 81.4 80.4 0.0 5.0 66.8 79.1 82.1 0.0 5.5
SD 15.9 12.1 16.11 0.7 2.5 17.8 12.8 16.4 0.7 2.5

Note. N ⫽ 108 couples. Effect sizes greater than .20 are significant at p ⬍ .05. The first letter in the variable name indicates gender (M ⫽ male, F ⫽
female). LOT ⫽ Life Orientation Test; MQ ⫽ Maintenance Questionnaire; CSS ⫽ Couple Satisfaction Survey; CONV ⫽ positive engagement in conflict
conversation (average of z-scored self-reports and partner-reports); RES ⫽ resolution of conflict.

disclose my thoughts and feelings to him/her”), (c) assurances (e.g., However, it was important to establish discriminant validity; a possible
“Stresses his or her commitment to me”), (d) social network (e.g., “Focuses counterhypothesis was that the measures simply reflected a general rela-
on common friends and affiliations”), and (e) sharing tasks (e.g., “Helps tional positivity factor. To test this counterhypothesis, we ran a confirma-
equally with tasks that need to be done”). The five subscales were all tory factor analysis in which all four of the men’s measures loaded on a
positively correlated (mean r ⫽ .38, ranging from .20 to .58), so we latent “men’s positivity” factor, all of the women’s measures loaded on a
averaged the five scales and converted to POMP metric to create a global latent “women’s positivity” factor, and the men’s and women’s factors
measure of perceived support from the dating partner. Scores ranged from were allowed to correlate.3 The analysis showed that the counterhypothesis
35 to 100. Alphas (computed at the item level) were .91 for men and .92 did not fit the data, ␹2(19, N ⫽ 108) ⫽ 52.9, p ⬍ .001; normed fit index
for women. (NFI) ⫽ .80; root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ⫽ .13.
Relationship satisfaction. To measure relationship satisfaction, we Analyses of reduced sets of variables, created by eliminating optimism or
used the Couple Satisfaction Scale (CSS; Cowan & Cowan, 1990). The offered support, did not show substantially better fit.
CSS includes eight items that are rated on scale from 1 (very dissatisfied)
to 5 (very satisfied). A sample item is, “In general, how do you feel about
the closeness and distance in your relationship with your partner now?” Results and Discussion
Whereas the MQ, our measure of perceived support, asks members of
couples to report what their partners do, the CSS asks individuals how they For our analyses we were interested in estimating both within-
feel about the relationship. CSS scores, computed in POMP metric, ranged person and between-persons effects—for example, how an indi-
from 9 to 100. Alphas for the CSS were .89 for men and .89 for women. vidual’s optimism relates to his or her own relationship satisfaction
Control measure: Offered support. We used a subset of 10 items from (a within-person effect) and to his or her partner’s relationship
the Investment Scale (IS; Lund, 1985), which asks the participant to rate satisfaction (a between-persons effect). Both of these kinds of
“how much you feel you have invested in your relationship in each of the questions are addressed by the actor–partner interdependence
following ways” on a scale from 1 (not invested) to 7 (very invested). Items model (APIM; Kashy & Kenny, 1997), a data analysis procedure
were selected to match the subscales of the MQ, for example, “Trying to for dyads. The APIM was also designed to deal with the violations
encourage and support your partner” (positivity), “Telling your partner
of statistical independence associated with dyadic data. Thus, we
your true feelings about the relationship” (openness), “Integrating your
partner into your family” (social network), “Making formal agreements
adopted the APIM as our basic data-analytic strategy.
about your relationship, such as deciding to go steady, get engaged, or get The APIM estimates two kinds of effects: actor effects and
married” (assurances), and “Doing favors for or helping your partner, such partner effects. Actor effects are within-person effects: They rep-
as lending money or doing errands” (tasks). The items were summed and resent the influence of an individual’s level of a predictor variable
converted to POMP metric to create a global self-report measure of offered on that individual’s level of an outcome variable. Partner effects
support. Scores ranged from 39 to 100; means were 76.8 (SD ⫽ 13.2) for are between-person effects: They represent the influence of an
men and 76.7 (SD ⫽ 11.1) for women. Alphas were .80 for men and .72 individual’s level of a predictor on that individual’s partner’s level
for women. of the outcome variable. APIM estimates also control for con-
Control measures: Extraversion, Neuroticism, self-esteem, and demo- founding due to partner similarity.
graphics. Extraversion and Neuroticism were measured with eight-item
The APIM is rooted in regression (Kashy & Kenny, 1997). As
scales from the Big Five Inventory (John & Srivastava, 1999). Alphas for
Extraversion were .89 for men and .88 for women; alphas for Neuroticism
with regression, it is possible to extend the APIM to include
were .77 for men and .82 for women. Self-esteem was measured with the moderators, control variables, and mediators. We had a substantive
10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965); alphas were .88 interest in taking advantage of all of these possibilities. One
for men and .90 for women. We also measured each partner’s age, how important question was whether gender moderated the actor and
long the couple had been together, and whether they were cohabiting. partner effects. In the APIM, actor and partner effects are aggre-
Discriminant validity among optimism and relational measures. Con-
ceptually, the measures of optimism, perceived support, offered support,
3
and relationship satisfaction are all supposed to measure different things. We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this analysis.
OPTIMISM IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 147

gated across both members of the couple. When members of Were the mediated paths significant? The bootstrap tests in-
couples are distinguishable on some variable—such as gender, in dicated that the actor effect of optimism on relationship satisfac-
the case of our heterosexual dating couples—it is possible to ask tion, reported earlier, was significantly mediated by perceived
whether actor and partner effects are moderated by gender. All of support: mediated actor effect ⫽ .18, p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ (.09,
the analyses we report were tested for moderator effects of gender. .27). Likewise, the effect of optimism on a partner’s relationship
Unless reported otherwise, such effects were not significant, and satisfaction was also significantly mediated by the optimist’s per-
thus results apply to both men and women.4 ceived support: mediated partner effect ⫽ .10, p ⫽ .003, 95% CI ⫽
The basic APIM can also be elaborated to test models with (.03, .18).
multiple predictors (for control analyses) or with mediated paths. Did the direct effects indicate full or partial mediation? If the
Shrout and Bolger (2003) reported that more sensitive tests of direct effect of optimism on an actor’s own relationship satisfac-
mediation can be conducted by using bootstrap analyses, as com- tion was still significant, that would indicate partial (rather than
pared with other methods. Thus, we ran our analyses in Amos 4.01 full) mediation of the actor effect. This effect was not significant:
(Arbuckle, 1999), which can conduct bootstrap analyses. direct actor effect ⫽ .10, p ⫽ .15, 95% CI ⫽ (–.03, .24). Nor was
the direct partner effect significant: direct partner effect ⫽ .07, p ⫽
Do Optimists and Their Partners Report Greater .17, 95% CI ⫽ (–.03, .18). Thus, the analyses indicated the effects
Relationship Satisfaction? of an individual’s optimism on both the individual’s own relation-
ship satisfaction and on a partner’s satisfaction could be explained
We expected that optimists and their partners would experience by the optimist’s perceived support.
their relationships as more satisfying. To test this hypothesis, we
performed an APIM analysis using optimism to predict relation- Control Analyses
ship satisfaction. The results indicated that optimists reported
greater relationship satisfaction. The standardized actor effect was To ensure that the effect of optimism on global perceived
.27, p ⬍ .001, with a 95% confidence interval (CI) ranging from support was not a result of optimists attracting more supportive
.15 to .38. (The p values and CIs reported for all APIM analyses partners, we conducted an APIM analysis testing the effect of
are bias-corrected values from bootstrap analyses.) Furthermore, optimism on perceived support while controlling for offered sup-
optimists’ partners also reported greater relationship satisfaction, port. The effects of optimism were virtually unchanged: actor
indicating that the positive relational consequences of optimism effect ⫽ .28, p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ (.16, .40); partner effect ⫽ .10,
were not just “in the head” of the optimists: standardized partner p ⫽ .13, 95% CI ⫽ (–.02, .22). Thus, optimists’ perceptions of
effect ⫽ .18, p ⫽ .006, 95% CI ⫽ (.06, .30). their partners’ supportiveness could not be explained away by
them attracting genuinely more supportive partners.
We also wanted to ensure that the effects of optimism on
Does Perceived Support Mediate Relations Between
relationship satisfaction were specific to optimism rather than
Optimism and Relationship Satisfaction? being attributable to related traits. To test this, we conducted
Having established that optimism was related to relationship APIM analyses with covariates, controlling for individual differ-
satisfaction, we then tested whether this relation was mediated by ences in Extraversion, Neuroticism, and self-esteem, as well as
perceived support. Following Shrout and Bolger’s (2003) proce- both partners’ ages, the length of the relationship, and whether the
dure (the logic of which is modeled on Baron & Kenny, 1986), this couple was living together; covariates were tested one by one
required four further steps. Each step must produce a significant because of concerns about multicollinearity. Pitted against each
result to proceed to the next. First, we tested whether optimism covariate, optimism always was a significant predictor; further-
predicts perceived support. Second, we tested whether perceived more, no covariate had a significant effect on relationship satis-
support predicts relationship satisfaction when controlling for op- faction after controlling for optimism (all absolute effects ⬍ .12;
timism. Third, we tested the mediated paths from optimism via all ps ⬎ .16). Thus, we felt fairly confident that the effects of
perceived support to relationship satisfaction; a significant boot- optimism on relationship satisfaction were not confounded with
strap test would support mediation. This bootstrap test is a more broader personality traits, with self-esteem, or with the demo-
powerful replacement for the Sobel test used in conventional graphic and background variables we examined.
mediation analysis. Fourth, we tested the direct paths from opti- Part 1 thus shows that the romantic relationships of optimists are
mism to relationship satisfaction when controlling for perceived characterized by greater relationship satisfaction than the relation-
support; this last step would indicate whether mediation was ships of those who are less optimistic. The mediation analyses
partial or complete. suggested that optimists’ general tendency to see their partners as
Did optimism predict perceived support? The results indicated supportive mediated these positive relationship outcomes. Not
that optimists perceived greater support from their partners: actor only did optimists report greater relationship satisfaction, but so
effect ⫽ .29, p ⬍ .001, 95% CI ⫽ (.17, .41). Optimists’ partners did their (not necessarily optimistic) partners, suggesting that the
had marginally higher levels of perceived support: partner effect ⫽
.12, p ⫽ .07, 95% CI ⫽ (–.01, .24). 4
The APIM can be specified as a path model with equality constraints
Did perceived support predict relationship satisfaction? The
between members of the dyad; in this study, the APIM was specified by
effect of perceived support on an actor’s own relationship satis- setting men’s parameter estimates equal to women’s. The unconstrained or
faction was substantial: actor effect ⫽ .58, p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ “saturated” model produces separate parameter estimates for men and for
(.44, .70). Individuals who perceived greater support also had women. Thus, the chi-square test of model fit (which compares the con-
more satisfied partners: partner effect ⫽ .16, p ⫽ .003, 95% CI ⫽ strained model to a saturated model) is, in the present context, a test of
(.07, .27). moderation by gender.
148 SRIVASTAVA, MCGONIGAL, RICHARDS, BUTLER, AND GROSS

positive relationship outcomes are not merely a Pollyanna-like examined the participants’ ratings of how intensely they disagree
fantasy of the optimists. Because Part 1 was based on cross- about various topics in their relationship, including the one dis-
sectional data, however, the ordering of variables in the media- cussed; this measure was taken before the conversation took place.
tional model was based on conceptual considerations rather than
on the design of the study. In Part 2, we adopted a design in which Method
the temporal structure of the design gave us a stronger basis to
evaluate whether the relationship benefits of optimism are ex- Procedure
plained by perceived support. The conflict conversation procedure was modeled after that used by
Part 1 focused on global perceptions and feelings about the Carstensen et al. (1995).5 On arrival at the laboratory at Time 2, a female
relationship. In Part 2, we wanted to move beyond this global level experimenter gave participants an overview of the study. Participants were
of analyses and take a closer look at the role of optimism in told that the study was about “how couples talk to each other about
relationship processes. To do this, we brought the same couples important conflicts or areas of disagreement in the relationship.” Thus, they
from Part 1 into the laboratory and facilitated a conflict conver- would need to talk to each other for 10 min and complete questionnaires
sation. We assessed whether optimists and their partners perceived concerning their reactions to the conversation. Both members of the couple
had separately reported on their area of greatest current disagreement in the
each other as supportive during the conflict, and whether that
Time 1 questionnaire set using the Couple Problem Inventory (Gottman,
perception of support contributed to both partners’ reports of how
Markman, & Notarius, 1977). In this questionnaire, participants indicated
well the conflict was resolved 1 week later. how much they disagree with their partner in a number of preestablished
areas (money, jealousy, recreation, etc.) and also had the opportunity to list
Part 2: The Conflict Conversation additional areas of disagreement. After rating disagreement across all
areas, participants then filled in a response to the question, “Which is
In dating relationships, a common stressor is disagreement be- currently the greatest area of disagreement in your relationship?” (addi-
tween partners, such as disagreement about finances or time spent tional questions asked for the second and third greatest areas). Prior to the
together. How members of a couple perceive and react to disagree- Time 2 session, the experimenter randomly selected either the male’s or
ments can be important for the health of the relationship (Bradbury female’s area of greatest disagreement. The experimenter then raised the
topic and asked each partner to describe (a) more specifically how the
& Fincham, 1990; Carstensen, Gottman, & Levenson, 1995; Ga-
problem area was relevant to their relationship, (b) the last time this
ble, Reis, & Downey, 2003). In Part 2, we examined how the
problem came up between them, (c) his or her emotions surrounding this
couples in our study responded to conflict by bringing them into specific incident, and (d) why he or she experienced these emotions. If the
the laboratory to have a conversation about the most stressful area partners’ responses indicated that the topic was not likely to be appropriate
of current disagreement in their relationship. Immediately after the for the experiment, the experimenter selected a different area of disagree-
interaction, we asked each member of the couple to report how ment. Disagreement in the area chosen for discussion, rated by participants
positively and constructively they had engaged in the conflict, and on a scale from 0 (don’t disagree at all) to 100 (disagree very much),
how positively and constructively their partner had engaged. One averaged 60 for men and 65 for women. The couples discussed the topic for
week later, we asked each member of the couple how well they had 10 min. At the end of the conversation, participants completed question-
resolved the conflict as a couple. In measuring positive conflict naires about positive conflict resolution behaviors.
One week later (at Time 3), participants returned to the laboratory to
engagement, we believed that it was critical to take advantage of
complete questionnaires about the conflict topic and the conversation they
the participants’ position as informants within their relationships to
had in the laboratory. Participants rated the degree to which the conflict had
tell us about how effective they were in addressing the conflict been resolved since the conversation.
together. In essence, we were interested in the shared reality of the
relationship—the couple’s joint construal of how effectively they
Measures
mutually dealt with the conflict.
We hypothesized that optimists and their partners would both Intensity of disagreements. On the Couple Problem Inventory, each
see the conflict as better resolved 1 week after their conversation. partner rated the intensity of their disagreement in each potential area of
We also hypothesized that this resolution would be explained, at disagreement, using a scale from 0 (don’t disagree at all) to 100 (disagree
least in part, by a shared perception that both partners engaged very much). We computed disagreement scores by averaging ratings across
the 13 preestablished areas and, if applicable, the 1 or 2 additional areas
positively and constructively during the conversation. These hy-
identified by the participant (for men, M ⫽ 23, SD ⫽ 14; for women, M ⫽
potheses were brought together in a double-mediation model. In 23, SD ⫽ 13). Alpha reliabilities of the disagreement composite, computed
Part 1, we found that optimists had a global tendency to perceive across the 13 preselected topics that every participant rated, were .81 for
their partners as supportive. Now, we anticipated that this global men and .74 for women.
tendency toward perceived support would be manifested in the Positive engagement in conflict. After the conversation, participants
more specific context of the conversation through positive engage- reported the extent to which they and their partners engaged in positive or
ment and that this positive engagement would be recognized by
both partners. More positive engagement in the conflict would, in
5
turn, lead to a better resolution to the conflict in the eyes of both The data presented in this article were originally collected as part of an
partners. experimental study of emotions and physiology. For the purposes of the
experimental study, physiological measurements were taken in the Time 2
In examining whether optimists and their partners reported
laboratory session, and couples were randomly assigned to have one
better conflict resolution, we considered the alternative explana- member suppress or reappraise his or her emotions or to a control condition
tion that optimists’ relationships might be characterized by rela- (Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003). Our present focus is on individual
tively low-intensity conflicts. That is, optimists might appear to be differences rather than the effects of the experimental manipulation, and
good at resolving conflict, but only because their conflicts are none of the effects reported in this article interacted with experimental
relatively easy to resolve. To address this possibility, we also condition.
OPTIMISM IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 149

supportive behaviors during the conflict conversation. Sample items in- Did Optimists and Their Partners See the Conflict as
clude, “During the conversation, to what extent were you [was your Better Resolved?
partner] a good listener?” “During the conversation, to what extent did you
[your partner] try to understand your partner’s [your] point of view?” and Were optimists’ relationships characterized by better positive
“During the conversation, to what extent did you [your partner] criticize conflict resolution, as perceived by both partners? In an APIM
your partner [you]?” (reverse scored). A total of 17 items were rated on a analysis entering optimism and intensity of disagreement simulta-
scale from 0 (none/not at all) to 10 (a great deal/extremely). We averaged
neously to predict conflict resolution, the actor effect of optimism
the items to create composites. Self-reports ranged from 3.8 to 9.7; partner
was .17, p ⫽ .018, 95% CI ⫽ (.05, .31), and the partner effect of
reports ranged from 3.0 to 9.4. Means (and SDs) were as follows: men’s
reports of women, 6.8 (1.3); women’s reports of men, 6.9 (1.3); men’s optimism was .15, p ⫽ .02, 95% CI ⫽ (.02, .28). Individuals who
self-reports, 6.9 (1.2); women’s self-reports, 6.9 (1.2). Alphas were .87 for rated their disagreements as relatively intense did report poorer
men’s reports of women, .86 for women’s reports of men, .83 for men’s conflict resolution, actor effect ⫽ ⫺.20, p ⫽ .01, 95% CI ⫽ (⫺.35,
reports of their own behavior, and .83 for women’s reports of their own ⫺.04); the partner effect was not significant ( p ⫽ .13). From this
behavior. analysis it can be concluded that both optimists and their partners
To simplify the analyses, we created a positive engagement variable for agreed that their conflicts had reached a more satisfactory resolu-
each individual that aggregated across data sources. That is, the positive tion 1 week later and that this effect could not be explained away
engagement variable for men was an average of the men’s self-reports with by baseline differences in the intensity of their disagreements.
women’s reports of their male partners, and vice versa to create an
aggregate for women; variables were converted to z scores before averag-
ing. These aggregates were justified by the substantial (though not perfect) Do Perceived Support and Positive Engagement Mediate
agreement between self-reports and partner reports: agreement between the Benefits of Optimism?
men’s self-reports and women’s partner reports, indexed as an alpha
coefficient, was .64; agreement between women’s self-reports and men’s We hypothesized that optimists’ global perceived support would
partner reports was .53. To make sure that we were measuring the shared promote positive engagement in the conflict conversation, as rec-
reality of the relationship and not merely the positive biases of optimists, ognized by both partners, and that this would explain the effects of
we also examined the reports that relied on a single data source (either self optimism on achieving a more satisfactory conflict resolution. Part
or partner) and attempted to replicate all of the analyses with the single- 1 already demonstrated the effect of optimism on global perceived
reporter variables. In the model run with partner reports, optimists’ own support; here we present evidence testing the remaining elements
positive engagement would be reported by their partners, and thus the actor
of the double-mediation hypothesis.
effects would be immune from any positive perceptual bias “in the heads”
of optimists. In the model run with individuals’ construals of their own
Did perceived support promote positive engagement? We ran
behavior, the optimists’ partners’ behavior would be reported by the APIMs testing the effects of global perceived support on the
partners rather than the optimists; thus the partner effects would be un- aggregated positive engagement measure. The analyses showed
tainted by optimists’ internal biases. If these analyses replicated the find- that individuals with greater perceived support were seen as en-
ings with the aggregated variables, that would ensure that the effects gaging more positively in the conflict: actor effect ⫽ .32, p ⫽ .004,
reflected the shared reality of the relationship rather than the idiosyncratic 95% CI ⫽ (.22, .42). In follow-up analyses in which we analyzed
views of one person. the self-reports and partner reports separately, this effect was
Conflict resolution. Two items, rated on a scale from 0 (none/not at significant regardless of whose reports of positive engagement we
all) to 10 (a great deal/extremely), were used to assess both partners’ analyzed: Individuals with higher global perceived support saw
feelings about how well the conflict was resolved 1 week after the con-
themselves as engaging more positively in the conflict, and their
versation: “At this point, to what extent is the conflict you talked about in
partners saw them that way as well.
your previous session resolved?” and “At this point, to what extent have
you and your partner moved in the right direction to resolve the conflict The analyses also showed that an individual’s perceived support
you talked about in your previous session?” These two items were aver- predicted the partner’s positive engagement: partner effect ⫽ .35,
aged to create conflict resolution scores. Actual scores covered the full p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ (.26, .45). Again, the follow-up analyses
range of the scale. Alphas were .80 for men and .81 for women. indicated that this effect was significant with both data sources:
Controls. As in Part 1, we examined Extraversion, Neuroticism, self- Individuals who were high in global perceived support saw their
esteem, both partners’ ages, length of relationship, and cohabitation status partners as engaging more positively in the conflict, and their
as control variables. We also included a measure of relationship satisfac- partners shared that perception.
tion (the CSS from Part 1) to rule out the possibility that participants were In an additional follow-up analysis that included intensity of
simply saying good things about how they resolved conflict because they disagreement as a control variable, perceived support still had
were generally satisfied with their relationships.
significant actor and partner effects on positive engagement. In-
tensity of disagreement did not have significant actor or partner
Results and Discussion effects in this analysis ( ps ⬎ .25).
Did positive engagement predict better resolution 1 week later?
Did Optimists Have Less Intense Disagreements? Individuals who engaged more positively in the conflict conver-
sation reported better conflict resolution 1 week later: actor ef-
We examined the effect of optimism on both individuals’ ratings fect ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .002, 95% CI ⫽ (.13, .41). Their partners also saw
of the intensity of disagreement in their relationships. Optimists the conflict as better resolved: partner effect ⫽ .26, p ⫽ .002, 95%
and their partners described their disagreements as somewhat less CI ⫽ (.11, .40). These analyses held up regardless of the data
intense: actor effect ⫽ ⫺.15, p ⫽ .02, 95% CI ⫽ (–.29, ⫺.02); source for the positive engagement variable.
partner effect ⫽ ⫺.16, p ⫽ .01, 95% CI ⫽ (–.30, ⫺.03). Thus, we Was the effect of optimism on conflict resolution mediated by
included intensity of disagreement as a control variable when perceived support and positive conflict resolution? We tested for
testing the effects of optimism on conflict resolution. mediation by evaluating whether the mediated paths from opti-
150 SRIVASTAVA, MCGONIGAL, RICHARDS, BUTLER, AND GROSS

mism, through perceived support, through positive engagement, to ners’ ages, length of relationship, cohabitation status, relationship
conflict resolution were significant. The compound mediated path satisfaction, intensity of disagreement, and which partner’s topic
from optimism to the optimist’s own report of conflict resolution was (randomly) selected by the experimenter. All of the links in
was significant: mediated actor effect ⫽ .07, p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ the double-mediation model remained significant when each of
(.03, .12). The compound mediated path from optimism to the these control variables was included.
partner’s conflict resolution was also significant: mediated partner Part 2 showed that optimism was associated not just with global
effect ⫽ .07, p ⫽ .001, 95% CI ⫽ (.04, .12). This analysis relationship satisfaction but also with how well both partners
supported the mediation hypothesis. perceived their engagement and resolution of a significant area of
Was the mediation full or partial? In the double-mediation conflict in a relationship. This effect seemed to be partially driven
model, there are six ways that direct effects could have “bypassed” by optimists’ tendency to perceive their partners as supportive,
the mediated pathways. Optimism could have had a direct effect on which not only led optimists to engage more positively in discuss-
an actor’s own conflict resolution or on a partner’s conflict reso- ing the conflict (according to both optimists and their partners) but
lution that was not mediated by perceived support or positive also elicited more positive engagement from their partners as well.
engagement. Optimism could have had direct actor or partner Our emphasis on the participants’ reports of engagement in the
effects on positive engagement that were not mediated by per- conflict discussion and resolution of the conflict allowed us to gain
ceived support. Additionally, perceived support could have had valuable insights into their relationships. By asking both partners
direct actor or partner effects on conflict resolution that were not for their assessments of their own engagement, their partners’
mediated by positive engagement. engagement, and the conflict resolution, we were able to assess the
To test these various possibilities together, we took advantage of shared social reality of these intimate relationships. Nevertheless,
Amos’s model-comparison capabilities to test models with full and if optimism affects the shared reality of a relationship, then at some
partial mediation. In the full-mediation model, depicted in Figure point that shared reality might affect outcomes that are objectively
1, we allowed only effects from optimism to perceived support, verifiable. For Part 3, we examined what is literally the ultimate
perceived support to positive engagement, and positive engage- relationship outcome: relationship dissolution.
ment to conflict resolution. (As noted earlier, we tested for mod-
erating effects of gender by constraining men’s and women’s paths Part 3: The 1-Year Follow-Up
to be equal; thus a ⫽ a’, b ⫽ b’, and so on. No gender moderation
was found, so we report the results of the analysis with equality Parts 1 and 2 demonstrated that optimism was associated with
constraints.) The second model, called the partial-mediation relationship satisfaction and subjective conflict resolution, largely
model, was a less-restricted model that added all of the previously owing to the association between optimism and the perception of
described indirect paths to the model depicted in Figure 1. greater social support. These findings suggest that optimism influ-
On its own, the full-mediation model was a good fit to the data: ences relationship processes relevant to relationship maintenance
␹2(22, N ⫽ 108) ⫽ 25.1, p ⫽ .29, NFI ⫽ .90, RMSEA ⫽ .036. and survival. In Part 3, we examined whether the dating couples
However, compared with the partial-mediation model, the full- were still together 1 year after the initial phases of the study.
mediation model’s fit was slightly worse: ⌬␹2(6, N ⫽ 108) ⫽ 12.7, Because dating relationships are not as enduring as marriages, it
p ⫽ .05. When we examined the individual paths in the partial- was reasonable to expect that enough relationships would have
mediation model, we found that all of the effects specified in the ended that we could test for effects of optimism on relationship
full-mediation model were still significant. In addition, however, longevity. We hypothesized that optimism would be associated
optimism had a direct effect on a partner’s conflict resolution (i.e., with relationship status at the 1-year follow-up and that this effect
its effect was partially but not wholly explained by the mediating would be mediated by perceived support.
variables): direct partner effect ⫽ .12, p ⫽ .04, 95% CI ⫽ (.01,
.26). Method
Control Analyses We attempted to contact all original participants via e-mail 1 year after
their participation in Part 2. Because data collection for Part 2 spanned
We separately analyzed each link in the double-mediation several months, we contacted couples within 1 week of the 1-year anni-
model controlling for Extraversion, Neuroticism, self-esteem, part- versary of their participation in Part 2. If neither member of a couple

Figure 1. The full-mediation model for Part 2. Error variances are not shown; men’s and women’s error
variances for the same measure were allowed to covary.
OPTIMISM IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 151

responded within that week, we contacted them a second and third time, via satisfaction was a significant predictor of relationship longevity
e-mail and phone. Through this procedure, we were able to obtain rela- ( ps ⬎ .42). The effect of men’s perceived support was in the
tionship status information from at least one member of 101 (94%) of the expected direction but was not statistically significant ( p ⫽ .10).
couples. Analyses showed that members of responding couples and non- Given the sample size, it should perhaps not be surprising that in
responding couples did not differ significantly on measures of optimism or
a model with six moderately intercorrelated predictors, the pre-
relationship satisfaction.
dicted effect was only marginally significant.
We asked all participants whether they were still in an exclusive dating
relationship with their partner. Couples responding yes were coded as still Part 3 showed that optimism is associated with an important
together at 1 year (1), and couples reporting no were coded as having social outcome: relationship survival. Intriguingly, this analysis
broken up (0). In the responding sample, 67 couples (66%) were still showed a sex difference, with men’s optimism being the important
together at the 1-year follow-up, and 34 had broken up. predictor; this was in contrast to the other analyses, which indi-
cated that men’s and women’s optimism did not have different
effects on perceived support, relationship satisfaction, or conflict
Results and Discussion
resolution processes.
Did Optimism Predict Relationship Status at 1 Year? Why might male optimism play a more important role than
female optimism in predicting relationship survival? One possible
We hypothesized that greater optimism would be associated explanation has to do with the nature of men’s and women’s social
with a higher probability of being together at a 1-year follow-up. support networks. A number of studies have suggested that men
To test this hypothesis, we performed a logistic regression with tend to rely more heavily on romantic partners for social support,
couple as the unit of analysis, using both male and female opti- whereas women tend to draw on a wider network of family and
mism to predict the couple’s 1-year status. Greater male optimism friends (e.g., Taylor et al., 2000; Voss, Markiewicz, & Doyle,
predicted relationship survival, B ⫽ 0.03, Wald ⫽ 7.53, p ⫽ .006, 1999; Walen & Lachman, 2000). Thus, the tendency of male
but female optimism did not predict relationship survival, B ⫽ pessimists to perceive their partners as less supportive might be
0.01, Wald ⫽ 0.48, p ⫽ .49. To illustrate this effect, we split the especially likely to produce shorter lived relationships, because for
couples into two groups according to the male optimism median men, negative perceptions of their partners would implicate their
and examined survival for each group. We found that 75% of entire support system and give them greater incentive to terminate
couples with men at or above the median were still together at 1 the relationship.
year, contrasted with 54% of couples with men below the median.
General Discussion
Did Perceived Support Mediate the Effects of Optimism
on Relationship Longevity? In a longitudinal study of dating couples, we found that opti-
mism was associated with better relationship outcomes in a num-
To evaluate whether perceived support mediated the effect of ber of domains. Part 1 found that optimists and their partners both
optimism on relationship longevity, we added male and female experienced greater overall relationship satisfaction; Part 2 found
perceived support as predictors in the logistic regression model. In that optimists and their partners saw themselves and each other as
this second model, male perceived support was a significant pre- engaging more positively in the conflict and as reaching a better
dictor, B ⫽ .06, Wald ⫽ 6.21, p ⫽ .01, the effect of male optimism resolution; and Part 3 found that the relationships of male optimists
was reduced, B ⫽ .02, Wald ⫽ 3.28, p ⫽ .07, and female optimism lasted longer than the relationships of male pessimists. Further-
and perceived support were not significant predictors ( ps ⬎ .75). more, all of the relationship consequences of optimism were me-
We evaluated the reduction in the effect of male optimism by diated by optimists’ tendency to perceive their partners as
computing a bootstrap confidence interval for the difference be- supportive.
tween the male optimism effect in the first and second models; the
confidence interval did not include a null effect, 95% CI ⫽ (.001, How Does Perceived Support Affect the Relational
.024), consistent with mediation. The effects of men’s optimism Environment?
and perceived support remained significant when controlling for
both partners’ Extraversion, Neuroticism, and self-esteem. Why should optimism be an asset in close relationships? This
Analyses controlling for relationship satisfaction were consis- study provided some insight into why optimism may lead to more
tent with our main conclusions, though they yielded somewhat satisfying and longer lasting relationships by identifying perceived
more complicated results. In a logistic regression in which men’s support as a mediator. Perceived support was hypothesized to be
and women’s relationship satisfaction were the only predictors, an important relational mediator because it creates a more adaptive
there was a significant effect of men’s relationship satisfaction, relational environment. We believe that perceived support proba-
B ⫽ .04, Wald ⫽ 7.59, p ⫽ .006, but not of women’s relationship bly helps relationships in a variety of ways. First, optimists’
satisfaction, B ⫽ ⫺.01, Wald ⫽ 0.27, p ⫽ .61. In a regression that tendency to perceive their partners as supportive may act as a
included both optimism and relationship satisfaction, there were buffer against negative attributions. Relationships in which indi-
significant effects both of men’s optimism ( p ⫽ .03) and men’s viduals attribute their partners’ negative behaviors to global, sta-
relationship satisfaction ( p ⫽ .03), indicating that men’s relation- ble, voluntary dispositions rather than narrow and temporary in-
ship satisfaction did not account for the effect of men’s optimism clinations tend to be marked by lower relationship satisfaction and
on longevity. When we ran the full mediational model with a other maladaptive outcomes (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). Opti-
control for relationship satisfaction, the results were in the gener- mists may attribute specific instances of unsupportive or ambigu-
ally expected direction but were not as clear as without the con- ous behavior to temporary and situationally limited states. Second,
trols. In this analysis, neither men’s nor women’s relationship optimists’ positive views of their partners may prevent or interrupt
152 SRIVASTAVA, MCGONIGAL, RICHARDS, BUTLER, AND GROSS

cycles of negative reciprocity by refocusing optimists’ attention on was strengthened by an analysis controlling for Time 1 levels of
the constructive things that their partners do and say, instead of on relationship satisfaction. In Part 3, relationship dissolution obvi-
their partners’ negative affect (Gottman, 1998). Third, optimists ously is an outcome that temporally follows all other characteris-
may be better at acting as a “secure base” (Bowlby, 1988; Hazan tics of the relationship.
& Shaver, 1987), providing their partners with a reliable source of The strengths of our naturalistic approach are balanced against
support. As a result, optimists and their partners may be more the limitations of a nonexperimental design, however, and we
satisfied because they feel that their relationship helps them pur- acknowledge that this sequencing is not airtight. It could be ar-
sue their personal goals (Brunstein et al., 1996; Kaplan & gued, for example, that perceptions of support are a consequence,
Maddux, 2002). rather than a cause, of higher quality relationships (Metts, Geist, &
Gray, 1994). From this perspective, individuals may form their
Optimism, Shared Reality, and Positive Illusions perceptions of their partner’s supportiveness based on some other
aspect of the relationship. We partially addressed this concern by
Did optimists and their partners benefit from positive illusions controlling for relationship satisfaction and other relationship char-
(Taylor & Brown, 1988)? In conceptualizing this study, we have acteristics in Part 2, but we cannot fully rule out the possibility that
sidestepped questions about accuracy and bias, instead focusing on some other, unmeasured feature of the relationship acted as a third
perceptions of support and on both partners’ assessment of positive variable.
engagement and conflict resolution. Partners’ beliefs and percep- Our reliance on partners’ reports about the conflict conversation
tions of themselves and each other define the shared reality of a in Part 2 might be regarded as a double-edged sword. As implied
relationship, a reality that is important independent of any objec- earlier, this approach gave us insight into the shared reality of the
tive analysis of accuracy (Gable et al., 2003). In support of the relationship, and the results showed that both self- and partner
value of such an approach, Part 3 suggests that shared reality can reports from both members of the couple led to the same conclu-
have very real consequences for the long-term success of a sions about conversation processes. In fact, if an “objective”
relationship. observer failed to corroborate the positive conversation processes
How might we apply a different perspective to our findings? evident in optimists’ relationships, we might have cause to suspect
Drawing on a positive illusions perspective, we could interpret the the observer rather than the couple. Nevertheless, it would be
results in Part 1 as stemming from positive illusions that optimists interesting in future research to examine more objectively the
hold about their relationships. In Part 2, we could conclude that specific processes that we believe are being promoted by opti-
such illusions drive optimists to practice and elicit “objectively” mists: adaptive attributions, interruption of cycles of negative
better conflict-related behavior; or alternatively, perhaps both part- reciprocity, and use of the relationship as a secure base. Such
ners share an illusion about how they handle conflict. We believe research would elucidate the mechanisms that link optimists’ pos-
that the former interpretation is more compatible with other find- itive expectations to the fulfillment of these expectations.
ings about positive illusions in close relationships (e.g., Murray &
Holmes, 1997), though the present data cannot speak strongly to References
questions of illusion or accuracy.
Arbuckle, J. L. (1999). Amos 4.0. Chicago: SmallWaters Corporation.
Relatedly, perceived support may function as a self-fulfilling
Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1992). Modeling cognitive adaptation:
prophecy: By virtue of optimists’ general tendency to see their A longitudinal investigation of the impact of individual differences and
partners as supportive, they may elicit actual support from their coping on college adjustment and performance. Journal of Personality
partners. That would explain why optimists report using social and Social Psychology, 63, 989 –1003.
support as a coping strategy in general (Scheier, Carver, & Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
Bridges, 1994) but not in response to specific, everyday stressors distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and
(Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Brissette et al., 2002): Optimists statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
expect to receive support from others, but they do not directly ask 51, 1173–1182.
for it. Barrera, M. Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts,
measures, and models. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14,
413– 455.
Limitations and Future Directions Belsher, G., & Costello, C. G. (1991). Do confidants of depressed women
provide less social support than confidants of nondepressed women?
Throughout this article, we have adopted the perspective that Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 516 –525.
optimism leads to perceived support, which leads to positive Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent– child attachment and healthy
relationship outcomes. This was based in large part on theoretical human development. New York: Basic Books.
considerations: We ordered the constructs from optimism, the most Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in marriage:
general and broad-based construct, to perceived support, which is Review and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 3–33.
more domain-specific but still fairly broad as an individual differ- Brissette, I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of optimism
ence (Sarason et al., 1986), to outcomes relevant to a specific in social network development, coping, and psychological adjustment
relationship (Part 1) or specific events within that relationship during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
82, 102–111.
(Parts 2 and 3), which are the most contextual. There is also a
Brunstein, J. C., Dangelmayer, G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (1996). Personal
temporal logic to the order of precedence: Optimism is a mean- goals and social support in close relationships: Effects on relationship
ingful construct even for an individual who has no experience or mood and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
beliefs about close relationships, and general perceptions of sup- chology, 71, 1006 –1019.
port can preexist specific experiences in relationships. In Part 2, Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotional
this temporal ordering was reflected in the design of the study and behavior in long-term marriage. Psychology and Aging, 10, 140 –149.
OPTIMISM IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 153

Carver, C. S., Kus, L. A., & Scheier, M. F. (1994). Effects of good versus among nursing professionals. In B. R. Burleson, T. L. Albrecht, & I. G.
bad mood and optimistic versus pessimistic outlook on social acceptance Sarason (Eds.), Communication of social support (pp. 113–135). Thou-
versus rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 138 – sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
151. Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). A leap of faith? Positive illusions
Chang, E. C. (2001). Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, in romantic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
research, and practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological 23, 586 – 604.
Association. Newcomb, M. D. (1990). What structural equation modeling can tell us
Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1999). The problem of about social support. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce
units and the circumstance for POMP. Multivariate Behavioral Re- (Eds.), Social support: An interactionist view (pp. 26 – 63). New York:
search, 34, 315–346. Wiley.
Coleridge, S. T. (1881). Reason for love’s blindness. In Bartlett’s familiar Park, C. L., & Folkman, S. (1997). Stability and change in psychosocial
quotations. Retrieved from http://www.bartleby.com/100/ resources during caregiving and bereavement in partners of men with
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1990). Couple Communication Question- AIDS. Journal of Personality, 65, 421– 447.
naire. Unpublished questionnaire, University of California. Pierce, G. R., Sarason, B. R., & Sarason, I. G. (1992). General and specific
Cozarelli, C. (1993). Personality and self-efficacy as predictors of coping support expectations and stress as predictors of perceived supportive-
with abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1224 – ness: An experimental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
1236. ogy, 63, 297–307.
Dehle, C., Larsen, D., & Landers, J. E. (2001). Social support in marriage. Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in
American Journal of Family Therapy, 29, 307–324. romantic relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feel-
Dougall, A. L., Hyman, K. B., Hayward, M. C., McFeeley, S., & Baum, A. ings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 – 620.
(2001). Optimism and traumatic stress: The importance of social support Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
and coping. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 223–245. NJ: Princeton University Press.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Downey, G. (2003). He said, she said: A Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, B. R. (1983).
quasi-signal detection analysis of daily interactions between close rela- Assessing social support: The Social Support Questionnaire. Journal of
tionship partners. Psychological Science, 14, 100 –105. Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 127–139.
Geers, A. L., Reilley, S. P., & Dember, W. N. (1998). Optimism, pessi- Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., Shearin, E. N. (1986). Social support as an
mism, and friendship. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, individual difference variable: Its stability, origins, and relational as-
Personality, Social, 17, 3–19. pects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 845– 855.
Gottman, J. M. (1998). Psychology and the study of the marital processes. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health:
Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 169 –197. Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.
Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977). The topography of Health Psychology, 4, 219 –247.
marital conflict: A sequential analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 461– 477. optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Person-
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, ality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078.
511–524. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (2001). Optimism,
Helgeson, V. S. (1994). The effects of self-beliefs and relationship beliefs pessimism, and psychological well-being. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Opti-
on adjustment to a relationship stressor. Personal Relationships, 1, mism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice
241–258. (pp. 189 –216). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2003). Mediation in experimental and non-
measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho-
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. logical Methods, 7, 422– 445.
102–138). New York: Guilford Press. Stafford, L., & Canary, D. J. (1991). Maintenance strategies and romantic
Kaplan, M., & Maddux, J. E. (2002). Goals and marital satisfaction: relationship type, gender and relational characteristics. Journal of Social
Perceived support for personal goals and collective efficacy for collec- and Personal Relationships, 8, 217–242.
tive goals. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 157–164. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being. A social
Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (1997). The analysis of data from dyads and psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103,
groups. In H. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods 193–210.
in social psychology (pp. 451– 477). New York: Cambridge University Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald,
Press. T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health.
Lakey, B., & Cassady, P. B. (1990). Cognitive processes in perceived American Psychologist, 55, 99 –109.
support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 337–348. Voss, K., Markiewicz, D., Doyle, A. B. (1999). Friendship, marriage and
Lakey, B., McCabe, K. M., Fisicaro, S. A., & Drew, J. B. (1996). Envi- self-esteem. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 103–122.
ronmental and personal determinants of support perceptions: Three Walen, H. R., & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Social support and strain from
generalizability studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, partner, family, and friends: Costs and benefits for men and women in
70, 1270 –1280. adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 5–30.
Lund, M. (1985). The development of investment and commitment scales
for predicting continuity of personal relationships. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 2, 3–23. Received August 11, 2003
Metts, S., Geist, P., & Gray, J. L. (1994). The role of relationship charac- Revision received February 6, 2006
teristics in the provision and effectiveness of supportive messages Accepted February 7, 2006 䡲
SOCIAL EXCLUSION AND PAIN 15

ness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. Journal of Person- Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P.
ality and Social Psychology, 85, 409 – 423. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp.
Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The 345– 411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
relationship between social support and physiological processes: A Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Kurtz, J., Dunn, E., & Gilbert, D. T. (2004).
review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for When to fire: Anticipatory versus post-event reconstrual of uncontrol-
health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488 –531. lable events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 340 –351.
Van Boven, L., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Social projection of transient Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D.
drive states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1159 – (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting.
1168. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821– 836.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali- Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go?
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of
Wiedenmayer, C. P., Goodwin, G. A., & Barr, G. A. (2000). The effect of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 560 –567.
periaqueductal gray lesions on responses to age-specific threats in infant
rats. Developmental Brain Research, 120, 191–198.
Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). CyberOstracism: Received February 24, 2005
Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Revision received August 15, 2005
Social Psychology, 79, 748 –762. Accepted August 25, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 154 –170 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.154

Discrepancies Between Explicit and Implicit Self-Concepts: Consequences


for Information Processing

Pablo Briñol Richard E. Petty


Universidad Autónoma de Madrid Ohio State University

S. Christian Wheeler
Stanford University

Individuals with discrepancies among their explicit beliefs often engage in greater elaboration of
discrepancy-related information in a presumed attempt to reduce the discrepancy. The authors predicted
that individuals with discrepancies between explicit and implicit self-conceptions might similarly be
motivated to engage in processing of discrepancy-related information, even though they might not be
aware of the discrepancy. Four studies were conducted in which various self-dimensions were assessed
with explicit and implicit measures. Across several different self-dimensions (e.g., need to evaluate,
self-esteem), the authors found that as the discrepancy between the explicit and implicit measure
increased (regardless of direction), people engaged in more thinking about information framed as related
to the self-dimension on which the discrepancy existed. This research suggests that individuals might be
motivated to examine relevant information as a strategy to minimize the implicit doubt that accompanies
an inconsistency between explicit and implicit self-conceptions.

Keywords: discrepancy, explicit–implicit, self, persuasion, attitude change

The psychological literature has clearly documented that people subjective discomfort that results from the discrepancy (e.g.,
can simultaneously hold incompatible explicit beliefs, attitudes, Hänze, 2001; Hodson, Maio, & Esses, 2001; Jonas, Diehl, &
feelings, and behavioral tendencies regarding oneself and others Bromer, 1997; Katz, Wackenhut, & Hass, 1986).
(e.g., Abelson & Rosenberg, 1958; Bem & Allen, 1974; Brehm & For example, Maio, Bell, and Esses (1996) measured partic-
Cohen, 1962; Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1997; Heider, 1958; ipants’ ambivalence regarding the issue of immigration to Can-
Higgins, 1987; Kaplan, 1972; Newcomb, 1968; Norton, 1975; ada (i.e., the extent to which they had both positive and negative
Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955; Priester & Petty, 1996). Virtually reactions to the issue) and then exposed them to a discrepancy-
every relevant theory holds that such internal discrepancies tend to related message favoring immigration from Hong Kong to
be unpleasant and can result in psychologically undesirable out- Canada that contained either strong or weak arguments. The
comes (e.g., Campbell, 1990; Carver & Scheier, 1990; Greenier,
extent to which participants processed the message information
Kernis, & Waschull, 1995; Higgins, 1987; Kernis & Waschull,
was assessed by examining the extent to which the quality of
1995). Because of this, people often attempt to resolve these
the arguments affected postmessage immigration attitudes
internal discrepancies. Perhaps the most common approach to
(Petty, Wells, & Brock, 1976). When people are thinking care-
addressing discrepancy is enhanced thinking or information pro-
cessing (e.g., Abelson et al., 1968; Aronson, 1969; Festinger, fully about information, they should be affected by the quality
1957; Heider, 1958; Hass, Katz, Rizzo, Bailey, & Moore, 1992). of the arguments a message contains (see Petty & Cacioppo,
By considering additional information, individuals may hope to 1986). As expected, Maio et al. (1996) found that individuals
gain enough information for one or the other side of the discrep- who had ambivalent attitudes toward immigration were more
ancy to resolve or minimize the inconsistency, or at least the influenced by argument quality than were unambivalent indi-
viduals, suggesting that they engaged in enhanced scrutiny of
the information.
Pablo Briñol, Department of Social Psychology and Methodology, Uni- Although research has focused extensively on explicit discrepan-
versidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; Richard E. Petty, Depart- cies, relatively little work has examined the potential existence of and
ment of Psychology, Ohio State University; S. Christian Wheeler, Graduate consequences of discrepancies in which one cognitive element may
School of Business, Stanford University. not be easily reported. Theory suggests that just as people can hold
We thank all of the members of the 2000 –2004 Groups for Attitudes and conscious, explicit self-beliefs, they may also hold less conscious (or
Persuasion at Ohio State University for providing feedback on the exper- explicitly denied) automatic self-associations that can conflict with the
iments reported in this article. We also thank Javier Horcajo and Isamel
more consciously endorsed ones (e.g., Greenwald et al., 2002). The
Gallardo for their help with data collection.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Richard present research examines the information-processing consequences
E. Petty, Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, 1835 Neil of discrepancies between self-conceptions as assessed with explicit
Avenue Mall, Columbus, OH 43210-1222. E-mail: petty.1@osu.edu and implicit measures of individual differences.

154
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 155

The Relationship Between the Implicit and the Explicit Simultaneous Activity of Explicit–Implicit Self-Discrepant
Self-Concept Dimensions

Compared with an explicit or conscious self-conception, an If discrepant explicit and implicit constructs always operated in
implicit self-view is generally defined as one that is held different situations (e.g., spontaneous vs. deliberative), then they
outside of conscious awareness, or at least is an association that should lead to little conflict in any given situation. But if the two
is not endorsed consciously (Fazio & Olson, 2003; Petty, self-conceptions were ever jointly activated or operative, there
Wheeler, & Tormala, 2003). Such self-conceptions can influ- would be the possibility of some consequences that mirror incon-
ence judgment and action automatically without the need for gruency between discrepant explicit constructs (Petty et al., 2006).
reflection and may only be apparent on disguised or implicit In fact, there is some evidence suggesting that possessing discrep-
measures. Considerable research has examined the predictive ant implicit and explicit self-concepts can be consequential. For
utility of such measures in the domains of motivation (e.g., example, Shedler, Mayman, and Manis (1993) studied individuals
McClelland, 1985), memory (e.g., Lindsay & Johnson, 1989), who reported minimal emotional disturbance on Eysenck’s Neu-
personality (e.g., Bornstein, 1995), self-esteem (e.g., Green- roticism scale (explicit measure) but who were simultaneously
wald & Banaji, 1995; Hetts, Sakuma, & Pelham, 1999), atti- judged on the basis of their early memories (implicit measure) to
tudes (e.g., Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998; Fazio, be relatively disturbed. In comparison with participants in the
Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995), and stereotypes (e.g., genuine self-esteem group (who scored as healthy on both mea-
Blair & Banaji, 1996; Dovidio, Kawakami, & Beach, 2001). sures), those with explicit–implicit discrepancies were signifi-
Currently the most popular implicit measures of self-conception cantly more reactive on a combined index of heart rate and blood
pressure and scored higher on behavioral indices of anxiety (see
are based on reaction times, for example, the Implicit Associ-
also Weinberger & Haradaway, 1990).
ation Test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998) and the Automatic
In conceptually similar research, Zelenski and Larsen (2003)
Evaluation Task (Fazio et al., 1995). These measures capitalize
found that having incongruent explicit (e.g., self-ratings) and im-
on automatic associations between the self and other con-
plicit (measured by the Thematic Apperception Test; Proshansky,
structs—reactions that may occur too quickly to come under
1943) motive profiles was associated with reduced emotional
deliberative control (see Petty, Fazio, & Briñol, in press, for
well-being. Other recent research has demonstrated that people
reviews).1
who scored relatively high on an explicit measure of self-esteem,
Sometimes, what is assessed with implicit and explicit measures
but relatively low on an implicit measure (the IAT), exhibited the
is the same, suggesting that there is just one underlying motive,
most self-aggrandizement across different indices (Bosson,
attitude, personality, or self-conception that is open to conscious
Brown, Zeigler-Hill, & Swann, 2003), which is the main charac-
awareness. However, explicit and implicit assessments are not teristic of a narcissistic personality (e.g., Wing & Gough, 1990).
always congruent. Although there are various explanations for this Additionally, individuals with the combination of relatively high
incongruency, some researchers have developed theoretical frame- scores on explicit measures of self-esteem and relatively low
works that account for the divergence by proposing that explicit scores on implicit measures have been shown to be particularly
and implicit constructs sometimes stem from two independent defensive (Jordan, Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Browne, & Correll,
systems that operate in different contexts and influence different 2003).
types of behavior. For example, Dovidio et al. (2001) have argued Finally, we have found that discrepancy between explicit and
that self-report (explicit) and response latency (implicit) measures implicit self-esteem scores is associated with implicit but not
of attitudes can tap into different evaluations that predict behaviors explicit self-doubt (Briñol, Petty, & Wheeler, 2003). Specifically,
in different situations (spontaneous vs. deliberative; see also Carl- as explicit–implicit self-esteem discrepancy (as assessed using the
ston & Skowronski, 1986; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, in press; absolute value of the difference between participants’ standardized
Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). A similar distinction has explicit and implicit self-esteem scores) increased, the strength
been made in the domain of motivation (see, e.g., McClelland, with which participants automatically associated doubt words with
1985; Patten & White, 1977). self-words on an IAT also increased. However, increased discrep-
Indeed, Carver (2005) documented that psychologists have pro- ancy was not associated with explicit reports of self-uncertainty.
mulgated a wide array of dual system approaches (e.g., Lieberman, Similarly, recent research has shown that when people’s attitudes
2000; Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999; Shastri & Ajjanagadde, 1993; change on explicit measures, they show more doubt on an implicit
Sloman, 1996; Smith & DeCoster, 2000; Smolensky, 1988). Al- but not explicit measure of confidence regarding the attitude object
though the various frameworks differ in some details, each has compared with people whose attitudes have not changed (Petty et
tended to emphasize the fact that some behavior is guided in a al., 2006). Because recent research also shows that when attitudes
relatively automatic and unconscious way, whereas other behavior
is guided in a more deliberative and conscious way. For example,
1
cognitive– experiential self-theory (Epstein, 1973, 2003) argues When not compatible with the endorsed (explicit) self-conception, the
self-associations measured with contemporary implicit measures can rep-
that humans are characterized by two independent, interactive
resent many things (e.g., repressed evaluations of oneself, prior evaluations
systems of thinking that jointly determine behavior: a preconscious
of oneself, others’ evaluations of oneself, hopes for oneself, strong societal
experiential system and a conscious rational system. Similarly, the prescriptions for self-conduct, and so forth). We postulate that regardless of
reflective–impulsive model (Strack & Deutsch, 2004) postulates the particular source of the association, inconsistency with one’s con-
two systems— one thoughtful and one more automatic—that op- sciously endorsed self-view can lead to psychological conflict (see Petty, in
erate in parallel and interact with one another. press; Petty, Tormala, Briñol, & Jarvis, 2006; Priester & Petty, 2001).
156 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

change, a residue of the prior attitude may still be available on an though the message actually contained discrepancy-unrelated in-
implicit (automatic) measure (e.g., Gregg, Seibt, & Banaji, 2006), formation. This manipulation was expected to moderate the impact
the enhanced implicit doubt that accompanies attitude change is of discrepancy on information processing. If thinking is aimed at
plausibly due to a discrepancy between old (implicit/automatic) reducing the discrepancy, then explicit–implicit incongruence
and new (explicit/deliberative) evaluations. should be associated with enhanced thinking only when partici-
Together, these studies suggest that having discrepant explicit pants expect the message to be related to the self-dimension on
and implicit self-dimensions is associated with numerous conse- which there is a discrepancy (i.e., when it presumably could help
quences that often appear to be negative, unpleasant, or dysfunc- reduce the discrepancy). When the same information is framed as
tional. Because of this, people with such discrepancies should be unrelated, it should be useless in resolving the discrepancy.
motivated to engage in some discrepancy resolution. Specifically, Thus, in studies in which the information was presumed relevant
we postulate that like possession of an explicit discrepancy (e.g., to the discrepancy (Studies 1 and 2), we predicted a two-way
attitudinal ambivalence), possession of a discrepancy between interaction on attitudes between size of the explicit–implicit dis-
implicit and explicit self-conceptions will be associated with at- crepancy and information quality. Information quality should have
tempts at discrepancy reduction, and that this would occur regard- a greater impact on attitudes as discrepancy increases. In studies in
less of the direction of the discrepancy. As described earlier, by which we manipulated the presumed relevance of the information
thinking about information presumed relevant to the issue on to the discrepancy, a three-way interaction on attitudes was ex-
which there is a discrepancy, individuals with explicit–implicit pected. Specifically, discrepancy should affect information pro-
discrepancies can possibly restore internal consistency. Thus, the cessing primarily when the information is framed as relevant. We
purpose of the present research was to examine the influence of did not expect these results to depend on the direction of the
explicit–implicit discrepancies on information processing. We ex- discrepancy (i.e., whether implicit scores were relatively higher or
amined conditions under which the discrepancy results both from lower than explicit scores). To the extent these hypotheses are
one’s explicit self-conception being relatively higher (more posi- supported, it would suggest that the presence of discrepant explicit
tive) than one’s implicit self-conception within the sample distri- and implicit self-dimensions, regardless of direction, is associated
bution as well as from one’s explicit self-conception being rela- with enhanced thinking. The enhanced thinking presumably re-
tively lower (less positive) than one’s implicit self-conception. flects an attempt at discrepancy reduction.

Overview of the Present Research


Experiment 1
We hypothesized that to reduce a discrepancy between explicit
and implicit self-conceptions, individuals with such discrepancies Experiment 1 was conducted to provide an initial test of the
would engage in more effortful elaboration of information pre- notion that divergence between explicit and implicit self-views can
sumed relevant to the self-dimension on which the discrepancy influence the extent of processing of information relevant to those
exists. To test this hypothesis, we conducted four studies in which self-views. In our first experiment, we used the trait of shyness as
explicit and implicit assessments of the self along four different a specific dimension of personality. There were several reasons for
dimensions were collected. In each study we used a standard this selection. First, shyness is a personality trait that is well
self-report as the explicit measure and an IAT as the implicit represented in common language and lay psychology, is easily
measure. The IAT was selected because (a) it taps into automatic judged by oneself, and is readily observable by others (Asendorpf,
associations that are less subject to conscious control and (b) 1987, 1989). Second, because of its observability and ubiquity in
previously validated procedures were available for some of the lay psychology, the self-concept dimension of shyness is relatively
dimensions of interest. We then examined the impact of explicit– accessible (Asendorpf, 1990). Third, it is easy to select shyness-
implicit divergence on the extent of processing of information descriptive adjectives for both explicit self-ratings and an IAT for
framed as discrepancy related. shyness, because previous research has pretested instruments for
The logic was similar across the four studies. In each study, an its assessment (Asendorpf, 1987, 1989). Last, research conducted
index of the relative explicit–implicit discrepancy was formed for by Asendorpf, Banse, and Mücke (2002) has demonstrated that the
each participant following procedures used in prior research (see implicit measure of shyness uniquely predicted spontaneous (but
Kehr, 2004, for a discussion). Then, all participants were exposed not controlled) shy behavior, whereas the explicit ratings uniquely
to a persuasive message. The extent to which participants pro- predicted controlled (but not spontaneous) shy behavior. That is,
cessed the information in the message was assessed by using an Asendorpf et al. validated the implicit and explicit shyness mea-
information quality manipulation. As noted earlier, the impact that sures used in Study 1 as useful independent indicators of behavior.
the quality of information in a message has on resulting attitudes The explicit shyness measure consisted of a series of self-
is a widely used indicator of the extent to which individuals reported responses to shyness-related adjectives, whereas the im-
carefully attend to and think about the information to which they plicit measure was a shyness IAT (see Asendorpf et al., 2002). In
are exposed (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). this study, all of the participants were exposed to a persuasive
Finally, across studies we varied whether the information con- message containing either strong or weak arguments directly re-
tained in the message was actually relevant (Study 1) or simply lated to shyness. After reading the message, participants were
framed as relevant (Studies 2, 3, and 4) to the self-dimension on asked to report their attitudes toward the proposal in the message.
which discrepancy existed. In two studies (Studies 3 and 4), we We expected participants with a large explicit–implicit discrep-
manipulated the extent to which participants expected the message ancy to be more attentive to the persuasive message than those
to be relevant to the issue on which discrepancy existed, even with a small discrepancy. This enhanced thinking would be evi-
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 157

denced in greater attitudinal responsiveness to the argument qual- daughters choose shy people as friends and roommates in college because
ity manipulation. they tend to be more self-controlled.
It is important to note that both the strong and weak arguments argued
in favor of shyness, but the strong arguments provided more compelling
Method reasons than did the weak arguments. This manipulation can be distin-
guished from other forms of message variations, such as arguing either in
Participants and Design favor of or against the proposal. Because the argument manipulation is
used to assess how much thinking people are doing about the message, all
Eighty-one undergraduate psychology students at the Universidad Au- arguments need to argue for the same position— but only with high or low
tónoma de Madrid in Madrid, Spain, participated in partial fulfillment of a convincingness. Because both sets of arguments are ostensibly in favor of
course requirement. The students were randomly assigned to the argument the issue, they may be equally persuasive if people do not think about their
quality conditions (strong or weak). Additionally, explicit and implicit implications. Individuals not thinking about the message carefully may
shyness were assessed for all the participants to form an index of explicit– respond simply to the number of arguments presented or their initial gut
implicit discrepancy. The independent variables thereby constituted an reaction to the proposal (e.g., Petty & Cacioppo, 1984; see also Petty &
Argument Quality (strong vs. weak) ⫻ Explicit–Implicit Discrepancy Wegener, 1998). The more attention paid to the information provided,
(continuously scored) ⫻ Direction of Discrepancy (higher on explicit or however, the greater the difference in subsequent attitudes to strong versus
implicit measure) design. weak arguments.
Explicit measure of shyness. Shyness was assessed by asking partici-
Procedure pants to rate the extent to which 10 adjectives described them on 4-point
scales anchored by extremely uncharacteristic of me (1) and extremely
Upon arrival, participants were seated at individual computer stations characteristic of me (4). The adjectives were presented in random order
and completed the IAT for shyness. After the IAT task, all of the partic- and included words such as inhibited, insecure, timid, reticent, reserved,
ipants reported their explicit shyness and completed several other ancillary daring, candid, open, secure, and assertive. These words were taken from
questions. Participants were then told that because of extra time remaining Asendorpf et al. (2002) and were also the items used for the implicit
in the session, they would also be participating in another experiment measure in the present research. Ratings on the scale items were highly
designed to examine personality characteristics that were good for being a consistent with each other (␣ ⫽ .86) and were averaged (reverse scoring
psychologist. Participants received a message arguing that being shy was a where appropriate) to form a single index of explicit shyness for each
positive trait for a psychologist. The message contained either strong or participant.
weak arguments. All of the participants were told in advance that the Implicit measure of shyness. In the IAT measure of shyness, partici-
message they were about to read had to do with shyness. After reading the pants classified target concepts (represented by me or other) and attributes
message, participants were told that it was important to know what their (represented by shy or nonshy categories of words) using two designated
personal views were on the benefits of shyness. Thus, they completed a keys on a computer. The me category was represented by the words I, self,
measure of their attitudes toward shyness as a trait. Finally, the participants my, me, and own, whereas the other category was represented by the words
were thanked, asked for permission for their responses to be analyzed (all they, them, your, you, and other. Attributes related to shyness were selected
gave permission), and given an appointment for a meeting to provide them from the items on the explicit measure and included the words inhibited,
with feedback about their results. insecure, timid, reticent, and reserved. In contrast, nonshy attributes in-
cluded the words daring, candid, open, secure, and assertive. The test was
similar to the original IATs used by Greenwald et al. (1998) and paralleled
Independent Variables the one used by Asendorpf et al. (2002). There were seven blocks of trials.
Argument quality. Participants were exposed to a message containing Blocks 1, 2, and 5 were practice blocks for which participants made single
information directly relevant to shyness and thus to any explicit–implicit categorizations (me vs. other, and shy vs. nonshy). In the remaining blocks,
discrepancy on this trait. The shyness-related message participants received participants discriminated shy versus nonshy words and me versus other
contained either strong or weak arguments. This manipulation was de- words on separate trials within the same block. In Block 4, participants
signed to assess the extent to which people were attentive to the content of used one response key to indicate if a word belonged to the nonshy or other
the message (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). The arguments selected were categories and the other key if the word belonged to the shy or me
pretested and were shown to produce the appropriate pattern of cognitive categories. In Block 7, participants used one response key to indicate if a
responding. That is, the strong arguments elicited mostly favorable word belonged to the nonshy or me categories and the other key if the word
thoughts and the weak arguments elicited mostly unfavorable thoughts belonged to the shy or other categories. Blocks 3 and 6 were combined
when people were instructed to think carefully about them. In brief, 58 blocks that served as practice for Blocks 4 and 7. Only data from Blocks
students were asked to list their thoughts for each version of the message. 4 and 7 were used to compute IAT scores. The main dependent variable
Analysis of the thoughts listed revealed that, on average, the strong (IAT score) was computed by subtracting participants’ average response
message elicited more favorable (M ⫽ 1.86, SD ⫽ 1.80) than unfavorable latencies during Block 4 from their average response latencies during
(M ⫽ 0.65, SD ⫽ 1.07) thoughts, t(28) ⫽ 5.54, p ⬍ .001. For the weak Block 7. Positive differences in this index indicated faster automatic
version of the message, participants generated more unfavorable (M ⫽ associations between me and shy than between others and shy.
2.58, SD ⫽ 1.93) than favorable (M ⫽ 0.86, SD ⫽ 1.57) thoughts, t(29) ⫽ It is evident from the above description that the IAT is a relative
7.19, p ⬍ .001. The gist of one strong argument in favor of shyness was measure. For example, in the present studies, the category me is contrasted
that shy people have more introspective ability, a quality that was highly with the category others. Indeed, opposing me with others might make the
valuable in the workplace. The gist of another strong argument was that shy IAT scores difficult to interpret in certain cases because they can reflect
people have been rated as better friends and partners, because they tend to associations with the self, with the others, or a combination of the two (e.g.,
have interpersonal relationships that are more sincere, committed, stable, Karpinski, 2004). Particularly relevant to the present research, however,
and satisfactory. In contrast, the gist of one weak argument in favor of the explicit self-report scale also likely involved a similar relative judgment
shyness was that shy people tend to talk less than extraverted interviewers, compared with others. Most subjective judgments about the self or others
making other shy people feel more comfortable. The gist of another weak require reference to some standard—typically provided by others. For
argument was that some students’ parents prefer that their sons and example, as Mussweiler (2003) noted, “To characterize oneself as athletic
158 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

. . . implies that one is more athletic than others and is thus, in essence, a Results of this analysis revealed a significant main effect for
comparative statement” (p. 472). In fact, it would be hard for people to extent of discrepancy, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.25, t(73) ⫽ ⫺2.19, p ⫽ .03, showing
develop their self-conceptions without comparing with others (Festinger, that participants’ attitudes were less favorable toward shyness as
1954). Thus, it is plausible to argue that both the implicit and the explicit explicit–implicit discrepancy increased (all ␤s reported are stan-
measures of shyness might include a comparative component.
dardized coefficients). In addition, a significant interaction be-
Following typical procedures, stimuli in the IAT task appeared within a
tween argument quality and direction of the discrepancy emerged,
centered white window. Reminder labels were positioned on top of the
stimuli on the left and right side. These reminders read me and other for ␤ ⫽ .43, t(70) ⫽ 2.42, p ⫽ .02, revealing that argument quality had
single target-classification blocks, shy and nonshy for single attribute- a larger effect on attitudes for the explicit ⬎ implicit direction than
classification blocks. Mixed target ⫹ attribute blocks were also accompa- the implicit ⬎ explicit direction. This effect was not obtained in
nied by appropriate labels (e.g., shy or other; nonshy or me). Incorrect any of the other studies and is not discussed further.
classifications were followed by error feedback (i.e., the word ERROR). More critical to our hypothesis, a significant interaction between
Summary feedback was provided at the end of each practice block inform- argument quality and extent of discrepancy was evident, ␤ ⫽ .46,
ing participants about their average response latency and percentage of t(70) ⫽ 3.17, p ⫽ .002, revealing that as discrepancy increased,
errors for that block. All practice trials in the IAT were administered in five argument quality had a larger effect on attitudes. Specifically,
blocks. Data-collection trials, consisting of combined target ⫹ attribute
decomposition of this interaction by recentering discrepancy at one
classifications, were collected in four blocks. Within each block, stimuli
standard deviation (SD) above and below the mean (Aiken &
were randomly selected without replacement, and no more than two con-
secutively presented stimuli belonged to the same category. To correct for West, 1991) indicated that there was a significant effect of argu-
anticipatory responses and momentary inattention, we recorded latencies ment quality among participants with high discrepancy, ␤ ⫽ .36,
shorter than 300 ms and longer than 3,000 ms as 300 and 3,000 ms, t(69) ⫽ 2.37, p ⫽ .02, but not among those with relatively low
respectively. Response latencies were then log transformed to normalize discrepancy, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.25, t(69) ⫽ ⫺1.69, p ⫽ .10. The three-way
the distribution. Further details about the IAT procedure are provided by interaction between argument quality, extent of discrepancy, and
Greenwald et al. (1998). direction of the discrepancy was not significant, ␤ ⫽ .26, t(69) ⫽
Explicit–implicit discrepancy. The explicit and implicit measures of 1.14, p ⫽ .26, indicating that the effects of explicit–implicit
shyness were significantly correlated (r ⫽ .40, p ⬍ .001). This correlation discrepancy on elaboration were not restricted to any particular
is similar to those obtained by Asendorpf et al. (2002) using similar explicit
direction of the discrepancy.
and implicit measures in a different sample.2 An index of explicit–implicit
discrepancy was formed as the absolute value of the difference between the
standardized explicit and implicit measures of shyness. The discrepancy Discussion
index considers where people fall within the distribution of participants in
the study on the implicit versus explicit measures. A zero on the index
Experiment 1 demonstrated that as the discrepancy between
indicates that the person’s place in the distribution is exactly the same on implicit and explicit measures of shyness increases, people are
the implicit and explicit measures (e.g., high in the distribution on both, more likely to think carefully about a shyness-related persuasive
low in the distribution on both, middling on both, and so forth). Discrep- message. This conclusion was supported by the finding that the
ancies can be in either direction. That is, people can be higher in the sample attitudes of relatively discrepant individuals were more reflective
distribution on the explicit measure than the implicit measure (a positive of the quality of the persuasive message that they received about
discrepancy) or they can be lower in the distribution on the explicit shyness than were the attitudes of individuals who were low in
measure than the implicit measure (a negative discrepancy). In this study, discrepancy. These findings suggest that participants with a high
38 participants had positive discrepancies and 39 participants had negative explicit–implicit discrepancy paid more careful attention to the
discrepancies. In addition to including size of the discrepancy, analyses
message than those with a low discrepancy, presumably in an
also included a factor for whether the discrepancy was positive or negative.
attempt to resolve the discrepancy.
Experiment 1 demonstrated that explicit–implicit discrepancies
Dependent Measure: Attitudes
in shyness can lead to greater thinking about information directly
After reading the message relevant to shyness, participants’ attitudes relevant to the dimension on which the discrepancy exists. As
toward shyness were assessed using a single-item 9-point semantic differ- noted earlier, shyness is a general and somewhat broad personality
ential scale ranging from bad (1) to good (9) on which they rated how self-dimension. To extend the utility and generality of our find-
favorably they viewed shyness. ings, we sought to test whether the same effects would emerge
when the explicit–implicit discrepancy concerned a more specific
Results dimension of the self: a person’s need to evaluate (Jarvis & Petty,
Attitudes toward shyness were submitted to a hierarchical re- 1996).
gression analysis, with extent of discrepancy (continuous vari-
able), manipulated argument quality (strong–weak; dummy Experiment 2
coded), and direction of discrepancy (explicit ⬎ implicit vs. ex-
Our second study was designed to provide a conceptual repli-
plicit ⬍ implicit; dummy coded) as the independent variables.
cation and extension of the first. In Study 2, we used the same
Scores on the discrepancy index were centered by subtracting the
paradigm to assess the extent to which participants engaged in
mean from each person’s score (Aiken & West, 1991). Main
effects were interpreted in the first step of the regression, the
two-way interactions in the second step, and the three-way inter- 2
Asendorpf et al. (2002) reported correlations between explicit and
action in the final step (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Responses to the implicit shyness that ranged from .20 to .44 depending on the format of the
attitude scales were scored so that higher values represented more explicit measure. For example, the correlation between the IAT and the
favorable opinions toward shyness. explicit measure based on the shyness adjectives scale was .40.
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 159

effortful elaboration (i.e., an argument quality manipulation). ipate in two different research projects. First, participants completed the
However, several changes were introduced. First, instead of as- IAT for the NE measure, ostensibly as part of a research project in the
sessing explicit and implicit shyness as in Study 1, we focused on cognitive psychology program. After the IAT task, participants were told
a motivational self-concept dimension, need to evaluate (NE; that because of extra time remaining in the session, they would also be
participating in another experiment designed to assess their attitudes to-
Jarvis & Petty, 1996; Petty & Jarvis, 1996), which refers to
ward a particular issue. To create a context of evaluation, all of the
individual differences in people’s tendencies to engage in evalu-
participants were explicitly told that the goal of the second research project
ative thought. People who are high in NE tend to spontaneously was to measure their opinions and evaluations of a current commercial
assess whether things are good or bad (e.g., Tormala & Petty, campaign. Participants received a persuasive message in favor of the
2001; see also the “need to assess”; Kruglanski et al., 2000). consumption of vegetables containing either strong or weak arguments.
Knowing whether things in the world are good or bad helps people Then, participants were told that it was important to know more specifi-
to understand the environment. Probably because of this and other cally what their opinions about the consumption of vegetables were. After
functions (e.g., Maio & Olson, 2000), people tend to form attitudes reporting their attitudes toward the proposal, participants completed the
about nearly everything (e.g., Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, & Pratto, explicit NE scale and several ancillary measures.
1992; Roskos-Ewoldsen & Fazio, 1992). Nevertheless, some peo-
ple are more chronic and spontaneous than others in their tendency Independent Variables
to evaluate, and the NE scale assesses this. In contrast to the Argument quality. The message about vegetable consumption con-
explicit self-report measure typically used to assess NE, we de- tained either strong or weak arguments in favor of this topic. This manip-
veloped an implicit NE measure using an IAT. Similar to the ulation was designed to influence the favorability of participants’ cognitive
previous study, an index of self-discrepancy was formed as the responses if participants were thinking about the message (Petty & Ca-
absolute value of the difference between the standardized values of cioppo, 1986). The gist of one of the strong arguments in favor of vegetable
the explicit and implicit measures. consumption was that vegetables have more vitamins than the majority of
Second, to generalize our results across topics, we used a vitamin supplements on the market, making them especially appropriate
different persuasive issue. Instead of presenting information di- during exams and workout periods. The gist of one of the weak arguments
was that vegetables are becoming more popular for wedding celebrations
rectly related to the discrepancy dimension as in Study 1, we used
because they are colorful and look beautiful on plates. The argument
a message containing substantive information that actually was not quality of the two messages was pretested to ensure that the strong version
relevant to the issue of the discrepancy. However, for all partici- of the message produced mostly favorable thoughts, whereas the weak one
pants, the message was framed to appear relevant to NE. That is, produced mostly negative thoughts when people were instructed to think
participants were told before reading the message that the research carefully about them (e.g., Briñol, Horcajo, Becerra, Falces, & Sierra,
concerned their evaluations, and so all participants had an expec- 2002).
tation that the message would contain information directly relevant Explicit measure of need to evaluate. Participants completed the 16-
to the domain of the discrepancy. item NE scale (Jarvis & Petty, 1996). This scale assesses the chronic
The topic of the message the participants were to evaluate was tendency to engage in evaluative responding. Jarvis and Petty (1996)
increasing the amount of vegetables in the diet. Comparing the demonstrated that, compared with people low in NE, those high in the NE
are more likely to form attitudes toward a variety of social and political
persuasive effect of strong and weak arguments tested the effects
issues (see also Bizer et al., 2004), and the attitudes of high NE individuals
of explicit–implicit self-discrepancy on information processing. tend to be more accessible (Hermans, DeHouwer, & Eelen, 2001; for a
We expected participants with a relatively high discrepancy be- review, see Briñol & Petty, 2005). The NE scale contains statements such
tween their explicit and implicit NE to think about information as “I enjoy strongly liking and disliking new things” and “I am pretty much
expected to be discrepancy related to a greater extent than indi- indifferent to many important issues” (reverse scored). Participants re-
viduals with a relatively low discrepancy. That is, we expected sponded to each statement on a 5-point scale anchored by extremely
argument quality to have a larger impact on attitudes for partici- uncharacteristic of me (1) and extremely characteristic of me (5). The
pants with a high discrepancy between explicit and implicit NE items on this self-concept dimension were intercorrelated (␣ ⫽ .79), so
compared with participants with a low discrepancy. Thus, as in responses to each item were summed to form a composite score of NE.
Study 1, we expected to find a Discrepancy ⫻ Argument Quality Participants’ NE scores were not affected by the argument quality manip-
ulation, F(1, 98) ⫽ 0.44, p ⫽ .5.
interaction on the measure of attitudes that was unmoderated by
Implicit measure of need to evaluate. As in Study 1, we used an IAT
the direction of the discrepancy. procedure as the implicit assessment of this self-conception (Greenwald et
al., 1998). The NE IAT was administered at the beginning of the experi-
Method mental session and was presented as part of a research project designed to
study how taxonomies are represented in people’s minds. In this IAT,
Participants and Design participants classified target concepts (represented by me or other) and
Ninety-nine undergraduates in psychology courses at the Universidad attributes (represented by neutral or extreme) using two designated keys.
Autónoma de Madrid participated in partial fulfillment of a course require- The words extreme and neutral were pretested as representative of high and
ment. We randomly assigned the students to the argument quality condi- low evaluation categories, respectively. Although the NE scale was de-
tions (strong or weak) and assessed their explicit and implicit (IAT) NE to signed to measure the tendency to engage in evaluation per se rather than
form an index of explicit–implicit self-discrepancy. Direction of discrep- the tendency to engage in extreme evaluation, a number of items on the NE
ancy (higher on explicit or implicit measure) was also coded. scale clearly refer to extremity. According to Jarvis and Petty (1996, p.
190), the reason those items were included was to maximize the variance
in participants’ scores on the NE scale. Also for that reason, we selected the
Procedure
words that were related to extremity to increase the variability in the IAT.
The procedure was similar to Study 1. Participants were seated at The me category was represented by the words I, me, mine, my, and self,
individual computer stations and were told that they were going to partic- whereas the other category was represented by the words they, others,
160 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

them, theirs, and their. The extreme category included words such as discrepancy on elaboration were not restricted to any particular
extremity, limit, radical, total, and extreme. In contrast, the neutral cate- direction of the discrepancy.3
gory included words as moderate, caution, prudent, temperate, and neutral.
These words were equally familiar to participants and were selected based
on some of the items of the NE scale (e.g., “It bothers me to remain Discussion
neutral,” “I prefer to avoid taking extreme positions” [reversed], and “I
often prefer to remain neutral about complex issues” [reversed]). The Experiment 2 conceptually replicated the first experiment by
difference in response latencies for (me ⫹ extreme and other ⫹ neutral) showing that participants with relatively high explicit–implicit
versus (other ⫹ extreme and me ⫹ neutral) provided our implicit measure
discrepancy in their NE processed the message more carefully than
of NE. Regarding the combination of blocks, random assignment of stim-
participants with relatively low discrepancy. That is, compared
uli, incorrect classifications, practice trials, anticipatory responses, momen-
tary inattention, and data transformation, we followed the standard IAT with those with a small discrepancy between their explicit and
procedures described in our first study (see also Greenwald et al., 1998). implicit NE, individuals with a high discrepancy were more influ-
Explicit–implicit discrepancy. Explicit and implicit NE were not cor- enced by the quality of the arguments in the message than were
related (r ⫽ ⫺.13, p ⫽ .17). An index of discrepancy was formed as the those with a low discrepancy.
absolute value of the difference between the standardized explicit and Although the message in the present study did not actually
standardized implicit measures. Higher scores on that variable reflected contain information directly related to the issue on which the
greater differences between the explicit and the implicit measures, and thus explicit–implicit discrepancies existed (i.e., a person’s own NE),
higher explicit–implicit discrepancy in NE. As in the previous study,
the whole context of the study was framed as dealing with the
direction of discrepancy was symmetrical, with 50 participants showing a
participants’ opinions. We argue that emphasizing that the task
positive discrepancy (i.e., higher in the sample distribution on the explicit
measure than the implicit measure) and 48 participants showing a negative was related to evaluation was sufficient for participants with high
discrepancy (i.e., lower in the distribution on the explicit than the implicit explicit–implicit discrepancies in NE to engage in more extensive
measure). thinking. From our first two studies, it appears that information can
be directly related to the issue of discrepancy (Study 1) or simply
Dependent Measure: Attitudes framed as related to the dimension in which the discrepancy exists
(Study 2).
Similar to Study 1, participants’ postmessage attitudes toward vegetable We argue that the mere existence of a discrepancy does not
consumption were assessed using a single item 9-point semantic differen-
result in the indiscriminate processing of any information present
tial scale anchored at bad (1) and good (9).
in the situation. Enhanced thinking is expected only if the dimen-
sion on which the discrepancy exists is activated by leading people
Results to believe that the message is going to pertain or be relevant to that
Attitudes toward vegetables were submitted to the same hierar- dimension. Without that, there should be no differential process-
chical regression analysis used in Study 1. Thus, the independent ing. To address this issue, our third study included a manipulation
variables included extent of discrepancy (continuous variable), of the message frame designed to induce participants to expect the
manipulated argument quality (strong–weak; dummy coded), and message to be related or unrelated to the issue on which there was
direction of discrepancy (explicit ⬎ implicit vs. explicit ⬍ im- an explicit–implicit discrepancy. If having an explicit–implicit
plicit; dummy coded). Scores on the discrepancy index were discrepancy enhances information processing in general, then high
centered by subtracting the mean from each person’s score (Aiken discrepancy individuals should be equally likely to process mes-
& West, 1991). Main effects were interpreted in the first step of the sages framed as relevant or irrelevant to the issue on which the
regression, the two-way interactions in the second step, and the discrepancy exists. If the information processing is in service of
three-way interaction in the final step (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). discrepancy reduction, however, individuals with high discrepan-
Responses to the attitude scales were scored so that higher values
represented more favorable opinions toward the proposal. 3
Participants’ attitudes were more favorable toward consuming Because of possible concerns about the use of difference scores in our
analyses (i.e., the discrepancy index), we also conducted an alternative
vegetables after receiving the strong (M ⫽ 8.29, SD ⫽ 0.71) than
analysis treating implicit and explicit measures separately. For maximum
the weak (M ⫽ 7.98, SD ⫽ 0.79) message, ␤ ⫽ .20, t(95) ⫽ 1.96, power, data from both Studies 1 and 2 were combined and submitted to
p ⫽ .05. No main effect for extent of discrepancy emerged ( p ⫽ another hierarchical regression analysis, with the implicit and explicit
.66). More important and consistent with expectations, the main measures (continuous variables) and manipulated argument quality
effect of argument quality was qualified by a marginally signifi- (dummy coded) as independent variables. Study was also included as a
cant interaction between argument quality and extent of discrep- factor in this analysis so that we could examine whether the results
ancy, ␤ ⫽ .28, t(92) ⫽ 1.83, p ⫽ .07. This interaction revealed that generalized across the study differences. As expected, this analysis re-
as discrepancy increased, argument quality had a larger effect on vealed a significant main effect for argument quality, ␤ ⫽ .14, t(175) ⫽
attitudes. That is, there was a significant effect of argument quality 1.97, p ⫽ .05, which was qualified by a three-way interaction between
among participants with relatively high discrepancy (recentered at argument quality, explicit self-concept, and implicit self-concept, ␤ ⫽
⫺.16, t(175) ⫽ ⫺2.08, p ⫽ .03. Also importantly, this significant three-
⫹1 SD), ␤ ⫽ .38, t(91) ⫽ 2.64, p ⫽ .01, but not among those with
way interaction was not moderated by the study independent variable, as is
relatively low discrepancy (recentered at –1 SD), ␤ ⫽ .005, evident in the absence of a four-way interaction ( p ⫽ .64). To facilitate
t(91) ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ .97. It is important to note that the three-way ease of presentation (i.e., interpreting two-way rather than three-way in-
interaction between argument quality, extent of discrepancy, and teractions) and matching more closely our conceptual variable (i.e., psy-
direction of the discrepancy was not significant, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.28, t(91) ⫽ chological discrepancy), we present the discrepancy analysis in the text and
⫺1.42, p ⫽ .16, revealing that the effects of explicit–implicit figures.
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 161

cies should be more likely to process the messages framed as crepancy related. As in previous studies, this enhanced thinking
relevant than irrelevant. would be evidenced by greater attitudinal responsiveness to the
manipulation of argument quality.
Experiment 3
Method
Studies 1 and 2 provided initial evidence supporting the notion
that divergence between explicit and implicit self-views can influ- Participants and Design
ence the extent of information processing of ostensibly
discrepancy-related information. Experiment 3 was conducted to One hundred seventy-three introductory psychology students at Ohio
replicate and extend the findings from these studies. Thus, several State University participated in partial fulfillment of a course requirement.
changes were introduced. First, instead of examining a general The students were randomly assigned to the argument quality conditions
dimension of personality (shyness) or motivation (NE), we focused (strong or weak) and the message context conditions (discrepancy related
on a more specific dimension of the self-concept: one’s beliefs or unrelated), which were manipulated orthogonally. Additionally, we
measured participants’ explicit and implicit resistance to persuasion to
concerning one’s own resistance to persuasion.
form an index of explicit–implicit discrepancy. The independent variables
Beliefs regarding one’s resistance to change play a central role
thereby constituted an Argument Quality (strong vs. weak) ⫻ Message
in people’s values and identities. For example, Schwartz’s (1992) Frame (discrepancy-related vs. discrepancy-unrelated) ⫻ Extent of
theory about universal human values is organized by two motiva- Explicit–Implicit Discrepancy (continuously scored) ⫻ Direction of Dis-
tional dimensions: the self-transcendence/self-enhancement di- crepancy (higher on implicit or explicit measure) design.
mension and the openness to change/conservation dimension. This
work implies that almost everyone might have beliefs about their
own resistance to change and that such beliefs might be an integral Procedure
part of the self-concept. In fact, resistance to change constitutes Upon arrival, participants were seated at individual computer stations
one of the most basic dimensions of personality according to the and were presented with all of the materials on the computer using
Big Five framework (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1985; Wiggins & MediaLab software (Jarvis, 2000). All of the participants were told that
Trapnell, 1997). Although the construct of resistance to change can they were going to participate in two different research projects. First,
be conceptualized quite differently in terms of personality, cogni- participants completed the IAT for resistance to persuasion, ostensibly as
tive ability, psychic structure, openness to experience, or openness part of a research project on semantic recognition and categorization
as culture (McCrae & Costa, 1997), we focus specifically on conducted by the cognitive psychology program. After the IAT task,
personal perceptions of resistance to persuasion. In this experi- participants were told that because of extra time remaining in the session,
they would also be participating in another experiment designed to assess
ment, the explicit self-dimension of resistance to persuasion was
possible changes in university policies. They read about a new school
assessed using the Resistance to Persuasion Scale (Briñol, Rucker,
policy and were told that students’ opinions about this policy were of
Tormala, & Petty, 2004), and implicit resistance was assessed importance to the university. Participants received a message in favor of
using an IAT developed for this study. As in our previous studies, instituting senior comprehensive exams that contained either strong or
an index of explicit–implicit discrepancy was created as the abso- weak arguments. After reading the message, participants were told that it
lute value of the difference between the standardized explicit and was important for the Board of Trustees to know what their opinions on the
implicit scores. topic were. Thus, they completed measures of their attitudes toward the
Second, to further generalize our results across topics, we used comprehensive exam policy. Finally, participants completed the Resistance
a different persuasive issue. Instead of presenting information to Persuasion Scale (Briñol et al., 2004) and several ancillary questions.
about a relatively proattitudinal topic (i.e., increasing the amount
of vegetables in the diet; Study 2) or a rather neutral one (i.e., the Independent Variables
benefits of shyness; Study 1), we moved to a relatively counter-
attitudinal topic (i.e., institution of required exams for college Argument quality. The comprehensive exam message participants re-
students). ceived contained either strong or weak arguments. The arguments selected
were adopted from previous research and have been shown many times to
Finally, and most importantly, we manipulated the presumed
produce the appropriate pattern of cognitive responding (see Petty &
discrepancy-relevance of the message information. Because the Cacioppo, 1986). That is, the strong arguments elicited mostly favorable
explicit–implicit discrepancy was related to individual differences thoughts and the weak arguments elicited mostly unfavorable thoughts
in susceptibility to change in attitudes and opinions, half of the when people were instructed to think carefully about them. The gist of
participants were told that the study had to do with their attitudes some strong arguments in favor of the exam policy were that students’
and opinions (similar to the frame in Study 2). This condition was grades would improve if the exams were adopted and that the average
compared with a discrepancy-unrelated frame in which partici- starting salary of graduates would increase. The gist of some weak argu-
pants were told that the experiment was part of a text comprehen- ments in favor of the exam policy were that implementing the exams would
sion study. Thus, all of the participants were exposed to a persua- allow the university to take part in a national trend and that the exams
sive message containing either strong or weak arguments that were would give students the opportunity to compare their scores with those of
students at other universities.
framed as either related or unrelated to the dimension on which the
Message frame. The frame of the message was manipulated to appear
discrepancy existed. After reading the message, participants were related or unrelated to participants’ opinions. That is, because the explicit–
asked to report their attitudes toward the proposal. We expected implicit discrepancy was related to persuasion (openness vs. resistance to
participants with a large explicit–implicit discrepancy to think changing one’s opinions), we framed the study as related (or not) to
more about the persuasive message than those with a small dis- opinions and attitudes. Thus, the message was introduced as part of an
crepancy, but only when the message was framed to appear dis- opinion-related study (discrepancy-related frame) or as part of a text
162 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

comprehension study (unrelated frame). Participants in the discrepancy- attributes (represented by easy to be persuaded or hard to be persuaded
related frame condition read the following: categories of words) using two designated keys. The me and other cate-
gories included the same words used in Study 2. Attributes related to
OPINION SURVEY persuasibility were selected from the items on the explicit scale and
This part of the study consists of a survey designed to obtain your included the words easy, flexible, open, variable, and changeable. In
opinions and thoughts about a campus issue. The message we are contrast, resistant attributes included the words resistant, stable, hard,
going to ask you to read is based on the transcript of an editorial from consistent, and committed. The difference in response latencies for me–
a college radio station. The editorial was introduced on the radio in easy (me ⫹ easy and other ⫹ hard) versus me– hard (other ⫹ easy and
order to familiarize students with this important issue. Please pay me ⫹ hard) responses provided a measure of relative self-association with
attention because your opinions will be measured. resistance (i.e., the IAT effect). Regarding the combination of blocks,
random assignment of stimuli, incorrect classifications, practice trials,
In the comprehension frame, participants were told that the message was anticipatory responses, momentary inattention, and data transformation, we
designed to measure their appreciation of new information. Comprehend- followed the standard IAT procedures described in our previous studies
ing a text would presumably not trigger the idea that the study involved any (see also Greenwald et al., 1998).
explicit or blatant persuasive attempt. In this condition, participants read Explicit–implicit discrepancy. The explicit and implicit measures of
the following: personal resistance were uncorrelated (r ⫽ ⫺.10, p ⫽ .16). An index of
explicit–implicit discrepancy was formed as the absolute value of the
TEXT COMPREHENSION difference between the standardized explicit and the implicit measures of
This part of the study has been designed to measure your appreciation resistance. Higher scores on that variable reflected greater differences
of new information. The information we are going to ask you to read between the explicit and the implicit measures (i.e., higher discrepancy).
and to examine is based on a transcript of a class project broadcast on The distributions of scores using this index revealed roughly equal num-
a college radio station. We want you to examine the content of the bers of people on each side of the discrepancy, with 87 participants
information presented carefully because the extent to which you showing positive discrepancy (i.e., higher in the sample distribution on the
understand the text will be measured. explicit measure than the implicit measure) and 86 showing negative
discrepancy (i.e., lower in the distribution on the explicit than the implicit
It is important to note that although attitude change was not salient for measure). Perhaps more relevant for the present research, the number of
the text comprehension frame, opinions toward the proposal were still participants in each direction was also equivalent in the conditions for
measured following the message. At the time opinions were measured, the which the information was presented as discrepancy related, with 42
idea of persuasibility may have become apparent to these participants, but participants revealing positive discrepancies and also 42 participants show-
by this time the extent of processing of the message was already deter- ing negative discrepancies.
mined. That is, in the discrepancy-relevant case, participants already
expected to give their opinion while they were reading the message
because the study was about opinions. In the discrepancy-irrelevant case, Dependent Measure: Attitudes
they thought that the study was on text comprehension while reading and
Participants’ attitudes toward the proposal were assessed using a series
processing the message, so forming opinions was not salient. We pretested
of five 9-point semantic differential scales ranging from 1 to 9 (i.e.,
the effectiveness of the induction by randomly assigning 50 students to one
bad– good, unfavorable–favorable, pro–against, foolish–wise, harmful–
of the two frames and asking them what kind of information they expected
beneficial) on which they rated the comprehensive exam policy. Ratings on
to receive after the frame. Specifically, we asked participants to respond on
these items were highly intercorrelated (␣ ⫽ .84), so they were averaged to
a 5-point scale to the question: “To what extent do you expect to receive
form one overall attitude index.
any information that might be related to your resistance to persuasion?” As
expected, participants who received the discrepancy-related frame reported
significantly higher expectations to receive persuasion-related information Results
(M ⫽ 3.14, SD ⫽ 1.00) than those who received the discrepancy-unrelated
frame (M ⫽ 2.68, SD ⫽ 1.21), t(48) ⫽ 10.39, p ⬍ .0001. It is also Attitudes were submitted to a hierarchical regression analysis,
important to note that although the message was framed to be related or not with extent of discrepancy (continuous variable), manipulated
to the basis of the self-discrepancy, the actual message participants re- argument quality (strong–weak; dummy coded), message frame
ceived was identical. (relevant–irrelevant; dummy coded), and direction of discrepancy
Explicit measure of resistance. The Resistance to Persuasion Scale was
(explicit ⬎ implicit vs. explicit ⬍ implicit; dummy coded) as the
used to assess participants’ explicit perceptions of their resistance to
persuasion. This scale measures individuals’ perceptions and beliefs about
independent variables. Scores on the discrepancy index were cen-
their own vulnerability to persuasion, willingness to change, and motiva- tered by subtracting the mean from each person’s score (Aiken &
tion and ability to resist persuasion. In prior research validating the scale, West, 1991). Main effects were interpreted in the first step of the
it has predicted the number of counterarguments people generate to a regression, two-way interactions in the second step, three-way
message and how resistant they were to influence (Briñol et al., 2004). The interactions in the third step, and the four-way interaction in the
scale contains 11 statements such as “My attitudes are open to change” and final step (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Responses to the attitude scales
“It is hard for me to change my ideas.” Participants responded to each were scored so that higher values represented more favorable
statement on a 5-point scale anchored by extremely uncharacteristic of me opinions toward the proposal.
(1) and extremely characteristic of me (5). Ratings on the scale items were Participants’ attitudes were more favorable toward the proposal
highly consistent with each other (␣ ⫽ .86) and were averaged to form a
after receiving the strong (M ⫽ 4.46, SD ⫽ 2.34) than the weak
single index of resistance to persuasion for each participant. Resistance to
persuasion scores were not affected by the argument quality manipulation,
(M ⫽ 3.68, SD ⫽ 2.09) message, ␤ ⫽ .18, t(168) ⫽ 2.35, p ⫽ .02.
F(1, 173) ⫽ 0.26, p ⫽ .61; the frame manipulation, F(1, 173) ⫽ 0.68, p ⫽ A significant interaction between argument quality and extent of
.41; or the interaction of the two, F(1, 173) ⫽ 0.02, p ⫽ .87. discrepancy also emerged, ␤ ⫽ .33, t(162) ⫽ 3.31, p ⫽ .001,
Implicit measure of resistance. In the IAT measure of resistance, revealing that as discrepancy increased, argument quality had a
participants classified target concepts (represented by me or other) and larger effect on attitudes. An interaction between frame and extent
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 163

of discrepancy also emerged, ␤ ⫽ .23, t(162) ⫽ 1.97, p ⫽ .05. This implicit and explicit views of their resistance to persuasion are
interaction suggested that attitudes tended to become more positive more likely to think carefully about a discrepancy-related persua-
as discrepancy increased for the relevant frame but more negative sive message than are people who have a small discrepancy
as discrepancy increased for the irrelevant frame. This interaction between their implicit and explicit self-conceptions. As in Studies
was only obtained in this study and is not discussed further. 1 and 2, this conclusion was supported by the finding that the
More critical to our primary concerns, the predicted three-way attitudes of relatively discrepant individuals were more reflective
interaction between argument quality, extent of discrepancy, and of the quality of the discrepancy-related persuasive message than
message frame was significant, ␤ ⫽ .42, t(158) ⫽ 2.84, p ⫽ .005. were the attitudes of less discrepant individuals. The fact that the
As depicted in Figure 1, this three-way interaction indicated that enhanced information processing only occurred for individuals
the two-way interaction between discrepancy and argument quality with large discrepancies when the message was framed so as to
was only significant for the discrepancy-related frame, ␤ ⫽ .56, seem related to the basis of the discrepancy (i.e., an opinion
t(157) ⫽ 4.26, p ⬍ .0001, but not for the discrepancy-unrelated context) rather than unrelated to the discrepancy (i.e., a compre-
frame, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.02, t(157) ⫽ ⫺.14, p ⫽ .89. For the discrepancy- hension context) is consistent with the idea that the purpose of the
related frame, there was a significant effect of argument quality processing was to resolve the discrepancy.
among participants with relatively high discrepancy (analyzed at Experiment 3 demonstrated that explicit–implicit discrepancies
⫹1 SD), ␤ ⫽ .72, t(157) ⫽ 4.86, p ⬍ .0001, but not among those in resistance to persuasion can lead to greater thinking about
with relatively low discrepancy (analyzed at –1 SD), ␤ ⫽ ⫺.13, information framed as related to the discrepancy. However, resis-
t(157) ⫽ ⫺.93, p ⫽ .36. The four-way interaction was not signif- tance to change is a very specific and descriptive self-dimension.
icant, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.12, t(157) ⫽ ⫺.51, p ⫽ .61, revealing that the effects Thus, Studies 1, 2, and 3 all focused on descriptive (shyness, NE,
of explicit–implicit discrepancy on elaboration were symmetrical and persuasibility, respectively) rather than evaluative self-
and not restricted to any particular direction of the discrepancy. dimensions. To extend the utility and generality of our findings, in
a final study we sought to test whether the same effects can emerge
Discussion when the explicit–implicit discrepancy concerns a broad, global
evaluation of the self. Thus, in our fourth study we used a similar
Experiment 3 conceptually replicated our previous findings by paradigm to test whether the information-processing effects can be
showing that people who have a large discrepancy between their found for explicit–implicit divergences on a general evaluative
self-dimension: a person’s self-esteem.

Experiment 4
Our fourth study was designed to extend the previous findings to
the domain of self-evaluation. That is, in this study we focused on
an evaluative dimension of the self-concept, self-esteem, and as-
sessed it with both explicit and implicit measures. Implicit self-
esteem typically has been defined as an evaluation of the self that
occurs automatically and unintentionally and can differ from one’s
more controlled and deliberative self-assessments (e.g., Farnham,
Greenwald, & Banaji, 1999; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Green-
wald & Farnham, 2000; Hetts & Pelham, 2001; Koole, Dijkster-
huis, & van Knippenberg, 2001).
In Study 4, we used the same paradigm to assess the extent to
which participants engaged in effortful thinking (i.e., an argument
quality manipulation). Similar to Study 3, we also manipulated the
ostensible discrepancy relatedness of the message information by
framing it as related or unrelated to the basis of the discrepancy.
Finally, in this study we used the relatively proattitudinal topic
used in Study 2 (i.e., increasing the amount of vegetables in the
diet). We expected participants with a relatively large discrepancy
between their explicit and implicit self-esteem to elaborate the
information more than individuals with a relatively small discrep-
ancy, but only when the information was framed to seem discrep-
ancy related. That is, we expected argument quality to have a larger
impact on attitudes for participants with a large discrepancy between
explicit and implicit self-esteem compared with participants with a
small discrepancy, but only when the message was framed to seem
Figure 1. Attitudes as a function of argument quality, message frame, and related to the discrepancy (i.e., when the message was framed to seem
extent of explicit–implicit (e-i) discrepancy graphed at –1 standard devia- related to their self-concept). More specifically, as in Study 3 we
tion (low discrepancy) and ⫹1 standard deviation (high discrepancy) in expected to find a Discrepancy ⫻ Argument Quality ⫻ Message
Experiment 3. Frame interaction on attitudes toward the message proposal.
164 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

Method related frame reported significantly higher expectations to receive self-


related information (M ⫽ 3.70, SD ⫽ 0.91) than those who received the
Participants and Design discrepancy-unrelated frame (M ⫽ 1.82, SD ⫽ 0.94), t(56) ⫽ 7.68, p ⬍
.0001.
One hundred seventeen undergraduate psychology students at the Uni- Explicit self-esteem. We used the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale as our
versidad Autónoma de Madrid were randomly assigned to the argument explicit measure (Rosenberg, 1965). Ratings on the items were highly
quality (strong or weak) and the message frame (discrepancy-related or consistent with each other (␣ ⫽ .76) and were averaged to form a single
discrepancy-unrelated) conditions, which were manipulated orthogonally. index of self-esteem for each participant. Self-esteem scores were not
Additionally, we measured participants’ explicit and implicit self-esteem to affected by the argument quality manipulation, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.08, p ⫽ .77;
form an index of explicit–implicit discrepancy and coded for the direction the frame manipulation, F(1, 117) ⫽ 1.41, p ⫽ .24; or the interaction of the
of discrepancy. The independent variables thereby constituted an Argu- two, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.11, p ⫽ .73.
ment Quality (strong vs. weak) ⫻ Message Frame (discrepancy-related vs. Implicit self-esteem. As in the prior studies, we used an IAT procedure
discrepancy-unrelated) ⫻ Extent of Explicit–Implicit Discrepancy in Self- to assess participants’ implicit self-evaluations (Greenwald et al., 1998).
Esteem (continuously scored) ⫻ Direction of Discrepancy (relatively The self-esteem IAT was administered at the beginning of the experimental
higher in explicit or implicit self-esteem) design. session and was presented as part of a research project designed to study
how taxonomies are represented in people’s minds. In the IAT, participants
Procedure classified target concepts (represented by me or other) and attributes
(represented by good and bad) using two designated keys. The me and
Upon arrival, participants were seated at individual computer stations other categories included the same words used in Studies 2 and 3. The good
and were told that they were going to participate in two different research category words included freedom, peace, love, cheer, and paradise, and the
projects. As in previous studies, participants first completed an IAT, bad category words included poison, cancer, death, vomit, and disaster
ostensibly as part of a research project on categorization conducted by the (selected from Greenwald et al., 1998). The difference in response latencies
cognitive psychology program. After the IAT task, participants were told for me ⫹ bad and other ⫹ good trials versus other ⫹ bad and me ⫹ good
that because of extra time remaining in the session, they would also be trials provided a measure of relative automatic self-esteem (i.e., the IAT
participating in another experiment designed to assess their attitudes to- effect). Comparable implicit measures have been used in prior research and
ward an opinion topic. Half of the participants were told that the message have been shown to be effective in predicting a variety of thoughts and
they were about to read had to do with plants and vegetables and their behaviors relevant to self-evaluations, especially under low thinking (spon-
qualities and properties (discrepancy-unrelated frame). The rest of the taneous) circumstances (e.g., Greenwald & Farnham, 2000; Hetts & Pel-
participants were told that the message concerned their personal habits and ham, 2001; Jones, Pelham, Mirenberg, & Hetts, 2002; Koole et al., 2001;
the way they interact with the world (discrepancy-related frame). Then, see also Bosson, Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000).
participants received a message containing strong or weak arguments in Explicit–implicit discrepancy. Explicit and implicit self-esteem
favor of the consumption of vegetables. After reading the message, all of showed a small negative correlation (r ⫽ ⫺.19, p ⫽ .04). Previous research
the participants were told that it was important to know more specifically has also shown negative correlations between explicit and implicit self-
what their opinions about the consumption of vegetables were. Finally, esteem (e.g., Bosson et al., 2000; Hetts et al., 1999; Karpinski, 2004;
after reporting their attitudes toward the proposal, participants completed Kitayama & Uchida, 2003). An index of discrepancy was formed as the
the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). absolute value of the difference between the standardized explicit and
implicit measures. Higher scores on that variable reflected greater differ-
Independent Variables ences between the explicit and the implicit measures, and thus higher
explicit–implicit self-esteem discrepancy. As in all previous studies, the
Argument quality. The message about vegetable consumption con- distributions of scores revealed equivalent numbers of participants on each
tained either strong or weak arguments in favor of this topic. This manip- side of the discrepancy, with 54 participants showing positive discrepan-
ulation was identical to that used in Study 2. cies and 63 showing negative discrepancies. This was also true for the
Message frame. The introduction to the message was framed to seem conditions in which the information was presented as discrepancy related,
as though the message would contain information unrelated or related to with 26 participants revealing positive discrepancies and 31 showing
the discrepancy domain. In the discrepancy-unrelated frame condition, negative discrepancies.
participants were told, “You are about to read an article about the charac-
teristics and properties of different plants and vegetables.” In the
discrepancy-related frame condition, they were told, “You are about to read Dependent Measure: Attitudes
an article about your self-concept, your personal diet habits, and the way As in the previous studies, participants were instructed to report their
you see your world.” To further enhance the discrepancy relevance, in the attitudes following the message. Participants’ attitudes toward the proposal
discrepancy-unrelated frame, the title of the message was “Research About (i.e., the increased consumption of vegetables) were assessed using a series
Vegetables” and in the discrepancy-related frame, the title of the message of five 9-point semantic differential scales anchored by 1 and 9 (i.e.,
was “Research About the Self-Concept.” bad– good, unfavorable–favorable, pro–against, foolish–wise, harmful–
It is important to note that, as in Experiments 2 and 3, we deliberately beneficial). Ratings on these items were highly intercorrelated (␣ ⫽ .84)
did not attempt to provide any real information about students’ self- and were averaged to form one overall attitude index. Responses to the
conceptions. Rather, the manipulation was oriented to influence what attitude scales were scored so that higher values represented more favor-
participants perceived was going to be the content of the message (not the able opinions toward increasing consumption of vegetables.
actual content itself). Thus, there would be no differences in ability to
process the message across conditions or other irrelevant confounds. We
pretested the effectiveness of the induction by randomly assigning 58 Results
students to one of the two frames and asked them what kind of information
they expected to receive after that. Specifically, we asked participants to Attitudes were submitted to a hierarchical regression analysis,
respond on a 5-point scale to the question, “To what extent do you expect with extent of discrepancy (continuous variable), manipulated
to receive any information that might be relevant to your self-concept and argument quality, message frame, and direction of discrepancy as
self-esteem?” As expected, participants who received the discrepancy- the independent variables. Scores on the discrepancy index were
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 165

centered by subtracting the mean from each person’s score (Aiken was a significant effect of argument quality among participants
& West, 1991). Main effects were interpreted in the first step of the with relatively high discrepancy (analyzed at ⫹1 SD), ␤ ⫽ .82,
regression, two-way interactions in the second step, three-way t(99) ⫽ 4.37, p ⬍ .0001. In contrast, those with relatively low
interactions in the third step, and the four-way interaction in the discrepancy (analyzed at –1 SD) showed no effect of argument
final step (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). quality, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.26, t(99) ⫽ ⫺1.34, p ⫽ .18. The effects of
The regression procedure described above revealed a significant explicit–implicit discrepancy on information processing were not
main effect for direction of discrepancy, ␤ ⫽ .21, t(110) ⫽ 2.23, restricted to any particular direction of the discrepancy, as indi-
p ⫽ .03, showing that participants’ attitudes were more favorable cated by a nonsignificant four-way interaction, ␤ ⫽ .06, t(99) ⫽
toward the proposal in one direction of discrepancy (explicit ⬎ .29, p ⫽ .77.4
implicit) than the other (implicit ⬎ explicit). This effect was not
obtained in any of the other studies and is not discussed further. In Discussion
addition, a significant interaction between argument quality and
extent of discrepancy was obtained, ␤ ⫽ .31, t(104) ⫽ 2.54, p ⫽ Experiment 4 conceptually replicated the earlier experiments by
.01, showing that argument quality had a greater impact on atti- showing that participants with relatively high explicit–implicit
tudes as discrepancy increased. discrepancy in their self-esteem processed more carefully the in-
Of most interest, the predicted three-way interaction between formation presumably related to the discrepancy dimension than
argument quality, extent of discrepancy, and frame was significant, participants with relatively low discrepancy. That is, individuals
␤ ⫽ .37, t(100) ⫽ 2.94, p ⫽ .004. To examine the basis of this with a large discrepancy found strong arguments to be more
interaction, we decomposed the interaction by frame conditions. persuasive than weak ones whereas those with a small discrepancy
As depicted in Figure 2, the two-way interaction between discrep- did not, but only when the messages were potentially relevant to
ancy and argument quality was only significant for the the discrepancy. Those with a high discrepancy presumably de-
discrepancy-related frame, ␤ ⫽ .86, t(99) ⫽ 3.76, p ⫽ .0002, but voted more cognitive resources to new information framed as
not for the discrepancy-unrelated frame, ␤ ⫽ .11, t(99) ⫽ 0.64, discrepancy related to reduce the uncertainty or subjective discom-
p ⫽ .52. More specifically, for the discrepancy-related frame, there fort that might result from holding simultaneously favorable and
unfavorable implicit and explicit self-evaluations.

General Discussion
Previous research has shown that there are many possible
sources of internal discrepancies (e.g., attitudes, motives, self-
conceptions), and these discrepancies are often associated with
negative affect and undesirable psychological outcomes. Although
most of the discrepancy research has relied on explicit and delib-
erative self-reports, previous studies identifying people who pos-
sess specific explicit and implicit self-discrepant dimensions have
also found that discrepancy is associated with some notable diffi-
culties in functioning.
The present research extends previous literature by showing that
explicit–implicit discrepancy is also associated with enhanced

4
Similar to Studies 1 and 2, alternative analyses were conducted in
which implicit and explicit measures were treated separately. For maxi-
mum power, data from both Studies 3 and 4 were combined and submitted
to another hierarchical regression analysis, with the implicit and explicit
measures (continuous variables) and manipulated argument quality and
message frame (dummy coded) as independent variables. Study was also
included as a factor in this analysis so that we could examine whether the
results generalized across the study differences. As expected, this analysis
revealed a significant main effect for argument quality, ␤ ⫽ .18, t(250) ⫽
3.61, p ⫽ .001, which was qualified by a three-way interaction between
argument quality, explicit self-concept, and implicit self-concept, ␤ ⫽
⫺.16, t(250) ⫽ ⫺2.31, p ⫽ .02. Most important for our concerns, a
significant four-way interaction also emerged, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.26, t(250) ⫽ 3.27,
p ⫽ .001. As predicted, this four-way interaction indicates that the three-
way interaction between argument quality, explicit, and implicit self-
concept was only significant for the discrepancy-related frame, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.38,
Figure 2. Attitudes as a function of argument quality, message frame, and t(127) ⫽ ⫺4.0, p ⬍ .0001, but not for the discrepancy-unrelated frame,
extent of explicit–implicit (e-i) discrepancy graphed at –1 standard devia- ␤ ⫽ ⫺.06, t(122) ⫽ ⫺0.58, p ⫽ .56. Also importantly, this significant
tion (low discrepancy) and ⫹1 standard deviation (high discrepancy) in four-way interaction was not moderated by the study independent variable,
Experiment 4. as is evident in the absence of a five-way interaction ( p ⫽ .44).
166 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

processing of ostensibly discrepancy-related information, presum-


ably in an attempt at inconsistency reduction. The present studies
also demonstrate that the tendency to process discrepancy-related
information applies to both narrow (Study 3) and relatively broad
(Studies 1, 2, and 4), as well as to descriptive (Studies 1, 2, and 3)
and evaluative (Study 4), dimensions of the self. Across different
types of self-concept elements, we have demonstrated that indi-
viduals for whom explicit and implicit self-dimensions are incon-
gruent process information that either is (Study 1) or is framed as
(Studies 2, 3, 4) discrepancy related more carefully than individ-
uals with relatively congruent explicit–implicit self-dimensions.
By thinking about information that is presumably related to the
explicit–implicit dimension of conflict, people may be attempting
to reduce the discrepancy between the implicit and explicit dimen-
sions, perhaps unconsciously.

Symmetric Versus Asymmetric Discrepancy


In prior research, most researchers interested in explicit–implicit
discrepancies have concentrated their studies in just one of the
possible directions of discrepancy: people with more positive
explicit than implicit self-views. For example, studies on narcis-
Figure 3. Attitudes (standardized scores) as a function of argument
sism (e.g., Bosson et al., 2003) and defensive self-esteem (Jordan quality and raw discrepancy scores across studies (only relevant-frame
et al., 2003; Shedler et al., 1993) have particularly focused on conditions are included for Studies 3 and 4). Actual discrepancy scores in
cases in which explicit self-esteem is higher than implicit self- the studies range from ⫺5.01 to ⫹ 4.52. E-i ⫽ explicit–implicit.
esteem and compared this particular discrepancy with cases in
which both implicit and explicit self-esteem are relatively high.
When looking at these asymmetric discrepancies, prior research studies and controlling for study as a factor (which did not mod-
has usually divided participants on the basis of relative scores erate the critical interaction).
within the sample distribution, as we did in the present research Although our discrepancy index does a good job of capturing
(see Weinberger & Hardaway, 1990, for an exception). Several people’s relative standing in the distribution (as in most prior
different procedures have been used for dividing people into research), our index can only deal with the range of scores ob-
discrepancy groups, such as by splitting people at the median, served in the sample. Thus, by an absolute criterion, it could be the
tertiles, or quartiles of the sample distribution (Barger, Kircher, & case that all of the participants are relatively high in both explicit
Croyle, 1997; Emmons & Colby, 1995; Mendolia, Moore, & and implicit self-esteem, high in explicit self-esteem and low in
Tesser, 1996; Newman, Duff, & Baumeister, 1997; Weinberger, implicit self-esteem, and so forth. If this were true, it would still be
1990) and by recentering at one SD above and below the mean of the case that some people were higher than others on each measure
the distribution (e.g., Bosson et al., 2003; Jordan et al., 2003; and that the relative discrepancies our index captures is successful
McGregor & Marigold, 2003). Other research (e.g., Kehr, 2004), in predicting the extent of information processing. Nevertheless,
like ours, used a procedure that created a continuous score by the generality of our conclusions would be limited if all of our
taking the difference between standardized explicit and implicit studies had the same profile of responses on the explicit and
measures of the self-construct. It is important to note that we had implicit measures (e.g., all absolutely high on both, all high on one
comparable numbers of individuals on each side of the discrepancy and low on the other, etc.).
(implicit ⬎ explicit and explicit ⬎ implicit), and we were able to To explore this issue further, we created an “absolute” discrep-
show that direction of discrepancy did not moderate the effects of ancy index in each study by using the middle point of the explicit
amount of discrepancy on information processing. scale (e.g., 3 in a 5-point scale) and the zero point in the IAT to
For exploration, we tried an alternative but conceptually similar approximate absolute differences between participants high and
analysis in which we used the raw discrepancy scores rather than low in each of the self-concept dimensions. This categorization
the absolute values and tested for curvilinear argument quality revealed that only Studies 1 and 3 had reasonable numbers of
effects across levels of raw discrepancy. In this alternative analy- individuals with discrepancies in both directions by this criterion
sis, both high-positive and high-negative discrepancy individuals (e.g., implicit measure suggests shyness but explicit measure sug-
should process the relevant frame message more than people who gests not shy; implicit measure suggests not shy but explicit
have little discrepancy. That is, because the direction of discrep- measure suggests shy). Notably, these studies produced the same
ancy does not matter according to our conceptualization, both pattern of results as the studies in which the discrepancies were
high-positive and high-negative discrepancies should look the present mostly on a relative basis.
same. When analyzed in this manner, the results from our exper- Our speculation is that the size of the discrepancy matters more
iments look exactly as predicted. Figure 3 shows the significant than whether the discrepancy is relative or absolute. For example,
interaction between discrepancy (quadratic term) and argument an individual with trivially positive self-esteem on an explicit
quality, ␤ ⫽ .70, t(315) ⫽ 6.87, p ⬍ .0001, collapsing across measure and trivially negative self-esteem on an implicit measure
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 167

would be categorized as discrepant in absolute terms, whereas an (e.g., Steele, Southwick, & Critchlow, 1981), or affirming oneself
individual with trivially positive self-esteem on an explicit mea- by expressing important values (e.g., Steele & Liu, 1983). Addi-
sure and extremely positive self-esteem on an implicit measure tionally, ignoring or defensively avoiding discrepancy-related in-
would not, even though the difference between implicit and ex- formation might constitute another strategy when exposure does
plicit scores of the latter individual would be much larger and more not automatically occur, as in the present experiments. Different
consequential in our view. Nevertheless, future research might mechanisms of reducing self-inconsistency might be substitutable
profitably recruit larger samples and use alternative measures so for each other (e.g., Tesser, 2001). If such mechanisms were
that the issue of absolute versus relative discrepancies can be interchangeable, then future research might profitably explore the
examined more systematically. potential differential impact of each of the discrepancy reduction
strategies.
Motivation to Reduce Self-Inconsistency A final avenue for further examination concerns the possibility
of actual resolution of the explicit–implicit discrepancy. For ex-
As noted earlier, explicit discrepancies are often associated with ample, in our Study 1 participants received information directly
doubt. Campbell (1990) found that self-concept instability corre- relevant to the dimension (shyness) on which the discrepancy
lated with lower subjective confidence in one’s own trait ratings existed. Thus, this information might have influenced their explicit
and also with slower “me/not me” responses to trait adjectives (see or implicit (or both) self-dimensions, thereby affecting the subse-
Wright, 2001, for a review). Previous literature has also related quent explicit–implicit discrepancy. Because follow-up measures
explicit evaluative inconsistency (attitudinal ambivalence) to lack related to the self-dimension were not included in that study after
of confidence. For example, Jonas et al. (1997) provided empirical message processing, such an influence on the resulting discrepancy
evidence that evaluative inconsistency evokes elaboration of re- could not be assessed. In the other studies of the present research,
lated information to achieve a sufficient level of confidence with it was less likely that message processing would help participants
respect to the overall evaluation of the object. Bargh et al. (1992) to resolve their internal conflict toward their implicit or their
suggested that evaluative inconsistency might be related to doubt, explicit self-view because the information provided to them was
because response latencies (i.e., attitude accessibility) were found not actually relevant to resolving the discrepancy. The experiments
to be slower for ambivalent participants (see also Costelo, Rice, & in this article examine whether people are motivated to process
Schoenfeld, 1974; Gilmore, 1982). Indeed, one function particular information when they have an explicit–implicit discrepancy,
to ambivalent attitudes—and, perhaps, also to explicit–implicit rather than examining the direction or means of discrepancy res-
discrepant selves—seems to be reducing action readiness and olution. This issue constitutes an intriguing question for future
promoting further and elaborated thinking about related informa- consideration.
tion to amplify confidence and knowledge about the target (e.g.,
Hänze, 2001; Hodson et al., 2001; Jonas et al., 1997). References
On the basis of this research, it seems reasonable that explicit–
implicit self-discrepancies might also be associated with uncer- Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcomb, T. M., Rosenberg,
M. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1968). Theories of cognitive consistency:
tainty or doubt (Briñol et al., 2003). If explicit–implicit self-
A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally.
discrepancies are associated with doubt, the enhanced information- Abelson, R. P., & Rosenberg, M. J. (1958). Symbolic psycho-logic: A
processing effects we observed might be due in part to this model of attitudinal cognition. Behavioral Science, 3, 1–13.
uncertainty. Individuals who are induced to doubt before receiving Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
a message have been shown to engage in greater thinking (Tiedens interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
& Linton, 2001; Weary & Jacobson, 1997). For example, in one Aronson, E. (1969). Cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L.
study Tiedens and Linton (2001) had participants write about a sad Bercowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4,
experience in which they felt uncertain about what was happening pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.
or a sad experience in which they felt certain. Following this doubt Asendorpf, J. B. (1987). Videotape reconstruction of emotions and cogni-
induction, participants received a message containing strong or tions related to shyness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
53, 542–549.
weak message arguments. The primary result was that uncertain
Asendorpf, J. B. (1989). Shyness as a final common pathway for two
participants engaged in greater information processing (i.e., greater different kinds of inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
attitudinal differentiation between strong and weak arguments) ogy, 57, 481– 492.
than certain participants. We speculate that just as explicit uncer- Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). The expression of shyness and embarrassment. In
tainty can guide information processing, so too might implicit W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from
uncertainty (see also Petty et al., 2006). social psychology (pp. 87–118). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation
Future Directions between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380 –393.
Future research might examine other consequences of explicit–
Barger, S. D., Kircher, J. C., & Croyle, R. T. (1997). The effects of social
implicit discrepancies apart from enhanced elaboration. In partic-
context and defensiveness on the physiological responses of repressive
ular, research might explore other ways in which people could copers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1118 –1128.
reduce these discrepancies. These could include changing self- Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality
discrepant elements (e.g., Festinger, 1957; Harmon-Jones & Mills, of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and
1999), minimizing the salience or perceived (explicit or implicit) Social Psychology, 62, 893–912.
importance of the dimension on which the inconsistency exists Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of
168 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Epstein, S. (2003). Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory of personality. In T.
Psychological Review, 81, 506 –520. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Vol. 5, pp.
Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., 159 –184). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
& Rucker, D. D. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behav- Farnham, S. D., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). Implicit
ioral, and affective political processes: The effects of need to evaluate. self-esteem. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and
Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027. social cognition (pp. 230 –248). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995).
stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial
1142–1163. attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social
Bornstein, R. F. (1995). Sex differences in objective and projective depen- Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.
dency tests: A meta-analytic review. Assessment, 2, 319 –331. Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition
Bosson, J. K., Brown, R. P., Zeigler-Hill, V., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
Self-enhancement tendencies among people with high self-esteem: The 297–327.
moderating role of implicit self-esteem. Self and Identity, 2, 169 –187. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the Relations, 7, 117–140.
perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA:
revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631– 643. Stanford University Press.
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive disso- Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (in press). Associative and propo-
nance. New York: Wiley. sitional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and
Briñol, P., Horcajo, J., Becerra, A., Falces, C., & Sierra, B. (2002). Cambio explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin.
de actitudes implı́citas [Implicit attitude change]. Psicothema, 14, 771– Gilmore, R. F. (1982). Conflict or dissonance: A reinterpretation of coun-
775. terattitudinal essay writing. Dissertation Abstracts International, 43,
Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2005). Individual differences in persuasion. In D. 1659.
Albarracı́n, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of Greenier, K. D., Kernis, M. H., & Waschull, S. B. (1995). Not all high (or
attitudes and attitude change (pp. 575– 615). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. low) self-esteem people are the same: Theory and research on stability
Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Wheeler, S. C. (2003, May). Implicit ambiva- of self-esteem. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem
lence: Implications for attitude change. Paper presented at the Attitudi- (pp. 51–71). New York: Plenum Press.
nal Incongruence and Information Processing Symposium, University of Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Atti-
Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. tudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4 –27.
Briñol, P., Rucker, D., Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Individual Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek,
differences in resistance to persuasion: The role of beliefs and meta- B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes,
beliefs. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109,
(pp. 83–104). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 3–25.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Associa-
conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative tion Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality
space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25. and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038.
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring
of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538 –549. individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association
Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1986). Trait memory and behavior task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464 –1480.
memory: The effects of alternative pathways on impression judgment Gregg, A. P., Seibt, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Easier done than undone:
response times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 5–13. Asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Per-
Carver, C. S. (2005). Impulse and constraint: Some perspectives from sonality and Social Psychology, 90, 1–20.
personality psychology, convergence with theory in other areas, and Hänze, M. (2001). Ambivalence, conflict, and decision making: Attitudes
potential for integration. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, and feelings in Germany towards NATO’s military intervention in the
312–333. Kosovo war. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 693–706.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. S. (Eds.). (1999). Cognitive dissonance:
and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC:
19 –35. American Psychological Association.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation Hass, R. G., Katz, I., Rizzo, N., Bailey, J., & Moore, L. (1992). When
analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. racial ambivalence evokes negative affect, using a disguised measure of
Costelo, R. M., Rice, D. P., & Schoenfeld, L. S. (1974). Attitudinal mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 786 –797.
ambivalence with alcoholic respondents. Journal of Consulting and Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
Clinical Psychology, 42, 303–304. Wiley.
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Beach, K. (2001). Implicit and explicit Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (2001). A time course analysis of
attitudes: Examination of the relationship between measures of inter- the affective priming effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 143–165.
group bias. In R. Brown & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of Hetts, J. J., & Pelham, B. W. (2001). A case for the nonconscious self-
social psychology: Vol. 4. Intergroup relations (pp. 175–197). Oxford, concept. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The
England: Blackwell. Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp.
Emmons, R. A., & Colby, P. M. (1995). Emotional conflict and well-being: 105–123). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Relation to perceived availability, daily utilization, and observer reports Hetts, J. J., Sakuma, M., & Pelham, B. W. (1999). Two roads to positive
of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture. Journal of
947–959. Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 512–559.
Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited: Or a theory of a theory. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect.
American Psychologist, 28, 404 – 416. Psychological Review, 94, 319 –340.
EXPLICIT–IMPLICIT DISCREPANCY 169

Hodson, G., Maio, G. R., & Esses, V. M. (2001). The role of attitudinal self: What makes you so sure? Journal of Personality and Social
ambivalence in susceptibility to consensus informative. Basic and Ap- Psychology, 85, 838 – 852.
plied Social Psychology, 23, 197–205. Mendolia, M., Moore, J., & Tesser, A. (1996). Dispositional and situational
Jarvis, W. B. G. (2000). MediaLab (Version Beta 2000) [Computer soft- determinants of repression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ware]. Columbus, OH: Empirisoft. ogy, 70, 856 – 867.
Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194. gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.
Jonas, K., Diehl, M., & Bromer, P. (1997). Effects of attitudinal ambiva- Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mech-
lence on information processing and attitude-intention consistency. anisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472– 489.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 190 –210. Newcomb, T. M. (1968). Interpersonal balance. In R. Abelson et al. (Eds.),
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Hetts, J. J. (2002). Name Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook (pp. 28 –51). Chicago:
letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism as Rand McNally.
self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 170 – Newman, L. S., Duff, K. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). A new look at
177. defensive projection: Thought suppression, accessibility, and biased
Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshino-Browne, E., & Correll, person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
J. (2003). Secure and defensive high self-esteem. Journal of Personality 980 –1001.
and Social Psychology, 85, 969 –978. Norton, G. R. (1975). Measurement of ambiguity tolerance. Journal of
Kaplan, K. J. (1972). On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude Personality Assessment, 39, 607– 619.
theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic Osgood, C. E., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1955). The principle of congruity in
differential technique. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 361–372. the prediction of attitude change. Psychological Review, 62, 42–55.
Karpinski, A. (2004). Measuring self-esteem using the Implicit Association Patten, R. L., & White, L. A. (1977). Independent effects of achievement
Test: The role of the other. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 39 –59.
30, 22–34. Petty, R. E. (in press). A meta-cognitive model of attitudes. Journal of
Katz, I., Wackenhut, J., & Hass, R. G. (1986). Racial ambivalence, value Consumer Research.
duality, and behavior. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Preju- Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on
dice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 35–59). New York: Academic responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral
Press. routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional 69 – 81.
depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion:
30, 315–327. Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer-
Kernis, M. H., & Waschull, S. B. (1995). The interactive roles of stability Verlag.
and level of self-esteem: Research and theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Briñol, P. (Eds.). (in press). Attitudes: Insights
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 27, pp. 93–141). San from the new implicit measures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Petty, R. E., & Jarvis, B. G. (1996). An individual differences perspective
Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2003). Explicit self-criticism and implicit on assessing cognitive processes. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.),
self-regard: Evaluating self and friend in two cultures. Journal of Ex- Answering questions: Methodology for determining cognitive and com-
perimental Social Psychology, 39, 476 – 482. municative processes in survey research (pp. 221–257). San Francisco:
Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s in a Jossey-Bass.
name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (2006). Implicit
and Social Psychology, 80, 669 – 685. ambivalence from attitude change: An exploration of the PAST model.
Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash, M. N., Pierro, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 21– 41.
A., Shah, J. Y., & Spiegel, S. (2000). To “do the right thing” or to “just Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple roles for
do it”: Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory impera- persuasion variables. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.). The
tives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 793– 815. handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 323–390). New
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience ap- York: McGraw-Hill.
proach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109 –137. Petty, R. E., Wells, G. L., & Brock, T. C. (1976). Distraction can enhance
Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility or reduce yielding to propaganda: Thought disruption versus effort
effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17, 349 –358. justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 874 –
Maio, G. R., Bell, D. E., & Esses, V. M. (1996). Ambivalence and 884.
persuasion: The processing of messages about immigrant groups. Jour- Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., & Tormala, Z. L. (2003). Persuasion and
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 513–536. attitude change. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of
Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.). (2000). Why we evaluate: Functions of psychology: Vol. 5. Personality and social psychology (pp. 353–382).
attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine what Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of
people do. American Psychologist, 40, 812– 825. ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985). Openness to experience. In R. subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Hogan & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality (Vol. 1, pp. 71, 431– 449.
145–172). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (2001). Extending the bases of subjective
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Conceptions and correlates of attitudinal ambivalence: Interpersonal and intrapersonal antecedents of
openness to experience. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), evaluative tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
Handbook of personality psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 825– 847). New York: 19 –34.
Academic Press. Proshansky, H. M. (1943). A projective method for the study of attitudes.
McGregor, I., & Marigold, D. C. (2003). Defensive zeal and the uncertain Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38, 393–395.
170 BRIÑOL, PETTY, AND WHEELER

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2001). On-line versus memory-based
NJ: Princeton University Press. processing: The role of “need to evaluate” in person perception. Per-
Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). On the orienting value of sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1599 –1612.
attitudes: Attitude accessibility as a determinant of an object’s attraction Weary, G., & Jacobson, J. A. (1997). Causal uncertainty beliefs and
of visual attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, diagnostic information seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
198 –211. chology, 73, 839 – 848.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Weinberger, D. M. (1990). The construct validity of the repressive coping
Theoretical advances and empirical test in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications for
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 1– 65). personality, psychopathology, and health (pp. 337–386). Chicago: Uni-
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. versity of Chicago Press.
Shastri, L., & Ajjanagadde, V. (1993). From simple associations to sys- Weinberger, J., & Hardaway, R. (1990). Separating science from myth in
tematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and subliminal psychodynamic activation. Clinical Psychology Review, 10,
dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony. Behavioral and Brain 727–756.
Sciences, 16, 417– 494. Wiggins, J. S., & Trapnell, P. D. (1997). Personality structure: The return
Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illusion of mental of the Big Five. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook
health. American Psychologist, 48, 1117–1131.
of personality psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 737–765). San Diego, CA: Aca-
Sloman, S. A., (1996). The empirical case for two forms of reasoning.
demic Press.
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual process models in social and
attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.
cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying
Wing, P., & Gough, H. G. (1990). New narcissism scales for the California
memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108 –
Psychological Inventory and MMPI. Journal of Personality Assessment,
131.
54, 3– 4.
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behav-
ioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–23. Wright, R. (2001). Self-certainty and self-esteem. In T. J. Owens, S.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self- Stryker, & N. Goodman (Eds.), Extending self-esteem theory and re-
affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5–19. search: Sociological and psychological currents (pp. 101–134). Cam-
Steele, C. M., Southwick, L., & Critchlow, B. (1981). Dissonance and bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
alcohol: Drinking your troubles away. Journal of Personality and Social Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (2003, February). The importance of
Psychology, 41, 831– 846. knowing what we want: How implicit and explicit motives interact to
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of predict well-being. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for
social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220 –247. Personality and Social Psychology, Los Angeles, CA.
Tesser, A. (2001). On the plasticity of self-defense. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 10, 66 – 69.
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty Received June 2, 2003
and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information pro- Revision received February 9, 2006
cessing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973–988. Accepted February 17, 2006 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 16 –32 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16

The Single Category Implicit Association Test as a Measure of Implicit


Social Cognition
Andrew Karpinski and Ross B. Steinman
Temple University

The Single Category Implicit Association Test (SC-IAT) is a modification of the Implicit Association
Test that measures the strength of evaluative associations with a single attitude object. Across 3 different
attitude domains—soda brand preferences, self-esteem, and racial attitudes—the authors found evidence
that the SC-IAT is internally consistent and makes unique contributions in the ability to understand
implicit social cognition. In a 4th study, the authors investigated the susceptibility of the SC-IAT to
faking or self-presentational concerns. Once participants with high error rates were removed, no
significant self-presentation effect was observed. These results provide initial evidence for the reliability
and validity of the SC-IAT as an individual difference measure of implicit social cognition.

Keywords: implicit social cognition, associative processes, individual differences

Over the past 20 years, there has been an increasing awareness young– old, weak–strong, warm– cold, liberal– conservative, aggressive–
that much social cognition occurs outside of conscious awareness peaceful, and so forth. (Greenwald & Farnham, 2000, p. 1023)
or conscious control (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Greenwald, 1992;
Kihlstrom, 1990). Implicit social cognition may be inaccessible to This property is both a strength and a limitation of the measure. As
conscious introspection, and thus it is necessary to develop mea- highlighted by Greenwald and Farnham (2000), many attitude
sures that do not rely on introspection and self-report in order to objects have a complementary category, and it makes sense to
understand and measure these processes (Greenwald & Banaji, consider these attitude objects relative to another category.
1995). One particularly fruitful approach to measuring implicit Yet for some research questions, evaluative associations with a
social cognition has been the development of individual difference single target concept may be of interest. For example, to measure
measures of associative strength. self-esteem by using the IAT, researchers have measured the
Although a large number of these association-based measures of positive and negative associations a person has with the self in
implicit social cognition have been developed (for a review, see comparison to an unspecified other (or with me in comparison to
Fazio & Olson, 2003), the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Green- not me). At the same time, an alternative approach to measuring
wald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) has become the most com- self-esteem would be to measure only evaluative associations with
monly used among the implicit measurement techniques because it the self with no complementary category (see Karpinski, 2004).
is reliable, easy to administer, and robust and produces large effect This approach is not possible within the standard IAT paradigm.
sizes, particularly in comparison to other implicit measures of Furthermore, there are also instances in which the choice of a
social cognition (Greenwald et al., 1998; Greenwald & Nosek, complement is not obvious. Consider a researcher interested in
2001). The IAT is unique among the recently developed predicting President Bush’s job approval rating. This researcher
association-based measures of implicit social cognition because it may want to obtain a measure of evaluative associations with
cannot reveal the evaluative associations with a single target concept. President Bush, but relative to whom? In such instances, it may be
useful to have an IAT-type task that does not require the use of a
Because it uses complementary pairs of concepts and attributes, the complementary category (see also Blanton & Jaccard, 2006; Blan-
IAT is limited to measuring the relative strengths of pairs of associ- ton, Jaccard, Gonzales, & Christie, 2006; De Houwer, 2002).
ations rather than absolute strengths of single associations. In practice, In addition, greater information may be obtained by measuring
however, the IAT can nevertheless be effectively used because many the evaluative associations with two concept domains indepen-
socially significant categories form complementary pairs, such as dently rather than examining only comparative associations.
positive–negative (valence), self– other, male–female, Jewish–Christian,
Whereas measures of two concept domains can reveal two dimen-
sions of information, the IAT provides only one. For example, on
a Black–White IAT, scores are interpreted as a comparison of
one’s positive White associations and/or negative Black associa-
Andrew Karpinski and Ross B. Steinman, Department of Psychology, tions with one’s negative White associations and/or positive Black
Temple University.
associations. A high score could indicate (a) the presence of many
Ross B. Steinman is now at the Department of Psychology, Widener
University.
positive White associations, (b) the presence of many negative
We thank Shelley Keiper and Jennifer Steinberg for their helpful comments. Black associations, (c) the lack of negative White associations,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrew and/or (d) the lack of positive Black associations. From the single
Karpinski, Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 North IAT score, it is impossible to determine which of these factors, or
13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 19122-6085. E-mail: andykarp@temple.edu which combination of these factors, contributes to the overall score

16
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 17

(Blanton & Jaccard, 2006; Blanton et al., 2006; Nosek, Greenwald, attitude object. The Single Target IAT (ST-IAT; Wigboldus, Hol-
& Banaji, 2005). If a single category IAT-type task were available, land, & van Knippenberg, 2005) is conceptually identical to our
then a measure of the evaluative associations with Whites and of SC-IAT, differing only in minor procedural details. In an initial
the evaluative associations with Blacks could be obtained inde- test of its utility, a Christian ST-IAT was found to correlate with
pendently, to eliminate some of the ambiguity in the interpretation explicit attitude questions about Christianity, and an Islamic ST-
of IAT scores. IAT was found to correlate with explicit attitude questions about
Several implicit social cognition measures have been developed Islam. Neither of the implicit measures correlated with a compar-
to assess evaluative associations with a single attitude object. ative Christianity versus Islam explicit attitude measure. Addition-
Priming-based measures (see Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson, John- ally, the difference between the Christian ST-IAT and Islamic
son, & Howard, 1997; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; ST-IAT correlated weakly and only marginally significantly with
Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997), the Go/No-Go Association Task a Christian–Islamic IAT (Wigboldus et al., 2005). These findings
(Nosek & Banaji, 2001), and the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task provide some empirical evidence that a measure of associations
(De Houwer, 2003) can all be used to assess associations with a with a single category or target may reveal different information
single attitude object. Each of these measures may be used to than a comparative IAT; however, additional research is needed to
assess the evaluative associations with a single target; however, the determine whether single-category or target IAT measures are
reliability of these measures has been very low (see Bosson, reliable, valid across content domains, and relatively impervious to
Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000; De Houwer, 2003; Nosek & Banaji, self-presentation.
200l; Olson & Fazio, 2003; Teige, Schnable, Banse, & Asendorpf,
2004). Thus, there is still the need for a reliable individual- Overview of Studies
difference measure of the evaluative associations with a single
attitude object. In the four studies that follow, we examined the reliability,
validity, and susceptibility to faking of the SC-IAT in four differ-
ent concept domains. For all four studies, we also examined the
The Single Category IAT (SC-IAT)
IAT and explicit measures of attitudes. In cases where the concept
Another possible type of measure to assess the strength of of interest was a comparative domain, a difference between two
evaluative associations with a single attitude object is a modified SC-IATs (such as a White SC-IAT and a Black SC-IAT) was
version of the IAT that eliminates the need for the second contrast computed to obtain a comparative SC-IAT score. We expected an
category. We designed the SC-IAT as a two-stage modification of IAT and a comparative SC-IAT to reveal similar findings in terms
IAT procedure to measure the evaluative associations with a single of known groups validity, correlations with explicit measures, and
category or attitude object. Because the SC-IAT is a modification predictive validity. Unlike the IAT, a comparative SC-IAT can be
of IAT procedure, it shares many properties with the IAT, includ- decomposed into its components, and thus, the SC-IATs may
ing its ease of use and interpretation. In each stage, target words allow for specific conclusions in these domains, to supplement the
associated with the attitude object and an evaluative dimension are overall findings obtained from the IAT and comparative SC-IAT.
presented in random order. In the first stage, good words and In cases where measurement of associations with a single category
attitude object words are categorized on one response key, and bad may be preferable to a comparative measure, compared with the
words are categorized on a different key. In the second stage, bad IAT, we expected the SC-IAT to provide unique and meaningful
words and attitude object words are categorized on one response information about the category of interest.
key, and good words are categorized on a different key (see Table We determined a fixed order of measures for each study in order
1 for a comparison of a self– other IAT to a self-SC-IAT). to reduce the large sample size requirements if we were to coun-
Independently, other researchers have developed similar modi- terbalance the presentation of all the measures. We presented all
fications of the IAT to assess evaluative associations with a single implicit measures of attitudes prior to the explicit measures of

Table 1
Comparison of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) and Single Category IAT (SC-IAT)

IAT SC-IAT

Block Trials Function Left-key response Right-key response Block Trials Function Left-key response Right-key response

1 30 Practice Pleasant words Unpleasant words


2 30 Practice Self words Other words
3a 30 Practice Pleasant words ⫹ Unpleasant words ⫹ 1c 24 Practice Good words ⫹ self Bad words
self words other words words
4a 30 Test Pleasant words ⫹ Unpleasant words ⫹ 2c 72 Test Good words ⫹ self Bad words
self words other words words
5 30 Practice Other words Self words
6b 30 Practice Pleasant words ⫹ Unpleasant words ⫹ 3d 24 Practice Good words Bad words ⫹ self words
other words self words
7b 30 Test Pleasant words ⫹ Unpleasant words ⫹ 4d 72 Test Good words Bad words ⫹ self words
other words self words

Note. Blocks with a common subscript were experienced as one continuous block.
18 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

attitudes. Recently, Nosek and colleagues (2005) have argued that IAT measure of soda brand associations. A Coke–Pepsi IAT proce-
the order of presentation of implicit and explicit measures does not dure followed the standard IAT paradigm (see Greenwald et al., 1998) with
alter their psychometric properties or their intercorrelation. Other minor modifications. Specifically, participants completed seven stages in
studies have found evidence that the completion of explicit mea- the same order (see Table 1 for an example). In each stage, participants
sures of attitudes affects subsequent responses on implicit mea- responded to 30 target presentations, and the target stimuli were selected
randomly, without replacement. The evaluative dimension was labeled
sures and artificially increases the observed correlation between
pleasant and unpleasant, and the object dimension was labeled Coke and
implicit and explicit measures of attitudes (Bosson et al., 2000). Pepsi. Five target words were used for each of the evaluative dimensions
We know of no studies where completing the implicit measures (pleasant: brilliant, diamond, joy, truth, and sunrise; unpleasant: awkward,
has significantly affected the responses on subsequent explicit hate, failure, slum, and stink). All target words were presented in lowercase
measures. Thus, to be conservative, we always presented the letters. Five target pictures were also selected to be associated with the
implicit measures prior to the explicit self-report measures. Coke brand (pictures of two-liter bottles and six-packs of cans of Coke and
Diet Coke) and with the Pepsi brand (pictures of two-liter bottles and
Study 1 six-packs of cans of Pepsi and Pepsi One).
The procedure of the IAT was similar to the SC-IAT with five excep-
The goal of Study 1 was to examine the SC-IAT in a situation tions. First, participants responded by using the a key and the 5 key on the
where a comparative measure, such as the IAT, would be useful. number keypad to categorize target words and pictures. Second, the target
We investigated attitudes toward soda brands, with the goal of word remained on the screen until the participants responded. Third,
predicting whether participants preferred Coke products to Pepsi participants were not given feedback regarding the accuracy of their
products. Previous studies using the IAT to examine evaluative responses. Fourth, the category reminder labels were appropriately posi-
associations with Coke and Pepsi have measured associations at tioned in the center of the screen, immediately to the left or right side of the
target word. Fifth, the evaluative dimension was labeled pleasant and
the product level (such as associations with the beverages Coca-
unpleasant.
Cola and Pepsi-Cola; see Maison, Greenwald, & Bruin, 2004). In SC-IAT measure of Coke and Pepsi brand associations. The Coke
Study 1, we used the IAT and SC-IAT to measure associations at SC-IAT consisted of two stages, which all participants completed in the
the brand level. The Coke brand included the beverages Coke and same order. Each stage consisted of 24 practice trials immediately followed
Diet Coke; the Pepsi brand included the beverages Pepsi and Pepsi by 72 test trials (three blocks of 24 trials each). In the first stage (Coke ⫹
One. As a consequence, the results of this study were expected to good), Coke pictures and good words were categorized on the z key, and
be similar, but not identical to, the results of previously conducted bad words were categorized on the 2 key on the numeric keypad. In an
studies. attempt to prevent a response bias from developing, Coke pictures, good
Because this study was the first test of the SC-IAT methodol- words, and bad words were not presented at equal frequency, but were
ogy, we examined whether the SC-IAT revealed known soda presented in a 7:7:10 ratio so that 58% of correct responses were on the z
attitudes. That is, the SC-IAT should reveal that Coke product key and 42% of correct responses were on the 2 key. In the second stage
(Coke ⫹ bad), good words were categorized on the z key, and Coke
drinkers have more favorable associations with Coke products than
pictures and bad words were categorized on the 2 key on the numeric key-
with Pepsi products and that Pepsi product drinkers have more
pad. Coke pictures, good words, and bad words were presented in a 7:10:7
favorable associations with Pepsi products than with Coke prod- ratio so that 42% of correct responses were on the z key and 58% of correct
ucts. When participants choose between a Coke and a Pepsi responses were on the 2 key.
product, the outcome variable is a dichotomous choice. Because The evaluative dimension was labeled good and bad, and the object
the outcome variable is a comparative, dichotomous choice, a dimension was labeled Coke. Twenty-one target words were used for each
comparative Coke–Pepsi IAT would seem to be an ideal attitude of the evaluative dimensions (see Appendix), and all target words were
measure. As a consequence, we expected a soda SC-IAT (the presented in lowercase letters. Seven target pictures were selected to be
difference between a Coke SC-IAT and a Pepsi SC-IAT) to be associated with Coke (pictures of six-packs and two-liter bottles of Coke
similar in its effects to a Coke–Pepsi IAT, with regards to known and Diet Coke). Within each category, words and pictures were selected
group differences, correlations with explicit attitude measures, and randomly without replacement.
prediction of soda choice. Each stage was preceded by a set of instructions concerning the dimen-
sions of the categorization task and the appropriate key responses. Each
target word or picture appeared centered on the screen. Category reminder
Method labels were appropriately positioned on the bottom fourth of the screen.
The target word remained on the screen until the participants responded or
Participants
for 1,500 ms. If participants failed to respond within 1,500 ms, a reminder
Fifty-six students (41 women, 15 men) enrolled in an introductory to “Please respond more quickly!” appeared for 500 ms. This response
psychology course at Temple University participated in this experiment. window is largely window dressing; pilot testing revealed that the response
All participants received course credit for their participation. window truncates less than 1% of all critical responses. Nevertheless, the
response window creates a sense of urgency and may decrease the likeli-
hood that participants engage in controlled processing during the task.
Procedure
Following each response, participants were given feedback regarding the
Participants were tested in groups of up to 3 at a time. Each participant accuracy of their response. A green O in the center of the screen for 150
was seated at a desk with a Gateway 1.5 Gz Pentium 4 desktop computer ms followed correct responses; a red X in the center of the screen for 150
using Medialab and Direct RT software. All tasks were presented on the ms followed each incorrect response.
computer, and all participants completed the tasks in the same order: a For the Pepsi SC-IAT, the procedure was repeated with the target
Coke–Pepsi IAT measure of soda associations, a Coke SC-IAT, a Pepsi category Pepsi and target pictures of Pepsi products (pictures of six-packs
SC-IAT, and explicit measures of soda preferences. At the conclusion of and two-liter bottles of Pepsi and Pepsi One). All participants completed
the session, the participants were thanked and completely debriefed. the Pepsi ⫹ good task followed by the Pepsi ⫹ bad task.
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 19

Explicit measures of soda preferences. Next, participants completed more positive than negative associations with Coke and Pepsi.
semantic differential, feeling thermometer, and rating scale measures re- Finally, a soda SC-IAT D score was computed by subtracting the
garding their attitudes toward the Coke and Pepsi brands. For the semantic Pepsi SC-IAT D score from the Coke SC-IAT D score.
differential, participants rated the Coke and Pepsi brands on five bipolar
dimensions: ugly– beautiful, bad– good, unpleasant–pleasant, foolish–
wise, and awful–nice. Each dimension was rated on a 7-point scale ranging Reliability of the SC-IAT and IAT
from ⫺3 (the negative pole) to 3 (the positive pole), and participants were
instructed to circle zero if the anchoring adjectives were irrelevant to the To determine the reliability of the SC-IAT, we divided each
concept (Coke, ␣ ⫽ .88; Pepsi, ␣ ⫽ .92). A semantic differential measure SC-IAT into thirds (blocks of 24 test trials) and calculated a
of soda brand preference was computed by subtracting semantic differen- SC-IAT score separately for each third of the trials without divid-
tial ratings of the Pepsi brand from semantic differential ratings of the Coke ing by the standard deviation of correct response times. A measure
brand. For the feeling thermometer, participants were asked to rate how of internal consistency was obtained by calculating the average
positive or negative they found the Coke and Pepsi brands on a scale from intercorrelation among these scores. Dividing the task into thirds
0 (extremely negative) to 100 (extremely positive). A feeling thermometer (or halves) underestimates the reliability of the entire measure.
measure of soda brand preference was computed by subtracting feeling Fortunately, the Spearman–Brown correction can be applied to
thermometer ratings of Pepsi from thermometer ratings of Coke. A rating
compensate for this underestimate of the true internal consistency
scale measure of Coke brand enjoyment was obtained by asking partici-
for the entire measure (designated adjusted r; Nunnally, 1978). All
pants to indicate their agreement or disagreement on a 6-point scale with
the following statements: “I enjoy drinking Coke (and Coke products)” and internal consistency correlations reported in this article have been
“Coke products satisfy my thirst.” Higher numbers indicated more agree- adjusted by using the Spearman–Brown correction. These adjusted
ment with the statement (Coke, ␣ ⫽ .49). These questions were repeated reliability coefficients are conceptually equivalent and directly
with Pepsi as the target brand (Pepsi, ␣ ⫽ .67). A rating scale measure of comparable to the Cronbach’s alphas computed for the explicit
soda preference was computed by subtracting ratings of Pepsi from ratings measures.
of Coke. All three explicit brand attitude measures correlated strongly with A reliability analysis on the SC-IAT measures from Study 1
each other (Coke attitudes, ␣ ⫽ .81; Pepsi attitudes, ␣ ⫽ .90; and soda revealed a reasonable level of internal consistency (Coke, adjusted
[Coke – Pepsi] attitudes, ␣ ⫽ .92). As a result, the three explicit measures r ⫽ .61; Pepsi, adjusted r ⫽ .69). For the IAT, a reliability
were standardized and averaged to create standardized explicit brand
correlation was computed by correlating the IAT score computed
attitude ratings of Coke, Pepsi, and soda (Coke – Pepsi).
from the practice trials with an IAT score computed from the test
Finally, participants answered a behavioral intention question. Partici-
pants indicated whether they would prefer a free Coke or Pepsi product trials (following the procedure outlined by Greenwald et al., 2003;
(indifference and refusal were also response options). On the basis of this adjusted r ⫽ .82). Overall, the reliability of the SC-IAT is some-
question, participants were defined as Coke drinkers (n ⫽ 17), Pepsi what low compared with the IAT. However, the reliability of the
drinkers (n ⫽ 30), or neither (n ⫽ 6). SC-IAT is similar to the reliability typically observed for IAT
measures (see Greenwald et al., 2003; Nosek et al., 2005) and
higher than the reliability of other implicit measures (see Bosson et
Results
al., 2000; Olson & Fazio, 2003).
IAT and SC-IAT Data Reduction
Implicit and Explicit Measures of Soda Attitudes
Compared with the IAT, error rates were significantly higher on
both the Coke SC-IAT, t(55) ⫽ 4.19, p ⬍ .01, and the Pepsi First, we divided the sample into Coke drinkers and Pepsi
SC-IAT, t(55) ⫽ 3.61, p ⬍ .01. This result is not surprising given drinkers, on the basis of the behavioral choices. The predicted
that the response window in the SC-IAT procedure was included to differences emerged on all the comparative soda measures (see top
facilitate quick responding, and quicker responding is likely to be of Table 2). Coke drinkers displayed a greater bias for Coke
accompanied by increased error rates. Participants with an error compared with Pepsi than did Pepsi drinkers on the IAT, SC-IAT,
rate greater than 20% on the soda IAT, the Coke SC-IAT, or the and explicit attitude measures ( ps ⬍ .02, ds ⱖ 0.87).
Pepsi SC-IAT were excluded from analysis, resulting in the elim- Single category measures also tended to reveal the expected
ination of 3 participants (average error rates: Coke SC-IAT ⫽ group differences (see bottom of Table 2). Coke drinkers had more
6.60%; Pepsi SC-IAT ⫽ 6.43%; soda IAT ⫽ 3.25%). favorable explicit attitudes toward Coke than did Pepsi drinkers
IAT scores were computed by using the newer D-score algo- (d ⫽ 0.69), and Pepsi drinkers had more favorable attitudes toward
rithm for IAT data (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). For the Pepsi than did Coke drinkers (d ⫽ 2.40). A Coke SC-IAT revealed
resulting IAT scores, higher numbers indicated a bias for Coke no significant difference in evaluative Coke associations for Coke
compared with Pepsi. For the SC-IAT, a scoring algorithm was and Pepsi drinkers (d ⫽ 0.20), but a Pepsi SC-IAT revealed that
modeled on the D-score algorithm used for the IAT data. Because Pepsi drinkers had more positive associations with Pepsi than did
the 24 practice trials in each stage were truly practice, data from Coke drinkers (d ⫽ 0.94).
the practice blocks were discarded (Blocks 1 and 3). Responses
less than 350 ms were eliminated, nonresponses were eliminated, Correlational Analyses
and error responses were replaced with the block mean plus an
error penalty of 400 ms. The average response times of Block 2 Soda IAT scores were significantly correlated with soda SC-
(e.g., Coke ⫹ good) were subtracted from the average response IAT scores, r(51) ⫽ .29, p ⫽ .04, suggesting that the soda brand
times of Block 4 (e.g., Coke ⫹ bad). This quantity was divided by associations measured by the SC-IAT were significantly related to
the standard deviation of all correct response times within Blocks the soda brand associations measured by the IAT, as expected.
2 and 4. Thus, Coke SC-IAT and Pepsi SC-IAT D scores indicate However, soda IAT scores were unrelated to explicit soda ratings,
20 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

Table 2
Study 1: Summary Statistics for the IAT and Explicit Attitude Measures by Soda Preference

Coke drinkers Pepsi drinkers


(n ⫽ 17) (n ⫽ 30) Difference

Attitude measure M SD M SD d t(45) p

Comparative measures

Soda IAT 0.35 0.21 0.02 0.44 0.87 2.91 ⬍.01


Soda SC-IAT 0.34 0.44 ⫺0.03 0.53 0.74 2.47 .02
Explicit soda attitudes 0.91 0.72 ⫺0.56 0.66 2.11 7.08 ⬍.01

Single category measures

SC-IAT
Coke SC-IAT 0.25 0.30 0.18 0.35 0.20 0.68 .50
Pepsi SC-IAT ⫺0.09 0.33 0.22 0.31 0.94 3.16 ⬍.01
Explicit attitudes
Explicit Coke attitude 0.42 0.83 ⫺0.15 0.80 0.69 2.32 .03
Explicit Pepsi attitude ⫺0.79 0.77 0.60 0.42 2.40 8.08 ⬍.01

Note. For comparative soda measures, positive values indicate a bias or preference for Coke over Pepsi. For
single category Coke and Pepsi measures, positive values indicate a bias or preference for Coke or Pepsi,
respectively. IAT ⫽ Implicit Association Test; SC-IAT ⫽ Single Category Implicit Association Test.

r(51) ⫽ .18, p ⫽ .20. Follow-up analyses revealed that soda IAT trolled, the IAT, Coke SC-IAT, and Pepsi SC-IAT significantly
scores were also unrelated to explicit Coke or explicit Pepsi predicted soda choice.1
attitudes, r(51) ⫽ .21, p ⫽ .14, and r(51) ⫽ ⫺.05, p ⫽ .74, An alternative method of comparing the predictive validity of
respectively. the SC-IAT and the IAT is to examine the percentage of variance
As expected, soda SC-IAT scores were positively correlated accounted for by SC-IAT and IAT measures in soda choice. A
with explicit soda attitudes, r(51) ⫽ .29, p ⫽ .04. Follow-up series of regression analyses revealed that the Coke and Pepsi
analyses revealed that Coke SC-IAT scores were positively corre- SC-IATs predicted 17% of the variance in soda choice (7% beyond
lated with explicit Coke attitudes, r(51) ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .05, and the variance explained by explicit measures). A soda IAT pre-
uncorrelated with explicit Pepsi attitudes, r(51) ⫽ ⫺.01, p ⫽ .97. dicted 12% of the variance in soda choice (4.9% beyond the
Similarly, Pepsi SC-IAT scores were positively correlated with variance explained by explicit measures). Taken together, these
explicit Pepsi attitudes, r(51) ⫽ .26, p ⫽ .06, and uncorrelated with results provide strong evidence for the utility of the SC-IAT above
explicit Coke attitudes, r(51) ⫽ ⫺.07, p ⫽ .61. These results and beyond the IAT and explicit attitude measures.
suggest that Coke and Pepsi SC-IAT scores reveal specific infor-
mation pertaining to Coke and Pepsi preferences, respectively, and Information Obtained From the SC-IAT and IAT
not general information about soda preferences.
Additionally, attitudes and associations with Coke were unre- One advantage to using two single attitude measures as opposed
lated to attitudes and associations with Pepsi on both explicit to one comparative attitude is that a greater amount of information
ratings, r(51) ⫽ ⫺.01, p ⫽ .93, and the SC-IAT measures, r(51) ⫽ is obtained from the two single attitude measures (see Figure 1).
⫺.06, p ⫽ .66. On the top panel of Figure 1, Coke SC-IAT scores are plotted
against Pepsi SC-IAT scores. From this panel, information about
Coke, Pepsi, and soda (Coke – Pepsi) associations may be ob-
Prediction of Soda Choice
tained. Some participants had favorable associations with both
Next, we examined the ability of the IAT, SC-IAT, and explicit Coke and Pepsi (the upper right quadrant), some had unfavorable
measures to predict soda choice. For the soda choice outcome associations with both Coke and Pepsi (the lower left quadrant),
variable, participants who chose a Coke product were given a and some had favorable associations toward one soda and unfa-
value of 1, participants who indicated no preference were given a
value of 0, and participants who chose a Pepsi product were given
1
a value of ⫺1. First, the Coke SC-IAT, Pepsi SC-IAT, and soda If the 6 individuals who did not indicate a preference for a Coke or
IAT were entered in a simultaneous regression predicting soda Pepsi product are eliminated from the analysis, then these relationships can
also be examined by using a logistic regression predicting soda choice. For
choice (see top of Table 3). These results indicated that both the
a logistic regression with implicit measures only, the results closely parallel
IAT and Pepsi SC-IAT significantly predicted soda choice, but the the standard regression analysis. IAT scores ( p ⫽ .05) and Pepsi SC-IAT
Coke SC-IAT was unrelated to soda choice. Next, the analysis was scores ( p ⫽ .06) predicted the soda choice, whereas Coke SC-IAT scores
repeated with explicit soda attitudes added to the model (see did not ( p ⫽ .63). However, when explicit soda attitudes were added to the
bottom of Table 3). Explicit attitudes were strong predictors of equation, none of the implicit measures or the explicit measure uniquely
soda choice. However, even once explicit preferences were con- predicted soda choice (all ps ⬎ .28).
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 21

Table 3 it is somewhat surprising that the soda SC-IAT measure correlated


Study 1: Predicting Soda Choice significantly with explicit soda preferences, whereas the IAT failed
to correlate with explicit measures of soda attitudes. Overall, these
Prediction of soda choice results suggest that the SC-IAT may have some utility above and
Predictor ␤ t p
beyond the IAT even in situations where the outcome variable is
comparative.
Implicit measures only Although the Coke and Pepsi SC-IATs performed well on a
number of aspects, there were a couple of curious aspects of these
Coke SC-IAT .06 0.47 .64
results. First, the reliability of the SC-IAT measures was lower
Pepsi SC-IAT ⫺.35 2.74 ⬍.01
IAT .28 2.10 .04 than the reliability observed for the IAT. One possibility is that not
having a comparative category for the attitude object may result in
Implicit and explicit measures extra error variance in responses. If this were true, then SC-IAT
measures would inherently have lower reliabilities than IAT mea-
Coke SC-IAT .16 1.76 .09
Pepsi SC-IAT ⫺.22 2.33 .02 sures. A second possible explanation for the low reliability of the
IAT .23 2.43 .02 SC-IATs is that the 1,500-ms response window not only resulted
Explicit soda attitudes .67 7.37 ⬍.01 in a higher error rate, but also increased unreliability for the
SC-IAT. A final possibility is that the order of the tasks adversely
Note. N ⫽ 52. All variables were entered simultaneously. SC-IAT ⫽
Single Category Implicit Association Test; IAT ⫽ Implicit Association
affected the reliability of the SC-IAT. For the IAT, all responses to
Test. the attitude objects of interest are comparative. After completing
an IAT, participants may continue to think about the attitude
objects in a comparative manner. If a SC-IAT were to follow an
vorable associations toward the other soda (the remaining two IAT, participants’ responses may be influenced by the comparative
quadrants). Information about participants’ relative soda associa- mind-set induced by the IAT, perhaps resulting in increased error
tions can be obtained by observing where the participant falls in
reference to the dashed diagonal line. Participants with SC-IAT
scores falling above the line had more positive and/or less negative
associations with Coke than Pepsi; participants with SC-IAT
scores falling below the line had more positive and/or less negative
associations with Pepsi than Coke. In the bottom panel of Figure 1,
soda IAT scores are plotted. Only information about participants’
relative soda associations is available from this panel. Participants
with scores on the left side of the graph (scores less than zero) had
more positive and/or less negative associations with Pepsi than
Coke, whereas participants with scores on the right side of the
graph (scores greater than zero) had more positive and/or less
negative associations with Coke than Pepsi.

Discussion
These results provide initial support for the SC-IAT as a valid
measure of evaluative associations with a single attitude object.
The validity of the SC-IAT was established on multiple levels:
known groups validity, convergent validity, and predictive valid-
ity. First, along with the IAT and explicit soda preferences, the
soda SC-IAT discriminated between Coke and Pepsi drinkers.
Second, the SC-IAT measure of soda preferences correlated sig-
nificantly with explicit soda preferences. The Coke and Pepsi
SC-IATs were specific in their measurement of associations. The
Pepsi SC-IAT revealed that Pepsi drinkers had more positive than
negative associations with Pepsi, compared with Coke drinkers.
The Pepsi SC-IAT also correlated with explicit Pepsi attitudes, but
not with explicit Coke attitudes. Likewise, the Coke SC-IAT
correlated with explicit Coke attitudes, but not with explicit Pepsi
attitudes. Third, the Pepsi SC-IAT, and to a lesser extent the Coke
SC-IAT, reliably predicted intended soda choice, even when con-
trolling for IAT scores and explicit attitude ratings. Figure 1. A comparison of information available from independent Coke
This study was ideally suited for a comparative IAT style of and Pepsi Single Category Implicit Association Tests (SC-IATs) and a
measurement, and as expected, the IAT discriminated between Coke–Pepsi Implicit Association Test (IAT). IAT and SC-IAT scores were
Coke and Pepsi drinkers and predicted intended soda choice. Thus, calculated by using the D-score algorithm (N ⫽ 53).
22 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

variance in the SC-IAT. The issue of the reliability of the SC-IAT ipant’s first name, participant’s last name, me, I, and myself). The good
is one that will be revisited in subsequent studies. and bad target words were identical to the words used in Study 1. All target
A second curious finding in Study 1 is that all Pepsi measures and category words were presented in lowercase letters. Participants first
(SC-IAT and explicit measures) discriminated between Coke and completed the self ⫹ positive blocks, followed by the self ⫹ negative
blocks.
Pepsi drinkers better and predicted soda choice better than did
IAT measure of self– other associations. The IAT procedure was iden-
Coke measures. We are puzzled by this finding, but it may be the
tical to the procedure used in Study 1, with the exception of the target
result of the Temple University environment. Temple University words and category labels. The category labels self and other replaced the
has an exclusive agreement with Pepsi to provide soft drinks on labels of Coke and Pepsi, respectively. Five target words were also selected
campus. All vending machines and restaurants on campus serve to be associated with each of the attitude objects (self: participant’s first
only Pepsi products. Thus, Temple University students may re- name, participant’s last name, me, I, and myself; other: he, her, his, hers,
ceive more information regarding Pepsi products than Coke prod- and person).
ucts, and this environmental bias may lead to greater predictive Explicit measures of self-esteem. Participants next completed three
validity and known-groups validity for the Pepsi SC-IAT than for explicit measures of self-esteem: a self semantic differential, a self feeling
the Coke SC-IAT. Regardless, evidence for the validity of the thermometer, and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). For
the semantic differential, participants rated self on five bipolar dimensions:
Coke SC-IAT was obtained; the Coke SC-IAT correlated with
ugly– beautiful, bad– good, unpleasant–pleasant, foolish–wise, and awful–
explicit Coke attitudes and not explicit Pepsi attitudes.
nice. Each dimension was rated on a 7-point scale ranging from ⫺3
(negative pole) to 3 (positive pole). The five items were summed to form
Study 2 a semantic differential measure of self-esteem (␣ ⫽ .55). The self feeling
thermometer consisted of a single item, with participants rating themselves
A comparative attitude or associative measure is not ideally on a thermometer ranging from 0 (cold or unfavorable) to 100 (warm or
suited for all contexts. For example, when investigators wish to favorable). For presentation purposes, feeling thermometer scores have
predict smoking behavior, there is no clear complementary behav- been rescaled to range from ⫺50 to 50 so that zero indicates neutral
ior to smoking; it depends on the nature of the research question. self-attitudes. For the Rosenberg scale, participants responded to each item
Similarly, self-esteem researchers may be interested in assessing on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly).
evaluative associations with the self and not comparative self– The 10 items were averaged to compute a measure of self-esteem (␣ ⫽
other associations (see Karpinski, 2004). In these situations, a .86). These three explicit measures of self-esteem were strongly interre-
lated (␣ ⫽ .80), and thus, the three measures were standardized and
SC-IAT measure of associations may provide a more specific
averaged to compute a single explicit measure of self-esteem.
measure of the evaluative associations in question than an IAT.
Because self-report measures of self-esteem are not explicitly
comparative, we expected to find that a self-SC-IAT would cor- Results
relate more strongly with explicit measures of self-esteem than
IAT and SC-IAT Data Reduction and Reliability Analysis
would a self– other IAT.
Study 2 was designed to investigate the use of a SC-IAT as a Because of a computer error, the explicit attitude data were lost
measure of self-associations. The procedure of Study 2 closely for 9 participants. Replicating the results of Study 1, error rates
followed the procedure of Study 1, with one key exception. To were significantly higher for the self-SC-IAT than for the self–
examine the possibility that taking a (comparative) IAT prior to a other IAT, t(65) ⫽ 3.15, p ⬍ .01. Participants with an IAT error
SC-IAT adversely affected the reliability of the SC-IAT, we rate greater than 20% were excluded from analysis, resulting in the
switched the order of the SC-IAT and the IAT. Thus, participants elimination of 5 participants. Participants with a SC-IAT error rate
completed a self-SC-IAT, a self– other IAT, and then explicit greater than 20% were also excluded, resulting in elimination of 9
measures of self-esteem. additional participants. The resulting error rates were consistent
with those observed in Study 1 (self-SC-IAT ⫽ 6.60%, self– other
Method IAT ⫽ 4.53%).
IAT and SC-IAT scores were computed by using the scoring
Participants algorithms described for Study 1. Self– other IAT scores were
Sixty-six students (16 men, 41 women, 9 unknown) enrolled in an computed so that higher numbers indicate more positive associa-
introductory psychology course at Temple University participated in this tions with the self (and/or negative associations with an other) than
experiment. All participants received course credit for their participation. negative associations with the self (and/or positive associations
with an other), and the self-SC-IAT scores were such that higher
Procedure scores indicate greater positive than negative associations with the
self.
All tasks were presented on the computer, and all participants completed To determine the reliability of the SC-IAT, we divided each
the tasks in the same order: a SC-IAT measure of self-associations, an IAT SC-IAT into thirds and calculated a SC-IAT score separately for
measure of self– other associations, and explicit measures of self-esteem. each block of 24 test trials. In this study, the SC-IAT displayed a
At the conclusion of the session, the participants were thanked and com-
level of reliability that is similar to the level of reliability typically
pletely debriefed.
SC-IAT measure of self-associations. The self-SC-IAT was identical to
found for the IAT (adjusted r ⫽ .73). For the IAT, a reliability
the Coke and Pepsi SC-IATs used in Study 1, with the exception of the correlation was computed by correlating the IAT score computed
target words and category labels. The evaluative dimension remained from the practice trials with the IAT score computed from the test
labeled good and bad, and the object dimension was labeled self. Five trials. The observed reliability coefficient was on the lower end of
target words were selected to be associated with the category self ( partic- what is typically observed in IAT data (adjusted r ⫽ .58).
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 23

Average Levels of Self-Esteem self-SC-IAT revealed more positive than negative self-
associations, suggesting positive self-esteem in the sample. Unlike
Overall, all measures showed a pattern consistent with an inter- the self– other IAT, the self-SC-IAT correlated significantly with
pretation of high self-esteem (see Table 4). All three explicit explicit measures of self-esteem. These correlations are larger than
measures of self-esteem revealed the presence of positive self- correlations typically observed between implicit and explicit mea-
evaluations (all ds ⱖ 1.35). Implicit measures revealed a similar sures of self-esteem (see Bosson et al., 2000; Greenwald & Farn-
pattern. The IAT revealed that participants had more positive ham, 2000) but are similar in size to correlations found between an
self-associations and/or negative other associations than negative affective priming measure of self-esteem and explicit measures of
self-associations and/or positive other associations (d ⫽ 1.84). The self-esteem (Wentura, Kulfanek, & Greve, 2005).
SC-IAT revealed that participants had more positive self- In addition, a small and nonsignificant relationship was ob-
associations than negative self-associations (d ⫽ 1.13). No signif- served between self-SC-IAT scores and self– other IAT scores.
icant gender differences were observed on any of the measures of Karpinski (2004) hypothesized that the evaluative self– other as-
self-esteem (all ps ⱖ .13). sociations measured by the IAT may be qualitatively different
from evaluative self-associations, and these results provide support
Relationship Between Implicit and Explicit Measures of for this claim. The self– other IAT has proven to be a useful
Self-Esteem measure of self-esteem in many contexts, yet the current findings
suggest that a self-SC-IAT may provide additional information
It is interesting to note that the self-SC-IAT and self– other IAT
about implicit self-esteem that is not captured by the self– other
scores were only marginally correlated, r(50) ⫽ .25, p ⫽ .07. In
IAT. However, the correlation we observed between explicit mea-
other words, the self-associations measured by the SC-IAT were
sures of self-esteem and the self– other IAT is lower than what is
only weakly related to the self– other associations measured by the
typically reported (e.g., see Bosson et al., 2000). The reasons for
self– other IAT. The self– other IAT failed to correlate with the
this discrepancy are unclear, but the end result is that this study
standardized explicit measure of self-esteem, r(42) ⫽ .01, p ⫽ .93.
may underestimate the IAT– explicit self-esteem relationship and
Conversely, a significant positive correlation was observed be-
the SC-IAT/IAT relationship.
tween the self-SC-IAT and the explicit measure of self-esteem,
The results of Study 2 provided stronger evidence for the
r(42) ⫽ .38, p ⫽ .01.
reliability of the SC-IAT than did Study 1. In this study, the
A regression analysis was conducted to investigate whether the
SC-IAT displayed a level of internal consistency similar to the
self-SC-IAT predicted unique variance in explicit reports of self-
reliabilities that are typically found by using the IAT. One differ-
esteem. Consistent with the correlation findings, the regression
ence between this study and Study 1 is that in the current study, the
analysis revealed that self-SC-IAT scores were uniquely predictive
SC-IAT measure was obtained prior to the IAT measure. This
of explicit self-esteem (␤ ⫽ .40, p ⫽ .01), whereas IAT scores
result provides indirect support to the hypothesis that the comple-
were not uniquely predictive of explicit self-esteem (␤ ⫽ ⫺.09,
tion of a (comparative) IAT may interfere with ensuing SC-IAT
p ⫽ .54).
measures. Thus, for subsequent studies, we will present the SC-
IAT measures prior to the IAT measures.
Discussion
Study 2 provided additional evidence for the reliability and Study 3
validity of the SC-IAT as a measure of implicit social cognition.
Like the self– other IAT and explicit measures of self-esteem, the One of the more interesting applications of the IAT is its use as
a measure of implicit racial associations. Studies using a Black–
White IAT have typically found that White participants display a
large racial bias in favor of Whites and/or against Blacks (Das-
Table 4 gupta, McGhee, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2000; Greenwald et al.,
Study 2: Descriptive Statistics 1998; Monteith, Voils, & Ashburn-Nardo, 2001; Nosek, Banaji, &
Difference from midpoint
Greenwald, 2002). One possible interpretation of this effect is that
“virtually all White Americans may have automatic negative as-
Measure M SD t p d sociations to African American names” (Greenwald et al., 1998, p.
1475). Yet, because of the comparative nature of the IAT, there are
Implicit measures
multiple interpretations of this IAT race bias. For example, a
Self–other IAT 0.58 0.32 t(51) ⫽ 13.24 ⬍.01 1.84 person who has no evaluative associations with Blacks, and mostly
Self-SC-IAT 0.45 0.40 t(51) ⫽ 8.13 ⬍.01 1.13 positive associations with Whites, would display the typical IAT
race bias. Likewise, an IAT racial bias may emerge for participants
Explicit measures who have mostly positive associations with Blacks but also have
Rosenberg SE Scale 5.39 1.03 t(43) ⫽ 8.94 ⬍.01 1.35 more positive associations with Whites (Blanton & Jaccard, 2006;
Self semantic differential 8.74 2.93 t(43) ⫽ 19.75 ⬍.01 2.98 Blanton et al., 2006; Gehring, Karpinski, & Hilton, 2003). In these
Self feeling thermometer 27.70 18.54 t(43) ⫽ 9.91 ⬍.01 1.49 alternative interpretations of the IAT race bias, participants would
not have automatic negative associations with Blacks.
Note. Midpoint is the middle point of the scale or the point of the scale
at which a person has neutral self-associations. IAT ⫽ Implicit Association
We hypothesized that the SC-IAT could be used to help inter-
Test; SC-IAT ⫽ Single Category Implicit Association Test; SE ⫽ Self- pret meaning of the IAT race bias. For Study 3, separate Black and
Esteem. White SC-IATs were used to assess the strength of evaluative
24 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

associations with Blacks and Whites independently. We expected priate for a Black–White IAT. The category labels White American and
a race bias SC-IAT (White SC-IAT–Black SC-IAT) to reveal a Black American replaced the labels of Coke and Pepsi, respectively. Six
similar race bias as the IAT; White SC-IAT scores were hypoth- target faces were also selected to be associated with each of the attitude
esized to be higher than Black SC-IAT scores. If this effect is due objects (three male and three female for each race, identical to the stimuli
used for the SC-IATs). In addition, several of the unpleasant target words
to out-group prejudice, then participants would also have negative
were changed to avoid overlap with the Black stereotype (see Appendix).
Black SC-IAT scores. However, if the SC-IAT and IAT race
Finally, unlike Studies 1 and 2, participants received response feedback for
biases are a result of in-group favoritism, then we would expect to correct and incorrect responses. Participants viewed a green O for 150 ms
observe positive evaluative associations on a White SC-IAT and following a correct response and a red X for 150 ms following an incorrect
less positive (but not negative) evaluative associations on a Black response.
SC-IAT. Furthermore, we explored differences in automatic race Explicit measures of race attitudes. Participants next completed two
biases between White and Black participants. Previous studies explicit measures of White and Black attitudes: a semantic differential
have found that Black participants tend to show either no IAT race (White, ␣ ⫽ .84; Black, ␣ ⫽ .89) and a feeling thermometer. These
bias or a small IAT race bias in favor of Whites and/or against measures were identical to those used in the previous studies, but the target
Blacks (Nosek et al., 2002). category labels White American and Black American were used to assess
attitudes toward Whites and Blacks, respectively. These two explicit atti-
In Study 3, participants completed a White SC-IAT, a Black
tudes measures were strongly related (White, ␣ ⫽ .64; Black, ␣ ⫽ .81), and
SC-IAT, a Black–White IAT, and explicit measures of racial
thus they were standardized and averaged to form composite explicit
attitudes. Several modifications were made to both the procedures attitude measures of Whites and Blacks. Explicit measures of comparative
of the IAT and SC-IAT. First, participants taking the IAT received racial attitudes were obtained by subtracting the explicit measures of
response feedback, seeing a green O following a correct response Blacks from the explicit measures of Whites, such that higher numbers
and a red X following an incorrect response. This modification is indicated more favorable attitudes of Whites compared with Blacks.
in line with standard versions of the IAT that typically include
response feedback. Second, we reduced the number of critical
Results
trials in each SC-IAT from 72 to 48 in order to reduce the time
required for each SC-IAT. Third, we increased the response win- IAT and SC-IAT Data Reduction and Reliability Analysis
dow for the SC-IAT from 1,500 ms to 2,000 ms. If the 1,500-ms
response window was contributing to high error rates on the Compared with the IAT, error rates were significantly higher on
SC-IAT, then increasing the response window may alleviate this the White SC-IAT, t(117) ⫽ 7.93, p ⬍ .01, and the Black SC-IAT,
problem. t(117) ⫽ 7.40, p ⬍ .01. Participants with an error rate greater than
20% on the Black SC-IAT, the White SC-IAT, or race IAT were
Method excluded from analysis, resulting in the elimination of 5 partici-
pants. The resulting average error rates were consistent with error
Participants rates observed in previous studies (White SC-IAT ⫽ 6.15%; Black
SC-IAT ⫽ 5.97%; race IAT ⫽ 3.08%). These findings suggest that
Eighty-one White students (24 men, 57 women) and 37 Black students
(9 men, 28 women) enrolled in an introductory psychology course at
increasing the response window did not result in a reduction of the
Temple University participated in this experiment. All participants re- SC-IAT error rate.
ceived course credit for their participation. As in the previous studies, IAT and SC-IAT scores were com-
puted by using the D algorithm. A comparative race SC-IAT score
Procedure was obtained by subtracting Black SC-IAT scores from White
SC-IAT scores. For both the race SC-IAT and IAT, positive scores
The procedure mirrored the procedure of Study 1, with Black American indicate more positive associations with Whites (and/or negative
and White American as the categories of interest. All tasks were presented associations with Blacks) than negative associations with Whites
on the computer, and all participants completed the tasks in the same order: (and/or positive associations with Blacks).
a White-SC-IAT, a Black-SC-IAT, a Black–White IAT, and explicit mea- To determine the reliability of the SC-IAT, we calculated a
sures of racial attitudes. At the conclusion of the session, the participants
SC-IAT score for each of the two test blocks of 24 test trials. A
were thanked and completely debriefed.
SC-IAT measure of Black and White associations. The Black SC-IAT
reliability analysis on these scores revealed greater internal con-
and White SC-IAT were similar to the soda SC-IATs used in Study 1, with sistency for the White SC-IAT (adjusted r ⫽ .70) than for the
three exceptions. First, the target words and category labels were changed Black SC-IAT (adjusted r ⫽ .55). For the IAT, a reliability
to be relevant for the categories Black and White. The evaluative dimen- correlation was computed by correlating IAT scores computed
sion remained labeled good and bad; the category dimension was labeled from the practice trials with IAT scores computed from the test
White American for the White SC-IAT and Black American for the Black trials (adjusted r ⫽ .75). Similar to Study 1, the SC-IAT displayed
SC-IAT. Six Black faces and six White faces were selected to be associated lower levels of internal consistency than did the IAT.
with the categories Black and White, respectively. These face stimuli were
identical to those used by Nosek et al. (2002). Second, the response
window was lengthened from 1,500 ms to 2,000 ms. If participants did not Implicit and Explicit Measures of Racial Attitudes
respond within 2,000 ms of the presentation of the target word or face, they
were reminded to respond more quickly. Third, the number of critical trials Explicit measures. For the comparative explicit race bias mea-
was reduced from 72 trials to 48 trials. sures, White and Black participants reported an in-group bias (see
IAT measure of Black–White associations. The IAT procedure was top of Table 5). On both the feeling thermometer and semantic
identical to the procedure used in Study 1, with two exceptions. First, the differential, White participants rated Whites more positively than
target words and pictures and category labels were modified to be appro- Blacks ( ps ⱕ .01, ds ⱖ 1.39), and Black participants rated Blacks
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 25

Table 5
Study 3: Descriptive Statistics

Difference between
White and Black
Whites (n ⫽ 79) Blacks (n ⫽ 34) participants

Attitude measure M SD d M SD d t(111) p d

Explicit measures

Race feeling thermometer 9.19 16.22 0.57 ⫺15.21 16.26 ⫺0.94 7.33 ⬍.01 1.39
White feeling thermometer 24.35 14.19 1.72 18.91 17.36 1.09 1.75 .08 0.33
Black feeling thermometer 15.16 16.65 0.91 24.12 15.82 2.15 5.63 ⬍.01 1.07
Race semantic differential 0.40 1.02 0.39 ⫺0.47 0.75 ⫺0.62 4.45 ⬍.01 0.85
White semantic differential 0.85 0.84 1.01 0.61 0.82 0.75 1.38 .17 0.26
Black semantic differential 0.45 0.80 0.56 1.08 0.98 1.10 3.60 ⬍.01 0.68

Implicit measures

Race IAT 0.63 0.30 2.07 0.09 0.42 0.22 7.68 ⬍.01 1.46
Race SC-IAT 0.16 0.52 0.30 ⫺0.07 0.63 ⫺0.10 2.03 .04 0.39
White SC-IAT 0.17 0.37 0.46 0.16 0.44 0.36 0.18 .86 0.03
Black SC-IAT 0.01 0.33 0.04 0.22 0.43 0.52 2.80 ⬍.01 0.49

Note. For comparative race measures, higher numbers indicate more positive (or fewer negative) attitudes or
associations with Whites than with Blacks. For single category measures, higher numbers indicate more positive
attitudes. IAT ⫽ Implicit Association Test; SC-IAT ⫽ Single Category Implicit Association Test.

more positively than Whites ( ps ⱕ .01, ds ⱖ .62). Examination of 0.49. Follow-up analyses revealed that Black participants had
the separate ratings of Whites and Blacks revealed that these positive associations with Blacks (d ⫽ 0.52, p ⬍ .01), and White
in-group biases were due to in-group favoritism and not out-group participants had equally positive and negative associations with
prejudice. On both the semantic differential and feeling thermom- Blacks (d ⫽ 0.04, p ⫽ .72). These results suggest that, on average,
eter measures, White and Black participants reported favorable the SC-IAT racial bias emerged for Whites because of implicit
attitudes toward both Whites and Blacks (all ps ⱕ .01, ds ⱖ 0.56). in-group favoritism (favorable associations with Whites) and not
Implicit measures. As expected, for White participants, the because of implicit out-group prejudice (negative associations with
implicit comparative measures of racial attitudes revealed signif- Blacks).
icant race biases (see bottom of Table 5). The IAT revealed a large SC-IAT scores can also be used to help interpret the contribu-
race bias (d ⫽ 2.07, p ⬍ .01), whereas the SC-IAT revealed a tion of evaluative White and Black associations to IAT scores. To
small- to medium-sized race bias (d ⫽ 0.30, p ⬍ .01). Echoing the investigate whether IAT scores reflect meaningful variations in
findings on the explicit measures, these findings indicate that both evaluative White and evaluative Black associations, a regres-
White participants showed an in-group bias at the implicit level, sion analysis was conducted to predict IAT scores from Black
favoring Whites over Blacks. For Black participants, both implicit SC-IAT and White SC-IAT scores. This analysis revealed that race
measures of race bias revealed no significant racial biases (IAT, IAT scores were positively associated with White SC-IAT scores
d ⫽ 0.22, p ⫽ .22; SC-IAT, d ⫽ ⫺0.10, p ⫽ .57). A test for the (␤ ⫽ .24, p ⬍ .01) and negatively associated with Black SC-IAT
difference between these effects revealed that White participants scores (␤ ⫽ ⫺.30, p ⬍ .01). Thus, as expected, a Black–White
displayed a significantly stronger in-group bias than did Black IAT measured evaluative associations with both Blacks and
participants on both the IAT (d ⫽ 1.46, p ⬍ .01) and the SC-IAT Whites.
(d ⫽ 0.39, p ⫽ .04). Because the IAT cannot be decomposed into Correlations between measures. As expected, race SC-IAT
component attitudes of Whites and attitudes of Blacks, no further scores were significantly correlated with IAT scores, r(111) ⫽ .40,
conclusions can be made about the nature of the race bias observed p ⬍ .01. This relationship remained significant when examining
with the IAT. only White participants, r(77) ⫽ .36, p ⬍ .01, and also when
However, the race SC-IAT is composed of a difference between examining only Black participants, r(32) ⫽ .40, p ⫽ .02. Further-
a White SC-IAT and a Black SC-IAT, and each of these compo- more, White SC-IAT scores were uncorrelated with Black SC-IAT
nents can be analyzed separately. An analysis of the White SC-IAT scores, r(111) ⫽ ⫺.11, p ⫽ .23, suggesting that evaluative asso-
component revealed that both White and Black participants had ciations with Whites were independent from evaluative associa-
more positive than negative associations with Whites (ds ⱖ 0.34, tions with Blacks.
ps ⱕ .04), and there was no significant difference in the valence of All of the implicit measures tended to not be correlated with
White associations held by Whites and Blacks, t(111) ⫽ 0.18, p ⫽ explicit measures of racial bias (see Table 6). For White partici-
.86, d ⫽ 0.03. But for the Black SC-IAT component, Black pants in this sample, both the IAT and a race SC-IAT failed to
participants had significantly more positive associations with correlate with explicit measures of racial attitudes (all ps ⬎ .22),
Blacks than did White participants, t(111) ⫽ 2.80, p ⬍ .01, d ⫽ whereas for Black participants, both the IAT and a race SC-IAT
26 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

Table 6 Whites. However, neither the IAT nor the SC-IATs consistently
Study 3: Correlations Between Implicit and Explicit Measures correlated with the explicit attitudes measures.
One area of concern regarding the SC-IAT is the low reliability
SC-IAT measures coefficients of the SC-IAT measures, particularly the Black SC-
Race Race White Black
IAT. Consistent with the findings of Study 1, the SC-IATs dis-
Explicit attitude measure IAT SC-IAT SC-IAT SC-IAT played lower levels of internal consistency than did the IAT.
Although the White SC-IAT displayed adequate levels of reliabil-
White participants (n ⫽ 79) ity, the reliability of the Black SC-IAT was poor. In this study, the
number of critical trials in the SC-IAT procedure was reduced
Explicit race attitudes ⫺.05 .14 .17 ⫺.08
Explicit attitudes toward Whites .08 .17 .16 ⫺.08 from 72 to 48. It is possible, and indeed likely, that this modifi-
Explicit attitudes toward Blacks .14 .01 .17 ⫺.02 cation adversely affected the reliability of the SC-IAT measure. A
second modification to the SC-IAT procedure was an increase in
Black participants (n ⫽ 34) the length of the response window from 1,500 ms to 2,000 ms.
Explicit race attitudes .29† .29† .48* .10 This change was intended to reduce the error rate, but this change
Explicit attitudes toward Whites .07 .01 .25 .23 was ineffective. The reliability and validity of the SC-IAT appear
Explicit attitudes toward Blacks ⫺.23 ⫺.23 ⫺.15 .16 to be greater when the SC-IAT procedure includes at least 72
critical trials and a 1,500-ms response window, as was used in
Note. SC-IAT ⫽ Single Category Implicit Association Test; IAT ⫽
Implicit Association Test.
Study 2.
† p ⬍ .10. * p ⬍ .01. A more troubling possibility is that the low reliability of the
SC-IAT was due to participants attempting to respond to the task
in a socially desirable manner. This concern arises from the finding
that the Black SC-IAT had poor reliability and from the reduced
correlated marginally with explicit measures of racial attitudes (all effect size on the race SC-IAT in White participants, compared
ps ⱕ .10). Additionally, little evidence was found for correlations with the IAT. The SC-IAT was designed to be relatively imper-
between the White and Black SC-IATs and their respective ex- vious to controlled processing in general and especially to social
plicit attitude measures, either for White or Black participants (all desirability concerns. The moderate-sized correlation between the
ps ⬎ .15). IAT and a race SC-IAT suggests that social desirability pressures
might not have affected SC-IAT responses in a sizable way.
Discussion Nevertheless, the possibility that the SC-IAT may not be as im-
plicit as previously thought is a disturbing finding, and Study 4
Study 3 provided evidence for the validity of the SC-IAT on was designed to investigate this issue more thoroughly.
multiple levels. First, similar mean-level effects were observed on
the IAT and the race SC-IAT. On both these measures, White Study 4
participants displayed a pro-White bias, and Black participants
displayed no significant evaluative race bias. For White partici- One of the advantages of implicit measures of social cognition
pants, the race bias observed by using the IAT was substantially is that they are impervious to self-presentational motivations and
larger than the race bias observed by using the SC-IAT. However, social desirability concerns. Thus, for the SC-IAT to be useful as
because no objective measure of implicit race bias exists, it is an implicit measure of social cognition, it must be shown that
unclear whether this difference in effect sizes between the mea- participants cannot control or fake their responses on the task.
sures is meaningful. There are reasons to suspect that it may be easier for participants
Second, the White SC-IAT and Black SC-IAT facilitated the to control or fake their responses on the SC-IAT than on the IAT.
interpretation of IAT and race SC-IAT scores. For White partici- Undoubtedly, it is easier for participants to reconceptualize the
pants, the SC-IAT race bias emerged because of positive evalua- instructions in order to disrupt measurement of the associations of
tive White associations and neutral evaluative Black associations; interest on the SC-IAT than on the IAT. For example, participants
for Black participants, the lack of a SC-IAT race bias emerged may recode the SC-IAT instructions as follows: “If it is a ‘bad’
because of positive evaluative Black associations and equally target word, press the right key; for all other target words, press the
positive evaluative White associations. This pattern of results left key.” Similarly, if the target category is represented by pictures
suggests that the SC-IAT and IAT race biases observed in White and the evaluative dimension is represented by words (as in
participants were, on average, a result of in-group favoritism and Studies 1 and 3), participants may use an “all pictures to the left”
not out-group prejudice. At the very least, the current results show rule and avoid encoding the content of the target pictures. Al-
that IAT and SC-IAT race biases can be due to in-group favoritism though participants could use these strategies to control or fake
and may occur in the absence of out-group negativity. their responses on the SC-IAT, it is not clear that they do use these
Third, the race SC-IAT correlated significantly with the race strategies spontaneously.
IAT. Thus, there is some evidence that the race SC-IAT and the Study 4 was conducted to investigate participants’ ability to
race IAT are measuring similar aspects of Black–White evaluative control or fake their responses on a SC-IAT measuring evaluative
associations. These results suggest that a Black–White IAT, a associations with women. Participants were randomly assigned to
White SC-IAT, and a Black SC-IAT may all provide useful infor- display positive or negative attitudes toward women and then
mation regarding evaluative associations regarding Blacks and completed a SC-IAT measuring evaluative associations with
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 27

women. Because the modifications of the SC-IAT procedure used Results


in Study 3 (reducing the number of critical trials in each stage to
48 and increasing the response window to 2,000 ms) were inef- IAT and SC-IAT Data Reduction and Reliability Analysis
fective, we used a SC-IAT with 72 critical trials in each stage and
As in the previous studies, IAT scores were computed by using
with a 1,500-ms response window (as was used in Study 2). For
the D algorithm, such that higher numbers indicate a profemale
comparison purposes, participants also completed a male–female
(and/or antimale) bias. For the IAT, a reliability coefficient was
IAT and explicit attitude measures toward women.
computed by correlating an IAT score computed from the practice
We expected that participants would easily be able to present the
trials with an IAT score computed from the test trials (adjusted r ⫽
desired response on the explicit attitude measures. A number of
.78). SC-IAT scores were computed by using the modified D
researchers have investigated the susceptibility of the IAT to
algorithm, such that higher numbers indicate more positive than
faking. Steffens (2004) reviewed the literature and concluded that
negative associations with women. To determine the reliability of
small, nonsignificant effects of faking on the IAT were typically
the SC-IAT, a SC-IAT score was calculated for each of the three
observed. Thus, we anticipated a small effect of instruction on the
test blocks of 24 test trials (adjusted r ⫽ .85). Similar to Study 2,
male–female IAT scores. The primary question of interest was the
the SC-IAT displayed slightly higher levels of internal consistency
effect of instruction on the female SC-IAT scores and how this
than the IAT.
effect compared with the effect observed for the IAT and explicit
attitude measures.
Effect of the Self-Presentation Instructions
Method As expected, participants instructed to present positive attitudes
toward women displayed significantly more positive explicit atti-
Participants tudes toward women than did participants instructed to present
negative attitudes toward women (present positive, M ⫽ 0.71;
Eighty-four students (20 men, 64 women) enrolled in an introductory
psychology course at Temple University participated in this study. All present negative, M ⫽ ⫺0.78), t(82) ⫽ 11.84, p ⬍ .01, d ⫽ 2.61.
participants received course credit for their participation. For the IAT, we first examined whether the error rate differed
across conditions. A t test of the IAT error rate revealed no
significant difference in the IAT error rate as a function of pre-
Procedure sentation instructions, t(82) ⫽ 0.40, p ⫽ .69, d ⫽ 0.09. Following
Participants were randomly assigned to respond to all questions and
the procedure from the previous studies, 1 participant with an error
tasks as if they had very positive attitudes or very negative attitudes toward rate larger than 20% was excluded from the remaining IAT anal-
women. Participants then completed a SC-IAT measure of evaluative yses, and the resulting error rate was 5.30%. Somewhat surpris-
associations with women, a male–female IAT, and self-report measures of ingly, a medium instruction effect was observed for the IAT.
attitudes toward women. Participants instructed to present positive attitudes toward women
SC-IAT measure of female associations. The female SC-IAT was displayed significantly higher IAT scores than did participants
identical to the self-SC-IAT procedure used in Study 2, with appropriate instructed to present negative attitudes toward women (present
modifications to measure associations with women. The evaluative dimen- positive, M ⫽ 0.60; present negative, M ⫽ 0.37), t(81) ⫽ 2.03, p ⫽
sion was labeled good and bad, and the concept dimension was labeled .05, d ⫽ 0.45.2
female. Five target words were used for the category female (her, woman,
For the SC-IAT, we also examined whether the error rate
girl, she, and lady).
IAT measure of male–female associations. The IAT procedure was
differed across conditions. A t test of the SC-IAT error rate
identical to the procedure used in Study 3 (i.e., unlike Studies 1 and 2, the revealed a significant medium-sized difference in the SC-IAT
procedure included response feedback), with appropriate modifications to error rate as a function of presentation instructions, t(82) ⫽ 2.15,
measure associations with men and women. The evaluative dimension was p ⫽ .04, d ⫽ 0.48. Participants who attempted to present negative
labeled pleasant and unpleasant, and the attitude object dimension was attitudes toward women had a significantly higher error rate on the
labeled female and male. Five target words were used for each of the task (M ⫽ 18.04%) than did participants who attempted to present
attitude object dimensions (male: him, man, boy, he, and guy; female: her, positive attitudes toward women (M ⫽ 12.94%). An analysis of the
woman, girl, she, and lady). In Blocks 3 and 4, participants paired fe- SC-IAT scores of all participants revealed a large instruction effect
male ⫹ pleasant and male ⫹ unpleasant. This pairing was reversed in (present positive, M ⫽ 0.68; present negative, M ⫽ 0.28), t(82) ⫽
Blocks 6 and 7: male ⫹ pleasant and female ⫹ unpleasant.
3.31, p ⬍ .01, d ⫽ 0.73. However, the standard practice with IAT
Explicit measures of attitudes toward females. Participants next com-
pleted three explicit measures of attitudes toward women: a female seman-
and SC-IAT data is to exclude participants with high error rates
tic differential (␣ ⫽ .98), a female feeling thermometer, and the Modern from the analyses. Once the 25 participants with a SC-IAT error
Sexism Scale (Swim, Aikin, Hall, & Hunter, 1995). The semantic differ- rate larger than 20% were removed from the data (10 from the
ential and feeling thermometer measures were identical to those used in the positive condition and 15 from the negative condition), the overall
previous studies, but the target category was changed to be females. SC-IAT error rate no longer differed by condition (M ⫽ 9.42%),
Participants responded to the items of the Modern Sexism Scale on a scale
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree; ␣ ⫽ .88). Modern sexism
2
scores were recoded so that higher numbers indicated less sexism. The This presentation effect on IAT scores was observed only when the
three attitude measures were highly interrelated (␣ ⫽ .92) and thus were D-score algorithm was used to compute IAT scores; when the original
averaged to compute a composite measure of self-reported attitudes toward log-based IAT scoring algorithm was used, no presentation effect emerged,
women. t(81) ⫽ 0.10, p ⫽ .92, d ⫽ 0.02.
28 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

t(57) ⫽ 0.47, p ⫽ .64, d ⫽ 0.12. Furthermore, a t test revealed that


SC-IAT scores did not differ significantly as a function of condi-
tion, although a small presentation effect in the anticipated direc-
tion was observed (present positive, M ⫽ 0.60; present negative,
M ⫽ 0.50), t(57) ⫽ 0.92, p ⫽ .36, d ⫽ 0.24. These analyses
suggest that participants can fake a SC-IAT score, but to do so they
also increase their error rate.
An alternative interpretation of these results is that the fakers
(those who had large SC-IAT error rates) are different from
nonfakers, confounding the interpretation of the results. Although
we cannot entirely rule out this possibility, we think it is unlikely.
First, the fakers were not significantly different from the nonfakers
on any demographic variable we had collected (gender, ethnicity,
and native language; all ps ⬎ .25). Second, it is possible that the
nonfakers were insensitive to the presentation manipulation and/or
that the fakers tried harder than the nonfakers. However, the
Figure 2. Single Category Implicit Association Test error rates by target,
nonfakers did show the predicted self-presentation effect on the
stage, and instruction condition (N ⫽ 84).
explicit attitude measures, t(57) ⫽ 7.73, p ⬍ .01, d ⫽ 2.05, and the
size of this self-presentation effect did not differ between fakers
and nonfakers, t(80) ⫽ 1.45, p ⫽ .15, d ⫽ 0.32. Furthermore, if the
nonfakers did not take the SC-IAT task seriously, then we would pairing female ⫹ good (d ⫽ 0.92), whereas participants who were
expect their responses to be mostly error variance. On the contrary, instructed to present negative attitudes did not significantly differ
these participants displayed a strong bias in favor of women, in their error rates across the blocks (d ⫽ 0.05). On average,
t(58) ⫽ 12.60, p ⬍ .01, d ⫽ 1.64, a result that is consistent with participants in both instruction conditions showed evidence of
other female SC-IAT findings (Karpinski & Lytle, 2005). Finally, more positive than negative associations with women, suggesting
the fact that the nonfakers had low error rates also suggests that that in the absence of faking, most participants have more positive
they approached the task with sincerity. than negative associations with women (see also Karpinski &
Lytle, 2005). Thus, participants instructed to present positive atti-
A Closer Analysis of SC-IAT Error Rates tudes may not have had to adjust their responses for the female ⫹
good pairings as a result of the instructions— only when they
The high error rates observed in this study appear to be specif- reached Block 2 (pairing female ⫹ bad) did they have to start
ically associated with the instruction manipulation. The SC-IAT monitoring their responses, resulting in higher error rates. Con-
error rate was higher in Study 4 than in Studies 1–3, and other versely, participants instructed to present negative attitudes had to
studies have found normal error rates on the female SC-IAT when adjust their responses for the female ⫹ good pairings in response
no presentation instructions were provided (see Karpinski & Lytle, to the instructions. As a consequence, they may have adjusted their
2005). responses from the start of the task, resulting in high error rates in
We conducted several follow-up analyses of the SC-IAT error both blocks. Therefore, this Block ⫻ Condition interaction is
rates to better understand why the instruction manipulation re- consistent with the claim that participants make more errors when
sulted in such high error rates on the SC-IAT. First, we investi- they are attempting to fake a SC-IAT score. Consistent with this
gated whether the increased error rate was due to an increase in interpretation, when participants with high error rates were ex-
true errors (pressing the wrong response key) or due to an increase cluded from the analysis, the Block ⫻ Condition interaction
in the failure to respond within the 1,500-ms response window. An dropped to nonsignificance, F(1, 114) ⫽ 1.52, p ⫽ .22.
inspection of the error rates revealed that the overall error rate The Target Word ⫻ Block interaction indicates that partici-
(15.37%) was due to a high rate of incorrect responses (13.69%) pants’ error rates for the types of target words differed across
and a low rate of failure to respond within the response window blocks. For Block 1 (female ⫹ good), participants had a lower
(1.68%). error rate for female target words than for good target words (d ⫽
Next, we computed overall error rates separately for each class 0.44) or for bad target words (d ⫽ 0.61; and error rates for good
of target word (female vs. good vs. bad) within each block of the and bad target words did not significantly differ, d ⫽ .17). For
SC-IAT (Block 1: female ⫹ good vs. Block 2: female ⫹ bad). A Block 2 (female ⫹ bad), participants had higher error rates for
Target Word ⫻ Block ⫻ Instruction Condition mixed ANOVA on good target words than for female or bad target words (ds ⱖ 0.43),
these error rates revealed main effects for each of the factors, but and error rates for female and bad words did not significantly
these main effects were qualified by two significant higher order differ (d ⫽ 0.01). The interpretation of this interaction is unclear,
effects: a Block ⫻ Condition interaction, F(1, 164) ⫽ 12.00, p ⬍ but it does not appear to be a result of the presentation instructions.
.01, and a Target Word ⫻ Block interaction, F(2, 164) ⫽ 6.70, p ⬍ Supporting this conclusion, the Target Word ⫻ Block interaction
.01 (see Figure 2). remained significant once individuals with high error rates were
The Block ⫻ Condition interaction indicates that participants excluded from the analysis, F(2, 114) ⫽ 7.87, p ⬍ .01.
who were instructed to present positive attitudes toward women Finally, we examined whether the D-score algorithm might
had higher error rates when pairing female ⫹ bad than when exaggerate the effect of a high error rate on SC-IAT scores. The
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 29

D-score algorithm replaces errors with the block mean plus a have found a presentation effect on the SC-IAT. Participants may
400-ms penalty. To determine whether this error penalty affected be more likely to spontaneously discover these alternative strate-
the results, we computed an alternative SC-IAT score by using the gies if they complete multiple SC-IATs (although in another study,
D-score algorithm, but we eliminated all error responses from the participants had to complete as many as four SC-IATs in a study
computation. An analysis on these adjusted SC-IAT scores paral- session and showed no evidence of faking or presentation effects;
leled the previous analysis. When all participants were analyzed, see Karpinski & Lytle, 2005).
an instruction effect was observed, t(82) ⫽ 2.53, p ⫽ .01, d ⫽
0.55, and this effect disappeared when participants with high error
rates were excluded, t(57) ⫽ 0.54, p ⫽ .59, d ⫽ 0.14. Thus, the General Discussion
observed effects are not dependent on the inclusion or exclusion of
incorrect response times in the scoring algorithm. We developed the SC-IAT as a single-category measure of
social cognition to complement the IAT. Whereas the IAT mea-
sures comparative associations between two attitude objects, the
Discussion SC-IAT can assess the evaluative associations with a single atti-
tude object. Across three different attitude domains—soda brand
The results of Study 4 suggest that participants can fake a
preferences, self-esteem, and racial attitudes—we found evidence
SC-IAT score, but to do so they also significantly increase their
that the SC-IAT makes unique contributions in the ability to
error rate. Once participants with high error rates were excluded
measure and understand implicit social cognition. In the soda
from the analysis, only a small, nonsignificant presentation effect
brand domain, a Coke SC-IAT and a Pepsi SC-IAT predicted
remained. The bad news about these findings is that when partic-
behavioral intentions above and beyond what was predicted by the
ipants wish to fake a SC-IAT score, a significant number of them
spontaneously develop strategies that enable them to present the IAT and explicit measures of soda brand attitudes. As a measure of
desired attitude. Participants in this study were not provided with self-esteem, the SC-IAT demonstrated a significant medium- to
strategies to fake the SC-IAT; they discovered strategies on their large-sized correlation with explicit measures of self-esteem. In
own. The good news about this finding is that when participants addition, the self-SC-IAT and the self– other IAT score were only
attempt to fake a SC-IAT score or to present a certain attitude on marginally correlated, supporting the claim that the SC-IAT’s
a SC-IAT, they are likely to make many errors, and they can be measure of self-associations is theoretically distinct from the
identified and excluded from subsequent analyses. IAT’s measure of self– other associations. As a measure of racial
Somewhat surprisingly, participants were also able to fake IAT attitudes, similar mean-level effects were observed on the race IAT
scores in this study. Participants who were able to present a and the race SC-IAT. However, the use of a White SC-IAT and a
particular attitude on the IAT did so without significantly increas- Black SC-IAT allowed for a more detailed interpretation of the
ing their error rate, making it impossible to identify them and implicit race bias.
remove them from the sample. We are somewhat skeptical of this We also extensively examined the reliability of the SC-IAT. In
result, as no faking effect was found when IAT scores were general, the reliability of implicit measures of social cognition has
computed by using the older log-based scoring algorithm. Never- been relatively poor, with the exception of the IAT (Bosson et al.,
theless, the concerns about the faking of SC-IAT scores appear to 2000; Olson & Fazio, 2003). Across the four studies and six
be no worse than concerns about faking of IAT scores. different SC-IAT measures, the internal consistency of the SC-IAT
The results of Study 4 suggest that several cautions regarding was reasonable (average r ⫽ .69; ranging from r ⫽ .55 to r ⫽ .85).
the use of the SC-IAT are in order. First, the ability of participants These reliability coefficients are similar to the internal consistency
to fake a SC-IAT score may decrease power (due to the discarding observed for the IATs used in these studies (average r ⫽ .73;
of those with high error rates). Second, it may be the case that ranging from r ⫽ .58 to r ⫽ .82) and similar to the internal
participants who are able to fake a SC-IAT score are different from consistency observed for the IAT in previous research (Greenwald
those who were unable to fake a SC-IAT, resulting in a biased et al., 2003; Nosek et al., 2005). Thus, the SC-IAT appears to have
sample, although there is no evidence for this claim in the current
a sufficient level of reliability to be of use as an individual
study. Third, as previously highlighted, it may be easier for par-
difference measure of implicit social cognition.
ticipants to fake a SC-IAT score if category targets are pictures and
For the SC-IAT to be useful as a measure of implicit social
evaluative targets are words. For potentially sensitive domains, it
cognition, the SC-IAT must be relatively unsusceptible to faking
may be prudent to present category targets and evaluative targets
or self-presentational concerns. We found that when participants
by using the same form of presentation (but there was no evidence
of high error rates or faking of SC-IAT scores in Study 3 when attempted to self-present an attitude on the SC-IAT, many of them
Black and White faces were used as the target stimuli). had high error rates. Once participants with large error rates were
Finally, although we did not find presentation effects on the removed from the sample, there was only a small, nonsignificant
SC-IAT once participants with high error rates were removed, it effect of self-presentation (d ⫽ .24). This result is consistent with
does not mean that participants cannot devise other strategies to previous research on the IAT, suggesting that there may be a small
fake a SC-IAT. Although participants did not spontaneously devise effect of faking or self-presentation on the IAT (Steffens, 2004).
effective strategies to fake a SC-IAT score without increasing their Together, these findings provide strong evidence for the reliability
error rate, if we had provided participants with a strategy, it is and validity of the SC-IAT as a measure of implicit social
likely that they could have carried out the strategy and we would cognition.
30 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

Interpreting the SC-IAT as a Measure of Evaluative on the reliability of single category or target measures, we recom-
Associations mend following the SC-IAT procedure to keep the reliability of the
measure high.
There has been some controversy surrounding the interpretation Second, we recommend that the SC-IAT be used with at least 24
of IAT scores (for overviews, see Arkes & Tetlock, 2004; Fazio & practice trials and 72 critical trials in each critical block. Unlike the
Olson, 2003). Because the methodology of the SC-IAT is a mod- IAT, the SC-IAT does not have separate practice stages; within
ification of the IAT, it is possible that the various proposed each block, it is necessary to include practice trials and to exclude
limitations and interpretations of the IAT also apply to the SC- those trials from the final calculation of SC-IAT scores. SC-IATs
IAT. A common theme of these alternative interpretations is that with only 48 trials have been found to have lower internal consis-
IAT effects may be due, at least in part, to factors other than tencies than SC-IATs with 72 trials. Likewise, the internal consis-
affective valence. For example, IAT effects may be observed tency of the ST-IAT, with only 20 trials per block, has also been
because of the cost of task switching (Mierke & Klauer, 2001), a found to be lower than the internal consistency of the SC-IAT
criterion shift across the different blocks of the task (Brendl, (Wigboldus et al., 2005). Thus, we suggest that 72 trials be the
Markman, & Messner, 2001), and the salience of the attitude lower bound for the number of trials that should be used for each
objects (Rothermund & Wentura, 2004). Additionally, the IAT has stage in a SC-IAT.
been described as a measure of environmental associations, or Third, we recommend that a 1,500-ms response window be
extrapersonal associations, rather than as a measure of one’s per- included in the SC-IAT procedure. The response window may
sonal attitudes (Arkes & Tetlock, 2004; Karpinski & Hilton, 2001; decrease the likelihood that participants engage in controlled pro-
Olson & Fazio, 2004). Some of these alternative interpretations are cessing during the task. Longer response windows do not appear to
likely to apply to the SC-IAT as well. For example, the SC-IAT increase the reliability or to decrease the error rate, but it may be
may reveal more about one’s environmental associations than possible to decrease the response window without adversely af-
one’s personal evaluative beliefs. fecting the error rate or the reliability of the measure.
Additionally, we have been careful to describe the SC-IAT as a Fourth, we recommend using the D-score algorithm to compute
measure of associations with a single attitude object and not as an SC-IAT scores. For the IAT, the D-score algorithm has been
absolute measure of attitudes toward an attitude object. It is likely shown to increase reliability, increase the correlations between the
that no attitude can be measured in absolute terms and that all IAT and explicit measures, reduce the correlation between the IAT
attitudes require some type of comparative judgment (Festinger, measure and speed of responding, and reduce the effect of proce-
1950). For example, to determine that one likes Pepsi, a person dural variables (Greenwald et al., 2003). Although not presented
must know how much he or she likes other beverages or how much here, we also examined SC-IAT scores by using a log-based
other people like Pepsi. Although a contrast category is not spec- scoring algorithm, modeled on the original IAT scoring algorithm
ified in the SC-IAT and the SC-IAT is less comparative than the (Greenwald et al., 1998). We observed a very small trend for larger
IAT (which explicitly requires a comparison category), the SC- effect sizes and larger SC-IAT-explicit correlations when the
IAT also may not be an absolute measure of associations, in the D-score algorithm is used compared with the log-based algorithm.
purest sense. Furthermore, although the SC-IAT avoids using a We did not have the large samples required to detect small differ-
contrast category for the target category of interest, it still mea- ences between the scoring algorithms by using tests of statistical
sures the evaluation dimension comparatively (see Blanton et al., significance, but because of the similar methodology shared by the
2006). SC-IAT and IAT, it is likely that many of the advantages of the
The presence of these possible alternative explanations for SC- D-score algorithm will carry over to the SC-IAT.
IAT effects and methodological issues regarding the SC-IAT do Fifth, when the SC-IAT and the IAT are used in the same
not suggest that the SC-IAT should not be used as a measure of experimental session, we recommend that the SC-IAT measures
associations, only that SC-IAT scores should be interpreted cau- precede any IAT measure. The IAT encourages a complementary,
tiously and with these potential limitations in mind. dichotomous mind-set toward the categories of interest. When an
IAT is completed prior to a SC-IAT, the complementary mind-set
Recommendations for Using the SC-IAT may carry over to the SC-IAT, possibly resulting in a task that is not
a measure of the associations with a single target category and in
On the basis of the results of the studies reported here and other lower reliability for the task. However, it is also possible that com-
studies conducted in our lab, we have several recommendations for pleting the SC-IAT may adversely affect the IAT, and future studies
researchers interested in using the SC-IAT as a measure of implicit are needed to examine the interplay between these two measures.
social cognition. First, as previously mentioned, the SC-IAT is Finally, we have presented the SC-IAT as a measure of the
conceptually identical to the ST-IAT and has only minor proce- evaluative associations with a single target category. However,
dural differences from the ST-IAT (Wigboldus et al., 2005). Com- like the IAT, the SC-IAT is a flexible measure that can be modified
pared with the SC-IAT, the ST-IAT includes an initial practice to assess other aspects of implicit social cognition. For example,
stage with only good and bad target words, has fewer target words the IAT has been used as a measure of gender identity by assessing
in each stage, and does not use a response window. Furthermore, the strength of associations between the self and the male–female
the internal consistency of the SC-IAT tends to be higher than the dimension, relative to the strength of associations between others
internal consistency for the ST-IAT (Christian ST-IAT, adjusted and the male–female dimension (Aidman & Carroll, 2003; Green-
r ⫽ .39; Islamic ST-IAT, adjusted r ⫽ .68). Until studies are wald & Farnham, 2000; Rudman & Goodwin, 2004). The SC-IAT
conducted to investigate the effects of these procedural differences can be easily modified to assess the strength of associations
SINGLE CATEGORY IMPLICIT ASSOCIATION TEST 31

between the self and the male–female dimension, without also Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition
assessing other associations. In this case, the gender dimension research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
(male–female) would still be assessed as a comparative dimension. 297–327.
The SC-IAT can be used to eliminate one comparative dimension Festinger, L. (1950). A theory of social comparison. Human Relations, 7,
117–140.
but not both of the comparative dimensions. Likewise, the SC-IAT
Gehring, W. J., Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2003). Cognition during
can be modified to provide measures of implicit stereotypes and
interracial interactions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1241–1243.
other aspects of implicit social cognition (see Karpinski & Lytle, Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New Look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed.
2005). Additionally, for investigators who wish to reduce the American Psychologist, 47, 766 –779.
contaminating effects of environmental, or extrapersonal, associ- Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition:
ations (see Olson & Fazio, 2004), a personalized SC-IAT can be Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102,
constructed by replacing the category labels good and bad with I 4 –27.
like and I don’t like. Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Associa-
tion Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality
Conclusion and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring
The IAT has been a breakthrough in the ability to measure and individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association
understand implicit processes. The SC-IAT is a measure that can Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464 –1480.
be used alone or in combination with the IAT to further illuminate Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Associ-
these implicit processes. By continually developing and refining ation Test at age 3. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48,
individual difference measures of implicit social cognition, social 85–93.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding
psychologists can improve their understanding of implicit and
and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algo-
explicit social cognition.
rithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.
Karpinski, A. (2004). Measuring self-esteem using the Implicit Association
References Test: The role of the other. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
30, 22–34.
Aidman, E. V., & Carroll, S. M. (2003). Implicit individual differences:
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Associa-
Relationships between implicit self-esteem, gender identity, and gender
tion Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774 –788.
attitudes. European Journal of Personality, 17, 19 –36.
Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or Karpinski, A., & Lytle, J. (2005). Measuring implicit gender attitudes,
“Would Jesse Jackson ‘Fail’ the Implicit Association Test?” Psycholog- gender identity, and self-esteem using the Single Category Implicit
ical Inquiry, 15, 257–278. Association Test. Unpublished manuscript.
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the Kihlstrom, J. (1990). The psychological unconscious. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.),
automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 445– 464). New
925–945. York: Guilford Press.
Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology. Amer- Maison, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Bruin, R. H. (2004). Predictive validity
ican Psychologist, 61, 27– 41. of the Implicit Association Test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes,
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzales, P. M., & Christie, C. (2006). Decoding and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 405– 415.
the Implicit Association Test: Perspectives on criterion prediction. Jour- Mierke, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2001). Implicit association measurement with
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 192–212. the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift
Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 107–122.
perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant Monteith, M. J., Voils, C. I., & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2001). Taking a look
revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631– 643. underground: Detecting, interpreting, and reacting to implicit racial
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect biases. Social Cognition, 19, 395– 417.
measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task.
the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Social Cognition, 19, 625– 664.
chology, 81, 760 –773. Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting
Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration Web site.
Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiarity Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 101–115.
explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 316 –328. Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding
De Houwer, J. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and con-
dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limita- struct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166 –
tions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 180.
115–133. Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
De Houwer, J. (2003). The Extrinsic Affective Simon Task. Experimental Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2003). Relations between implicit measures
Psychology, 50, 77– 85. of prejudice: What are we measuring? Psychological Science, 14, 636 –
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. 639.
(1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extraper-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510 –540. sonal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653– 657.
Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social NJ: Princeton University Press.
Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the
32 KARPINSKI AND STEINMAN

Implicit Association Test (IAT): Dissociating salience from associa- Affective Simon Task (EAST). European Journal of Personality, 18,
tions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 139 –165. 495–520.
Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic Wentura, D., Kulfanek, M., & Greve, W. (2005). Masked affective
in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? priming by name letters: Evidence for a correspondence of explicit
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 494 –509. and implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking? 41, 657– 663.
Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179. Wigboldus, D. H. J., Holland, R. W., & van Knippenberg, A. (2005).
Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and Single target implicit associations. Unpublished manuscript.
racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial
and Social Psychology, 68, 199 –214. prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire
Teige, S., Schnable, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2004). Assess- measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262–
ment of multiple implicit self-concept dimensions using the Extrinsic 274.

Appendix

Target Words Used in the SC-IAT and IAT


IAT target words SC-IAT target words

Pleasant Unpleasant Good Bad

brilliant awkward beautiful angry


diamond deatha celebrating brutal
joy hateb cheerful destroy
truth failureb excellent dirty
sunrise filtha excitement disaster
slumb fabulous disgusting
stink friendly dislike
uglya glad evil
glee gross
happy horrible
laughing humiliate
likable nasty
loving noxious
marvelous painful
pleasure revolting
smiling sickening
splendid terrible
superb tragic
paradise ugly
triumph unpleasant
wonderful yucky

Note. SC-IAT ⫽ Single Category Implicit Association Test; IAT ⫽


Implicit Association Test.
a
Unpleasant words used in IAT for Study 3 only. b Unpleasant words
used in IAT for Studies 1, 2, and 4 only.

Received May 20, 2005


Revision received August 18, 2005
Accepted August 19, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 171–187 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.171

Investigating the Dopaminergic Basis of Extraversion in Humans:


A Multilevel Approach
Jan Wacker, Mira-Lynn Chavanon, and Gerhard Stemmler
Philipps-Universität Marburg

A recent theory suggests that the agency facet of Extraversion (E) is based on brain dopamine (DA). The
paucity of human data relevant to this model is probably due to the lack of widely accessible noninvasive
psychophysiological indices and well-established behavioral measures sensitive to both E and manipu-
lations of DA activity. Aiming to identify such measures, the authors assessed the electroencephalogram
and n-back task performance in groups of introverts and extraverts after administration of either placebo
or a selective DA D2 receptor antagonist. As predicted, the antagonist’s effects on n-back reaction time
measures and frontal versus parietal electroencephalogram theta activity were strongly and specifically
modulated by E. New research avenues and theoretical extensions suggested by these results are
discussed.

Keywords: agentic Extraversion, dopamine D2 receptor antagonist, n-back task, electroencephalogram


theta activity, frontal electroencephalogram asymmetry

Thirty-five years ago, Jeffrey A. Gray (1970) proposed that Luciana, Arbisi, Collins, & Leon, 1994) or extremely expensive
Extraversion (E) and other personality traits reflect individual neuroimaging techniques (e.g., Farde, Gustavsson, & Jonsson,
differences in motivational systems that evolved to deal with 1997). These methodological requirements considerably impede
certain classes of stimuli associated with positive and negative empirical evaluation of the model because the necessary laboratory
reinforcement. Depue and Collins (1999) have integrated this equipment is typically either not available to personality psychol-
general idea with the extensive database generated by behavioral ogists or too expensive to be used with the large sample sizes
neuroscience during the last decades into a novel psychobiological necessary for the investigation of individual differences. There-
theory of E. In brief, their model holds that E is based on a broad fore, it is not surprising that the paucity of relevant data noted by
motivational system that “facilitates” behavior motivated by pos- Depue and Collins (1999) 7 years ago is still largely uncompen-
itive incentives and that is neurobiologically based on the meso- sated. Noninvasive and more accessible psychophysiological indi-
corticolimbic dopamine (DA) system. ces of central DA functioning are needed for progress in this
Although Depue and Collins (1999) firmly rooted their theory of exciting field of research. Furthermore, even if the hypothesized
E in basic neuroscience insights gleaned from animal research, link between E and brain DA had already been firmly established
they have thus far not been particularly interested in using their empirically, the important question of how individual differences
model to derive new hypotheses and viable empirical approaches in DA functioning are translated into extraverted versus intro-
for personality research with humans. As also noted by others, “the verted patterns of behavior would remain largely unanswered. To
model in its present form specifies, at the human level, little more begin to address this problem at the human level, behavioral
than an expected pattern of correlations between trait measures and paradigms that are sensitive to both manipulations of functional
indices of dopaminergic neurotransmission” (commentary by brain DA activity and individual differences in E need to be
Pickering in Depue & Collins, 1999, p. 534). However, currently, established.
such indices of dopaminergic neurotransmission either require The aims of the present study were: (a) to introduce an easily
invasive measurements of blood hormone levels (e.g., Depue, obtainable electroencephalogram (EEG) index that can be used to
measure those individual differences in functional brain DA ac-
tivity that are relevant to E and (b) to identify a behavioral
paradigm that is sensitive to both manipulations of functional brain
Jan Wacker, Mira-Lynn Chavanon, and Gerhard Stemmler, Faculty of DA activity and individual differences in E. These psychophysio-
Psychology, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Germany. logical and behavioral measures could then be used as a basis to
This article is based on the doctoral dissertation of Jan Wacker under the further extend the theory proposed by Depue and Collins (1999) to
supervision of Gerhard Stemmler. The reported research was conducted
the human level.
with the help of Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant Ste 405/9-1. We
thank Thomas Scherer and Erwin Hennighausen for hardware and software
support and Christof Kemper for his assistance in conducting the E and DA
experiment.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jan Although in several influential psychobiological theories (e.g.,
Wacker, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Gutenberg- Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993; Pickering & Gray, 1999;
strasse 18, D-35032, Marburg, Germany. E-mail: wackerj@staff.uni- Zuckerman, 1991) the dopaminergic system is regarded as (part of)
marburg.de the basis of one of several highly correlated traits from the

171
172 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

impulsivity-sensation seeking spectrum (novelty seeking, impul- impulsivity sensation seeking with respect to DA indicators remain
sivity, impulsive antisocial sensation seeking, or impulsive sensa- to be conducted.
tion seeking), based on psychometric analyses and theoretical The development of dopaminergic tracers that can be used in
considerations Depue and Collins (1999) argue that impulsivity- neuroimaging studies to more directly measure certain aspects of
sensation seeking is rather heterogeneous in terms of both the central DA activity recently has opened up new possibilities to
underlying motivations and brain systems and that therefore con- investigate the hypothesized association between personality traits
sistent and replicable associations with markers of DA neurotrans- and DA. However, the few studies that have already employed this
mission are not to be expected. Depue and Collins (1999) further novel technique do not yet provide a conclusive picture (Breier et
note that E can be subdivided into sociability on the one hand and al., 1998; Farde et al., 1997; Gray, Pickering, & Gray, 1994;
agency on the other. It is the agency facet of E (i.e., a motivational Kestler, Malhotra, Finch, Adler, & Breier, 2000; Laakso et al.,
disposition that comprises social dominance, enthusiasm, energy, 2000, 2003).
assertiveness, ambitiousness, and achievement striving), which In summarizing the evidence, it can still be concluded with
they regard as the central characteristic of E and for which they Pickering and Gray (2001, p. 124) that “it seems premature to
propose a dopaminergic basis in their theory. conclude that measures of E are more strongly and/or more
Depue and Collins (1999) suggest that agentic E is based on a consistently associated with DA functioning than measures of
broad motivational system, which they term the behavioral facil- ImpASS [impulsive antisocial sensation seeking].” We would add
itation system (BFS). The BFS modulates or “facilitates” behavior to this conclusion that fast and significant progress on this question
motivated by positive incentives through increasing their motiva- is unlikely as long as the established approaches to measurement
tional salience. Agentic extraverts possess a more reactive BFS of central DA activity in humans [i.e., invasive measurement of
and therefore are generally more enduring, motivated, and vigor- blood hormone levels and positron emission tomography (PET)
ous in their pursuits of positive incentive goals. In the neurobio- scans with dopaminergic tracers that cost several thousand dollars
logical part of their theory, Depue and Collins (1999) propose that per participant] are largely inaccessible to personality psycholo-
the mesocorticolimbic DA system (i.e., the dopaminergic projec- gists. Eysenck’s (1967) famous arousal theory of E inspired more
tions of the ventral tegmental area in the midbrain to the nucleus than 1,000 empirical studies and stimulated some of the most
accumbens, medial orbital prefrontal cortex (PFC), and other cor- creative research in personality psychology for the last 4 decades.
tical and subcortical structures) form the basis of the BFS and that It is conceivable that this enormous success of Eysenck’s (1967)
individual differences in functional properties of this neurobiolog- theoretical proposals depended significantly on easily accessible
ical system are the basis of agentic E. To be more specific, they noninvasive psychophysiological methods (e.g., EEG indices) to
argue in their “psychobiological threshold model” that positive measure the central construct of the theory (i.e., general cortical
incentive stimuli induce a certain state level of postsynaptic DA arousal) in humans. Thus, easily obtainable noninvasive psycho-
receptor activation in terminal structures of the mesocorticolimbic physiological indices of individual differences in DA functioning
DA system. This state level adds to the tonic trait level of DA that are relevant to E could prove very useful for progress in this
activation, which is higher in individuals high versus low in field of research. In the following section, we selectively review
agentic E. A certain level of trait plus state DA activity is neces- the current literature in search of such indices in the EEG.
sary to induce behavioral facilitation. As a consequence, agentic
extraverts reach this critical threshold more easily. Therefore, for E, DA, and Frontal EEG Activity
these individuals, relatively weak incentive stimuli are sufficient to
induce behavioral facilitation; in accordance, they experience and The EEG signal measured on the scalp is generated largely in
display the affective and behavioral concomitants of positive in- cortical areas (Davidson, Jackson, & Larson, 2000). Therefore, the
centive motivation more frequently. EEG cannot be used to directly measure activity in those subcor-
To date, the strongest empirical support for Depue and Collins’ tical elements of the mesocorticolimbic system that constitute the
(1999) central theoretical proposals comes from studies in which a neurobiological core of the BFS. However, the PFC receives
selective DA receptor agonist was administered while blood sam- strong dopaminergic inputs from mesolimbic structures, and it is
ples were taken to measure plasma levels of the hormone prolactin. well known that DA plays an important modulatory role in PFC
It is known that the secretion of prolactin is modulated by central functioning (e.g., Seamans & Yang, 2004). As a consequence,
DA activity. Thus, changes in prolactin blood levels in response to EEG indices of PFC activity are a good place to start searching for
challenges with dopaminergic drugs can be used as a peripheral noninvasive psychophysiological measures of central DA
indicator of central DA activity. In their landmark study, Depue et functioning.
al. (1994) reported strong and specific correlations between agen-
tic E as measured by Tellegen’s Multidimensional Personality Asymmetrical Frontal EEG Activity
Questionnaire (MPQ; scale Positive Emotionality; Tellegen &
Waller, in press) and several characteristics of the DA agonist- During the past 25 years, research on the relationship between
induced inhibition of prolactin secretion in a sample of N ⫽ 11 frontal cortical asymmetry and emotion, motivation, and person-
female volunteers. These findings were replicated using the same ality has attracted considerable attention (for recent review, see
methods in a larger sample, and no significant correlations Coan & Allen, 2004a). Besides many other interesting associa-
emerged for the MPQ scales Negative Emotionality and Constraint tions, significant correlations between relative left frontal cortical
(Depue, 1995, 1996). Although these observations strongly sup- activity, inferred from greater right than left frontal EEG activity
port the theory, both a replication by other research groups and a in the alpha band (8 –13 Hz), and several personality traits from the
direct statistical comparison of the validities of agentic E and E spectrum have been reported repeatedly (e.g., Coan & Allen,
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 173

2003; Harmon-Jones & Allen, 1997; Schmidt, 1999; Sutton & Gray, 2004). The ACC is an area of the PFC that receives strong
Davidson, 1997). Findings like these typically have been inter- dopaminergic inputs (Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Seamans & Yang,
preted within the theoretical framework proposed by Davidson and 2004) and whose activity is influenced by genes controlling PFC
colleagues (e.g., Davidson, 1998), according to which the left and DA neurotransmission (Blasi et al., 2005). It is interesting that
the right anterior region of the brain are part of two separate neural there is now substantial evidence (e.g., Pizzagalli, Oakes, & Da-
systems underlying approach and withdrawal motivation, respec- vidson, 2003) that activity in this region is reflected in frontal
tively. The approach system is activated by the perception of goals midline EEG activity in the theta band (4 – 8 Hz). Taken together,
and is thought to underlie initiation of goal-directed approach the recent literature suggests that both increased activity in the
behavior toward these goals as well as elicitation of approach- frontal cortex (as measured by reduced EEG alpha activity at
related motivational and emotional states (i.e., desire, wanting, frontal sites) and either increased or reduced activity in the ACC
enthusiasm, pregoal attainment positive affect, but also anger, see (as measured by increased or reduced frontal midline EEG theta
Harmon-Jones, 2004). On the neuroanatomical level, the approach activity) are not only related to E and/or related traits but are also
system encompasses the left dorsolateral and medial prefrontal modulated by brain DA.
cortices and the dopaminergic circuitry of the basal ganglia. To measure individual differences in resting frontal EEG activ-
Several investigators (see, e.g., the commentary by Kline in ity, two issues need to be considered. First, irrelevant individual
Depue & Collins, 1999; Harmon-Jones & Allen, 1997; Sutton & differences in skull thickness affect the amplitude of the EEG
Davidson, 1997; Wacker, Heldmann, & Stemmler, 2003) have signal measured on the scalp. Second, due to volume conduction,
noted a considerable conceptual overlap between Davidson’s electrical activity generated in other areas of the cortex is also
(1998) approach system and Depue and Collins’ (1999) BFS or measurable at the sites of interest and vice versa. In prior research,
Gray’s behavioral activation system (BAS; e.g., Pickering & Gray, two approaches have been used to control for these effects. The
1999). However, it is still a matter of debate whether and how first approach employs a regression procedure that corrects the
Davidson’s description of a left frontal approach system can be EEG power at the target site in the frequency band of interest for
integrated with the latter two models that focus on dopaminergic global EEG power in that frequency band computed as the average
neurotransmission in largely subcortical brain structures and do power across all electrodes. However, to use this method, the EEG
not make any assumptions about asymmetric cortical activation needs to be recorded from a relatively large number of electrodes
patterns (Coan & Allen, 2004b; Hewig, Hagemann, Seifert, Nau- to derive valid estimates of global EEG power (a minimum of 20
mann, & Bartussek, 2004; Wacker et al.). An empirical demon- electrodes is recommended by Davidson et al., 2000). The second
stration that frontal cortical asymmetry is sensitive to changes in approach is to simply compute difference values between EEG
brain DA functioning (or vice versa) would, thus, constitute an power measured at the site of interest and EEG power measured at
important step toward conceptual integration. In summary, frontal a separate site, thought to be influenced by different brain pro-
EEG asymmetry in the alpha band has not only been associated cesses. This approach has been employed with great success in
with traits from the E spectrum in several empirical studies, but research on frontal EEG asymmetry by calculating an asymmetry
there are also good theoretical reasons to expect that this psycho- index as the difference between homologous right and left elec-
physiological index is related to certain aspects of central DA trodes. Although this method only yields measures of relative
activity. activity at the two sites used to compute the difference values, it
bears the advantage that as few as two active electrodes are
Symmetrical Frontal EEG Activity sufficient to obtain the EEG index of interest. Application of this
approach, however, requires that the second electrode site is not
Several researchers recently have emphasized the importance of chosen randomly but based on relevant theoretical and empirical
bilateral (or symmetrical) resting frontal EEG activity in the con- information. Difference values should only be computed when the
text of personality traits (Davidson, 2004; Hewig et al., 2004; relative activity at the two sites arguably captures a (theoretical)
Hewig, Hagemann, Seifert, Naumann, & Bartussek, 2006) and meaningful aspect of EEG activity (e.g., EEG lateralization in
affective disorders (Nitschke, Heller, Etienne, & Miller, 2004). For research on frontal asymmetry).
example, Hewig et al. proposed that low resting bilateral frontal It is interesting that Hewig et al. (2004, 2006) did not only
EEG activity in the alpha band (i.e., presumably high frontal repeatedly observe negative correlations between BAS sensitivity
cortical activity) is associated with high habitual activity in Gray’s and EEG alpha power at frontal scalp sites, as already noted above,
BAS. The BAS is a DA-based motivational system closely related but also an (unexpected) positive correlation of similar magnitude
to Depue and Collins’ (1999) BFS, which responds to stimuli of between BAS sensitivity and EEG alpha power at parietal and
reward and nonpunishment, elicits positive emotions, and moti- parietooccipital sites. As a consequence, in these two studies, a
vates goal-directed behavior (e.g., Pickering & Gray, 1999). difference value computed as the difference between EEG alpha
Hewig et al. (2004, 2006) repeatedly observed significant negative activity at frontal sites minus activity at parietal sites would have
correlations between scores in Carver and White’s (1994) BAS been maximally suited to capture the aspect of brain electrical
scale and resting EEG alpha power at frontal scalp sites. Further- activity related to habitual BAS activation. Also, it has long been
more, several recent functional magnetic resonance imaging stud- known that two different manifestations of EEG theta rhythm can
ies have found substantial correlations between traits from the E be distinguished (Pizzagalli et al., 2003; Schacter, 1977). In addi-
spectrum and either increased or reduced activity in the anterior tion to the already described frontal midline variant, which has
cingulate cortex (ACC) during a number of cognitive and emo- been linked to alert states characterized by focused attention and
tional tasks (Canli, Amin, Haas, Omura, & Constable, 2004; Canli mental effort, a second variant exists, which exhibits a more
et al., 2001; Gray & Braver, 2002; Kumari, ffytche, Williams, & posterior and widespread distribution and has been associated with
174 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

decreased prestimulus alertness and less focused, more freely The Present Study
floating attention (e.g., during hypnagogic states). Because the
mental processes associated with the two types of theta rhythm are Based on prior findings, agentic E is hypothesized to be asso-
at least in part mutually exclusive, it would seem likely that the ciated with: (a) asymmetrical frontal EEG activity and in the alpha
activity in brain sources of frontal midline and posterior theta are band and symmetrical frontal EEG activity in the alpha and theta
also reciprocally related. If this were in fact the case, the difference bands and (b) performance in the n-back task. One aim of the
between frontal midline theta activity and theta activity at posterior present study was to demonstrate these associations empirically. It
(e.g., parietal) sites would be well suited to capture a meaningful is more important that the present work aimed at examining
aspect of EEG theta activity in a single measure. whether these associations can be attributed to a common dopa-
In summary, it can be concluded that based on prior findings, minergic basis. To this end, we measured the EEG and n-back
symmetrical frontal EEG activity both in the alpha and theta bands performance in extreme groups of participants either high or low in
can be hypothesized to be associated with both E-related traits and agentic E after administration of either placebo or a selective
brain DA functioning and that difference values of EEG band dopaminergic drug. Because dopaminergic drugs have been shown
power at frontal sites minus band power at parietal sites are to invert preexisting group differences in measures of brain activ-
well-suited to capture these aspects of brain electrical activity in ity and task performance (e.g., Corr & Kumari, 1997; Kimberg et
two simple measures. al., 1997; Mattay et al., 2000, 2003; Takeshita & Ogura, 1994), we
predicted that the differences between introverts and extraverts1
expected for the placebo condition would be inverted in the drug
E, DA, and Behavior: The N-Back Task condition, resulting in a significant interaction between E (intro-
verted vs. extraverted) and pharmacological substance (placebo vs.
There is ample evidence for an involvement of DA in working dopaminergic drug). If such a significant interaction could be
memory (WM) functions from a variety of different methodolog- demonstrated for our behavioral and/or EEG parameters, this
ical approaches (for recent reviews, see, e.g., Barch, 2004; Sea- would not only add to the still very limited database currently
mans & Yang, 2004). It is interesting that researchers have repeat- supporting the suggested link between agentic E and DA (Depue &
edly reported that DA agonists (e.g., amphetamine or the D2 Collins, 1999) but also open up new avenues for experimental
receptor agonist bromocriptine) improve performance in WM personality research on the human level by providing more acces-
tasks (and other tasks known to strongly recruit the PFC, such as sible indicators of central DA functioning.
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task) only in individuals with poor
baseline performance but impair performance in individuals with
Method
good baseline performance (e.g., Kimberg, D’Esposito, & Farah,
1997; Mattay et al., 2000, 2003). Using the well-known n-back Participants
WM task with increasing levels of task difficulty (i.e., WM loads
of 1-, 2-, and 3-back), Mattay et al. (2000, 2003) recently demon- A sample of 46 participants who were either high or low in agentic E
strated that there are genetically determined individual differences was selected from a pool of N ⫽ 507 male student volunteers (see below).
in DA neurotransmission that are associated with both perfor- We only used male participants because in females, the possibility of an
undetected pregnancy constitutes an additional risk in pharmacological
mance in the most difficult version of the n-back task (3-back)
studies and because the menstrual cycle is known to introduce error
under normal conditions and changes in performance and PFC variance into physiological recordings. Further inclusion criteria were
activity induced by dopaminergic drugs. German native language, age 18 to 40 years, right-handedness, and no
However, are these individual differences in DA neurotransmis- history of mental disorders as assessed with a standardized clinical inter-
sion also associated with E? Several recent observations indicate view (Margraf, 1997). Exclusion criteria were treatment with prescription
that this may indeed be the case. It has been demonstrated that both drugs during the last 3 months, acute diseases, history of abnormal con-
E and BAS sensitivity predict the magnitude of increases in brain ditions of the liver or the kidney, habitual smoking of more than 10
activity in dorsolateral PFC and the ACC produced by the difficult cigarettes per day, or use of illegal drugs. Six participants were excluded
3-back task (Gray & Braver, 2002; Kumari et al., 2004). In because of strong artifacts in the EEG recordings (excessive blinking
and/or muscle artifacts). The average age of the N ⫽ 40 participants in the
addition, Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001) have shown that reac-
final sample was M ⫽ 24.4 years (range, 18 –34 years), and all were
tion times (RTs) in the difficult (2- and 3-back) but not in the easy university or high school students. Participants were told that they were
(0- and 1-back) versions of the n-back task are negatively corre- selected based on self-ratings of certain aspects of their personality and that
lated with E, that is, extraverts reacted faster than introverts the aim of the study was to further delineate the brain processes and
specifically in those versions of the task that impose strong de- neurotransmitters underlying a RT task and to find out how these relate to
mands on WM. However, until now, the negative correlation personality. They were paid 45 EUR (55 USD) for approximately 5 hours
between E and n-back RTs has not been successfully replicated, of involvement in the study.
and it is unknown whether it is indeed (partly) based on individual
differences in DA functioning as proposed by Lieberman and 1
To facilitate reading, we will occasionally use the terms E or extra-
Rosenthal (2001). If a dopaminergic basis could be demonstrated
verted/introverted instead of agentic E and high/low in agentic E when
for this E effect, this would not only support Depue and Collins’ referring to the extreme groups of participants examined in the present
(1999) claim of a link between DA and E but also suggest that the study. However, we do not wish to imply that agentic E and E are one and
n-back task could be a useful behavioral paradigm to investigate the same: In addition to the agency facet, the super trait E also includes
the question of how individual differences in DA functioning are sociability and, in some conceptions, impulsivity (see Depue & Collins,
translated into extraverted versus introverted patterns of behavior. 1999).
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 175

Experimental Design and Extreme Group Selection In an adjacent room were placed a 32-channel SynAmps 5083 amplifier
(NeuroScan), a Macintosh Power Mac G4/450 (Apple Computer, Cuper-
Participants were assigned to one of four experimental groups formed by tino, CA) with a PCI 6503 SCSI card (National Instruments, Austin, TX)
the combination of E (extraverted, introverted) and Substance (placebo, D2 that performed recording and storage of the digitized EEG data under
antagonist).2 For the analyses of n-back performance and posttask waiting Labview 5.0 (National Instruments), and a personal computer that per-
period EEG, this 2 ⫻ 2 design was supplemented by the repeated factor formed experimental control under Presentation 0.5 (Neurobehavioral Sys-
Task (0- to 3-back task with the possibility to obtain reward and 2-back tems, Albany, CA).
task without reward; see below) with the effect of presentation order
controlled via cyclic permutation in a Latin Square. Procedure
Depue et al. (1994) recommended the MPQ scale Positive Emotionality
(Tellegen & Waller, in press) for measuring agentic E. Therefore, we Informed consent was obtained at the beginning of the session. The
developed a German short version of this scale to use it as an economical experimenter then conducted a standardized clinical interview (Margraf,
screening instrument for extreme group selection. This brief questionnaire 1997) to check for lifetime absence of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
contains three positively correlated 10-item scales that correspond to the of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) Axis I
three MPQ primary scales most relevant to Positive Emotionality: Well- psychiatric disorders. If the participant met this final inclusion criterion, the
being, Achievement, and Social Potency (see Tellegen & Waller, in press). experimenter administered identical capsules of either sulpiride (200 mg)
In prior unpublished work from our laboratory, the three newly developed or placebo under double-blind conditions at 9:30 a.m. ⫾ 75 minutes.
scales correlated highly with the respective MPQ primary scales (r ⬎ .85, Participants fasted and abstained from nicotine and caffeine from 10 p.m.
N ⫽ 140). The sum of the three scales constituted our aggregate measure of the previous night until completion of the experiment with the exception
of agentic E. A total of N ⫽ 507 male study volunteers were contacted on of a light breakfast (two rolls with butter, cheese, and/or jam) and an
several locations across the university campus and filled out the newly 8-ounce glass of 1.5% milk taken at the time of medication (or placebo) to
developed 30-item personality questionnaire. Internal reliabilities calcu- prevent nausea. Directly after completion of the breakfast, a brief (20 –25
lated in this pool of volunteers were satisfactory for both the three 10-item minute) intelligence test was conducted; then, participants completed sev-
primary scales (Cronbach’s ␣ ⱖ .81) and the 30-item aggregate scale eral personality questionnaires (see below).
(Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .87). Participants scoring above the median in all three The experimenter, aided by an assistant, then positioned electrodes and
primary scales constituted the extraverted group, whereas participants transducers and explained the n-back task. Afterward, 40 practice trials of
scoring not higher than the median on any of the three scales constituted the 2-back task were presented. Before leaving the room, the experimenter
the introverted group. The 40 participants in the sample of the present study reminded the participant to sit quietly to help prevent artifacts in the
scored either above the 67th percentile (20 participants in the extraverted physiological recordings and also told him that further instructions and task
extreme group) or below the 33rd percentile (20 participants in the intro- feedback were prerecorded and would be presented to him over the
verted extreme group) on the aggregate scale of agentic E. Ten participants loudspeakers. On average, the experiment was started 186 minutes (range,
of each extreme group were randomly assigned to either the placebo or the 150 –205 minutes) after administration of the medication (or placebo)
D2 antagonist group. capsule.
First, participants were instructed to relax during a 10-min rest period
(with three embedded 1-min EEG recording periods) while keeping their
Sulpiride: A Selective DA D2 Receptor Antagonist eyes open. Next, the n-back tasks were presented in five consecutive blocks
with the effect of block position controlled via cyclic permutation in a
We used the selective D2 antagonist sulpiride (200 mg) to challenge the
Latin Square. Each block was followed by a 1-min period, during which
DA system. The substituted benzamide sulpiride exerts its antagonistic
participants were instructed to simply wait for a moment, before either the
effects predominantly at D2 receptor sites to which it binds with high
next block would begin (2-back task without possibility to obtain reward)
affinity (Missale, Nash, Robinson, Jaber, & Caron, 1998). D1 receptors,
or the experimenter would briefly enter to give performance feedback and,
GABA receptors, and adrenergic, cholinergic, histaminergic, and seroto-
if the participant passed the criterion, to hand out the promised reward (0-,
nergic receptors are largely unaffected by sulpiride (Caley & Weber, 1995;
1-, 2-, and 3-back task with the possibility to obtain reward). During each
Mauri, Bravin, Bitetto, Rudelli, & Invernizzi, 1996). The drug accumulates
of the five 1-min posttask (prereward) waiting periods, the EEG was
more strongly in the mesolimbic than in the nigrostriatal DA system, and
recorded. After completion of the final task block, the participant filled out
it completely blocks stimulatory effects induced by the selective D2
a task questionnaire presented on his flat screen display and answered a
agonist bromocriptine (Jackson & Hashizume, 1987). Sulpiride reaches its
brief postexperimental interview. The experiment ended approximately 4 h
maximum plasma concentration 1 to 6 h after oral administration (Caley &
and 20 min after administration of the medication (or placebo) capsule.
Weber, 1995; Mauri et al., 1996). A single dose of sulpiride is well
tolerated by healthy volunteers, who are typically not able to tell whether
they received placebo or sulpiride (e.g., McClelland, Cooper, & Pilgrim, N-Back Task and Incentive Motivational Context
1990; Meyer-Lindenberg, Rammsayer, Ulferts, & Gallhofer, 1997).
The version of the n-back task employed was similar to the one used by
Braver et al. (1997). Single white letters (Times New Roman, 60 point)
Setting and Apparatus
The experimental room (4 ⫻ 3.4 m) was sound-attenuated and air- 2
Initially, the factor Substance included a third group of participants
conditioned and had a largely nontechnical appearance. Participants sat who received a low dose (1.25 mg) of the selective D2 agonist bromocrip-
comfortably in a reclined position. Electrodes were connected to a cus- tine. However, unexpectedly, several participants in this drug condition
tomized headbox (NeuroScan, Sterling, VA), where signals were pream- experienced severe side effects (nausea, dizziness). Because blindness to
plified with a gain of 30 (input impedance, 10 M⍀). N-back stimuli were the experimental condition thus could not be secured for the bromocriptine
presented on a 15-inch flat screen display placed in front of the participant group, rendering the obtained results difficult to interpret, we decided not
(distance to the eyes, approximately 60 –70 cm). Participants reacted by to report the bromocriptine data. The few statistically reliable effects that
pressing buttons on a response box (XQMS, Frankfurt, Germany). Prere- were observed for bromocriptine despite the error variance that was prob-
corded instructions and verbal task feedback were presented via two ably introduced by the aversive side effects were similar to those reported
loudspeakers located on the walls of the experimental room. for sulpiride, although somewhat smaller in magnitude.
176 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

were presented successively on a black computer screen. For each of the 1978). As a consequence, in studies like the present one, it is mandatory to
four levels of difficulty (0- to 3-back), there was a different decision consider the possibility that potential E effects are based on a significant
criterion for making the target response of either yes or no (see Braver et speed–accuracy trade-off. To simplify the presentation of the results and to
al., 1997) with the index or middle fingers of the right hand, respectively. ensure that measures of speed and accuracy were based on the same trials,
For each version of the task, a sequence of 60 practice trials (20 targets) we decided to quantify accuracy as the percentage of correct target reac-
followed by 120 evaluated trials (40 targets) was presented. The last 50 tions (see also Gray et al.). An alternative analysis using signal detection
practice trials of each task version were used to compute the 90th percentile measures (d⬘ and C) did not yield any additional information. (c) Individual
of the individual RT distribution for correct reactions (only RTs that were distributions of RTs are typically skewed leading to a positive correlation
not slower than the individual RT mean plus 3 times the individual RT SD between average RTs and individual SD values of RTs: Individuals whose
were considered). This served as an individual RT criterion for the fol- RTs vary more strongly on a trial to trial basis (e.g., between targets and
lowing evaluated task sequence. Each letter was presented for 500 ms nontargets) also tend to have longer average RTs. Therefore, it is possible
followed by a 1,650-ms interval, during which the screen was black. The that the RT differences between introverts and extraverts observed by
participant was expected to react during this 2,150-ms interval. Each trial Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001) would have been even more pronounced
ended with the presentation of a verbal feedback (350 ms) that indicated had these authors used measures of RT variability instead of RT means.
whether the participant’s preceding reaction was “correct”, “wrong” (i.e., To prevent distortion of the results by invalid reactions, all performance
either a wrong reaction or no reaction), or “slow” (i.e., correct, but slower measures were based only on trials in which participants pressed either the
than the individual RT criterion). Directly after the feedback had ended, the yes button or the no button in a 1,950-ms time window starting 200 ms after
next letter was presented. the beginning of the presentation of the letter and ending with the begin-
As explained in more detail elsewhere (Pauls, Wacker, & Crost, 2005; ning of the verbal feedback. The mean percentage of valid trials was at
Stemmler, 1997), we conceptualize E and other personality traits as dis- least 96.7% in all possible combinations of tasks and experimental groups.
positions that are activated only in relevant situational contexts (for dis- For the calculation of all statistical tests, the two RT measures were square
cussion of related theoretical perspectives, see Mischel & Shoda, 1998). root transformed to normalize distributions. Reliability estimates for the
Even though the current study was not designed to provide evidence for three performance parameters are shown in Table 1.
this traits-as-dispositions approach, this general concept nonetheless sig-
nificantly influenced certain aspects of the present experimental setup. EEG Recording and Analysis
When traits are only activated in relevant situational contexts, their pre-
dictive power concerning behavior and the associated brain states will also In keeping with the 10-20 International System (Jasper, 1958) and using
be limited to such relevant contexts. For agentic E, as defined by Depue Easy Cap electrode caps (Falk Minow Services, Herrsching-Breitbrunn,
and Collins (1999), relevant contexts are characterized by the presence of Germany), recordings were made from midfrontal (F3/F4), frontal midline
positive incentives. As a consequence, we took special precautions to (Fz), and parietal midline (Pz) regions of the scalp. EEG data were also
obtained from central (C3/C4/Cz), anterior temporal (T3/T4), and parietal
create an experimental setting that is strongly characterized by positive
(P3/P4) sites, but these recordings are not analyzed here. All sites were
incentives. Right at the beginning of the laboratory session, the participant
referenced to resistor-linked mastoids (M1–M2; 5-K⍀ resistors). To record
was told that in each version of the task, he had the chance to obtain a
eye blinks and vertical eye movements, electrodes were placed midline
reward for good performance. The potential rewards consisted of a variety
above and below the right eye. Electrodes on the outer canthi of both eyes
of popular goodies each worth about 1 EUR (1.3 USD). Goodies were
were applied to record horizontal eye movements. Electrode impedances
displayed throughout the whole experimental session in a large cardboard
were kept under 5 K⍀ for the EEG electrodes and under 1 K⍀ for the
box placed across the room from the sitting participant. After completion
ground electrode by cleaning the skin with alcohol and treating it with a
of each task version, the participant was allowed to pick one of the goodies
mild abrasive. For all EEG sites, InVivo-Metrics (Healdsburg, CA) Ag-
if he reached the following relatively low performance criterion not known
to him: He had to react correctly and fast enough to more than 50% of the
targets and to more than 50% of the nontargets. Because, by definition, the Table 1
first three letters of the evaluated sequence could not be targets in the 3-back
Grand Means (SDs) and Reliabilities of the Three Performance
version of the task, only the last 117 trials were used to check the performance
Measures in the Five Versions of the N-Back Task
criterion and to compute performance measures for statistical analyses. Par-
ticipants were told to react as quickly and as accurately as possible. RT Correct target
To check whether the possibility to obtain a reward moderated E effects, Target RT Variability reactions (%)
the 2-back version of the task was presented twice (using different se-
quences of letters): once with and once without the possibility to obtain M M M
reward. In contrast to the task presentation with possible reward already Task (SD) Rel (SD) Rel (SD) Rel
described above, in the presentation without possible reward, the partici-
pant was told that this time, he could not obtain a reward and that the 0-Back 452 .91 87 .79 94 .66
(57) (27) (05)
experimenter would not enter the room after the task to give performance
1-Back 490 .96 116 .86 87 .62
feedback. However, the participant was explicitly asked to engage in the (91) (38) (10)
task even though a reward could not be obtained. 2-Back 600 .94 157 .89 72 .82
For each of the five task presentations, the following three performance (149) (53) (17)
measures were calculated: (a) the average RT for correct reactions to 2-Back (no reward) 595 .95 165 .90 72 .85
targets, (b) the percentage of correct reactions to targets, and (c) the SD of (147) (63) (16)
RTs for all correct reactions. These choices were based on the following 3-Back 672 .95 177 .92 55 .58
considerations: (a) Gray, Chabris, and Braver (2003) have convincingly (214) (72) (14)
argued that the performance in target trials is a more valid indicator of
Note. N ⫽ 40. Rel ⫽ reliability estimated by applying the Spearman-
frontal cortical functions than the average RT of all correct reactions as Brown prophecy formula to the correlations between individual values
used by Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001). (b) It has long been known that calculated for the first and second halves of the trials. RT values were
when given the choice, introverts compared with extraverts tend to work square root transformed before calculation of correlations. For each task, both
more slowly but also more accurately on a variety of tasks (e.g., Wilson, halves contained 20 target trials each.
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 177

AgCl electrodes (8 mm) were used. For the ground electrode and the (ANCOVA) with Substance and E as between-subjects factors, Task (5; 0-
electrooculogram (EOG) sites, disposable VivoMed (Servoprax, Wesel, to 3-back with reward and 2-back without reward) as repeated factor, and
Germany) Ag-AgCl electrodes (10 mm) were employed. Block Number (coded from ⫺2 to 2) as a covariate. Making full use of the
EEG and EOG were amplified (EEG, gain ⫽ 500; EOG, gain ⫽ 100), Latin Square design employed to control for possible effects of task
filtered (bandpass set to 1–50 Hz for EEG, lowpass set to 1,000 Hz for presentation order, the covariate Block Number was included to reduce the
EOG, 50-Hz notch filter enabled), digitized at 2,000 Hz, and stored. In a error term by controlling irrelevant variance attributable to linear changes
second step, the signal was down-sampled to 250 Hz and converted to across task blocks. Using PROC MIXED of SAS/STAT (SAS 6.12 for
physical units and visually scored for artifact. Data were excluded if Macintosh, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC), the error variance-covariance
muscle, eye movement, or other artifacts were present. Spectral power was matrix for the repeated factor was specified to be completely general
then computed for 1-s artifact-free epochs overlapping by 50% using a fast (TYPE ⫽ UN option). EEG parameters from the initial rest period were
Fourier transform with a Hamming window. The Fourier transform was analyzed with two-way ANOVAs with Substance (2; placebo, sulpiride)
performed on 4-s segments each containing 1-s epochs of windowed data
and E (2; introverted, extraverted) as between-subjects factors.
padded with zeros on both sides. The resulting estimates of power density
ANOVAs and ANCOVAs were followed by a priori-specified contrasts
(microvolts squared per hertz) for each artifact-free 0.5-s interval were
tested with an ␣ level of .05. Contrasts for the EEG parameters from the
clustered into two bands, theta (4.00 –7.75 Hz) and low alpha (8.00 –10.25
initial rest period were defined on the level of the Substance ⫻ E interac-
Hz). We decided to focus on low instead of broad alpha (8 –13 Hz) because
tion and depicted E effects within substance groups and Substance effects
low alpha power has been shown to be more strongly associated with PET
measures of cortical activity (Oakes et al., 2004) and because we have within E groups. The E effects for the two EEG alpha measures (frontal
previously found frontal asymmetry effects to be more pronounced in the asymmetry and frontal vs. parietal activity) under placebo were tested
low alpha band (Wacker et al., 2003). one-tailed, and the remaining contrasts for EEG variables were tested
Power values for the two bands were transformed to natural logarithms two-tailed. Contrasts for the behavioral variables and the EEG parameters
to normalize the distributions of scores to be used in statistical analyses from the posttask waiting periods were defined on the level of the Sub-
(see, e.g., Davidson et al., 2000). Because estimates of spectral power stance ⫻ E ⫻ Task interaction and were designed to test for: (a) differences
based on short time intervals are unreliable (Davidson et al.), means of between introverts and extraverts within substance groups for each of the
ln-transformed power estimates per 1-min recording phase were obtained five n-back tasks; (b) differences of linear changes in performance with
only if more than 20% of the 0.5-s data (i.e., at least 12 s of EEG data) were increasing task difficulty (0- to 3-back) between introverts and extraverts
nonmissing, otherwise the phase was set missing data for a given region- within substance groups; (c) differences of these E effects (in each of the
band combination. We then computed the EEG indices of interest sepa- five tasks and in the linear performance changes) between substance
rately for each recording phase as follows: (a) frontal EEG asymmetry ⫽ groups or, put differently, differences of the substance effects (sulpiride-
ln-transformed power at F4 minus ln-transformed power at F3 and (b) placebo) between introverts and extraverts; and (d) differences between the
frontal versus parietal EEG activity ⫽ ln-transformed power at Fz minus 2-back tasks with versus without possible reward within each of the four
ln-transformed power at Pz. experimental groups. For the behavioral variables with the exception of the
An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with E (extraverted, introverted) and contrasts subsumed under (d) directed hypotheses could be derived from
Substance (placebo, sulpiride) as between factors did not reveal any sig- prior findings (see the Results section). Thus, for the behavioral variables,
nificant group differences in the mean number of artifact-free epochs used one-tailed tests were calculated for contrasts (a), (b), and (c), whereas
to compute within-subject averages of frontal EEG asymmetry and sym- contrasts (d) were tested two-tailed. For the two EEG alpha measures
metrical frontal versus parietal EEG activity for each of the eight data (frontal asymmetry and frontal vs. parietal activity) during the waiting
recordings, F(1, 36) ⱕ 0.34, p ⬎ .57. On average, the resting EEG indices
periods, the differences between introverts and extraverts in the placebo
for each 1-min recording were based on M ⫽ 62.0 (SD ⫽ 20.5) artifact-free
group for each of the five tasks were tested one-tailed (compared with
epochs. Finally, we averaged the three recordings of the rest period.
introverts, extraverts were expected to display greater relative right vs. left
frontal EEG alpha activity and lower frontal vs. parietal EEG alpha
Intelligence Test and Personality Questionnaires activity). The remaining EEG contrasts were tested two-tailed.
We assessed general fluid intelligence using the German short version of
Cattell’s Culture Fair Test (Scale 3, Cattell & Weiß, 1971). Besides our
short version of the MPQ scale Positive Emotionality (see above) used for Results
selection of extreme groups according to agentic E, we also used German
versions of the revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R; Ruch, Check for Side Effects and Blindness to Substance Group
1999) and the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ;
Zuckerman, 2002). The EPQ-R measures Eysenck’s personality traits of E Participants did not report any adverse side effects. The ratings
(mainly the sociability component), Neuroticism, and Psychoticism. The of nausea and dizziness averaged across experimental phases were
ZKPQ measures Zuckerman’s “Alternative Big Five” Aggression-
very low (⬍1) in all four experimental groups (0 ⫽ not at all
Hostility, Neuroticism-Anxiety, Sociability, Activity, and Impulsive Sen-
applicable and 1 ⫽ not applicable). Also, the percentage of
sation Seeking. The ZKPQ was employed for two reasons: (a) It nicely
separates the sociability component from the agency component of E by participants, who guessed (in a forced choice question posed after
providing separate scales for Sociability and Activity. Based on Depue and completion of the experiment) that they had received a pharma-
Collins’ (1999) theory, strong relationships with indicators of brain DA are cologically active substance, did not differ between the four ex-
expected only for Activity. (b) It also measures Impulsive Sensation perimental groups (extraverts-sulpiride, 2/10; introverts-sulpiride,
Seeking, that is, the trait considered by some to be more closely related to 5/10; extraverts-placebo, 5/10; introverts-placebo, 3/10). When
brain DA than agentic E (see, e.g., Pickering & Gray, 1999). asked to evaluate the confidence in their own guess concerning the
substance received in a probability rating, none of the participants
Statistical Data Analysis reported to be 100% sure (M ⫽ 63.8%, SD ⫽ 18.7). Thus, it can
N-back performance measures and EEG parameters from the posttask be concluded that participants were indeed blind to the experimen-
waiting periods were analyzed with three-way analysis of covariance tal condition as intended.
178 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

Check for Preexisting Differences Between Experimental observed in any of the four experimental groups (all p ⱖ .07, two-
Groups tailed). The tests of the central a priori contrasts are documented in
Table 2. The corresponding means are shown in Figure 1. Based on
A series of ANOVAs with Substance and E as between-subjects the findings by Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001), we predicted per-
factors and age, general fluid intelligence, and all ZKPQ and formance differences between introverts and extraverts under placebo
EPQ-R scales as dependent variables revealed no significant main specifically in the more difficult n-back tasks and a reversal of these
effects of Substance and no significant E ⫻ Substance interactions. differences under sulpiride, as suggested by prior reports of reversals
Several significant main effects of E were observed; supporting of preexisting performance differences after application of dopami-
convergent validity of our brief MPQ scale of agentic E extraverts nergic agents (e.g., Corr & Kumari, 1997; Kimberg et al., 1997).
compared with introverts scored higher in the alternative measure Indeed, the pattern of results observed for the target RTs in the
of agentic E; ZKPQ Activity, F(1, 36) ⫽ 36.82, p ⬍ .0001, ␩2 ⫽ placebo group closely replicates the findings reported by Lieberman
.48; as well as in ZKPQ Sociability, F(1, 36) ⫽ 16.25, p ⬍ .001, and Rosenthal (2001): Introverts had longer target RTs than extraverts
␩2 ⫽ .30, and in broader E as assessed by EPQ-R E, F(1, 36) ⫽ in all tasks, and this difference was statistically reliable only for the
39.01, p ⬍ .0001, ␩2 ⫽ .50. However, extraverts compared with 3-back task. Also, the linear increase in target RTs from 0- to 3-back
introverts also scored somewhat lower on EPQ-R Neuroticism, tasks under placebo was more pronounced in introverts than in ex-
F(1, 36) ⫽ 8.80, p ⬍ .01, ␩2 ⫽ .19, and ZKPQ Neuroticism-
traverts. In addition, the effects of sulpiride on target RTs differed
Anxiety, F(1, 36) ⫽ 8.35, p ⬍ .01, ␩2 ⫽ .19, possibly as a result
significantly between introverts and extraverts specifically in the 2-
of the moderate negative correlation between E and neuroticism
and 3-back tasks (significant tetrad differences in the sulpiride-
factors frequently described in the literature (e.g., John & Srivastava,
placebo column of Table 2): Sulpiride tended to slow target RTs in
1999). Finally, extraverts compared with introverts unexpectedly also
extraverts and to speed up target RTs in introverts.4 Put differently,
scored lower in general fluid intelligence, F(1, 36) ⫽ 8.63, p ⬍ .01,
under sulpiride, the pattern of results observed under placebo tended
␩2 ⫽ .18. No further main effects of E were observed.
to be reversed as predicted; compared with introverts, extraverts had
numerically slower target RTs in the 2- and 3-back tasks and signif-
N-Back Task Performance icantly larger linear increases in RTs from 0- to 3-back tasks.
It is interesting that both the E effects observed under placebo
The main effect of Task was significant for all performance and the modulation of the sulpiride effects by E could not only be
measures: target RT, F(4, 36) ⫽ 27.18, p ⬍ .0001; RT variability, documented for target RTs but also (and even more strongly) for
F(4, 36) ⫽ 47.56, p ⬍ .0001; and percent correct target reactions, RT variability (see Table 2 and Figure 1). In addition, under
F(4, 36) ⫽ 111.27, p ⬍ .0001. As expected, both target RT and RT placebo, introverts compared with extraverts did not only react
variability increased with increasing task difficulty, whereas the more slowly but also more accurately in target trials of the two
percentage of correct target reactions decreased (see Ms and SDs in difficult n-back tasks, indicating a significant speed–accuracy
Table 1). For each of the three performance measures, a significant trade-off, correlation between target RT and percentage of correct
linear effect of task block was observed: target RT, F(1, 36) ⫽ target reactions for the 3-back task, r(40) ⫽ .30, p ⬍ .05, one-
74.08, p ⬍ .0001; RT variability, F(1, 36) ⫽ 54.33, p ⬍ .0001; and
tailed.5 Also, the linear decrease in accuracy from 0- to 3-back was
percent correct target reactions, F(1, 36) ⫽ 6.01, p ⬍ .05. The
significantly less pronounced for introverts than for extraverts.
more task blocks participants had already completed, the shorter
However, apart from a weak tendency in the expected direction
were the observed RTs in the current task block and the higher
observed for the rewarded 2-back task, the predicted reversal of the
were the observed percentages of correct target reactions. These
E effects under sulpiride could not be shown for the percentage of
differences between task blocks are probably due to practice
correct target reactions (see Table 2).
effects that are not relevant to our hypotheses.
For the percentage of correct target reactions, no main effects or
interactions besides an unpredicted interaction of Substance and
Task were significant, F(4, 36) ⫽ 2.93, p ⬍ .05; all remaining p ⱖ
3
.09.3 However, for both RT measures, a significant two-way This effect resulted from the fact that compared with placebo the percent-
interaction, E ⫻ Substance was observed: target RT, F(1, 36) ⫽ age of correct target responses was significantly higher under sulpiride only in
the 2-back task without reward, t(36) ⫽ 3.19, p ⫽ .003, but not in the other
4.26, p ⬍ .05; RT variability, F(1, 36) ⫽ 8.85, p ⬍ .01; and for RT
tasks, p ⬎ .40. Because all other observed sulpiride effects on performance
variability, but not for target RT, the predicted three-way interac-
measures (see Table 2) were similar in the 2-back task with and without
tion E ⫻ Substance ⫻ Task was significant: target RT, F(4, 36) ⫽ reward, this unpredicted observation is difficult to interpret.
2.08, p ⫽ .10; RT variability, F(4, 36) ⫽ 3.27, p ⬍ .05. Besides a 4
For the 3-back task this tetrad difference (i.e., difference of the effects
two-way interaction, E ⫻ Task for RT variability, F(4, 36) ⫽ 2.39, of sulpiride-placebo in introverts vs. extraverts) was also significant when
p ⫽ .07, no further main effects or interactions approached sig- an analogous ANCOVA model was calculated for mean RT of all correct
nificance (all p ⱖ .28). answers, t(36) ⫽ ⫺2.18, p ⬍ .05, one-tailed, or for mean RT of incorrect
answers, t(36) ⫽ ⫺2.30, p ⬍ .05, one-tailed.
5
The correlations between target RT and percentage of correct target
Tests of a Priori Contrasts reactions for the 3-back task within the two substance groups were of
similar magnitude, although not significant due to the smaller sample size:
For none of the performance measures, a significant difference placebo, r(20) ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .13, one-tailed; sulpiride, r(20) ⫽ .33, p ⫽ .08,
between the 2-back tasks with versus without possible reward was one-tailed.
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 179

Table 2
Differences in N-Back Performance Between Introverts and Extraverts Within the Two Substance Groups and in Substance Effects
(Sulp-Plac): t-Values of Contrasts

Target RT RT Variability (SD) % correct target reactions

Extraversion contrast (I-E) Plac Sulp Sulp-Plac Plac Sulp Sulp-Plac Plac Sulp Sulp-Plac

0-back 1.86 0.60 ⫺0.89 1.78 0.29 ⫺1.06 1.66 0.47 ⫺0.84
1-back 1.15 ⫺0.00 ⫺0.81 2.07* ⫺0.32 ⫺1.69 0.27 ⫺0.61 ⫺0.62
2-back 1.38 ⫺1.10 ⫺1.76† 1.96† ⫺1.83† ⫺2.68†† 2.09† ⫺0.59 ⫺1.90†
2-back (no reward) 1.40 ⫺1.54 ⫺2.08† 2.69†† ⫺2.08† ⫺3.38†† 0.93 0.45 ⫺0.34
3-back 2.54†† ⫺1.26 ⫺2.68†† 3.41†† ⫺1.33 ⫺3.36†† 1.69† 0.63 ⫺0.75
Linear trend 0- to 3-back 1.96† ⫺1.78† ⫺2.64†† 2.60†† ⫺2.06† ⫺3.29†† 1.73† 0.38 ⫺0.96

Note. RT parameters were square root transformed before calculation of statistical analyses. df ⫽ 36. Plac ⫽ placebo; Sulp ⫽ sulpiride. Contrasts for the
0-back task and for the 1-back task were tested two-tailed; the remaining contrasts were tested one-tailed.
* p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. † p ⬍ .05, one-tailed. †† p ⬍ .01, one-tailed.

EEG Indices: Posttask Waiting Periods possible reward was observed in any of the four experimental
groups (all p ⱖ .12, two-tailed). The tests of the central a priori
Frontal EEG Asymmetry contrasts are documented in Table 3. For the theta band, the
Three-way ANCOVAs with E and Substance as between-groups corresponding means are shown in Figure 2.
factors, Task as within-group factor, and Block Number as covari- Under placebo, compared with extraverts, introverts had greater
ate did not reveal any significant main effects or interactions for relative frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity during the wait-
frontal EEG asymmetry in the theta or low alpha bands, for all ing periods after all tasks except the 0-back task. Under sulpiride,
effects F(1, 36) ⱕ 1.65, p ⱖ .21 or F(4, 36) ⱕ 2.25, p ⱖ .08. an opposite difference between introverts and extraverts was ob-
served after all tasks except the 0-back task. Furthermore, under
Frontal Versus Parietal EEG Activity placebo, extraverts, but not introverts, showed a linear decrease in
frontal versus parietal theta activity from 0- to 3-back task, t(36) ⫽
Three-way ANCOVAs computed for frontal versus parietal EEG ⫺2.02, p ⫽ .05, two-tailed, whereas under sulpiride only, intro-
activity in the theta and low alpha bands revealed the following verts, but not extraverts, showed this decrease, t(36) ⫽ ⫺2.62, p ⫽
statistically reliable effects: a main effect for Task in the theta .01, two-tailed. Thus, sulpiride did not only completely reverse the
band, F(4, 36) ⫽ 3.21, p ⬍ .05, resulting from a linear decrease
overall differences in frontal versus parietal theta activity observed
from 0- to 3-back waiting period, t(36) ⫽ ⫺3.02, p ⬍ .01, two-tailed,
under placebo, it also reversed the differences in linear decreases
a two-way interaction E ⫻ Substance for the theta band, F(1,
from 0- to 3-back: Under placebo, extraverts showed a (numeri-
36) ⫽ 19.77, p ⬍ .0001, and, less pronounced, for the low alpha
cally) larger decrease than introverts, whereas under sulpiride,
band, F(1, 36) ⫽ 4.40, p ⬍ .05, and finally a three-way interaction,
introverts showed a significantly larger decrease than extraverts.
E ⫻ Substance ⫻ Task, for both the theta band, F(4, 36) ⫽ 4.99, p ⬍
As shown in Table 3, the pattern of results observed for relative
.01, and the low alpha band, F(4, 36) ⫽ 4.67, p ⬍ .01.
frontal versus parietal EEG activity in the low alpha band was similar
to the one just described for relative frontal versus parietal EEG
Tests of a Priori Contrasts
activity in the theta band. However, the effects were generally less
Neither for the theta band nor for the low alpha band a signif- pronounced with the differences between introverts and extraverts
icant difference between the 2-back tasks with versus without within substance groups only significant for the 1-back task.

Figure 1. Target RT and RT variability in the four rewarded n-back tasks (M and SEM values of untransformed variables).
180 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

Table 3
Differences in Frontal vs. Parietal EEG Activity (Post Task Waiting Periods) Between Introverts
and Extraverts Within the Two Substance Groups and in Substance Effects (Sulp-Plac):
t-Values of Contrasts

Frontal vs. Parietal Theta Frontal vs. Parietal Low Alpha

Extraversion Contrast (I-E) Plac Sulp Sulp-Plac Plac Sulp Sulp-Plac

0-back 1.55 ⫺1.11 ⫺1.89 0.16 ⫺1.01 ⫺0.82


1-back 3.17** ⫺4.08** ⫺5.12** 2.02† ⫺2.91** ⫺3.48**
2-back 3.12** ⫺2.47* ⫺3.95** 0.27 ⫺1.15 ⫺1.01
2-back (no reward) 2.49* ⫺2.44* ⫺3.49** 0.73 ⫺1.26 ⫺1.40
3-back 2.42* ⫺3.67** ⫺4.29** 1.21 ⫺1.72 ⫺2.07*
Linear trend 0- to 3-back 1.29 ⫺3.51** ⫺3.35** 0.64 ⫺0.46 ⫺0.78

Note. df ⫽ 36. Plac ⫽ placebo; Sulp ⫽ sulpiride. Contrasts for low alpha in the placebo group were tested
two-tailed except for the linear trend contrast; the latter as well as the remaining contrasts were tested one-tailed.
* p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. ** p ⬍ .01, two-tailed. † p ⬍ .05, one-tailed.

EEG Indices: Initial Rest Period task versions and, most notably, did not differ between the re-
warded and the nonrewarded version of the 2-back task. In addi-
As explained in the Method section, our conceptualization of tion, prior research on the relationship between personality traits
traits as dispositions that are activated only in relevant situational and asymmetrical and symmetrical frontal EEG activity focused
context led us to expect strong correlations between agentic E and almost exclusively on recordings made under resting conditions
the EEG, particularly when the recording situation is characterized without any explicit reward (or punishment) signals (Coan &
by the presence of positive incentives. In the present study, posi- Allen, 2004a; Hewig et al., 2004, 2006). Therefore, we also
tive incentives were arguably most salient in the waiting periods checked whether the effects reported for the posttask waiting
after each of the four potentially rewarded n-back tasks, during periods can be documented for the initial rest period.
which participants were waiting to be rewarded for their perfor-
mance. As documented above, we found some strong relationships
between agentic E and EEG parameters obtained for these prere- Frontal EEG Asymmetry
ward recordings. However, the pattern of results, although some- Paralleling the observations for the posttask waiting periods,
what weaker for the 0-back task, was quite similar across the four two-way ANOVAs with the factors E and Substance did not reveal
significant main effects or interactions for frontal EEG asymmetry
in the theta and low alpha bands, for all effects F(1, 36) ⱕ 1.42,
p ⱖ .24, ␩2 ⱕ .038. Also, a priori contrasts did not reveal any
significant differences between introverts and extraverts within
either the placebo group or the sulpiride group (all p ⱖ .38,
two-tailed).

Frontal Versus Parietal EEG Activity


Two-way ANOVAs with the factors E and Substance did not
show significant main effects for frontal versus parietal EEG
activity in the theta or low alpha bands, for all main effects F(1,
36) ⱕ 1.88, p ⱖ .18, ␩2 ⱕ .045. However, paralleling the obser-
vations obtained for the posttask waiting periods, the two-way
interaction E ⫻ Substance was highly significant for the theta
band, F(1, 36) ⫽ 18.62, p ⬍ .0001, ␩2 ⫽ .332, and just failed to
reach formal significance for the low alpha band, F(1, 36) ⫽ 3.21,
p ⫽ .08, ␩2 ⫽ .076. A priori contrasts revealed that introverts had
greater frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity than extraverts
under placebo, t(36) ⫽ 2.17, p ⬍ .05, two-tailed, whereas the
opposite was true under sulpiride, t(36) ⫽ ⫺3.39, p ⬍ .01, two-
tailed (M ⫽ 0.40, 0.66, 0.77, and 0.44, SD ⫽ 0.18, 0.22, 0.22, and
0.23, for Extraverts-Placebo, Introverts-Placebo, Extraverts-
Sulpiride, and Introverts-Sulpiride, respectively). The difference
between sulpiride and placebo was significant for both introverts
Figure 2. Frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity during the waiting and extraverts, t(36) ⫽ ⫺2.25, p ⬍ .05, two-tailed, and t(36) ⫽
periods after the four rewarded n-back tasks (M and SEM values). 3.86, p ⬍ .001, two-tailed, respectively.
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 181

Relationship Between Resting EEG of the other personality scales, age or general fluid intelligence,
and 3-Back Performance was controlled for, F(1, 34) ⱖ 12.93, p ⱕ .001, Cohen’s d ⱖ 1.23.
Likewise, substance no longer modulated the E effects (cf.
As shown above, the pattern of results observed for frontal sulpiride-placebo column in Table 2) for 3-back target RT, t(34) ⫽
versus parietal EEG activity in the theta band is not only highly ⫺0.55, p ⫽ .29, one-tailed, and 3-back RT variability, t(34) ⫽
consistent across the different recording periods (initial rest period ⫺1.38, p ⫽ .08, one-tailed, after controlling for ZKPQ Activity
and posttask waiting periods) but also highly similar to the pattern (Cohen’s d ⱕ 0.47). However, these contrasts remained significant
observed for RT measures in the 3-back task. Thus, the question of for 3-back target RT, t(34) ⱕ ⫺2.15, p ⱕ .02, one-tailed, and
whether 3-back RT measures can be predicted from resting frontal 3-back RT variability, t(34) ⱕ ⫺2.72, p ⱕ .005, when either one
versus parietal EEG activity in the theta band arises. Correlation of the other personality scales, age or general fluid intelligence,
analyses revealed that 3-back target RT and RT variability were was controlled for (Cohen’s d ⱖ 0.74). It is notable that controlling
not only highly interrelated, r(40) ⫽ .88, p ⬍ .0001, one-tailed, but for general fluid intelligence even strengthened the E ⫻ Substance
also could be predicted from frontal versus parietal EEG theta interaction (i.e., the tetrad difference contrast) for both 3-back
activity measured during the initial rest period, target RT, r(40) ⫽ target RT, t(34) ⫽ ⫺3.63, p ⫽ .0013, and 3-back RT variability,
.26, p ⫽ .05, one-tailed; RT variability, r(40) ⫽ .29, p ⬍ .05, t(34) ⫽ ⫺4.05, p ⫽ .0003, Cohen’s d ⱖ 1.25.
one-tailed. In summary, these results show that (a) the effects are indeed
specific for agentic E (⫽discriminant validity) and (b) not specific
Are the Observed Effects Specific to Agentic E? to the measure of agentic E used to select groups of introverts and
extraverts but also observable with an alternative questionnaire
To check whether the effects of sulpiride on target RT, RT
scale (⫽convergent validity; see Figure 3 for a scatterplot of the
variability, and frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity during
simple correlations between ZKPQ Activity and frontal vs. parietal
the initial rest period were indeed only modulated by agentic E and
EEG theta activity within the two substance groups).
not by other (correlated) personality traits, age, or general fluid
intelligence, we calculated a series of ANCOVAs for each of the
three dependent variables using the statistical models described Discussion
above, but now entering an additional covariate (either an EPQ-R
scale, a ZKPQ scale, age, or intelligence scores), its two-way With the present study, we aimed at identifying an easily ob-
interaction with Substance, and, for the RT data, its two-way tainable EEG index and a behavioral paradigm that can be used to
interaction with Task and its three-way interaction with Substance investigate Depue and Collins’ (1999) suggestion of a dopaminer-
and Task (see Table 4 for the intercorrelations of the questionnaire gic basis for the personality trait agentic E in humans. Based on
scales, age, and intelligence scores). If the effects reported above recent findings, we argued that performance in the difficult ver-
are indeed specific to agentic E, they should disappear when the sions of the n-back task and both asymmetrical frontal EEG
alternative measure of agentic E (ZKPQ Activity) is used as a activity in the low alpha band and symmetrical frontal versus
covariate but remain significant when either one of the other parietal EEG activity in both theta and (low) alpha bands can be
personality scales, age, or general fluid intelligence is used as a hypothesized to be related not only to agentic E but also to certain
covariate. In brief, this is exactly what we found. After controlling aspects of central DA functioning. To put these hypotheses to an
for ZKPQ Activity, the E ⫻ Substance interaction was no longer initial test, we assessed performance in the n-back task and the
statistically reliable for frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity, EEG variables of interest in groups of healthy young men high
F(1, 34) ⫽ 3.18, p ⬎ .05, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.61. However, the E ⫻ versus low in agentic E after administration of either placebo or the
Substance interaction was still highly significant when either one selective DA D2 antagonist sulpiride. We predicted that under

Table 4
Intercorrelations of Personality Scales, General Fluid Intelligence, and Age in the Whole Sample

ZKPQ ZKPQ ZKPQ ZKPQ ZKPQ EPQ-R EPQ-R EPQ-R EPQ-R CFT 3
Act Sy N-Anx ImpSS Agg-H E N P L (gf)

ZKPQ Sy .60**
ZKPQ N-Anx ⫺.23 ⫺.31
ZKPQ ImpSS .21 .38* ⫺.23
ZKPQ Agg-H .22 .34* .21 .11
EPQ-R E .60** .81** ⫺.53** .58** .22
EPQ-R N ⫺.23 ⫺.24 .77** ⫺.11 .13 ⫺.31
EPQ-R P ⫺.26 ⫺.10 .00 .30 .30 ⫺.10 .06
EPQ-R L .08 ⫺.19 .02 ⫺.19 ⫺.17 ⫺.05 ⫺.07 ⫺.35*
CFT 3 (gf) ⫺.37* ⫺.29 .04 ⫺.06 ⫺.26 ⫺.44** .20 .20 ⫺.32*
Age ⫺.26 ⫺.31 .11 ⫺.22 .02 ⫺.16 .09 ⫺.09 .28 ⫺.08

Note. N ⫽ 40. Act ⫽ activity; Agg-H ⫽ aggression-hostility; CFT 3 (gf) ⫽ general fluid intelligence as measured by the short version of the Culture Fair
Test (Scale 3); E ⫽ extraversion; ImpSS ⫽ impulsive sensation seeking; L ⫽ lie scale; N ⫽ neuroticism; N-Anx ⫽ neuroticism-anxiety; P ⫽ psychoticism;
Sy ⫽ sociability.
* p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. ** p ⬍ .01, two-tailed.
182 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

linear increase in RTs from the 0- to the 3-back task. In addition,


under sulpiride, the RT differences between introverts and extra-
verts tended to be completely reversed as predicted.
Two further results of the present study indicate that the introverts’
slower responding in the difficult n-back tasks does not represent a
simple performance deficit, as suggested by Lieberman and Rosenthal
(2001), but may be more accurately interpreted as a different way of
performing the task: First, the pattern of differences between intro-
verts and extraverts under placebo and the partial reversal of these
differences under sulpiride documented for mean target RT were also
observable (even somewhat more pronouncedly) for the variability of
RTs across trials. Thus, it is unclear whether the observed RT effects
represent differences in the overall speed level (i.e., mean RTs), in the
variability of this level across trials, or both. In future studies, an
in-depth examination of differences in patterns of RT trial-to-trial
variation between introverts and extraverts could provide some inter-
esting insights concerning this issue (e.g., introverts compared with
extraverts may show more pronounced post-error slowing, resulting
in increased RT variability). Second, several observations indicate the
Figure 3. Scatterplot of the correlation between agentic E (ZKPQ Ac-
tivity) and frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity during the initial rest presence of a significant speed–accuracy trade-off at least for the
period within sulpiride (closed triangles; r ⫽ .64, p ⬍ .01, two-tailed) and more difficult versions of the n-back task: Under placebo and relative
placebo (open circles; r ⫽ ⫺.50, p ⬍ .05, two-tailed). The correlations to extraverts, introverts reacted not only slower on target trials, but
differed significantly between sulpiride and placebo, z ⫽ 3.81, p ⬍ .0002, they also made fewer mistakes. Furthermore, speed and accuracy in
two-tailed. Significant correlations within substance groups were not ob- the target trials of the 3-back task were negatively correlated: Partic-
served for age, general fluid intelligence, the EPQ-R scales, or the remain- ipants who reacted relatively slowly on target trials (e.g., introverts)
ing ZKPQ scales. tended to make relatively few mistakes. The fact that the predicted
reversal of the E effect under sulpiride could be observed more clearly
for the RT measures than for the percentage of correct target trials is
placebo, participants high versus low in agentic E would show (a) difficult to interpret because it may simply be a consequence of the
shorter target RTs and lower RT variability in the difficult n-back lower reliability of the latter parameter (see Table 1).
tasks, (b) greater relative left frontal cortical activity (i.e., greater Even though the n-back performance differences between intro-
relative right frontal EEG activity in the low alpha band; see Oakes verts and extraverts cannot be represented on a continuum ranging
et al., 2004), and (c) lower EEG activity in the low alpha band and from good to poor performance and, thus, seem to be more
either lower or higher EEG theta activity at frontal versus parietal complex than suggested by Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001), the
electrode locations. Furthermore, we predicted that all of these present results provide initial support for these authors’ proposal
differences between participants high versus low in agentic E that they are related to brain DA. Because the RT differences
would be reversed under sulpiride, resulting in significant two-way between introverts and extraverts (in particular, the differences in
interactions of the two factors E (high vs. low) and Substance the linear RT increases from easy to difficult tasks) were almost
(placebo vs. sulpiride) for each of the dependent variables. completely reversed by application of the selective D2 antagonist
These predictions were strongly supported by the present data sulpiride, it seems prudent to conclude that the observed individual
for n-back RT measures and for relative frontal versus parietal differences in behavior are at least partly based on individual
resting EEG theta activity but not for frontal EEG asymmetry. In differences in functional brain DA activity at D2 receptor sites.
addition, supplementary analyses showed that the observed effects These conclusions certainly are not only limited by several caveats
were indeed specific to agentic E and not due to different person- that we will discuss below, but they also do not speak to the important
ality traits (e.g., Sociability, Neuroticism, or Impulsive Sensation question of which presumably dopaminergically modulated cognitive
Seeking), age, or general fluid intelligence. We will now discuss processes underlie the observed differences in behavior. This question
each of these findings in more detail before sketching some im- cannot be answered with the present data because it is well known that
plications for further empirical and theoretical work on the psy- the difficult n-back tasks involve a rather large variety of cognitive
chobiological basis of agentic E. processes (e.g., Gray et al., 2003), and it is currently unclear which of
these processes are tapped by the available performance measures.
Because there is at least some consensus in the literature that the
E, DA, and N-Back Performance
difficult n-back tasks tap certain aspects of cognitive control, our
The differences in n-back performance observed between par- findings conform with the conclusion recently drawn by Gray and
ticipants high versus low in agentic E in the placebo group closely Burgess (2004, p. 36) that “some component of the cognitive control
replicate the observations of Lieberman and Rosenthal (2001): network depends on BAS [or agentic E] in a true functional sense,
Exactly as reported by these authors, (agentic) introverts had albeit the relation is complex.”
longer RTs than extraverts, and this difference was maximal (and Recent models of DA functioning may serve as a starting point
statistically significant) in the 3-back task. Also, in our study, just to tackle this complexity in future studies: Seamans and Yang
like in theirs, (agentic) introverts showed a significantly larger (2004) suggested that DA acts to contract or expand the breadth of
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 183

information held in WM buffers in PFC networks. In a similar metry relevant to agentic E. Direct comparisons between these two
manner, Dreisbach and Goschke (2004) proposed that DA adjusts alternatives are needed to reach reliable conclusions concerning
the balance of goal maintenance versus flexible switching between the reasons for the inconsistent trait-asymmetry correlations ob-
multiple goals. It is possible that under normal conditions, extra- served here and elsewhere (for review, see Hagemann et al., 2002).
verts compared with introverts tend more toward flexible switch- Likewise, it would certainly be premature to reject the idea of a partly
ing and to holding a greater breadth of information in WM (see dopaminergic basis of frontal EEG asymmetry based on the complete
Avila, Barros, Ortet, Parcet, & Ibanez, 2003). Of course, this absence of reliable effects of the selective DA D2 receptor antagonist
interpretation fits nicely with Lieberman and Rosenthal’s (2001) sulpiride observed in this study. As detailed in the Introduction, there are
suggestion that the E-related differences in n-back RTs result from well-founded reasons to expect an association between resting frontal
superior multitasking ability in extraverts. However, the notion of brain asymmetry and DA, and initial indirect evidence that such an
a trade-off between flexible switching and stable maintenance of association indeed exists comes from studies in which significant asso-
goals also implies that life may not be quite as unfair for introverts as ciations between resting frontal EEG asymmetry in the alpha band and
surmised by these authors: The extraverts’ greater multitasking or spontaneous eye blink rates (i.e., a peripheral indicator of DA activity, see
switching ability may come at the price of increased distractibility. Karson, 1983) were observed (e.g., Pauls et al., 2005). It is interesting that
very recent work with monkeys and humans suggests that spontaneous
E, DA, and Frontal EEG Activity eye blink rate may depend more strongly on DA D1 than DA D2 receptor
signaling (Jutkiewicz & Bergman, 2004; van der Post, de Waal, de Kam,
Asymmetrical frontal EEG activity. In the present study, under Cohen, & van Gerven, 2004).6 If this relative D1-specificity of dopami-
placebo, agentic E was not significantly related to resting frontal nergic effects also holds for frontal EEG asymmetry, this would offer a
EEG asymmetry in either the theta or low alpha bands. With regard straightforward explanation for the absence of reliable D2 antagonist
to the low alpha band, these observations contrast with several effects in the present study. Research with different dopaminergic agents
studies (e.g., Coan & Allen, 2003; Harmon-Jones & Allen, 1997; that also tap the D1 receptor system is needed to directly test this
Schmidt, 1999; Sutton & Davidson, 1997), in which E and related hypothesis.
traits have been linked to relative left frontal resting cortical Symmetrical frontal versus parietal EEG activity. As pre-
activity (i.e., relative right frontal EEG activity in the alpha band). dicted, we observed a significant difference in EEG theta activity
However, Hagemann, Naumann, Thayer, and Bartussek (2002)
at frontal relative to parietal electrode locations between partici-
have shown that this measure is strongly influenced by unknown
pants low versus high in agentic E under placebo: High agentic E
occasion-specific state factors, which account for almost as much
was associated with stronger parietal (vs. frontal) theta activity.
variance as a temporally stable latent trait factor. Thus, it is
This finding is consistent with earlier work by Stenberg (1992),
possible that in the present study, these unknown occasion-specific
who reported an association of a BAS-E factor with posterior theta
influences on frontal EEG asymmetry completely masked an ex-
activity and an association of a behavioral inhibition system (BIS)-
isting association of the latent trait component with agentic E.
Anxiety factor with frontal theta activity.
An alternative explanation for the unexpected absence of cor-
The finding that E is related to posterior versus frontal brain
relations between E and frontal asymmetry is suggested by our
activity is also echoed by observations from a PET study of
conceptualization of E and other personality traits as dispositions
cerebral blood flow correlates of E. In summarizing their findings,
that predict behavior and the associated brain states only in rele-
Johnson et al. (1999) note that on the whole, introversion was
vant situational contexts (for details, see Stemmler, 1997). In the
present study, participants knew that they were about to engage in associated with activity in frontal areas, whereas E was associated
a challenging task, and the goodies that could be obtained for good with activity in more posterior areas. In addition, as already noted
performance were placed directly in front of them during the above, a number of recent functional magnetic resonance imaging
whole experiment, including the initial rest period. However, be- studies reported significant associations between E-related traits
cause we observed virtually no differences between the potentially and activity in the ACC (Canli et al., 2001, 2004; Gray & Braver,
rewarded and the nonrewarded 2-back task, we simply do not 2002; Kumari et al., 2004). Given the evidence that frontal midline
know whether these efforts to create an experimental setting that is EEG theta activity reflects activity in the ACC (Pizzagalli et al.,
strongly characterized by positive incentives and therefore suitable 2003), it seems prudent to assume that the effects observed in the
to activate agentic E were actually successful. It is possible that present study for frontal versus parietal theta activity partly result
different features of the EEG recording situation were even more from E-related activity differences in the ACC. If this hypothesis
salient for the participants. For instance, recent findings suggest holds up in future studies that combine EEG source localization
that for male participants female experimenters represent a situa- algorithms and brain imaging technology or at least employ some-
tional context that activates an affiliative form of approach moti- what more densely spaced EEG derivations that allow to tease
vation that is not mediated by brain DA but nonetheless strongly apart the individual contributions of frontal midline and parietal
related to frontal EEG asymmetry (Kline, Blackhart, & Joiner,
2002; Pauls et al., 2005). In the present study, both men and 6
Also, note that Depue et al. (1994) did not observe a statistically
women served as experimenters. Thus, besides the possibility that
reliable main effect of the D2 agonist bromocriptine on (maximum) eye
in the predicted correlations between agentic E and EEG asym- blink rate, nor a modulation of such an effect by E. Only the time to blink
metry were masked by a sizable contribution of irrelevant maximum after administration of bromocriptine was found to correlate
occasion-specific variance to asymmetry scores, it could also be with E. However, in contrast to the link between E and the bromocriptine-
that we did not succeed in providing a sufficiently “pure” positive induced prolactin response, Depue and colleagues did not report a repli-
incentive context to specifically produce variance in EEG asym- cation of this correlation in a larger sample (Depue, 1995, 1996).
184 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

theta by providing a means to control for volume conduction and 1970; Patterson & Newman, 1993) maintain that E is not only
individual differences in skull thickness (see Introduction section), related to increased sensitivity or activity of a BFS-like system that
then the current knowledge on the functions of the ACC could be facilitates behavior in response to incentives (i.e., in Gray’s ter-
used as a basis to develop hypotheses concerning the functional minology, the BAS) but also to reduced sensitivity or activity of a
significance of frontal versus parietal theta activity; currently, the second broad motivational system, the BIS, that serves to inhibit
ACC is hypothesized to function as a discrepancy detector that behavior in response to events that are potentially incompatible
signals the organism when things are going worse than expected with the organism’s current goals (e.g., signals for punishment and
(e.g., Holroyd & Coles, 2002) and that serves to recruit lateral nonreward). Thus, for these authors, E represents the habitual
prefrontal cortices to exert top-down control to resolve the de- balance between behavioral activation/facilitation on the one hand
tected discrepancies and/or bias responding in a goal-congruent and behavioral inhibition on the other. It is interesting that several
manner (e.g., MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000). Thus, of the present findings may also be viewed from this theoretical
greater frontal versus parietal theta may indicate the initiation of perspective. According to Gray and McNaughton (2000), the core
conflict- or discrepancy-triggered goal shielding or, in terms of function of the BIS lies in the detection of conflict among con-
Seamans and Yang’s (2004) model of DA functioning in the PFC, a currently activated goals. As explained above, the detection of
contraction (vs. expansion) of the breadth of information held in WM. such goal conflicts is thought to be associated with activity in the
Although the present data thus do not allow definite conclusions ACC and frontal midline EEG theta. Also, some authors (e.g.,
concerning both the underlying neural generators and the func- Stenberg, 1992) have reported a positive correlation between traits
tional significance of frontal versus parietal theta activity, they do from the BIS spectrum and frontal EEG theta. Thus, the greater
provide some interesting insights with respect to the implicated frontal versus parietal theta we observed in introverts compared
neurochemistry. For this EEG measure, we observed a strong with extraverts under placebo may indicate stronger BIS sensitiv-
interaction of the two factors E (high vs. low) and Substance ity in addition to (or even rather than) weaker BAS/BFS sensitivity
(placebo vs. sulpiride); under sulpiride, E effects were completely in the former. In a similar manner, the introverts’ tendency to
reversed, with high agentic E associated with stronger frontal perform more slowly but somewhat more accurately than extra-
versus parietal theta activity. The selective DA D2 receptor antag- verts in the difficult n-back tasks under placebo may indicate not
onist sulpiride resulted in a shift toward more frontal versus only lower BAS/BFS sensitivity, but also greater BIS sensitivity,
parietal EEG theta activity in participants high in agentic E and in resulting in a greater responsiveness to the negative feedback
a shift toward more parietal versus frontal EEG theta activity in associated with mistakes (see also Patterson & Newman, 1993).
participants low in agentic E leading to no discernible effects for In brief, our suggestion that both the electrocortical and the
the group as a whole. These observations suggest that frontal behavioral differences between introverts and extraverts reported
versus parietal EEG theta activity contains a considerable amount here indicate individual differences in the balance of the contrac-
of trait variance associated both with agentic E and DA function- tion versus expansion of the breadth of information held in WM
ing. Therefore, the present study provides initial evidence that could potentially be mapped onto earlier suggestions that E results
frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity qualifies as an easily from individual differences in the balance between two neurobe-
obtainable EEG index that can be used as an efficient tool to havioral systems, the BIS and the BAS/BFS. This would of course
further probe the DA basis of E. provide a straightforward explanation for the fact that some of the
For frontal versus parietal EEG activity in the low alpha band, associations we observed for agentic E are about as suggestive of
we observed only weak and (with the exception of the waiting individual differences in BIS activity as of individual differences
period after the 1-back task) statistically unreliable effects that in BAS/BFS activity. However, in the present study, we did not
mirrored the strong effects observed for the theta band. Why were observe any significant effects for traits more closely aligned with
we not able to demonstrate a more consistent relationship between habitual BIS sensitivity (i.e., EPQ-R Neuroticism and ZKPQ
frontal versus parietal EEG activity and E in the placebo group as Neuroticism-Anxiety), and the current psychobiological personal-
the results reported by Hewig et al. (2004, 2006) would seem to ity literature provides no theoretical basis to predict an association
suggest? A number of possible explanations are offered by several between habitual BIS sensitivity and brain DA. Therefore, it may
significant methodological differences between the Hewig et al. be more appropriate to stick to Depue and Collins’ (1999) sug-
studies and the present work (e.g., sample size, number and length gestion that agentic E maps only onto a single, affectively unipolar
of EEG recordings, parametrization, and EEG reference scheme). motivational system, which is neurobiologically based on brain
Anyway, the similarity of the patterns we observed in the low DA (i.e., the BFS) and to conceptualize the involvement of the BIS
alpha and theta bands points to the intriguing possibility that both as somewhat more indirect. For example, one could simply add the
the Hewig et al. findings and the present results are based on the idea that high state activity in the BFS/BAS is incompatible with
same electrocortical phenomenon: Boundaries between traditional high state activity in the BIS, which places partly antagonistic
EEG frequency bands are somewhat arbitrary. Maybe the presum- constraints on behavior and the associated brain states.
ably DA-related aspects of oscillatory brain activity that we ob-
served most clearly in the theta range can sometimes (or for some Underlying Mechanisms: The Return of Eysenck’s (1967)
individuals) also be detected in the alpha band. Inverted U?

(Agentic) E and the BIS? The present findings do not speak to the important question of
why the D2 antagonist sulpiride had opposite psychophysiological
In contrast to the theoretical perspective adopted here (see and behavioral effects in individuals high versus low in agentic E.
Depue & Collins, 1999), other personality theorists (e.g., Gray, Opposing effects of dopaminergic drugs in different groups of
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 185

participants have been described frequently before (e.g., Corr & specific to DA or rather attributable to unknown unspecific side
Kumari, 1997; Kimberg et al., 1997; Mattay et al., 2000, 2003; effects of sulpiride. In addition, in this initial study, we only
Takeshita & Ogura, 1994). It is typical that such effects are employed one single dosage of sulpiride and made no attempt to
explained by assuming that the different groups occupy separate investigate dose-response relationships. Future studies will have to
locations on a continuum of baseline dopaminergic functioning use a greater variety of pharmacological agents and dosages to
and that the relationship between the dependent variable, in which provide converging evidence for the interpretation in terms of a
effects of the dopaminergic drug were observed, and baseline DA DA mechanism suggested here. Also, it should be noted that
follows an inverted U-shaped function (e.g., Depue & Collins, because at present no pharmacological agents exist that exclu-
1999; Mattay et al., 2003). Invoking such an inverted U-shaped sively influence the mesocorticolimbic DA system rather than the
function to explain differences between introverts and extraverts other DA systems of the brain (nigrostriatal, tuberoinfundibular,
will surely remind many readers of Eysenck’s (1967) famous and incertohypothalamic), the strong Depue and Collins (1999)
arousal theory of E in which he postulated that by virtue of hypothesis of a specific role of the mesocorticolimbic DA system
hardwired individual differences in the brain’s ascending reticular cannot be validated with the methods employed here. Furthermore,
activating system, introverts are typically more aroused/arousable
because we only investigated preselected groups of male partici-
than extraverts except for particularly arousing conditions in which
pants, it remains an open question whether the present findings
the protective mechanism of transmarginal inhibition leads to
generalize to randomly drawn female samples.
paradoxical arousal decreases in introverts, resulting in a reversal
Finally, because we conceptualize traits as dispositions that are
of the typical arousal differences. Indeed, given that the ascending
only operative in relevant situational contexts (see Stemmler,
DA fibers are an important (although not exclusive) component of
the ascending reticular activating system (Robbins, 1997), the 1997), the present study was conducted in an incentive motiva-
present findings could be interpreted as evidence that, on the tional setting specifically designed to activate agentic E. However,
whole, Eysenck (1967) was right all along (see also Corr & because we made only a relatively weak and absolutely unsuccess-
Kumari, 1997). However, what exactly are the hypothesized dif- ful attempt to manipulate the situational context (i.e., announcing
ferences in baseline DA functioning between extraverts and intro- for one of the more challenging tasks that no reward and no
verts, and where do they come from? Novel and more specific performance feedback would be given afterward), we cannot tell
hypotheses concerning this issue may eventually come from mo- whether our observations are indeed specific to trait-relevant pos-
lecular genetic studies that have recently begun to identify func- itive incentive situations. If such a situational specificity of the
tionally relevant gene polymorphisms that are linked to both E and observed associations could be demonstrated in future studies, this
functional differences in DA neurotransmission (see, e.g., Mattay would not only support the general concept of traits as dispositions
et al., 2003; Reuter & Hennig, 2005). Combining such molecular (Stemmler, 1997), but also Depue and Collins’ (1999) claim that
genetic assessments with the methods employed in the present agentic E is based on positive incentive motivation, thereby forg-
study could thus be a promising approach for further research ing further links between these authors’ theoretical model and the
aimed at going beyond the inverted U to specify the precise extensions proposed here.
mechanisms underlying the differences between introverts and
extraverts reported here.
Conclusions
Limitations and Future Directions In the present study, we demonstrated that the effects of the
Even though we were able to demonstrate that the observed selective DA D2 antagonist sulpiride on both RT measures in the
effects were indeed due to agentic E rather than several other difficult n-back tasks and frontal versus parietal EEG theta activity
correlated personality traits, age, or general fluid intelligence, of are strongly and specifically modulated by agentic E. These ob-
course the possibility still remains that some other third variable servations not only offer novel support for Depue and Collins’
correlated with E actually underlies our findings. Such potential (1999) DA theory of agentic E, they also provide new opportuni-
alternative variables range from other unmeasured personality ties to further extend this theory to the human level by identifying
dimensions to established correlates of E like the responsiveness to behavioral indicators as well as an easily accessible, noninvasive
pharmacological manipulations of general arousal (e.g., with caf- EEG measure that are sensitive to both E and manipulations of
feine) and the sensitivity of task performance to time of day functional brain DA activity and thus open up a range of new
(Revelle, Humphreys, Simon, & Gilliand, 1980), just to name a research avenues for further work on the psychobiological basis of
few. As one of the reviewers noted, such potential third variable this important dimension of human personality.
explanations represent a general problem for this kind of research
as long as we have not found a way to experimentally vary E itself,
7
which would probably be akin to changing the engrained hardwir- At least a third variable explanation based on the Revelle et al. (1980)
ing of the brain. For the time being, all we can do is include findings concerning E, time of day, and caffeine effects would not seem
particularly convincing because these authors observed consistent findings
multiple methods of assessment for the targeted construct to foster
only for impulsivity, which is a facet of Eysenck’s early E concept quite
integration of different lines of investigation of E and measure as distinct from agentic E as defined and measured in the present article.
many potentially relevant third variables as possible to rule out at There were no associations between ZKPQ Impulsive Sensation Seeking
least the more obvious alternative explanations.7 and either one of our two alternative measures of agentic E. Also, ZKPQ
Because we only used one (albeit highly selective) dopaminer- Impulsive Sensation Seeking (even though strongly correlated to EPQ-R E)
gic drug, it remains unclear whether the observed effects are in fact did not produce any significant effects in the present study.
186 WACKER, CHAVANON, AND STEMMLER

References dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality and


Social Psychology, 67, 485– 498.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates
ual of disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased dis-
Avila, C., Barros, A., Ortet, G., Parcet, M. A., & Ibanez, M. I. (2003). tractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
Set-shifting and sensitivity to reward: A possible dopamine mechanism for and Cognition, 30, 343–353.
explaining disinhibitory disorders. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 951–959. Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL:
Barch, D. M. (2004). Pharmacological manipulation of human working Charles C Thomas.
memory. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 174, 126 –135. Farde, L., Gustavsson, J. P., & Jonsson, E. (1997). D2 dopamine receptors
Blasi, G., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., Elvevag, B., Callicott, J. H., Das, S., and personality traits. Nature, 385, 590.
et al. (2005). Effect of catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met geno- Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-
type on attentional control. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5038 –5045. extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249 –266.
Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., & Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety
Noll, D. C. (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
in human working memory. Neuroimage, 5, 49 – 62. Gray, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2002). Personality predicts working-memory-
Breier, A., Kestler, L., Adler, C., Elman, I., Wiesenfeld, N., Malhotra, A., related activation in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex. Cognitive,
et al. (1998). Dopamine D2 receptor density and personal detachment in Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 64 –75.
healthy subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1440 –1442. Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2004). Personality differences in cognitive
Caley, C. F., & Weber, S. S. (1995). Sulpiride: An antipsychotic with control? BAS, processing efficiency, and the prefrontal cortex. Journal
selective dopaminergic antagonist properties. Annals of Pharmacother- of Research in Personality, 38, 35–36.
apy, 29, 152–160. Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of
Canli, T., Amin, Z., Haas, B., Omura, K., & Constable, R. T. (2004). A general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316 –322.
double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the Gray, N. S., Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1994). Psychoticism and
anterior cingulate. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 897–904. dopamine D2 binding in the basal ganglia using single photon emission
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. tomography. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 431– 434.
(2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Thayer, J. F., & Bartussek, D. (2002). Does resting
emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33– 42. electroencephalograph asymmetry reflect a trait? An application of latent state-
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, trait theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 619–641.
and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Contributions from research on anger and cog-
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. nitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asym-
Cattell, R. B., & Weiß, R. H. (1971). Grundintelligenztest Skala 3 (CFT 3). metrical frontal brain activity. Biological Psychology, 67, 51–76.
Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1997). Behavioral activation sensi-
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobi- tivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: Covariation of putative indi-
ological model of temperament and character. Archives of General cators related to risk for mood disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Psychiatry, 50, 975–990. ogy, 106, 159 –163.
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2004). On the
activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology, 40, 106–114. Selective Relation of Frontal Cortical Asymmetry and Anger-Out Versus
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2004a). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a Anger-Control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 926–939.
moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67,7– 49. Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2006).
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2004b). The state and trait nature of frontal The relation of cortical activity and BIS/BAS on the trait level. Biolog-
asymmetry in emotion. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The ical Psychology, 71, 42–53.
asymmetrical brain (pp. 565– 615). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error
Corr, P. J., & Kumari, V. (1997). Sociability/impulsivity and attenuated processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related
dopaminergic arousal: Critical flicker/fusion frequency and procedural negativity. Psychological Review, 109, 679 –709.
learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 805– 815. Jackson, D. M., & Hashizume, M. (1987). Bromocriptine-induced loco-
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspec- motor stimulation in mice is modulated by dopamine D-1 receptors.
tives from affective neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 307–330. Journal of Neural Transmission, 69, 131–145.
Davidson, R. J. (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: Perspectives Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Feder-
on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biological Psychology, 67, 219–233. ation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375.
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Human electro- John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History,
encephalography. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary & G. G. Berntson measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John
(Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 27–52). New York: (Eds.), Handbook of personality: theory and research (2nd ed., pp.
Cambridge University Press. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Depue, R. A. (1995). Neurobiological factors in personality and depres- Johnson, D. L., Wiebe, J. S., Gold, S. M., Andreasen, N. C., Hichwa, R. D.,
sion. European Journal of Personality, 9, 413– 439. Watkins, G. L., et al. (1999). Cerebral blood flow and personality: A
Depue, R. A. (1996). Neurobiology and the structure of personality: positron emission tomography study. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Implications for the personality disorders. In J. Clarkin & M. F. Len- 156, 252–257.
zenweger (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorders. New York: Jutkiewicz, E. M., & Bergman, J. (2004). Effects of dopamine D1 ligands on
Guilford Press. eye blinking in monkeys: Efficacy, antagonism, and D1/D2 interactions.
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 311, 1008 –1015.
personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extra- Karson, C. N. (1983). Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic
version. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569. systems. Brain, 106, 643– 653.
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Kestler, L. P., Malhotra, A. K., Finch, C., Adler, C., & Breier, A. (2000).
Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced The relation between dopamine D2 receptor density and personality:
EXTRAVERSION AND DOPAMINE 187

Preliminary evidence from the NEO personality inventory-revised. Neu- L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and
ropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 48 –52. research (2nd ed., pp. 277–299). New York: Guilford Press.
Kimberg, D. Y., D’Esposito, M., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Effects of Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (2001). Dopamine, appetitive reinforce-
bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. ment, and the neuropsychology of human learning: An individual dif-
Neuroreport, 8, 3581–3585. ferences approach. In A. Eliasz & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Advances in
Kline, J. P., Blackhart, G. C., & Joiner, T. E. (2002). Sex, lie scales, and research on temperament (pp. 113–149). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.
electrode caps: An interpersonal context for defensiveness and anterior Pizzagalli, D. A., Oakes, T. R., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Coupling of theta
electroencephalographic asymmetry. Personality and Individual Differ- activity and glucose metabolism in the human rostral anterior cingulate
ences, 33, 459 – 478. cortex: An EEG/PET study of normal and depressed subjects. Psycho-
Kumari, V., ffytche, D. H., Williams, S. C., & Gray, J. A. (2004). physiology, 40, 939 –949.
Personality predicts brain responses to cognitive demands. Journal of Reuter, M., & Hennig, J. (2005). Association of the functional catechol-
Neuroscience, 24, 10636 –10641. O-methyltransferase VAL158MET polymorphism with the personality
Laakso, A., Vilkman, H., Kajander, J., Bergman, J., Paranta, M., Solin, O., et al. trait of extraversion. Neuroreport, 16, 1135–1138.
(2000). Prediction of detached personality in healthy subjects by low dopamine Revelle, W., Humphreys, M. S., Simon, L., & Gilliand, K. (1980). The
transporter binding. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 290–292. interactive effect of personality, time of day, and caffeine: A test of the
Laakso, A., Wallius, E., Kajander, J., Bergman, J., Eskola, O., Solin, O., et arousal model. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 109, 1–31.
al. (2003). Personality traits and striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in Robbins, T. W. (1997). Arousal systems and attentional processes. Biolog-
healthy subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 904 –910. ical Psychology, 45, 57–71.
Lieberman, M. D., & Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always Ruch, W. (1999). Die revidierte Fassung des Eysenck Personality Ques-
tell who likes them: Multitasking and nonverbal decoding. Journal of tionnaire und die Konstruktion des deutschen EPQ-R bzw. EPQ-RK.
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 294 –310. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised and the construction of
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). the German standard and short versions (EPQ-R and EPQ-RK).
Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 1–24.
cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838. Schacter, D. L. (1977). EEG theta waves and psychological phenomena: A
Margraf, J. (1997). Mini-DIPS (Kurz-)Interview bei psychischen Störun- review and analysis. Biological Psychology, 5, 47– 82.
gen. Berlin: Springer. Schmidt, L. A. (1999). Frontal brain electrical activity in shyness and
Mattay, V. S., Callicott, J. H., Bertolino, A., Heaton, I., Frank, J. A., sociability. Psychological Science, 10, 316 –320.
Coppola, R., et al. (2000). Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive Seamans, J. K., & Yang, C. R. (2004). The principal features and mech-
performance and cortical activation. Neuroimage, 12, 268 –275. anisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in
Mattay, V. S., Goldberg, T. E., Fera, F., Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Egan, Neurobiology, 74, 1–58.
M. F., et al. (2003). Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype Stemmler, G. (1997). Selective activation of traits: Boundary conditions for the
and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proceed- activation of anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 213–233.
ings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 6186 – 6191. Stenberg, G. (1992). Personality and the EEG: Arousal and emotional
Mauri, M. C., Bravin, S., Bitetto, A., Rudelli, R., & Invernizzi, G. (1996). arousability. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1097–1113.
A risk-benefit assessment of sulpiride in the treatment of schizophrenia. Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A
Drug Safety, 14, 288 –298. biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems.
McClelland, G. R., Cooper, S. M., & Pilgrim, A. J. (1990). A comparison Psychological Science, 8, 204 –210.
of the central nervous system effects of haloperidol, chlorpromazine and Takeshita, S., & Ogura, C. (1994). Effect of the dopamine D2 antagonist
sulpiride in normal volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacol- sulpiride on event-related potentials and its relation to the law of initial
ogy, 30, 795– 803. value. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 16, 99 –106.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Rammsayer, T., Ulferts, J., & Gallhofer, B. (1997). Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (in press). Exploring personality through test
The effects of sulpiride on psychomotor performance and subjective construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Ques-
tolerance. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 7, 219 –223. tionnaire (MPQ). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and van der Post, J., de Waal, P. P., de Kam, M. L., Cohen, A. F., & van
personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229 –258. Gerven, J. M. (2004). No evidence of the usefulness of eye blinking as
Missale, C., Nash, S. R., Robinson, S. W., Jaber, M., & Caron, M. G. a marker for central dopaminergic activity. Journal of Psychopharma-
(1998). Dopamine receptors: From structure to function. Physiological cology, 18, 109 –114.
Review, 78, 189 –225. Wacker, J., Heldmann, M., & Stemmler, G. (2003). Separating emotion
Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Etienne, M. A., & Miller, G. A. (2004). and motivational direction in fear and anger: Effects on frontal asym-
Prefrontal cortex activity differentiates processes affecting memory in metry. Emotion, 3, 167–193.
depression. Biological Psychology, 67, 125–143. Wilson, G. (1978). Introversion/extroversion. In H. London & J. E. Exner
Oakes, T. R., Pizzagalli, D. A., Hendrick, A. M., Horras, K. A., Larson, (Eds.), Dimensions of personality (pp. 217–262). New York: Wiley.
C. L., Abercrombie, H. C., et al. (2004). Functional coupling of simul- Zuckerman, M. (1991). The psychobiology of personality. New York:
taneous electrical and metabolic activity in the human brain. Human Cambridge University Press.
Brain Mapping, 21, 257–270. Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman-Kuhlman personality questionnaire
Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from (ZKPQ): An alternative five-factorial model. In B. d. Raad & M.
aversive events: Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes Perugini (Eds.), Big Five assessment (pp. 377–396). Göttingen, Ger-
of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100, 716 –736. many: Hogrefe & Huber.
Pauls, C. A., Wacker, J., & Crost, N. W. (2005). The two components of
social desirability and their relations to resting frontal brain asymmetry. Received August 12, 2005
Journal of Individual Differences, 26, 29 – 42. Revision received January 25, 2006
Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality. In Accepted February 3, 2006 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 188 –204 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.188

Possible Selves and Academic Outcomes: How and When Possible Selves
Impel Action
Daphna Oyserman, Deborah Bybee, and Kathy Terry
The University of Michigan

Puzzled by the gap between academic attainment and academic possible selves (APSs) among low-
income and minority teens, the authors hypothesized that APSs alone are not enough unless linked with
plausible strategies, made to feel like “true” selves and connected with social identity. A brief interven-
tion to link APSs with strategies, create a context in which social and personal identities felt congruent,
and change the meaning associated with difficulty in pursuing APSs (n ⫽ 141 experimental, n ⫽ 123
control low-income 8th graders) increased success in moving toward APS goals: academic initiative,
standardized test scores, and grades improved; and depression, absences, and in-school misbehavior
declined. Effects were sustained over a 2-year follow-up and were mediated by change in possible selves.

Keywords: possible selves, African American, Hispanic, prevention, self-regulation

From early adolescence, the future is an important component of of self-concept. Another possibility is that PSs fail to sustain
self-concept (McGuire & Padawe-Singer, 1976), and doing well in self-regulatory action because sustaining self-regulatory effort
school is a common element of youths’ future-oriented selves over time is difficult, and youth may misinterpret difficulty as
(Oyserman, Johnson, & Bybee, 2006). These future or possible evidence that the PS is not a reasonable goal and should be
selves (PSs) are positive and negative images of the self already in abandoned. To predict when a particular PS is likely to motivate
a future state—the “clever” self who passed the algebra test, the action, is it necessary to have a model that predicts which PSs are
“fat” self who failed to lose weight, the “fast” self who fell in with likely to be online in working memory and, of these PSs, which
the “wrong” crowd (Oyserman & Markus, 1990). A number of will be invested in over time.
self-regulatory models have posited positive and negative conse- In the current paper, we focus on school-related PSs and outline
quences of PS–PS motivate current action (Baumeister, 1998; and test a predictive model. We propose that youth have difficulty
Carver & Scheier, 1990; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Higgins, 1996a; creating and sustaining school-focused PSs when they perceive
Oyserman & Markus, 1990; Weinstein, 1993). Failure to attain PSs these PSs to be incongruent with important social identities (e.g.,
(Oyserman & Fryberg, 2006), hoped for selves, or ideal selves racial-ethnic identities), misinterpret difficulties in working on
(Strauman, 2002; Strauman & Higgins, 1988) may increase risk of these PSs as evidence that academic goals are unrealistic PSs, and
depression. There is some evidence that imagining successful PSs live in social contexts that fail to cue strategies for attaining their
improves well-being (King, 2001) and performance (Ruvolo & PS goal. Youth will commit sustained self-regulatory effort to a PS
Markus, 1992). However, PSs do not always sustain self- when the PS itself is effective and contains behavioral strategies
regulatory action. Youth fail algebra, fall off weight maintenance, and social context supports working on the PS, when the PS feels
and engage in risky behavior, perhaps also increasing their risk of congruent with important social identities, and when difficulty
feeling depressed at their inability to move toward their self-goals. working on the PS is construed as normative. In the following
Why might PSs fail to sustain self-regulatory action? Given that sections, we outline each of these links and the intervention we
the self is multidimensional and includes multiple potentially developed based on this model with a goal of enhancing the
competing goals (e.g., Abrams, 1994; Burke, 2003; King & Smith, self-regulatory impact of PS.
2004; Oyserman, 2001; Settles, 2004), a particular PS may fail to
sustain self-regulatory action because it conflicts with other parts PSs and Self-Regulatory Behavior
Effective PSs
Daphna Oyserman, Deborah Bybee, and Kathy Terry, The Institute for A number of studies suggest that PSs differ in self-regulatory
Social Research, The University of Michigan. effectiveness. Self-regulatory effort improves when youth have
This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant both positive PSs (goals) and negative PSs (fears) in the same
R01 MH 58299. We thank the students, parents, trainers, teachers, school domain (“balanced” PSs) (Oyserman & Markus, 1990) and when
staff, and community members involved in the intervention, data collec-
youth have incorporated detailed strategies into their PSs (“plau-
tion, and tracking; Jim Klein for help in obtaining school data; and our
coordinating and database managers, Carol Carlin, Johnessa Dimicks,
sible” PSs) (Oyserman, Bybee, Terry, & Hart-Johnson, 2004).
Tami Hart-Johnson, and Angelique Lange. When PSs are balanced, individuals select strategies that both
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Daphna increase the likelihood of becoming like the positive PSs and
Oyserman, The Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan, decrease the likelihood of becoming like the negative PSs, thereby
Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1248. E-mail: daphna.oyserman@umich.edu focusing self-regulation and broadening effort (Oyserman &

188
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 189

Markus, 1990). Plausibility provides automatic cuing of predevel- The negative link youths perceive between minority status and
oped strategies (e.g., “set my alarm,” “go to class even if my academic attainment and eventual occupation can be seen both in
friends skip”); indeed, youth with plausible academic PSs (APSs) ethnographic descriptions and in experimental paradigms. Ethno-
are able to improve their grades over the course of the school year, graphic research suggests that high school students perceive Lati-
whereas those who simply had APSs are not (Oyserman et al., nos as more likely to become manual laborers, Asians to do well
2004). Despite the fact that most low-income youth have at least in school, and African Americans to do poorly in school (Kao,
one PS focused on school, few of these PSs include strategies 2000). The same results emerge from scenario-based experimental
(Oyserman et al., 2006). Many youths fail to attain even the basic research, whether focused on the link between minority status and
academic PS goal of graduating high school. Thus, the national low academic attainment or on the link between low social class
average for on-time graduation is 75% overall, 50% for African and low academic attainment. Thus, when a failing student is
Americans, and 53% for Hispanics) (Orfield, Losen, Wald, & described, Latino and African American students are more likely
Swanson, 2004). In urban centers like Detroit, graduation esti- to predict that the target is Latino or African American than White
mates are even lower (between 40% and 44% on-time graduation) (Graham, 2001). When asked to predict academic performance of
(Detroit News, 2005). a target student, low-income students infer worse performance
from low (vs. middle) social class peers (Régner, Huguet, &
Monteil, 2002; Weinger, 2000).
Contextual Cuing of PSs When imagining what future is possible for one’s self, such
Not only is the potential self-regulatory impact of having a PS negative preformed group images are likely to be highly accessi-
goal undermined when PSs are not balanced or plausible, self- ble, making social group membership feel like it conflicts with
regulatory effectiveness is also undermined when social contexts APSs. Working toward one’s APS is likely to feel harder in the
do not cue the PS. Because information that is cued (chronically or presence of accessible images of in-group members engaging in
situationally made salient) is likely to be used in judgments and behaviors that undermine chances of attaining APSs and failing to
decision-making (Higgins, 1996b), contextually cued PSs should engage in behaviors that would help attain APSs. Although there
influence self-regulatory behavior more than those that are not is debate as to the degree of evidence that school success is viewed
cued. Moreover, to sustain ongoing engagement in school, PSs as a White middle-class goal (Cook & Ludwig, 1997; Ferguson,
must be linked with behavioral strategies; positive expected APSs 1998), there is consistent evidence of the stereotyped link between
need to be linked with strategies to attain them, and feared APSs minority status and low achievement (e.g., Steele, 1997).
need to be linked with strategies to avoid them. The stereotype threat literature documents that simply bringing
Although resource-rich contexts such as a middle-class neigh- to mind category membership as minority or working class damp-
borhood and school provide models of success and a developed ens academic performance (e.g., Croizet & Claire, 1998; Steele,
structure to guide the process of attaining APSs, this is unlikely to 1997). Individuation or separation of self from in-group alleviates
be the case in resource-limited contexts. In middle-class contexts, this effect (Ambady, Paik, Steele, Owen-Smith, & Mitchell, 2004),
strategies may be automatically cued; parents, teachers, and par- but the idea of intervening to disconnect youth from their racial-
ents of friends all converge to emphasize homework, persistence in ethnic in-group (e.g., creating “racelessness”; see Fordham &
the face of difficulty, tutoring, or staying after school if needed. In Ogbu, 1986) is unappealing and likely to have other negative
low-socioeconomic status (SES) contexts, strategies are unlikely to consequences (Arroyo & Zigler, 1995). Thus, rather than attempt-
be automatically cued because these contexts are less likely to ing to dampen the centrality of important social identities as a way
present easily accessible models or to guide success (e.g., Rod- to improve success in attaining APSs, increasing felt congruence
erick, 2003). In low-SES contexts, youth encounter adults who are between APSs and social identity, is a more reasonable strategy.
likely to be unemployed, have low academic attainment, and hold
nonprofessional jobs. Given lack of easily accessible models or
APSs and Inoculation From Overinterpretation of
automatically cued strategies, youth may maintain an abstract
commitment to education without connecting these PSs to every-
Difficulty
day behavior, expressing high aspirations even as their behavior
In addition to the linkage of the APS to self-regulatory behavior
reflects avoidance or even flight from school (for qualitative
and integration with important social identities, taking into account
description, see Roderick, 2003).
research on how salient information influences judgment and
behavior may close what appears to be a puzzling gap between the
APSs and Social Identities high value placed on education and the actual attainment of low-
income and minority youth. Working on one’s APS is likely to be
Self-concepts include both personal and social identities. Social difficult. To judge what this difficulty means, whether APSs are
identities are aspects of self-concept based not in individual traits “true” PSs or contradict other important social identities, to judge
and goals but on group-based traits and goals (Oyserman, in press). whether attaining these PSs is plausible or not worth the effort,
It seems reasonable that social identities will incorporate commu- teens must answer the implied questions “Why is engaging in this
nity expectations about the occupations and academic attainment APS so hard for me; is this really the true me? Do we have PSs like
of in-group members. Minority youth living in low-SES contexts are this?” Likewise, they must judge whether particular behavioral
exposed to images of the in-group as low achieving (Thomas, patterns (e.g., asking for help) are likely to work and if they contradict
Townsend, & Belgrave, 2003), raising the possibility that the selves in-group identity (e.g., “Will asking the teacher for help actually help
possible for in-group members may not feel congruent with APSs. me succeed in school or is it just a ‘White’ thing to do?”).
190 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

To better understand how the feelings of difficulty that are tive part of the process (e.g., “success is 1% inspiration and 99%
experienced as adolescents imagine and pursue their PS influence perspiration”). Difficulty can also provide evidence of progress
commitment to that PS, it is fruitful to reconsider James’ (1890) (e.g., “the important things in life are the ones you really have to
original formulation of the self. James viewed the self as com- work for”); if difficulty and failures along the way are viewed as
prised both of content—what one thinks about when one thinks critical to eventual success, then difficulty is evidence of striving.
about one’s self and the accompanying metacognitive process— Successful movement toward positive APSs and away from
the feeling of thinking about one’s self. This implies that self- feared APSs requires ongoing behavior; it is not enough to com-
judgments about who one is or may become are based on both plete one homework assignment or stay after class one day. If
content (what comes to mind) and process. Considering the content one’s metacognitive experience is that working on a PS is difficult
of thoughts about the self separately from the feelings associated and if this difficulty is interpreted with a naı̈ve theory that ease is
with these thoughts parallels work in social cognition (Schwarz, associated with truth, then difficulties associated with working
1998, 2002). This work proposes that human reasoning is accom- toward the PS will undermine it. As we have outlined, low-income
panied by metacognitive experiences of relative ease (difficulty) and minority youth are likely to experience at least three sources of
and fluency (disfluency) (Schwarz, 1998, 2002). difficulty— difficulty bringing to mind APSs and linking them to
Following from this research in social cognition, when imagin- strategies, difficulty sustaining the behavioral self-regulation PS
ing a PS is accompanied by a metacognitive experience of diffi- strategies entail, and difficulty integrating APSs and social
culty, the feeling of difficulty is interpreted with a naı̈ve theory— identities.
things that are hard to think of are less likely to be true (Higgins, Youth growing up in low-SES contexts have multiple models of
1998; Schwarz & Bless, 1992; Schwarz & Clore, 1996). The adults who failed to attain their PSs, making it unlikely that they
experience of ease or difficulty when bringing to mind a PS can will recognize the normativeness of difficulties and instead likely
provide the basis for inferring whether a PS is a “true” self that is that they will misinterpret feelings of difficulty as a sign of
worth pursuing and investing effort in or a “false” self, conflicting inevitable failure. This misinterpretation is crucial because it is
with social identities. Metacognitive experience of ease also pro- likely to undermine behavioral persistence in pursuit of PS goals.
vides feedback as to whether the gap between the current and PS Thus, rather than assuming that youth are able to make sense of
is manageable or unmanageable and therefore whether the PS difficulty as normative, low-SES youth are likely to need specific
should be expanded or abandoned. inoculation from overinterpreting current difficulty and failure as
Although the experience of metacognitive difficulty is generally predictive of future possibilities. Taken together, their metacogni-
interpreted as meaning “not true for me,” a number of studies have tive experience of difficulty is likely to provide feedback that
documented that other interpretations are possible (Rothman & APSs are false rather than true selves, cuing disengagement from
Schwarz, 1998). Sports stories abound with reinterpretation of the these PSs and the goal pursuit they imply.
meaning of experienced difficulty (e.g., “no pain, no gain”) and the
need to keep trying (e.g., “you miss 100% of the shots you don’t PSs: A Process Model and Proposed Intervention
take”). In the case of attempting to attain APS, although the
metacognitive experience of difficulty is generally interpreted as Figure 1 presents our process model of the connections among
“not the true me,” the experience of difficulty could be reinter- PSs, self-regulatory behaviors, academic outcomes, and mental
preted to mean other things. Difficulty can be viewed as a norma- health. The process model links social identities and metacognitive

Social Academic
Identity Outcomes

STJ Possible Self-Regulatory


Intervention Selves Behaviors

Meta-cognitive Well-being
Experience Mental Health

Figure 1. Process model. Theorized effects of the STJ intervention on PSs and of PSs on academic engage-
ment, academic outcomes, and depression.
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 191

experience to PSs and links PSs to persistent engagement in Sessions 8 to 10 focused on decoupling difficulty and genuine-
self-regulatory behavior. To test this model, we developed an ness explicitly through work in smaller groups on everyday prob-
intervention (labeled STJ in Figure 1). The goals of the interven- lems, social problems, academic problems, and the process of
tion were to evoke PSs and strategies to attain them, forge links getting to high school graduation, and the metatheme was that all
between PSs and strategies that are not otherwise automatic, in- students care about these issues, that difficulties are normative and
oculate youth from misinterpreting failure and setbacks in attain- not self-defining. Session 11 cemented the new metacognitive
ing these PSs, and create a link between social identity and PSs. interpretations by having participants review and critique the ses-
Ease is typically the basis for metacognitive judgment, so we sions. Two follow-up sessions included parent/guardians and com-
took care to ensure that sessions felt easy, so that students would munity members, with the goal of helping youth broker their APSs
not immediately develop a metacognitive sense that thinking of the in the community outside of school in ways that link APSs with
future is hard and therefore “not for me.” We based the interven- social identities.
tion in school so that strategies articulated in the intervention
would be cued in school. We structured intervention activities to Hypotheses
make APSs salient, create linking connections between APSs and
strategies, make salient naı̈ve theories of the meaning of difficulty Two key hypotheses follow from our model. (a) The interven-
that link difficulty with progress toward meaningful goals, and tion will directly influence PSs, self-regulatory behaviors, aca-
create a space in which APSs and social identity are congruent. demic outcomes, and risk of depression. (b) Effects of the inter-
Together, these activities were designed to “inoculate” youth vention on self-regulatory behaviors, academic outcomes, and
from withdrawing effort to attain positive APSs and avoid feared depression will be mediated by intervention effects on PSs. That is,
APSs. In the long term, this change in PSs was expected to evoke shift in PSs will improve engagement in self-regulatory behaviors,
persistent change in self-regulatory behavior. That is, we expected and engagement in self-regulatory behaviors will increase aca-
that behavior relevant to APSs (e.g., doing homework) would demic success and reduce feelings of depression. We also hypoth-
increase and that behavior undermining APSs (e.g., misbehaving esized (c) that the intervention would influence the relationship
in class) would decrease. Over time, sustained self-regulatory between APSs and social identity, creating conditions for APSs
behavior should produce better academic outcomes, that is, grade and social identity to feel congruent. To test these hypotheses, we
point average (GPA). Moreover, because sustained self-regulatory examined the direct effects of the intervention on change in PSs
behavior should reduce discrepancy between current and future and the longitudinal effects of the intervention, both direct and as
selves, intervention youth should have the added benefit of re- mediated by PSs. Finally, we examined the long-term effects of the
duced risk of depression (Higgins, 1997) and other mental health intervention on the relationship between APSs and social identity
problems. as operationalized by racial-ethnic identity.
Session 1 focused on making APSs salient and relevant as well
as relevant to social identity: Each group member introduced a Method
partner in terms of the skills or ability the partner possessed that
would help him or her complete the school year successfully (e.g., Sample
“well-organized,” “positive attitude”). This provided an initial Data were collected at three Detroit middle schools; 71.6% of students
example of APSs and social identity as congruent; because all were African American, 17.4% were Latino, and 11.0% were White; total
youth engaged in the task, the metamessage was “we all care about N for analyses ⫽ 264 as detailed below. Students were low-income, as
school.” Session 2 focused on adult PSs: Youth picked photo- evidenced by school lunch program (two-thirds received free or reduced
graphs that fit their adult “visions” (PSs). Because most adult PSs lunch), neighborhood (in the students’ census tracts, n ⫽ 105, 54.1% of
youth describe are images of material success, the metamessage households were below the poverty line and only 43.4% of adults were
was “we all want a good future.” Because all youth choose employed; U.S. Bureau of Census, 2000), housing stability (45.2% moved
photographs of adult PSs, successful PSs and social identity are at least once over the 2 years we tracked them; those who moved averaged
1.54 moves), and school stability (20% changed schools during the 1st
presented as congruent.
year).
In Session 3, students drew role models and negative forces—
people or things that provide energy to work toward PSs and those
that are draining or nay-saying. The metamessage was “everyone Procedure
faces obstacles and difficulties; this does not make the PSs less Obtaining permission. School district and school principals approved
part of the ‘true’ self.” In Session 4, students drew timelines into random assignment and provision of the intervention during the elective
the future, including forks in the road and obstacles. The metames- hour. School-based data collection in low-income and minority populations
sage was “everyone has difficulties, and failures and setbacks are typically is hampered by low consent rates (Ellickson & Hawes, 1989).
a normal part of timelines and do not mean that APSs are not true Parents who are lower income and have more academically at-risk students
selves.” are less likely to return consent forms, reducing generalizability of findings
Session 5 introduced action goals, allowing students to practice (Kearney, Hopkins, Mauss, & Weisheit, 1983; Pokorny, Jason, Schoeny,
Townsend, & Curie, 2001). Because not returning a consent form mailed
articulating specific strategies to attain their APSs, further high-
home (the standard protocol) is not the same as refusing participation when
lighting the normativeness of difficulty in attaining APSs. Sessions asked directly, these authors argue for passive rather than signed consent to
6 and 7 focused on PSs and strategies to attain them, building on protect generalizability of findings. We also obtained the typical low
previous sessions, with a different concrete medium, poster board, response rate with the standard mailing procedure (15% return rate). Rather
stickers, and markers. Students chose next year feared and to-be- than limit generalizability, we followed up the 85% nonresponders with
expected PSs and linked them with current and possible strategies. personal contact (by phone if they had one or by going to the home) to
192 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

explain the study and what we would do with the data and to ask for PSs. PSs were assessed in 8th grade and spring of 9th grade using the
permission or refusal. Together, the initial mailing and direct communica- standard open-ended format (available at http://www.sitemaker.umich.edu/
tion were well received, resulting in a total signed consent rate of 94.3% culture.self/files/possible_selves_measure.doc, 07–22-2003, Oyserman &
and no differences by race/ethnicity or condition. Saltz, 1993; Oyserman, Terry, & Bybee, 2002). Youth generated expected
Data collection. Core subject teachers (blind to experimental assign- PSs, noted strategies to attain each PS, and repeated this process for feared
ment) assessed student in-class behavior. Test scores, GPA, and attendance PSs. PSs were content coded following Oyserman and Markus (1990) by
were obtained from school records. Student surveys were administered five coders with access only to PS responses. All PSs were double coded,
in-class. Teacher and student baseline data collection occurred prior to the and interrater reliability was 94% (disagreements discussed to agreement).
intervention. Teacher and student postintervention data were collected at Analyses focused on the two most commonly generated PSs, academic PSs
three additional times, at the end of the 8th grade school year and again in and feared off-track PSs. At baseline, 96.6% of youth generated an ex-
the fall and spring of the following year. School records were obtained each pected APS, 63.6% generated a feared APS, and 51.1% generated a feared
time report cards were issued over the 2-year data collection period (1st off-track PSs.
quarter of 8th grade through the end of 9th grade). Because the intervention Feared off-track PSs. These PSs focused on involvement in gangs or
occurred prior to 1st quarter 8th grade grades, the youth baseline survey violence (30% of responses), drugs (30%), delinquency or involvement
included self-report of grades to provide a prior academic attainment with the police (30%), and becoming pregnant and other “status” offenses
control variable. Teachers were not present for student surveys to assure (10%), fall 8th grade, M ⫽ 0.74, SD ⫽ .90; spring 8th grade, M ⫽ .85,
confidentiality. Instructions were read aloud, and research staff circulated SD ⫽ .83; spring 9th grade, M ⫽ 1.41, SD ⫽ 1.04. Raw counts were
to assist students. In-home interviews were completed in year 1 for the 56 skewed so they were transformed into change scores (spring-fall), improv-
students suspended (n ⫽ 11), expelled (n ⫽ 9), transferred (n ⫽ 10), or ing distribution and focusing on the impact of the intervention on change
otherwise not in school (n ⫽ 26). By year 2, students were enrolled in 80 in PSs during 8th grade; change scores are appropriate when groups are
schools; the at-home interview procedure was followed when in-school randomly assigned and variances similar across observations (Cribbie &
survey completion was not feasible. Teachers were reimbursed $5 for each Jamieson, 2000; Maris, 1998). Change in feared off-track PSs ranged from
assessment ($10 in year 2); students were reimbursed $5 (year 2 only). ⫺3 to ⫹ 3, M ⫽ 0.11, SD ⫽ 1.04.
Missing data. Extensive tracking efforts minimized data loss. We APS balance. Balance (the number of pairs of expected and feared
obtained data from at least one source across all four semesters of data APSs, e.g., expecting to “pass the 8th grade” while wanting to avoid
collection for a full 96% of our sample, 89.8% to 97.3% of student, 92.4% “failing and having to be an 8th grader again”) was coded following
to 95.8% of school records, and 83.3% to 96.6% of teacher ratings. All but Oyserman and Markus (1990). About one-third of youth had at least one
4 youth had data from at least one source at semester 4. To preserve balanced APS pair, fall 8th grade, M ⫽ 0.39, SD ⫽ .56; spring 8th grade,
generalizability, expectation maximization methods were used to estimate M ⫽ .40, SD ⫽ .57; spring 9th grade, M ⫽ .30, SD ⫽ .59. Raw counts were
the approximately 7.9% of the data matrix that were missing due to skipped skewed so they were transformed into change scores (spring-fall), improv-
items or unavailable information. Although missingness was not com- ing distribution, and focusing on the impact of the intervention on change
pletely random, Little’s missing completely at random ␹2(df ⫽ 16,115) ⫽ in PSs during 8th grade; change in APS balance ranged from ⫺2 to ⫹ 3,
30,685.46, p ⬍ .001, there was no evidence that it was not ignorable (Little M ⫽ 0.01, SD ⫽ 0.69. For moderated regression analysis (Hypothesis 3),
& Rubin, 2002). All results reported use estimated data. 9th grade APS balance scores were log-transformed (raw spring 9th grade
Experimental manipulation. In each homeroom, approximately one- scores had skew of 2.5 and kurtosis of 8.47), reducing skew and kurtosis
half of students were randomly assigned to attend the regular elective to ⬍1.5.
period (control group), and one-half were assigned to attend the interven- APS plausibility. The set of expected and feared APSs and strategies
tion as the elective period (experimental group). We called the intervention was coded using a 6-point scale (0 ⫽ no APSs to 5 ⫽ at least 4 APSs and
School-to-Jobs (STJ) and the overall project Pathways for Youth to prevent APS strategies) (Oyserman et al., 2004). Youth averaged about two APSs
any potential stigmatization. Students who enrolled in school late (n ⫽ 3) and strategies, fall 8th grade, M ⫽ 2.26, SD ⫽ 1.30; spring 8th grade, M ⫽
or had an elective period broken by lunch (n ⫽ 34) were not randomized, 2.08, SD ⫽ 1.25; spring 9th grade, M ⫽ 2.18, SD ⫽ 1.26. APS plausibility
resulting in an effective cohort of 280. Analyses involve the 264 students was not skewed, so log-transformation was not necessary, and intervention
with signed parental consent (n ⫽ 141 experimental, n ⫽ 123 control; n ⫽ effects were assessed by examining plausibility in spring 8th grade. The
140 female; n ⫽ 191 African American, n ⫽ 45 Latino, n ⫽ 28 White). The coding manual and detailed instructions are found on http://sitemaker
STJ intervention thumbnail sketched in the introduction is fully manualized .umich.edu/daphna.oyserman/files/plausibility_instructions_only012903.doc.
and includes a detailed fidelity assessment protocol (both available from
the first author). STJ was provided twice weekly over a 7-week period (due
to elective periods missed for school half days) with the two supplemental Social Identity
parent-youth STJ sessions provided the following evenings or weekend.
Social identity was operationalized with a 4-item, 5-point Likert re-
Groups averaged 12 participants. Of youth in school, attendance was 80%
sponse (1 ⫽ strongly disagree, 5 ⫽ strongly agree) in-group connectedness
to 90% for the youth in-school sessions, and 40% attended at least one
scale, spring 9th grade, M ⫽ 4.18, SD ⫽ .59, ␣ ⫽ .76. Students filled in
parent-youth supplemental session. Two trainers ran each group. Fidelity to
their main racial-ethnic group and then responded. For the group “African
protocol was maintained via in vivo rating and weekly staff meetings.
American,” example items are “I feel close to African Americans,” “It is
Trainers and observers were female Detroiters with previous experience
important to me to think of myself as an African American,” and “I feel a
who received intensive structured training in the protocol (for trainers, 39.5
part of the African American community.” Analysis of connectedness to
hours; for observers, 69.5 hours).
racial-ethnic in-group excluded the n ⫽ 28 White students because “White
Verification of random assignment. Randomization was successful.
identity” is unlikely to have the same meaning as minority racial-ethnic
Comparison of the experimental and control groups on the 13 baseline
identity.
measures showed no significant differences on the multivariate, F(13,
250) ⫽ 1.07, p ⫽ .39, or on any of the univariate Fs.
Self-Regulatory Behavior
Measures
Time spent doing homework. In 8th grade, we asked “How many hours
Demographics. Schools provided gender and birthdates; youths pro- a week do you usually spend doing homework?” Open ended responses
vided their race/ethnic description. ranged from 0 to 20 hours and were log-transformed to reduce skew. On an
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 193

8-point closed ended follow-up question (0 ⫽ no time to 7 ⫽ more than 10 tion of youth meeting the clinical threshold for depression (sum scores of
hours), fall M ⫽ 3.89 (SD ⫽ 1.77) and spring M ⫽ 3.89 (SD ⫽ 1.95), mean at least 16) (Radloff & Locke, 2000) was 22.7%, comparable with the
responses reflected about 2 hours a week. In 9th grade, we used a 1-week proportion of youth reaching the threshold for depression in national
event history calendar (Belli, 1998); again, students reported about 2 hours surveys (Costello, Mustillo, & Erkanli, 2003; Roberts, Attkisson, & Rosen-
of weekly homework, fall M ⫽ 160.20 minutes, SD ⫽ 151.89; spring M ⫽ blatt, 1998).
112.15 minutes, SD ⫽ 111.28. Homework time was low; as a benchmark,
national daily diary report data for 12 to 14 year olds are 4.40 hours weekly
(Juster, Ono, & Stafford, 2004). Results
Disruptive behavior. Eighth grade youth reported “How often does the
teacher make you leave the classroom because of your behavior?” on a We first examined the direct effect of the STJ intervention on
7-point scale (1 ⫽ never, 2 ⫽ once or twice a year, 3 ⫽ less than once a PSs, self-regulatory behaviors, academic outcomes, and depres-
month, 4 ⫽ once a month or so, 5 ⫽ once every few weeks, 6 ⫽ once a sion. Then, we examined PSs as mediators of intervention effects
week or so, 7 ⫽ more than once a week), fall M ⫽ 2.18, SD ⫽ 1.55; spring on self-regulatory behaviors and, via their impact on self-
M ⫽ 2.64, SD ⫽ 1.94. Teachers reported on youth behavior with a 4-item
regulatory behavior, as mediators of intervention effects on aca-
5-point response (1 ⫽ never, 5 ⫽ always) (Finn Disruptive Behavior Scale,
demic outcomes and depression. We also examined whether the
Finn, Pannozzo, & Voelkl, 1995; 8th grade revision, J. Finn, personal
communication, October 14, 1998) scale each semester, ␣ ⫽ 79, .81, .78, STJ intervention influenced the association between APSs and
and .80, respectively. Beginning with the stem “This student . . . ,” items social identity. Prior to describing these analyses and results, we
were “annoys peers or interferes with peers’ work,” “is critical of peers outline our decision to present analyses of both the “intention-to-
who do well in school,” “needs to be reprimanded or sent to the office,” treat” (ITT) and compliant sample analyses.
and “is verbally or physically abusive to the teacher.”
Initiative-taking behavior. Teachers reported on youth behavior with a
4-item 5-point response (1 ⫽ never, 5 ⫽ always) (Finn Initiative Scale, ITT and Compliant Sample Analyses
Finn et al., 1995; 8th grade revision, J. Finn, personal communication,
October 14, 1998) scale each semester, ␣ ⫽ .75, .82, .82, and .85, Following standard procedure, direct effects analyses (Hypoth-
respectively. Beginning with the stem “How often does this student . . . ,” esis 1) were conducted twice, once with the ITT sample and a
items were “do more than the work assigned?,” “persist when confronted second time with the compliant sample. Analyses of meditating
with difficult problems?,” “actively participate in class discussions?,” and effects of PSs (Hypothesis 2) and of the impact of the intervention
“engage me in conversation about subject matter before or after school or
on the relationship between APSs and social identity (Hypothesis
outside of class?”
3) utilized the compliant sample. The ITT sample included all 264
Absences. Eighth grade youth reported “How often are you absent
from school or miss a class during the day?” on a 7-point scale (1 ⫽ never, youth randomized into experimental or control conditions, whether
2 ⫽ once or twice a year, 3 ⫽ less than once a month, 4 ⫽ once a month they were in school to receive the intervention or not.
or so, 5 ⫽ once every few weeks, 6 ⫽ once a week or so, 7 ⫽ more than Although analysis based on an ITT sample preserves random
once a week), fall M ⫽ 3.17, SD ⫽ 1.67; spring M ⫽ 3.82, SD ⫽ 1.79. As assignment, it is not a good test of the intervention effects to the
a second measure of absences, a count of unexcused absences was obtained extent that some people who did not receive the intervention are
from school records in 8th and 9th grades (log transformed to reduce included in the treatment group for analyses. The alternative is to
skew). Each day not enrolled in any school was counted as an absence. compare those who received a meaningful dose of the intervention
with those who did not. This is termed compliance analyses. Use
Academic Outcomes of compliance analyses to estimate effects for individuals who
received a meaningful dose of an intervention is appropriate if
GPA. School record of core (math, history, science, English) grades
assessed variables provide a useful and valid estimate of those
(0.0 ⫽ F to 4.0 ⫽ A) was obtained; 0.0 was assigned as GPA for students
likely to participate and if nonparticipators among those assigned
not enrolled in any school during the semester. As a baseline measure of
grades prior to the intervention, we obtained youth report of prior grades. to the experimental condition do not differ in outcomes from
Three additional academic outcome measures were obtained at the end control condition youth (Jo, 2002a, 2002b). In the current study,
of 8th grade: (a) proportion of subject tests passed on the 8th grade both requirements were met, as outlined in more detail at http://
Essential Skills Attainment Test (based on the Michigan Educational As- www.sitemaker.umich.edu/culture.self/files/ appendix_a_on_web_
sessment Program (MEAP) test), M ⫽ .80, SD ⫽ .25, range ⫽ 0.0 to 1.0), site.doc, 26.0 kb, the compliant sample (n ⫽ 228) included the
(b) referral to remedial summer school as a prerequisite to high school “participating” youth (n ⫽ 116) and a comparable sample of
promotion (47% of youth), and (c) retained in 8th grade according to control youth (n ⫽ 112). Participating youth were assigned to the
school records by the end of the summer (9.5% of students). These experimental condition and received a reasonable dose of the
measures were not available in 9th grade (standardized tests are not
intervention (i.e., attended at least five sessions). The comparable
administered each year); therefore, 9th grade academic outcomes analyses
focus on GPA only.
sample of control youth (n ⫽ 112) were youth who had a similar
unobserved likelihood of “participating” had they been random-
ized to intervention according to the expectation maximization
Depression compliance model incorporating the full set of preintervention
variables (Little & Yau, 1998). Most of the difference in sample
Youth report of depression, the standard 20-item, 4-point Center for
Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Radloff, 1977), anchored at
size between the ITT and compliant analyses was due to youth
0 ⫽ not at all or less than one day in the past week and 3 ⫽ 5 to 7 days being suspended or expelled within the 1st month of school; some
in the past week, was obtained spring of 9th grade (M ⫽ 11.35, SD ⫽ 8.33). youth simply were not in school to receive the intervention. Effects
Items include affective (e.g., “I felt depressed”) and somatic (e.g., “I did presented are from the compliant (participating) sample unless
not feel like eating; my appetite was poor”) aspects of depression. Propor- otherwise specified.
194 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Hypothesis 1: STJ Intervention Has Direct Effects on PSs, slope and intercept variances of the unconditional model (i.e., the
Self-Regulatory Behavior, and Outcomes model with no predictors other than time and homeroom) for the
longitudinal MLM. Standardized effect sizes are interpreted like
Analysis Plan Cohen’s d.
We used multilevel modeling (MLM; Raudenbush & Bryk,
2002; Snijders & Bosker, 1999) to appropriately analyze variance STJ Effects by End of 8th Grade
at three levels—time nested within students and students nested
within homerooms. We used standard methods for MLM, first Effects of STJ for the total ITT sample and the compliant
building baseline models containing random effects for homeroom (participating) sample are presented in Table 1. For each depen-
and time and then adding condition assignment and baseline con- dent variable, the raw coefficient is interpreted as a regression
trol variables to optimize statistical power by accounting for pre- weight and indicates, in the metric of the dependent variable, the
intervention variability in sex, race, age, baseline GPA (student average within-homeroom difference between students assigned to
report), baseline behavioral problems (teacher report), and baseline STJ rather than the control group. The standardized effect size is
assessment of each dependent measure. For ease of interpretation, interpreted as Cohen’s d and expresses the same difference in SD
control variables (other than baseline assessment of the dependent units. To further facilitate interpretation of intervention effects, esti-
variable) were constant across analyses. We show direct effects at mated means for spring 8th grade are in the first columns of Table 2.
the end of 8th grade and over time (through the end of 9th grade). We summarize findings by dependent variable in the sections below.
For the longitudinal models predicting change in self-regulatory PSs. Following Hypothesis 1, both ITT and compliant sample
behavior and academic outcomes, we set the intercept for time at analyses showed significant intervention effects on PSs in the
spring of 9th grade to obtain estimates of between-condition ef- spring of 8th grade. Experimental youth generated more balanced
fects at the final measurement point. We used Raudenbush and APSs, more plausible APSs, and more feared off-track PSs, setting
Liu’s (2000, 2001) methods to compute standardized effect sizes; the stage for self-regulation.
these effects are calibrated to the residual level 1 variance of the Self-regulatory behavior. As can be seen in the negative co-
unconditional model (i.e., the model containing no predictors other efficients connoting fewer absences, participation in the STJ in-
than homeroom) for 8th Grade MLM analyses and to the random tervention significantly reduced absences, whether assessed via

Table 1
Multilevel Models: Effects of the STJ Intervention in Spring 8th Grade

Total ITT sample Participating samplea


(N ⫽ 264) (N ⫽ 228)

Raw coefficients Raw coefficients


Standardized
Dependent variables B p B p effect sizeb

Change in APS balance .157 .023 .186 .014 .269


Change in feared off-track PS .286 .004 .308 .003 .302
APS plausibility .340 .015 .400 .008 .329
Absences (student report) ⫺.417 .043 ⫺.457 .036 ⫺.267
Unexcused absences (school records)c ⫺.183 .049 ⫺.202 .022 ⫺.726
Homework time (hrs/week; open ended)c .081 .090 .099 .135 .155
Homework time (hrs/week; closed ended) .263 .229 .406 .079 .240
Disruptive behavior (student report) ⫺.307 .165 ⫺.380 .094 ⫺.209
Disruptive behavior (teacher report) ⫺.099 .261 ⫺.139 .149 ⫺.172
In-class initiative (teacher report) .179 .043 .142 .142 .170
Core academic GPA (school records) .229 .021 .218 .039 .252
Standardized tests (school records; proportion passed) .068 .017 .086 .006 .360
Referral to remedial summer schoold ⫺.194 .457 ⫺.419 .140
Retention in 8th graded ⫺.598 .164 ⫺.979 .076

Note. The cross-sectional multilevel models analyzed variance at two levels: students (Level 1), nested within
homerooms (Level 2). All Level 1 intercepts were random, and all modeled fixed effects were at Level 1
(student). Models controlled for the following Level 1 (student) covariates: race, sex, baseline age, preinter-
vention GPA (student-report), preintervention behavioral problems (teacher report), and preintervention assess-
ment of the dependent variable, when available.
a
The participating sample were 116 students who attended at least five in-school STJ sessions plus 112 students
in the control group who had similar likelihood of attending, had they been randomly assigned to the
intervention, according to maximum likelihood (EM) modeling of intervention compliance. b Interpretation is
similar to d for STJ intervention effects on individuals within homerooms. ES ⫽ B/V⫺1 p , where B ⫽ raw
coefficient and Vp ⫽ residual variance in the unconditional intercept model, following Raudenbush & Liu
c
(2001). These positively skewed dependent variables were log-transformed to reduce the influence of
extreme scores. d Multilevel models for these dichotomous dependent variables used a logit link function.
Standardized effect sizes could not be computed for logit models.
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 195

Table 2
Multilevel Models: Estimated Means in Experimental and Control Conditions: Spring 8th and
9th Grades

Estimated condition means/percentages

Spring of 8th grade Spring of 9th grade

STJ Control STJ Control


Dependent variables (n ⫽ 116) (n ⫽ 112) (n ⫽ 116) (n ⫽ 112)

Change in APS balance 0.07 ⫺0.12


Change in feared off-track PS 0.31 0.00
APS plausibility 2.36 1.96
Absences (student report) 3.49 3.95
Unexcused absences (school records)b 12.28 14.53 22.52 24.77
Homework time (hours/week; open ended)b 3.49 3.28 2.51 1.57
Homework time (hours/week; closed ended) 4.15 3.74
Disruptive behavior (student report) 2.37 2.63
Disruptive behavior (teacher report) 1.74 1.83 1.55 1.73
In-class initiative (teacher report) 2.51 2.44 2.48 2.25
Core academic GPA (school records) 1.98 1.83 1.64 1.36
Standardized tests (school records; proportion passed) 0.83 0.77
Referral to remedial summer school 36.2% 48.2%
Retention in 8th grade 4.3% 10.7%
Depression (CESD—spring of 9th grade)c 10.35 12.29

Note. N ⫽ 228 youth in the participating sample.


a
For variables with estimated means for both 8th and 9th grades, estimates were from the intercept term of a
three-level longitudinal multilevel model (time, students, homerooms); for variables with estimated means for
only one grade, estimates were from cross-sectional two level multilevel models (students in homerooms).
b
These positively skewed variables were log transformed for analysis; however, to facilitate interpretation,
means are presented here in the original metric (i.e., in exponentiated form). c Depression was measured only
in the spring of 9th grade; means were estimated by a two-level cross-sectional multilevel model.

school or youth report, in both ITT and compliant samples. For less disruptive, and less likely to skip class than control group
school-reported absences, the effect of the intervention is in Co- youth. As displayed in Figure 2a, time spent on homework de-
hen’s large range, with d near .80. Each semester intervention clined for all youth, but the decline was significantly less for STJ
youth attended more school, over 2 additional days, than control youth. By spring of 9th grade, STJ youth were spending on
youth. With regard to time spent in homework and behavior in average 2.51 hours a week on homework, nearly an hour more per
class, results are suggestive of STJ impact (for homework, student week than control youth who averaged 1.57 hours per week. Both
and teacher rated behavior, all p ⬍ .15). ITT and compliant sample analyses showed a large and significant
Academic outcomes. Intervention youth showed significantly effect of STJ on both level and trajectory of change over time in
better GPA and better standardized test scores using both ITT and homework.
compliance analyses. With regard to 8th grade retention, more than Trajectories of in-class behavior are displayed in Figure 2b
twice as many control youths (10.7%) as intervention youths (initiative taking) and 2c (disruptive behavior). As can be seen,
(4.3%) were retained, p ⬍ .10). initiative-taking declined for control youth, not for STJ youth;
disruptive behavior declined for all youth, but the decline was
STJ Effects by End of 9th Grade significantly steeper for STJ youth. Size of intervention effects is
Estimated means for the spring of 9th grade are in Table 2. moderate for the trajectory of change in initiative-taking and large
Table 3 presents the direct effects of the STJ intervention on the for the trajectory of change in disruption over time.
trajectory of change over time and on the difference between With regard to absences (skipping), as displayed in Figure 3a,
intervention and control groups in the spring of 9th grade. For each the large and significant effect of the intervention at the end of 8th
dependent variable, the raw slope coefficient indicates the average grade remained stable and significant through 9th grade. Each
within-homeroom difference in linear trajectory between students semester of 9th grade, STJ youth averaged 2.25 more days in
assigned to STJ and the control group. The raw intercept coeffi- school than control youth.
cient indicates the average spring 9th grade within-homeroom Academic outcomes. STJ and control youth differed signifi-
difference between students assigned to STJ and control group. cantly in GPA by the end of 9th grade, with the size of gap
Standardized effect sizes are interpreted as Cohen’s d and express between groups increasing over time, as displayed graphically in
the same differences in SD units. The trajectories of change for each Figure 3b; intervention effect sizes for both end of 9th grade level
dependent variable are presented graphically in Figures 2 and 3. and trajectory over time were “small-to-moderate” using Cohen’s
Self-regulatory behavior. STJ youth spent significantly more (1988) rules of thumb. Within-time models comparing the STJ and
time doing homework, were more likely to take initiative in class, control groups at each grading period showed that a significant
196 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Table 3
Longitudinal Multilevel Models: Effects of STJ Intervention on Change (Slope) and Level
(Spring of 9th Grade) of Self-Regulatory Behavior, Academic Outcomes, and Depression

Total ITT sample Participating samplea


(N ⫽ 264) (N ⫽ 228)

Raw coefficients Raw coefficients


Standardized
Dependent variables B p B p effect sizeb

Homework time (open ended)c


Spring 9th grade (intercept) 0.166 .043 0.183 .035 0.742
Change trajectory (linear slope) 0.011 .050 0.012 .041 1.044
Teacher-report of in-class initiative
Spring 9th grade (intercept) 0.166 .115 0.234 .037 0.326
Change trajectory (linear slope) 0.012 .073 0.015 .036 0.430
Teacher-report disruptive behavior
Spring 9th grade (intercept) 0.085 .283 ⫺0.178 .030 ⫺0.326
Change trajectory (linear slope) ⫺0.005 .366 ⫺0.010 .040 ⫺0.776
School record of unexcused absencesc
Spring 9th grade (intercept) ⫺0.144 .051 ⫺0.175 .027 ⫺0.296
Change trajectory (linear slope) 0.000 .994 0.001 .847 0.000
School record of core academic GPA
Spring 9th grade (intercept) 0.211 .074 0.277 .031 0.300
Change trajectory (linear slope) 0.164 .266 0.030 .055 0.354
Depression (CESD)d
Spring 9th grade (intercept) ⫺1.355 .187 ⫺1.943 .049 ⫺0.257

Note. The longitudinal multilevel models analyzed variance at three levels: time (Level 1) nested within
students (Level 2), nested within homerooms (Level 3). All Level 2 intercepts and slopes were random. Models
controlled for the following Level 2 (student) covariates: race, sex, baseline age, and preintervention GPA
(student-report).
a
The participating sample was 116 students who attended at least five in-school STJ sessions plus 112 students
in the control group who had similar likelihood of attending, had they been randomly assigned to the
intervention, according to maximum likelihood (EM) modeling of intervention compliance. b Interpretation is
similar to d. For STJ intervention effects on the intercept, ES ⫽ B/T⫺1
00 , where B ⫽ raw coefficient and T00 ⫽
intercept variance in the unconditional change model. For STJ effects on change in linear slopes, ES ⫽ B/T⫺111 ,
where B ⫽ raw coefficient and T11 ⫽ variance of the individual linear change trajectories in the unconditional
change model. For STJ effects on individuals within homerooms (for depression in spring of 9th grade), ES ⫽
B/V⫺1p , where B ⫽ raw coefficient and Vp ⫽ residual variance in the unconditional intercept model (Raudenbush
& Liu, 2001). c These positively skewed dependent variables were log-transformed to reduce the influence of
extreme scores. d Depression was measured only in spring of 9th grade; coefficients are from a cross-sectional,
two-level MLM.

difference in GPA emerged at the 3rd quarter, 2 quarters after the model fit, and to calculate and test indirect effects. All SEM
intervention (B ⫽ .22, p ⬍ .02). GPA between groups continued to analyses used AMOS 5 software (Arbuckle & Wothke, 2003).
diverge over time. By the end of the 9th grade, estimated average Figure 4 displays the structural model of the hypothesized direct
GPA was 1.64 for STJ youth, compared with 1.36 for control and indirect effects including four observed constructs (8th grade
group youth. APS balance, APS plausibility, feared off-track PSs, and 9th Grade
Depression. Intervention effects on depression were signifi- GPA) and four latent constructs (attendance, homework, class-
cant; estimated mean Center for Epidemiological Studies Depres- room behavioral problems, and depression). The measurement
sion Scale scores were nearly 2 points lower for STJ (M ⫽ 10.35) model was developed to test associations between latent constructs
compared with control youth (M ⫽ 12.29) at the end of 9th grade. and the observed indicators that measure them. As presented in
Table 4, all loadings were above .46 (all p ⬍ .01). For the
Hypothesis 2: PSs Mediate STJ Effects on Self-Regulatory multiindicator latent constructs (all but depression), measurement
Behavior, Academic Outcomes, and Depression error was reflected in the loadings linking observed indicators to
latent constructs. For the single-indicator latent construct (the
Analysis Plan multiitem depression scale), measurement error was reflected by
Having found support for hypothesis 1 (significant direct effects weighting the scale error variance by 1 ⫺ Cronbach’s alpha for the
of STJ on PSs, self-regulatory behavior, academic outcomes, and depression scale (1 ⫺ .84 ⫽ .16). Following standard practice, we
depression) allowed for examination of hypothesis 2 (mediating allowed correlated errors among those indicator variables that
effects of PSs). To test for mediation, we used latent structural shared measurement variance due to common source and item
equation modeling, employing standard methods to develop the format. These were closed-ended student-report of attendance,
measurement model defining the latent constructs, to assess overall homework, and classroom behavioral problems (error correlations
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 197

Figure 2. Effect of the STJ intervention over time on hours spent on homework each week (a), teacher-rated
initiative (b), and teacher-rated disruptive behavior (c).

ranged from r ⫽ .12 to r ⫽ .26). Correlations among measurement index ⱖ .95. With the sample size of 228 and df exceeding 100,
errors of other indicators were fixed at zero. power exceeds .80 for a test of overall model fit using RMSEA
The structural model of hypothesized direct and indirect effects (MacCallum, Browne, & Sugawara, 1996). By current criteria, our
was applied to the latent constructs defined in the measurement sample size of 228 is adequate for stable and unbiased estimation
model. Single-headed arrows in Figure 4 indicate the directional of indirect effects and SEs (e.g., MacKinnon, Warsi, & Dwyer,
conceptualization of the structural model: The influence of the 1995). Bootstrapped, bias-corrected SEs were used to test the
intervention on PSs, self-regulatory behavior, academic outcomes, significance of indirect effects (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman,
and depression. Associations among 8th grade measures of self- West, & Sheets, 2002; MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams,
regulatory behavior (homework effort, classroom behavior prob- 2004). Power to test the significance of mediated effects generally
lems, and absences) were specified as nondirectional correlations, exceeded .80 given sample size, measure reliability, extent of
indicated by curved, double-headed arrows. Multivariate normality collinearity between independent and mediating variables, and use
of the full model was acceptable: Mardia’s (1985) test of multi- of latent constructs (see Hoyle & Kenny, 1999).
variate kurtosis ⫽ 10.57, with univariate skew and kurtosis less
than 1.2 for all endogenous variables. Fit of the full structural Testing the Full Process Model
model to the data was assessed by ␹2, supplemented by tests and
critical thresholds recommended by Hu and Bentler (1999): stan- The final structural model, shown in Figure 4, was an excellent
dardized root mean residual ⱕ .08, root mean square error of fit to the data: ␹2(df ⫽ 146) ⫽ 156.36, p ⫽ .26; standardized root
approximation (RMSEA) ⱕ .06, and Bollen’s incremental fit mean residual ⫽ .05, RMSEA ⫽ .02, incremental fit index ⫽ .99.
198 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Figure 3. Effect of the STJ intervention on days absent per semester (a) and core academic grades (b).

The intervention had direct positive effects on PSs as well as a Interpreting the Mediating Effects of PSs
direct negative effect on school absences (reducing absences).
Each PS variable had significant effects on 8th and 9th grade APSs. The STJ intervention had positive direct effects on
outcomes; all mediation paths were significant at p ⬍ .05. Non- APSs (APS balance B ⫽ .14 and APS plausibility, B ⫽ .12), as
directional correlations among constructs were moderate and an- well as an indirect effect on APS plausibility through APS balance
ticipated: 8th grade classroom behavioral problems were corre- (indirect B ⫽ .02). APS balance and APS plausibility were com-
lated positively with absences (r ⫽ .50) and negatively with plete mediators of intervention effects on two of the self-regulatory
homework (r ⫽ ⫺.33). Table 5 lists the total and specific medi- behaviors in the 8th grade— behavioral problems (indirect B ⫽
ation paths linking the intervention with 8th and 9th grade effects. ⫺.02) and time spent in homework (indirect B ⫽ .03). On 9th

Figure 4. Effects of the STJ intervention over time as mediated by PSs.


POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 199

Table 4 sences (indirect B ⫽ ⫺.02). For 9th grade outcomes, off-track PS


Measurement Coefficients for Latent Structural Equation Model partially mediated intervention effects on academic outcomes ⫺
GPA (indirect B ⫽ .01), as well as 9th grade depression (indirect
Standardized B ⫽ ⫺.01).
Latent constructs and observed indicators coefficient

Classroom behavior problems (8th grade) Hypothesis 3: STJ Influences the Relationship Between
Teacher report of disruptive behaviora 0.817
Student report of disruptive behavior 0.465
APS and Social Identity
Teacher report of initiative ⫺0.468
Homework (8th grade)
Analysis Plan
Closed-ended rating of time spent on homework each We used hierarchical multiple regression to examine the mod-
weeka 0.888
Open-ended report of time spent on homework each erating effect of the STJ intervention on the Spring 9th Grade
week 0.764 APS-social identity relationship. Fall 8th Grade APS was entered
Absences (8th grade) as a control variable at Block 1, experimental assignment at Block
School record of unexcused absencesa 0.736 2, Social Identity Connectedness at Block 3, and the Connected-
Student report of absences 0.568
Depression (9th grade)b
ness ⫻ Intervention interaction at Block 4. Methods recommended
CESD 0.910 by Aiken and West (1991) were used to probe interaction effects
and test significance of simple slopes.
Note. N ⫽ 228 youth in the participating sample. All coefficients signif-
icant at p ⬍ .001.
a
These indicators were set to 1 to define the scale of the construct. Effects on APS
b
Measurement error in this latent construct was modeled by weighting the
error variance of the single indicator (the CESD scale score) by APS plausibility. The full model containing main effects and
1-Cronbach’s alpha (⫺.84 ⫽ .16). the STJ ⫻ Social Identity Connectedness interaction was signifi-
cant, F(4, 223) ⫽ 5.02, p ⬍ .001, with added variance explained
by the STJ ⫻ Social Identity Connectedness interaction, B ⫽ .57,
grade academic outcomes, APS balance and APS plausibility ⌬F(1, 223) ⫽ 3.78, p ⫽ .05. For intervention youth, Connected-
partially mediated intervention effects on GPA (indirect B ⫽ .03). ness and APS plausibility were positively associated, simple slope
APS balance and plausibility also partially mediated intervention B ⫽ .80, p ⬍ .001, but for control youth, there was no significant
effects on 9th grade depression (indirect B ⫽ ⫺.01). relationship, simple slope B ⫽ .22, p ⫽ .31.
Feared off-track PSs. The intervention had positive direct APS balance. The full model containing main effects and the
effects on feared off-track PSs (B ⫽ .13). Off-track PS was a STJ ⫻ Social Identity Connectedness interaction was marginally
partial mediator of intervention effects on 8th grade school ab- significant, F(4, 223) ⫽ 1 .95, p ⫽ .10, and the STJ ⫻ Social

Table 5
Mediational Paths Linking the STJ Intervention to Spring 8th Grade and Spring 9th Grade Effects

Standardized
General and specific mediational paths effect p

STJ effects on spring 8th grade outcomes


Total STJ effect on classroom behavior problems (8th grade) ⫺0.022 0.023
STJ 3 APS balance 3 APS plausibility 3 classroom behavior problems ⫺0.003 0.034
STJ 3 APS plausibility 3 classroom behavior problems ⫺0.019 0.044
Total STJ effect on homework (8th grade) 0.026 0.015
STJ 3 APS balance 3 APS plausibility 3 homework 0.003 0.036
STJ 3 APS plausibility 3 homework 0.023 0.025
Total STJ effect on absences (8th grade) ⫺0.208 0.002
STJ 3 feared off-track PS 3 absences ⫺0.021 0.027
STJ 3 absences ⫺0.186 0.003
STJ effects on spring 9th grade outcomes
Total STJ effect on GPA (spring 9th grade) 0.090 0.002
STJ 3 APS balance 3 GPA 0.019 0.018
STJ 3 APS balance 3 APS plausibility 3 classroom behavior 3 GPA 0.002 0.028
STJ 3 APS plausibility 3 classroom behavior problems 3 GPA 0.009 0.024
STJ 3 absences 3 GPA 0.054 0.006
STJ 3 feared off-track PS 3 absences 3 GPA 0.006 0.024
Total STJ effect on depression (spring 9th grade) ⫺0.091 0.001
STJ 3 APS balance 3 APS plausibility 3 homework 3 depression ⫺0.001 0.023
STJ 3 APS plausibility 3 homework 3 depression ⫺0.005 0.014
STJ 3 feared off-track PS 3 absences 3 depression ⫺0.009 0.017
STJ 3 absences 3 depression ⫺0.076 0.008

Note. N ⫽ 228 youth in the participating sample. Italicized entries are for general indirect effects; nonitalicized
entries are for specific indirect effect paths.
200 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Identity Connectedness interaction, B ⫽ .21, ⌬F(1, 223) ⫽ 6.39, these social identities are often assumed to be at odds with aca-
p ⫽ .01, added significantly to variance explained. For interven- demic self-goals (Steinitz & Solomon, 1986), and school failure
tion youth, Connectedness and APS balance were positively asso- has important consequences, increasing risk of depression and
ciated, simple slope B ⫽ .13, p ⬍ .03, but for control youth, there delinquency (Joseph, 1996; Kasen, Cohen, & Brook, 1998), future
was no significant relationship, simple slope B ⫽ ⫺.08, p ⫽ .20. unemployment, problems in parenting, and mental health problems
(e.g., Stoep, Weiss, Kuo, Cheney, & Cohen, 2003) and making
Discussion successful completion of other developmental tasks less likely
(Sandler & Chassin, 2002). Low SES is a clear risk factor for
Although PSs focused on school success are common, so is school failure (Blair, Blair, & Madamba, 1999; Orfield et al.,
failure in school. We asked why PSs fail to produce sustained 2004); the combination of minority race/ethnicity and low social
self-regulation, outlining a process model in which self-regulation class together account for about 19% of variance in academic
and, therefore behavior change, is likely when PSs are linked with attainment (McDermott, 1995). Improving outcomes in this high-
contextually salient strategies, when they are congruent with social risk group is both critical and difficult given that poverty and
identity, and when difficulty is understood as a normative part of minority status are “fixed-risk” factors, unlikely to change dramat-
the process of attaining PSs. Focusing explicitly on low-income ically over the student’s middle and high school years.
and minority youth, we developed an intervention to link PSs and As a test of our model, we chose to intervene in the fall of the
strategies, to incorporate difficulty as a normative part of pursuing final year of middle school. We chose this point both because the
PSs, and to facilitate a positive link between APSs and important transition from middle to high school is associated with increased
social identities. Our basic premises were that self-concept is not salience of important social class and racial ethnic social identities
monolithic, that PSs are differentially accessible, and that they are and because school failure rates begin to rise dramatically from the
likely to influence behaviors only when linked to strategies, when 1st year of high school (e.g., Seidman, LaRue, Aber, Lawrence, &
experienced as compatible with social identity, and when difficulty Feinman, 1994). The transition to high school is stressing, as can
working toward the PS is viewed as normative. be seen in the outcomes for control group youth; without the
To ensure that the intervention effects would be sustained over intervention, self-regulatory behavior declines, academic out-
time, the intervention occurred in school, with peers, during the comes erode, and risk of depression is higher. An intervention that
school day and targeted each aspect of our process model. Struc- bolsters APSs and behavioral engagement will improve outcomes
tured group activities evoked academically focused PSs, made in the last year of middle school, making the transition to high
clear that APSs were held by peers (and therefore something that school less risky. In addition, middle schools are on average
“we” aspire to), and highlighted the normativeness of difficulties smaller and more homogeneous than high schools, making inter-
and failures along the way to attaining PS goals. Thus, the inter- vention focused on linking PSs and social identity simpler.
vention operationalized our theory of how PSs might influence Youth who are not equipped with specific PSs that make en-
self-regulatory behaviors (and through these behaviors, academic gagement in school (attending, putting effort into homework,
outcomes and well-being). engaging teachers) a necessity are more likely to fall behind in
Although attaining and sustaining an intervention effect is no- high school simply because high schools are more impersonal and
toriously difficult, we documented effects that were stable and less likely to routinely fulfill their educational needs. Indeed, the
even increasing over time. This sustained effect over 2 years is transition to high school has been described as a consciousness-
particularly impressive, given that the high-poverty neighborhoods raising experience due to the influx of students of diverse racial-
the youth were embedded in and the difficulty of improving ethnic and economic backgrounds (Seidman, Aber, Allen, &
academic outcomes when prior academic attainment accounts for French, 1996). Ethnographic accounts suggest that low-SES teens
much of the variance to be explained. We were able to directly do not realize that they are poor in homogeneously low-SES
assess each aspect of the process model except the positive self- schools and neighborhoods where they appear average (Roderick,
regulatory consequences of changing metacognitive experience. 2003; Steinitz & Solomon, 1986). Low-SES students report learn-
Moreover, we documented effects using two very different ana- ing that they are part of a low-SES group when they enter high
lytic strategies (structural equation modeling and longitudinal school., making the transition to high school very stressing for
MLM) with different strengths and assumptions. these youth. Not surprisingly, racial-ethnic identity also becomes
Structural equation modeling provided explicit test of hypothe- more salient in the transition (Altschul, Oyserman, & Bybee, in
sized mediation paths via PSs to self-regulatory behavior to be- press). Not only does the transition to high school raise conscious-
havioral outcomes, using multiple sources of information to ac- ness of social identities and their relative standing, it also high-
count for measurement error. MLM, by allowing us to look lights ambiguities about the one’s probable future. Schools no
directly at intervention effects on trajectories of individual change longer provide vocational or technical training, so youths not in
as well as at specific points, provided an explicit test of the advanced classes are in “general studies,” implying that the point
hypothesized effects over time. MLM documented that interven- of school is either college or marking time. For all of these reasons,
tion effects on self-regulatory behavior (attendance, homework transition to high school is characterized by dropping grades and
time, and behavior problems) and academic outcomes were stable school involvement for low-SES and minority children (Seidman
or increased over our 2-year follow-up, while accounting for the et al., 1994).
nesting of students within homerooms. We documented that our intervention does change PSs, increas-
Our focus on PSs of low-income and minority youths and their ing both feared off-track and academically focused PSs and strat-
fit with social identity as working class, African American, or egies to attain them. As hypothesized, intervention youth both had
Latino was intentional. We chose PSs and social identity because more of these PSs and were better able to use them to improve
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 201

behavioral self-regulation. The intervention produced measurable Whether focused on positive expected or feared PSs, self-
change in PSs, and change in PSs predicted change in behavioral regulation fails when individuals do not realize that a particular
self-regulation— going to school rather than skipping, behaving action is antithetical to goal achievement, when PSs do not provide
and participating in class, and spending time on homework; self- clear standards of what to attain or avoid, or when PSs are not
regulation not only improved academic outcomes but, equally linked with self-regulatory behaviors (e.g., Carver, 2004; Higgins,
importantly, reduced risk of depression. 1987). For low-income teens, lapses in self-regulation may be
The impact of academically focused PSs on self-regulatory difficult to repair—when risk of failure is high, any misstep can
behaviors was distinct from the impact of feared off-track PSs on spell disaster. This contrasts with the situation of middle-class
self-regulatory behaviors. Youth with balanced and plausible ac- teens whose self-regulatory lapses can be compensated for by
ademically focused PSs spent more time doing homework, were contextual regulation set in place by neighborhood, school, and
less disruptive, and more behaviorally engaged in class-room parents. Middle-class students are more likely to be provided
activities. Youth with feared off-track PSs attended school more mentoring, tutoring, monitoring, and enrichment activities whether
(had fewer school absences). The distinct role of feared off-track they seek them out or not (e.g., Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls,
PSs is congruent with a number of self-regulatory models: Carver 1999). Low-income students are more likely to live in contexts
(2004) describes self-regulation to avoid feared PSs or antigoals as lacking such collective efficacy resources (Sampson et al., 1999).
discrepancy-enlarging self-regulation. Larsen (2004) describes the Thus, for low-income youths, self-initiated engagement in self-
self-regulatory system as vigilant to environmental dangers; when regulation that focuses on attaining positive PSs and avoiding
danger is cued, individuals are more cautious about engaging in negative PSs is likely necessary.
behaviors that may increase risk. Higgins (Higgins & Spiegel, Our emphasis on the situation of low-income and minority
2004) describes prevention-focused self-regulation as risk averse. youth is congruent with research using the stereotype threat model
Following these perspectives, youth with feared off-track PSs can (Steele, 1997) that demonstrates the undermining effect on aca-
be expected to be cautious about risk-increasing behaviors and demic outcomes of making stigmatized in-group social identity
likely to engage in action (e.g., attending school) they perceive as salient. To date, stereotype threat research has not documented the
antithetical to their off-track selves of becoming pregnant, in- mechanism by which stereotype threat produces negative conse-
quences over time. Our results suggest that stereotype threat may
volved in drugs, crime, or the police. Indeed, increased feared
undermine academic attainment by making APSs less salient and
off-track PSs reduced risk of school absences. Although vigilant
school-focused strategies less accessible as ways of avoiding off-
focus on antigoals or prevention is likely to reduce risk of harm,
track PSs. This process model is congruent with a number of
active engagement in goal attainment (discrepancy-reducing self-
studies documenting that when stereotype threat is activated, pre-
regulation) is likely to increase chances of success.
vention (discrepancy enhancing) focus increases (Oyserman,
Although self-regulatory systems theories posit roles for both
Uskul, Yoder, Nesse, & Williams, 2005; Seibt & Förster, 2004), as
discrepancy-reducing (promotion) and discrepancy-increasing
do negative thoughts about one’s math capacity (Cadinu, Maass,
(prevention) systems, prevention focus, engaging in self-
Rosabianca, & Kiesner, 2005).
regulatory behavior to avoid feared PSs, is not particularly prom-
Our results demonstrate the real-world power of a social psy-
inent in the academic goal literature. Perhaps this is because in
chological conceptualization of the self as a motivational resource.
middle-class contexts, pursuit of academic goals may more com- By integrating PSs, social identity and metacognitive perspectives,
monly involve the discrepancy reducing feedback system— engag- we developed a process model that, when operationalized, pro-
ing in self-regulatory behavior to attain positive expected PSs. duced lasting change on PSs, self-regulation, academic outcomes,
There is evidence that college students are more likely to use and depression. These results are particularly promising given the
promotion- than prevention-focused self-regulation (e.g., Lock- difficulty in producing sustained improvement in the educational
wood, Sadler, Fyman, & Tuck, 2004). However, prevention fo- outcomes of low-income youths and the great need to develop
cused self-regulation may become more salient in a number of integrative social science models that can be applied to ameliorat-
circumstances. First, prevention focused self-regulation may be ing this large-scale social problem.
more likely when social contextual risk is high, such as in circum-
stances of poverty. For example, first generation college students References
are more likely to engaging in strategies to avoid feared PSs than
Abrams, D. (1994). Social self-regulation. Personality and Social Psychol-
strategies to attain positive PSs (Oyserman, Gant, & Ager, 1995, ogy Bulletin, 20, 473– 483.
Study 1). Ethnographic evidence from low-income high school Aiken, L., & West, S. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
students also highlights the salience of feared off-track PSs— interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
becoming unemployed, homeless, and destitute (Steinitz & So- Altschul, I., Oyserman, D., & Bybee, D. (in press). Racial-ethnic identity
lomon, 1986; see also Kaiser Foundation, 2002). Second, it is in mid-adolescence: Content and change as predictors of academic
possible that culture influences choice of self-regulatory system; attainment. Child Development.
Euro-Canadian college students find promotion-focused strategies Ambady, N., Paik, S., Steele, J., Owen-Smith, A., & Mitchell, J. (2004).
Deflecting negative self-relevant stereotype activation: The effects of
compelling, whereas Asian Canadian college students find
individuation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 401– 408.
prevention-focused strategies compelling (Lockwood, Marshall, &
Arbuckle, J., & Wothke, W. (2003). AMOS 5.0 user’s guide. Chicago:
Sadler, 2005). Similarly, Hong Kong Chinese college students SmallWaters.
found prevention-focused reasons for action more convincing than Arroyo, C., & Zigler, E. (1995). Racial identity, academic achievement,
did American college students (Lee, Aaker, & Gardner, 2000, and the psychological well-being of economically disadvantaged ado-
Study 4). lescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 903–914.
202 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Baumeister, R. (1998). The self. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey and salience. In T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology,
(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680 –740). New handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press.
York: McGraw-Hill. Higgins, T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52,
Belli, R. (1998). The structure of autobiographical memory and the event 1280 –1300.
history calendar: Potential improvements in the quality of retrospective Higgins, T. (1998). The aboutness principle: A pervasive influence on
reports in surveys, Memory, 6, 383– 406. human inference. Social Cognition, 16, 173–198.
Blair, S., Blair, M., & Madamba, A. (1999). Racial/ethnic differences in Higgins, T., & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for
high school students’ academic performance: Understanding the inter- self-regulation: A motivated cognition perspective. In R. Baumeister &
weave of social class and ethnicity in the family context. Journal of K. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and
Comparative Family Studies, 30, 539 –555. applications (pp. 171–187). New York: Guilford Press.
Burke, P. (2003). Relationships among multiple identities. In P. Burke & Hoyle, R., & Kenny, D. (1999). Sample size, reliability, and tests of
T. Owens (Eds.), Advances in identity theory and research (pp. 195– statistical mediation. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small
214). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press. sample research (pp. 195–222). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., & Kiesner, J. (2005). Why do Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
negative thinking. Psychological Science, 16, 572–578. tural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Carver, C. (2004). Self-regulation of action and affect. In R. Baumeister & James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.
K. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and Jo, B. (2002a). Estimation of intervention effects with noncompliance:
applications (pp. 13–39). New York: Guilford Press. Alternative model specifications. Journal of Educational and Behav-
Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (1990). Principles of self-regulation: Action and ioral Statistics, 27, 385– 409.
emotion. In T. Higgins & R. Sorrentinoeds (Eds.), Handbook of moti- Jo, B. (2002b). Model misspecification sensitivity analysis in estimating
vation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 3–52). New York: Guilford Press. causal effects of interventions with noncompliance. Statistics in Medi-
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences cine, 25, 305–327.
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Joseph, J. (1996). School factors and delinquency: A study of African
Cook, P., & Ludwig, J. (1997). Weighing the “burden of acting White”: American youths. Journal of Black Studies, 26, 340 –355.
Are there race differences in attitudes toward education. Journal of Juster, T., Ono, H., & Stafford, F. (2004). Changing times of American
Policy Analysis and Management, 16, 256 –278. youth, 1981–2003. Retrieved September 14, 2005, from the University
Costello, E., Mustillo, S., & Erkanli, A. (2003). Prevalence and develop- of Michigan Institute for Social Research Child Development Supple-
ment of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of ment to the Panel Study for Income Dynamics: http://www.umich.edu/
General Psychiatry, 60, 837– 844. news/Releases/2004/Nov04/teen_time_report.pdf
Cribbie, R., & Jamieson, J. (2000). Structural equation models and the Kaiser Foundation (2002). National Survey of Latinos. Summary of find-
regression bias for measuring correlates of change. Educational and ings. Retrieved September 14, 2005, from http://www.kff.org/
Psychological Measurement, 60, 893–907. kaiserpolls/20021217a-index.cfm
Croizet, J., & Claire, T. (1998). Extending the concept of stereotype and Kao, G. (2000). Group images and possible selves among adolescents:
threat to social class: The intellectual underperformance of students from Linking stereotypes to expectations by race and ethnicity. Sociological
low socioeconomic backgrounds. Personality and Social Psychology Forum, 15, 407– 430.
Bulletin, 24, 588 –594. Kasen, S., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. (1998). Adolescent school experiences
Detroit News (2005, May 29). Graduation rates. Retrieved September 15, and dropout, adolescent pregnancy, and young adult deviant behavior.
2005, from http://www.detnews.com/2005/specialreport/0505/30/ Journal of Adolescent Research, 13, 49 –72.
A01-196697.htm. Kearney, K., Hopkins, R., Mauss, A., & Weisheit, R. (1983). Sample bias
Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. resulting from a requirement for written parental consent. Public Opin-
Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109 –132. ion Quarterly, 47, 96 –102.
Ellickson, P., & Hawes, J. (1989). An assessment of active versus passive King, L. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality
methods for obtaining parental consent. Evaluation Review, 13, 45–55. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 798 – 807.
Ferguson, R. (1998). Comment on the burden of acting White: Do Black King, L., & Smith, N. (2004). Gay and straight possible selves: Goals,
adolescents disparage academic achievement? by P. J. Cook and J. identity, subjective well-being, and personality development. Journal of
Ludwig. In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black-White test score Personality, 72, 967–994.
gap (pp. 394 –397). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution. Larsen, R. (2004, November). Emotion and cognition: The case of auto-
Finn, J., Pannozzo, G., & Voelkl, K. (1995). Disruptive and inattentive- matic vigilance. APA online. Retrieved September 14, 2005, from http://
withdrawn behavior and achievement among fourth graders. The Ele- www.apa.org/science/psa/sb-larsen.html
mentary School Journal, 95, 421– 434. Lee, A., Aaker, J., & Gardner, W. (2000). The pleasures and pains of
Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. (1986). Black students’ school success: Coping distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus.
with the “burden of acting white.” Urban Review, 18, 176 –206. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1122–1134.
Graham, S. (2001). Inferences about responsibility and values: Implication Little, R., & Rubin, D. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data (2nd
for academic motivation. In F. Salili, & C. Chiu (Eds.), Student moti- ed). New York: Wiley.
vation: The culture and context of learning (pp. 31–59). Dordrecht, Little, R., & Yau, L. (1998). Statistical techniques for analyzing data from
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. prevention trials: Treatment of no-shows using Rubin’s causal model.
Higgins, T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Methods, 3, 147–159.
Psychological Review, 94, 319 –340. Lockwood, P., Marshall, T., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or
Higgins, T. (1996a). The “self digest”: Self-knowledge serving self- preventing failure: Cultural differences in motivation by positive and
regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31,
1062–1083. 379 –392.
Higgins, T. (1996b). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, Lockwood, P., Sadler, P., Fyman, K., & Tuck, S. (2004). To do or not to
POSSIBLE SELVES AND ACADEMIC OUTCOMES 203

do: Using positive and negative role models to harness motivation. Radloff, L., & Locke, B. (2000). Center for Epidemiologic Studies De-
Social Cognition, 22, 422– 450. pression Scale (CESD). In Taskforce for the Handbook of Psychiatric
MacCallum, R., Browne, M., & Sugawara, H. (1996). Power analysis and Measures (Eds.), Handbook of psychiatric measures: American Psychi-
determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psycho- atric Association (pp. 523–526). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
logical Methods, 1, 130 –149. Association.
MacKinnon, D., Lockwood, C., Hoffman, J., West, S., & Sheets, V. (2002). Raudenbush, S., & Bryk, A. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applica-
A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening vari- tions and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
able effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. Raudenbush, S., & Liu, X. (2000). Statistical power and optimal design for
MacKinnon, D., Lockwood, C., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits multisite randomized trials. Psychological Methods, 5, 199 –213.
for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling meth- Raudenbush, S., & Liu, X. (2001). Effects of study duration, frequency of
ods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99 –128. observation, and sample size on power in studies of group differences in
MacKinnon, D., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. (1995). A simulation study of polynomial change. Psychological Methods, 6, 387– 401.
mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30, 41– 62. Régner, I., Huguet, P., & Monteil, J. (2002). Effects of socioeconomic
Mardia, K. (1985). Mardia’s test of multinormality. In S. Kotz & N. status (SES) information on cognitive ability inferences: When low-SES
Johnson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistical sciences (Vol. 5, pp. 217– students make use of a self-threatening stereotype. Social Psychology of
221). New York: Wiley. Education, 5, 253–269.
Maris, E. (1998). Covariance adjustment versus gain scores: revisited. Roberts, R., Attkisson, C., & Rosenblatt, A. (1998). Prevalence of psycho-
Psychological Methods, 3, 309 –327. pathology among children and adolescents. American Journal of Psy-
McDermott, P. (1995). Sex, race, class, and other demographics as expla- chiatry, 155, 715–725.
nations for children’s ability and adjustment: A national appraisal. Roderick, M. (2003). What’s happening to the boys? Early high school
Journal of School Psychology, 33, 75–91. experiences and school outcomes among African American male ado-
McGuire, W., & Padawe-Singer, A. (1976). Trait salience in the sponta- lescents in Chicago. Urban Education, 38, 538 – 607.
neous self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, Rothman, A., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Constructing perceptions of vulner-
743–754. ability: Personal relevance and the use of experiential information in
Orfield, G., Losen, D., Wald, J., & Swanson, C. (2004). Losing our future: health judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
How minority youth are being left behind by the graduation rate crisis.
1053–1064.
Cambridge, MA: The Civil Rights Project at Harvard University. Re-
Ruvolo, A., & Markus, H. (1992). Possible selves and performance: The
trieved September 14, 2005, from http://www.urban.org/UploadedPDF/
power of self-relevant imagery. Social Cognition, 10, 95–124.
410936_LosingOurFuture.pdf
Sampson, R., Morenoff, J., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capital:
Oyserman, D. (2001). Self-concept and identity. In A. Tesser & N.
Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociolog-
Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology (pp. 499 –
ical Review, 64, 633– 660.
517). Malden, MA: Blackwell Press.
Sandler, I., & Chassin, L. (2002). Training of prevention researchers:
Oyserman, D. (in press.) Social identity and self-regulation. In A. Kruglan-
Perspectives from the Arizona State University Prevention Research
ski & T. Higgins, Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New
Training Program. Prevention & Treatment, 5, Item 5. Retrieved April 10,
York: Guilford Press.
2006, from http://journals.apa.org/prevention/volume5/pre0050006a.html
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Terry, K., & Hart-Johnson, T. (2004). Possible
Schwarz, N. (1998). Accessible content and accessibility experiences: The
selves as roadmaps. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 130 –149.
interplay of declarative and experiential information in judgment. Per-
Oyserman, D., & Fryberg, S. A. (2006). The possible selves of diverse
sonality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 87–99.
adolescents: Content and function across gender, race, and national
origin. In C. Dunkel & J. Kerpelman (Eds.), Possible selves: Theory, Schwarz, N. (2002). Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: Cog-
research, and application (pp. 17–39). Huntington, NY: Nova. nitive tuning. In L. Feldman Barrett & P. Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom in
Oyserman, D., Gant, L., & Ager, J. (1995). A socially contextualized feelings (pp. 144 –166). New York: Guilford Press.
model of African American identity: Possible selves and school persis- Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives:
tence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1216 –1232. An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in
Oyserman, D., Johnson, E., & Bybee, D. (2006). Effects of low socioeco- social judgment. In: L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of
nomic status on early adolescent possible selves. Manuscript under social judgments (pp. 217–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
editorial review. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences.
Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. (1990). Possible selves and delinquency. In: T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 112–125. of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press.
Oyserman, D., & Saltz, E. (1993). Competence, delinquency, and attempts Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How
to attain possible selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Jour-
65, 360 –374. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38 –56.
Oyserman, D., Terry, K., & Bybee, D. (2002). A possible selves interven- Seidman, E., Aber, J., Allen, L., & French, S. (1996). The impact of the
tion to enhance school involvement. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 313– transition to high school on the self-esteem and perceived social context
326. of poor urban youth. American Journal of Community Psychology, 24,
Oyserman, D., Uskul, A., Yoder, N., Nesse, R., & Williams, D. (2005). 489 –515.
Unfair treatment and self-regulatory focus. Manuscript submitted for Seidman, E., Allen, L., Aber, J. L., Mitchell, C., & Feinman, J. (1994). The
editorial review. impact of school transitions in early adolescence on the self-system and
Pokorny, S., Jason, L., Schoeny, M., Townsend, S., & Curie, C. (2001). Do perceived social context of poor urban youth. Child Development, 65,
participation rates change when active consent procedures replace pas- 507–522.
sive consent. Evaluation Review, 25, 567–580. Settles, I. H. (2004). When multiple identities interfere: The role of identity
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for centrality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 487–500.
research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to
1, 385– 401. basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
204 OYSERMAN, BYBEE, AND TERRY

Steele, C. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual Thomas, D., Townsend, T., & Belgrave, F. (2003). The influence of
identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613– 629. cultural and racial identification on the psychosocial adjustment of
Steinitz, V., & Solomon, E. (1986). Starting out: Class and community inner-city African American children in school. American Journal of
in the lives of working-class youth. Philadelphia: Temple University Community Psychology, 32, 217–228.
Press. U.S. Bureau of the Census (2000). Census 2000. Retrieved April 10, 2006,
Stoep, A., Weiss, N., Kuo, E., Cheney, D., & Cohen, P. (2003). What from http://www.census.gov
proportion of failure to complete secondary school in the U.S. popula- Weinger, S. (2000). Opportunities for career success: Views of poor and
tion is attributable to adolescent psychiatric disorder? Journal of Behav- middle-class children. Children & Youth Services Review, 22, 13–35.
ioral Health Services and Research, 30, 119 –124. Weinstein, N. (1993). Testing four competing theories of health-protective
Strauman, T. (2002). Self-regulation and depression. Self & Identity, 1, behavior. Health Psychology, 12, 324 –333.
151–157.
Strauman, T., & Higgins, T. (1988). Self-discrepancies as predictors of Received December 22, 2004
vulnerability to distinct syndromes of chronic emotional distress. Jour- Revision received January 4, 2006
nal of Personality, 56, 685–707. Accepted January 24, 2006 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 33– 48 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.33

Thinking Within the Box: The Relational Processing Style Elicited by


Counterfactual Mind-Sets

Laura J. Kray Adam D. Galinsky


University of California, Berkeley Northwestern University

Elaine M. Wong
University of California, Berkeley

By comparing reality to what might have been, counterfactuals promote a relational processing style
characterized by a tendency to consider relationships and associations among a set of stimuli. As such,
counterfactual mind-sets were expected to improve performance on tasks involving the consideration of
relationships and associations but to impair performance on tasks requiring novel ideas that are
uninfluenced by salient associations. The authors conducted several experiments to test this hypothesis.
In Experiments 1a and 1b, the authors determined that counterfactual mind-sets increase mental states
and preferences for thinking styles consistent with relational thought. Experiment 2 demonstrated a
facilitative effect of counterfactual mind-sets on an analytic task involving logical relationships; Exper-
iments 3 and 4 demonstrated that counterfactual mind-sets structure thought and imagination around
salient associations and therefore impaired performance on creative generation tasks. In Experiment 5,
the authors demonstrated that the detrimental effect of counterfactual mind-sets is limited to creative
tasks involving novel idea generation; in a creative association task involving the consideration of
relationships between task stimuli, counterfactual mind-sets improved performance.

Keywords: counterfactuals, mind-sets, relational processing, analytical, creativity

Whenever individuals consider how the past might have turned In the present article, we examined the impact of counterfactual
out differently, they are engaging in counterfactual thinking. For thinking on subsequent thinking styles and problem solving.
example, people who ponder, “What would life be like if I had Broadly speaking, we explored how reflecting back on events in
married that other person?” or “Would I be better off if I had which an outcome almost turned out differently impacts future
selected that other job?” are implicitly comparing reality with what problem solving. More precisely, we explored the nature of the
might have been. Thoughts of “if only” and “what if” are signposts mind-set that results from constructing counterfactual thoughts.
for counterfactual musings, and their presence in mental life is Because counterfactuals involve a consideration of both reality and
both pervasive and predictable. A growing body of literature what might have been, they are inherently relational in nature. Our
suggests that counterfactuals are not merely fodder for daydream- central thesis is that constructing counterfactual thoughts in one
ers stuck in the past but rather serve important functions for context produces a counterfactual mind-set characterized by a
directing future behavior (Roese & Olson, 1995). tendency to process information relationally in subsequent con-
texts. Thus, the mental structure of logical relationships created
through counterfactual thinking increases the ability to understand
and perceive relationships in subsequent contexts, structuring
Laura J. Kray and Elaine M. Wong, Haas School of Business, University
thought around salient associations and the pursuit of connections.
of California, Berkeley; Adam D. Galinsky, Kellogg Graduate School of
Because the counterfactual mind-set occurs regardless of the con-
Management, Northwestern University.
Elaine M. Wong is now at the School of Communication, Northwestern tent or valence of the counterfactual thoughts (Galinsky & Kray,
University. 2004; Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Kray & Galinsky, 2003),
This research was supported in part by National Science Foundation what lingers is the form and structure of the counterfactual (i.e., the
Grants SES-0233294 and SES-0136931. We gratefully acknowledge in- tendency to consider relationships and connections between a set
sightful comments provided by Phil Tetlock on an earlier version of this of stimuli).
article and the valuable feedback provided by participants in colloquia at Although previous research has explored the consequences of
the Haas School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University, and the Uni- the counterfactual mind-set on subsequent performance across a
versity of Chicago. We are indebted to the many research assistants who
variety of tasks, the present research is the first to identify the
helped out in every stage of the research process. In particular, we are
grateful to Joyce Chen, Joe Gacula, Linda Pham, and Alissa Roberts. mental states and thinking styles that counterfactuals activate and
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Laura J. the mechanisms by which counterfactual mind-sets have their
Kray, Haas School of Business, 545 Student Services Building, #1900, effects. We characterize the counterfactual mind-set as a structured
Berkeley, CA 94720. E-mail: kray@haas.berkeley.edu form of thought involving a consideration of relationships and

33
34 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

associations between a set of stimuli. We expect this processing tual mind-set is activated, it persists because the relational pro-
style to have three consequences. First, it should create phenom- cessing style that accompanies it is a well-learned and useful
enological experiences and preferences for structured thought strategy for assessing causality, a critical tool for comprehending
(Sternberg, 1988). Second, it should promote lay conceptions of the world (cf. Kelley, 1973).
analytic thought, defined as “an examination of a complex, its Past research has demonstrated that situations with an obvious
elements and their relations” (Merriam-Webster, 2005). Third, it mutable component tend to elicit counterfactual thoughts, which
should promote structured imagination, or the tendency to build on then orient cognition in subsequent contexts. This cognitive ori-
existing knowledge structures (Ward, 1994). We have chosen the entation has been called a “counterfactual mind-set” (Galinsky &
term relational processing style to capture the essence of the Kray, 2004; Kray & Galinsky, 2003). The idea that the activation
counterfactual mind-set because it both describes the nature of of a counterfactual mind-set impacts subsequent cognition and
counterfactual thoughts and because it is broad enough to encom- performance has received strong support across a variety of indi-
pass the full range of effects produced by the activation of a vidual and group tasks. More specifically, counterfactual mind-
counterfactual mind-set. sets appear to be an asset when the consideration of alternatives
facilitates performance (Galinsky & Kray, 2004; Galinsky & Mos-
The Conceptual Link Between Counterfactuals and kowitz, 2000; Kray & Galinsky, 2003). Counterfactual mind-sets
Relational Processing also appear to encourage skepticism about the dominant hypoth-
esis. For example, having just considered an alternate reality can
Although the ability to undo events and construct possible reduce the confirmation bias or the tendency to seek information
worlds is theoretically unlimited, in reality, when and how coun- that is consistent with an existing hypothesis to the relative detri-
terfactuals are constructed is fairly predictable. The commence- ment of information that could potentially disconfirm it (Galinsky
ment of counterfactual thinking is often initiated when an event & Moskowitz, 2000; Kray & Galinsky, 2003). In fact, the number
nearly occurred (Kahneman & Miller, 1986; Roese & Olson, of counterfactual thoughts generated in response to a mutable
1997). For example, missing a plane by 5 min tends to evoke more scenario has been shown to be an important predictor of subse-
counterfactuals than missing a plane by 1 hr, presumably because quent disconfirmatory information search (Kray & Galinsky,
the former case more readily conjures up elements of the day 2003). Counterfactual mind-sets also improve decision accuracy
leading up to the flight that could have been undone. In addition to by increasing the discussion of unique information critical for
near misses, “abnormal” events tend to produce counterfactual group decision making and promoting synergistic coordination, or
thoughts. For example, it is easier to undo missing one’s flight the tendency of group members to build on and develop relation-
when a new, atypical route to the airport was taken than it is after ships between each other’s ideas (Galinsky & Kray, 2004; Liljen-
taking one’s usual route. It is often the mere presence of an quist, Galinsky, & Kray, 2004). Finally, counterfactual thinking
obvious mutable component to an event that leads to the sponta- has been shown to increase the scrutiny of persuasive message
neous generation of counterfactuals. content (Krishnamurthy & Sivaraman, 2002), enabling decision
Just like the rules that govern when counterfactuals are gener- makers to distinguish between strong and weak arguments. Despite
ated, how counterfactual thoughts are constructed is predictable. the diversity of tasks explored in this growing body of literature, a
Counterfactuals involve a comparison of the relationship between common facilitative effect has emerged after the activation of a
reality and what might have been. Constructing a counterfactual counterfactual mind-set. Overall, it appears that by considering
thought implicitly involves laying out a causal chain of events in alternative realities in one context, greater clarity regarding task
an action sequence and mutating one step in the process to con- associations is gained in later contexts. Although the counterfac-
struct an alternate reality. As such, running a counterfactual sim- tual mind-set has been described generally as promoting “a con-
ulation in one’s head is the mental equivalent of conducting an sideration of alternatives” (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000), in the
experiment. Like the experimental process, counterfactual thinking present investigation, we seek to better understand the content of
involves a logical consideration of relationships and causal asso- and processing style associated with this counterfactual mind-set
ciations between events (Einhorn & Hogarth, 1986; Mandel, 2003; and the mechanisms by which it impacts performance.
Mandel & Lehman, 1996; Wells & Gavanski, 1989). For example,
Wells and Gavanski showed that an initial event was judged to be Research Overview
causally connected to a subsequent event to the extent that a
mutation to the initial event would have undone the occurrence of Despite numerous demonstrations that counterfactual mind-sets
the subsequent event. affect subsequent problem solving, to date, our understanding of
As described above, a logical structure underlies when and how the exact nature of a counterfactual mind-set has been limited
counterfactuals are created. Counterfactuals are most likely when because little effort has been made to explore the underlying
potential mutations to a sequence of events are salient or when process by which counterfactual mind-sets influence performance.
potential alternative worlds are close in time and space. Implicit Although previous research has examined the content and structure
comparisons assessing similarities and differences between the of counterfactual thoughts (Roese, 1994; Roese & Olson, 1995),
alternate world and reality are then made that facilitate the iden- no research to date has explored the phenomenological experience
tification of causal connections (Markman & McMullen, 2003). produced by counterfactual thinking. We argue that the mind-set
Broadly speaking, relational processing is used in counterfactual promotes a relational processing style, characterized by a tendency
thinking in that it involves the consideration of relationships and to ponder associations and make connections between a set of
connections between events. We expect that once the counterfac- stimuli. As such, we suggest that counterfactual mind-sets will
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 35

improve performance on analytic tasks, which typically require considering how this processing style might translate into a mental
that one identify and understand the relationships among a set of state, we reasoned that an individual who is primed to consider
stimuli. logical relationships should feel poised for analytic and critical
In seeking to understand the unique characteristics of the coun- thinking.
terfactual mind-set, we also explored its impact on two types of A second goal was to rule out the possibility that a third variable
creative tasks. Our hypothesis that counterfactual mind-sets pro- is responsible for the effect of counterfactual mind-sets on perfor-
mote a relational processing style suggests that they might lead to mance. Because analytic thinking is affected by mood (Schwarz &
imaginative processes that build from existing knowledge struc- Bless, 1991), it is possible that counterfactual mind-sets simply
tures. Ward (1994) termed the tendency to rely on existing knowl- depress moods. Previous research has addressed this possibility by
edge in the creative process as “structured imagination.” Although manipulating whether participants generate downward versus up-
Ward demonstrated that structured imagination is fairly character- ward counterfactuals (Roese, 1994). Downward counterfactuals, or
istic of how creative tasks are approached in general, we argue that thoughts about how events could have been worse, tend to evoke
this tendency is intensified by the activation of a counterfactual positive feelings such as joy, relief, and surprise. Upward coun-
mind-set. Just as the generation of counterfactual thoughts are terfactuals, or thoughts about how events could have been better,
structured and conform to certain rules and logic, we contend that tend to evoke negative feelings such as regret, remorse, and
imagination following the activation of a counterfactual mind-set disappointment. Because upward and downward counterfactuals
is structured around salient knowledge structures. Therefore, coun- tend to evoke different emotional experiences, the fact that the
terfactual mind-sets should have a positive effect on creative tasks valence of the counterfactual thoughts has not moderated any of
that require the identification of associations within and between a
the findings to date on counterfactual mind-sets and problem
set of stimuli. However, if a counterfactual mind-set consists of a
solving (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Kray & Galinsky, 2003)
relational processing style that structures imagination, then it
suggests that affect and counterfactual mind-sets have independent
might hinder the generation of novel ideas. This increased atten-
effects on problem solving. In the present experiment, in addition
tion to associations among task stimuli may actually decrease
to manipulating the valence of the scenario, we also measured
one’s ability to “think outside the box.”
mood to determine whether it operates as a mediator. Recognizing
Each of the findings described above is consistent with the basic
that common psychological measures of affect rely on self-
hypothesis that counterfactual mind-sets foster not just a consid-
assessments (cf. Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), we compared
eration of alternatives, as previous research has suggested, but
more precisely a consideration of relationships and connections self-reported cognitive and affective states after engaging in coun-
between or among a set of stimuli, structuring thought and imag- terfactual thinking.
ination around these associations. The present set of experiments
was conducted to test this hypothesis. In Experiment 1a and 1b, we Method
examined whether counterfactual mind-sets promote a phenome-
nological experience consistent with a relational processing style. Design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs.
In Experiment 2, we examined whether counterfactual mind-sets noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence of prime: positive vs. negative) between-
improve performance on an analytic task requiring the consider- subjects factorial design.
ation of relationships and associations. Specifically, we examined Participants. Participants were 65 students on a large western univer-
sity campus. Participants were approached in several cafes on campus and
performance on a standardized test designed to assess the analytic
asked to complete a short questionnaire. In return for their compliance,
reasoning skills of potential graduate school applicants. In Exper-
they were given a pencil bearing the university’s logo.
iments 3 and 4, we explored the impact of counterfactual mind-sets Procedure. Participants read a scenario that described the actions of
on structured imagination. In Experiment 5, we examined the Jane, a woman who was attending a rock concert (see Galinsky & Mos-
relative impact of counterfactual mind-sets on creative generation kowitz, 2000). In each scenario, an individual at the rock concert wins a
versus creative association tasks, which generally differ in the valuable prize, a trip to Hawaii. In half of the scenarios, Jane is the winner
optimal degree of conceptual attention devoted to relationships and of the prize (positive valence); in the other half of the scenarios, Jane is not
associations between task stimuli. Overall, the studies presented the winner of the prize (negative valence). In addition, half of the scenarios
here clarify the nature and phenomenology of the counterfactual describe a sequence of events designed to elicit counterfactual thoughts,
mind-set by demonstrating that activating a counterfactual mind- and half of the scenarios describe a sequence of events that is not expected
set promotes a relational processing style and therefore has pre- to elicit counterfactual thoughts. In the downward counterfactual scenario,
dictable effects, both beneficial and detrimental, on a range of Jane wins the trip to Hawaii when the new seat she had just switched to (in
problem-solving tasks. order to get a better view of the stage) was chosen. In the upward
counterfactual scenario, Jane loses the trip to Hawaii when the seat that she
had just switched from wins the trip. In the noncounterfactual conditions,
Experiment 1a: Counterfactual Mind-Sets and Mental Jane does not switch seats. After reading one of the four scenarios,
States participants were asked to consider some thoughts likely to be running
through Jane’s mind.
The purpose of Experiment 1a was twofold. First, we sought to After considering the scenario, participants were asked to “Please indi-
better understand how a counterfactual mind-set is experienced at cate the extent to which your current mental state is characterized by the
a phenomenological level. As described above, we expected that following attributes.” This measure allowed us to evaluate the degree to
the mental structure of logical relationships created through coun- which participants’ current cognitive state was consistent with a sense of
terfactual thought would promote a relational processing style. In being analytic. Specifically, participants’ cognitive states were assessed
36 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

along the following dimensions (␣ ⫽ .85): analytic, critical, focused, and read a scenario in which a negative outcome occurred (M ⫽ 3.63,
smart.1 SD ⫽ 2.7). An unanticipated two-way interaction between valence
We also sought to distinguish the impact of counterfactual thinking on of prime and type of prime also emerged, F(1, 61) ⫽ 4.17, p ⫽ .05.
cognitive states from its impact on affective states. As affective experience In the noncounterfactual condition, participants reported more
can be distinguished along valence-based and arousal-based dimensions
arousal after the positive outcome (M ⫽ 6.22, SD ⫽ 2.86) relative
(Feldman, 1995; Russell, 1980), we measured both of these affective
to the negative outcome (M ⫽ 2.96, SD ⫽ 2.54), t(31) ⫽ 3.17, p ⫽
responses separately. To measure valence-based affect, we included a
two-item mood measure (␣ ⫽ .80): mood and happy. To measure arousal- .01; however, in the counterfactual condition, the difference be-
based affect, we simply asked participants to assess their perceived arousal. tween the two outcomes (M ⫽ 4.6, SD ⫽ 2.99 vs. M ⫽ 4.4, SD ⫽
Mood was measured on a 9-point scale ranging from 1 (very negative) to 2.72) was not statistically significant, t(30) ⫽ 0.22, ns. No other
9 (very positive). All other assessments were done on 9-point scales effects emerged as statistically significant.
ranging from 1 (not at all characteristic) to 9 (very characteristic). The main purpose of this study was to provide support for our
assertion that counterfactual mind-sets create a phenomenological
Results and Discussion experience consistent with a relational processing style. Consistent
with this hypothesis, participants in the counterfactual condition
To test our hypotheses regarding the cognitive and affective reported feeling more poised for critical thinking than did baseline
states elicited by a counterfactual mind-set, we conducted 2 (type participants. After having engaged in relational processing through
of prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence of the construction of counterfactual thoughts, participants in the
prime: positive vs. negative) between-subjects analyses of variance resulting mind-set reported more affinity for analytic thinking.
(ANOVAs). Table 1 provides a correlation matrix of all experi- Another goal of this study was to bolster support for the asser-
mental variables. tion that the impact of counterfactual mind-sets on cognitive
Cognitive state. We hypothesized that the process of thinking processes operates independently of emotional experiences. The
counterfactually would elicit a sense of being analytic. Consistent fact that the counterfactual manipulation had no effect on partic-
with our hypothesis, participants in a counterfactual mind-set ipants’ reported affective state casts serious doubt on the possibil-
(M ⫽ 5.8, SD ⫽ 1.6) rated their cognitive state as being more ity that mood accounts for the relationship between counterfactual
consistent with a sense of being analytic than did participants mind-sets and relational processing. Whereas the counterfactual
exposed to a noncounterfactual scenario (M ⫽ 4.3, SD ⫽ 1.9), F(1, mind-set had no effect on the mood of participants, it had a clear
61) ⫽ 14.12, p ⬍ .001.2 No other effects emerged as statistically effect on their cognitions. Given the reasonable assumption that
significant. participants are similarly adept at assessing their cognitive and
Valence-based affective experience. Although we did not ex- affective experiences, this finding suggests that the effects of
pect our counterfactual manipulation to affect participants’ affec- counterfactual thinking on cognitive processing occur indepen-
tive experience, we explored whether the protagonist winning or dently of moods.
losing the valuable prize would affect participants’ emotional
reaction. Participants exposed to a positive outcome scenario in-
Experiment 1b: Counterfactual Mind-Sets and Thinking
deed reported a more positive emotional experience (M ⫽ 6.9,
SD ⫽ 1.5) than did participants exposed to a negative outcome Styles
scenario (M ⫽ 5.39, SD ⫽ 2.1), F(1, 61) ⫽ 8.43, p ⫽ .005. Neither In Experiment 1a, we observed that counterfactual mind-sets led
the effect for type of prime, F(1, 61) ⫽ 1.2, p ⫽ .28, nor the to self-assessed mental states consistent with a relational process-
interaction between type and valence of prime, F(1, 61) ⫽ 0.08, ing style. Because the measures we used to assess internal states
p ⫽ .78, were statistically significant. had not been independently validated, we thought it important to
Arousal-based affective experience. Like affect, we expected replicate the effect with established measures of cognitive process-
that only the valence of the outcome in the rock concert scenario ing styles. To this end, we examined the impact of our counter-
would impact participants’ perceived level of arousal. Consistent factual manipulation on Sternberg’s (1988) thinking style
with this hypothesis, a main effect for valence of prime emerged as preferences.
statistically significant, F(1, 61) ⫽ 5.57, p ⫽ .02. Participants who Sternberg proposes that abilities may be used for three distinct
read the scenario in which a positive outcome occurred (M ⫽ 5.37, functions in mental self-government, including legislative, execu-
SD ⫽ 2.97) reported feeling more aroused than participants who tive, and judicial thinking styles (O’Hara & Sternberg, 2001).
Individuals with executive styles prefer tasks that have clearly
defined structure and guidelines from which to solve problems and
Table 1 build. Individuals with legislative thinking styles prefer tasks that
Experiment 1a: Correlations Between Variables
1
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 We included several other exploratory, distractor variables that were
not theoretically relevant. As no statistically significant effects emerged for
1. Type of prime — these additional variables, they will not be discussed further.
2. Valence of prime .04 — 2
3. Cognitive state ⴚ.41 .08 — In a separate sample of 51 participants, we replicated this effect. That
4. Valence-based affective state .14 ⴚ.34 ⫺.20 — is, participants exposed to a counterfactual prime rated themselves higher
5. Arousal-based affective state ⫺.10 ⴚ.28 ⫺.20 .12 — on terms characteristic of an analytic style of thinking (M ⫽ 6.74, SD ⫽
1.06) than those exposed to the noncounterfactual prime (M ⫽ 5.88, SD ⫽
Note. Significant correlations ( p ⬍ .05) are in boldface. 1.06), F(1, 47) ⫽ 7.26, p ⬍ .01.
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 37

have little assigned structure and that allow them to invent new related to decision making. The experimental manipulations and the de-
ideas and tend to enjoy creating original works. Individuals with pendent variables were embedded in a single packet of questionnaires.
judicial styles like to evaluate and critique others’ ideas and enjoy Participants read the same rock concert scenario used in Experiment 1a,
giving feedback and advice. considered thoughts going through the main character’s mind (for approx-
imately half the participants), and then completed both thinking style and
We hypothesized that the executive thinking style preference
mood measures.
would be stronger for individuals in a counterfactual mind-set. We
Experimental manipulation. The counterfactual prime manipulations
expected that the mental structure of logical relationships created were identical to those used in Experiment 1a. We also include an addi-
through counterfactual thinking would increase preferences for the tional control condition, with no counterfactual prime and no valence. In
structured, rule-based, logical nature of the executive thinking this scenario, participants read the following: “Three weeks ago, Jane
style. bought a general admission ticket to a rock concert of her favorite band.
Another goal of the present experiment was to explore the Jane is now at the concert, which is about to begin.” Approximately half
connection between the construction of counterfactual thoughts the participants were asked simply to read the scenario as in Experiment 1a.
and the strength of the resulting mind-set. Past research has dem- The other participants were asked to “List some thoughts going through
onstrated that the counterfactual mind-set’s impact occurs regard- Jane’s mind.” The sheet on which they listed their thoughts was numbered
from 1 to 10, but participants were told to list only as many thoughts as
less of the content or valence of the counterfactual thoughts
came to their mind. Because none of the scenarios contain actual counter-
(Galinsky & Kray, 2004; Kray & Galinsky, 2003), which suggests
factual statements, but rather only possess the potential to generate coun-
that what lingers is the implicit mental structure of the counter- terfactual thoughts, the number of counterfactual thoughts that participants
factual. An established methodology for assessing the strength of subsequently listed was our measure of the strength of the counterfactual
the counterfactual mind-set involves counting the number of coun- mind-set.
terfactual thoughts generated in the pretask scenario. Because no Thinking style. Participants’ preferred thinking style was assessed us-
counterfactual is explicitly stated in the scenario, the tendency to ing a 24-item subset of Sternberg and Wagner’s (1991) Thinking Styles
infer counterfactual thoughts following the mutable versus non- Inventory. The Thinking Styles Inventory comprises three subscales, in-
mutable scenario can be assessed through this process. Kray and cluding Executive Style, Legislative Style, and Judicial Style. Participants
Galinsky (2003) observed that the strength of counterfactual acti- were asked to indicate how well each statement characterized their current
preferred approach to solving problems and making decisions using a
vation mediated the relationship between the experimental manip-
7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (extremely uncharacteristic) to 7
ulation and disconfirmatory information search. In the present
(extremely characteristic). The subscales had a high degree of reliability
experiment, we examined whether counterfactual activation (the ranging from .72 to .76.
number of counterfactual thoughts) mediated the relationship be- Mood. Mood was assessed using a one-item 9-point scale ranging from
tween type of prime and thinking styles. Finally, because we were 1 (very negative) to 9 (very positive).
interested in determining whether writing out the counterfactual
thoughts (vs. simply pondering them) intensifies the impact of the Results and Discussion
prime on thinking styles, we included this as a factor in our design.
Table 2 provides a correlation matrix for all experimental
Method variables.
Counterfactual activation. Two independent coders identified
Design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs. the number of counterfactual thoughts listed by participants. The
noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence: positive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (thought reliability for counterfactual thoughts was high (␣ ⫽ .95), and,
listing: yes vs. no) between-subjects factorial design. We also included an therefore, the ratings of the two coders were averaged. We sub-
additional no-valence control condition to establish that our valence-based
mitted the number of counterfactual thoughts to a 2 (type of prime:
control conditions serve as appropriate baseline comparisons.
Participants. Participants were 139 students from a large midwestern
counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence: positive vs.
university campus. Participants were paid $10 an hour for their negative) between-subjects factorial design. As expected, counter-
participation. factual prime participants (M ⫽ 1.78, SD ⫽ 1.1) listed signifi-
Procedure. Participants were greeted in the laboratory by an experi- cantly more counterfactual thoughts than noncounterfactual prime
menter who explained that they would complete several questionnaires participants (M ⫽ 0.19, SD ⫽ 0.42), F(1, 67) ⫽ 69.48, p ⬍ .001.

Table 2
Experiment 1b: Correlations Between Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Type of prime —
2. Valence of prime .03 —
3. Number of counterfactual thoughts .69 ⴚ.24 —
4. Executive style .21 .10 .28 —
5. Judicial style ⫺.01 .04 .03 .10 —
6. Legislative style ⫺.03 ⴚ.23 .00 ⫺.29 .34 —
7. Mood .08 ⫺.09 .08 .10 .24 .25 —

Note. Significant correlations ( p ⬍ .05) are in boldface.


38 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

Consistent with previous research (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; variables. No effects were statistically significant. We also con-
Roese & Hur, 1997), participants exposed to a negatively valenced ducted an ANOVA, with executive thinking style as the dependent
event (M ⫽ 1.25, SD ⫽ 1.2) generated more counterfactual variable, including our independent variables and the covariate
thoughts than participants exposed to a positively valenced event mood. The main effect of type of counterfactual prime remained
(M ⫽ 0.70, SD ⫽ 1.0), F(1, 67) ⫽ 7.85, p ⫽ .007. More important reliable, F(1, 113) ⫽ 4.47, p ⫽ .04. These findings suggest that
for establishing the independence of counterfactual thinking and mood did not mediate the effect of counterfactual prime on exec-
mood, however, the two-way interaction was not significant, F(1, utive thinking style.
67) ⫽ 1.79, ns. Three noteworthy findings emerged from this set of experi-
Thinking styles. To test our hypotheses, we conducted separate ments. First, the heightened sense that one was poised for analytic
2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (va- thinking (see Experiment 1a) and the greater preference for an
lence of prime: positive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (thought listing: yes vs. executive thinking style (see Experiment 1b) following the acti-
no) between-subjects ANOVAs for each thinking style. We hy- vation of a counterfactual mind-set is consistent with the idea that
pothesized that exposure to a counterfactual prime would elicit an the mind-set promotes a relational processing style. Second, the
executive thinking style. In support of our hypothesis, participants number of counterfactual thoughts was implicated as the mediating
in a counterfactual mind-set (M ⫽ 5.28, SD ⫽ 0.68) rated them- mechanism through which our experimental manipulation elicited
selves higher on executive thinking style than participants exposed a preference for an executive style of thinking (see Experiment
to a noncounterfactual scenario (M ⫽ 4.97, SD ⫽ 0.82), F(1,
1b). And finally, across both experiments, we failed to observe any
114) ⫽ 4.85, p ⫽ .03. The only other effect to emerge was a
indication that mood is a driving force behind counterfactual
tendency for participants exposed to a scenario in which the
mind-sets. This conclusion is backed by a lack of a significant
protagonist did not win the trip to Hawaii to indicate a greater
effect of the counterfactual manipulation on mood and by the fact
preference for the legislative thinking style (M ⫽ 5.38, SD ⫽ 0.81)
that mood did not reduce the effect of counterfactual primes on
than participants exposed to the positive emotional experience
executive thinking style. In addition, the observation that the
(M ⫽ 5.04, SD ⫽ 0.66), F(1, 114) ⫽ 5.74, p ⫽ .02. No other
no-valence control was identical to the valence-control conditions
effects emerged as statistically significant.
Relationship between counterfactual activation and executive gives us confidence that the remaining experiments in which
thinking style. To better understand the relationship between valence-based controls were used establish valid baseline
counterfactual mind-sets and the executive thinking style, we conditions.
examined whether the amount of counterfactual activation medi-
ated the relationship between the counterfactual primes and an Experiment 2: Counterfactual Mind-Sets and Analytical
increased executive thinking style. When we regressed executive
Reasoning
thinking style on both type of counterfactual prime and number of
counterfactual thoughts listed, the effect of number of counterfac- The demonstration that counterfactual mind-sets affect mental
tual thoughts on the outcome approached significance (␤ ⫽ .15), states in a manner consistent with a relational processing style was
t(68) ⫽ 1.77, p ⫽ .08, but the partial effect of the type of prime on
our first step in understanding the process through which counter-
the outcome was significantly reduced in magnitude once the
factual mind-sets have their impact. The second step is to explore
number of thoughts was controlled (␤ ⫽ ⫺.00), t(68) ⫽ ⫺0.01, ns.
how counterfactual mind-sets impact analytical reasoning abilities.
A Sobel test determined the reduction in the significance level was
Because the analytical process involves the examination and iden-
statistically significant (Z ⫽ 2.35, p ⬍ .05). This finding suggests
tification of logical relationships between task variables, we ex-
that the generation of counterfactual thoughts accounted for the
pected the relational processing style activated by counterfactual
relationship between the mutable scenario and thinking style
thoughts to facilitate analytical reasoning abilities.
preferences.
Supplementary control condition analyses. We also conducted To test this hypothesis, we had participants complete a version
analyses that included the additional no-valence control condition of the Law School Admission Test (LSAT) analytical reasoning
(M ⫽ 5.00, SD ⫽ 0.83) to build confidence that our counterfactual section. The LSAT analytical reasoning section assesses one’s
manipulation was responsible for the increased preference for an ability to understand and apply rules, determine relationships be-
executive thinking style. To do so, we conducted planned contrasts tween concepts, analyze situations and draw conclusions, and
comparing the two counterfactual conditions with the three control apply logic to ambiguous or complex situations (Princeton Re-
conditions (no-valence, negative outcome, positive outcome non- view, 2005). Specifically, this LSAT section consists of problems
counterfactuals), which revealed that participants in the counter- designed to “simulate the kinds of detailed analyses of relation-
factual conditions scored higher on executive thinking style than ships that a law student must perform in solving legal problems”
those in the control conditions, t(134) ⫽ 2.24, p ⬍ .05. Addition- (Law School Admission Council, 2005, p. 10). For example, one
ally, we tested whether the additional control differed from the two might be told to arrange guests at a dinner party and be given rules,
original control conditions and found no significant differences for including who may sit next to whom. These rules are followed by
their effects on executive thinking style, t(134) ⫽ 0.16, ns. several questions that examine one’s understanding of the relation-
Supplementary mood analyses. To determine whether mood ships between dinner guests. If counterfactual mind-sets promote a
was affected by our experimental manipulation, we first conducted relational processing style, then invoking a counterfactual mind-set
an ANOVA, with mood as the dependent variable and type of should be positively associated with LSAT analytical reasoning
prime, valence of prime, and thought listing as the independent performance.
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 39

Method negative emotional experience (M ⫽ 13.30, SD ⫽ 4.65), F(1,


108) ⫽ 5.53, p ⫽ .02. No other results emerged as statistically
Design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs.
significant.
noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence: positive vs. negative) between-subjects
factorial design. We also included an additional control condition in which Relationship between counterfactual activation and LSAT per-
participants simply took the LSAT exam without first reading a scenario. formance. We next examined whether the amount of counterfac-
Participants. Participants were 135 students from a large midwestern tual activation mediated the relationship between the mutable
university campus. Participants were paid $10 an hour for their primes and LSAT performance. When we regressed LSAT score
participation. on both type of counterfactual prime and number of counterfactual
Procedure. Participants were greeted in the laboratory by an experi- thoughts listed, number of counterfactual thoughts was statistically
menter who explained that they would complete several questionnaires significant (␤ ⫽ .33), t(109) ⫽ 2.82, p ⫽ .006, but not type of
related to decision making. The experimental manipulations and the de-
prime (␤ ⫽ .07), t(109) ⫽ 0.58, ns. A Sobel test determined that
pendent variables were embedded in one large packet of questionnaires.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the five experimental
the reduction in the significance level was statistically significant
conditions described in Experiment 1b. After reading one of the scenarios, (Z ⫽ 3.85, p ⬍ .001). This finding suggests that the generation of
all participants were asked to consider some thoughts likely to be running counterfactual thoughts accounted for the relationship between the
through Jane’s mind and then to indicate their current mood on a 9-point mutable scenario and performance.
scale ranging from 1 (very negative) to 9 (very positive). Finally, partici- Supplementary control condition analyses. We also conducted
pants completed one section of the LSAT analytical reasoning test. analyses that included the additional no-valence control condition
LSAT analytical reasoning problems. Each analytical reasoning sec- (M ⫽ 5.95, SD ⫽ 3.84) to build confidence that our counterfactual
tion of the LSAT has four games with 24 questions that must be accurately manipulation was responsible for the better LSAT performance.
diagrammed in order to be answered correctly. Our main dependent vari-
To do so, we conducted planned contrasts comparing the two
able was task performance. We followed LSAT scoring guidelines within
which the overall score consists of the number of questions answered
counterfactual conditions with the three control conditions, which
correctly adjusted for the number of guesses. We also measured task effort, revealed that participants in the counterfactual conditions per-
as gauged by the number of attempted answers. formed better than those in the control conditions, t(130) ⫽ 3.28,
p ⫽ .001. Additionally, we tested whether the additional control
Results and Discussion differed from the two original control conditions and found no
significant differences for their effects on LSAT performance,
Counterfactual activation. Two independent coders identified t(130) ⫽ ⫺0.18, ns.
the number of counterfactual thoughts in relation to the rock Supplementary mood analyses. We sought to determine
concert scenario. The reliability for counterfactual thoughts was whether mood played a role in the relationship between counter-
high (␣ ⫽ .95), and, therefore, the ratings of the two coders were factual mind-sets and LSAT task effort and performance. To do so,
averaged. We submitted the number of counterfactual thoughts to we first conducted an ANOVA, with mood as the dependent
a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 variable and type of prime and valence of prime as between-
(valence: positive vs. negative) between-subjects factorial design.
subjects factors. No effects were statistically significant. We also
As expected, counterfactual prime participants (M ⫽ 1.32, SD ⫽
conducted analyses of covariance using LSAT task effort (LSAT
0.66) listed significantly more counterfactual thoughts than non-
attempted) and task performance (overall LSAT score) as the
counterfactual prime participants (M ⫽ 0.29, SD ⫽ 0.54), F(1,
dependent variables, type of prime and valence as our independent
108) ⫽ 88.08, p ⬍ .001. We also observed a main effect for
variables, and mood as a covariate. For the LSAT number at-
valence of prime, with the negative outcome (M ⫽ 0.99, SD ⫽
tempted, the main effect for valence remained reliable, F(1,
0.79) generating more counterfactuals than the positive outcome
107) ⫽ 5.49 p ⬍ .05. For the overall LSAT score, the main effect
(M ⫽ 0.62, SD ⫽ 0.75), F(1, 108) ⫽ 10.61, p ⫽ .002. The
of type of prime remained reliable, F(1, 107) ⫽ 7.16, p ⫽ .009.
interaction between type and valence of prime was not significant,
These findings suggest that mood did not mediate the effect of
F(1, 108) ⫽ 0.71, p ⫽ .40.
counterfactual prime or valence on LSAT task effort or task
Task performance. To measure analytical performance, we
performance.
assessed the LSAT score using an ANOVA, which included the
One goal of the present study was to provide support for the
type of prime and valence as factors. We hypothesized that coun-
assertion that counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational pro-
terfactual mind-sets would improve analytical reasoning perfor-
cessing style, thereby facilitating the recognition and better under-
mance. Consistent with this hypothesis, counterfactual mind-set
standing of ambiguous and complex relationships. In support of
participants (M ⫽ 7.92, SD ⫽ 2.74) outperformed noncounterfac-
tual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 6.09, SD ⫽ 3.45), F(1, 108) ⫽ this hypothesis, we found that participants in a counterfactual
9.51, p ⫽ .003.3 No other effects emerged as statistically signifi- mind-set outperformed participants who were not in a counterfac-
cant for this analysis. tual mind-set on the LSAT analytic reasoning test. In combination
Task effort. To determine whether our manipulations affected with the results of Experiment 1, which demonstrated that the
task effort, we conducted an ANOVA in which the dependent
variable was the number of LSAT items attempted, and the inde- 3
We also tested our hypothesis by examining only the number of correct
pendent variables were the type of prime and valence. Results LSAT items (unadjusted for guesses). We found the same pattern of
indicated an effect for the valence of the prime such that partici- results, with participants in a counterfactual mind-set (M ⫽ 9.21, SD ⫽
pants exposed to the positive emotional experience attempted more 2.61) outperforming noncounterfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 7.71,
LSAT questions (M ⫽ 15.35, SD ⫽ 4.50) than those exposed to the SD ⫽ 3.20), F(1, 108) ⫽ 7.42, p ⫽ .008.
40 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

activation of a counterfactual mind-set was predictive of phenom- bilateral symmetry, and ordinary sensory receptors and append-
enologies and preferences consistent with a relational processing ages (Ward, 1994). On the basis of Ward’s concept of structured
style, Experiment 2 demonstrated that performance on a task imagination, we expected participants in a counterfactual mind-set
measuring one’s ability to understand logical relationships and to adopt a more structured approach to generating creative output
make connections between task stimuli is improved by counter- than under baseline conditions. Given the relational processing
factual mind-sets. style characteristic of counterfactual thinking, the resulting mind-
set should increase the tendency to structure one’s imagination
Experiment 3: Counterfactual Mind-Sets and Structured around existing knowledge structures. That is, in contrast to the
Imagination “thinking outside the box” characteristic of creative generation
processes, we expected counterfactual mind-sets to promote
Thus far, we have provided evidence consistent with the idea “thinking within the box.”
that counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational processing style
in terms of both mental states and analytical reasoning perfor-
mance. Because we expect the effects of a relational processing Method
style to be quite broad, in Experiment 3, we explore whether and Design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs.
how counterfactual mind-sets influence the tendency to build on noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence: positive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (type of
existing knowledge structures during the creative generation pro- scenario: rock concert vs. spelling bee) between-subjects factorial design.
cess, or structured imagination. Participants. Participants were 93 students from a large western uni-
Intuitively it might seem that thoughts of “if only” associated versity campus enrolled in an introductory organizational behavior course.
with counterfactual thinking would facilitate creative generation. By participating in the experiment, participants received partial credit
The construction of alternative possible worlds would seem to be toward a class requirement.
Procedure. Participants were greeted in the laboratory by an experi-
the epitome of creative generation, and it is easy to speculate that
menter who told them that they would complete several tasks predictive of
counterfactual thinking would encourage imagination to roam un- future job performance. Participants first read one version of the two prime
fettered and unencumbered by mental constraints. To the contrary, scenarios. As in previous experiments, approximately half the participants
we contend that the counterfactual mind-sets may not be the read a version of the rock concert scenario. To ensure that our results
springboard to freewheeling generation. A key insight underlying generalize to different instantiations of the counterfactual mind-set, the
this counterintuitive hypothesis concerns the fact that logical rules other half of participants read a version of a spelling bee scenario, in which
and clear structure govern when and how counterfactuals are a young boy named Paul competes to advance in the National Junior
constructed. As we have repeatedly demonstrated, counterfactual Spelling Bee. In each scenario, an individual at the spelling bee advances
mind-sets have their impact regardless of the content or direction to the next round. To manipulate valence, in half of the spelling bee
of the preceding counterfactual thoughts. This observation sug- scenarios, Paul correctly spells the assigned word and advances to the next
round, whereas in the other half, he misspells the word and is eliminated
gests that what lingers following the construction of counterfactual
from the competition. Additionally, half of the scenarios describe a se-
thoughts is their underlying logical form. We argue that the mental
quence of events designed to elicit counterfactual thoughts, whereas the
structure of logical relationships created through counterfactual other half of the scenarios describe a sequence of events not expected to
thought increases the tendency to structure thought around salient elicit counterfactual thoughts. In the downward counterfactual scenario,
associations and the pursuit of connections. Paul advances to the next round of the competition after his place in line
In addition to the conceptualization described above, the coun- is altered (because he had to use the restroom), and he is asked to spell a
terfactual mind-sets increase a preference for structured thinking, word he knows (but had he stayed in his original place in line, he would
as was empirically demonstrated in Experiment 1b. Structured have been given a word he did not know how to spell). In the upward
thinking differs from the mental states that encourage creative counterfactual scenario, Paul is eliminated from the competition after his
generation, which generally requires an expansion of conceptual place in line is altered, and he is asked to spell a word he does not know
(but had he stayed in his original place in line, he would have been given
attention that goes beyond the bounds of what is presently known
a word he knew how to spell). In the noncounterfactual conditions, he
or salient (Guilford, 1950). Some have even characterized the
either spells or misspells a word but does not alter his place in line. After
mind-set that encourages creative generation as a “risky” process- reading the scenario and listing thoughts in the protagonists’ mind, partic-
ing style (Friedman & Forster, 2001; quotations in original). Not- ipants began the creative task.
ing the connections and relationships between stimuli and making Creative task. Following Ward (1994), we asked participants to “imag-
structured associations, as counterfactual participants did with ine going to another galaxy in the universe and visiting a planet very
relative ease in Experiment 2, may inhibit the tendency to go different from earth” and to spend 7 min drawing a picture of an animal
beyond what is already known or salient, and thereby impair that is local to this planet. Immediately upon completion of the drawing
creative generation. task, participants completed a questionnaire in which they provided a short
Creative generation tasks typically elicit responses that are written description of how they went about approaching this creative task
and then evaluated their approach along several domains. Participants’
loosely defined, which creates the potential for an infinite number
evaluations of their approach to the task were used to determine the extent
of unique responses (Guilford, 1950). However, performance on
to which they engaged in structured imagination. Specifically, they indi-
creative generation tasks tends to be impaired because participants cated the extent to which they considered the following five items of
borrow from and rely too heavily on existing knowledge struc- knowledge: known science fiction creatures, general attributes of science
tures. For example, participants instructed to create creatures “be- fiction creatures, known earth animals, general attributes of earth animals,
yond their wildest imagination” tended to produce results that and consideration for the local environment. Each item was rated on a
conformed to the attributes of realistic earth creatures, including 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely).
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 41

Creative coding. Because Ward’s (1994) study suggests potential ceil- drawings and descriptions than noncounterfactual mind-set partic-
ing effects for some characteristics of the imagined creatures (e.g., bilateral ipants (M ⫽ 1.69, SD ⫽ 1.61), F(1, 85) ⫽ 4.92, p ⬍ .03. No other
symmetry) and demonstrated greater variability for atypical sensory or- effects were statistically significant.5
gans, our dependent variable for the drawings was the atypicality of the The primary goal of this study was to demonstrate that the
sensory organs.4 Three trained coders who were blind to the study hypoth-
relational processing style associated with counterfactual mind-
eses coded the drawings and descriptions for atypical sensory organs.
sets can lead to “thinking within the box.” The results of this
Following Ward’s coding, sensory organs were considered atypical if they
(a) lacked a major sensory organ (i.e., eyes, ears, nose), (b) had atypical experiment are consistent with our hypothesis. Participants in a
numbers of a sensory organ (e.g., three eyes), (c) demonstrated an unusual counterfactual mind-set reported structuring their imaginative pro-
configuration of the senses (e.g., eyes located below the nose), (d) had an cess around existing knowledge structures to a larger degree than
exaggerated or unusual ability (e.g., eyes that had laser beams), or (e) control participants. In addition, they showed evidence of consid-
served an atypical function (e.g., ears for protection). The total number of ering the local environment in constructing their drawings. Finally,
atypicalities were tallied for each participant. The codings for the drawing participants in a counterfactual mind-set incorporated fewer atypi-
were highly reliable (␣ ⫽ .93), as were the codings for the descriptions calities into their drawings and descriptions than participants in the
(␣ ⫽ .89), and discrepancies were resolved through discussion. The coders’ noncounterfactual mind-set. Together, these findings reinforce the
data were averaged to create one measure for sensory atypicality in the
notion that counterfactual mind-sets promote a form of structured
drawing and one measure for sensory atypicality in the description.
imagination.
A secondary goal of the present study was to demonstrate that
Results and Discussion
the results found in the previous studies were not driven by the
Counterfactual activation. Three independent coders identi- nature of the rock concert scenario. To do so, we included a
fied the number of counterfactual thoughts after the rock concert different scenario that involved a mutable event at a spelling bee.
and spelling bee scenarios. The reliability for both was high (␣s ⫽ Regardless of which mutable scenario participants read, counter-
.88 and .87, respectively), so the ratings were averaged. We factual mind-sets promoted a form of structured imagination. This
submitted the number of counterfactual thoughts to a 2 (type of observation gives us confidence that our results thus far generalize
prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence: posi- to different instantiations of the counterfactual mind-set.
tive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (type of scenario: rock concert vs. spelling
bee) between-subjects ANOVA. As expected, counterfactual Experiment 4: Counterfactual Mind-Sets and Creative
prime participants (M ⫽ 1.32, SD ⫽ 0.96) listed significantly more Generation
counterfactual thoughts than noncounterfactual prime participants
(M ⫽ 0.94, SD ⫽ 0.71), F(1, 85) ⫽ 4.00, p ⬍ .05. Participants In Experiment 4, we further explore whether counterfactual
exposed to a negatively valenced event (M ⫽ 1.33, SD ⫽ 0.77) mind-sets impact the creative generation process. Because the
generated more counterfactual thoughts than participants exposed drawings created in the previous experiment were highly variable,
to a positively valenced event (M ⫽ 0.93, SD ⫽ 0.90), F(1, 85) ⫽ and it was difficult to determine what aspects of science fiction
6.30, p ⫽ .01. No other effects were statistically significant. creatures counterfactual mind-sets might have been borrowing
Self-reported structured imagination. We expected partici- from, in the present experiment, we used a creative generation task
pants in the counterfactual mind-set condition to have a more that can be more easily coded for structured imagination.
structured imagination than noncounterfactual mind-set partici- In the previous experiments, participants reported relying on
pants. To test our hypothesis, we conducted a multivariate existing knowledge and examples in constructing their alien crea-
ANOVA, including each of the five statements described above, as tures. The presence of salient examples in a creative generation
well as type of prime, valence of prime, and type of scenario as task leads to less novel solutions than when examples are not
between-subjects factors. Consistent with our hypothesis, the only provided a priori (Smith, Ward, & Schumacher, 1993). If coun-
statistically significant effect to emerge was a tendency for coun- terfactual mind-sets facilitate a relational processing style that
terfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 4.49, SD ⫽ 0.77) to report seeks connections with readily available cognitive representations,
more structured imagination than noncounterfactual mind-set par- then participants in this mind-set should be more attentive to and
ticipants (M ⫽ 4.04, SD ⫽ 0.85), F(5, 80) ⫽ 3.18, p ⫽ .01. An influenced by a set of provided examples. As a result, the output of
examination of the univariate effects revealed that counterfactual participants in a counterfactual mind-set should be less novel than
mind-sets promoted a reliance on specific science fiction creatures, baseline conditions. To test this hypothesis, we examined perfor-
F(1, 84) ⫽ 4.34, p ⬍ .04; general attributes of science fiction mance on a creative generation task involving the creation of novel
creatures, F(1, 84) ⫽ 4.28, p ⬍ .05; and local environment product labels. Prior to starting the task, participants were provided
considerations, F(1, 88) ⫽ 8.18, p ⬍ .01. No other effects were with a set of examples. In the present experiment, we measured the
statistically significant. degree to which individuals were able to ignore these examples in
Creative coding. Although the self-report results support our
hypothesis, we further tested it by examining the actual drawings 4
Our data replicate this ceiling effect. For instance, 92% of the drawings
and drawing descriptions. To do so, we used a mixed-model demonstrated bilateral symmetry, 1% did not, and the remaining 7% were
ANOVA, with type of prime, valence of prime, and type of excluded from the data analysis because the drawings were profiles, and
scenario as between-subjects factors and type of coding (drawings bilateral symmetry could not be determined.
vs. descriptions) as a repeated measure. In support of our hypoth- 5
We also examined whether participants differed in terms of appendage
esis, counterfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 1.11, SD ⫽ 1.00) atypicalities in their drawings and descriptions, but no effects emerged as
were rated as incorporating fewer sensory atypicalities into their statistically significant.
42 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

their creation of labels for the new products. That is, we measured Creative generation coding. Three independent coders evaluated each
the extent to which participants’ own product labels resembled the idea generated by participants on 9-point scales ranging from 1 (not at all)
examples we provided them. to 9 (extremely) for creativeness and descriptiveness. Specifically, coders
In addition to coding for resemblance to examples, we also were asked to consider “How creative is this response?” and “How de-
scriptive is this response in revealing the type of product?”
coded the product labels on two dimensions: overall creativity and
descriptiveness of the labels in representing the product category.
Results and Discussion
In the previous experiment, participants in a counterfactual mind-
set reported that they were more likely to consider the local Creative generation. To examine the novelty of name gener-
environment in drawing their space aliens. This suggests that ation, we submitted the total number of names with endings
counterfactual mind-sets gear participants toward structuring deviating from the supplied examples to a 2 (type of prime:
imagination to reveal something about the essence of the object. If counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence of prime: pos-
this is true, then counterfactual mind-sets should produce product itive vs. negative) ⫻ 3 (product category: pasta vs. nuclear element
labels that are representative of the product. We predicted the vs. pain reliever) mixed model ANOVA, with repeated measures
counterfactual mind-set participants would both draw on the given on the third factor. The only significant effect to emerge from this
examples and be more likely to produce labels that were more analysis was a main effect for counterfactual prime, F(1, 25) ⫽
descriptive of the product. However, to the extent that the labels 4.92, p ⬍ .05. Across the three categories, counterfactual mind-set
were not judged to be particularly novel, they should suffer in participants (M ⫽ 1.26, SD ⫽ 1.44) generated significantly fewer
terms of their perceived creativity. novel names than noncounterfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽
2.70, SD ⫽ 2.20). We also conducted a similar analysis examining
the raw number of ideas generated and observed no statistically
Method significant effects, suggesting our experimental manipulation did
Overview and design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: coun- not impact effort.
terfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence of prime: positive vs. Creative versus descriptive ratings. Three independent coders
negative) between-subjects factorial design. who were blind to condition and our hypotheses evaluated each
Participants. Participants were 29 undergraduate business students idea generated by participants for creativeness and descriptiveness.
from a large western university campus enrolled in an introductory orga- Reliabilities were good (␣ ⫽ .72 and .73, respectively), so we
nizational behavior course. The experiment was conducted outside of the combined the ratings of each coder. We submitted the ratings to a
classroom setting. By participating in the experiment, partial fulfillment of 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (va-
a course requirement was granted. lence of prime: positive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (rating: creativity vs.
Procedure. Participants were greeted in the laboratory by an experi-
descriptiveness) mixed model ANOVA, with repeated measures
menter who explained that they would complete several questionnaires
on the third factor. A main effect for type of coding emerged,
related to decision making. The experimental manipulations and our de-
pendent variables were embedded in the packet of materials that partici-
indicating that labels were judged to be more descriptive (M ⫽
pants received. Participants were given up to 30 min to complete the packet 5.75, SD ⫽ 0.69) than they were creative (M ⫽ 5.09, SD ⫽ 0.76),
of materials. F(1, 25) ⫽ 8.61, p ⬍ .01. More important for testing our hypoth-
Experimental manipulations. The manipulations were identical to esis was the statistically significant two-way interaction between
those used in Experiment 1a. Because Experiment 1b revealed that the type of prime and type of coding, F(1, 25) ⫽ 4.66, p ⬍ .05.
strength of the counterfactual mind-set is not dependent on whether Counterfactual prime labels (M ⫽ 4.86, SD ⫽ 0.54) were judged
thoughts are listed, we chose not to have participants list their thoughts for to be less creative than noncounterfactual prime labels (M ⫽ 5.34,
this experiment. Instead, participants were simply asked to ponder thoughts SD ⫽ 0.90), t(27) ⫽ 1.76, p ⫽ .09; yet, counterfactual prime labels
running through the protagonist’s mind. (M ⫽ 5.97, SD ⫽ 0.59) were judged to be more descriptive than
Creative generation task. We used a modified version of the creative
noncounterfactual prime labels (M ⫽ 5.51, SD ⫽ 0.73), t(27) ⫽
generation task, described in Rubin, Stoltzfus, and Wall (1991). Partici-
1.86, p ⫽ .07. Counterfactual mind-sets led to more descriptive
pants were asked to imagine that they were interviewing with a top
marketing firm, and part of the interview involved testing their aptitude for labels that were nonetheless lacking in creativity. No other effects
business. To do so, participants were tasked with creating new labels for emerged as statistically significant for this analysis.
new products. Specifically, they were instructed to create at least one (and The results of this experiment support our hypothesis that coun-
up to three) new label for each of three categories of products (pasta, terfactual mind-sets can impair creative generation. Individuals
nuclear element, pain reliever). Six examples were provided for each who had previously pondered counterfactual thoughts generated
category, as described in Appendix A. To encourage creative output, new product labels that were less novel than individuals who had
participants were advised not to use or copy aspects of the examples not previously pondered counterfactual thoughts. This pattern
provided. emerged regardless of whether novelty was judged in terms of the
For each of the categories, the examples provided had two common similarity to the provided examples or a global evaluation of
endings, which are defined as a letter or cluster of letters that ended at least
creativity by independent coders. The fact that counterfactual
one multisyllabic word. For example, all of the examples provided of
thinking did not impact the number of names generated suggests
nuclear elements ended in _on or _ium (e.g., radon, plutonium, argon,
carbon, radium, uranium). Creative output was operationalized in terms of sheer effort was not responsible for this effect.
the number of product names created for each category that did not share Although participants were instructed to be as creative as pos-
the word endings of the examples. We also examined the sheer number of sible and to refrain from borrowing from the examples provided,
product names created for each category to determine whether motivational counterfactual mind-set participants were less effective at breaking
differences were evident. out of the mold set by the examples. In essence, the relational
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 43

processing style characteristic of the counterfactual mind-set pro- objects. In the present experiment, we sought further evidence in
moted structured imagination, or thinking within the box. How- favor of this interpretation by examining performance on another
ever, within that box, counterfactual mind-sets led to more de- creative association task, the remote associates task (RAT; M. T.
scriptive and potentially useful labels. Like participants in the Mednick, Mednick, & Mednick, 1964). The RAT requires an
previous experiment who reported considering the local environ- individual to form “mutually distant associative elements into new
ment when drawing space aliens, counterfactual mind-sets led combinations which are useful and meet specified as well as
participants to consider the essence of what the product was about. unforeseen requirements” (S. A. Mednick, 1962). Specifically, the
The finding that labels generated by counterfactual mind-set par- test requires identifying a unique association among three distinct
ticipants were actually judged to be more descriptive of the prod- words. For example, the common link for the words sore—shoul-
uct category than labels generated by control participants is con- der—sweat is cold. Similar to the Duncker candle problem, by
sistent with the hypothesis that counterfactual mind-sets promote a considering the relationships between task stimuli, performance on
relational processing style. That is, a heightened attention to the the RAT improves.
attributes generally associated with the categories for which labels In the present experiment, we used a within-subject design so
were created (i.e., pasta) led to the generation of labels that were that we could explore the relative effect of counterfactual mind-
deemed to be representative of the category by lay judges. sets on a creative generation task versus a creative association task.
We aimed to replicate the facilitative effect of the counterfactual
Experiment 5: Creative Generation Versus Creative mind-set observed previously with the Duncker candle for the
Association RAT and also demonstrate a replication of the impairing effect for
the creative generation task used in Experiment 4. By showing
We demonstrated in the previous experiment that creative gen- both facilitation and debilitation across tasks but within individu-
eration is impaired following the invocation of a counterfactual als, we aimed to provide strong evidence that the counterfactual
mind-set. Measuring creativity in terms of idea generation is but mind-set promotes a relational processing style, which has wide-
one approach, as creativity is generally regarded to be a multidi- ranging effects.
mensional construct (Amabile, 1983). The purpose of Experiment
5 was to determine whether counterfactual mind-sets might im-
prove performance on creative association tasks that involve the Method
consideration of relationships and making connections between
Overview and design. The experiment had a 2 (type of prime: coun-
disparate knowledge structures.
terfactual vs. noncounterfactual) ⫻ 2 (valence of prime: positive vs.
If counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational processing style
negative) ⫻ 2 (type of creative task: association vs. generation) mixed
involving the consideration of relationships between task stimuli, design, with repeated measures on the third factor.
then the mind-set should improve performance on creative tasks Participants. Participants were 50 undergraduate students at a mid-
involving the identification of unusual associations between stim- western university who were recruited through e-mail solicitations; they
uli, associations that are adaptive and responsive to the present participated in the experiment, along with several other unrelated tasks.
context. Galinsky and Moskowitz (2000) provided some evidence They were compensated $15 for their time.
consistent with this hypothesis with respect to the Duncker candle Procedure. Participants were greeted in the laboratory by an experi-
problem. This task gauges the ability of individuals to overcome menter who explained that they would complete several tasks assessing
functional fixedness, characterized by a failure to recognize a use ability in business contexts. Participants were given a packet of materials
for a particular object in a given context as a result of a fixation on that contained the experimental manipulations, a modified version of the
its typical use (Duncker, 1945). An individual is given a candle, a RAT (M. T. Mednick et al., 1964), and the identical creative generation
task used in Experiment 4. All participants read the prime scenarios and
box of tacks, and a book of matches and challenged with affixing
then completed the RAT first before completing the creative generative
the candle to the wall so that it can be lit without dripping wax onto
task.6 Participants were instructed to complete each task before proceeding
the floor or wall. Because the box initially functions as a container to the next one. Participants were allowed to work on each task until they
for tacks, the problem solver often fails to recognize its potential had finished or could not answer any more questions. Finally, participants
as a solution to the problem: The tacks are dumped out of the box, were debriefed and then proceeded to work on several unrelated tasks.
which is affixed to the wall with a couple of tacks, and the candle Experimental primes. The primes were the four rock concert scenarios
is placed on top of or in the box and lit. The solution to the task used in the previous experiments. As in Experiment 4, participants simply
requires the problem solver to see potential relationships other than pondered thoughts running through the protagonist’s mind without listing
the obvious ones—the box is not just a repository for tacks but can any thoughts.
also be used as stand (Glucksberg & Weisberg, 1966). In addition, The RAT (M. T. Mednick et al., 1964). The RAT is designed to
the solution involves recognizing a relationship between the candle measure the creative ingenuity of individuals and requires identifying a
and the box. Galinsky and Moskowitz (2000) showed that, by unique common denominator among three distinct words. We shortened
the original task designed by Mednick et al. to include only 10 items (see
invoking a counterfactual mind-set prior to engaging in the task,
Appendix B for the items). Consistent with our analysis of the generative
problem solvers were more likely to recognize the potential use of
the box of tacks as a platform for the candle relative to control
participants. 6
Although we did not counterbalance the order of tasks in the present
We argue that performance on the Duncker candle problem was experiment, the findings of Experiment 3 established that performance on
facilitated by the counterfactual mind-set because it promoted a the creative generation task was negatively impacted when the task imme-
tendency to explore the possible relationships between the critical diately followed the counterfactual manipulation.
44 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

cognitive task, the dependent variables for the RAT included the number of
attempted items and the number of correct items.

Results
First, because the effect of our experimental manipulations did
not vary depending on product type in the generative cognitive
task (replicating the results of Experiment 4), we collapsed across
product type in the present set of analyses. To facilitate a compar-
ison of performance across the two creative tasks, we first com-
puted z scores separately for each task and then submitted each z
score to a 2 (type of prime: counterfactual vs. noncounterfac-
tual) ⫻ 2 (valence of prime: positive vs. negative) ⫻ 2 (type of
task: creative association vs. creative generation task) mixed
model ANOVA, with repeated measures on the third factor.
Number of correct items and novel names. To measure cre-
ative performance on the two tasks, we assessed the number of
correct associations on the RAT and the number of novel names
generated on the new products task. Consistent with our hypoth-
esis, the only effect to emerge as statistically significant was the
expected two-way interaction between type of task and counter-
factual prime, F(1, 45) ⫽ 22.37, p ⬍ .001 (see Figure 1). Coun-
terfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 0.43, SD ⫽ 0.78) outper-
formed noncounterfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ ⫺0.48,
SD ⫽ 1.02) on the RAT, F(1, 49) ⫽ 12.51, p ⬍ .01, yet counter-
factual mind-set participants (M ⫽ ⫺0.34, SD ⫽ 0.68) performed
worse than noncounterfactual mind-set participants (M ⫽ 0.40,
SD ⫽ 1.18) on the creative generative task, F(1, 50) ⫽ 7.97, p ⬍
.01.7 Counterfactual mind-sets improved performance on a task
requiring the identification of a common association for a set of
words but impaired performance on a task requiring the generation
of novel ideas. No other effects emerged as statistically significant
for this analysis.
Number of items attempted and labels generated. We also
looked at task effort in terms of items attempted on the RAT and
labels generated on the new products task. The only effect to
emerge was a two-way interaction between task and counterfactual
prime that approached significance, F(1, 45) ⫽ 3.72, p ⫽ .06 (see Figure 1. Mean performance as a function of type of task and type of
prime in Experiment 5. CF ⫽ counterfactual condition; NCF ⫽ non-
Figure 1). Whereas the number of items attempted on the RAT was
counterfactual condition; RAT ⫽ remote associates task.
greater in the counterfactual condition (M ⫽ 0.36) than in the
noncounterfactual condition (M ⫽ ⫺0.40), F(1, 49) ⫽ 8.16, p ⬍
.01, the difference between the two counterfactual conditions was
not statistically significant in the creative generation task (Ms ⫽ the new products labels task. The fact that both tasks measure
⫺0.05 and 0.02, F ⬍ 1, ns). No other effects emerged as statisti- aspects of creativity suggests that the debilitating effect of coun-
cally significant for this analysis. terfactual mind-sets is limited to thinking “outside the box.”

Discussion General Discussion


The question of whether thoughts about alternate worlds borne
The results of this study provide strong support for the assertion
out of mutated pasts impact the future has important theoretical
that counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational processing style,
and applied implications. Although the adage “What’s done is
which has differential effects on creative tasks measuring the
done” may suggest that pondering the past is an unproductive use
generation of novel ideas versus the identification of associations.
of time, the present research provides strong evidence to suggest
Rather than uniformly impairing creative performance, the effect
that imagining alternatives to past realities by considering a dif-
of counterfactual mind-sets appears to depend on the underlying
creative process being assessed. Specifically, counterfactual mind-
sets improved the ability of participants to identify unusual and 7
The relationship between performance on the creative generation task
useful associations between sets of words in the RAT, yet caused and the creative association task was not statistically significant, r(49) ⫽
participants to borrow too heavily from the provided examples in ⫺.12, ns; this relationship did not depend on experimental condition.
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 45

ferent path, choice, or action has a powerful impact on how future differential emotional experiences associated with upward versus
analytic and creative problems are solved. In particular, generating downward counterfactuals appear to operate independently on
counterfactuals in one context appears to alter thought processes to subsequent thinking styles and cognitive processing.
be more relational in subsequent contexts, despite the new con- The present article is important because it is the first to our
text’s irrelevance to the imagined world. Independent of the con- knowledge that delves directly into the phenomenological experi-
tent or valence of the imagined world, the act of generating ence of a counterfactual mind-set. The findings of Experiment 1a
counterfactuals produces a lingering tendency to consider relation- and 1b suggest that counterfactuals exert a powerful impact on
ships and associations and to problem solve from within existing how individuals perceive their own cognitive state but do not
frameworks. appear to affect perceptions of affective states. Previously, re-
The present article provides a range of evidence to support the searchers have argued that counterfactual mind-sets involve a state
hypothesis that counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational pro- of heightened awareness of multiple possible worlds, thereby
cessing style. We demonstrated in Experiment 1 that counterfac- promoting mental simulations. But mental simulations brought
tual mind-sets increase a sense of being poised for analytic and about by a salient counterfactual tend not to be free-form. Instead,
critical thinking and preferences for a structured style of thinking. they follow systematic laws of mutability that involve tweaking
Evidence that the counterfactual mind-set improves performance particular aspects of the counterfactual context to undo a known
on an analytic task involving the assessment of relationships outcome, thereby promoting a consideration of cause– effect rela-
between task variables was provided in Experiment 2. We dem- tionships. The present research suggests that counterfactual think-
onstrated in Experiments 3 and 4 that counterfactual mind-sets ing primes a relational processing style in subsequent contexts that
increase the tendency to structure imagination around existing facilitate the examination of the relationship between clues, cues,
knowledge structures, leading to more descriptiveness but less examples, and props embedded within a problem-solving task.
novelty. Finally, using a within-subject design, we demonstrated in The observation that the counterfactual mind-set promotes a
Experiment 5 that the heightened tendency to build on existing relational processing style sheds light on one detrimental, yet
knowledge structures following the activation of the counterfactual seemingly anomalous, effect resulting from its activation. Specif-
mind-set leads to better performance on a creative association task ically, counterfactual mind-sets have been shown to impair per-
involving the consideration of associations between task stimuli formance on the Wason (1966) card selection task involving four
but to worse performance on a creative generation task. cards, each bearing a symbol: “E, K, 4, 7” (Byrne & Tasso, 1994;
The counterfactual thought-listing methodology used in Exper- Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). The challenge is to determine what
iments 1b and 2 also shed light on the process through which cards must be turned over to determine whether the following
counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational processing style. In conditional statement is true: “If a card has a vowel on one side,
Experiment 1b, we demonstrated that the effect of counterfactual then it has an even number on the other side.” Both sets of
mind-sets on executive thinking style preferences was mediated by researchers independently found that counterfactual thinking im-
counterfactual activation. In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that paired performance by leading participants to incorrectly select the
the listing of counterfactual thoughts mediated the relationship “4” card. Because the conditional statement to be tested is not
between the mutable prime and LSAT performance. In combina- bidirectional, the selection of this card is an error of commission.
tion with previous research in which a similar methodology was The conditional statement central to the task can be misinterpreted
used (Galinsky & Kray, 2004; Kray & Galinsky, 2003), the to imply a bidirectional hypothesis (Byrne & Tasso, 1994). We
amount of counterfactual activation has proved to be a robust contend that it is the consideration of the relationship and connec-
mediating mechanism in the relationship between mutable primes tions between the antecedent and consequent characteristic of a
and performance. The greater the number of counterfactual relational processing style that promoted this misguided tendency
thoughts generated following exposure to a scenario in which an to entertain two hypotheses at once. The fact that counterfactual
event almost turned out differently, the more ingrained the mental mind-set participants did not incorrectly solve the problem because
structure of logical relationships becomes, thus increasing prefer- of a failure to select the potentially falsifying “E” and “7” cards or
ences for structure and performance on analytical tasks. incorrectly selecting the irrelevant “K” card further supports this
Counterfactual primes influence not just what we think but how explanation.
we think. The present experiments consistently demonstrated that We have claimed that counterfactual thinking elicits a particular
what we think, as determined by the direction of the counterfactual cognitive style characterized by a consideration of relationships
elicited (upward or downward), did not moderate any of the effects and associations. The idea that cognitive styles can differentially
of how we think, as determined by the mutable nature of the prime. affect performance on tasks that require focus from those that
Across two experiments (1a and 2), we also demonstrated that the require flexibility is supported by the theorizing and data of
valence-based conditions produced results identical to both a base- Peterson and Nemeth (1996). These researchers found that minor-
line condition without a pretask scenario and a pretask scenario in ity influence encourages flexible thinking and improves perfor-
which no valence-based event occurs. This finding suggests that mance on tasks in which flexibility is an asset but that majority
the valence of the outcome did not impact the results. Finally, the influence can aid performance on tasks that require focusing on
fact that measurements of mood did not differ across our counter- one dimension of a two-dimension task (e.g., the Stroop task). We
factual prime manipulation across multiple experiments gives us are not claiming that counterfactual thinking is akin to majority
more confidence that it is the process of thinking counterfactually, influence but rather suggesting that the work by Peterson and
and not the content of the counterfactuals, that is responsible for Nemeth is a useful demonstration that cognitive processing styles
the observed effects. In total, counterfactual mind-sets and the can have a differential influence, depending on whether a task
46 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

requires flexibility, focus, or consideration of task-embedded brought about by “what-if” scenarios may moderate the relation-
alternatives. ship between counterfactual mind-sets and a relational processing
style.
Limitations and Future Directions Another possible direction for future research is to explore
whether different types of counterfactual thoughts have differential
The research presented here shows a uniformly negative impact effects on relational processing. A particularly important distinc-
of counterfactual mind-sets on creative generation tasks. However, tion may be the additive versus subtractive nature of the counter-
one limitation of the studies presented here is that they only factual. Additive counterfactuals refer to an action that may have
explore one aspect of the creative generation process. Creative been taken to create an alternate world, whereas subtractive coun-
generation can be broken down into fluency, or the number of terfactuals refer to an action that may not have been taken to create
ideas generated, flexibility, or the number of different categories of an alternate world (Roese, Hur, & Pennington, 1999). Although
ideas represented, and novelty, or the uniqueness of the idea counterfactuals generally aid in making causal judgments, Roese et
generated (Amabile, 1983). Perhaps counterfactual mind-sets dif- al. demonstrated that additive counterfactuals more often express
ferentially affect these components of creativity. The experiments causal sufficiency, whereas subtractive counterfactuals more often
presented here provide some evidence that counterfactual mind- express causal necessity (McGill, 1998; McGill & Klein, 1993). In
sets decrease novelty, as name generation was more constrained by the present research, the most common reaction to the rock concert
the suffixes of the examples after counterfactual primes. Although scenario was “If only she had not switched seats. . .,” which
no differences were observed in fluency, or the quantity of names expresses a subtractive counterfactual. If the causal necessity as-
generated overall in Experiments 4 and 5, this null finding may be sociated with subtractive counterfactuals creates a closer associa-
a reflection of the fact that the number of names generated had a tion between the antecedent and consequent events than the causal
limited range (from 1 to 3). The new product labels task also did sufficiency associated with additive counterfactuals, then the re-
not allow for a clear gauge of flexibility, or the number of defin- sulting relational processing style may be more strongly activated
able categories of names generated. Given that counterfactual for the former type of counterfactual than the latter. One conse-
mind-sets promote a relational processing style that involves the quence of this possibility is that additive counterfactuals may be
consideration of relationships and associations, fluency and flex- more beneficial for creative generation than subtractive counter-
ibility may actually be facilitated following the mind-set’s factuals have proved to be.
activation. Given the powerful effect counterfactual thinking has been
The observation that new product names generated by counter- shown to have on a wide variety of problem-solving tasks, it is
factual mind-set individuals were judged to be more descriptive of important to consider the implications of these findings. For ex-
the product category than the names generated by noncounterfac- ample, an important practical consideration is how the use of a
tual mind-set individuals suggests a practical approach to idea counterfactual mind-set as a debiasing technique stacks up against
generation when in a counterfactual mind-set. By broadening the other procedures for promoting analytic thinking. Procedures that
examination to consider the impact of counterfactual thinking on encourage decision makers to “consider the opposite” (Lord, Lep-
innovation, which involves both the generation of novel ideas and per, & Preston, 1984; Mussweiler, Strack, & Pfeiffer, 2000) or
their successful implementation (Amabile, Conti, Coon, Lazenby, assign group members the role of a “devil’s advocate” (Cosier,
& Herron, 1996), researchers may find that the counterfactual 1978; Janis & Mann, 1977) work by explicitly directing decision
mind-set facilitates the pragmatic process of turning a novel idea makers to become more critical. However, no explicit training or
into reality by modeling the execution process after previously assignment of roles is required when a counterfactual mind-set is
successful ventures. subtly activated. Perhaps the activation of a counterfactual mind-
Future research that explores different instantiations of counter- set to promote analytic thinking may be advantageous in delicate
factual mind-sets is needed. In all of the experiments in this article, situations in which it is particularly important to avoid the appear-
our manipulation involved the presentation of a scenario that ance of a heavy hand guiding the decision-making process. Like-
resulted in the spontaneous generation of counterfactual thoughts wise, because blatant attempts to restrict an individual’s freedom
resulting from the presence of a salient mutable component. Indi- often provoke reactance (Brehm, 1966), the counterfactual mind-
viduals constructed counterfactual thoughts without any direction set approach to guiding decision-making processes may be more
or guidance from the experimenter. Whereas the results of this set readily embraced by decision makers than the more directive
of experiments show consistent effects, it is important to consider approaches described above.
their boundaries. For example, other research has explored the
effect of counterfactual thinking after encouraging individuals to Conclusion
imagine the implications of various “what-if” scenarios of how the
past could have played out differently (cf. Tetlock & Lebow, 2001) The present set of experiments provides evidence across multi-
or prefactual considerations of what may be (Gleicher, Boninger, ple domains that counterfactual mind-sets promote a relational
Strathman, Armor, Hetts, & Ahn, 1995). Mind-sets resulting from processing style, which is characterized by a tendency to consider
these approaches, which more directly focus thought in an explor- relationships and associations among a class of stimuli and to
atory, imaginative direction, may have a beneficial effect on the structure thought and imagination around those associations. As a
subsequent generation of novel ideas. Instead of promoting think- result, performance on analytic and creative tasks requiring the
ing within the box, they may actually facilitate out-of-the-box identification of logical relationships and associations is aided, but
thinking. Whether the counterfactual is spontaneously generated or performance on creative tasks requiring the generation of novel
COUNTERFACTUAL MIND-SETS 47

ideas matters is hindered. On a general level, the present research Krishnamurthy, P., & Sivaraman, A. (2002). Counterfactual thinking and
suggests that reflecting back on events in which an outcome almost advertising responses. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 650 – 658.
turned out differently and mentally constructing an alternate world Law School Admission Council. (2005).Preparing for the LSAT. Retrieved
can impact how future problems are approached. More specifi- April 11, 2006, from http://www.lsac.org/pdfs/2006 –2007/TestPrep06.pdf
Liljenquist, K. A., Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2004). Exploring the
cally, thinking about what may have been can prevent one from
rabbit hole or possibilities by myself or with my group: The benefits and
creating novel ideas but can lead that same person to notice hidden
liabilities of activating counterfactual mind-sets for information sharing
connections. Simply put, counterfactual mind-sets promote think- and group coordination. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17,
ing within the box. 263–279.
Lord, C. G., Lepper, M. R., & Preston, E. (1984). Considering the opposite:
References A corrective strategy for social judgment. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 47, 1231–1243.
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componen- Mandel, D. R. (2003). Judgment dissociation theory: An analysis of dif-
tial conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
ferences in causal, counterfactual, and covariational reasoning. Journal
357–376.
of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 419 – 434.
Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. (1996).
Mandel, D. R., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Counterfactual thinking and
Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of Management
ascriptions of cause and preventability. Journal of Personality and
Journal, 39, 1154 –1184.
Social Psychology, 71, 450 – 463.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York:
Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation
Academic Press.
model of comparative thinking. Personality and Social Psychology
Byrne, R. M. J., & Tasso, A. (1994, August). Counterfactual reasoning:
Review, 7, 244 –267.
Inferences from hypothetical conditional. In A. Ram & K. Eiselt (Eds.),
McGill, A. L. (1998). Relative use of necessity and sufficiency information
Proceedings of the 16th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science
in causal judgments about natural categories. Journal of Personality and
Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Social Psychology, 75, 70 – 81.
Cosier, R. A. (1978). The effects of three potential aids for making
McGill, A. L., & Klein, J. G. (1993). Contrastive and counterfactual
strategic decisions on prediction accuracy. Organizational Behavior and
reasoning in causal judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Human Decision Processes, 22, 295–306.
chology, 64, 897–905.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58,
Mednick, M. T., Mednick, S. A., & Mednick, E. V. (1964). Incubation of
270.
creative performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Ab-
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1986). Judging probable cause. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 99, 3–19. normal and Social Psychology, 69, 84 – 88.
Feldman, L. A. (1995). Valence focus and arousal focus: Individual dif- Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.
ferences in the structure of affective experience. Journal of Personality Psychological Review, 69, 220 –232.
and Social Psychology, 69, 153–166. Merriam-Webster. (2006). Merriam-Webster online dictionary. Retrieved
Friedman, R. S., & Forster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and April 11, 2006, from http://www.m-w.com/dictionary/analysis.
prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Mussweiler, T., Strack, F., & Pfeiffer, T. (2000). Overcoming the inevita-
chology, 81, 1001–1013. ble anchoring effect: Considering the opposite compensates for selective
Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2004). From thinking about what might accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1142–
have been to sharing what we know: The effects of counterfactual 1150.
mind-sets on information sharing in groups. Journal of Experimental O’Hara, L. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). It doesn’t hurt to ask: Effects of
Social Psychology, 40, 606 – 618. instructions to be creative, practical, or analytical on essay-writing
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Counterfactuals as behavioral performance and their interaction with students’ thinking styles. Cre-
primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alterna- ativity Research Journal, 13, 197–210.
tives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 257–383. Peterson, R. S., & Nemeth, C. J. (1996). Focus versus flexibility: Majority
Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., Hetts, J., & Ahn, and minority influence can both improve performance. Personality and
M. (1995). With an eye toward the future: The impact of counterfactual Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 14 –23.
thinking on affect, attitudes, and behavior. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson Princeton Review. (2005). The LSAT in detail. Retrieved May 3,
(Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual 2006, from http://www.princetonreview.com/law/testprep/testprep.asp?
thinking (pp. 283–304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. TPRPAGE⫽87&TYPE⫽LSAT-SECTIONS
Glucksberg, S., & Weisberg, W. R. (1966). Verbal behavior and problem Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking.
solving: Effects of labeling in a functional fixedness problem. The Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805– 818.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 659 – 664. Roese, N. J., & Hur, T. (1997). Affective determinants of counterfactual
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444 – 454. thinking. Social Cognition, 15, 274 –290.
Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Emergency decision making: A theoretical Roese, N. J., Hur, T., & Pennington, G. L. (1999). Counterfactual thinking
analysis of responses to disaster warnings. Journal of Human Stress, 3, and regulatory focus: Implications for action versus inaction and suffi-
35– 48. ciency versus necessity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to 77, 1109 –1120.
its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136 –153. Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Functions of counterfactual thinking.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psy- In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), The social psychology of counter-
chologist, 28, 107–128. factual thinking (pp. 169 –197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2003). The debiasing effect of counterfac- Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: The inter-
tual mind-sets: Increasing the search for disconfirmatory information in section of affect and function. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
group decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 1–59). New York: Aca-
cesses, 91, 69 – 81. demic Press.
48 KRAY, GALINSKY, AND WONG

Rubin, D. C., Stoltzfus, E. R., & Wall, K. L. (1991). The abstraction of Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1991). MSG thinking styles inventory
form in semantic categories. Memory & Cognition, 19, 1–7. manual. Unpublished manuscript.
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality Tetlock, P. E., & Lebow, R. N. (2001). Poking counterfactual holes in
and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. covering laws: Cognitive styles and historical reasoning. American Po-
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and litical Science Review, 95, 829 – 843.
smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In J. P. Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of category structure
Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments: International series in in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 1– 40.
experimental social psychology (pp. 55–71). Elmsford, NY: Perga- Wason, P. C. (1966). Reasoning. In B. Foss (Ed.), New horizons in
mon Press. psychology (pp. 135–151). Middlesex, England: Penguin.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory & Cognition, dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
21, 837– 845. scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Mental self-government: A theory of intellectual Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality.
styles and their development. Human Development, 31, 197–224. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 161–169.

Appendix A

Examples Provided in Creative Generation Task Used in Experiments 4 and 5

(1) Please generate a name for: a new pasta Examples: radon, plutonium, argon, carbon, radium, uranium

Examples: spaghetti, lasagna, fettuccini, rotini, pastina, rigatoni (3) Please generate a new name for: an analgesic (pain reliever)

(2) Please generate a name for: a nuclear element Examples: Tylenol, Anacin, aspirin, bufferin, panadol, Midol

Appendix B

Modified Version of the Remote Associates Task

Please identify a common word that links each set of 3 words together. (6) chocolate—fortune—tin (cookie)
You should answer as many questions as you can.
Example: sore—shoulder—sweat Answer: cold (7) barrel—root— belly (beer)

(1) blank—white—lines ( page) (8) broken— clear— eye (glass)

(2) magic—plush—floor (carpet) (9) pure— blue—fall (water)

(3) thread—pine—pain (needle) (10) widow— bite—monkey (spider)

(4) stop—petty—sneak (thief) (Note: Answers appear in parentheses.)

(5) envy— golf— beans (green)

Received June 17, 2005


Revision received September 28, 2005
Accepted September 29, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 49 – 62 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.49

Self-Regulatory Processes Defend Against the Threat of Death:


Effects of Self-Control Depletion and Trait Self-Control
on Thoughts and Fears of Dying

Matthew T. Gailliot Brandon J. Schmeichel


Florida State University Texas A&M University

Roy F. Baumeister
Florida State University

Nine studies (N ⫽ 979) demonstrated that managing the threat of death requires self-regulation. Both trait
and state self-control ability moderated the degree to which people experienced death-related thought and
anxiety. Participants high (vs. low) in self-control generated fewer death-related thoughts after being
primed with death, reported less death anxiety, were less likely to perceive death-related themes in
ambiguous scenes, and reacted with less worldview defense when mortality was made salient. Further,
coping with thoughts of death led to self-regulatory fatigue. After writing about death versus a control
topic, participants performed worse on several measures of self-regulation that were irrelevant to death.
These results suggest that self-regulation is a key intrapsychic mechanism for alleviating troublesome
thoughts and feelings about mortality.

Keywords: self-regulation, mortality salience, death anxiety, thought suppression, terror management

The thought of death can be frightening. As a consequence, demand and consume some of an individual’s limited resources for
people prefer to avoid thinking about death (e.g., Aries, 1981; self-regulation (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000).
Becker, 1973). By what means are people capable of avoiding
thoughts of death? The current work assesses the role of self-
regulation in minimizing thoughts and anxiety surrounding death. Self-Regulation: Trait and State Differences
Self-regulation (or self-control) is the capacity to override one’s
Some individuals are highly adept at self-regulation whereas
thoughts, feelings, and habitual patterns of behavior. Self-
others are not, and these individual differences in trait self-control
regulation is a highly adaptive capacity that facilitates success in
have been associated with a diverse range of behavior. For exam-
myriad domains of life, including interpersonal relationships, ac-
ple, compared with those lower in trait self-control, people higher
ademic achievement, and coping with and adjusting to stress (e.g.,
in trait self-control are better at coping with anxiety and other
Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994; Mischel, Shoda, & Peake,
negative moods, avoiding addictive behaviors, and responding to
1988; Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990; Tangney, Baumeister, &
other people in prosocial, constructive ways (e.g., Finkel & Camp-
Boone, 2004). Awareness of mortality creates the potential for
bell, 2001; Mischel et al., 1988; Tangney et al., 2004). The diverse
stress (e.g., see Greenberg et al., 2003; Pollak, 1979), and so it
and substantial benefits of high trait self-control underscore the
seems plausible that one benefit of self-regulation may be to
idea that self-regulation is a valuable tool in many aspects of life.
minimize death-related thoughts and anxiety. Hence our central
Self-regulation can vary not just as trait but also as state. For any
hypotheses in this investigation were (a) that low capacity for
individual, self-regulatory success is more likely at some times
self-regulation, either as state or trait, would increase vulnerability
than at others. Recent work has suggested that self-regulation
to disturbing thoughts and feelings about death and (b) that coping
relies upon a limited resource or strength (for reviews, see Gailliot
with thoughts of death, like other acts of self-control, should
& Baumeister, in press; Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). Effortful
acts of self-regulation appear to consume or deplete this limited
resource, thereby impairing later attempts at self-regulation. For
Matthew T. Gailliot and Roy F. Baumeister, Department of Psychology, instance, participants in one study who performed a task that
Florida State University; Brandon J. Schmeichel, Department of Psychol- required self-control (i.e., suppressing or exaggerating responses to
ogy, Texas A&M University. an emotional film) were less able to exert self-control on a sub-
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant MH
sequent task (i.e., squeezing a handgrip) than were participants
65559. Thanks to Lauren Brewer, Shenika Thomas, Savana Carroll, and
Daniel Donaldson for helping to conduct the studies reported here.
who first completed a task that did not require self-control (i.e.,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Matthew watching the film without regulating emotional responses; Mu-
T. Gailliot or Roy F. Baumeister, Department of Psychology, Florida State raven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998). Presumably, the initial act of
University, One University Way, Tallahassee, FL 32306-1270. E-mail: self-control temporarily depleted self-regulatory strength, thereby
gailliot@psy.fsu.edu or baumeister@psy.fsu.edu impairing subsequent self-regulated performance.

49
50 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

Thus, the ability to self-regulate may differ both across individ- A second way that self-regulation might facilitate the manage-
uals (trait self-control) and within individuals across time (self- ment of concerns about death is the regulation of emotion. An
control depletion). The current work examined whether poor trait individual who capably controls his or her emotions is likely to
and state self-regulation, respectively, would hamper the manage- experience less of the fear and anxiety associated with death.
ment of concerns about death and whether the management of Emotion regulation requires self-control, and therefore both low
concerns about death would hamper self-regulation. trait self-control and self-control depletion undermine emotion
regulation (e.g., Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998;
Self-Regulation and the Management of Mortality Finkel & Campbell, 2001; Muraven et al., 1998; Tangney et al.,
Concerns 2004). Consequently, low trait self-control and self-control deple-
tion should be associated with increased emotional preoccupation
The present work is based on the assumption that the idea of with death. In sum, we posited that self-regulation should facilitate
death is threatening and can evoke aversive thoughts and feelings. suppressing thoughts about death and, to some extent, regulating
There are at least two ways in which self-regulation might facili- emotional responses to concerns about death.
tate the defense against these unwanted states. The first is thought Another potential basis for predicting a link between self-
control. Research indicates that people cope with thoughts of death regulation and death-thought accessibility, however, involves self-
and minimize preoccupation with death in part by suppressing esteem. Terror management theory has suggested that self-esteem
such thoughts or redirecting their thoughts away from death (e.g., is primarily a defense against death, so chronic low self-esteem
Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Simon, & Breus, 1994; could leave people more vulnerable to thoughts and anxieties
Harmon-Jones et al., 1997; Pollak, 1979). For instance, on explicit about death (e.g., see Pyszczynski, Greenberg, Solomon, Arndt, &
measures of mortality concerns (e.g., measures of death anxiety), Schimel, 2004). The present work is not concerned with self-
most people score relatively low. On more implicit measures (e.g., esteem per se but with the possibility that self-esteem could
word-association tests), however, people score significantly higher account for the effects of self-regulation. Trait self-esteem and trait
(e.g., Feifel & Branscomb, 1973; Pollak, 1979). This suggests that self-control are positively correlated (Tangney et al., 2004), hence
people possess an implicit awareness of death and that such it is theoretically possible that any effects of self-control are due to
thoughts are suppressed from explicit awareness. self-esteem. It is also plausible that high self-esteem results from
Moreover, suppressing thoughts about death requires effortful, having more effective self-control, so people with high self-esteem
controlled processing (Greenberg et al., 1994; Harmon-Jones et al., might be less vulnerable to self-control depletion. To test these
1997; Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999; see also Weg- possibilities, we assessed the role of self-esteem.
ner, 1994). For example, immediately after thinking about mortal-
ity, one’s death thoughts are suppressed and are less accessible to Research Overview
awareness (e.g., Arndt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Si-
mon, 1997; Greenberg et al., 1994; Harmon-Jones et al., 1997). If In nine studies, using both correlational and experimental meth-
cognitive resources are diverted by a concurrent task, however, ods, we tested whether self-regulation facilitates the management
death thoughts remain highly accessible to awareness (Arndt et al., of mortality concerns. We first examined whether poor self-
1997; Greenberg, Arndt, Schimel, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, regulation would hamper the management of mortality concerns.
2001). Apparently the cognitive load diverts the resources needed Our first studies tested the prediction that people with low (vs.
to suppress death thoughts, so death thoughts remain highly ac- high) trait self-control would be more prone to come up with
cessible (see Smart & Wegner, 1999; Wegner & Zanakos, 1994; death-related thoughts in response to ambiguously evocative stim-
Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). uli (Studies 1A and 1B) and would have higher death anxiety
Low trait self-control and depleted self-control strength might (Study 1C). Next, we manipulated temporary self-regulatory ca-
also undermine the suppression of death thoughts. Thought sup- pacity by having participants complete either an initial task that
pression requires self-control (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1994; Weg- required self-regulation or one that did not require self-regulation.
ner, 1994), and so dispositionally low or temporarily depleted To the extent that exercising self-control depletes the capacity for
self-control should be linked to poorer suppression, leading to self-regulation (e.g., see Muraven & Baumeister, 2000), we ex-
increased accessibility of death-related thoughts and anxiety. Ef- pected to find greater death-thought accessibility (Study 2) and a
fortfully suppressing a thought reduces both its explicit and im- greater proclivity to perceive death-related themes in an ambigu-
plicit accessibility (Anderson & Green, 2001; MacLeod, 1989; ous stimulus (Study 3) among participants who had (vs. had not)
McBride & Dosher, 1997), and disrupting controlled efforts to- previously expended their self-regulatory resources. Subsequently,
ward suppressing death thoughts increases the implicit accessibil- we assessed (rather than manipulated) self-control depletion and
ity of those thoughts (e.g., Arndt et al., 1997; Pyszczynski et al., death-thought accessibility and death anxiety (Study 4). In the final
1999). Low self-control should therefore be associated with in- study in this group (Study 5), we sought to show that low trait
creased accessibility of both implicit and explicit death thoughts. self-control would leave people more vulnerable to the threat of
Likewise, thought suppression depletes self-control strength mortality salience and would result in a heightened tendency to
(e.g., Gordijn, Hindriks, Koomen, Dijksterhuis, & Van Knippen- respond to that threat by bolstering support for a political leader
berg, 2004; Muraven et al., 1998). After thinking about death, (Landau et al., 2004). Such an externalizing response would pre-
people suppress thoughts about death. Self-regulatory resources sumably indicate that the inner defense mechanisms against death
should be depleted as a result. Self-regulation should therefore be were inadequate.
impaired shortly after people think about and suppress thoughts of Having thus shown the effects of poor self-regulation on mor-
death. tality defenses, we turned this around to investigate the effects of
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 51

mortality defense on self-regulation. If (as argued above) self-


regulation consumes a limited resource and defending against the
threat of death consumes some of that resource then people’s
capacity for self-regulation should be diminished after they defend
against such a threat. Participants in the last four studies wrote
about either death or a control topic. We predicted that writing
about death would activate defensive processes that would deplete
self-regulatory resources, as indicated by poorer subsequent per-
formance on the Stroop task (Study 6), impaired logical reasoning
abilities (Study 7), reduced success at solving anagrams (Study 8),
and less effortful persistence on word puzzles (Study 9).

Studies 1A–1C
In Studies 1A–1C we relied on measurement of trait self-control
to test the hypothesis that weaker self-control would be associated
with greater susceptibility to thoughts about death. We measured
death-thought accessibility in different ways in each of these
studies. In Study 1A we used ambiguous word puzzles that could
be solved with either death-related or death-irrelevant words (e.g.,
grave, grape). We believed that greater accessibility of death
thoughts among persons low in self-control would be reflected in
higher rates of death-related word solutions. In a similar vein, in
Study 1B we showed participants an ambiguous visual stimulus
that could be construed as either related or unrelated to death. We
then assessed spontaneous thoughts to determine the frequency of
death-related thoughts the image evoked. In Study 1C we relied on
a straightforward measure of death anxiety (Templer, 1970) to
ascertain whether low self-control would be associated with
greater fear related to death.

Method
Participants. Participants in these and all subsequent studies were
undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology courses who
received either extra credit or credit toward fulfilling a course requirement. Figure 1. “All Is Vanity” by Charles Allan Gilbert. This picture, shown
The size and gender makeup of the samples in Studies 1A–1C were as to participants in Study 1B, could be interpreted as either a woman or a
follows: Study 1A (N ⫽ 279; 166 women, 113 men), Study 1B (N ⫽ 13; skull.
12 women, 1 man), and Study 1C (N ⫽ 163; 118 women, 45 men).
Materials and procedures. At the start of the academic semester,
participants completed measures of trait self-control (the Self-Control related words used to complete the relevant word fragments to serve as a
Scale; Tangney et al., 2004) and self-esteem (the Rosenberg Self-Esteem measure of implicit death-thought accessibility (see Greenberg et al.,
Scale; Rosenberg, 1965) during a mass-testing session. We used the brief 1994).1
version of the Self-Control Scale, which contains 13 items (e.g., “I have a The questionnaire packet completed by participants in Study 1B in-
hard time breaking bad habits” [reverse scored]; “I am good at resisting cluded a measure of explicit death-thought accessibility. Specifically, these
temptation”) answered on a scale from 1 (not at all like me) to 5 (very much participants were shown a picture that could be interpreted as depicting
like me). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale contains 10 items (e.g., “On the either (a) a woman sitting in front of a mirror while putting on makeup or
whole, I am satisfied with myself.”) answered on a scale from 1 (strongly (b) a skull (see Figure 1). Participants were instructed to list the first 10
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores on these measures indicate words that came to mind as they viewed the image. A judge who was blind
higher self-control and self-esteem, respectively. to trait self-control scores determined which thoughts on the thought-listing
Participants in Study 1C also completed the Social Interaction Anxiety task were related to death (e.g., skull and skeleton). The number of thoughts
Scale (Mattick & Clarke, 1998), as a measure of social anxiety, and the related to death constituted the measure of explicit death-thought
State–Trait Anxiety Index (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970), as a accessibility.
measure of general trait anxiety. These measures were used to control for The questionnaire packet completed by participants in Study 1C con-
general negative affect. tained the Death Anxiety Scale (Templer, 1970), which includes 15 items
Studies 1A–1C were conducted 4 –12 weeks after the initial mass-testing (e.g., “I am very much afraid to die” and “I fear dying a painful death”)
session. In each study, participants completed questionnaire packets that
contained the focal materials along with other, non-death-related measures.
1
In Study 1A, the packet included a list of 20 word fragments, some of Participants completed a list that contained either 5 or 7 word frag-
which could be completed with death-related thoughts (e.g., sk_ll and ments that could be solved with death-related thoughts. Accordingly, the
gra_e could be solved with the words skull and grave or skill and grape, number of death-related thoughts was standardized among participants
respectively). We counted and then standardized the number of death- who completed each list.
52 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

answered as either “true” or “false.” Higher scores indicate greater death suppression requires self-control and has been shown to deplete
anxiety. self-regulatory strength (Muraven et al., 1998).
In these and all subsequent studies, participants were last thanked and Following this initial task, participants completed the same
fully debriefed. Because of the threatening nature of death, we were careful measure of implicit death thoughts used in Study 1A (word frag-
to explain the importance of and need for the research and to assuage any
ment completion). We predicted that participants in the depletion
fears or concerns participants may have had. In this fashion, we tried to
condition would complete the word fragments with death-related
ensure that no participant left feeling distressed.
words more often than would participants in the no-depletion
condition, indicating greater accessibility of death-related thoughts
Results when self-regulatory capacity had been reduced.
In each study, higher trait self-control was associated with less
preoccupation with death. Trait self-control correlated negatively Method
with the number of word fragments completed with death-related
Participants. Participants were 19 undergraduates (13 women, 6 men)
words, r(279) ⫽ ⫺.13, p ⬍ .05 (Study 1A), the number of death who completed the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale during a mass-testing
thoughts listed to the ambiguous image, r(13) ⫽ ⫺.59, p ⬍ .05 session at the start of the semester. The present study occurred approxi-
(Study 1B), and death anxiety, r(163) ⫽ ⫺.36, p ⬍ .01 (Study 1C). mately 3 months later. Participants were run individually and told the study
Further analyses indicated that each relationship remained sig- was to investigate impression formation and thought patterns.
nificant when controlling for self-esteem (all ps ⱕ .05) and that Procedure. The first task served as the manipulation of self-regulatory
controlling for self-esteem did not change the strength of any resources. Specifically, participants completed a thought-recording exer-
relationship (all ps ⬎ .11). Self-esteem thus did not appear to cise (borrowed from Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). Partici-
account for the relationship between self-control and mortal pants were given a sheet of paper and asked to write down all of their
thoughts for 5 min. Participants randomly assigned to the no-depletion
concern.
condition received no additional instructions. Participants assigned to the
Moreover, the relationship between self-control and death anx-
depletion condition were instructed further not to think about a white bear.
iety in Study 1C remained significant when controlling for social Each time they did happen to think about a white bear, they were to place
anxiety and trait anxiety (scores on the Social Interaction Anxiety a mark on the page and attempt to stop thinking about a white bear.
Scale and State–Trait Anxiety Index, respectively; ps ⬍ .05). This After the thought-listing task, participants rated the difficulty of the task
suggests that the association between high trait self-control and (as a manipulation check), and they completed the Brief Mood Introspec-
low death anxiety was not simply the result of trait self-control tion Scale (BMIS; Mayer & Gaschke, 1988). The BMIS contains 20 items
buffering against negative affect in general. indicative of mood (e.g., happy, sad) and arousal (e.g., peppy, drowsy).
Participants rated each item to indicate how they were feeling at the present
moment, using a scale from 1 (definitely do not feel) to 7 (definitely feel).
Discussion Participants also indicated to what extent they tried to follow the task
instructions (effort) and how well they felt that they followed those
Studies 1A–1C provided initial evidence for the view that self-
instructions (perceptions of task performance).
regulation protects people from the aversive awareness of death
Next, participants completed an unrelated filler task (i.e., making per-
and mortality. High self-control predicted less explicit (death anx- sonality judgments of children in a video) that bolstered the cover story.
iety and responses to an image of a skull) and implicit (responses The personality-judgment task took approximately 4 min. Afterward, par-
to word fragments) preoccupation with death. ticipants completed the same series of word fragments used in Study 1A.
We assume that people periodically confront cues or thoughts
that remind them of death and that they therefore exercise self- Results and Discussion
regulation to prevent these thoughts from lingering in conscious
awareness and escalating into greater anxiety. These results sug- Manipulation check. Participants in the depletion condition
gest that low self-control leaves people less effectively defended rated their suppression task as being somewhat more difficult
against the threatening idea of death and hence more prone to (M ⫽ 3.78, SD ⫽ 1.92) than did the participants in the no-
suffer both disturbing thoughts and anxious emotions in connec- depletion condition (M ⫽ 2.40, SD ⫽ 1.26), t(17) ⫽ 2.53, p ⫽ .08.
tion with death. This pattern suggests that the depleting task required more effort-
ful self-regulation than did the nondepleting task.
Study 2 Implicit death thoughts. We predicted and confirmed that
death-related thoughts would be more accessible to the minds of
In Studies 2 and 3 we used laboratory manipulations of self- depleted than to those of nondepleted participants. Depleted par-
regulation (rather than correlational evidence) to test the causal ticipants (M ⫽ 2.22, SD ⫽ 1.48) solved more word fragments with
hypothesis that low self-control weakens defenses against death. In death-related words than did nondepleted participants (M ⫽ 0.90,
Study 2 we examined whether temporarily depleted self-regulatory SD ⫽ 1.20), t(17) ⫽ ⫺2.15, p ⬍ .05. This pattern is consistent
strength would increase the cognitive accessibility of death-related with the view that self-regulatory resources help reduce death-
thought. People typically try to minimize or suppress thoughts thought accessibility. Participants who possessed greater self-
associated with death. We hypothesized that if self-regulation is regulatory strength (i.e., nondepleted participants) exhibited less
needed to suppress death thoughts, then self-regulatory depletion death-thought accessibility than did participants who had previ-
should undermine this suppression and lead to increased accessi- ously expended their self-regulatory strength. Presumably, de-
bility of death-related thoughts. pleted participants were less able to suppress thoughts of death,
Participants in Study 2 first completed either a task that required and thus death thoughts increased in accessibility. The word-
self-regulation (thought suppression) or a control task. Thought fragment completion task required participants to execute a mental
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 53

search for possible solutions, and solutions related to death ap- BMIS) 4 –5 min after the initial video-watching task. Participants subse-
peared to be more accessible when self-regulatory resources were quently completed a measure of death-thought accessibility (listing
depleted. thoughts to an ambiguous drawing) that was identical to the one used in
Self-esteem, mood, arousal, task performance, and effort. Ad- Study 1B except that a different drawing was used (see Figure 2).
ditional analyses suggested that the difference in accessibility of
implicit death thoughts between depleted and nondepleted partic- Results and Discussion
ipants was not attributable to differences in self-esteem, mood, Explicit death thoughts. Depleted participants (M ⫽ 1.34,
arousal, effort, or perceptions of task performance. Regression SD ⫽ 1.00) listed more death-related thoughts while viewing the
analysis indicated that self-esteem did not moderate the effect of drawing than did nondepleted participants (M ⫽ .97, SD ⫽ .45),
depletion on the accessibility of death thoughts (t ⬍ 1, ns). Further, t(65) ⫽ 1.99, p ⫽ .05. Presumably, participants who possessed full
depleted and nondepleted participants did not differ in arousal, task self-regulatory strength were able to suppress thoughts of death
performance, or effort (all ps ⬎ .24), nor did any of these factors while viewing the drawing. Depleted participants, lacking self-
significantly predict the number of implicit death thoughts (all regulatory strength, were less able to suppress such thoughts. The
ps ⬎ .24). Depleted and nondepleted participants did differ in results of Study 3 thus replicated those of Study 2, using different
self-reported mood valence ( p ⬍ .05) such that depleted partici- methods. In both studies, weakening self-control by depleting its
pants were in a more negative mood, but mood did not signifi- resources led participants to have more thoughts about death.
cantly predict the number of implicit death thoughts ( p ⬎ .32), and Moreover, the finding that depletion increased death thoughts
the difference in death thoughts between the two conditions was and anxiety after a short delay indicates that the effects of deple-
significant even when controlling for mood, F(1, 16) ⫽ 3.19, p ⬍ tion are somewhat enduring. One may very well be susceptible to
.05 (one-tailed). greater mortal concern long after having exerted self-control.
Mood and arousal. Analyses indicated that the effect of de-
Study 3 pletion on the accessibility of death-related thoughts was not
mediated by mood or arousal. Depleted and nondepleted partici-
The purpose of Study 3 was to provide converging evidence that pants did not differ in arousal ( p ⬎ .19), but depleted participants
self-regulatory depletion increases death-thought accessibility. The
design of the study mirrored that of Study 2. Participants first
completed either a task (controlling attention while watching a
video) that has been shown to deplete self-regulatory strength (e.g.,
Schmeichel, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2003) or a control task. Partic-
ipants then completed a measure of death-thought accessibility for
which they listed their thoughts about an ambiguous image. We
predicted that participants who first performed the self-regulatory
(vs. the control) task would respond to the ambiguous image with
more death-related thoughts because they had depleted self-
regulatory strength.

Method
Participants. Sixty-seven undergraduate students (49 women, 18 men)
participated in a classroom setting. They were told that the study was to
investigate people’s attitudes and opinions.
Procedure. The first task served as the manipulation of self-regulatory
resources. Specifically, participants watched a 6-min video (without sound)
of a woman talking (modified from Gilbert, Krull, & Pelham’s, 1988,
study). In the bottom corner of the screen, common one-syllable words
(e.g., hair, hat) appeared individually for 10 s. Participants randomly
assigned to the depletion condition were instructed to focus their attention
on the woman’s face and to refrain from looking at the words. If they
happened to look at the words, they were to refocus their attention on the
woman as quickly as possible. Attention automatically orients toward
novel stimuli appearing in the environment (e.g., see Shiffrin & Schneider,
1977), and so the task required these participants to exert self-control by
overriding prepotent orienting of attention to the words and maintain
attention instead on the woman’s face only. Participants assigned to the
no-depletion condition were instructed to watch the video as they would
normally (i.e., as if they were sitting at home watching TV) and hence were
not required to exert self-control.
After the video-watching task, participants completed the target materi-
als (described below) that were embedded among other measures (not Figure 2. Artwork (Gillam) from the May 1894 cover of Judge maga-
related to death) that helped bolster the cover story about attitudes and zine. This picture, shown in Study 3, could be interpreted as either two men
opinions. Participants completed measures of mood and arousal (the or a skull.
54 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

were in a somewhat more negative mood ( p ⫽ .06). However, Study 5


neither mood nor arousal significantly predicted the number of
death thoughts ( ps ⫽ .22 and .38, respectively), and the difference The purpose of Study 5 was to test the hypothesis that good
in death thoughts between the two conditions was significant, even self-regulation reduces concerns about death by examining the
when controlling for mood, F(1, 63) ⫽ 2.93, p ⬍ .05 (one-tailed). consequences of increased death concerns rather than the extent of
them. Specifically, Study 5 examined the role of trait self-control
in one well-documented consequence of mortality salience: in-
Study 4 creased worldview defense.
When mortality is made salient, people bolster faith in their own
The purpose of Study 4 was to provide additional evidence that culture and react more positively toward others who support their
self-regulatory depletion increases death thoughts and anxiety. cultural norms and values, and they react more negatively toward
Specifically, we assessed (rather than manipulated) temporary others who disagree with their cultural norms and values (e.g.,
self-regulation abilities using a recently developed scale that has Florian & Mikulincer, 1997; Greenberg et al., 1990; Heine, Hari-
been shown to measure self-regulatory depletion (Twenge, Mu- hara, & Niiya, 2002; Ochsmann & Mathey, 1994). Likewise,
raven, & Tice, 2004; see also Finkel & Campbell, 2001, for a mortality salience has been shown to increase support for political
similar measure). Participants then completed measures of implicit leaders. For instance, after writing about death or the September
death-thought accessibility (word fragments) and death anxiety. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks on the United States, participants (from
We hypothesized the following: If self-regulation helps to mini- the United States) showed increased support for current President
mize death-thought accessibility and death anxiety, then partici- George W. Bush compared with participants who wrote about
pants who report more self-regulatory depletion should exhibit merely aversive topics such as dental pain (Landau et al., 2004).
more death-thought accessibility and death anxiety than should Study 5 of the current investigation was a partial replication of the
participants who report relatively less self-regulatory depletion. Landau et al. (2004) study that assessed the relationships among
mortality salience, trait self-control, and support for the President.
We made a few hypotheses about the results of Study 5: If
Method self-control is used to reduce death-related thought and anxiety,
Participants. One hundred fifty-nine undergraduate students (110 then participants higher in trait self-control should be more capable
women, 49 men) participated. The study was conducted in a classroom of managing concerns about death following a mortality-salience
setting, and participants were told that it was to investigate attitudes and induction than should participants lower in trait self-control. The
vocabulary. They completed questionnaire packets that contained all in- more effective suppression abilities of individuals high (vs. low) in
structions and materials (i.e., the focal materials along with other, non- trait self-control should reduce both implicit and explicit death-
death-related personality measures.) thought accessibility (Anderson & Green, 2001; MacLeod, 1989;
Procedure. Specifically, participants completed a measure that as-
McBride & Dosher, 1997). To the extent that support for President
sessed perceptions of the momentary availability of self-regulatory re-
sources (the State Depletion Scale; Twenge et al., 2004). The State De-
Bush is exacerbated by death concerns, participants high in trait
pletion Scale contains 25 items (e.g., “I feel like my willpower is gone.” self-control should not show increased support for the President
“My mental energy is running low.”) answered on a scale from 1 (not true) after thinking about death.
to 7 (very true; ␣ ⫽ .91). Higher scores indicate greater self-regulatory Participants first did or did not write about their own death.
depletion. Then they read a short passage in support of President Bush and
Subsequent measures in the packet assessed implicit death-thought ac- indicated how much they agreed with the passage. Our prediction
cessibility and death anxiety. Specifically, participants completed the was that mortality salience would boost support for the President
Death Anxiety Scale (Templer, 1970) and the same word fragments as used but only among participants with low trait self-control.
in Study 1A and Study 2.

Method
Results and Discussion Participants. Participants were 88 undergraduates (41 women, 47
men) who completed a brief measure of trait self-control (Tangney et al.,
State self-control depletion correlated positively with implicit
2004) and self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965) during a mass-testing session
death thoughts, r(159) ⫽ .20, and death anxiety, r(159) ⫽ .43 earlier in the semester. The present study was conducted in classrooms in
(both ps ⬍ .05). Participants who indicated that they were rela- groups of 10 –25 participants approximately 2 weeks prior to the 2004 U.S.
tively more depleted solved more word fragments with death- presidential election (some 7 weeks after the mass-testing session). Partic-
related words and reported more death anxiety than did partici- ipants were told that the study was to investigate personality characteristics
pants whose self-regulatory resources were less depleted. and political attitudes. They received a packet that contained all materials
These findings converge with those of the previous studies. for the experiment.
Measured self-control depletion revealed the same association Procedure. Participants first completed a demographic questionnaire
between depletion and death-thought accessibility as did manipu- and two filler personality-related questionnaires. Participants randomly
assigned to the mortality-salience condition then completed a questionnaire
lated self-control depletion in Studies 2 and 3. Presumably, par-
that asked them to describe the emotions that the thought of their own death
ticipants who were relatively less depleted were more capable of aroused in them and to write about what will happen to their bodies as they
managing concerns about death and thus experienced relatively physically die (see Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon,
less preoccupation with death. The results thus far are highly 1989). Participants in the control condition did not complete this question-
consistent with the hypothesis that self-regulation minimizes naire but instead proceeded immediately to the next items in the packet,
death-thought accessibility and death anxiety. two personality-relevant measures intended to bolster the cover story.
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 55

These measures were included immediately after the mortality-salience apparently less cognizant of death after mortality salience, how-
manipulation but before the target dependent variable (described below), ever, and so after thinking about death they did not increase
because the effects of the mortality-salience manipulation used typically support for President Bush.
emerge only after a short delay or distraction (see Pyszczynski et al., 1999).
To test the effect of mortality salience on worldview defense, we asked
participants next to read and evaluate a statement (used in Landau et al.’s, Study 6
2004, study) regarding President Bush and his antiterrorism policies. The
passage praised the United States and President Bush and his actions
The central hypothesis of this investigation is that defending
against terrorism.
After reading the passage, participants responded to the following items against the threatening idea of death requires self-regulation and
on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree): “To what extent consumes self-regulatory resources. Thus far we have presented
do you endorse this statement?”; “I share many of the attitudes expressed evidence that when the capacity for self-regulation is low, thoughts
in the above statement”; and “Personally I feel secure knowing that the and fears of death are more common. We turn now to a comple-
President is doing everything possible to guard against any further attacks mentary way of testing the hypothesis, which is to show that when
against the United States.” Participants also indicated their political orien- people defend themselves against thoughts and feelings about
tation by placing a mark on a continuous spectrum that ranged from Liberal
death, they deplete their self-regulatory resources. Past work
to Conservative. The responses to the Likert items and spectrum (measured
in centimeters across the spectrum) were standardized (z scored) and has shown that following mortality salience, people actively
averaged (␣ ⫽ .91), and the single score served as our dependent measure and effortfully suppress thoughts of death (Arndt et al., 1997;
of support for President Bush (higher scores indicated greater support). Greenberg et al., 1994; Harmon-Jones et al., 1997). The result
should be impaired performance on subsequent tasks requiring
Results self-regulation.
In Study 6, participants wrote about death or a control topic. We
A regression analysis was conducted to predict support for the assumed that writing about death but not the control topic would
President from trait self-control scores, mortality-salience condi- activate disturbing thoughts and feelings that would require self-
tion, and their interaction, while controlling for self-esteem.2 The regulatory exertion to defend the self against them. Participants
analysis indicated a significant effect of mortality-salience condi-
were then provided a delay that allowed participants sufficient
tion (␤ ⫽ 1.35, p ⬍ .05). Participants in the mortality-salience
time to suppress thoughts of death (if they had them). After the
condition indicated supporting President Bush to a greater extent
delay, participants completed the Stroop color–word interference
than did participants in the control condition. This replicates the
task as a measure of self-regulation. For the Stroop task, partici-
findings of Landau et al. (2004), in which mortality salience
increased support for President Bush. The effect of self-control pants saw words (i.e., red, blue, green) on a computer screen, and
approached significance (␤ ⫽ .29, p ⫽ .06) as did its interaction they were to respond by indicating the font color of the word. On
with mortality-salience condition (␤ ⫽ ⫺1.89, p ⫽ .06). some trials (incongruent trials), the meaning of the word differed
To interpret the interaction between mortality salience and self- from its font color (e.g., red appeared in a blue font), so on these
control, and also to test directly our specific hypothesis, we as- trials participants had to exert self-control by overriding the ten-
sessed the simple effect of mortality-salience condition among dency to read the word and respond instead according to the font
participants who were relatively high versus relatively low in trait color. Performance on the Stroop task was operationalized as the
self-control (one standard deviation above and below the mean on number of errors made during incongruent trials. We predicted that
the self-control scale, respectively; Aiken & West, 1991), control- participants who wrote about death would make more Stroop
ling for trait self-esteem. Results indicate that the effect of mor- errors than would those who wrote about a neutral topic, thereby
tality salience on support for the President was significant and suggesting that their self-regulatory resources had been depleted
robust for participants low in self-control ( p ⬍ .05) but was by the mortality-salience induction.
nonsignificant and negligible for those high in self-control ( p ⬎ We also assessed performance on Stroop trials that did not
.67; see Figure 3). Thus, although mortality salience increased require self-control (i.e., congruent trials or trials for which the
support for the President, this effect occurred primarily among font color and meaning of the word were the same). This allowed
participants lower in trait self-control. Participants higher in self- us to determine whether mortality salience would impair automatic
control did not show increased support for the President after cognitive processing, as might be expected if the self-regulatory
thinking about death. impairments following mortality salience are caused by another
factor aside from self-control depletion (e.g., mood or arousal). We
Discussion predicted that mortality salience would not influence performance
These findings converge upon the hypothesis that self- on congruent trials.
regulation facilitates the management of concerns about death.
Whereas the results of the previous studies indicate that good trait 2
Including self-esteem and its higher order interactions with self-control
and state self-control reduces death-thought accessibility and death
and mortality-salience condition yielded no significant effects as a function
anxiety, the results of Study 5 suggest that high trait self-control of self-esteem (all ps ⬎ .21). In addition, our primary results remained
helps to reduce defensive reactions to mortality salience. Mortality relatively unchanged when excluding self-esteem from the model. We
salience increases liking for charismatic and decisive leaders (Co- included self-esteem in the model to demonstrate that the relationship
hen, Solomon, Maxfield, Pyszczynski, & Greenberg, 2004; between trait self-control and worldview defense (support for President
Landau et al., 2004). People with high trait self-control were Bush) was not attributable to self-esteem.
56 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

Figure 3. Support for President George W. Bush as a function of mortality-salience condition and trait
self-control (Study 5).

Method which the meaning and font color of the word were different. At the end
of the task, participants completed demographic information, indicated
Participants. Fifty-seven undergraduate students (45 women, 12 men) whether they had completed the Stroop task before (“yes” or “no”), and
participated. Five participants were excluded from all analyses because of were probed for suspicion. No participant indicated that he or she
technical difficulties (e.g., being interrupted while completing the Stroop thought that writing about death or dental pain might influence perfor-
task) or for not following instructions, leaving a final sample of 52 (42 mance on the Stroop task.
women, 10 men). Participants were randomly assigned to condition.
Procedure. Participants were run individually and were told that the
study was to investigate personality and perception. Participants first Results
completed a computer task that was similar to the Stroop task so they could
become familiarized with how to respond on the keyboard. Specifically, Stroop task performance. In all of our analyses involving Stroop
participants completed 60 trials in which a string of Xs (XXXXX) appeared task performance, we controlled for whether participants had ever
on the computer screen in either a red, green, or blue font. Participants were performed the Stroop task before, because there is a large practice
to indicate the color of the Xs by pressing one of three computer keys (the effect on Stroop task performance (MacLeod, 1991). An analysis of
R, G, or B key) as quickly as possible. Following each response, the next covariance indicated that mortality-salience participants made more
string of Xs appeared immediately. errors (M ⫽ 2.50, SD ⫽ 1.86) on the incongruent trials on the Stroop
After practicing the computer task, participants wrote about either death
task than did dental-pain participants (M ⫽ 1.40, SD ⫽ 1.85), F(1,
or dental pain. As in Study 5, mortality-salience participants described the
emotions that the thought of their own death aroused in them and what will
49) ⫽ 4.26, p ⬍ .05. This result suggests that mortality salience
happen to them as they physically die, whereas the dental-pain participants impaired participants’ ability to exert self-control.
described the emotions that the thought of their own dental pain aroused in Additional analyses indicated that the two groups did not differ
them and what would happen to them if they were to experience dental pain on the number of errors on either block of congruent trials (both
(see Rosenblatt et al., 1989). Participants then completed the BMIS, as a Fs ⬍ 1, ns). This pattern of results suggests that mortality salience
measure of mood and arousal, and a filler task (a crossword puzzle) for 4 did not impair automatic processing (congruent trials) but did
min to provide sufficient time for them to suppress thoughts of death impair controlled processing (incongruent trials). Further, partici-
(Pyszczynski et al., 1999). pants did make some errors on the congruent trials on the first and
Participants then completed the final Stroop task. For this task, third blocks (in the mortality-salience condition, M ⫽ .32 and .63
participants completed 3 blocks of trials in which the word red, blue, or
[excluding one outlier on this measure], respectively; in the dental-
green appeared on the computer screen in either a red, blue, or green
font. Participants were to indicate the font color by pressing one of three
pain condition, M ⫽ .25 and .63, respectively). This suggests that
computer keys (the R, G, or B key) and were asked to respond as the congruent trials were somewhat cognitively demanding, yet
quickly and accurately as possible. The first and third blocks consisted mortality salience did not impair performance on these trials.
of 30 congruent trials in which the meaning and font color of the word In addition, mortality-salience and dental-pain participants did
were the same. The second block consisted of 60 incongruent trials in not differ in their reaction times on either the incongruent or
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 57

congruent trials (Fs ⬍ 1, ns). This suggests that the increased completed a filler questionnaire for approximately 5 min to allow them to
number of errors among mortality-salience participants was not the suppress any thoughts about death they may have had.
result of a speed–accuracy trade-off, such as if they had made more Participants then completed either 6 analytical reasoning problems or 20
errors because they responded faster. verbal definition problems requiring rote memory (e.g., defining morato-
rium, allude, augment). These problems were borrowed from a Graduate
Mood and arousal. Mortality-salience and dental-pain partic-
Record Examinations preparation book and were classified as easy. Each
ipants did not differ in mood valence or arousal (as assessed by the
problem had 4 or 5 multiple-choice answers. Participants were instructed to
BMIS; ts ⬍ 1, ns). This suggests that mortality-salience partici- choose the most appropriate answer.
pants’ performing worse on the Stroop task than dental-pain par-
ticipants was probably not attributable to mood or arousal.
Results and Discussion
A 2 (mortality vs. uncertainty salience) ⫻ 2 (analytical vs. rote
Discussion
memory problems) between-subjects analysis of variance indi-
Participants who wrote about death performed worse (i.e., made cated a significant interaction between mortality-salience condi-
more errors) on the Stroop task than did control participants but tion and problem type, F(1, 33) ⫽ 4.76, p ⬍ .05. Simple contrasts
only on trials that required self-control (i.e., incongruent trials). indicated that, among participants who completed the analytical
This suggests that mortality salience impaired the ability to exert reasoning problems, mortality-salience participants (M ⫽ 1.38,
self-control. Presumably, after thinking about death, participants SD ⫽ 1.19) solved fewer problems correctly than did uncertainty-
suppressed thoughts of death, and the act of thought suppression salience participants (M ⫽ 2.56, SD ⫽ 1.13), F(1, 33) ⫽ 6.34, p ⬍
depleted their self-regulatory strength. Participants who wrote .05. Among participants who completed the rote memory problems
about death rather than dental pain did not perform worse on trials (verbal definitions), mortality-salience (M ⫽ 16.20, SD ⫽ 1.55)
that did not require self-control (i.e., congruent trials), which and uncertainty-salience (M ⫽ 15.50, SD ⫽ 3.27) participants did
suggests that mortality salience did not have an adverse effect on not differ in the number of problems solved correctly (F ⬍ 1, ns).
automatic cognitive processes. Mortality salience thus impaired performance on problems re-
quiring self-control (analytical reasoning skills) but not problems
requiring only more basic cognitive processing (rote memory for
Study 7 verbal definitions). This underscores the notion that mortality
salience depletes self-regulatory strength and that the deficits in
The results of Study 6 suggest that mortality salience subse- self-regulation following mortality salience are particular to self-
quently impairs self-control but not cognitive processing in gen- regulation and not cognitive processing in general.
eral. It is plausible, however, that the congruent Stroop trials were An alternative interpretation of why mortality salience might
relatively easy and therefore a poor measure of general cognitive impair self-regulation, however, might be that thoughts of death
abilities. In Study 7 we therefore provided another test of the rebound after mortality salience (e.g., Arndt et al., 1997; Wegner,
hypothesis that mortality salience impairs self-control but not other 1994) and thereby distract participants from self-regulatory tasks.
cognitive processes by using a more difficult task to assess general Similarly, it is plausible that other aftereffects of mortality salience
cognitive abilities. (e.g., worldview activation; Arndt, Greenberg, & Cook, 2002)
Participants first wrote about either death or uncertainty. Un- could somehow interfere with self-regulation. The results of Stud-
certainty salience has been shown to be a poignant self-threat (e.g., ies 6 and 7 directly contradict these possibilities, however. Mor-
van den Bos, Poortvliet, Maas, Miedema, & van den Ham, 2005) tality salience did not impair performance on congruent Stroop
and therefore seemed an adequate control topic. Participants then trials (Study 6) or on rote memory (Study 7), as one might have
completed problems requiring either analytical reasoning or rote expected if the hyperaccessibility of death thoughts or worldview
memory. constructs impaired cognitive performance. If participants’ minds
Research has indicated that analytical reasoning suffers more were full of rebounding thoughts of death, for instance, they
from self-regulatory depletion than does rote memory (Schmeichel probably would not have been able to perform very well at rote
et al., 2003). Consistent with the idea that coping with thoughts of memory.
death depletes self-regulatory resources, we therefore predicted To provide another test of the alternative explanation that mor-
that mortality salience would cause participants to perform worse tality salience impairs self-regulation via distraction, in Study 8 we
on problems requiring analytical reasoning but not rote memory. tested for moderation by trait self-esteem. There is some evidence
that mortality salience might not increase the accessibility of death
Method thoughts or worldview defense among individuals with high self-
esteem (see Pyszczynski et al., 2004). We made the following
Participants. Participants were 38 undergraduates (24 women, 14 hypothesis: If the hyperaccessibility of death thoughts and not the
men). Data from 1 participant who failed to follow instructions were
depletion of self-regulatory resources impairs self-regulation fol-
excluded from all analyses. Participants were run together in a large group
lowing mortality salience, then self-esteem might moderate the
and were told that the study was to investigate personality and cognitive
abilities. effect of mortality salience on self-regulation.
Procedure. Participants randomly assigned to the mortality-salience
condition first completed the essay about death used in the previous Study 8
studies. Participants assigned to the uncertainty-salience condition de-
scribed the emotions uncertainty arouses in them and what would happen Participants in Study 8 first wrote about either death or dental
to them if they were to experience uncertainty. Afterward, participants pain and then solved anagrams. Solving anagrams has been used
58 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

frequently in prior research on self-regulation (e.g., Baumeister et participants to solve all of the word fragments but that they could
al., 1998; Gordijn et al., 2004). The task requires self-control stop when they wanted. The word fragments were relatively easy
insofar as one must combine letters into different groupings and and with enough persistence (self-control), one could eventually
then break them apart (overriding) to try a different combination. solve each one by substituting into the word fragment different
Persistence and attention control are also required, insofar as one letters in the alphabet. Still, the task is lengthy and discouraging,
must work diligently on the task and avoid being distracted, and so and so persisting requires one to override the impulse (strength-
self-regulation is required to remain focused and succeed. We ened by the experimenter’s permission) to quit. In this sense,
predicted that participants would solve fewer anagrams after writ- quitting early and leaving more word fragments unsolved was an
ing about death than after writing about dental pain, consistent indicator of impaired self-regulation.
with the idea that suppressing thoughts of death requires and Further, as in the previous studies, some of the word fragments
therefore depletes self-regulatory strength. could be solved with words related to death. Responses to these
items served as a measure of death-thought accessibility that
Method allowed us to test whether impaired self-regulation following
mortality salience might be attributable to the hyperaccessibility of
Participants. Participants were 46 undergraduates (32 women, 14
death thoughts.
men) who completed the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale during a mass-
As another approach to examine whether the hyperaccessibility
testing session at the start of the semester. The present study occurred
approximately 3 months later. The study was conducted in a classroom of death thoughts (rather than self-control depletion) after mortal-
setting, and participants were told that the study was to investigate per- ity salience impairs subsequent self-regulatory performance, we
sonality and cognition. provided an opportunity for participants to engage in worldview
Procedure. At the beginning of the session, participants were given a defense before solving the word fragments. There is some evi-
list of 80 five-letter anagrams to solve to serve as a baseline measure of dence that engaging in worldview defense after thinking about
anagram-solving ability. They were given 5 min to complete as many death reduces the accessibility of death thoughts (Arndt et al.,
anagrams as they could. Next, participants were randomly assigned to 1997; Greenberg et al., 2001). Therefore, if mortality salience
respond to the questions about death or dental pain that were used in Study
leads to worldview defense, then any subsequent impairments in
6. To provide a delay that would allow time for participants to suppress
self-regulation would likely not be due to heightened accessibility
thoughts of death, we then asked participants to complete two filler
questionnaires for approximately 5 min. For the final task, participants of death thoughts.
were given another list of anagrams to solve for 5 min.
Last, participants were probed for suspicion. No participant indicated
that he or she thought that writing about death or dental pain might Method
influence performance at solving anagrams. Participants. Participants were 55 undergraduates (24 women, 18
men, 13 unreported). The study was conducted in a classroom setting, and
Results and Discussion participants were told that it was to investigate the relationship between
people’s attitudes and their lexicon. Participants received a packet that
Anagram performance. An analysis of covariance that used contained all materials for the experiment and were allowed to work
the number of anagrams solved at the start of the session as a through the packet at their own pace.
covariate indicated that mortality-salience participants solved Procedure. Participants first completed a filler questionnaire and then
fewer anagrams at the end of the session (M ⫽ 18.27, SD ⫽ 5.68) the same mortality-salience manipulation used in Study 8. Specifically,
than did dental-pain participants (M ⫽ 21.80, SD ⫽ 5.68), F(1, participants were randomly assigned to write about either death or dental
43) ⫽ 4.41, p ⬍ .05. Mortality-salience and dental-pain partici- pain. After this questionnaire, participants completed two more filler
questionnaires for approximately 5 min to provide time for participants to
pants did not differ in the number of anagrams solved at the start
suppress any thoughts about death.
of the session (t ⬍ 1, ns). These results are consistent with the idea
The next questionnaire constituted the measure of worldview defense.
that coping with thoughts of death requires and therefore depletes Specifically, participants read two handwritten essays about the United
self-control strength. States that were ostensibly written by two foreigners (borrowed from
Self-esteem. A regression analysis indicated that the effect of Greenberg, Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel’s, 1992, study). The
mortality salience on anagram performance was not moderated by order of the two essays was counterbalanced across participants. One essay
self-esteem (t ⬍ 1, ns). This suggests that the effect of mortality was in support of the United States and praised Americans, whereas the
salience was not related to self-esteem, as might have been ex- other essay was not in support of the United States and criticized Ameri-
pected if the relationship between mortality salience and anagram cans. Participants evaluated the truth and validity of the essay and the
performance was attributable to hyperaccessibility of death likability, intelligence, and knowledgeability of each essay’s author on
thoughts or worldview constructs. 9-point scales. The summed evaluations of each essay served as the
measures of favorability toward worldview-consistent and worldview-
inconsistent opinions, respectively. In accord with past research (e.g.,
Study 9 Greenberg et al., 1994), worldview defense was defined as the difference
between these two measures. Larger differences indicate more pronounced
The purpose of Study 9 was to provide additional evidence that worldview defense.
mortality salience depletes self-regulatory strength and that the For the final task, participants completed the same list of 20 word
self-regulatory impairments following mortality salience are not fragments used in Study 1A. Specifically, 15 word fragments could be
caused by worldview activation or a rebound in thoughts of death. solved with only neutral words not related to death (e.g., soda), and 5 word
Specifically, after writing about death or dental pain, participants fragments could be solved with either neutral or death-related words.
completed a sheet filled with word fragments (e.g., so_a). We told Participants were asked to do their best to solve all of the word fragments
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 59

and were given as much time as they needed to complete the task. It was to account for their reduced persistence. The most parsimonious
left to their discretion as to when they decided to stop. explanation for these results is that participants’ self-regulatory
resources were depleted following mortality salience.
Results and Discussion
General Discussion
Worldview defense. Mortality-salience participants engaged in
worldview defense (M ⫽ 13.71, SD ⫽ 12.93) to a greater extent The current work shows that self-regulation plays a key role in
than did dental-pain participants (M ⫽ 7.69, SD ⫽ 13.06), t(51) ⫽ managing thoughts of death and death-related anxiety. Using a
⫺1.65, p ⫽ .05 (one-tailed). This finding is consistent with pre- variety of measures and manipulations, in nine studies we showed
vious research showing that mortality salience increases world- that poor self-regulation undermines the management of mortality
view defense. Given past findings that worldview defense reduces concerns and that the management of mortality concerns under-
the accessibility of death thoughts (Arndt et al., 1997; Greenberg mines subsequent self-regulation. Specifically, people with low
et al., 2001), this suggests that the accessibility of death thoughts trait self-control had more thoughts about death and higher death
may have been attenuated among mortality-salience participants. anxiety than did people with high trait self-control (Studies 1A–
Unsolved word fragments. Mortality-salience participants left 1C). Low state self-control, created by prior and seemingly irrel-
more neutral word fragments unsolved (M ⫽ 3.52, SD ⫽ 3.86) evant exercises that depleted self-regulatory strength, likewise
than did dental-pain participants (M ⫽ 1.43, SD ⫽ 1.58), made people more susceptible to intrusive thoughts about death
t(27.37) ⫽ ⫺2.45, p ⬍ .05 (degrees of freedom have been adjusted (Studies 2–3). Perceiving oneself to be low in self-regulatory
to correct for a violation of the homogeneity of variance assump- strength was associated with a similar upsurge in thoughts of death
tion). This is consistent with the idea that mortality salience, or at and death anxiety (Study 4). In addition, one defensive reaction to
least the typical response to it, is depleting. Mortality-salience the threat of death is to increase support for an ostensibly strong
participants persisted less on the subsequent task than did dental- and patriotic authority figure (Cohen et al., 2004; Landau et al.,
pain participants. 2004), and such defensive shifts in political preferences in re-
Further, the difference between the two conditions in the num- sponse to thoughts of death were found mainly among people low
ber of word fragments left unsolved remained significant even in trait self-control (Study 5).
when controlling for the number of target items completed with Further, mortality salience led to impairments in self-regulation.
words related to death (implicit death thoughts) and worldview Participants who had to think and write about death later per-
defense scores (both Fs ⱖ 6.87, ps ⬍ .05). This suggests that any formed worse on the Stroop task (Study 6) and analytical reason-
difference between mortality-salience and dental-pain participants ing problems (Study 7), solved fewer anagrams (Study 8), and quit
in the extent to which death thoughts were cognitively accessible sooner on a task of persistence (Study 9), as compared with
or the extent to which they engaged in worldview defense did not participants who wrote about a control topic. People effortfully
account for the finding that mortality-salience participants left suppress thoughts of death following their activation (Arndt et al.,
more word fragments unsolved. The impairments in self-regulation 1997; Greenberg et al., 1994; Harmon-Jones et al., 1997; Pyszc-
among mortality-salience participants therefore appeared indepen- zynski et al., 1999), and so it seems likely that the act of suppress-
dent of death-thought accessibility and worldview defense. ing thoughts of death after writing about death depleted partici-
In addition, mortality-salience and dental-pain participants did pants’ self-regulatory strength.
not differ in the number of target items related to death unsolved These findings are consistent with the view that thoughts about
(t ⬍ 1.05, ns). This suggests that mortality-salience participants death and mortality are disturbing and are widely treated as threats
did not leave more neutral word fragments unsolved simply be- that must be kept at bay. Specifically, we propose that the world
cause they solved instead more death-related word fragments. contains many cues that could evoke thoughts and feelings about
Further analysis indicated that greater worldview defense was death, and that people use self-regulation to prevent these cues
associated with leaving more neutral word fragments unsolved, from flooding the conscious mind with troublesome thoughts and
r(53) ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .05. This suggests that being more threatened by aversive emotions such as anxiety (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, &
the thought of death (and hence being more defensive) was asso- Solomon, 1986). But because the capacity for self-regulation is
ciated with greater depletion. The fact that worldview defense did limited, and because some people have more of it than others, the
not account for the effect of mortality salience on depletion, regulatory process is not consistently effective. Hence some stim-
however, indicates that engaging in worldview defense was not the uli will evoke conscious thoughts about death, at least for some of
cause of depletion. Rather, it was likely the self-regulatory effort the people some of the time. Trait and state capacity for self-
of having to cope with the threat of death that depleted self- regulation appears to offer an effective basis for predicting and
regulatory resources. understanding who will suffer most from such thoughts and fears.
In sum, participants who wrote about death solved fewer neutral
word fragments than did participants who wrote about dental pain. Implications, Limitations, and Alternative Explanations
Results also indicated that the accessibility of death thoughts
probably did not account for the differences in self-control. These results are consistent with previous work showing that
Mortality-salience participants persisted less even though they people actively and effortfully suppress thoughts of death (e.g.,
were given the opportunity to engage in worldview defense, which Greenberg et al., 1994; Harmon-Jones et al., 1997). Whereas
should have attenuated the accessibility of death thoughts (Arndt et previous work showed that a concurrent cognitive load undermines
al., 1997; Greenberg et al., 2001). Likewise, the number of implicit the suppression of death thoughts (Arndt et al., 1997), the current
death thoughts and the extent of worldview defense did not seem work indicates that depleted self-regulatory strength and low trait
60 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

self-control also undermine the suppression of death thoughts. And uncertainty as control conditions was intended to differentiate
conversely, the suppression of death thoughts depletes self-control death from other bad thoughts, and clearly in the present studies
strength. Taken together, the picture that emerges is that people death had effects that were not duplicated by dental pain or
actively seek to suppress or minimize awareness of death and that uncertainty. Death could represent a particularly important prob-
concurrent cognitive activity, previous self-regulatory exertion, or lem because it threatens to nullify one’s life and its meaning, and
habitually poor self-regulatory abilities undermine such efforts. so people may seek to defend against the thought of death more
Though a few alternative explanations may be proposed for the extensively than they would defend against the thought of other
link between self-control and mortal concern, they seem largely misfortunes. Future research may profitably examine whether self-
unable to account for the bulk of the current findings. Specifically, regulation and self-control depletion are relevant to psychological
the results appeared independent from any effects of self-esteem, defenses against more than death.
arousal, mood, or general negative affect (i.e., general anxiety and
social anxiety in Study 1C). Some effects may be explainable in Concluding Remarks
terms of social desirability, such as if people giving socially
desirable responses reported having both high self-control and low Coping with unwanted thoughts and feelings can be an effortful
death anxiety, yet this view cannot adequately account for all of struggle. Death is inevitable and inescapable, and so people must
the findings. It is unclear, for example, how social desirability protect themselves from thoughts of death if they are to reduce the
would have been related to support for President Bush (Study 5). anxiety that the awareness of death might evoke. Rational attempts
The social desirability explanation also does not easily account for to deny death ultimately fail, however, and so people must engage
the fact that mortality salience impaired performance on tasks that in other defenses to ward off the threat of death (Pyszczynski et al.,
required self-control but not other cognitive tasks (Studies 6 and 1999). The current work indicates that self-regulation is a useful
7). We also controlled for self-esteem in most of the studies. tool in the psychological defense against death. Through effective
Insofar as reporting having high self-esteem indicates giving so- self-regulation, people can minimize their awareness of death and
cially desirable responses, then the results do not appear caused by reduce anxiety associated with death. But this peace of mind
differences in social desirability. comes at a cost. When thoughts of death arise, it takes effortful
Further, the effect of depletion following mortality salience did self-control to suppress them, and these efforts consume resources
not appear attributable to distraction by thoughts about death that leave people with less self-control afterward— causing people
(Study 9), active attempts toward defending one’s worldviews to perform less effectively on many tasks. Conversely, expending
(Study 9), or deficits to general cognitive abilities (Studies 6 and resources on any sort of self-control task reduces one’s defenses,
7). The most parsimonious conclusion is that mortality salience thereby opening the door for disturbing thoughts of death.
impaired self-control by depleting self-regulatory resources. In-
deed, mortality salience increased worldview defense (Study 9), a References
defensive reaction aimed at coping with the threat of death, and
greater worldview defense predicted (but did not account for) Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interaction. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
greater depletion. Participants who seemingly attempted to cope
Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001, March 15). Suppressing unwanted
the most with death and therefore found it the most frightening memories by executive control. Nature, 6826, 366 –369.
were the most depleted. Aries, P. (1981). The hour of our death. New York: Oxford University
Studies 1A–1C were correlational, which makes it difficult to Press.
draw causal conclusions. Still, they do indicate a personality Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., & Cook, A. (2002). Mortality salience and the
pattern that seems to make people more vulnerable to unwelcome spreading activation of worldview-relevant constructs: Exploring the
thoughts and emotions. Preliminary evidence has linked low self- cognitive architecture of terror management. Journal of Experimental
control to a variety of maladaptive behavior patterns (e.g., Gott- Psychology: General, 131, 307–324.
fredson & Hirschi, 1990; Tangney et al., 2004). It is plausible that Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Simon, L. (1997).
one reason people with low versus high self-control engage in such Suppression, accessibility of death-related thoughts, and cultural world-
view defense: Exploring the psychodynamics of terror management.
maladaptive behaviors is that they have substantial death anxiety.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 5–18.
For example, both low self-control and death anxiety are associ- Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998).
ated with increased aggression (McGregor et al., 1998; Stucke & Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Person-
Baumeister, in press; Tangney et al., 2004) and risky sexual ality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
behavior (Taubman Ben-Ari, 2004; Wills, Gibbons, Gerrard, Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control:
Murry, & Brody, 2003). Perhaps people with low self-control How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic
engage in such behaviors in part as a response to preoccupation Press.
with death (see Tice, Bratslavsky, & Baumeister, 2001). Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Academic Press.
If we could generalize the present results to other sources of Cohen, F., Solomon, S., Maxfield, M., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J.
stress, threat, and trauma, this work would be even more impor- (2004). Fatal attraction: The effects of mortality salience on evaluations
of charismatic, task-oriented, and relationship-oriented leaders. Psycho-
tant, but we hesitate to generalize without further evidence. It is
logical Science, 15, 846 – 851.
plausible that self-regulation might be more closely tied to coping Feifel, H., & Branscomb, A. B. (1973). Who’s afraid of death? Journal of
with the threat of death than with other threats or aversive states. Abnormal Psychology, 81, 282–288.
The thought of death can be especially threatening and trigger Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accommodation
active efforts toward suppression (e.g., Arndt et al., 1997), perhaps in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Person-
more so than most other thoughts. Our use of dental pain and ality and Social Psychology, 81, 263–277.
THOUGHTS AND FEARS OF DEATH AND SELF-REGULATION 61

Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgment of implicit and explicit memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
social transgressions: A multidimensional test of terror management General, 126, 371–392.
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 369 –380. McGregor, H. A., Lieberman, J. D., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J.,
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (in press). Self-control and business Simon., L., & Pyszczynski, T. (1998). Terror management and aggres-
ethics: How strengthening the self benefits the corporation and the sion: Evidence that mortality salience motivates aggression against
individual. In R. A. Giacalone, C. L. Jurkiewicz, & C. Dunn (Eds.), worldview-threatening others. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Positive psychology, ethics, and social responsibility in organizational chology, 74, 590 – 605.
life. Greenwich, CT: Information Age. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent
Gilbert, D., T., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1988). Of thoughts competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of
unspoken: Social inference and the self-regulation of behavior. Journal Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687– 696.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 685– 694. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of
Gordijn, E. H., Hindriks, I., Koomen, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Knip- limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological
penberg, A. (2004). Consequences of stereotype suppression and internal Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
suppression motivation: A self-regulation approach. Personality and Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 212–224. limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. and Social Psychology, 74, 774 –789.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Ochsmann, R., & Mathey, M. (1994). Depreciating of and distancing from
Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. foreigners: Effects of mortality salience. Unpublished manuscript, Uni-
(2001). Clarifying the function of mortality salience-induced worldview versitat Mainz, Mainz, Germany.
defense: Renewed suppression or reduced accessibility of death-related Pollak, J. M. (1979). Correlates of death anxiety: A review of empirical
thoughts? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 70 –76. studies. Omega: Journal of Death & Dying, 10, 97–121.
Greenberg, J., Martens, A., Jonas, E., Eisenstadt, D., Pyszczynski, T., & Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual process
Solomon, S. (2003). Psychological defense in anticipation of anxiety: model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related
Eliminating the potential for anxiety eliminates the effect of mortality thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological
salience on worldview defense. Psychological Science, 14, 516 –519. Review, 106, 835– 845.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J.
consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical
R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 189 –212). New review. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 435– 468.
York: Springer-Verlag. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., NJ: Princeton University Press.
Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D.
II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mor-
bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- tality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural
chology, 58, 308 –318. values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681– 690.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual
(1994). Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and
in mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ogy, 67, 627– 637. chology, 85, 33– 46.
Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
(1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience al- information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychologi-
ways intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one’s world- cal Review, 84, 1– 66.
view? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212–220. Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent
Harmon-Jones, E., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of
& McGregor, H. (1997). Terror management theory and self-esteem: gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychol-
Evidence that increased self-esteem reduces mortality salience effects. ogy, 26, 978 –986.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 24 –36. Smart, L., & Wegner, D. M. (1999). Covering up what can’t be seen:
Heine, S. J., Harihara, M., & Niiya, Y. (2002). Terror management in Concealable stigma and mental control. Journal of Personality and
Japan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 187–196. Social Psychology, 77, 474 – 486.
Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual
Arndt, J., et al. (2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
salience and reminders of 9/11 on support for President George W. Psychologists.
Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136 –1150. Stucke, T. S., & Baumeister, R. F. (in press). Ego depletion and aggressive
MacLeod, C. M. (1989). Directed forgetting affects both direct and indirect behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a limited resource? European
tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem- Journal of Social Psychology.
ory, and Cognition, 15, 13–21. Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interper-
integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. sonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–322.
Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of Taubman Ben-Ari, O. (2004). Intimacy and risky sexual behavior: What
measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. does it have to do with death? Death Studies, 28, 865– 887.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 455– 470. Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a death anxiety
Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta- scale. Journal of General Psychology, 82, 165–177.
experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional
102–111. distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel
McBride, D. M., & Dosher, B. A. (1997). A comparison of forgetting in an bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 53– 67.
62 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

Twenge, J. M., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2004). Measuring state Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression.
self-control: Reliability, validity, and correlations with physical and Journal of Personality, 62, 615– 640.
psychological stress. Unpublished manuscript, San Diego State Univer- Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual
sity. Review of Psychology, 51, 59 –91.
van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, P. M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., & van den Ham, Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Murry, V. M., & Brody, G. H.
E. J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural norms and (2003). Family communication and religiosity related to substance use
values: The impact of uncertainty and mortality salience on reactions to and sexual behavior in early adolescence: A test for pathways through
violations of bolstering of cultural worldviews. Journal of Experimental self-control and prototype perceptions. Psychology of Addictive Behav-
Social Psychology, 41, 91–113. iors, 17, 312–323.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological
Review, 101, 34 –52.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Received May 1, 2005
Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Revision received November 1, 2005
Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. Accepted November 3, 2005 䡲
62 GAILLIOT, SCHMEICHEL, AND BAUMEISTER

Twenge, J. M., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2004). Measuring state Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression.
self-control: Reliability, validity, and correlations with physical and Journal of Personality, 62, 615– 640.
psychological stress. Unpublished manuscript, San Diego State Univer- Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual
sity. Review of Psychology, 51, 59 –91.
van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, P. M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., & van den Ham, Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Murry, V. M., & Brody, G. H.
E. J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural norms and (2003). Family communication and religiosity related to substance use
values: The impact of uncertainty and mortality salience on reactions to and sexual behavior in early adolescence: A test for pathways through
violations of bolstering of cultural worldviews. Journal of Experimental self-control and prototype perceptions. Psychology of Addictive Behav-
Social Psychology, 41, 91–113. iors, 17, 312–323.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological
Review, 101, 34 –52.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Received May 1, 2005
Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Revision received November 1, 2005
Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. Accepted November 3, 2005 䡲
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES

Peacocks, Picasso, and Parental Investment: The Effects of Romantic


Motives on Creativity
Vladas Griskevicius, Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T. Kenrick
Arizona State University

Four experiments explored the effects of mating motivation on creativity. Even without other incentives
to be creative, romantic motives enhanced creativity on subjective and objective measures. For men, any
cue designed to activate a short-term or a long-term mating goal increased creative displays; however,
women displayed more creativity only when primed to attract a high-quality long-term mate. These
creative boosts were unrelated to increased effort on creative tasks or to changes in mood or arousal.
Furthermore, results were unaffected by the application of monetary incentives for creativity. These
findings align with the view that creative displays in both sexes may be linked to sexual selection,
qualified by unique exigencies of human parental investment.

Keywords: creativity, sexual selection, parental investment, self-presentation, mating goals

“In order to create there must be a dynamic force—and what force is mythology—are universally female. Yet if “there is no biological
more potent than love?” reason why a man can’t provide the elements of inspiration” (p. 9,
—Igor Stravinsky Prose, 2002), how could it be that the elixir of inspiration seems to
be primarily concocted by women and predominantly imbibed by
The Guinness Book of World Records lists Pablo Picasso as the
men?
most prolific artist in history with an astounding 147,800 works of
The current research presents an evolutionary cognitive frame-
art. Picasso’s career is often depicted as a tortuous series of
work designed to shed light on the mystery of the muses and also
profoundly inspired artistic periods— blue, rose, cubist, surreal-
to explain and predict a wider range of behavioral phenomena.
ist—in which his subjects underwent extravagant visual transfor-
This framework is grounded in two underlying premises: First, that
mations at the hands of a creative genius performing at the apogee
a number of human mental traits—including the capacity for and
of his ability. But a closer look at Picasso’s generative periods
display of creativity—may in part be linked to evolutionary pro-
reveals an intriguing constant: Each new epoch blossoms with
cesses of sexual selection and differential parental investment
paintings of a new woman—not a sitter or a model, but a mis-
(Miller, 1999, 2000; Trivers, 1972); and second, that evolutionar-
tress— each of whom is touted to have served Picasso as an
ily relevant contextual cues can serve to activate goals (e.g., the
incandescent, albeit temporary, muse (Crespelle, 1969;
goal to attract a mate) that facilitate behaviors historically associ-
MacGregor-Hastie, 1988). Picasso’s artistic history, however, is
ated with success for the attainment of such goals (e.g., Maner et
not unique: Creative juggernauts such as Salvador Dalı́, Friedrich
al., 2005; Roney, 2003; Wilson & Daly, 2004). From this foun-
Nietzsche, and Dante were also acutely inspired by their own
dation, four experiments examine whether cues designed to tem-
muses (Prose, 2002). The enigmatic notion of a muse is rooted in
porarily activate a goal to attract a mate can increase people’s
Greek mythology, in which nine godly muses traversed the land,
creativity. Drawing on the theory of differential parental invest-
stirring the creative spirits of mortal artists and scientists. And
ment, the research also explores which specific cues might stim-
according to historian Francine Prose (2002), all muses share one
ulate a creative boost for men versus women. Finally, the studies
striking and inextricable feature: Muses— both in history and in
gather evidence regarding the nature of the psychological mecha-
nism that may underlie how mating motives serve to foster
creativity.
Vladas Griskevicius, Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T. Kenrick, De-
partment of Psychology, Arizona State University.
This research was facilitated by a National Science Foundation Graduate Natural and Sexual Selection of Creative Displays
Research Fellowship awarded to Vladas Griskevicius and by National
The construct of creativity is composed of multiple dimensions
Institutes of Health Grant 5R01MH64734 to Douglas T. Kenrick. We thank
Steve Neuberg, Cathy Cottrell, Noah Goldstein, and Josh Tybur for their
that share a common thread: Creativity is the ability to produce
helpful comments on a draft of this article. work that is both novel (i.e., original, unexpected) and appropriate
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Vladas (i.e., useful, valuable; Sternberg & Lubart, 1999). Although clas-
Griskevicius, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, sical creativity research has not been concerned with the ultimate
Tempe, AZ 85287-1104. E-mail: vladasg@asu.edu origins of human creativity (e.g., see Simonton, 2000), those who
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 63–76
Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.63

63
64 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

have offered evolutionary explanations have generally presumed Given the central role of reproduction in evolutionary processes,
that creative abilities evolved because they somehow enhanced the a functional perspective suggests that mating goals are likely to be
likelihood of our ancestors’ survival without adding unnecessary closely linked to adaptive outcomes (Bugental, 2000; Kenrick, Li,
metabolic costs (e.g., Byrne, 1995; Gibson & Ingold, 1993; King- & Butner, 2003). Cues related to mating can serve to both activate
don, 1993). For example, a creative way to spear a fish or build a a mating goal and its affective responses (Scott, 1980) and trigger
hut could have enhanced survival for their creator and his or her specific mating-related cognitive mechanisms (Gutierres, Kenrick,
kin. Other evolutionary theorists have speculated that creative & Partch, 1999; Haselton & Buss, 2000; Kenrick, Sadalla, &
capacities may be a byproduct of various perceptual and cognitive Keefe, 1998). Furthermore, mating-related motives appear to fa-
mechanisms (e.g., Pinker, 1997). However, Miller (2000; Haselton cilitate particular perceptions, cognitions, and behaviors associated
& Miller, in press) has recently argued that such explanations are with reproductive success (Griskevicius, Goldstein, Mortensen,
inadequate for several reasons. For instance, not only have other Cialdini, & Kenrick, in press; Maner et al., 2005; Roney, 2003). If
large-brained animals not evolved similar capacities, but many displays of creativity have evolved in part because of their benefit
human displays of creativity are highly valued socially yet are in courtship, cues designed to activate mating motives may also
difficult to explain in terms of survival value. For example, a trigger displays of creativity.
farmer produces more tangible survival benefits in a week than a
team of musicians, poets, and sculptors will produce in a lifetime. Courtship Displays and Parental Investment
Yet a provocative melody, poem, or sculpture is likely to elicit
greater appreciation than an absolutely perfect melon, potato, or But should mating motives produce creative displays in both
zucchini. sexes? Across most species—and in 95%–97% of all mammals—it
Instead of producing direct survival benefits, Miller (2000) and is exclusively males who display sexually selected traits during
others have proposed that several human mental traits, including courtship (Cronin, 1993). This sex-differentiated pattern is consis-
creativity, are likely to have at least in part evolved via sexual tent with the notion of predominantly female muses eliciting
creative courtship displays in male artists (Prose, 2002). Given this
selection (e.g., Eysenck, 1995; Kanazawa, 2000). Unlike natural
evidence, it is reasonable to hypothesize that mating cues should
selection, whereby traits evolve solely because they enhance the
elicit creativity only for males—the male-only creative display
probability of an individual’s survival, Darwin (1871) suggested
hypothesis.
that some traits, such as the elaborate plumage of peacocks, evolve
However, it is also plausible that mating cues could stimulate
via sexual selection—they evolve because they enhance an indi-
creative displays for both men and women. Sex differences in
vidual’s ability to attract a mate, which may or may not be
mammalian sexually selected traits are primarily linked to a spe-
independent of whether the trait enhances survival (see Andersson,
cies’ levels of maternal and paternal parental investment—the time
1994).
and energy devoted to producing viable offspring (Trivers, 1972).
Supporting this viewpoint, human creativity has several features
In those species in which courtship displays are an exclusively
in common with sexually selected traits across different species.
male sport, such as peacocks, males tend to invest the absolute
Just as members of various species prefer partners with prominent
minimum in offspring—sperm. However, in some species these
sexually selected traits, such as brilliant tails, humans show a
sex roles are reversed such that the males contribute the majority
desire for creativity in a romantic partner (Buss & Barnes, 1986;
of parental investment (e.g., Mormon cricket, pipefish seahorse,
Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002). Sexually selected traits
Panamanian poison arrow frog). Consequently, in such species it is
across species also tend to function as markers of “good genes”
the females and not the males who display their elaborate sexually
(Møller & Petrie, 2002; Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997). Correspondingly,
selected traits in courtship (Andersson, 1994; Buss, 2005).
Haselton and Miller (in press) have found suggestive empirical
Humans are somewhat different from most mammals in that
evidence indicating that creativity may partly serve a similar good
both men and women tend to contribute significant parental in-
genes function in humans.1 Moreover, because one of the distinct
vestment to their offspring (Geary, 2000). Thus, it is reasonable to
markers of sexually selected traits across species is the conspicu-
predict that courtship might stimulate creative displays for both
ous display of such traits in courtship (Andersson, 1994), the
men and women. Consistent with this reasoning are findings that
current research explores the extent to which people may be more
men and women express relatively equal preferences for creativity
likely to display creativity when they are primed with cues related in a mate (Li et al., 2002). Thus, a consideration of the particulars
to courtship. of human parental investment suggests the alternative hypothesis
that mating cues will elicit creativity for both men and women—
Cueing Creativity Through Mating Motivation the unisex creative display hypothesis.

Mental mechanisms that evolved to solve specific adaptive Study 1


problems are often highly sensitive to ecological cues indicating a
particular adaptive problem or opportunity, such as a potential The initial experiment tested whether priming people with cues
related to mating would cause an increase in their creative displays
threat or mating opportunity (Cosmides & Tooby, 1992; Öhman &
relative to people primed with neutral cues unrelated to mating. To
Mineka, 2001; Schaller, Faulkner, Park, Neuberg, & Kenrick,
2004; Todd & Gigerenzer, 2000). Moreover, much research has
shown that various cues can automatically activate certain goal and 1
More specifically, Haselton & Miller (in press) found that ovulating
need states (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996; Schaller, 2003), and that women tend to unconsciously shift their preferences toward preferring
such states can influence perception and behavior without explicit creativity-related traits compared with other desirable “good dad” traits in
conscious awareness (Bargh, 1990; Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). potential sex partners.
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 65

test this idea, we used photographs of desirable and available viewed an array of six total photos of three attractive opposite-sex indi-
targets to prime mating. Qualitative differences in people’s dis- viduals—two photos of each person. (The photos were collected from
plays of creativity were assessed via ratings of short stories written Match.com—a dating website—and were prerated by students as being
by participants in each condition. Comparative cross-species find- highly attractive). All Ps were then asked to select one person from the
array whom they thought was the most desirable romantic partner. After
ings, data on human mate preferences, and parental investment
making their selection, the photo of the selected person remained on the
theory lent themselves to two competing hypotheses: the male-
screen, and Ps were asked to imagine that they were preparing to go on a
only creative display hypothesis (that a mating prime will elicit first date with this individual. In an effort to make the mating prime more
creativity only for males) and the unisex creative display hypoth- powerful, Ps were asked to write about their idea of the perfect first date
esis (that a mating prime will increase creative displays by both with this person. All Ps had up to 3 min to write their descriptions,
men and women). although if they finished before time was up they had the option of
advancing to the next part of the study.
Method Control participants underwent a similar procedure devoid of any ro-
mantic connotations. They saw a photo of a street with several buildings
Participants and were asked to imagine being on that street. They were then given up
to 3 min to write about their idea of the most pleasant weather conditions
Ninety-one participants (Ps; 35 men and 56 women) were recruited from in which to walk around and look at the buildings.
introductory psychology classes as partial fulfillment of their class require- Priming booster shots. After the prime manipulation, all Ps wrote a
ment. All Ps came to the lab in groups of 2 to 6 and were each seated story about one of the cartoon images. Before going on to write the fourth
between partitions at a computer. The mean age for women was 19.2 and last story (about the abstract painting), all Ps underwent a prime
(SD ⫽ 1.7), and the mean age for men was 19.7 (SD ⫽ 2.1). “booster shot” to ensure that they were still in a romantic or a control frame
of mind. The booster shots were procedurally identical to the original prime
Design and Procedure manipulations except that they consisted of a different array of photos of
attractive individuals and a photo of another building.
The overall design of the experiment was a between-participants 2 Creativity measures. All four stories written by each P were rated for
(P sex) ⫻ 2 (Prime: mating vs. control) design. At the beginning of the creativity by four student judges (two men and two women) who were
study, all Ps wrote two short stories that were used to assess their dispo- blind to experimental conditions. The judges were not experts on creativity;
sitional creative ability and their general motivation to display it. After the instead, they were fellow students intended to resemble the type of person
first two writing tasks, half of the participants were primed with cues who might find him or herself on a date with one of the participants. The
related to mating. Following the prime, all Ps wrote an additional two judges rated each story on eight attributes: the extent to which they thought
stories that were later rated for their levels of creativity. These ratings the story was creative, original, clever, imaginative, captivating, funny,
served as the main dependent measure in the study. entertaining, and charming. Each attribute was rated on a 1 (not at all) to
Creativity tasks. Although there are multiple psychological measures 9 (very) scale. Before beginning the rating process, judges read a selection
designed to evaluate various forms of creativity (see Kerr & Gagliardi, of the stories for each image to familiarize themselves with what to expect.
2004), these measures are generally not designed to allow participants to Thus, the creativity ratings of the stories were relative to the other stories
display creativity in ways akin to a courtship situation. To allow Ps the written in the same situation.
freedom to display their creativity in multiple ways—to allow them to Measures of effort. The study also included two measures designed to
produce something that could be perceived as both novel and appropri- ascertain how much effort Ps put into writing each story. First, the amount
ate—we developed a methodology in which Ps were asked to write a short of time Ps took to work on each story (maximum 5 min) was recorded.
story about an ambiguous image. Ps viewed and wrote stories about two Second, the number of words Ps used to compose each story was also
types of images: cartoon drawings and abstract paintings. Each of the counted.
cartoon drawings showed a pair of individuals in an ambiguous situation
(in a prison cell and in a café), and both abstract paintings were composed
Results
of multiple abstract colorful shapes. Each image was sequentially presented
on a computer screen, and Ps were asked “What do you think is happening Overview of Stories
in this image?” All Ps had up to 5 min to write a story for each image,
although if they finished earlier they could advance to the next part of the The stories written by participants showed quite a bit of vari-
study. ability in creativity. Because Ps were not specifically instructed to
Because there is considerable variation in people’s creative abilities, a be creative, a sizable portion of the stories were somewhat bland.
baseline level was established for each person. Thus, each P saw and wrote For instance, when writing about the cartoons, some Ps merely
about two of the four ambiguous images (one cartoon and one painting)
wrote a sentence or two describing the situation with little addi-
before the prime manipulation, and they wrote about the two other images
(the counterpart cartoon and the counterpart painting) after the prime
tional insight (e.g., for the prison cartoon: “These are two men in
manipulation. The premanipulation stories served as a baseline measure of prison. They are there because they were suspected of terrorism.”
each P’s creative ability; the postmanipulation stories served as the main For the café cartoon: “These two people work together and are on
dependent measures. The order of the two types of images that Ps saw was a break from work at a coffee shop.”). However, many Ps were
always the same: The cartoon drawing was first and the abstract painting also inspired to spontaneously include more information, which
was second both before and after the manipulation. However, the order of usually resulted in more creative answers, as in the following
which version of the cartoon and painting that Ps saw was counterbalanced, description of what could be going on in the café:
and a P never saw the same image twice. Thus, the sequence of the study
for all Ps was: Cartoon A (or B), Painting A (or B), prime manipulation, Nigel is trying to decide whether or not to a get a nose job. He just
Cartoon B (or A), and Painting B (or A). can’t decide. However, his friend Reginald had one and his nose was
Mating prime. After establishing a baseline level of creativity, half of simply stunning. Reginald is a very particular sort of fellow you
the participants were primed with mating cues similar to those that have know. That latte he’s drinking had to be just so. Soy milk with a
been successfully used to activate mating goals in similar procedures dollop of foam and merely a whisper of cinnamon. Too much of one
(Roney, 2003; Wilson & Daly, 2004). To induce a romantic mindset, Ps ingredient might completely throw off the balance of his day. When
66 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

one is so particular about cinnamon, you could only imagine how he’d
prefer his nose. All of these things Nigel noted to himself as Reginald
went on and on.

Despite the fact that Ps wrote about what could be interpreted as


a comedic cartoon, the majority of the answers lacked any element
of humor. Only a few participants responded to the cartoon ques-
tion by writing a witty one-sentence tagline common in comic
strips (e.g., for the two imprisoned men in shackles: “I am badly in
need of a pedicure.”).
The two abstract images had fewer standard responses. Al-
though something about a psychedelic experience was periodically
mentioned, a good portion of Ps made up a more interesting short
description (e.g., “I think in this painting there’s a butterfly break-
ing out of its cocoon. However, I think this is a metaphor for
someone breaking free of their past” or “I see a basketball game in
a poor neighborhood with a lot of graffiti. The people are playing
because they hope that basketball will help them get out of this
neighborhood.”). Multiple Ps wrote responses that were more
interesting, such as the following:

The setting is a seedy, underground jazz club, where bands have to Figure 1. Men’s and women’s story creativity depending on type of
compete with drug dealers for the patrons’ attention. A good quintet prime in Study 1 with standard error bars (adjusted means).
is performing, with a tenor saxophone, two trumpets, a trombone and
a drummer. The instruments are old and worn, but the music that they
make is enough to turn the attention of the crack dealers and the effect on their creativity compared with the women in the control,
junkies. The music is haphazard and at times seems arrhythmic and t(86) ⫽ .17, p ⫽ .88. Thus, results supported the male-only
amelodic, but it fits the scene like a velvet glove. creative display hypothesis, indicating that mating cues increased
creativity for men but not for women.
Story Creativity
Expended Effort
The eight measures of creativity showed high cohesiveness for
each of the four judges (alphas for the eight ratings for each judge The amount of effort Ps expended on each story was assessed by
ranged from .88 –.94).2 Thus, the eight ratings were combined into the amount of time they took to write the stories and the number
a creativity index for each of the four coders. The ratings of all four of words used. We first assessed whether the prime manipulation
coders also showed high interrater reliability (␣ ⫽ .86 for the had a similar impact on the expended effort measures for the two
cartoon; ␣ ⫽ .88 for the painting), and the ratings for the four types of postmanipulation images. One ANCOVA was performed
judges were combined into a creativity index for the cartoon and a on time taken to write the story using the premanipulation time as
creativity index for the painting. a covariate, and another ANCOVA was performed on number of
We next tested whether the postmanipulation creativity ratings words in a story using the premanipulation number of words as a
of the stories about the cartoon and the painting were differentially covariate. Both analyses indicated that type of image did not
influenced by P sex and the prime manipulation. This was done via interact with P sex or with prime (all ps ⬎ .3), meaning that the
a repeated-measures mixed-model 3-factor analysis of covariance primes had a similar effect on both men’s and women’s effort on
(ANCOVA) with the premanipulation creativity ratings as a co- stories about the cartoon and the painting. Thus, the effort mea-
variate; P sex and prime were entered as between-participants sures for the two types of stories were collapsed.
factors and type of image was entered as a within-participants A two-factor ANCOVA on time spent writing the stories with
factor. As expected, type of image did not interact with P sex or the premanipulation time as a covariate revealed no significant
prime (all ps ⬎ .35), meaning that creativity for both types of interaction or main effects of P sex and prime (all ps ⬎ .35). A
images was similarly affected by the prime and by P sex. Thus, the two-factor ANCOVA for the number of words used in the stories
creativity indices for the two types of stories were combined in the with the premanipulation words as a covariate also showed no
remainder of the analysis. significant main effects of P sex and prime (all ps ⬎ .13) or
The mean rating of creativity for the premanipulation (covari- interaction ( p ⬎ .70). Overall, despite a difference for men in the
ate) stories was 3.83 (SD ⫽ 1.41). Men tended to be more creative creative quality of the stories in the mating prime condition, there
than women (M ⫽ 4.30, SD ⫽ 1.65 for men vs. M ⫽ 3.54, SD ⫽ was no indication that this creative jump was related to exerting
1.15 for women; p ⫽ .011). To test the specific hypotheses of the more effort—spending more time or words— on the stories.
study, we performed two planned comparisons— one for men and
one for women— using the premanipulation creativity scores as a 2
Of the eight measures, humor had the lowest correlation with the other
covariate, which took into account the differences at baseline. For seven measures. This was primarily because very few Ps (less than 20%)
men, the mating prime significantly elevated creativity compared received a rating above 1 on humor. However, when Ps did display humor
with men in the control, t(86) ⫽ 2.81, p ⫽ .006, ␩2 ⫽ .088 (see it was generally considered very creative, and humor still had a correlation
Figure 1). However, for women the same romantic prime had no above .50 with each of the other seven measures.
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 67

Discussion female who does the evaluating. When people are pursuing a
short-term strategy, therefore, it would be expected that only the
The results of Study 1 supported the male-only creative display males would increase their displays of creativity. It seems likely
hypothesis, whereby a mating prime stimulated only men to write that just this kind of a romantic relationship was made salient in
stories that were judged as more creative. Notably, this creative Study 1, in which participants saw photos of multiple attractive
boost occurred despite there being no actual incentive for men to opposite sex strangers and had no information indicating whether
be more creative; that is, the stimuli were photographs and the these strangers were potential long-term mates. Moreover, attrac-
romantic situations were imaginary. This creative increase was tive males may be more likely to be perceived to be inclined
also not accompanied by a parallel increase in either the time spent toward unrestricted mating strategies (Gangestad, Haselton, &
or the number of words used to write the stories. Thus, it does not Buss, in press).
seem that a mating prime generated greater creativity because it Unlike most other mammals, however, human mating often
induced participants to exert more effort into writing the story. involves substantial investment by fathers— up to and including a
Instead, mating cues appear to inspire an increase in the quality of lifetime monogamous commitment of the male’s effort and re-
men’s displays. sources. High male parental investment, though rare in mammals,
tends to coevolve with slow-developing high-cost offspring that
Study 2 are helpless at birth (Geary, 2000). In species with high male
parental investment, fathers and mothers invest a great deal in a
Although the results of Study 1 were supportive of the male- long-term mate, and both sexes tend to be choosy about the
only creative display hypothesis, the findings were limited. For characteristics of a long-term partner. It would follow that when
instance, research indicates that men are more sexually aroused by people are pursuing a long-term mate, both men and women would
visual images than women (Hamann, Herman, Nolan, & Wallen, be under selective pressure to display desirable sexually selected
2004). Because the key part of the mating prime procedure con- characteristics, including the ability to be creative.
sisted of viewing photographs of attractive individuals, it is pos- Study 2 tested whether priming participants with cues for an
sible that this procedure may have been stronger at inducing a explicitly short-term versus a long-term mating situation would
romantic mindset for males than for females. produce a different pattern in creative displays for males versus
However, if the priming procedure was successful for both men females. Given cross-species findings and differences in expected
and women, the lack of a creative spike for females is both slightly parental investment for the two types of mating strategies, two
puzzling and highly instructive. It is puzzling because prior re- predictions were generated. For men, it was predicted that both
search indicates that both men and women indicate a relatively short-term and long-term mating primes would elicit creativity.
equal preference for creativity in a mate (Li et al., 2002). More- However, for women it was predicted that only the long-term
over, because women’s creative displays are likely to be attractive prime would produce a creative boost. To prime the two different
to potential mates, it seems odd that courtship would not spur mating situations, all participants read a short scenario in which
creativity. However, the lack of a creative increase for women in they imagined themselves desiring a short-term or a long-term
Study 1 is also instructive: Perhaps it reflects a tendency for mate. To avoid the potentially problematic sex difference in
women to not display their creativity when encountering a partic- arousal to visual cues, we did not use photographs.
ular type of potential mate but not when encountering other types
of potential mates.
Method

Short-Term and Long-Term Mating Strategies Participants

A closer inspection of the research on human mating indicates Two hundred participants (64 men and 136 women) were recruited from
introductory psychology classes as partial fulfillment of their class
abundant evidence that women and men behave differently in their
requirement.
pursuit of long-term and short-term mates (Buss & Schmitt, 1993;
Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost,
1990). Whether a relationship is likely to be short-term (e.g., a Design and Procedure
one-night stand) or long-term (e.g., a marriage) has vastly different The study design was a between-participants 2 (P sex) ⫻ 3 (Prime:
implications for the expected parental investment of men and short-term mating vs. long-term mating vs. control) design. The general
women. If offspring result from a short-term mateship, the female procedure of the study was very similar to that of Study 1, including using
is likely to contribute the majority of parental investment, whereas the identical method to measure creativity. However, the method used to
the male contributes practically nothing. Within a long-term rela- prime mating was different.
tionship, however, both parents expect to contribute significantly Mating primes. All Ps read and imagined themselves in one of three
to offspring. scenarios (short-term mating, long-term mating, and control) that were of
A short-term relationship is more akin to courtship in the vast similar length (about 850 words). In the short-term scenario, Ps imagined
majority of other mammalian species, in which fathers contribute themselves during the last day of their vacation on an exotic island. On this
last day they met a desirable person and spent a romantic afternoon and had
little or nothing to the offspring. Under such circumstances, fe-
dinner with the new romantic interest. The scenario ends as the two lovers
males tend to be highly selective about their mating partners, are passionately kissing on a moonlit beach. The short-term scenario
choosing only those mates who manifest characteristics associated repeatedly emphasized that the two people would likely never see each
with good genes (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000). Consequently, in other again.
most mammalian species and in many other vertebrate species In the long-term scenario, Ps imagined meeting someone desirable on
such as the peacock, it is the male who does the displaying, and the the university campus. Ps imagined spending a wonderful afternoon and a
68 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

romantic evening with this person, including a candlelight dinner and a


sweet kiss goodnight. Throughout the scenario, the reader ponders that this
person may be a good long-term partner, and the scenario ends as the
reader is anticipating going out on an “official” first date with this person.
In the control scenario, Ps imagined getting ready to go to a much-
anticipated concert with a same-sex friend. During the night of the show,
Ps imagined that they could not find the tickets. However, the scenario has
a happy ending as the friend shows up with the tickets, and they both head
off in a great mood anticipating a delightful musical experience.
To test whether the scenarios were successful at eliciting romantic
feelings, self-report measures of romantic and sexual affect were collected
from a total of 58 male and female Ps. After reading one of the three
scenarios, Ps indicated to what extent they were experiencing sexual and
romantic arousal on a 7-point scale with the endpoints at 1 (not at all) and
7 (very much). Results indicated that there were no differences in the
measures across the two romantic scenarios for men or women (all ps ⬎
.35). However, compared with Ps in the control scenario, Ps who read the
mating scenarios reported significantly more romantic arousal (mating
M ⫽ 5.88, SD ⫽ .61; control M ⫽ 1.85, SD ⫽ .48; p ⬍ .01) and
significantly more sexual arousal (mating M ⫽ 4.93, SD ⫽ .78; control
M ⫽ 1.18, SD ⫽ .29; p ⬍ .01).
Priming booster shots. As in Study 1, the current experiment had a
booster of the prime manipulation after Ps wrote the first postprime
creative story. In the booster for both romantic prime conditions, Ps were
Figure 2. Men’s and women’s story creativity depending on type of
asked to imagine themselves back in the scenario that they read earlier.
prime in Study 2 with standard error bars (adjusted means).
Then, Ps were given up to 3 min to write in detail about what physical
characteristics they would desire in this person. In the control condition, Ps
were also asked to go back to the scenario they read earlier. However, they
wrote about the physical characteristics of the anticipated concert venue.
term condition did indeed write significantly more creative stories
Dependent measures. As in Study 1, four student judges (two men and
two women) who were blind to experimental condition rated the stories on
than men in the control condition, t(193) ⫽ 3.77, p ⫽ .003, ␩2 ⫽
the same creativity dimensions. The amount of time Ps spent writing each .047. Although men in the short-term mating condition also dis-
story and the number of words used were also recorded. played more creativity than men in the control condition, this
difference was only marginally significant, t(193) ⫽ 1.69, p ⫽
.096, ␩2 ⫽ .016.4 Nevertheless, men in the long-term and the
Results
short-term conditions did not differ from one another, t(193) ⫽
Creativity 1.41, p ⫽ .16, and the combination of the two mating conditions
was significantly different from the male control condition,
The eight creativity measures again showed high cohesiveness t(193) ⫽ 2.71, p ⫽ .009, ␩2 ⫽ .037. Thus, both a short-term and
for each of the four judges (alphas for the eight ratings for each a long-term mating prime boosted men’s creative displays to some
judge ranged from .90 –.94), so the eight measures were combined extent.
into a creativity index for each judge. The ratings of all four judges As expected, women in the short-term mating condition showed
also showed high interrater reliability (␣ ⫽ .87 for the cartoon; no significant difference in creativity from women in the control,
␣ ⫽ .88 for the painting). Thus, the ratings for the four judges were t(193) ⫽ .34, p ⫽ .73. This predicted lack of difference concep-
combined into a cartoon creativity index and a painting creativity tually replicated the findings for women from Study 1. However,
index. a planned comparison of the long-term mating prime and the
To test whether the prime had a similar effect on men’s and control conditions did not indicate the predicted increase in cre-
women’s creativity on each type of postmanipulation image, we ativity for women, t(193) ⫽ .98, p ⫽ .32. Thus, neither the
used the same analysis strategy from Study 1. A repeated-measures
mixed-model 3-factor ANCOVA with type of image as a within-
participants factor and the premanipulation ratings of creativity as 3
The lack of interactions also indicates that imagining the physical
a covariate again indicated that type of image did not interact with characteristics of the potential mate in the booster shot did not have a
the other two factors (all ps ⬎ .60). Thus, the creativity indices for different impact on creativity than just reading the original scenario.
4
the two types of stories were combined into one measure.3 The slightly lower creativity for men in the short-term versus the
The mean rating of creativity for the premanipulation (covari- long-term condition can be primarily attributed to 2 Ps, who reported
ate) stories was 4.18 (SD ⫽ 1.44), and males again tended to be currently being in a committed romantic relationship and showed a sizable
more creative than females at baseline (M ⫽ 4.52, SD ⫽ 1.48 for creative drop after imagining pursuing a short-term fling. However, the
identical short-term scenario did produce significant increases in male
males vs. M ⫽ 4.02, SD ⫽ 1.40 for females; p ⫽ .021). To test the
creativity in Studies 3 and 4, and Ps’ current relationship status did not
specific hypotheses of the study, we performed a series of planned significantly affect the results in any of the studies. In addition, analyses of
contrasts using the premanipulation stories as covariate, which Ps’ sociosexual orientation (Simpson & Gangestad, 1991, 1992) and cre-
took into account the differences at baseline. ativity in the different conditions also did not indicate any significant
It was predicted that both of the romantic scenarios would effects, perhaps because the scenarios primed specific mating strategies
increase men’s creativity. As seen in Figure 2, men in the long- irrespective of a person’s sociosexual inclination.
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 69

short-term nor the long-term mating primes boosted women’s and commitment—may have been too ambiguous to ensure that
creativity.5 women would perceive this person to be a truly high-quality
mating prospect. Study 3 was undertaken to examine this
Expended Effort possibility.
Study 3 was conceptually similar to Study 2, except that a new
Two three-factor ANCOVAs (one for time and one for number key condition was added in which men and women read about a
of words) again indicated that type of image (cartoon or painting) long-term mate who was a clearly committed long-term partner.
did not interact with either P sex or prime (all ps ⬎ .60). Thus, the The results of Study 2, along with male and female differences in
effort measures for the two types of stories were collapsed. An parental investment, led to two separate predictions— one for men
ANCOVA for time spent writing the stories again indicated no and one for women. For men, it was predicted that creativity would
significant interaction or main effects of P sex or prime (all ps ⬎ increase after all of the three mating primes. However, the pattern
.40). An ANCOVA for the number of words used in the stories for women was predicted to be distinctly different: The committed
also indicated no significant interaction or main effects (all ps ⬎ long-term mating prime should be the only one to produce a higher
.30). level of creativity when compared with the other conditions, which
should again not differ from one another.
Discussion In an attempt to explore whether specific mating cues can
produce creative boosts in other domains of creativity besides
The results of Study 2 provided further qualified evidence that story-writing, Study 3 also used a different measure of creativity—
cues related to mating can lead to an increase in displayed cre- the Remote Associates Test (RAT; Mednick, 1962). The RAT was
ativity, at least in males. As predicted, men showed an increase in originally developed as an objective test of creativity, whereby
creativity when primed with thoughts of pursuing either a short- creativity is defined in a much narrower scope: Creativity is the
term or a long-term mate. Also as predicted, women did not show ability to make rapid appropriate associations between various
an increase in creativity in the short-term prime condition. How- concepts. Each RAT question consists of providing people three
ever, contrary to prediction, women failed to show an increase in words (e.g., “dress, dial, flower”) and giving them a limited
creativity in the long-term condition. The overall pattern for men amount of time (15 seconds in the current study) to come up with
and women was consistent with the results of Study 1. Also the one correct word linked to all three of the original words
following the results of Study 1, none of the increases in creativity (“sun”). Success on the RAT has been shown to correlate reliably
produced by the mating primes was accompanied by any indica- with success on classic insight problems, and the RAT is often
tion of increased effort to produce the display. used in the study of creative problem solving (Bowden & Beeman,
1998; Schooler & Melcher, 1995).
Study 3 Finally, to assure that the results of Studies 1 and 2 were not
caused by some peculiar effect produced by the content of the
Although the results of Study 2 supported a mating-inspired control conditions, Study 3 used a no-prime control condition.
increase in creativity for men, the supposed long-term mating That is, participants in the control did not read a scenario.
prime did not lead to the predicted increase in women’s creative
displays. Why not? First, it is possible that as in many species, Method
women may simply not display this sexually selected characteristic
in courtship. Instead, women may have evolved creative abilities Participants
primarily to judge the quality of men’s creative displays (Miller,
2000). However, it is possible that our long-term mating prime One hundred and fifty-seven participants (85 men and 72 women) were
recruited from introductory psychology classes to participate as partial
manipulation, which involved preparing for a first date with a man
fulfillment of their class requirement.
who had long-term potential, may have been insufficient to reach
the threshold for women’s mating-linked displays of creativity.
Because women incur significantly higher reproductive costs if Design and Procedure
they are abandoned by their mates, women selecting a long-term
The design of the study was a between-participants 2 (P sex) ⫻ 4
mate should be especially focused on and sensitive to the trust- (Prime: short-term vs. potential long-term vs. committed long-term vs.
worthiness and commitment levels of the man (Haselton & Buss, control) design. The conceptual procedure was similar to that of Study 2,
2000; Hrdy, 1999; Hurtado, Hill, Kaplan, & Hurtado, 1992). except for the addition of a new romantic scenario for the committed
Following this logic, women would need to be assured of the long-term condition and the use of the RAT to assess creativity.
trustworthiness and the commitment level of a potential mate RAT. The RAT can be administered using any number of questions
before committing themselves strongly. This reasoning is consis- while giving Ps any amount of time to answer them. In the current study,
tent with findings that women are, compared with men, slower to Ps saw 40 RAT-like questions and had 15 s to answer each question. The
report feelings of love during courtship and have a higher desire
for commitment from their romantic partner before consenting to 5
In addition to judging creativity, the stories were also judged on the
sex (Peplau, 2003). Women are also, compared with men, rela-
perceived intelligence of the participant. The measures of intelligence
tively more conservative about believing professions of love by showed high concordance with the measures of creativity, whereby the
members of the opposite sex (Haselton & Buss, 2000). Although mating primes led males to be perceived as more intelligent. However, it
the long-term scenario used in Study 2 specified that the imagined may have been difficult to meaningfully separate creativity and intelligence
mate was a potential long-term partner, the overall perceived mate within the judging context, making it uncertain whether mating primes
quality of the person— especially as it relates to trustworthiness indeed boosted a purer form of intelligence display.
70 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

first 20 questions were used to establish a baseline score for each P Results
regarding their general RAT performance. Then, after the priming manip-
ulation, Ps worked on a different set of 20 RAT questions; performance on Creativity
this last set of 20 constituted the dependent measure in the study. All of the
specific RAT questions were adapted from Bowden and Beeman (2003), For the 20 baseline RAT questions, participants solved 8.72
who provide normative statistics regarding the percentage of people who (SD ⫽ 3.44) questions, and there were no sex differences in
tend to solve specific RAT questions within various time limits. The 40 performance. In addition to testing the predictions of the study via
questions used in the current study were shown to have been solved planned contrasts, we also tested whether the predicted contrasts
40%– 60% of the time within a 15-s time limit by university students. Ps explained most of the between-cell variance by testing the residual
in all conditions responded to the same first set of 20 questions (baseline) contrast, which was predicted to be nonsignificant (see Levin &
and to a different set of 20 subsequent questions (dependent measure). The Neumann, 1999).
two sets were matched to be of equal difficulty, and the order of the Male creativity. For men, it was predicted that the three mat-
questions within each set of 20 was randomized. ing primes would produce a higher level of creativity than in the
Mating primes. The same procedure from Study 2 was used to prime control condition. As seen in Figure 3, a planned contrast using the
mating. Two of the romantic scenarios (short-term and potential long-term) baseline RAT measures as covariates indicated that this was in-
were identical to those used in Study 2, except that the original long-term deed the case, F(1, 80) ⫽ 2.58, p ⫽ .012, ␩2 ⫽ .077. Creativity in
scenario has now been labeled potential long-term. To prime cues of a the three mating conditions did not differ from one another ( p ⫽
committed high-quality long-term mate, a brief paragraph was added to the .99). The test of the residual contrast was also not significant,
original long-term scenario. The paragraph contained three new pieces of
Fresidual (2, 80) ⫽ 0.55, ns, meaning that the predicted contrast
information: (a) the couple had already been dating for a while, which was
accounted for most of the between-cell variance.
intended to signify commitment; (b) the P had met and approved of the
Female creativity. For women, it was predicted that only the
target’s friends, which was intended to signify trustworthiness; and (c) the
prime for the committed long-term mate would produce a creative
target had met the P’s friends who had given their approval, which was
intended to signify that the target was good relationship material. Unlike in
boost above that of the other three conditions. As seen in Figure 3,
Studies 1 or 2, Ps in the control condition did not read a story or see any a planned contrast with baseline RAT scores as the covariate
photos. Instead, they were given a short break and saw a blank screen indicated that this was indeed the case, F(1, 72) ⫽ 2.03, p ⫽ .048,
before continuing to the next set of RAT questions. ␩2 ⫽ .056. As would be predicted from Studies 1 and 2, the
Priming booster shot. As in Studies 1 and 2, all Ps received a booster remaining three conditions—short-term, potential long-term, and
shot to refresh the mating prime after completing the first 10 of the second control— did not differ from one another ( p ⫽ .70). The test of the
set of the dependent-measure RAT questions. The specific nature of the residual contrast was also not significant, Fresidual (2, 72) ⫽ 2.96,
booster shot was identical to that in Study 2, in which people were asked ns, meaning that the predicted contrast explained most of the
to write about the physical characteristics of the imagined person from the between-cell variance in creativity. Thus, women showed a cre-
romantic scenario. In the control condition, participants were merely given ative increase only when they were primed with thoughts of a
another short break and saw a blank screen before continuing with the committed long-term mate who was clearly high-quality relation-
remaining 10 RAT questions. ship material.

Figure 3. Men’s and women’s performance on the Remote Associates Test (RAT) depending on type of prime
in Study 3 with standard error bars (adjusted means).
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 71

Differences in Imagined Mate Across Scenarios Mood and Arousal


To examine whether participants indeed perceived the key dif- One possible mechanism for how the romantic primes may have
ferences across the romantic scenarios, we collected additional produced the specific patterns of creativity was by influencing
data to ascertain what Ps actually perceived to be the differences men’s and women’s mood or arousal. To investigate this possibil-
and similarities of the imagined mate in each of the three scenarios. ity, a separate group of a total of 63 male and female Ps underwent
A total of 92 separate male and female Ps underwent the same the priming procedure from Study 3. Afterward, Ps rated to what
priming procedure used in the current study, whereby each person extent they felt positive arousal (energetic, excited, passionate; ␣⫽
only read one of the three scenarios. Afterward, Ps indicated the .75), negative arousal (upset, tense, nervous; ␣ ⫽ .83), and positive
quality of various characteristics regarding the person whom they mood (happy, upbeat, joyful; ␣ ⫽ .83). Ratings were provided on
imagined desiring in the scenario. More specifically, Ps were a 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much) scale.
asked 13 questions in random order that were generally phrased in Results indicated that none of the romantic primes produced
the following way: “To what extent is this person __________ ,” much negative arousal for men or women (Ms between 1.98 and
and Ps provided their responses on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 9 2.40). However, the patterns for positive mood and positive
(very). arousal were instructively different across the different conditions
The questions asked about the imagined mate’s perceived trust- (see Table 2). For men, the two long-term primes produced similar
worthiness (trustworthy, truthful, honest; ␣ ⫽ .93), level of com- levels of mood and arousal, and ratings for both primes were
mitment (committed, faithful, likely to cheat [reverse scored]; ␣ ⫽ slightly (nonsignificantly) higher than those for the short-term
.80), and desirability as a long-term partner (good relationship prime. For women, mood and arousal for the two long-term mating
material, the right person for me, confidence that they’re a good primes also did not differ from each other, and both were signif-
boyfriend or girlfriend; ␣ ⫽ .91). In addition, Ps were also asked icantly higher than the mood and arousal produced by the short-
to report the extent to which the potential mate was seen as being term prime ( p ⬍ .05). This overall pattern for women is quite
creative, intelligent, funny, and physically attractive. To test different from the pattern of women’s performance on the RAT.
whether men and women differed in their perceptions of the mate Specifically, although the two long-term conditions produced
on the characteristics across the scenarios, 13 two-factor analyses highly similar patterns of positive mood and positive arousal,
of variance (P sex ⫻ Prime) were performed. Results indicated no women’s RAT performance in these two conditions differed.
significant interactions (all ps ⬎ .30). Thus, male and female Moreover, despite men’s and women’s different performance on
ratings were combined for the remainder of the analysis. the RAT in the short-term mating condition, the short-term sce-
As seen in Table 1, Ps did indeed perceive different levels of nario produced no sex differences in positive arousal or mood (all
trustworthiness, commitment, and long-term mate value across the ps ⬎ .60). Taken as a whole, the RAT performance patterns
three conditions: The short-term mate was always perceived as produced by the romantic scenarios appear to be different from the
having the least of the three qualities, and the committed long-term mood and arousal patterns produced by these primes.
mate was always perceived as having the most of the three qual-
ities. When compared with the potential long-term scenario, the Discussion
person imagined in the committed long-term scenario was per- Results from Study 3 provided a clearer picture of how mating
ceived as significantly more trustworthy, t(89) ⫽ 2.48, p ⫽ .015, cues influence creativity. Study 3 replicated both the short-term
␩2 ⫽ .064, more committed, t(89) ⫽ 2.10, p ⫽ .038, ␩2 ⫽ .047, and the (potential) long-term findings from Study 2. It is important
and of higher long-term mate quality, t(89) ⫽ 2.08, p ⫽ .040, ␩2 ⫽ to note that these results were replicated with a different measure
.047. Thus, men and women did perceive the key differences of creativity—the RAT—and a no-prime control condition. As
between the two long-term primes. Analyses of Ps’ perceptions of predicted, Study 3 also showed that when participants were primed
physical attractiveness, creativity, intelligence, and humor indi- with the desire for a committed long-term mate—a mate who was
cated no significant differences among the three conditions (all perceived to be more trustworthy, committed, and generally better
ps ⬎ .30), with all of the ratings being relatively high (see Table relationship material— both men and women showed higher levels
1 for ratings of attractiveness and creativity as an example). of creativity. This finding supports the notion that when males or
females are pursuing a mating strategy in which they each expect
to contribute significant parental investment, both sexes are likely
Table 1 to display a sexually selected trait such as creativity. However,
Perceived Attributes in Imagined Mate Depending on Scenario men and women appear to have different mate-quality thresholds
for displaying creativity: For men, the requirements are relatively
Type of romantic scenario low—the desire to attract any desirable woman will do; for
Potential Committed women, the requirements are high— only a desire for a long-term
Perceived attribute Short term long term long term mate who is clearly a high-quality mate produces the display.
Additional data also indicated that the precise pattern of creative
Trustworthy 6.00 (1.58) 6.69 (1.18) 7.53 (1.25) boosts for men and women could not be explained solely by the
Committed 5.32 (1.48) 6.43 (1.16) 7.15 (1.55)
Relationship material 5.83 (1.82) 7.01 (1.46) 7.77 (1.14) arousal or positive mood that were produced by each of the
Intelligent 8.84 (0.76) 8.00 (1.02) 8.16 (1.32) romantic scenarios.
Creative 6.85 (1.46) 6.75 (1.27) 6.97 (1.14)
Study 4
Note. Means are on a 1–9 scale in which higher numbers indicate more
of the perceived characteristic. Numbers in parentheses denote standard Thus far, findings from all three studies show that specific cues
deviations. related to mating can produce an increase in creativity for men and
72 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

Table 2 RAT problems. This question served as a manipulation check of


Men’s and Women’s Mood and Arousal Depending on Scenario the incentive instructions. Analyses indicated that motivation for
men and women did not interact with condition ( p ⬎ .70), and that
Type of romantic scenario men’s and women’s motivation did not differ from each other
Potential Committed
across the three conditions (for men, M ⫽ 4.79, SE ⫽ .16; for
Short term long term long term women, M ⫽ 4.80, SE ⫽ .15), F(2, 161) ⫽ .085, p ⫽ .92. Thus,
the motivation scores for men and women were combined. Results
Positive mood also indicated that Ps in the mating and control conditions also did
Males 4.42 (1.65) 5.00 (1.27) 5.07 (1.16)
not differ from each other in motivation (for mating, M ⫽ 4.59,
Females 4.27 (1.65) 5.70 (0.85) 5.73 (0.77)
Positive arousal SE ⫽ .18; for control, M ⫽ 4.71, SE ⫽ .19; p ⫽ .63). Thus, these
Males 4.18 (1.94) 4.54 (1.61) 4.57 (1.41) two groups were combined into a no-incentive condition.
Females 3.67 (1.90) 5.03 (0.96) 5.20 (0.92) A comparison of the monetary incentive condition and the
combined no-incentive group revealed that participants in the
Note. Means are on a 1–7 scale in which higher numbers indicate a more
intense state. Numbers in parentheses denote standard deviations. monetary incentive condition reported being significantly more
motivated to do well on the RAT items (for monetary incentive,
M ⫽ 5.09, SE ⫽ .19; for no incentive, M ⫽ 4.65, SE ⫽ .13), F(1,
women. However, would mating cues continue to produce a cre- 165) ⫽ 3.90, p ⫽ .051, ␩2 ⫽ .023. Thus, the manipulation to
ative boost when compared with people who are motivated to do induce motivation to do well on the task had the desired effect.
well on the creative task? To test this question, the current study
used a methodology almost identical to that of some conditions of
Study 3, except that it added a key new condition in which
Creativity
participants were provided with a monetary incentive to do well on For the 20 baseline RAT questions, participants solved 8.01
the RAT but in which no romantic prime was used—the monetary (SD ⫽ 3.12) questions and there were no sex differences in
incentive condition. The study had two competing hypotheses with performance. To test the specific hypotheses of the study, we
different implications for the process by which mating cues may performed a series of planned comparisons using the baseline RAT
stimulate creativity. First, it was plausible that Ps in the monetary scores as a covariate. First, examining the men, a planned contrast
incentive and the men in the (short-term) mating condition would revealed the predicted increase in RAT performance in the mating
perform significantly better on the RAT than the Ps in the control condition when compared with the control condition, t(160) ⫽
condition. Such a result would indicate that mating cues might 2.83, p ⫽ .005, ␩2 ⫽ .048. However, as seen in Figure 4, men in
facilitate creativity by motivating people to somehow work harder the mating condition also performed significantly better than men
at the task. However, a different outcome was also deemed plau- in the monetary incentive condition, t(160) ⫽ 2.21, p ⫽ .029, ␩2 ⫽
sible. The second possibility was that men in the mating condition .030. Although men in the monetary incentive condition performed
would perform significantly better than men in any of the other slightly better than men in the control condition, this difference
conditions. Such a result would indicate that mating cues do not
increase creativity simply by leading people to try harder at the
task.

Method
Participants
One hundred and sixty-seven participants (78 men and 89 women) were
recruited from introductory psychology classes to participate as partial
fulfillment of their class requirement.

Design and Procedure


The design of the study was a between-participants 2 (P sex) ⫻ 3
(Condition: mating vs. control vs. monetary incentive) design. The proce-
dures in the (short-term) mating and the control prime conditions were
identical to those of Study 3. In the monetary incentive condition, the
procedure was similar to that of the control, except that before Ps started
the last 20 RAT problems, they received the following instructions: “On
the remaining problems, we would like you to try your hardest to do the
best you can. If you are in the top 30% of everyone’s scores, you will be
entered in a raffle to win $60!”

Results
Motivation Check
Figure 4. Men’s and women’s performance on the Remote Associates
After Ps finished the RAT questions, they were asked to what Test (RAT) depending on condition in Study 4 with standard error bars
extent they were motivated to do well on the 20 postmanipulation (adjusted means).
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 73

was not significant ( p ⫽ .31). For women, there were no signifi- in gibbons, in which each sex invests substantially in offspring.
cant differences across conditions, F(2, 85) ⫽ 2.24, p ⫽ .44. During courtship, both male and female gibbons have evolved to
sing complex and elaborate musical patterns to each other—a
Discussion behavior called dueting (Geissmann, 2000; Raemaekers & Rae-
maekers, 1984). Despite the fact that human males often invest
The results of Study 4 indicated that men primed with mating substantially in shared offspring, women always stand a reasonable
cues performed better on the creativity task even when compared danger of substantially higher costs if they choose a long-term
with people who were trying harder at the task. Previous research mate who does not intend to commit (Kenrick et al., 1990). In this
(Amabile, 1996) has indicated that extrinsic motivation, such as a light, it makes some sense that women require assurance that a
monetary incentive, does not always result in higher creative prospective mate is really going to invest in offspring before
quality, so perhaps the finding that an incentive to do well on the investing the energy in creative displays.
RAT did not significantly improve performance is not surprising.
However, the fact that a mating prime led to a creative boost above The Psychological Mechanism
that produced by merely trying hard at the task suggests that the
process by which mating cues stimulate creativity is not related to The current research examined several plausible psychological
external motivation. Consistent with the findings from Studies 1 processes by which mating cues could stimulate creativity. Results
and 2, the mating-inspired creative boost appears to be unrelated to from three of the studies indicated that mating cues are unlikely to
expending more effort on the creative task. elicit creativity by stimulating one to try harder to be creative. In
Studies 1 and 2, participants neither spent more time nor used more
General Discussion words to write creative stories. In addition, participants given a
monetary incentive in Study 4 still performed worse than people
The current research set out to explore whether priming men and primed with mating cues. Although it is possible that a chance to win
women with a variety of theoretically relevant mating cues would $60 may not have been a strong incentive, participants in this condi-
lead to an increase in creativity. This question was tested in four tion nevertheless reported being more motivated to try harder.
studies, in which men and women first either looked at photo- Study 3 indicated that the mood and arousal states produced by
graphs of attractive opposite sex individuals (Study 1) or imagined each of the specific romantic scenarios showed a distinctly differ-
being in a particular romantic scenario (Studies 2– 4), and then ent pattern from that of creativity. For instance, although women
performed tasks assessing creativity on subjective (Studies 1 & 2) were equally positively aroused by the potential and the committed
or objective (Studies 3 & 4) measures. For men, cues designed to long-term mate scenarios, only the committed scenario produced a
stimulate a motive to attract either a desirable short-term or a creative boost for women. Moreover, men and women reported
long-term mate produced an increase in creativity. That is, after relatively equal mood and arousal after reading the short-term
just thinking of attracting a desirable woman as any kind of a scenario, although only men showed a creative increase in that
romantic partner, men showed an increase in creativity— even if situation. Although the mood and arousal ratings were self-
they themselves could not actually benefit romantically from this reported, these findings suggest that the particular pattern of in-
creative burst in the current setting. In contrast, women only creases in creativity cannot be explained solely by changes in
increased their creative output after imagining wanting to attract a mood and arousal— or at least the kind of general arousal typically
clearly high-quality (i.e., trustworthy and committed) long-term measured in psychological research.
mate. Women did not show a creative increase when primed to A third process, which we believe is most likely to underlie these
think about attracting a short-term mate or a potential long-term effects, can be inferred from Studies 3 and 4, in which the RAT was
mate who had yet to prove his worth as good relationship material. used to assess creativity. Because the purpose of each RAT question
These findings are consistent with the theories of sexual selec- is to ascertain whether a person can rapidly locate a particular concept
tion and differential parental investment. When pursuing a short- in their mind, the RAT is an appropriate and rather specific test of the
term mating strategy, women are expected to provide the vast accessibility of various concepts. The results of Study 4 indicated that
majority of parental investment. In other species, selectively high merely trying harder did not improve performance on the RAT;
female investment is also associated with courtship displays pri- however, a mating prime did improve performance, most likely be-
marily by males. For example, male bowerbirds, who do not cause it enabled superior accessibility to relevant but remote infor-
contribute direct resources to offspring, have evolved to construct mational links. The results of Studies 1 and 2 are also consistent with
elaborate bowers during courtship; they then artfully decorate the the possibility that mating cues enabled better accessibility to various
bowers with a colorful assortment of flower petals, berries, and concepts: By having access to a richer set of creative avenues brought
snail shells, or even “paint” it with regurgitated bluish residue about by the romantic primes, people could write stories that were
(Borgia, 1986). Females inspect the bowers and preferentially more creative.
mate with males who have the largest, most symmetric, and best
decorated displays because, like the peacock’s tail, the creative Alternative Explanations
bower displays seem to serve as a reliable indicator of fitness
(Borgia, 1995). The current research has adopted a framework inspired by existing
When pursuing a long-term mate, however, both men and theory regarding sexual and natural selection accounts of creativity
women expect to invest significantly in offspring. In other species, (Miller, 2000). It would surely be possible to derive predictions
high male parental investment is associated with both sexes being regarding creativity and mating from several other theoretical per-
choosier when selecting a mate and with both sexes tending to spectives, though none seem to offer as straightforward an account of
display desirable qualities in courtship. Such a pattern can be seen the pattern of results obtained in this series of studies. For example, it
74 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

is possible that any vivid fantasy could prime a creative process and dict that the presence of a desirable mate is likely to produce the
lead to an increase in creativity. Thus, for women, perhaps only strongest creative boost, as long as other forces, such as social
imagining a high-quality long-term mate may have led to a fantasy anxiety, are not working against the display. The current studies
that was vivid. However, because both the potential and committed also explored only two specific forms of creativity in one culture.
long-term mating primes were very similar to each other but only one However, the proposed evolutionary framework would predict that
of them produced an increase in creativity, this explanation seems mating-related cues should spur creativity across cultures, al-
unlikely. Not only did both primes produce highly similar mood and though the specific forms of creativity will surely depend on
arousal for women, but both primes are likely to have produced cultural or local norms (Norenzayan & Heine, 2005; Norenzayan,
relatively equally vivid fantasies. Schaller, & Heine, in press). Moreover, there may also be multiple
Perhaps an association between creativity and mating arises person variables that could be used to better predict the display.
because of simple mechanisms of associative priming (Srull & For instance, mating cues might lead to a boost in people’s most
Wyer, 1979; see Higgins, 1996 for a review). For example, Char- favored or best-practiced form of creativity: a musician might
trand, Fitzsimons, and Fitzsimons (2004) have shown that partic- become more musically creative, a comedian might be seen as
ipants primed with thoughts about Apple computers—a brand funnier, a poet might be inspired to display verbal fluidity, and a
marketed as the computer for creative individuals— became more dancer may become more expressive through movement. Future
creative compared with people primed with other computer brands. research might also examine why creativity isn’t always “turned
Although priming participants with photos of attractive individuals on” in men as it appears to confer significant benefits. One
or romantic scenarios may very well activate concepts of creativ- possibility is that there are costs associated with allocating one’s
ity, it is difficult to see how an associative model framework could energies to permanent creative displays, such as decreased capac-
account for the very specific pattern of sex differences and sex ity to attend to other matters or decreased functioning of short-term
similarities found across the different conditions. memory. Another possibility is that there are limitations in most
The functional framework used in this research is by no means people’s abilities to be creative, and that creativity is in a sense a
an alternative to the associative network model of cognition. Both form of “truth in advertising” for a highly functioning brain (see
models imply that there are certain links between motivation, Miller, 2000).
cognition, and behavior. However, the functional model does more Although the current findings are consistent with a sexual se-
than just assert that priming specific ideas will lead to the activa- lection account of creativity, these studies were not designed to
tion of associatively linked semantic and affective categories. ascertain whether creativity is an exclusively sexually selected
Rather, the functional model leads to more finely articulated pre- versus naturally selected trait. Indeed, it is often difficult to draw
dictions about how activating specific functional goals should lead a line between sexual selection and natural selection, partly be-
to specific goal-consistent—and sex-consistent— cognitive and cause mating choices are often based on traits that are themselves
behavioral responses (Maner et al., 2005). naturally selected adaptations (such as symmetrical and healthy
A social learning model might also suggest that men and women feather displays or the ability to fly well in birds). Thus, it is
have been differentially rewarded for producing creativity, al- plausible that some more rudimentary form of creativity in humans
though it is again difficult to predict the precise prime-specific may have originally evolved because of its association with sur-
pattern of sex differences and similarities that we found from this vival ability (e.g., enhancing ability to create new tools or verbally
perspective. Further, the final study suggested that tangible re- negotiate one’s way out of a conflict). Given that such abilities
wards actually failed to increase creativity. A social role theory were adaptive for whatever reason, the members of one sex would
might also posit that it is part of the male role to be creative. have been well served to use them as cues to infer mate value in
However, without a consideration of sex differences in parental the other sex. At that point, it would have become useful to display
investment, it would be difficult for that theory to have predicted creativity as a courtship tactic. Thus, creativity would be a function
a priori why creativity is part of the male role to be creative in all of both natural and sexual selection. That is, creativity might
mating contexts but part of the female role to be creative only in provide a survival advantage and function as a heritable fitness
committed long-term mating contexts. indicator (Haselton & Miller, in press). The fact that our research
Neither social role theories nor social learning theories are indicates that creativity is likely to be displayed by males across
mutually exclusive with evolutionary accounts, as evolutionary mating contexts, but only selectively by females in contexts asso-
theorists presume that social roles across societies are to some ciated with quality long-term mates, does suggest, however, that
extent a function of evolved adaptations in men and women and sexual selection plays some role in human creative displays (what-
that many behaviors involve an adaptive interplay of learning and ever their ultimate origin).
evolved predispositions (Kenrick, Trost, & Sundie, 2004; Öhman A larger question pertains to how creativity fits in with other
& Mineka, 2001). We are not aware, however, of social role or possibly related constructs that may also be in part sexually se-
social learning theorists who have offered predictions for the lected. Although the kinds of creativity measured in the first two
pattern of results obtained here—a pattern which follows directly studies (story-writing) may be similar to artistic displays, the
from considerations of sexual selection and parental investment. creativity displayed via the RAT in the latter two studies appears
to be more closely related to problem solving rather than artistic
Limitations and Future Directions ability. From the perspective adopted here, creativity is likely to be
related to intelligence (Miller, 2000). In this light, future research
One of the limitations of the current research is that it did not might address whether mating motives provide a boost in many
test whether people become more creative during actual courtship types of creative intelligence or perhaps whether only specific
(as suggested by anecdotal evidence). Although future research forms of creative intelligence are associated with courtship dis-
needs to explore this question, the current framework would pre- plays. If such displays evolved as fitness indicators, one interesting
ROMANTIC MOTIVES AND CREATIVITY 75

possibility is that those individuals who show greater creative Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection.
boosts in response to mating primes might also be more likely to Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 559 –570.
possess characteristics associated with heritable fitness. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An
evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100,
204 –232.
Conclusion Byrne, R. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence.
Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
This set of studies began with a puzzling question: Why are muses Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impres-
predominantly women who inspire men? Four experiments indicated sion formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming
that when men merely thought of pursuing a desirable romantic reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality
partner, either for a long-term relationship or a short-term fling, such and Social Psychology, 71, 464 – 478.
thoughts consistently produced a “muse effect,” whereby they Chartrand, T. L., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2004, February).
boosted male creativity. These findings are consistent with the expe- The effects of priming anthropomorphized objects on behavior. Paper
riences of Picasso and other male artists who were inspired by female presented at the meeting for the Society for Personality and Social
muses. The findings also generally align with the view that creative Psychology, Austin, TX.
displays may be linked to sexual selection, which tends to select for Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social ex-
more intensive male displays, as witnessed in the courtship tail dis- change. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted
plays of peacocks. Unlike in peacocks, however, because men often mind (pp. 163–228). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
Crespelle, J. P. (1969). Picasso and his women. New York: Hodder &
invest heavily in their offspring, women are also subject to sexual
Stoughton.
selection pressures when it comes to long-term mating. Consistent
Cronin, H. (1993). The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual selection
with parental investment theory, when women were primed with cues
from Darwin to today. New York: Cambridge University Press.
designed to instill a desire to attract a high-quality long-term mate, Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man, and sexual selection in relation to
these cues also served to produce a muse effect for women by sex (Vols. 1 & 2). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (Original
spurring their creativity.6 work published 1871)
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cam-
bridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
6 Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (in press). Evolutionary
The renowned Victorian poet Elizabeth Barrett Browning may provide
foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences.
one such example (Winwar, 1950). Mired in artistic mediocrity in her 20s,
Psychological Inquiry.
Elizabeth received a letter from an adoring fan, Robert Browning, who
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). On the evolutionary psychol-
professed his love for her. Not surprisingly, Elizabeth was unconvinced of
ogy of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral
his intentions until a year—and many of Robert’s letters—later, at which
and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–587.
point the two lovers agreed to elope in the near-distant future. It was
precisely during this phase of her relationship when Elizabeth was inspired Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human pater-
to pen Sonnets from the Portuguese, which would become her greatest and nal investment. Psychological Review, 126, 55–77.
most critically acclaimed creative work. Geissmann, T. (2000). Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus:
I. Structure and organisation. Primate Report, 56, 33– 60.
Gibson, K. R., & Ingold, T. (Eds.). (1993). Tools, language, and cognition
References in human evolution. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social psy- Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Cialdini, R. B., &
chology of creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview. Kenrick, D. T. (in press). Going along versus going alone: When
Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer- fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. Journal of
sity Press. Personality and Social Psychology.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinant of social
Gutierres, S. E., Kenrick, D. T., & Partch, J. J. (1999). Beauty, dominance,
interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
and the mating game: Contrast effects in self-assessment reflect gender
motivation and social cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2,
differences in mate selection. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,
pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press.
25, 1126 –1134.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of
Hamann, S., Herman, R. A., Nolan, C. L., & Wallen, K. (2004). Men and
being. American Psychologist, 54, 464 – 479.
women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nature
Borgia, G. (1986). Sexual selection in bowerbirds. Scientific American,
254, 92–100. Neuroscience, 7, 411– 416.
Borgia, G. (1995). Complex male display and females choice in the spotted Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new
bowerbird: Specialized functions for different bower decorations. Ani- perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality
mal Behavior, 49, 1291–1301. and Social Psychology, 78, 81–91.
Bowden, E. M., & Beeman, M. J. (1998). Getting the right idea: Semantic Haselton, M. G., & Miller, G. F. (in press). Women’s fertility across the
activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems. cycle increases the short-term attractiveness of creative intelligence
Psychological Science, 9, 435– 440. compared to wealth. Human Nature.
Bowden, E. M., & Beeman, M. J. (2003). Normative data for 144 com- Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability,
pound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instru- and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social
ments, and Computers, 35, 634 – 639. psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York:
Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A Guilford Press.
domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 187–219. Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature: Maternal instincts and how they shape
Buss, D. M. (2005). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind the human species. New York: Ballantine Books.
(2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Hurtado, A. M., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., & Hurtado, I. (1992). Trade-offs
76 GRISKEVICIUS, CIALDINI, AND KENRICK

between female food acquisition and child care among Hiwi and Ache Peplau, L. A. (2003). Human sexuality: How do men and women differ?
foragers. Human Nature, 3, 185–216. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 37– 40.
Kanazawa, S. (2000). Scientific discoveries as cultural displays: A further Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.
test of Miller’s courtship model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, Prose, F. (2002). The lives of the muses: Nine women and the artists they
317–321. inspired. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.
Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary Raemaekers, J. J., & Raemaekers, P. M. (1984). Vocal interactions be-
psychology: Individual decision-rules and emergent social norms. Psy- tween two male gibbons, Hylobates lar. Natural History Bulletin of the
chological Review, 110, 3–28. Siam Society, 32, 95–106.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990). Evolution, Roney, J. R. (2003). Effects of visual exposure to the opposite sex:
traits, and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental invest- Cognitive aspects of mate attraction in human males. Personality and
ment model. Journal of Personality, 58, 97–117. Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 393– 404.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., & Keefe, R. C. (1998). Evolutionary Schaller, M. (2003). Ancestral environments and motivated social percep-
cognitive psychology: The missing heart of modern cognitive science. In tion: Goal-like blasts from the evolutionary past. In S. J. Spencer, S.
C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychol- Fein., M. P. Zanna, & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Motivated social perception
ogy (pp. 485–514). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (pp. 215–231). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kenrick, D. T., Trost, M. R., & Sundie, J. M. (2004). Sex-roles as Schaller, M., Faulkner, J., Park, J. H., Neuberg, S. L., & Kenrick, D. T.
adaptations: An evolutionary perspective on gender differences and (2004). Impressions of danger influence impressions of people: An
similarities. In A. H. Eagly, A. Beall, & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Psychology evolutionary perspective on individual and collective cognition. Journal
of gender (pp. 65–91). New York: Guilford Press. of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 2, 231–247.
Kerr, G., & Gagliardi, C. (2004). Measuring creativity in research and Schooler, J. W., & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In S. M.
Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition
practice. Unpublished manuscript, Arizona State University, Tempe.
approach (pp. 249 –268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kingdon, J. (1993). Self-made man and his undoing. New York: Simon &
Scott, J. P. (1980). The function of emotions in behavioral systems: A
Schuster.
systems theory analysis. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion:
Levin, J. R., & Neumann, E. (1999). Testing for predicted patterns: When
Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.
interest in the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Psychological
Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental,
Methods, 4, 44 –57.
and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 1.
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The
Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in
necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the trade-offs.
sociosexuality: Evidence for converging and discriminant validity. Jour-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947–955.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 870 – 883.
MacGregor-Hastie, R. (1988). Picasso’s women. London: Lennard
Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1992). Sociosexuality and romantic
Publishing. partner choice. Journal of Personality, 60, 31–51.
Maner, J., Kenrick, D., Becker, D., Robertson, T., Hofer, B., Neuberg, S., Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the
et al. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social motives interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and
can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660 –
Psychology, 88, 63–78. 1672.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Pros-
Psychological Review, 69, 220 –232. pects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity
Miller, G. F. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In R. Dunbar, (pp. 3–15). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture (pp. 71–91). Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Precis of “simple heuristics that
Edinburgh, United Kingdom: Edinburgh University Press. make us smart.” Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 1, 727–780.
Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B.
evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man: 1871–1971
Møller, A. P., & Petrie, M. (2002). Condition dependence, multiple sexual (pp. 136 –179). Chicago: Aldine.
signals, and immunocompetence in peacocks. Behavioral Ecology, 13, Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount
248 –253. the future? Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 271(Suppl. 4),
Norenzayan, A., & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals across 177–179.
cultures: What are they and how do we know? Psychological Bulletin, Winwar, F. (1950). The immortal lovers: Elizabeth Barrett & Robert
131, 763–784. Browning; a Biography. New York: Harper.
Norenzayan, A., Schaller, M., & Heine, S. J. (in press). Evolution and Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece
culture. In M. Schaller, J. Simpson, & D. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and of Darwin’s puzzle. New York: Oxford University Press.
social psychology. New York: Psychology Press.
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Received June 30, 2005
Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Revision received October 24, 2005
Review, 108, 483–522. Accepted October 31, 2005 䡲
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 77–96 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.77

Navigating the Interdependence Dilemma: Attachment Goals and the Use


of Communal Norms With Potential Close Others
Jennifer A. Bartz and John E. Lydon
McGill University

Four studies investigated attachment in the context of new relationship development. Anxiously attached
individuals overwhelmingly used communal norms and avoided using exchange norms when interacting
with a potential close other; however, when a potential close other used communal norms, anxious
individuals experienced increased interpersonal anxiety. Anxious individuals also used discrete commu-
nal behaviors to diagnose relationship potential. By contrast, secure individuals were more comfortable
in potential communal situations. Moreover, implicit thoughts about closeness were associated with
improved performance on a mental concentration task for secure individuals, whereas implicit closeness
thoughts were associated with poorer performance for anxious individuals. Finally, avoidant individuals
disliked the potential close other when the other used communal norms and downplayed relational
motives for the other’s communal behavior.

Keywords: attachment, communal, exchange, relationship development, interdependence dilemma

The need to establish close bonds is a basic human characteristic the human experience, understanding the factors that are involved
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Maslow, 1962; in the development of these bonds is an important endeavor.
Sullivan, 1953). The achievement of close bonds has been found to At the outset of a relationship, there is a great deal of uncer-
be associated with improved mental and physical well-being tainty: “Is the other person equally interested in developing a
(House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, relationship?” “How do I communicate interest?” “Will I be re-
2001), whereas the failure to achieve closeness has been linked to jected?” People want to demonstrate interest and commitment but
mental (Bowlby, 1969; Davilia, Burge, & Hammen, 1997; Leary, are reluctant because trust is not yet established. Ironically, as
1990) and physical illness (Lynch, 1979) and suicide (Trout, Holmes (1991) noted, although feelings of trust influence one’s
1980). Because of the central role that close relationships play in level of involvement, trust cannot be assessed unless one is at least
somewhat involved. Thus, one is presented with an interdepen-
dence dilemma in which the desire to express interest and com-
mitment must be weighed against the risk of rejection. Impor-
Jennifer A. Bartz and John E. Lydon, Department of Psychology, tantly, though the ability to navigate the interdependence dilemma
McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
has consequences for relationship progress: If one is not willing to
Portions of this research were presented in symposia presentations at the
Fourth Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychol-
take a leap of faith, the relationship is unlikely to get off the
ogy, Los Angeles, California, February 2003, and at the Fifth Annual ground. Although some find it relatively easy to deal with this
Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Austin, uncertainty, others find it more difficult. What factors are associ-
Texas, January 2004. In addition, Study 3 was presented at the Seventh ated with the ability to navigate the interdependence dilemma?
Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Attachment theory has made substantial contributions to the
Palm Springs, California, January 2006. This research is based on Jennifer understanding of close relationships (Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Fraley
A. Bartz’s doctoral dissertation. & Shaver, 2000; Hazan & Shaver, 1987, 1994; Mikulincer &
This research was supported in part by a Social Sciences and Humanities
Shaver, 2003; Simpson & Rholes, 1998). Although considerable
Research Council of Canada doctoral fellowship and the Mary Louise
Taylor McGill Major fellowship to Jennifer A. Bartz and by grants from
research has been conducted to investigate attachment in the
the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and the context of established adult relationships, less research has been
Fonds Pour la Formation de Chercheurs et l’Aide a la Recherche (Quebec, conducted to probe the role of attachment in the context of new
Canada) to John E. Lydon. We thank Mark Baldwin and Jeff Simpson for relationship development. Yet the notion that attachment models
their helpful comments and suggestions, as well as Niall Bolger and Ringo play a part in new relationships, guiding interpersonal perceptions,
Moon-Ho Ho for advising about statistical analyses. We also gratefully expectations, and behaviors with new partners, is a basic assump-
acknowledge Hugo Gagnon, Sarah Kerner, Dominique Pipher, Kristin tion of adult attachment theory (Collins & Read, 1994; Fraley &
Tallman, Laura Monner, Rachel Firmer, Clara Wagner, and Miriam Ro-
Shaver, 2000; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003;
zenek for their help in conducting this research.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jennifer Pietromonaco & Feldman Barrett, 2000). The goal of this research
A. Bartz, who is now at the Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School was to explore attachment differences in the development of
of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1230, New York, NY closeness, specifically with regard to the interdependence dilemma
10029. E-mail: jennifer.bartz@mssm.edu people face at the outset of a close relationship.

77
78 BARTZ AND LYDON

Close Relationships and the Use of Communal and ably to signal interest in a communal relationship, but people use
Exchange Norms exchange norms when the potential for friendship does not exist.

What is a close relationship? One feature distinguishing close


New Relationship Development and the Interdependence
relationships from more casual relationships is the set of norms
that govern the giving and receiving of benefits. According to
Dilemma
Clark and colleagues (Clark, 1984a, 1984b; Clark & Mills, 1979; Lydon, Jamieson, and Holmes (1997) conducted a series of studies
Clark, Mills, & Powell, 1986), close relationships, such as those focusing on the awkward position people face when they hope to
between family members, friends, and romantic partners, are as- establish a communal relationship with an acquaintance. These re-
sociated with communal norms, whereas more casual relation- searchers found that when people desire a communal relationship with
ships, such as those between business partners, acquaintances, and someone, consistent with Clark’s (1984b) theory, they appear to know
strangers, are associated with exchange norms. Communal norms the communal script (i.e., give freely without concern for reciproca-
reflect a genuine concern for the welfare of the other. Benefits are tion), and they try to behave communally, for example, by going out
given on the basis of need or to please, and people do not keep of their way to do a favor for or help the potential friend. However,
track of individual contributions. Giving help to or doing a favor putting themselves on the line in this way creates anxiety, and to
for the other person does not necessitate that the other person reduce their anxiety, people look for signs of interest and commitment
reciprocate. Likewise, receiving help or a favor does not require (“Does he or she care about me?”), especially in patterns of social
one to respond in kind. By comparison, exchange norms reflect the exchange. As Holmes (1991) noted, patterns of social exchange are
idea that no obligation is felt toward the needs of the other person. often used to diagnose signs of caring because they yield information
Benefits are given in return for benefits received or with the about the other’s willingness to make personal sacrifices and to
expectation of repayment, and people tend to keep track of indi- respond to one’s needs. However, Holmes (1981, 1991) warned, this
vidual contributions. In essence, the use of communal norms microlevel perspective on social exchange patterns and on the other’s
means that benefits and aid are given freely and that if there is behavior can have the ironic effect of increasing feelings of vulnera-
reciprocation, it is performed to meet the needs of the other person, bility. Specifically, he argued, when people look to discrete behaviors
whereas the use of exchange norms means that benefits and aid are to diagnose relationship potential, they tend to attach more meaning to
not given freely and that receiving a benefit or aid calls for prompt those behaviors than is appropriate and, as a result, the importance of
reciprocation, preferably in kind, to eradicate the outstanding debt. the behavior for the relationship becomes exaggerated; moreover, he
Clark and colleagues have found considerable support for this stated, when discrete behaviors are analyzed in isolation, it tends to
theory. In one study (Clark, 1984b), participants worked with a lead to perceived imbalances in what was given relative to what has
confederate on a task for a shared reward. On completion of the been received (Holmes, 1981, 1991).
task, participants were responsible for dividing the reward between Thus, at the outset of a relationship, there is a strong desire for
the two group members. The dependent variable was whether closeness but uncertainty about the other’s motives, and this cre-
participants chose to work with the same color pen as or a different ates a great deal of anxiety. The hope is that the relationship will
color pen from their partner. Choosing a different color pen more move forward, but for this to happen, feelings of anxiety and
often than chance was thought to indicate exchange norms, as uncertainty need to be regulated. This leads to a vigilant monitor-
individual contributions would be clear, whereas choosing the ing of the other’s behavior in an effort to confirm the other’s
same color pen more often than chance was thought to indicate the interest and commitment. Ironically, though, this vigilant monitor-
avoidance of exchange norms and the use of communal norms,1 as ing and microlevel perspective can perpetuate the very feelings of
individual contributions to the task would be obscured. Supporting anxiety and uncertainty they are performed to control.
Clark’s theory, participants avoided using exchange norms when
they believed friendship with their partner was possible—presum-
ably to signal their interest in a relationship with the other per- Adult Attachment Theory
son— but participants used exchange norms when they thought According to adult attachment theory, over the course of re-
friendship was unlikely. peated interactions with significant others, individuals develop
In a second study investigating the behavior of existing friends
(communal condition) and strangers (exchange condition), Clark
(1984b) found that whereas strangers generally chose to work with 1
The distinction between the avoidance of the use of exchange norms
a different color pen from their partner (i.e., used exchange and the use of communal norms is important. Simply not keeping track of
norms), pen choice for existing friends was random. It was theo- individual contributions (indicated by random pen choice) is thought to
rized that the existing friends did not go out of their way to actively reflect the use of communal norms, as it suggests participants were not
avoid using exchange norms because they had an established anticipating using task contribution information when it came time to
relationship and, therefore, should not need to send a message distribute the reward. By comparison, the active avoidance of the use of
exchange norms (indicated by participants’ choosing the same color pen
signaling interest in friendship (Clark, 1984b). It is important to
significantly more often than chance) is thought to reflect an effort to avoid
note, however, that random pen choice is consistent with the use of looking as though one would prefer an exchange relationship (Clark,
communal norms because it suggests participants were not trying 1984b). As Clark (1984b) stated, “when people are trying to form a
to keep track of individual contributions. In sum, these studies communal relationship, they are not only concerned with following com-
suggest that people avoid using exchange norms when the poten- munal norms but also with avoiding any perception on others’ parts . . . that
tial (and desire) exists for friendship with another person, presum- they might prefer an exchange relationship” (p. 553).
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 79

mental models for close relationships that contain beliefs about tionships and to engage in such non-attachment-related activities
whether the self is worthy of love and affection and whether others as affiliation and exploration (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003).
are trustworthy and reliable (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, 1994; Miku-
lincer & Shaver, 2003). Moreover, in addition to the importance of The Present Investigations
attachment models in guiding cognition, theorists have begun to
stress the emotion and behavioral regulation properties of the We believe the attachment system should be critical in guiding
attachment system when describing differences in attachment (Fra- perceptions, expectations, and behaviors with potential relation-
ley & Shaver, 2000; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). Whereas adult ship partners, especially with respect to navigating the interdepen-
attachment models have been conceptualized in different ways, dence dilemma. Although attachment models arise over the course
current theory emphasizes a dimensional approach in which dif- of repeated interactions with significant others and primarily re-
ferences in attachment lie along the two dimensions of anxiety and flect expectations about close others, we believe they nevertheless
avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Fraley & Waller, should come into play with potential partners—that is, individuals
1998). with whom one would like to be close. Collins and Read (1994)
Attachment anxiety is associated with a heightened desire for argued that chronic attachment models should be especially influ-
closeness and intimacy combined with concerns about attachment- ential in interpersonal situations in which little is known about the
figure (un)availability. Furthermore, attachment anxiety is theo- other person (also see Pietromonaco & Feldman Barrett, 2000).
rized to be associated with the use of hyperactivation strategies Interpersonal situations occurring at the outset of a relationship are
that aim to secure attention from an unresponsive or inconsistently ambiguous, and little is known about the other person; thus, people
responsive attachment figure (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). Spe- should be especially likely to draw on their working models of
cifically, as Mikulincer and Shaver (2003) described, attachment attachment to guide expectations and to gauge behavior. Collins
anxiety is associated with hypervigilance and increased sensitivity and Read (1994) also noted that attachment models should come
to cues of acceptance and rejection, attachment figures are in- into play when the situation is relevant to chronic attachment goals
tensely monitored, and efforts are made to maintain contact with (e.g., seeking acceptance or establishing independence). To the
extent that people see these situations as opportunities to satisfy
the attachment figure. Furthermore, as these researchers noted,
chronic attachment goals, attachment models and associated be-
attachment anxiety is associated with a preoccupation with self-
liefs, plans, and strategies should come into play (Mikulincer &
worth, sensitivity to one’s internal distress, and emotion-focused
Shaver, 2003).
coping (e.g., ruminating over worries and concerns). Finally, be-
cause the attachment system is chronically engaged in the pursuit
of attachment-related goals, individuals high in attachment anxiety Hypotheses
tend to have few resources left for the pursuit of such nonattach-
Situations involving the desire and opportunity for closeness
ment endeavors as exploration and affiliation (Mikulincer &
should activate the chronic goals (for closeness and acceptance)
Shaver, 2003).
and the hyperactivation strategies associated with attachment anx-
Attachment avoidance, on the other hand, is associated with a
iety and should magnify the approach–avoidance conflict associ-
strong need for independence and the tendency to minimize the ated with this attachment orientation. Thus, we predicted that in
importance of closeness. As Mikulincer and Shaver (2003) ex- potential communal situations (i.e., when interacting with an at-
plained, attachment figures are seen as unreliable and unable to tractive, friendly, available partner), individuals high in anxiety
provide protection, and so, to deal with ensuing feelings of vul- and low in avoidance ( preoccupied) would be especially likely to
nerability, attachment needs are denied, and self-reliance is pur- signal their interest in a communal relationship by avoiding the use
sued. Thus, in contrast to attachment anxiety, attachment avoid- of exchange norms. At the same time, given their uncertainty about
ance is associated with the use of deactivation strategies that the reliability of others and their chronic concerns about self-
function to prevent distress from the failure to attain closeness by worth, anxious individuals should be most susceptible to interde-
shutting down the attachment system (Mikulincer & Shaver, pendence dilemma concerns inherent in these situations. Interde-
2003). In this case, monitoring of the attachment figure is avoided, pendence dilemma concerns should also be particularly salient
and efforts are made to prevent confrontations with threatening because, in contrast to their more secure counterparts, anxiously
information that could activate the attachment system and cause attached individuals’ strong desire for closeness means they have
distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). In contrast to attachment more to lose if the relationship does not materialize. Thus, we
anxiety, attachment avoidance is associated with distance coping predicted that, ironically, when the potential close other used
in which efforts are made to suppress worries and concerns (Miku- communal norms, anxious individuals would feel especially anx-
lincer & Shaver, 2003). ious and uncertain (“Is this person really communicating interest in
Finally, attachment security (reflecting low anxiety and low me?”) and concerned about self-worth (“How am I coming across?
avoidance) is associated with comfort with closeness and auton- Is this person attracted to me?”). Furthermore, given their uncer-
omy and the use of primary attachment strategies (e.g., seeking tainty about others’ interpersonal motives, anxious individuals
closeness and support) in times of need, rather than the secondary should require greater reassurance and, thus, should be especially
strategies of hyperactivation or deactivation. Basically, a history of likely to focus on discrete events in an effort to diagnose relation-
attachment-figure availability reinforces the beliefs that people are ship potential. Finally, anxious individuals’ interpersonal uncer-
generally well intentioned and that the world is a safe place, thus tainty and anxiety should have consequences: In situations involv-
allowing more securely attached individuals to develop new rela- ing the possibility of closeness, anxiously attached individuals’
80 BARTZ AND LYDON

interpersonal concerns should interfere with their ability to engage different color pen from their partner) to signal their aversion to
in non-attachment-related activities. closeness. Finally, it was predicted that although secure individu-
Potential communal situations should also arouse the chronic als might not actively avoid using exchange norms, nevertheless,
goals and deactivation strategies associated with attachment avoid- they would not adopt an exchange orientation (i.e., their pen choice
ance. Highly avoidant individuals, especially those who are low in would be random).
anxiety (dismissive), are distrustful of close others, desire indepen-
dence and self-reliance, and prefer to maintain a distance between Method
themselves and close others. Thus, it was predicted that avoidant
individuals would use exchange norms with a potential close other Participants
to signal their aversion to intimacy and to establish boundaries.
Seventy university students volunteered to participate. Single partici-
Similarly, when a potential close other communicated interest in
pants were targeted to ensure that they would be available and interested in
closeness, it was theorized that although avoidant individuals a communal relationship with the confederate (Clark, 1986). Three partic-
would likely be concerned about the other encroaching on their ipants were excluded from the analyses because they used their own pen to
independence, they might nevertheless regulate their distress by work on the group task. Thus, there were 67 participants in the final sample
avoiding confrontations with these unpleasant thoughts. Thus, we (32 men and 35 women, M age ⫽ 19.5 years). Fifty-six participants
predicted that when a potential close other signaled interest, described themselves as single, and 11 participants described themselves as
avoidant individuals would disparage the other so as to avoid dating.2 Participants received either extra credit or $10 (Canadian) for their
dealing with the possibility of closeness (if the other is seen as participation.
unattractive or unlikable, one does not have to think about what
might happen in a relationship with that person). Finally, given Procedure
their desire to downplay the importance of closeness, avoidant
individuals should be less likely to make relational attributions for University students were recruited to participate in a study supposedly
investigating group performance and monetary incentives. In trying to
a potential close other’s communal behavior.
create the potential for a communal relationship, we followed Clark’s
In contrast to their more insecure counterparts, secure individ- (1984b, 1986) induction and had the participant interact with an attractive,
uals should demonstrate greater interpersonal confidence in poten- opposite-sex partner (a confederate) who, the participant was subtly in-
tial communal situations because of their positive beliefs about formed (see discussion of the group information sheet, below), was single
themselves and others. In contrast to their more anxious counter- and a recent transfer student. In addition, the participant and the confed-
parts, secure individuals should be less desperate to communicate erate had a 2-min interaction in the hallway prior to entering the testing
interest in closeness. That is, they might not feel compelled to go room, purportedly because the experimenter was running late. This inter-
out of their way to signal interest in a close relationship because of action was designed to encourage a sense of connection between the
their trust that the relationship would develop. Moreover, their participant and the confederate. During the interaction, the confederate
interpersonal confidence should translate into greater tolerance for attempted to engage the participant in a conversation first by asking
whether the participant had ever participated in a psychology experiment
uncertainty, reducing the need to microscopically analyze the
and then continuing with casual conversation (e.g., asking, “Where are you
other’s behavior for signs of interest. In contrast to their more from?”, “What is your major?”, etc.).
avoidant counterparts, secure individuals should not feel threat- After the hallway interaction, the participant and the confederate were
ened by another’s interest in closeness because they are less brought into the lab. In accordance with Clark (1984b), the participant and
concerned with preserving their independence. Thus, we predicted the confederate were told that the study was designed to investigate the
that in contrast to their more insecure counterparts, secure indi- effects of monetary incentives on group performance and attitudes and that
viduals would feel relatively comfortable when a potential close they would be working on two group tasks for which they would be
other expressed interest in closeness. rewarded. They were further told that one participant would be in charge
of dividing the reward from the first task and that the other participant
would be in charge of dividing the reward from the second task. It was
Study 1 emphasized that they were free to divide the reward however they wanted
(in actuality, they never divided the reward). The first group task was then
This study was designed to investigate the association between explained. This task consisted of finding a series of number sequences
attachment and the use of communal and exchange norms with a imbedded in a matrix. Fifty cents would be given for each number se-
potential close other. The procedure for this study was based on quence found. The participant was always in charge of dividing the reward
Clark’s (1984b) paradigm. The participant and the confederate on the first group task, and the confederate was always in charge of
worked on a group task for a shared reward; the dependent variable dividing the reward on the second group task (which never occurred).
was whether the participant chose to work with the same color pen The participant and the confederate were then informed that, to save
as the confederate. Choosing a different color pen was considered time, they would be working separately so that one person could complete
indicative of exchange norms because individual contributions the personality questionnaires while the other began the first group task.
would be clear, whereas choosing the same color pen was consid-
ered indicative of the avoidance of exchange norms because indi- 2
The 11 dating participants were included in the analyses because
vidual contributions would be obscured. It was predicted that participants were sometimes confused about what dating meant (i.e., the
anxious individuals would go out of their way to avoid appearing question did not specify dating one person exclusively). Including the
exchange oriented (i.e., they would use the same color pen as their dating participants did not alter the results. Although we targeted single
partner) to signal interest in closeness, whereas avoidant individ- participants in Studies 2, 3, and 4, those who described themselves as
uals would adopt an exchange orientation (i.e., they would use a dating were included in the analyses.
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 81

The experimenter then escorted the confederate to a separate testing room avoidant-dismissive, preoccupied, and avoidant-fearful, respec-
and returned with an informed-consent form, the personality measures, and tively (one participant did not answer the attachment categoriza-
a group information sheet for the participant to complete. The group tion item). The Pearson chi-square test revealed that pen choice
information sheet, which requested demographic information including was associated with attachment, ␹2(3, N ⫽ 66) ⫽ 13.67, p ⬍ .005
relationship status and length of attendance at the university, was used to
(see Table 1). Moreover, as predicted, the proportion of preoccu-
make salient the potential for a communal relationship with the confederate
pied participants who chose the same color pen (93%) was signif-
(see Clark, 1984b, 1986). Specifically, the group information sheet was
given to the participant after the confederate had completed the top portion, icantly greater than the proportion expected by chance (50%; Z ⫽
indicating that she or he was single and a recently arrived transfer student. 3.22, p ⬍ .001). With respect to the other three groups, pen choice
In this way, the participant was made aware that the confederate was did not differ significantly from chance. The proportion of secure
available for a communal relationship. participants who chose the same color pen (39%) was not signif-
After completing the personality measures, which included the Relation- icantly less than the proportion expected by chance (Z ⫽ 1.17, ns),
ship Questionnaire (RQ; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), a measure of the proportion of dismissive participants who chose the same color
chronic attachment, and the group information sheet, the participant was pen (31%) was not significantly less than the proportion expected
given the first task, already partially completed by the confederate, to by chance (Z ⫽ 1.37, ns), and the proportion of fearful participants
finish. Three red pens and three black pens were placed in a pen-holder on
(46%) who chose the same color pen was not significantly less
the participant’s desk. The participant thus had the choice to work with
than the proportion expected by chance (Z ⬍ 1, ns). Finally, the
either the same color pen as or a different color pen from the confederate
(the confederate alternated working with either a red or black pen). The Pearson chi-square test unexpectedly revealed a marginal effect for
participant was allotted 4 min to work on the task, after which he or she sex, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 67) ⫽ 3.39, p ⫽ .066. Overall, men were slightly
was informed that the study was over, probed for suspicions, debriefed, and more likely to use communal norms (63%) than were women
compensated. (40%).

Measures Continuous Analyses


The RQ (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) consists of four short para- Inspection of the anxious and avoidant composites showed that
graphs describing the secure, preoccupied, avoidant-dismissive, and those who chose the same color pen were more anxious, M(34) ⫽
avoidant-fearful attachment styles. Participants rated the extent to which .31, than those who chose a different color pen, M(33) ⫽ ⫺.32,
they resembled each of the four styles in their close relationships (i.e., t(65) ⫽ 2.67, p ⫽ .01, but there was no difference between the
relationships with parents, siblings, close friends, relatives, or romantic
same and different pen choice groups on the avoidant dimension
partners) based on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4
(completely). Participants also selected the one attachment style that best
(t ⬍ 1.5). Analyses of the individual attachment items similarly
described how they felt in their close relationships. Following Fraley and found that those who chose the same color pen were more preoc-
Shaver (1997), we created composites of anxiety and avoidance by sub- cupied (M ⫽ .41) than those who chose a different color pen (M ⫽
tracting participants’ dismissive ratings from their preoccupied ratings and ⫺.42), t(60.79) ⫽ 3.73, p ⬍ .001, whereas there were no differ-
by subtracting their secure ratings from their fearful ratings. We then ences between the same and different pen choice groups on at-
standardized these composite scores. tachment security, dismissive, or fearful ratings (all ts ⬍ 1.7). It is
interesting to note, however, that when we investigated whether
Results sex qualified any of these findings, results revealed a significant
interaction between sex and pen choice on dismissive ratings, F(1,
To investigate whether attachment was associated with partici-
63) ⫽ 7.44, p ⬍ .01. Specifically, men who chose a different color
pants’ pen choice, we adopted two data-analysis strategies. First,
pen were significantly more dismissive, M(12) ⫽ .84, than men
participants were grouped according to their self-categorized at-
who chose the same color pen, M(20) ⫽ ⫺.10, t(63) ⫽ ⫺2.76, p ⬍
tachment style, and the Pearson chi-square test was conducted to
.05, whereas dismissive scores did not differ for women as a
investigate whether pen choice was associated with chronic attach-
function of pen choice (t ⬍ 1.2).
ment. Following Clark (1984b), we also conducted difference of
proportion tests to determine whether group differences in pen
choice differed significantly from chance. Second, participants Discussion
were grouped according to whether they chose the same color pen In sum, our predictions for the anxiously attached were sup-
as or a different color pen from the confederate, and independent- ported: Preoccupied individuals were more likely to choose the
samples t tests were conducted to investigate group differences on
the continuous attachment scores (i.e., the anxious and avoidant Table 1
composites and the individual attachment items). Finally, although Pen Color Choice as a Function of Attachment (Study 1)
we did not have specific predictions related to sex, we investigated
whether sex was associated with the use of communal and ex- Pen color choice
change norms and whether sex qualified any of the findings, Attachment style Same Different
recognizing that sex was confounded with confederate.
Secure 10 16
Preoccupied 13 1
Categorical Analyses Avoidant-dismissive 4 9
In this study, 39% of participants categorized themselves as Avoidant-fearful 6 7
Total 33 33
secure, and 20%, 21%, and 20% categorized themselves as
82 BARTZ AND LYDON

same color pen as their partner than one would expect by chance, cially susceptible to interdependence dilemma concerns in situa-
and those who chose the same color pen as their partner were more tions involving possible closeness because of their chronic
anxious and more preoccupied. Thus, in accordance with Clark’s interpersonal insecurities. Thus, it was predicted that, compared
(1984b) theorizing, these findings suggest that when interacting with the exchange condition, anxious individuals in the communal
with an attractive, available, opposite-sex partner, anxiously at- condition would experience more interpersonal anxiety because of
tached individuals go out of their way to avoid appearing exchange the potential for closeness in this condition. Avoidant individuals,
oriented, presumably to signal interest in their partner. on the other hand, strive for independence and do not like it when
The findings for the other three groups were less clear-cut. As others try to get too close. Thus, it was predicted that, compared
predicted, pen choice for secure individuals was random; however, with the exchange condition, avoidant individuals in the communal
pen choice for fearful individuals was also random. Finally, al- condition would respond to their partner’s overtures with de-
though dismissive individuals were more likely to choose a dif- creased liking and would try to cast their partner’s communal
ferent color pen than preoccupied individuals—suggesting their behavior in a negative light. Finally, although situations involving
preference for an exchange orientation— contrary to our predic- possible closeness should be distressing to avoidant individuals
tions, their pen choice was not significantly different from chance. given their preference for independence, avoidant individuals tend
Moreover, those who chose a different color pen were not more to suppress worries and concerns. Thus, we did not have specific
dismissive. That said, additional analyses revealed that men who predictions regarding avoidant individuals’ affective response in
chose a different color pen were more dismissive. However, very this study.
few women (n ⫽ 2) categorized themselves as dismissive, making
it difficult to investigate this effect for women. Method
On a related note, unexpectedly, women overall were slightly
less likely than their male counterparts to use the same color pen Participants
as their partner; this may also have made it difficult to detect an
Sixty-three university students volunteered to participate. Single partic-
association between women’s avoidant attachment and the use of ipants were targeted; 2 participants were excluded because they were in a
exchange norms (i.e., if women were less likely to use communal serious relationship, and 2 participants were excluded because they sus-
norms, dismissive women would have had to have been that much pected that their partner was a confederate. There were 59 participants in
less communal). Why were women slightly less likely to use the final sample (26 men and 33 women, 50 single and 8 dating [1
communal norms? If anything, one would expect women to be participant did not answer this question], M age ⫽ 21.05 years). Partici-
more communal. One possible factor may have been the confed- pants were randomly assigned to the communal (n ⫽ 28) or exchange
erate. Male and female participants interacted with different con- conditions (n ⫽ 31). Participants were given either extra credit or $10
federates; although we tried to select comparable confederates, (Canadian) for their participation.
slight differences (e.g., one confederate may have been liked more
than the other) could have influenced participants’ desire for Procedure
closeness and their use of communal norms.
University students were recruited to participate in a study supposedly
investigating personality, group performance, and incentives. As in Study
Study 2 1, the participant had a 2-min interaction with an attractive, opposite-sex
confederate on arrival at the testing session. After the interaction, the
In Study 1, when interacting with an available, desirable partner, participant and the confederate were brought into the lab and were in-
anxious individuals, compared with their more secure and avoidant formed that they would be working on two group tasks for which they
counterparts, went out of their way to express interest in closeness, would be rewarded; when the first group task was completed, one person
but how do anxious individuals respond when an available, desir- would be given the reward to divide between the members of the group,
able partner expresses interest in them? The goal of this study was and when the second group task was completed, the other person would be
to investigate the relationship between attachment and partici- given the opportunity to divide the reward. Different from Study 1, it was
explained that even though participants would be working together on both
pants’ response to the use of communal or exchange norms by a
tasks, because the study was interested in the effects of two different
potential close other. In this study, we created a new psychological working conditions, they would be working in two separate rooms during
situation by modifying the procedures from Study 1. Specifically, the first task, so they would be unable to communicate, and they would be
the confederate worked on the group task after the participant, and working in the same room during the second task. The group task, which
the completed group task was returned to the participant, who was was similar to the number matrix task used in Study 1, was then explained.
in charge of tabulating the results. In this way, the participant was Participants were told that they would be given 25¢ for each number
able to see whether the confederate used the same or different sequence found. Again, the participant was responsible for dividing the
color pen (experimental manipulation of communal vs. exchange reward on the first group task (the second group task never took place).
norms). State feelings of interpersonal anxiety, partner liking, and The participant was then escorted to a room next door, was given an
partner perceptions were assessed. informed-consent form to complete, and then began the first group task.
The experimenter left and returned after 6 minutes with the group infor-
It was theorized that whereas secure individuals would feel
mation sheet (similar to that used in Study 1), partially completed by the
comfortable with a potential close other’s use of communal norms, confederate, and the personality measures, which again included a measure
insecure (anxious and avoidant) individuals would feel more dis- of chronic attachment. The experimenter then brought the group task to the
tressed. Anxious individuals have a strong desire for closeness but confederate, who used either the same color pen as (communal condition)
are uncertain about others’ interest and tend to doubt their self- or a different color pen from (exchange condition) the participant. (Al-
worth. As we have argued, anxious individuals should be espe- though participants were not specifically told that their partner had an
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 83

option with regard to pen choice—primarily because it was difficult to Participants also indicated the extent to which (a) they liked the person
convey this information without drawing attention to the importance of with whom they were working, (b) they would like to continue a conver-
pens in the study— care was taken to display the pens on the participant’s sation with that person on another occasion, (c) their partner was the kind
desk in an artificial manner so that the participant would assume that the of person they would want to have as a friend, and (d) their partner was the
other’s desk space was similarly laid out.) The completed group task was kind of person they would want to work with on another project by placing
then returned to the participant, who was instructed to tabulate how many a slash through a line, anchored from 0 (not at all) to 50 (completely). We
number sequences the team had found. In this way, the participant was able created a partner-liking score by taking participants’ mean response to the
to see whether the confederate used a pen of the same color or one of a four partner-liking items (␣ ⫽ .87).
different color. At this point, the participant was told that before beginning
the second group task in which he or she would be working with the other
participant in the same room, it was necessary to complete a social Results
interaction prestudy questionnaire to control for individual differences in
expectations about working together (Clark & Mills, 1979). The question-
This study was designed to investigate attachment differences in
naire assessed situational feelings of interpersonal anxiety (i.e., state affect participants’ response to the use of communal or exchange norms
and self-esteem), partner perceptions, and partner liking. After completing by a potential close other. The dependent variables were situational
these measures, the participant was probed for suspicions, debriefed, and interpersonal anxiety, partner liking, and partner perceptions. It
compensated. was predicted that attachment anxiety would be more strongly
associated with interpersonal anxiety in the communal condition
Measures compared with the exchange condition, whereas attachment avoid-
ance would be more strongly associated with partner liking (neg-
Experience in Close Relationships scale (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998).
This 36-item questionnaire is designed to assess the two dimensions of ative correlation) and negative partner perceptions in the commu-
attachment avoidance and anxiety. Avoidant items reflect comfort with nal condition compared with the exchange condition. To
closeness and dependency, and anxious items reflect anxiety about being investigate these predictions, we used the Fisher z-transformation
abandoned. Participants indicate on a 7-point scale how much they agree or test (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003) to compare the within-
disagree with each item, in terms of how they experience romantic rela- cell correlations between attachment and interpersonal anxiety,
tionships. We added the ECR to this study because of the limitations partner perceptions, and partner liking between experimental
associated with the RQ (i.e., assessment of discrete dimensions and cate- conditions.
gories). After reverse scoring items for which lower numbers reflected
greater attachment avoidance or anxiety, we computed attachment avoid-
ance and anxiety scores for each participant by taking the mean response Attachment Anxiety and Interpersonal Anxiety
on the 18 avoidance (␣ ⫽ .90) and 18 anxiety (␣ ⫽ .90) items. Avoidance
and anxiety scores were not correlated, r(57) ⫽ .19, ns. Overall, attachment anxiety was associated with situational feel-
Situational feelings of interpersonal anxiety. To assess situational feel- ings of interpersonal anxiety across experimental conditions,
ings of interpersonal anxiety, we created a composite that included state r(57) ⫽ .58, p ⬍ .001. However, as predicted, the association
affect and aspects of state self-esteem. Specifically, the composite included between attachment anxiety and interpersonal anxiety was signif-
the Anxious and Uncertain subscales from the Profile of Mood States icantly greater in the communal condition, r(25) ⫽ .80, than in the
(POMS; McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971). (These items were inter- exchange condition, r(28) ⫽ .37, Z ⫽ 2.59, p ⫽ .01.4 Moreover,
spersed among items from the Depressed, Hostile, Happy, and Confident
although avoidant attachment was also associated with interper-
subscales of the POMS.) The composite also included the Appearance
sonal anxiety, r(57) ⫽ .37, p ⬍ .005, this association did not differ
subscale from the State Self-Esteem Scale (SSES; Heatherton & Polivy,
1991). Examples from this subscale include “I feel that others respect and between experimental conditions, Z ⫽ 1.12, ns. It is important to
admire me,” “I feel good about myself,” “I am pleased with my appearance note that the difference in the association between attachment
right now,” and “I feel unattractive” (this last items was reverse scored).3 anxiety and interpersonal anxiety in the communal and exchange
The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the Anxious, Uncertain, and Appear-
ance Self-Esteem subscales (reverse scored) was .73. To calculate the
3
index, we summed participants’ ratings from the Anxious and Uncertain Although the SSES taps performance, social competence, and appear-
subscales and subtracted their Appearance Self-Esteem rating (standard- ance, it is thought that different components of state self-esteem should be
ized scores were used in the calculation to adjust for scale differences in affected by different experimental situations (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991).
these measures). We focused on the appearance component because it was theorized that
Partner perceptions and partner liking. Partner perceptions and part- appearance concerns should be especially salient in the communal condi-
ner liking were assessed in the Social Interaction Prestudy Questionnaire. tion. That is, in both the communal and exchange conditions, participants
In this questionnaire, participants rated how well certain traits applied to expected to work on a task with their partner, and thus, performance and
their partner (the confederate). Because we were particularly interested in social interaction concerns should be salient. However, appearance con-
how avoidant participants would construe their partner’s communal behav- cerns (e.g., “I feel unattractive”) should be more salient in the communal
ior, we selected traits from the Extended Personality Attributes Question- condition than in the exchange condition given the potential for closeness
naire (Spence, Helmreich, & Holahan, 1979), which taps positive com- and increased likelihood of being evaluated by another person on the basis
munion (warm, helpful, and understanding), negative communion of one’s appearance in the communal condition.
(spineless, fussy, and servile), positive agency (independent, competitive, 4
This effect was moderated somewhat by sex ( p ⬍ .10). Anxiously
and self-confident), and negative agency (arrogant, greedy, and hostile). attached women experienced more interpersonal anxiety than their secure
Participants indicated their response by placing a slash through a line, counterparts across conditions, r(31) ⫽ .53, p ⫽ .001, whereas for anx-
anchored from 0 (not at all) to 50 (completely). Cronbach’s alpha coeffi- iously attached men, this relationship was weak in the exchange condition,
cients for communal-positive, communal-negative, agency-positive, and r(11) ⫽ .35, ns, but very strong in the communal condition, r(11) ⫽ .92,
agency-negative composites were .78, .58, .36, and .66, respectively. p ⬍ .001.
84 BARTZ AND LYDON

conditions remained when controlling for attachment avoidance. acting with a stranger is exchange, so anxious individuals may
That is, a comparison of the partial correlations between attach- have thought their partner’s behavior in the exchange conditions
ment anxiety and interpersonal anxiety (controlling for attachment (i.e., choosing to work with a different color pen) was consistent
avoidance) in the communal, r(24) ⫽ .77, and exchange condi- with the situation and thus did not take it as a personal slight. It
tions, r(27) ⫽ .39, remained significant (Z ⫽ 2.17, p ⬍ .05). was when the confederate deviated from the norm, expressing
interest in closeness, that they felt especially anxious, uncertain,
Attachment Avoidance and Partner Liking and Partner and concerned about their appearance. Thus, it appears that when
opportunities arise for anxiously attached individuals to pursue
Perceptions
their goals for closeness and acceptance, they experience more
Partner liking. Overall, avoidant attachment was negatively interpersonal anxiety.
associated with partner liking, r(57) ⫽ ⫺.28, p ⬍ .05; however, as Avoidant individuals had a very different response to the po-
predicted, this association was stronger in the communal condi- tential close other’s use of communal norms. Attachment avoid-
tion, r(25) ⫽ ⫺.54, than in the exchange condition, r(28) ⫽ ⫺.02, ance was associated with decreased partner liking in the communal
Z ⫽ 2.12, p ⬍ .05. By contrast, attachment anxiety was not condition, whereas there was no association between attachment
associated with partner liking, r(57) ⫽ ⫺.03, ns. Moreover, at- avoidance and partner liking in the exchange condition. Thus, it
tachment anxiety did not qualify the differential association be- was not simply that avoidant individuals disliked their partner;
tween avoidant attachment and partner liking in the communal and rather, they disliked their partner when their partner attempted
exchange conditions. That is, a comparison of the partial correla- closeness. Avoidant individuals value independence and self-
tions between attachment avoidance and partner liking (controlling sufficiency and prefer to keep a distance between themselves and
for attachment anxiety) in the communal, r(24) ⫽ ⫺.56, and close others; they also tend to avoid confrontation with worries or
exchange conditions, r(27) ⫽ ⫺.02, remained significant (Z ⫽ concerns. We believe their partner’s communal overtures likely
2.18, p ⬍ .05). Finally, although this effect was not qualified by threatened avoidant individuals’ need for self-reliance, and they
sex, men in general liked the female confederate more than women responded by disparaging their partner.
liked the male confederate, t(57) ⫽ 2.28, p ⬍ .05. (This finding Rather than response to possible closeness, an alternate expla-
supports our speculation in Study 1 that any sex differences may nation of the findings from this study is that the confederate’s
have been due to differences in male and female participants’ obscuring of contributions in the communal condition made it
liking of the confederates rather then sex per se.) difficult for participants to divide the reward because each per-
Partner perceptions. Overall, avoidant attachment was asso- son’s contribution to the task was ambiguous. We believe it is
ciated with ascribing more negative communal traits (e.g., servile) unlikely that this would explain anxious individuals’ heightened
to the partner, r(56) ⫽ .31, p ⬍ .05 (one participant did not interpersonal anxiety. Referring back to Study 1, the large majority
respond to this question); however, as predicted, the association of anxious individuals used the same color pen as their partner, and
between avoidant attachment and perceptions of the partner’s given that they were also responsible for dividing the reward in
negative communal traits was stronger in the communal condition, Study 1, they would have been presented with the same quandary.
r(25) ⫽ .56, than in the exchange condition, r(29) ⫽ .09, Z ⫽ 1.95, Avoidant individuals, on the other hand, may indeed have been
p ⫽ .051. Unexpectedly, attachment anxiety was also associated perturbed when their partner used the same color pen because it
with negative communal partner perceptions, r(56) ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .05. made it difficult for them to divide the reward. However, given
Although this association did not differ between experimental that distributing benefits on the basis of task contribution is an
conditions (Z ⫽ .70, ns), controlling for attachment anxiety qual- exchange norm, this would seem to support the notion that
ified the aforementioned association between attachment avoid- avoidant individuals prefer to operate from an exchange rather
ance and perceptions of the partner’s negative communal traits than a communal perspective.
(Z ⫽ 1.58, ns). No sex differences were found in the association These findings also shed light on Study 1. In Study 1, anxiously
between attachment avoidance and perceptions of the partner’s attached individuals chose to work with the same color pen as their
negative communal traits. Finally, with respect to agency traits, partner more often than chance. In line with Clark’s (1984b)
avoidant attachment was associated with seeing the partner as theorizing, we interpret this behavior as an effort to signal interest
more negatively agentic (e.g., greedy), r(57) ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .05, and in closeness by avoiding the use of exchange norms. An alternate
less positively agentic (e.g., less independent), r(57) ⫽ ⫺.31, p ⬍ explanation, however, is that anxiously attached individuals ob-
.05, but these associations did not differ between experimental scured contributions because they were concerned they would be
conditions. evaluated negatively if they performed poorly. Study 2 suggests
that this was not the case: Anxiously attached individuals had less
Discussion interpersonal anxiety when their performance was distinguishable
from their partner’s performance (i.e., in the exchange condition).
As predicted, attachment anxiety was strongly associated with In sum, Studies 1 and 2 suggest that situations involving the
interpersonal anxiety when a potential close other used communal opportunity for closeness elicit an approach–avoidance conflict in
norms. Why were anxiously attached individuals not more dis- anxiously attached individuals. In Study 1, anxiously attached
tressed when their partner used exchange norms, as this might individuals were more likely that their secure and avoidant coun-
suggest their partner’s disinterest in a communal relationship? We terparts to symbolically approach their partner, whereas in Study 2,
believe it is unlikely that the anxious participants interpreted their attachment anxiety was associated with greater feelings of inter-
partner’s exchange behavior as a rejection. The norm when inter- personal anxiety following a potential close other’s communal
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 85

overtures. We believe the confederate’s communal behavior likely friend and not as a romantic prospect. Thus, in this study, partic-
aroused anxious individuals’ interdependence dilemma concerns ipants were instructed to nominate someone with whom they
(“Is this person interested in me?”) and their deep-seated ambiv- hoped to establish a deeper relationship (a potential close other);
alence about close others. On the one hand, given their desire for the nature of that relationship (friendship vs. romantic) was left
closeness, anxiously attached individuals were probably highly open, which allowed us to compare romantic versus nonromantic
motivated to detect signs of interest in the other’s behavior, but on potentially communal relationships. Participants then visualized
the other hand, given their chronic uncertainty about others, their themselves in one of two scenarios. In one scenario, participants
hopes may have been tempered by concerns that they would be needed to borrow notes for a class they have missed, and the
found unworthy (Holmes, 1991). Avoidant individuals, by con- potential close other offered to loan them the notes; in the second
trast, disparaged their partner when their partner used communal scenario, the potential close other suggested being study partners
norms presumably because their partner’s communal overtures and exchanging phone numbers. The offer to lend someone notes
threatened their desire for independence. Studies 3 and 4 were is a relatively trivial, mundane event that can be attributed to a
designed to explore these ideas. variety of nonrelationship factors, whereas the suggestion to ex-
change phone numbers is less common and the motives for the
Study 3 behavior, we believe, are more suggestive of relationship potential.
Affect, significance of the event for the future of the relationship,
A core prediction of the interdependence dilemma is that in and relational attributions for the other’s communal behavior were
potential communal situations, there is the desire for closeness but assessed.
uncertainty about the other’s motives and this uncertainty needs to It was hypothesized that overall, participants would believe the
be resolved for the relationship to move forward. Consequently, as event had greater significance for the future of the relationship and
Holmes (1991) noted, people seek confirmation about the other’s would make more relational attributions when the potential close
motives, often using patterns of social exchange as a testing other suggested being study partners than when the potential close
ground—that is, they look for evidence that the other genuinely other offered to lend them notes. Similarly, with respect to affect,
cares as indicated by, for example, the other’s use of communal it was hypothesized that the study partner scenario would elicit less
norms (“Is the other responsive to my needs?”). As we have indifference and greater unease compared with the notes scenario
argued, because of their strong desire for closeness and their because the prospect of closeness would arouse interdependence
chronic uncertainty, anxiously attached individuals should be most dilemma concerns.
susceptible to interdependence dilemma concerns. Indeed, Study 2 Attachment, however, was hypothesized to moderate these ef-
found attachment anxiety to be more strongly associated with fects. Secure individuals (low anxious and low avoidant) were
feelings of interpersonal anxiety in situations involving the possi- predicted to be the baseline group—that is, they should infer
bility of closeness. If anxious individuals are more susceptible to greater significance and make more relational attributions in the
interdependence dilemma concerns, then they should be especially less ambiguous study partner scenario than in the more trivial
likely to engage in this process of uncertainty reduction, evaluating notes scenario. They should also experience less indifference and
even relatively trivial behaviors for their diagnostic information greater unease in the study partner scenario, in line with interde-
about relationship potential. Indeed, the notion that anxiously pendence dilemma predictions. Anxious individuals, by compari-
attached individuals are especially alert to environmental cues son, should infer greater significance, make more relational attri-
conveying information about acceptance and rejection is also a butions for the potential close other’s behavior, and feel more
central prediction of attachment theory (Fraley & Shaver, 2000). uneasy regardless of scenario; that is, even the discrete communal
The goal of Study 3 was to investigate the idea that potential behavior scenario should set in motion interdependence dilemma
communal situations arouse anxiety and uncertainty in anxiously concerns. Finally, avoidant individuals should be less likely to
attached individuals, which then sets in motion a need to micro- infer significance and should make fewer relational attributions for
scopically analyze and diagnose the other’s behavior— even trivial the potential close other’s communal behavior because of their
behaviors—for signs of interest and commitment. Moreover, we tendency to downplay the importance of closeness; for the same
wanted to probe whether anxious individuals’ hypervigilance and reason, they should also be more indifferent to the potential close
need to reduce uncertainty were associated with assigning greater other’s communal behavior. Lastly, it was predicted that avoidant
relational significance to more trivial situations that are less clearly individuals would be less happy in the notes scenario, in which
communal or whether this process would be more apparent in they must rely on the other for help, because of their aversion to
situations clearly suggestive of communal potential. Although dependency.
everyone should respond with interdependence dilemma concerns
when the behavior of the other is clearly suggestive of communal Method
potential, we believe that even trivial behaviors should elicit in-
terdependence dilemma concerns for the anxiously attached. Participants
In this study, we departed from the laboratory methodology and
Two hundred seventy-nine participants were recruited on a volunteer
instead used a guided-visualization, self-report methodology to basis to complete one of two relationship surveys. Single participants were
assess preexisting potential communal relationships from partici- targeted to ensure participants were adequately interested in pursuing a
pants’ own social world. Moreover, although we sought to create relationship with their chosen person. We excluded 2 participants who
the potential for a romantic relationship in Studies 1 and 2, some were unable to nominate a potential close other, 23 participants who were
participants may have thought of the confederate as a potential insufficiently interested in developing a relationship with the potential
86 BARTZ AND LYDON

close other (i.e., their interest ratings were below the midpoint; these since the last midterm and __________ has the notes for the classes you
participants did not differ from the other participants in their attachment missed.” After rating how comfortable they would feel in that situation,
avoidance or anxiety scores), 1 participant who did not complete the participants were asked to imagine that their chosen person had offered to
attachment questionnaire, and 1 participant who was married, had known lend them the notes.
the potential close other for 30 years, and was more than 10 standard Study partner (less ambiguous gesture of friendship). “Imagine that
deviations above the mean participant age. Thus, 252 participants com- you and __________ are taking a class together. One day after class
pleted one of two relationship surveys (126 men and 125 women, 219 __________ gives you his or her phone number and suggests being study
single and 32 dating, M age ⫽ 20.94 years). (There were missing data on partners for the upcoming exam.”
some of the questions because of nonresponse; however, there were never After reading the scenario, participants answered questions about their
more than 5 [2%] missing data points for the relational indices, which were affective response, the significance of the event for the future of the
the main dependent variables). relationship, and attributions for the potential close other’s behavior (see
Measures, below). At the end of the questionnaire, participants indicated
(a) whether they were able to think of someone with whom they desired a
Procedure deeper friendship, (b) how long they had known the person, (c) how
interested they were in developing a relationship with the person, (d) what
Prospective participants were asked if they would be interested in type (romantic or friendship) of relationship they desired, and (e) how
completing a survey about new relationship development. Participants realistic the scenario was to them. We also assessed chronic attachment
were told that it was important to complete the survey in one sitting, alone, (the measure was presented as a relationship style questionnaire) and
and in a quiet place. Agreeing participants completed an informed-consent demographic information at this point.
form, were given the survey and an envelope, and were instructed to return
the survey in the sealed envelope to ensure anonymity. Surveyors were
unaware of the hypotheses when administering the survey, and participants Measures
were debriefed on returning the survey.
The survey began with a brief introduction, which stated that the goal of Significance of the event for the future of the relationship. Participants
the research was to learn more about the development of new relationships. rated, on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (a lot), the extent to which they
Participants were informed that the survey involved nominating someone thought the event would say something about the progress of the relation-
with whom they were not currently friends but with whom they could ship and how much closer they thought they would feel to their chosen
person after the event compared with prior to the event. Using the same
imagine being close friends, visualizing themselves and this person in a
scale, participants also rated the extent to which they agreed with the
scenario, and answering some questions about the scenario as well as some
following statements: “We will probably get closer,” “This event cements
demographic questions and some personality measures. Participants were
our relationship,” and “This event has no particular implications or signif-
encouraged to fully immerse themselves in the scenario (e.g., by visualiz-
icance for the relationship” (this item was reverse scored). An index of the
ing themselves and their chosen person in the situation, imagining the
significance of the event for the development of the relationship was
surroundings, etc.). Moreover, to help participants mentally simulate the
created by averaging participants’ responses to these five items (␣ ⫽ .74).
experience, blank spaces were inserted throughout the scenario, and par-
Relational attributions for the potential close other’s behavior. Partic-
ticipants were instructed to write their chosen person’s first name in the
ipants rated, on a scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly), the
spaces provided.
extent to which they agreed with seven statements reflecting different
After the introduction, participants were asked to think of a same-sex or
attributions for their chosen person’s behavior. A relational attribution
opposite-sex acquaintance. Specifically, they were instructed to think of
composite was created by taking the mean of participants’ relational
attributions (i.e., “because he or she likes me [i.e., this event occurred
Someone with whom you are not currently friends, but someone with
because of the way this person feels about me],” “because he or she cares
whom you could imagine being close friends if you got to know each
about me,” “to become closer,” and “as a gesture of friendship”) and
other better. This person could be a casual friend with whom you
subtracting their situational attribution (i.e., “I don’t think this event had
would like to establish a deeper friendship, or, possibly, a romantic
much to do with who the other person is, or the way he or she feels about
relationship. . . . Although you are not close friends right now, you
me; outside circumstances were probably the primary cause of this
think you might really enjoy spending time together in the future. It
event”).5
remains to be seen how your relationship will develop.
Affect. Participants rated how indifferent, uneasy, and happy they
would feel in the situation described in the scenario, using a scale from 1
Because of the nature of the scenarios (borrowing class notes and exchang- (not at all) to 7 (extremely).
ing phone numbers to be study partners), participants were directed to ECR (Brennan et al., 1998). The 36-item questionnaire used in Study
select a peer, that is, someone relatively close in age, so that the scenario 2 was used to assess attachment avoidance (␣ ⫽ .91) and anxiety (␣ ⫽ .91).
would be appropriate and realistic.
After selecting their chosen person but before going on to read the social
interaction scenario, participants were instructed to visualize their chosen 5
Participants also responded to a dispositional attribution statement
person. To aid in the visualization process, participants were presented (“because he or she is a nice person”) and a self-interested attribution
with a series of questions about their chosen person (e.g., “What is it like statement (“out of self-interest [i.e., to improve his or her standing in the
being with this person?” and “How do you feel when you are with this class]”). However, these items were not included in the relational attribu-
person?”). The purpose of the guided visualization was to help participants tion composite because we believe they likely tapped participants’ chronic
immerse themselves so as to discourage top-of-the-head responses (Lydon beliefs about others and not simply beliefs about whether the behavior had
et al., 1997). After the visualization, participants read one of the following meaning for relationship development. Indeed, there was no correlation
two scenarios. between making relational attributions and making dispositional or
Notes (relatively trivial communal behavior). “Imagine that you and self-interested attributions, r(245) ⫽ ⫺.01, ns, and r(245) ⫽ .04, ns,
__________ are taking a class together. You have missed a few classes respectively.
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 87

Results pants inferred greater significance from the suggestion to be study


partners than from the notes offer, providing construct validation
A series of hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to for the two scenarios—that is, as intended, participants viewed the
investigate the influence of attachment anxiety and avoidance on study partner scenario, which involved a less ambiguous instance
participants’ affective response to the potential close other’s com- of the potential close other’s communal behavior, as having a
munal behavior, their beliefs about the significance of that behav- greater impact on relationship development than the notes sce-
ior for relationship development, and the kind of attributions they nario. As predicted, however, analyses also revealed a significant
made about the potential close other’s communal behavior. At- effect for attachment anxiety (B ⫽ .17, sr ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .005).
tachment anxiety and avoidance were standardized and entered in Anxiously attached individuals inferred greater significance from
the first step of the regression along with scenario (notes vs. study the potential close other’s communal behavior regardless of sce-
partner; contrast coded as 1 and ⫺1, respectively); the two-way nario. In addition, there was a marginal interaction between sce-
interactions of attachment anxiety and avoidance, scenario and nario and attachment anxiety (B ⫽ .10, sr ⫽ .09, p ⫽ .095). Simple
attachment anxiety, and scenario and attachment avoidance were effects tests found that attachment anxiety was positively associ-
entered in the second step; and the three-way interaction of sce- ated with beliefs about the significance of the event in the notes
nario, attachment anxiety, and attachment avoidance was entered scenario, r(127) ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .005, but was not associated with
in the third step of the regression. significance beliefs in the study partner scenario, r(121) ⫽ .11, ns,
We also investigated whether the type of relationship partici- primarily because everyone had more optimistic beliefs for the
pants thought about (friendship or romantic) or participants’ sex development of the relationship in the study partner scenario.
influenced any of the dependent variables and whether relationship Results also revealed an unexpected three-way interaction between
type or participants’ sex qualified any of the effects of attachment scenario, attachment anxiety, and attachment avoidance (B ⫽ .12,
and/or scenario. There were no significant main or interaction sr ⫽ .11, p ⬍ .05). This interaction is explained by the fact that for
effects of relationship type on any of the dependent variables. the notes scenario, high avoidant, low anxious (dismissive) partic-
Similarly, with one exception, there were no main or interaction ipants were considerably less likely than their peers to believe that
effects of sex on any of the dependent variables. Specifically, the event had significance for relationship development.
attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety were negatively
correlated with feeling uneasy for male participants, whereas
Relational Attributions for the Potential Close Other’s
women’s uneasiness ratings were not correlated with avoidance or
Behavior
anxiety. Basically, highly avoidant or anxious men were less likely
to report feeling uneasy compared with their less avoidant and less Analyses investigating the relational attribution composite also
anxious counterparts. revealed an effect for scenario (B ⫽ ⫺.29, sr ⫽ ⫺.29, p ⬍ .001),
again providing construct validation and suggesting that partici-
Relationship Characteristics pants perceived their potential close other’s suggestion to be study
partners as more relationally motivated than the offer to lend them
One hundred two participants thought of a potential friend, and notes. There was also an interaction between scenario and attach-
149 participants thought of a potential romantic partner (if partic- ment anxiety (B ⫽ .16, sr ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .01; see Figure 1). Simple
ipants selected both, they were categorized as seeking a romantic effects tests revealed that attachment anxiety was positively asso-
relationship because it was assumed that if participants indicated ciated with relational attributions in the notes scenario, r(125) ⫽
any interest in a romantic relationship, then that was their true .24, p ⬍ .01, but was not associated with relational attributions in
preference). The mean interest in developing a relationship was
5.27 (range ⫽ 4.00 –7.00). The mean relationship length was
slightly less than a year and a half (497 days), and the median was
approximately 6 and a half months (196 days; range ⫽ 1 day–11
years). The mean scenario realism score was 4.82 (range ⫽ 1.00 –
7.00). Attachment was not associated with the type of relationship
participants were interested in developing (both ts ⬍ 1), nor was
attachment associated with interest in developing a relationship,
perceived scenario realism, or relationship length (all rs ⬍ .11).
Relationship type, however, was associated with interest in devel-
oping a relationship, t(230.167) ⫽ 6.37, p ⬍ .001. Those seeking
a romantic relationship were significantly more interested in de-
veloping a relationship with their chosen person (M ⫽ 5.56) than
were those seeking a friendship (M ⫽ 4.83).

Significance of Event for Relationship Development


Analyses investigating the composite measure of beliefs about
the significance of the event for relationship development revealed Figure 1. Relationship attributions (for potential close other’s behavior)
an effect for scenario (B ⫽ ⫺.43, sr ⫽ ⫺.42, p ⬍ .001). Partici- as a function of scenario and attachment anxiety (Study 3).
88 BARTZ AND LYDON

the study partner scenario, r(118) ⫽ ⫺.10, ns, again, primarily to make positive relational attributions for the other’s offer to lend
because everyone made more relational attributions in the study them notes but that they were especially unwilling to discount the
partner scenario. Thus, similar to the significance of the event for behavior as purely situational. That is, they appear to have been
relationship development analyses, anxiously attached individuals clinging to the notion that there must be some meaning in the
made more relational attributions regardless of the quality of the gesture but at the same time were hesitant to conclude that the
other’s communal behavior. Finally, as predicted, analyses also other was definitely interested in a relationship. These findings are
revealed a negative association between attachment avoidance and consistent with the ambivalence that characterizes attachment anx-
making relational attributions (B ⫽ ⫺.22, sr ⫽ ⫺.22, p ⬍ .001), iety and point to a possible source for the uncertainty anxious
suggesting avoidant individuals’ reluctance to make relational individuals evidenced in Study 2 in response to possible closeness.
attributions for the other’s communal behavior. These findings are also consistent with Lydon et al.’s (1997)
research, which found that when participants were faced with an
Affective Response to Potential Close Other’s Communal interdependence dilemma, they were especially likely to believe
Behavior that communal gestures had some implication for relationship
development.
Overall, participants felt less indifferent (B ⫽ .20, sr ⫽ .20, p ⬍ With respect to affect, overall, participants felt less indifferent in
.005) and more uneasy (B ⫽ ⫺.15, sr ⫽ ⫺.15, p ⬍ .05) in the the study partner scenario suggesting that they perceived the offer
study partner scenario than in the notes scenario. Again, this to be study partners as having greater significance for the relation-
finding suggests that the study partner scenario had more emo- ship. It is interesting to note that participants also felt more uneasy
tional relevance for participants than did the notes scenario. The when the potential close other suggested being study partners. This
fact that participants were more uneasy in the study partner sce- finding is in agreement with Lydon et al. (1997), and we believe it
nario also reinforces the notion that interdependence dilemma is indicative of the feelings of uncertainty and anxiety people
situations are associated with increased anxiety. As predicted, experience in association with the interdependence dilemma.
attachment anxiety was associated with feeling uneasy across Avoidant individuals, characteristically, reported feeling more in-
scenarios (B ⫽ .13, sr ⫽ .12, p ⬍ .05). Finally, avoidant attach- different to the potential close other’s communal behavior across
ment was associated with feeling more indifferent across scenarios scenarios; they also felt more uneasy about the potential close
(B ⫽ .13, sr ⫽ .13, p ⬍ .05) and, interestingly, was also associated
other’s communal behavior across scenarios. This indifference is
with feeling more uneasy across scenarios (B ⫽ .22, sr ⫽ .22, p ⫽
consistent with avoidant individuals’ tendency to downplay the
.001). Moreover, results revealed a significant Scenario ⫻ Avoid-
importance of closeness, but their unease suggests that avoidant
ance interaction on happiness ratings (B ⫽ ⫺.14, sr ⫽ ⫺.14, p ⬍
individuals are nonetheless uncomfortable with situations promot-
.05). As predicted, avoidant attachment was negatively associated
ing closeness. Finally, avoidance was negatively associated with
with happiness ratings in the notes scenario, r(125) ⫽ ⫺.21, p ⬍
happiness in the notes scenario. This decreased happiness in re-
.05, in which participants received help from the other, but was not
sponse to a potential close other’s helping behavior is consistent
associated with happiness ratings in the study partner scenario,
with the avoidant preference for self-reliance.
r(121) ⫽ .04, ns.
How do these findings illuminate Study 2? The anxiously at-
tached assigned greater meaning and significance to possible com-
Discussion munal overtures. They interpreted even a relatively trivial com-
As predicted, overall, participants felt the suggestion to be study munal behavior as a sign of potential closeness. This is consistent
partners and exchange phone numbers was more significant for with Holmes’s (1991) predictions about chronic uncertainty in the
relationship development than the potential close other’s offer to context of the interdependence dilemma. Although everyone is
lend class notes; however, attachment moderated these effects. thought to be susceptible to interdependence dilemma concerns—
Anxious individuals, to a greater extent than their more secure indeed, secure individuals also read meaning into the potential
counterparts, believed the event had significance for relationship close other’s behavior in this study—it appears that anxiously
development and were more likely to make relational attributions attached individuals, who are chronically uncertain, were more
for the potential close other’s communal behavior regardless of susceptible to interdependence concerns as evidenced by their
scenario. Even in the notes scenario, which involved a relatively tendency to hopefully latch onto even relatively trivial communal
mundane, trivial communal behavior, anxious individuals were gestures. Unfortunately, as noted earlier, this microanalytic pro-
more likely to detect evidence of caring and to use that behavior to cess may actually fuel the interpersonal anxiety anxious individ-
diagnose relationship potential. Finally, avoidant individuals were uals are trying to regulate. The findings from Study 3 also shed
less likely to make relationship attributions for the potential close light on the avoidant individuals in Study 2. The findings from
other’s behavior, reflecting their desire to minimize the importance Study 3 suggest that avoidant individuals have a strong desire to
of closeness and their more pessimistic expectations about others. downplay the importance of closeness and that this may have
Interestingly enough, when we decomposed the relational attri- contributed to their negative partner evaluations in Study 2. Fi-
bution composite and investigated relational and situational attri- nally, it is important to note that—at least in Study 3—avoidant
butions separately, although the pattern of results was the same, individuals were not significantly less interested in developing a
the Scenario ⫻ Attachment Anxiety interaction only attained sta- relationship with their chosen person than were their more secure
tistical significance for the situational attribution. This suggests or anxious counterparts, thus reducing the likelihood that the
that anxious individuals were somewhat more willing than others avoidants’ response to the potential close other’s communal be-
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 89

havior was simply due to insufficient interest in developing a were given the option to work as a team, and the confederate
relationship. reached out to the participant and suggested working together for
the Trivial Pursuit portion of the study. In the baseline perfor-
Study 4 mance-anxiety condition, participants were not given the option to
work as a team but were simply informed that they would have to
In Study 1, we found that when interacting with a potential close answer the Trivial Pursuit questions individually (hence, perfor-
other, anxious individuals went out of their way to signal interest mance anxiety). We hypothesized that anxiously attached individ-
in closeness. In Study 2, we found that ironically, when a potential uals would respond to the prospect of closeness in the communal
close other signaled interest in closeness, attachment anxiety was condition with increased rumination and, consequently, poorer
associated with more interpersonal anxiety than when the potential performance on the mental concentration task. By contrast, we
close other acted more neutrally. The findings from Study 3 predicted that secure individuals would not be distressed about the
suggest that situations involving possible closeness elicit a great prospect of closeness in the communal condition—in fact, we
deal of uncertainty and that anxiously attached individuals may go thought that they might benefit from contact with a supportive
into a mode of uncertainty reduction, looking for evidence of the other, resulting in enhanced performance. Finally, although we
other’s interest and caring, even in more trivial communal behav- thought that avoidant individuals would likely be distressed about
iors. Taken together, Studies 1, 2, and 3 suggest that in potential the prospect of closeness in the communal condition, we did not
relationship development situations, anxiously attached individu- predict impaired performance on the mental concentration task
als are especially susceptible to interdependence dilemma con- because their typical coping response is to distance themselves
cerns. What, however, are the implications of these interdepen- from unpleasant thoughts.
dence dilemma concerns? A central prediction of attachment In sum, all participants were expected to exhibit performance
theory is the notion of a secure base. When attachment needs are deficits on the mental concentration task in the performance-
met, people are free to engage in non-attachment-related activities anxiety condition, but secure individuals’ performance was ex-
(e.g., exploration); however, when the attachment system is chron- pected to be facilitated in the communal condition, whereas anx-
ically engaged in the pursuit of attachment goals (as is often the iously attached individuals’ performance was expected to be
case with those who feel anxiously attached), fewer resources are impaired in the communal condition. Moreover, it was predicted
available for non-attachment-related activities (Mikulincer & that secure and anxious individuals’ performance on the mental
Shaver, 2003). The goal of Study 4 was to investigate the conse- concentration task in the communal condition would be differen-
quences of regulating concerns about closeness for anxious indi- tially associated with proximity accessibility, with proximity ac-
viduals’ personal functioning. cessibility being associated with better performance for the secure
In addition, although Study 3 investigated the kind of inferences participants but worse performance for the anxious participants.
and attributions people make about a potential close other’s com- By comparison, avoidant individuals’ performance was not pre-
munal behavior, it was limited because of the self-report method- dicted to be affected by the communal condition because it was
ology used and because participants did not interact with a real thought they would likely keep in check or suppress concerns
person. Thus, in Study 4, we again explored how participants about closeness.
construed another’s communal behavior; however, rather than
asking participants explicitly about their perceptions, we decided Method
to use an implicit measure. Not only do implicit measures avoid
drawing attention to specific research questions but also they Participants
circumvent participants’ self-presentation motives. Thus, in this
Sixty-nine male university students volunteered to participate in the
study, we used a lexical decision task to assess the activation of
study (M age ⫽ 19.7 years). Again, single participants were targeted (54
attachment themes related to proximity– closeness and distance–
single and 15 dating). Participants were randomly assigned to either the
rejection. Because the experimental manipulation concerned the communal (n ⫽ 32) or the performance-anxiety condition (n ⫽ 37) and
possibility of closeness, it was hypothesized that closeness themes received $10 (Canadian) for their participation.
would primarily be activated; however, we also wanted to explore
the possible activation of rejection concerns. A final goal of this
Procedure
study was to provide further evidence— using a different opera-
tionalization of the confederate’s use of communal norms—that As in the previous studies, the participant had a 2-min interaction with
situations affording the possibility of closeness, compared with an attractive, opposite-sex confederate, who was also supposedly a partic-
situations in which closeness is less likely, are distressing to ipant. After the interaction period, the participant and confederate were
anxiously attached individuals. brought into the testing room. The experimenter explained that the study
To meet these goals, we returned to the laboratory methodology was investigating cognitive abilities and distributive reasoning and that
and had participants again interact with an opposite-sex confeder- first they would be working on some cognitive tasks individually to assess
mental concentration and then would be working on a Trivial Pursuit task.
ate in a study supposedly investigating cognitive abilities and
After being given a brief description of the Trivial Pursuit task (see
distributive reasoning. Participants were informed that they would Experimental Manipulation, below), the participant was escorted to a
be performing some cognitive tasks (which included a task of separate room and was left alone to complete an informed-consent form
mental concentration and a lexical decision task) and then answer- and a thought-listing task (unrelated to this study).
ing some general knowledge (Trivial Pursuit; Hasbro, Inc., Paw- The experimenter returned after a few minutes with some personality
tucket, RI) questions. In the communal condition, participants measures (including the ECR) and the group information sheet for the
90 BARTZ AND LYDON

participant to complete. Once the participant had finished these items, the The d2 mental concentration test (Brickenkamp, 1981). This test con-
experimenter explained the d2 test (Brickenkamp, 1981), which was pre- sists of a matrix of 14 rows of random sequences of the letters d and p with
sented as a test of mental concentration. This test consists of a matrix of one, two, or no apostrophes above and/or below each letter. Participants
rows containing random sequences of the letters d and p with one, two, or were instructed to cross out all the ds with two apostrophes and were given
no apostrophes above and/or below each letter; the objective is to go 15 s to go through each row (the experimenter timed participants with a
through each row and cross out all the ds with two apostrophes. The stopwatch). Participants were instructed to work as quickly and accurately
experimenter timed the participant as he worked on the d2 test. After as possible. The d2 test is theorized to be an index of resistance against
completing a short evaluation form (see d2 evaluation form, below), the interference and has been used to assess rumination (Kuhl, 1981). Perfor-
participant began the lexical decision task, which was also presented as a mance on the d2 test was calculated by summing the total number of hits
test of mental concentration. Under the pretext of checking on the other (i.e., the total number of items crossed out) and subtracting the total
participant, the experimenter left so that the participant could work on the number of mistakes (i.e., the number of non-d2s erroneously marked), in
lexical decision task alone (participants completed the d2 test and lexical this way taking into account both errors of omission (i.e., the number of d2s
decision task in the absence of the confederate). The experimenter returned missed) and errors of commission.
shortly after to probe for suspicions, debrief, and compensate the
The d2 evaluation form. This questionnaire assessed participants’ ex-
participant.
perience during the d2 test. Of interest, participants rated on a scale from
0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) how much attention they devoted to the task
Experimental Manipulation and how much time they spent thinking about how well they were doing on
Communal condition. In this condition, the participant and the confed- the task by placing a slash through a line from 0% to 100%. An attention–
erate were informed that the second part of the study was investigating distraction score was calculated by subtracting how much participants
distributive reasoning and that they would be working on a task in which reported they were thinking about how well they were doing from how
they would each be asked Trivial Pursuit questions. Moreover, they were much attention they said they devoted to the d2 (higher numbers reflecting
told that they had the option to work with their partner on this task. greater attention).
Specifically, the experimenter said, “For this task, you have the option to Lexical decision task. This task was used to assess the cognitive
work with your partner; so, if you choose to work with your partner, accessibility of proximity and distance–rejection themes. Participants were
although each question will be directed at one of you, you will be able to presented with a series of letter strings on a PC and were instructed to judge
help each other.” The experimenter then paused, and the confederate as quickly and as accurately as possible whether each letter string was a
looked at the participant, smiled, and suggested working together as a team, word or nonword by pressing the appropriate key on the keyboard. Par-
in this way expressing the desire to be communal. The experimenter ticipants were given 12 practice trials to familiarize themselves with the
confirmed that they would be working as a team on the Trivial Pursuit task task, and then they began the 87 experimental trials.
(all participants agreed to work with the confederate) and asked if there Each trial began with a star presented in the middle of the screen,
were any questions. Finally, as in Studies 1 and 2, in this condition, the followed by the target stimulus after a pause of 500 ms. The target stimulus
participant interacted with an attractive, opposite-sex partner (a confeder- lasted on the screen for 1,000 ms during which participants indicated
ate) who was presented as single and a recent transfer student, in this way whether the target was a word or nonword. After they had indicated their
increasing the desirability and perceived likelihood of a communal rela- response, a blank screen followed for 500 ms, and then, the next trial
tionship with the confederate. began. The target letter strings were taken from Mikulincer, Birnbaum,
Performance-anxiety control condition. Our objective in designing Woddis, and Nachmias (2000) and consisted of 3 proximity words (close-
this condition was to create a noncommunal condition—that is, an inter- ness, love, and affection)6 and 6 distance–rejection words (separation,
action in which the possibility of closeness was unlikely— however, we did rejection, abandonment, distance, loneliness, and alone). For exploratory
not want participants to feel rejected by their partner. Thus, the participant purposes, we also included 5 coping strategy words (intimacy, escape,
and the confederate were informed that the second part of the study was
worry, security, and control), taken from Mikulincer (1998), which were
investigating general knowledge and would involve a Trivial Pursuit game.
theorized to reflect the coping strategies associated with different attach-
In this condition, they were not given the option to work together; rather,
ment orientations.7 In addition, 9 communion and 9 agency words were
they were told that the experimenter would be asking them the Trivial
included for pilot-testing purposes, along with 10 neutral words and 45
Pursuit questions individually and that each in turn would have the oppor-
nonwords. The 10 neutral words had no positive or negative connotations
tunity to answer the questions. Although the primary goal was to create a
and no link to proximity or rejection themes (e.g., elephant, book, and
noncommunal condition in which the confederate did not express the desire
picture). The 45 nonwords were created by taking common English words
to be communal by suggesting to work as a team, this meant having the
participant work individually on the Trivial Pursuit task and answer ques- and changing one letter. All words and nonwords were matched for number
tions in front of the confederate and experimenter. As a result, this of letters, and trials were randomly ordered across participants. This task
condition was conceptualized as a performance-anxiety condition. Finally, was programmed using E-prime and run on a Dell Pentium 3 XPS T700r
in this condition, we followed Clark’s (1984b) manipulation of an ex- PC.
change relationship and presented the confederate as dating and not a
transfer student to decrease the likelihood that the participant would see her 6
as available for a communal relationship. The confederate was also slightly Mikulincer et al. (2000) used six proximity words; however, during
less talkative during the hallway interaction (i.e., she did not initiate a pilot testing, three of these words (kiss, hug, and caress) were repeatedly
conversation, but if the participant asked her questions, she would respond, mentioned as curious and out of place given the context of the study; thus,
so as to not appear rude). these words were dropped from the lexical decision task.
7
It was thought that worry and security might be more accessible for the
anxiously attached, whereas escape and control might be more accessible
Materials
for the avoidantly attached, and that this might interact with experimental
ECR (Brennan et al., 1998). The 36-item questionnaire used in Studies condition. However, no mean differences in reaction times (RTs) to these
2 and 3 was used to assess attachment avoidance (␣ ⫽ .91) and anxiety words were found, nor were any correlations between these RTs and
(␣ ⫽ .89). mental concentration found as a function of attachment.
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 91

Manipulation check. After participants were debriefed, they rated the


extent to which they (a) liked their partner, (b) were attracted to their
partner, and (c) would like to be friends with their partner by placing a
slash through a line ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 (very much).
Participants were given privacy to answer the questionnaire and were
assured that their responses would remain anonymous and confidential. A
composite manipulation check measure was created by averaging these
three items (␣ ⫽ .71).

Results
Manipulation Check
The independent samples t test investigating the manipulation
check composite revealed a significant difference between exper-
imental conditions, t(67) ⫽ 3.48, p ⫽ .001. Supporting the exper-
imental manipulation, those in the communal condition liked their
partner more, were more attracted to their partner, and were more Figure 2. Mental concentration (d2 test performance) as a function of
interested in a relationship with their partner, M(32) ⫽ 69.55, than experimental condition and attachment anxiety (Study 4).
those in the performance-anxiety condition, M(37) ⫽ 58.62. The
interactive effects of experimental condition and attachment on
attraction to partner were also investigated in an analysis of vari-
ance (ANOVA) after splitting the attachment dimension scores at the low and high anxious participants in the performance-anxiety
the median. The ANOVA revealed a three-way interaction be- condition, nor was there a difference between conditions for the
tween condition, attachment anxiety, and attachment avoidance, high anxious participants (both ts ⬍ 1.5). Thus, as predicted,
F(1, 61) ⫽ 4.63, p ⬍ .05. Focused comparisons showed that low overall, participants were less able to concentrate in the perfor-
anxious–low avoidant participants and high anxious– high mance-anxiety condition, suggesting their preoccupation with an-
avoidant participants were more attracted to the confederate in the swering the Trivial Pursuit questions in front of their partner.
communal condition, M(7) ⫽ 77.19 and M(9) ⫽ 70.30, respec- However, whereas the communal condition facilitated perfor-
tively, than in the performance-anxiety condition, M(12) ⫽ 55.08 mance for the more secure participants, it impaired performance
and M(11) ⫽ 57.52, respectively, t(61) ⫽ 3.58, p ⬍ .001, and for the more anxiously attached.
t(61) ⫽ 2.19, p ⬍ .05, whereas there were no between-condition
differences in partner attraction for low anxious– high avoidant
participants or for high anxious–low avoidant participants. There
Self-Reported Attention–Distraction
were no attachment group differences within the communal and The ANOVA investigating the effects of condition and attach-
performance-anxiety conditions. ment on self-reported attention– distraction yielded a marginally
significant interaction between condition and attachment anxiety,
Mental Concentration–Rumination F(1, 61) ⫽ 3.32, p ⫽ .073. In the communal condition, high
anxious participants reported being less attentive and more dis-
To investigate the influence of condition and attachment on tracted, M(18) ⫽ 29.39, than their less anxious counterparts,
mental concentration–rumination a 2 (condition: communal vs. M(14) ⫽ 62.36, t(61) ⫽ 2.04, p ⬍ .05. There were no other group
performance anxiety) ⫻ 2 (attachment anxiety: low vs. high) ⫻ 2 differences (ts ⬍ 1.5).
(attachment avoidance: low vs. high) ANOVA was conducted on
participants’ d2 scores (i.e., the sum of d2 hits minus mistakes).8
The ANOVA investigating the effects of experimental condition Lexical Decision Analyses9
and attachment anxiety and avoidance on d2 performance yielded
a main effect for condition, F(1, 58) ⫽ 4.70, p ⬍ .05. Overall, Drawing on Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz (1998), we
performance was worse in the performance-anxiety condition, recoded reaction times (RTs) on the lexical decision task less than
M(37) ⫽ 183.36, than in the communal condition, M(29) ⫽ 300 ms as 300 ms and RTs greater than 3,000 ms as 3,000 ms.
200.66; however, this effect was qualified by an interaction be-
tween condition and attachment anxiety, F(1, 58) ⫽ 4.49, p ⬍ .05, 8
Three participants with errors-of-commission scores (mistakes) greater
but not by an interaction with avoidance (F ⬍ 1.5).
than 60 (i.e., more than 4 standard deviations above the group mean of
As depicted in Figure 2, participants low in attachment anxiety
5.75) were excluded from the d2 analyses because they most likely mis-
in the communal condition performed significantly better, understood the instructions to the d2 test. The fourth highest participant had
M(13) ⫽ 212.67, than their low anxious counterparts in the per- an error-of-commission score of 17.
formance-anxiety condition, M(20) ⫽ 178.48, t(58) ⫽ 3.05, p ⬍ 9
Although native English speakers were targeted, 13 participants re-
.01, and, importantly, also performed significantly better than their ported that English was their second language. One participant who rated
high anxious counterparts in the communal condition, M(16) ⫽ his English proficiency as moderate (3 on a scale from 1 to 5) was dropped
188.64, t(58) ⫽ 2.05, p ⬍ .05. There was no difference between from the lexical decision analyses. The 12 remaining participants who
92 BARTZ AND LYDON

Overall, the error rates were low (the mean was 2.7%).10 RTs for light the importance of a secure base: When secure participants
words with incorrect responses were replaced with the mean RT interacted with someone who expressed liking and acceptance, as
for that word. Error rates were not related to experimental condi- opposed to someone who was simply neutral, they went on to excel
tion or attachment. The mean RT across all conditions was 740.96 at the mental concentration task. Unfortunately, the potential for
ms (SD ⫽ 146.84). closeness did not have the same effect on their anxious counterparts.
Within-cell correlations were calculated to investigate the asso- Anxious individuals ruminated even in response to a relatively posi-
ciation between the accessibility of proximity or distance–rejection tive interpersonal event—that is, an attractive, available other express-
and d2 performance. Specifically, standardized residuals of the ing interest in them. Finally, situations involving possible closeness
proximity and distance–rejection RTs were created using control did not trigger concerns about rejection for the anxiously attached, a
word RT as the predictor (negative numbers reflecting quicker RTs point to which we return in the General Discussion.
and increased accessibility). Participants’ d2 scores were also Avoidant individuals did not exhibit performance deficits in the
standardized (negative numbers reflecting poorer performance). communal condition. Given that a core feature of avoidant attachment
The cross-products of these standardized scores were taken (e.g., is discomfort when others try to get too close and that avoidant
the product of proximity accessibility and d2 score). We then individuals liked their partner less when their partner worked as a unit
conducted 2 (condition: communal vs. performance anxiety) ⫻ 2 in Study 2, it is likely that the confederate’s suggestion to work as a
(attachment anxiety: low vs. high) ⫻ 2 (attachment avoidance: low team would have been somewhat distressing to the avoidant partici-
vs. high) ANOVAs on the within-cell correlations between (a) pants. However, avoidant attachment is associated with a deactivation
Proximity RT ⫻ d2 Performance and (b) Distance–Rejection of the attachment system, and avoidant individuals tend to cope with
RT ⫻ d2 Performance.11 distress by suppressing their concerns, thus, their distress in the
The ANOVA investigating the correlation between proximity communal condition may not have compromised their performance
accessibility and d2 performance yielded a significant interaction on the mental concentration task.
between condition and attachment anxiety, F(1, 57) ⫽ 4.49, p ⬍
.05. In the communal condition, proximity accessibility was asso- General Discussion
ciated with better d2 performance for low anxious individuals,
M(13) ⫽ ⫺.69, but proximity accessibility was associated with Our goal in conducting this research was to investigate whether
poorer d2 performance for high anxious individuals, M(15) ⫽ .47, attachment is related to people’s ability to tolerate the uncertainty
t(57) ⫽ 2.37, p ⬍ .05. Thus, in the communal condition, proximity inherent in the early phases of relationship development. People
accessibility facilitated performance for the less anxiously attached who hope to establish a relationship try to follow the communal
but impaired performance for the more anxiously attached. script to signal interest, but the desire to express interest is tem-
In addition, a second interaction was found between attachment pered by their uncertainty about the prospect for a relationship and
anxiety and avoidance independent of experimental condition, F(1, the other’s motives. Because differences in attachment reflect
57) ⫽ 4.29, p ⬍ .05. Among those high in anxiety, proximity beliefs about whether the self is worthy of affection and whether
accessibility was associated with increased performance for those others are trustworthy and reliable, it was theorized that attachment
low in avoidance, M(15) ⫽ ⫺.46, but was associated with decreased would be related to the ability to tolerate the interdependence
performance for those high in avoidance, M(17) ⫽ .76, t(57) ⫽ 2.68, dilemma. Overall, our predictions have been supported.
p ⬍ .01. Finally, there were no significant main or interaction effects
of condition and/or attachment on the cross-product of distance– Summary and Discussion of Findings
rejection accessibility and d2 performance (all Fs ⬍ 2.1).
When anxiously attached individuals interacted with an attrac-
tive, desirable, opposite-sex partner, they obscured individual con-
Discussion
tributions to a group task. Drawing on Clark’s (1984b) theorizing,
As predicted, performance on the mental concentration task was we believe they did this to signal their interest in closeness (i.e., by
impaired for everyone in the performance-anxiety condition, but obscuring individual contributions, they conveyed their preference
anxiously attached individuals also performed poorly in the com- for a communal relationship over an exchange relationship). The
munal condition. Secure participants, on the other hand, appeared notion that anxiously attached individuals would go out of their
to benefit from the communal condition—performing better than way to signal interest in a communal relationship with a potential
their anxious counterparts in the communal condition and better close other is consistent with their chronic desire for closeness and
than their secure counterparts in the performance-anxiety condi- acceptance. These results are also in accord with Mikulincer and
tion. What was it about the communal condition that helped the Nachshon’s (1991) research findings that anxiously attached indi-
secures but hurt the anxiously attached participants? Analyses viduals are especially prone to self-disclosure and tend to self-
looking at the association between d2 performance and proximity
accessibility suggest that whereas thoughts about closeness facil- 10
itated the secure individuals’ performance, these thoughts under- Three real words and seven nonwords had exceptionally high error
rates (greater than 15%) and consequently were dropped from the analyses.
mined the anxious individuals’ performance. These findings high-
The real words that were dropped were not target words.
11
We also looked at the effects of experimental condition and attach-
ment on the accessibility of the proximity– closeness, distance–rejection,
reported that English was not their native language did not differ from the and control strategy words. These analyses yielded one marginal effect.
native English speakers on error rates (t ⬍ 1) or RT (t ⬍ 1). There were no significant main effects or interactions.
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 93

disclose indiscriminatingly to strangers. Indeed, these researchers individuals inferred more meaning and were less likely to discount
proposed that anxiously attached individuals use self-disclosure even relatively trivial communal behaviors as purely situational.
opportunities to merge with others. In the same way, anxiously On the other hand, it is likely that these judgments were guarded
attached individuals may have obscured contributions in the cur- by feelings of vulnerability and the acute possibility that they may
rent investigation to symbolically merge with their partner. Yet ultimately be found unworthy. As Holmes (1991) stated, “the
were anxiously attached individuals obscuring contributions to specter of things going well reminds [uncertain] people of the costs
avoid their partner’s negative evaluation should they perform of their being wrong” (p. 84). This idea is echoed in the attachment
poorly on the group task? The findings from Study 2 suggest this literature as well. For example, Mikulincer and Shaver (2003)
explanation is unlikely. If anxious individuals were obscuring speculated that for anxiously attached individuals, positive states
contributions to avoid negative evaluation, they should have been may quickly bring to mind other instances in which things began
less anxious in the communal condition in Study 2 because their well but ended badly.
performance was indistinguishable from their partner’s, but this Although Study 2 suggests that anxious individuals were con-
was not the case. cerned about evaluation, Study 4 did not find evidence that anx-
So, when interacting with an attractive, desirable, opposite-sex iously attached individuals were specifically concerned about re-
partner, anxious individuals went out of their way to signal inter- jection, so how and when might concerns about rejection come
est. However, when such a partner signaled interest by following into play? Holmes (1991) described the interdependence dilemma
the communal script, anxious individuals were not reassured. In as a dialectic that unfolds over time. People signal their interest
Study 2, attachment anxiety was associated with greater interper- and then look for signs of interest and commitment in the other’s
sonal anxiety (i.e., state feelings of anxiety, uncertainty, and con- behavior; this back and forth continues as the relationship unfolds
cerns about appearance) when a potential close other used com- (or not, as the case may be). This research focused on the very
munal norms (behaved as a unit) compared with when the other early stages of relationship development, a point at which anxious
used exchange norms. In Study 4, anxiously attached individuals individuals may have been quite hopeful. Perhaps research that
performed poorly on an unrelated mental concentration task when focuses on interactions occurring later in relationship development
a potential close other expressed interest in closeness by suggest- would yield increased concerns about rejection because there is
ing to work as a team; moreover, results from the lexical decision greater investment and, consequently, greater risk. Anxiously at-
task in Study 4 suggest that it was thoughts about closeness in tached individuals’ mounting interpersonal anxiety may eventually
particular that undermined anxious individuals’ performance in the bias their perceptions in a negative direction. It may also be that in
communal condition. Given that securing closeness is a chronic the same way that anxious individuals magnify relatively trivial
goal for anxiously attached individuals, why would they experi- positive behaviors in this process of uncertainty reduction, they
ence more interpersonal anxiety when a potential close other also magnify relatively trivial negative behaviors. This notion is
expressed interest in a communal relationship? consistent with Fraley and Shaver’s (2000) conceptualization of
We believe that anxious individuals felt especially vulnerable in the anxiety dimension of attachment as being critically involved in
these potential communal situations and that this vulnerability led the appraisal and monitoring of attachment related cues and the
to increased feelings of anxiety, uncertainty, and concerns about idea that individuals who are high in attachment anxiety have a
how their partner would evaluate them. In Study 1, anxious indi- low threshold for detecting cues of rejection and acceptance.
viduals made greater overtures than their secure and avoidant Indeed, Campbell, Simpson, Boldry, and Kashy (2005) found
counterparts; as a result, they may have felt more anxious about support for this idea in a recent diary study in which it was found
whether those overtures would be met with equal interest. That is, that anxious individuals tended to magnify both positive and
by putting themselves on the line this way, they had more to lose. negative relationship-relevant behaviors in their established rela-
On a related note, Vorauer, Cameron, Holmes, and Pearce (2003) tionships. Our research adds to this literature by showing that
found that anxious individuals tend to imbue their overtures to a anxious individuals’ vigilant monitoring of attachment-related be-
potential romantic partner with increased significance and mean- haviors occurs even in the context of potential new relationships.
ing. Even when they do not objectively make greater overtures, In contrast to their more anxious counterparts, secure partici-
anxious individuals think they have communicated more interest pants did not actively avoid using exchange norms when interact-
because they believe others will take into account their inhibitions ing with an attractive, available, opposite-sex partner, but impor-
when judging their behavioral intentions. Thus, in the present tantly, they also did not use exchange norms (i.e., their pen choice
investigation, when anxious individuals did make greater over- was random). Secure participants also felt comfortable with the
tures, it is likely they felt especially vulnerable. potential close other’s use of communal norms. They were not
At the heart of the matter, we believe that situations involving distressed when their partner expressed the desire to be a unit
possible closeness amplify anxious individuals’ interpersonal am- (Studies 2 and 4), and in fact, they actually benefited from the
bivalence and elicit an approach–avoidance conflict leading to communal condition in Study 4. Were secure participants simply
heightened feelings of interpersonal anxiety. Study 1 suggests that not interested in a relationship with their partner? Because they
anxious individuals were looking for closeness in these encounters, were the most likely group to have a strong network of friends,
and this may have lead to an internal dialectic between their secure participants may not have been especially motivated to
deepest hopes and fears (Holmes, 1991). On the one hand, their form a new relationship. However, we believe this explanation is
desire for acceptance should have led them to look for evidence of unlikely for a few reasons. First, in the current studies, we tried to
caring and to infer meaning from their partner’s behavior—indeed, create situations in which the potential for a close relationship was
Study 3 suggests this was the case. In Study 3, anxiously attached desirable and feasible. Drawing on Clark (1984b, 1986), we tar-
94 BARTZ AND LYDON

geted single participants and paired them with an attractive, This hypothesis would be consistent with avoidant individuals’
opposite-sex confederate who was single and a recent transfer pessimistic beliefs about others; however, if this were the case, one
student. So, even if secure participants had several close friends, would think avoidant individuals would have ascribed more neg-
they were still not involved in a romantic relationship and thus ative agency traits (e.g., greedy) and not more negative communal
should have been interested in the confederate. Second, secure traits (e.g., servile) to their partner in the communal condition.
participants did not differ from their anxious counterparts on Avoidant individuals strive for independence, tend to downplay
partner liking (Studies 2 and 4) or interest in developing a rela- the importance of closeness, and use avoidance strategies to cope
tionship (Study 3). Finally, thoughts about closeness, love, and with concerns. It may be that when avoidant individuals enter
affection were associated with a dampening of rumination in Study situations involving possible closeness (situations that, in theory,
4. If secure participants were not at least somewhat interested, they should be distressing), they attempt to suppress the possibility of
should not have benefited from the potential for closeness in this closeness. Consistent with this idea, in Study 3, avoidant individ-
study. uals downplayed relational motives for the potential close other’s
Rather than secure individuals being disinterested in their part- communal behavior. Moreover, although they professed to be
ner, we believe secure individuals’ self-esteem, trust in others, and more indifferent to the potential close other’s communal behavior
previous successful close relationship experiences account for in Study 3, avoidant attachment was associated with feeling uneasy
their behavior in the present investigations. In contrast to their in Studies 2 and 3, suggesting that their professed indifference may
more anxious counterparts, in Study 1, secure individuals felt have been a façade. Indeed, this notion is in line with research
comfortable letting the situation unfold without a great deal of suggesting that avoidant individuals’ defensive strategies may
effort on their part. Indeed, secure individuals may have been more actually be quite fragile (Mikulincer, Dolev, & Shaver, 2004).
like the existing friends (whose pen choice was also random) in
Clark’s (1984b) original study. Clark (1984b) theorized that pen Research Strengths and Limitations
choice was random for existing friends because existing friends
Three of the four studies presented had participants interact with
know they are friends, so there is no need to send a message
a real person (a confederate), and behavioral responses were as-
conveying interest in closeness. In the current investigation, secure
sessed; nevertheless, there were limitations to this approach. Stud-
participants may have felt confident that the relationship would
ies 1 and 2 did not assess how participants were construing their
develop and, as a result, did not feel the need to go out of their way
behavior or the behavior of the confederate, mainly because of
to make it happen. The findings from Study 3 also support this
concerns about drawing attention to the research questions and
idea. Secure participants were less likely than their anxious coun-
possibly undermining participants’ behavioral responses. Were
terparts to invest their potential close other’s discrete communal
participants intending to convey interest by obscuring contribu-
behavior with increased significance for relationship development,
tions? Did they perceive their partner’s communal behavior as an
suggesting that although they may have been interested in devel-
expression of interest? Study 3 shed some light on the kind of
oping a relationship, they were not preoccupied with evaluating the
attributions participants were making and what meaning they saw
meaning of discrete events and using that information to infer the
in the potential close other’s communal behavior. That said, we
other’s motives and to diagnose relationship potential.
believe that although people use communal and exchange behav-
Another possible factor distinguishing the secure participants
iors to convey how they feel about closeness, this may often occur
from their more anxious counterparts may have been the goals that
at an implicit level; thus, explicitly asking participants about their
were activated in our experimental situations. In a study looking at
intentions may provide limited information. The lexical decision
attachment and the pursuit of attachment and affiliation goals,
task used in Study 4 is one way to examine the implicit cognitions
Mikulincer and Selinger (2001) found that secure adolescents
and motivations that guide these interactions.
pursued attachment goals in attachment-appropriate contexts and
In addition, in our attempt to create a noncommunal condition
affiliation goals in affiliation-appropriate contexts, whereas anx-
that paralleled the communal condition in Study 4, concerns about
ious adolescents focused exclusively on attachment goals and
performance were aroused, and anxious participants performed
tended to pursue attachment goals in attachment and affiliation
poorly in both conditions. Would they have performed poorly if
contexts. In the current studies, secure participants may have
they had simply been asked to do the d2 test without interacting
viewed these situations as opportunities to pursue affiliation goals,
with another participant? Although anxious participants did not
whereas anxious participants may have viewed them as opportu-
perform more poorly than the secures in the performance-anxiety
nities to pursue attachment goals.
condition, it is possible that anxious individuals are generally poor
Finally, contrary to our predictions, when avoidant individuals
at mental concentration tasks, and future research should be con-
interacted with a potential close other, overall, they did not use
ducted to explore this possibility. Nonetheless, we believe the
exchange norms. Perhaps avoidant individuals only feel the need
important finding from Study 4 is that unlike their more secure
to establish boundaries when they perceive that others are trying to
counterparts, anxious participants did not benefit from the com-
get too close. Indeed, the findings from Study 2 suggest this may
munal condition—for them, thoughts about closeness, love, and
be the case. Compared with when their partner used exchange
affection did not dampen performance anxiety and rumination.
norms, when their partner tried to work as a unit (i.e., used
communal norms), avoidant individuals liked their partner less.
Theoretical Contributions and Future Directions
Perhaps avoidant individuals felt exploited when their partner used
the same color pen; by obscuring how much each person contrib- A major proposition of attachment theory is that attachment
uted to the task, it would be easier for the confederate to freeload. models are carried forward into new relationships, guiding percep-
NAVIGATING THE INTERDEPENDENCE DILEMMA 95

tions, expectations, and behaviors with new partners (Collins & References
Read, 1994); however, little is known about how the attachment
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among
system operates during the early phases of relationship develop-
young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality
ment. This research establishes the importance of attachment at the and Social Psychology, 61, 226 –244.
outset of a relationship and points to some circumstances under Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
which attachment models should come into play. Attachment interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psycho-
models appear to be influential when little is known about what logical Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
can be expected from the other person (Collins & Read, 1994; Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York:
Pierce & Lydon, 2001; Pietromonaco & Feldman Barrett, 2000). Basic Books.
The match between the situation and chronic attachment goals Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and
related to security attainment also appears to be important. Finally, anger. New York: Basic Books.
behaviors related to the exchange of social commodities that are Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure-
ment of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson &
used to define the nature of the relationship appear to be associated
W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp.
with attachment. 46 –76). New York: Guilford Press.
One question arising from the current research is whether these Brickenkamp, R. (1981). Test d2 (7th ed.). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
findings apply to all close relationships, friendships, or romantic Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2005). Percep-
relationships in particular. To increase the chances that participants tions of conflict and support in romantic relationships: The role of
would desire a relationship with the confederate, the possibility of attachment anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,
a romantic relationship was made salient. That said, as noted 510 –531.
earlier, some participants may have thought of the confederate as Clark, M. S. (1984a). A distinction between two types of relationships and
a potential romantic partner, whereas others may have thought of its implications for development. In J. C. Masters & K. Yarkin-Levin
the confederate as a friend. Study 3 spoke to this issue. Participants (Eds.), Boundary areas in social and developmental psychology (pp.
241–270). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
were allowed to nominate either a potential friend or potential
Clark, M. S. (1984b). Record keeping in two types of relationships. Journal
romantic partner, and relationship type did not qualify the results, of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 549 –557.
suggesting that the present findings would apply to close relation- Clark, M. S. (1986). Evidence for the effectiveness of manipulations of
ships in general. It is worth noting that participants were more communal and exchange relationships. Personality and Social Psychol-
interested in developing a romantic relationship in Study 3. More- ogy Bulletin, 12, 414 – 425.
over, adult attachment relationships tend to be romantic (Fraley & Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and
Shaver, 2000; Hazan & Zeifman, 1999). Given this, we believe the communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
present findings should be especially relevant to potential romantic 37, 12–24.
relationships, but may also apply to close friendships, depending Clark, M. S., Mills, J., & Powell, M. C. (1986). Keeping track of needs in
on the level of interest and the extent to which the other is communal and exchange relationships. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 51, 333–338.
perceived as a potential attachment figure.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S., & Aiken, L. (2003). Applied multiple
Is attachment associated with the use of communal and ex- regression/correlation analysis for the behavioural sciences (3rd ed.).
change norms in established relationships? The early stages of a Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
relationship are fraught with uncertainty, but in existing relation- Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations of attach-
ships, people presumably have established a certain level of trust ment: The structure and functions of working models. In K. Bar-
and are more knowledgeable about their partner’s intentions and tholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships
level of commitment. That said, we believe attachment would (Vol. 5, pp. 53–90). London: Jessica Kingsley.
similarly be associated with the use of communal and exchange Davilia, J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C. (1997). Why does attachment style
norms in established relationships to the extent that satisfying change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 826 – 838.
chronic attachment goals continues to be important. Uncertainty Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment and the suppres-
sion of unwanted thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
may also be a critical factor. Perhaps if anxiously attached indi-
ogy, 73, 1080 –1091.
viduals felt particularly uncertain—for example, when making a Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theo-
major personal or relational decision—they may be especially retical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered ques-
likely to monitor their partner’s discrete behaviors for reassurance. tions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132–154.
Fraley, R. C., & Waller, N. G. (1998). Adult attachment patterns: A test of
the typographical model. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Concluding Comments
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 77–114). New York:
In conclusion, the early stages of a relationship are marked by Guilford Press.
uncertainty and an interdependence dilemma in which people must Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring
wager on the motives of the other person and decide whether being individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464 –1480.
communal is worth the risk. They must also tailor their own
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an
behavior to communicate interest but not appear overly concerned attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
with social transactions. Although people share the same script for 511–524.
close relationships, the belief structures and goals associated with Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational
chronic attachment orientations provide direction on how that framework for research on close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5,
script will be played out as the relationship unfolds. 1–22.
96 BARTZ AND LYDON

Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1999). Pair bonds as attachments: Evaluating Mikulincer, M., Dolev, T., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Attachment-related
the evidence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach- strategies during thought suppression: Ironic rebounds and vulnerable
ment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 336 –354). New self-representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
York: Guilford Press. 940 –956.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and patterns of
scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 321–
Psychology, 60, 895–910. 331.
Holmes, J. G. (1981). The exchange process in close relationships: Mi- Mikulincer, M., & Selinger, M. (2001). The interplay between attachment
crobehavior and macromotives. In M. J. Lerner & S. C. Lerner (Eds.), and affiliation systems in adolescents’ same-sex friendships: The role of
The justice motive in social behavior (pp. 261–284). New York: Plenum attachment style. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18,
Press. 81–106.
Holmes, J. G. (1991). Trust and the appraisal process in close relationships. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system
In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships in adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes.
(Vol. 2, pp. 57–104). London: Jessica Kingsley. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988, July 29). Social 35, pp. 53–152). New York: Academic Press.
relationships and health. Science, 241, 540 –545. Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. E. (2001). Global and specific relational models in
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His the experience of social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472–503. Psychology, 80, 613– 631.
Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating Pietromonaco, P. R., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2000). The internal working
effects of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality and models concept: What do we really know about the self in relation to
Social Psychology, 40, 155–170. others? Review of General Psychology, 4, 155–175.
Leary, M. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: Social anxiety, jealousy, Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (1998). Attachment in adulthood. In J. A.
loneliness, depression, and low self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clin- Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relation-
ical Psychology, 9, 221–229. ships (pp. 3–21). New York: Guilford Press.
Lydon, J. E., Jamieson, D. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). The meaning of Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and
social interactions in the transition from acquaintanceship to friendship. positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 536 –548. their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors.
Lynch, J. J. (1979). The broken heart: The medical consequences of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673–1682.
loneliness. New York: Basic Books. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York:
Maslow, A. H. (1962). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Norton.
Nostrand. Trout, D. L. (1980). The role of social isolation in suicide. Suicide and
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Profile of Mood Life-Threatening Behavior, 10, 10 –23.
States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services. Vorauer, J. D., Cameron, J. J., Holmes, J. G., & Pearce, D. G. (2003).
Mikulincer, M. (1998). Attachment working models and the sense of trust: Invisible overtures: Fears of rejection and the signal amplification bias.
An exploration of interaction goals and affect regulation. Journal of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 793– 812.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1209 –1224.
Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress
and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative Received September 21, 2004
and intraindividual components of attachment theory. Journal of Per- Revision received October 26, 2005
sonality and Social Psychology, 78, 509 –523. Accepted October 28, 2005 䡲

Instructions to Authors
For Instructions to Authors, please visit www.apa.org/journals/psp and click on the “Instructions to
Authors” link in the Journal Info box on the right.
96 BARTZ AND LYDON

Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1999). Pair bonds as attachments: Evaluating Mikulincer, M., Dolev, T., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Attachment-related
the evidence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach- strategies during thought suppression: Ironic rebounds and vulnerable
ment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 336 –354). New self-representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
York: Guilford Press. 940 –956.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and patterns of
scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 321–
Psychology, 60, 895–910. 331.
Holmes, J. G. (1981). The exchange process in close relationships: Mi- Mikulincer, M., & Selinger, M. (2001). The interplay between attachment
crobehavior and macromotives. In M. J. Lerner & S. C. Lerner (Eds.), and affiliation systems in adolescents’ same-sex friendships: The role of
The justice motive in social behavior (pp. 261–284). New York: Plenum attachment style. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18,
Press. 81–106.
Holmes, J. G. (1991). Trust and the appraisal process in close relationships. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system
In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships in adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes.
(Vol. 2, pp. 57–104). London: Jessica Kingsley. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988, July 29). Social 35, pp. 53–152). New York: Academic Press.
relationships and health. Science, 241, 540 –545. Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. E. (2001). Global and specific relational models in
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His the experience of social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472–503. Psychology, 80, 613– 631.
Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating Pietromonaco, P. R., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2000). The internal working
effects of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality and models concept: What do we really know about the self in relation to
Social Psychology, 40, 155–170. others? Review of General Psychology, 4, 155–175.
Leary, M. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: Social anxiety, jealousy, Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (1998). Attachment in adulthood. In J. A.
loneliness, depression, and low self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clin- Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relation-
ical Psychology, 9, 221–229. ships (pp. 3–21). New York: Guilford Press.
Lydon, J. E., Jamieson, D. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). The meaning of Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and
social interactions in the transition from acquaintanceship to friendship. positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 536 –548. their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors.
Lynch, J. J. (1979). The broken heart: The medical consequences of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673–1682.
loneliness. New York: Basic Books. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York:
Maslow, A. H. (1962). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Norton.
Nostrand. Trout, D. L. (1980). The role of social isolation in suicide. Suicide and
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Profile of Mood Life-Threatening Behavior, 10, 10 –23.
States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services. Vorauer, J. D., Cameron, J. J., Holmes, J. G., & Pearce, D. G. (2003).
Mikulincer, M. (1998). Attachment working models and the sense of trust: Invisible overtures: Fears of rejection and the signal amplification bias.
An exploration of interaction goals and affect regulation. Journal of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 793– 812.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1209 –1224.
Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress
and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative Received September 21, 2004
and intraindividual components of attachment theory. Journal of Per- Revision received October 26, 2005
sonality and Social Psychology, 78, 509 –523. Accepted October 28, 2005 䡲

Instructions to Authors
For Instructions to Authors, please visit www.apa.org/journals/psp and click on the “Instructions to
Authors” link in the Journal Info box on the right.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 91, No. 1, 97–110 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.97

Intergroup Helping as Status Relations: Effects of Status Stability,


Identification, and Type of Help on Receptivity to High-Status
Group’s Help

Arie Nadler Samer Halabi


Tel Aviv University University of Haifa

Integrating research on social identity processes and helping relations, the authors proposed that
low-status group members who are high identifiers will be unwilling to receive help from the high-status
group when status relations are perceived as unstable and help is dependency-oriented. The first
experiment, a minimal group experiment, found negative reactions to help from a high-status outgroup
when status relations were unstable. The 2nd and 3rd experiments, which used real groups of Israeli
Arabs and Israeli Jews, replicated this finding and showed that high identifiers were less receptive to help
from the high-status outgroup than low identifiers. The 4th experiment, a help-seeking experiment with
real groups of competing high schools, found that the least amount of help was sought from a high-status
group by high identifiers when status relations were perceived as unstable and help was dependency-
oriented. Theoretical and applied implications are discussed.

Keywords: reactions to help, help-seeking, perceived status stability, ingroup identification, dependency-/
autonomy-oriented help

In his classic essay The Gift, anthropologist Marcel Mauss SOCIAL IDENTITY PERSPECTIVE
(1907/1957) described the tribal custom of potlatch, in which a
clan leader engages in lavish displays of gift-giving to other clan Over the last three decades, the social identity perspective on
leaders. Mauss noted that the “motives for such excessive gifts . . . intergroup relations (Turner & Reynolds, 2001) has progressed in
are in no way disinterested. . . . To give is to show one’s superi- two complementary lines of research and theory: social identity
ority” (p. 72). This is group-based behavior in that tribal leaders theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986) and self-categorization the-
establish their hierarchical position “to the ultimate benefit . . . of ory (Tajfel, 1978; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell,
their own clan” (italics added, p. 4). For recipients of potlatch, “to 1987). Self-categorization theory holds that people’s identity fluc-
accept without returning . . . [is] to become client and subservient” tuates across a continuum ranging from individual (e.g., “I am a
(p. 72). Phrased in the language of modern social psychology, this generous person”) to social (e.g., “I am a fan of a particular soccer
suggests that helping relations can be mechanisms through which team”) identity, and research has explored variables that affect the
groups create, maintain, and change status relations. movement between these two poles and its consequences (e.g.,
The present research examined the willingness of low-status Onorato & Turner, 2004; Roccas, 2003). Social identity theory
group members to seek and receive help from a high-status out- originated with the pioneering work of Tajfel and his colleagues,
group as affected by the perceived stability of intergroup status who argued that in their quest for positive identity, group members
relations, the ingroup identification of the recipient, and type of positively distinguish themselves from outgroups by discriminat-
help (i.e., autonomy- or dependency-oriented help). The research ing against them (i.e., Tajfel & Turner, 1979). In support of this,
hypotheses follow the model of intergroup helping as status rela- research has demonstrated that the division of people into groups,
tions (Nadler, 2002), which draws on social identity research even on a relatively insignificant dimension (e.g., as specific vs.
(Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and helping relations (Bierhoff, 2002; global perceivers), is sufficient to produce ingroup favoritism (i.e.,
Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin, & Schroeder, 2005; Schroeder, Penner, discrimination against that outgroup) and outgroup devaluation
Dovidio, & Piliavin, 1995). The present research represents the (e.g., Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1999).
first empirical effort to assess the major tenets of this model. More recent research indicates that the phenomena of ingroup
favoritism and outgroup devaluation are affected by the status of
the ingroup and the outgroup (Bourhis & Gagnon, 2003). Whereas
members of high-status groups show greater discrimination toward
Arie Nadler, Department of Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, low- than equal-status groups (Sachdev & Bourhis, 1991), low-
Israel; Samer Halabi, Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, status groups sometimes exhibit outgroup favoritism: When they
Haifa, Israel.
have internalized their low status, they favor the high-status out-
This research was supported by the Argentina Chair for Research on the
group on dimensions of comparison that are related to their status
Social Psychology of Conflict and Cooperation, Tel-Aviv University. We
thank Sonia Roccas for helpful comments on earlier versions of this article. inferiority (Sachdev & Bourhis, 1987). The inconsistency that is
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Arie created between outgroup favoritism and people’s general need for
Nadler, Department of Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv positive identity can be resolved by individual mobility (joining the
69978, Israel. E-mail: arie@freud.tau.ac.il high-status group), social creativity (e.g., reframing the intergroup

97
98 NADLER AND HALABI

comparison in a way that favors the ingroup), or social competition action programs can be a stigmatizing experience for its benefi-
(working to elevate the status of the ingroup through social ciaries (Pratkanis & Turner, 1996; Steele, 1992).
change; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Social competition is more likely The autonomy or dependency orientation of help may also
when members of the low-status group perceive the existing status influence whether dependence on a more privileged outgroup will
hierarchy as relatively unstable and illegitimate and intergroup threaten the recipient’s social identity (Nadler, 1997, 1998).
boundaries as impermeable (Tajfel & Turner, 1986). When status Dependency-oriented help consists of providing a full solution to
relations are perceived as illegitimate and unstable, members of the problem at hand and reflects the helper’s view that the needy
low-status groups are likely to view the existing social hierarchy as cannot help themselves. When recipients agree that they cannot
changeable, and because the impermeability of intergroup bound- make it on their own, dependency-oriented help is consistent with
aries prevents individual mobility, they are expected to channel their view of themselves and they may readily seek and accept it.
their motivation for social equality toward elevating ingroup sta- However, when recipients believe they can succeed independently,
tus. Regarding the differential effects of perceived legitimacy and dependency-oriented help is inconsistent with their view of them-
perceived stability, research has indicated that variations in levels selves as capable actors. In this case, potential recipients of help
of perceived stability have stronger effects on the behavior and are likely to reject offers of dependency-oriented help and refrain
perceptions of low-status group members toward the high-status from seeking it. Autonomy-oriented help is partial and temporary
outgroup (e.g., Ellemers, Wilke, & van Knippenberg, 1993; Mum- (e.g., taking the form of instructions or hints) and reflects the
mendey, Klink, Mielke, Wenzel, & Blanz, 1999). Because the helper’s view that given the appropriate tools, recipients can help
present focus is on behavior and perceptions directed at the high- themselves (Brickman et al., 1982). Autonomy-oriented help al-
status outgroup helper, we centered our attention on the effects of lows recipients to retain their independence despite their reliance
perceived stability on receptivity to help from the high-status on the more resourceful helper (Nadler, 1997, 1998). Therefore
group. this type of help is not likely to clash with recipients’ view of
themselves as capable and equal actors. Applied to the present
HELPING RELATIONS AS POWER RELATIONS research context, this suggests that dependency-oriented help, but
not autonomy-oriented help, will be inconsistent with the low-
Helping relations are inherently unequal social relations. The status group’s motivation for equality. Because this motivation
helper has sufficient resources to confer on a recipient, who is grows higher when status relations are perceived as unstable,
dependent on the helper’s goodwill. This inequality makes receiv- members of low-status groups are expected to be reluctant to seek
ing help a potentially self-threatening experience for the benefi- or receive dependency-oriented help under these conditions.
ciary (Nadler & Fisher, 1986). In this line, empirical research has
found that when help is self-threatening, people respond negatively Intergroup Helping Relations as Status Relations
to its receipt (e.g., Nadler, 1987; Nadler & Fisher, 1986) and prefer
to endure hardships rather than seek it (Nadler, 1991). Yet, this Figure 1 is a schematic representation of intergroup helping as
research has converged almost exclusively on interpersonal help- status relations (Nadler, 2002). It suggests two clusters of inter-
ing encounters. In an exception to this interpersonal focus, Schnei- group helping relations: (a) when status relations are perceived as
der, Major, Luhtanen, and Crocker (1996) studied reactions to stable and legitimate and (b) when they are viewed as unstable and
interracial help and found that African American participants who illegitimate. In the first case, the high-status group is expected to
received assumptive help (i.e., unsolicited help) from a European try to maintain its social advantage by providing dependency-
American experienced lower self-esteem than African Americans oriented help to the low-status group. Under these conditions, the
who received assumptive help from a fellow African American. low-status group is expected to be receptive to dependency-
Similar findings were reported in a study that examined the reac- oriented help. When status relations are perceived as unstable
tions of Arab-Israelis to the receipt of help from a Jewish-Israeli as and illegitimate, members of the high-status group view their
opposed to an Arab-Israeli helper (Halabi, 2003). The study found privileged position as being threatened and are expected to try
that Arab-Israeli recipients reported lower self-evaluations when to reaffirm their social advantage through increased efforts to
they were helped by Jewish-Israelis (the dominant group in Israeli provide dependency-oriented help to the low-status group. Un-
society) than when they were helped by Arab-Israelis (the less der these conditions, members of low-status groups—which
dominant group). Taken together, these studies suggest two impli- should be motivated to gain equal status—are expected to react
cations. First, it seems that when there is a salient distinction negatively to the receipt of dependency-oriented help from the
between social groups, as is the case for African Americans and high-status group and be willing to seek such help only when it
European Americans in the United States and Arabs and Jews in is autonomy-oriented.
Israel, interpersonal helping encounters between members of these
groups tend to be experienced as an intergroup interaction. In The Role of Ingroup Identification
support of this, Suleiman (2004) noted that in Israeli society,
interactions between Israeli Arabs and Israeli Jews are perceived The model of intergroup helping does not account for the effects
by participants as intergroup encounters even when they take the of individual variation between group members. Yet, research on
form of interpersonal dialogue (Suleiman, 2004). Second, these social identity shows that not all ingroup members respond to
studies may be interpreted to indicate that because dependency on threats to ingroup identity the same way. These responses are
the high-status outgroup reinforces its dominant position, help affected by individuals’ identification with their group. Mem-
from a member of the dominant outgroup is threatening to recip- bers who identify strongly with the ingroup are more defensive
ients from the low-status group. This conclusion is echoed in when ingroup identity is threatened. They express stronger
discussions that note that being on the receiving end of affirmative identification with the “threatened” ingroup, more ingroup fa-
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 99

Figure 1. Intergroup helping relations as affected by perceived legitimacy and stability of power relations
between groups. From “Inter-group helping relations as power relations: Maintaining or challenging social
dominance between groups through helping,” by A. Nadler, 2002, Journal of Social Issues, 58, 487–502.
Copyright 2002 by Blackwell Publishing. Reprinted with permission.

voritism, and increased stereotyping against the outgroup, group members to help from the high-status outgroup. The
whereas low identifiers lower their identification with the in- fourth experiment used a different real group context (two
group (Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1999). Also, threat to social competing high schools) to examine help-seeking behavior as
identity increases the perceptions, particularly among those affected by the perceived stability of intergroup status relations,
who identify highly with their group (Doosje, Spears, Ellemers, degree of ingroup identification, and the autonomy- versus
& Koomen, 1999), of the homogeneity of members of the dependency-oriented nature of help.
ingroup and of the outgroup (Rothgerber, 1997). Applied to the Our main predictions rest on our theorizing that when status
present context of intergroup helping, we expect the reluctance relations are perceived as relatively unstable, dependence on the
of low-status group members to receive or seek dependency- high-status outgroup is inconsistent with group members’ quest
oriented help from the high-status group to be more character- for equality and results in a threat to social identity. This threat
istic of high ingroup identifiers than low identifiers. We do not should be expressed in relatively low affect, drive group mem-
expect similar differences when help is autonomy-oriented. bers to positively distinguish the ingroup by discriminating
against and devaluing the outgroup, and perceive the ingroup
and the outgroup as more homogeneous (Studies 1 and 2).
PRESENT RESEARCH AND HYPOTHESES These reactions to help will be more characteristic of high than
The present studies focused on the reactions and behaviors of low ingroup identifiers (Study 3). Finally, high identifiers are
low-status group members to offers of help from the high-status expected to be the least willing to seek needed help from the
group as affected by the perceived stability of intergroup status high-status outgroup when status relations are perceived as
relations, their degree of ingroup identification, and the unstable and dependency-oriented help is offered (Study 4).
dependency- or autonomy-oriented nature of help. The first
study was a minimal group experiment and assessed the link STUDY 1
between the perceived stability of status relations and reactions
to receiving help from a high-status group. The second exper- Method
iment examined the same link with real groups (i.e., Israeli Jews
and Israeli Arabs). The third experiment used the same Arab-
Participants and Design
Israeli and Jewish-Israeli intergroup context to examine the The first experiment used the minimal group paradigm and consisted of
effects of ingroup identification on the reactions of low-status a 2 (help vs. no help) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status relations) between-
100 NADLER AND HALABI

participants experimental design. Sixty-seven Israeli undergraduate stu- interaction. Following this, participants were fully debriefed and the ex-
dents ages 19 to 24 years (44 female and 23 male, equally distributed periment ended.1
across the four experimental cells) participated in the experiment.
Manipulation Checks
Procedure Perceived stability of status relations. Participants were asked to name
the group they belonged to (i.e., global or specific perceivers) and rate the
Participants were seated in individual booths and were told that they degree to which they thought that the differences between global and
were participating in a study of the links between perceptual style, inte- specific perceivers would remain constant throughout the experiment, on a
grative thinking abilities, and social decision making. We informed par- 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all constant) to 7 (very constant).
ticipants that people usually fall into either of two perceptual style cate- Help versus no-help manipulation. At the beginning of the debriefing,
gories, global perceivers or specific perceivers, and that global perceivers participants were asked to report if they had received any information from
have higher integrative abilities. We then had participants take the dot their partner (i.e., the outgroup member) and to describe the nature of this
estimation task (Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1996), which would ostensi- communication.
bly determine which perceptual style they belonged to. On completing the
task, all participants learned that they were specific perceivers (i.e., that
they belonged to the group with lower integrative abilities). Dependent Measures
We assessed recipients’ reactions to receiving help in terms of affect,
behavior (ingroup favoritism), and attitudes (evaluation of the outgroup
Independent Variable Manipulation
and perceived homogeneity).
Stability of status relations. In the stable status condition, participants Affect. Participants were asked to rate “How I feel now” on nine
were told that the designation of individuals as specific or global perceivers bipolar adjectives: good/bad, happy/sad, negative/positive, strong/weak,
tends to remain consistent across multiple administrations of the dot calm/nervous, angry/not angry, satisfied/dissatisfied, secure/insecure, and
estimation task. In the unstable status condition, participants were in- successful/unsuccessful. Because of the high internal consistency between
formed that the distinction between the two groups is somewhat inconsis- items (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .89), ratings were summed to obtain a single
tent and may change from test to test (for a similar manipulation of measure of affect, with higher scores indicating higher affect.
perceived status stability, see Boen & Vanbeselaere, 2000; Turner & Ingroup favoritism. As noted previously, the experiment was presented
Brown, 1978). as assessing the links among perceptual style, integrative thinking abilities,
Help versus no help. Participants were told that in the next part of the and social decision making. It was indicated to participants that as part of
experiment they would work in pairs to complete a task requiring integra- a social decision-making task, they were now asked to divide a hypothet-
tive ability and that each pair would include one specific and one global ical sum of 3,000 New Israeli Shekels (NIS; equivalent to roughly $700)
perceiver; the other pair member was said to be working in another room. between a member of “your group—that is, a specific perceiver—and a
Each participant would be asked to solve 20 anagrams, and participants person who belongs to the group of global perceivers.” Participants were to
were informed that students like themselves could usually solve 12 of these choose one of seven alternatives: three representing ingroup favoritism
20 anagrams in the allotted time. However, they had to solve at least 10 (1,800/1,200 NIS, 1,700/1,300 NIS, and 1,600/1,400 NIS for the ingroup
anagrams to proceed to the next phase of the experiment. To make the and outgroup, respectively), one representing equal division (1,500 NIS to
second phase of the experiment seem more appealing, we told participants each group), and three representing outgroup favoritism (1,200/1,800 NIS,
that those who made it to the next phase would have the chance of winning 1,300/1,700 NIS, and 1,400/1,600 NIS to the ingroup and outgroup, re-
a large monetary prize. spectively; see Federico, 1998, for a similar assessment of ingroup
Following these instructions, the 20 anagrams appeared consecutively on favoritism).
the computer screen. Each anagram was presented for 5 s. On the basis of Evaluation of the outgroup. This was measured on six 7-point bipolar
a pilot test, we predetermined that 8 of the anagrams were easily soluble adjectives. Although the internal consistency for these items was relatively
and the other 12 extremely difficult. At the end of the anagram test, high (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .82), it increased when ratings for the aggressive/
participants learned that they had solved 8 anagrams correctly, whereas the nonaggressive item were deleted (to ␣ ⫽ .89). This, together with previous
outgroup member (i.e., a global perceiver) had solved 14 anagrams cor- findings that reactions to assumptive help from a high-status outgroup are
rectly. At this point a message appeared on the screen stating that to characterized by feelings of hostility toward the helper (Schneider et al.,
compensate for decrements in performance due to time pressure, partici- 1996), suggested that the ratings of the outgroup on aggressiveness repre-
pants would be given 2 more minutes to work on the anagrams they had not sented a relatively different dimension than the overall evaluation. This
been able to solve. We told the participants that because students in past was borne out in a factor analysis, using varimax rotation, which yielded
sessions had asked to communicate with their pair member, they could now two distinct factors: The first, accounting for 58% of the variance, was a
do so by writing a message that the experimenter would deliver to the other general evaluation factor (i.e., ratings on honest/dishonest, high abilities/
participant. The experimenter then left the room and returned, 2 min later, low abilities, successful/unsuccessful, trustworthy/untrustworthy, and de-
with an envelope containing a message from the other participant (the help pendable/not dependable), and the second, accounting for 18% of the
condition). The message stated: “It seems that you’re having some diffi-
culty—let me help.” The note included the solution to 4 of the difficult
1
anagrams. In the no-help condition, participants were exposed to the same Of the participants, 21% (13 out of 62) wrote back a note to the other
information but did not receive a message from the other participant. participant (i.e., the outgroup member). Six were in the stable status/help
At this point, before they could write a message of their own, partici- cell, 3 in the unstable status/help cell, and 4 in the stable status/no-help cell.
pants were asked to respond to a number of questions. These included the This small number does not allow for statistical analyses, yet the contents
dependent measures (measure of affect, ingroup favoritism, evaluation of of the messages are revealing and are consistent with our theoretical logic.
the outgroup, and perceived homogeneity of the ingroup and outgroup). All 6 messages in the stable status/help cell consisted of hopeful expecta-
Participants were asked “how you experienced participation in the exper- tions for future cooperation, words of thanks, and the like. The 4 messages
iment” by choosing one of three possibilities: “The experiment feels like an in the stable status/no-help condition were all informative requests (e.g.,
interaction between (1) groups, (2) representatives of two groups, or (3) “Did you do well?” and “Was it hard?”). All 3 messages in the unstable
two individual students.” This allowed us to ascertain that participants status/help condition included an element of resentment and were a vari-
perceived the social interaction within the experiment as an intergroup ation on “Thanks, but I can handle it myself.”
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 101

variance, consisted of ratings of the outgroup ␮s aggressiveness (i.e., revealed that in line with our hypothesis, participants in the un-
aggressive/nonaggressive). Higher scores on the first factor denote more stable status/help cell tended to be more discriminatory toward the
favorable evaluation of the outgroup, and higher scores on the aggressive- outgroup than those in the stable status/help cell; means were 3.52
ness item denote more perceived aggressiveness. and 2.81, respectively, F(1, 31) ⫽ 3.70, p ⬍ .07. The comparable
Perceived homogeneity of ingroup and outgroup. Perceived homoge-
difference in the no-help condition was not significant; means
neity was measured by asking participants to indicate the degree to which
were 3.10 and 2.70, respectively, F(1, 32) ⬍ 1.
specific perceivers were similar to each other (ingroup homogeneity) and
the degree to which global perceivers were similar to each other (outgroup Another approach to assessing the experimental hypothesis was
homogeneity). Both scales ranged from 1 (not at all similar to each other) to compare the percentages of ingroup favoritism choices. We
to 7 (very similar to each other; see Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1997, for collapsed the ingroup favoritism index, which assessed the mag-
a similar assessment). nitude of discrimination on a 7-point scale, into the three catego-
ries of ingroup favoritism, equal division, and outgroup favoritism.
Results Because no participant made an outgroup favoritism choice, par-
ticipants’ allocation decisions were compared between the two
All of the participants correctly identified themselves as mem- categories of ingroup favoritism and equal division. As predicted,
bers of the specific-perceivers group. Five of the 67 participants participants in the help/unstable status cell made significantly
perceived the interaction as an interpersonal interaction. They were more ingroup favoritism choices than those in the help/stable
evenly distributed across the four experimental cells (2 in one cell status cell; average percentages were 62.5% and 23.5%, respec-
and 1 in each of the other three cells). The analyses reported here tively, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 33) ⫽ 5.10, p ⬍ .05. The equivalent comparison
are based on the results of all 67 participants.2 Analyses revealed in the no-help condition was not significant; average percentages
no significant main effects or interactions involving gender, and it were 47% and 30%, respectively, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 34) ⫽ 1.12, ns.
was therefore not included as a factor in subsequent analyses in
this research.
Evaluation of the Outgroup
Manipulation Checks A 2 (help vs. no help) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status) ANOVA
Status Manipulation on the general evaluation score indicated that when status relations
were perceived as unstable, the outgroup was evaluated less pos-
Participants in the stable status condition, more than participants itively than when they were perceived as stable; means were 4.30
in the unstable status condition, thought that the differences be- and 5.00, respectively, F(1, 63) ⫽ 11.54, p ⬍ .001. Also, partic-
tween the two groups would remain the same throughout the ipants who had received help rated the outgroup less favorably
experiment; means were 4.60 and 3.50, respectively, F(1, 65) ⫽ than those who had not received help; means were 4.40 and 4.80,
8.07, p ⬍ .01. respectively, F(1, 63) ⫽ 4.70, p ⬍ .05. The Stability ⫻ Help
interaction was not significant, F(1, 63) ⬍ 1.
Help Manipulation A 2 (help vs. no help) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status) ANOVA
on perceived aggressiveness of the outgroup revealed a significant
All participants in the help condition correctly remembered that interaction, F(1, 63) ⫽ 3.70, p ⬍ .05. Participants who had
they had received a communication from their partner and that this received help viewed the outgroup as more aggressive when status
communication contained help. relations were perceived as unstable than when they were per-
ceived as stable; means were 3.60 and 4.50, respectively, t(63) ⫽
Dependent Measures 2.12, p ⬍ .05. The difference between the stable and unstable
status cells in the no-help condition was nonsignificant (means
Measure of Affect
were 4.60 and 4.40, respectively).
A 2 (help vs. no help) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status) analysis
of variance (ANOVA) yielded a main effect for status stability,
Perceived Ingroup and Outgroup Homogeneity
F(1, 63) ⫽ 20.08, p ⬍ .001, which is qualified by a significant
Stability ⫻ Help interaction, F(1, 63) ⫽ 3.80, p ⬍ .05. This A 2 (stable vs. unstable status) ⫻ 2 (help vs. no help) ANOVA
interaction reflected the finding that participants who had received on the item assessing perceived ingroup homogeneity revealed no
help in the unstable status condition had lower ratings of affect significant effects. A similar ANOVA on the perceived homoge-
than those who had received help in the stable status condition; neity of the outgroup revealed a significant Status Stability ⫻ Help
means were 3.20 and 4.58, respectively, t(63) ⫽ 4.12, p ⬍ .001. interaction, F(1, 63) ⫽ 8.27, p ⬍ .005. This interaction derived
The comparable difference in the no-help condition was not sig- from the finding that participants in the unstable status conditions
nificant; means were 4.10 and 4.80, respectively. who had received help from the outgroup perceived the outgroup
as more homogeneous than those who had received help in the
Ingroup Favoritism stable status conditions; means were 4.70 and 3.00, respectively,
t(63) ⫽ 3.77, p ⬍ .001. The comparable difference was not
A 2 (help vs. no help) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status) ANOVA significant in the no-help condition (means were 3.50 and 3.60,
on the measure of ingroup favoritism revealed no significant respectively; see Table 1).
effects. Although the predicted Help ⫻ Stability interaction was
not significant, F(1, 63) ⬍ 1, we tested the a priori hypotheses by
comparing the mean score of ingroup favoritism in the unstable 2
The main analyses with and without these 5 participants revealed
status/help cell to the stable status/help cell. This comparison similar patterns of findings.
102 NADLER AND HALABI

Table 1 the empirical and theoretical tradition of the social identity per-
Means (and Standard Deviations) of Affect Scores, Percentage spective, it limits the generalizability of our findings. Several
of Ingroup Favoritism Choices, Perceived Aggressiveness of authors suggest that there are differences between empirical rela-
Outgroup, and Perceived Outgroup Homogeneity as a Function tionships observed with ad hoc groups in the laboratory and similar
of Perceived Status Stability and Help (N ⫽ 62): Study 1 relationships in studies that examine real groups (Jetten et al.,
1996; Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992).
Experimental condition To address this possibility and replicate the findings, the second
Help No help
experiment assessed the same hypothesis with real groups. It
examined the reactions of Arab-Israeli participants to the receipt of
Dependent measure Unstable Stable Unstable Stable help from an Arab-Israeli or a Jewish-Israeli helper (i.e., lower and
higher status groups in Israeli society, respectively) under varying
Affect 3.20 4.58 4.10 4.80
degrees of perceived stability of intergroup status relations. To
(0.55) (1.01) (0.81) (1.20)
Percentage of extend generalizability, we used the same theoretical logic but
discriminatory choices 62.5% 23.5% 47.0% 30.0% changed the focus of the empirical comparison. Whereas the first
Perceived aggressiveness of 3.60 4.50 4.60 4.40 experiment focused on a comparison between the help and no-help
outgroup (1.20) (0.99) (0.99) (1.50) conditions, the second experiment focuses on comparing reactions
Perceived outgroup 4.70 3.00 3.50 3.60
homogeneity (1.06) (1.50) (1.06) (1.41) to receiving help from a high-status outgroup versus help from an
ingroup helper under varying levels of perceived stability of status
Note. Lower scores on the measure of perceived aggressiveness denote relations. We predict that consistent with the theoretical model
greater level of perceived aggressiveness. (Nadler, 2002) and the findings of the first experiment, Arab-
Israelis’ social identity will be most threatened when they receive
help from a Jewish-Israeli helper and status relations are perceived
Discussion
as unstable. We expect this high threat to be reflected in depressed
The results of the first experiment support the central hypothe- affect and to result in high motivation for positive ingroup distinc-
sis. When the status hierarchy was perceived as relatively stable, tiveness, which will be expressed by increased ingroup favoritism
the receipt of help from the high-status outgroup did not influence and devaluation of the outgroup.
recipients’ affect, ingroup favoritism, and perceptions of the out-
group. Yet when the status hierarchy was perceived as unstable, STUDY 2
being helped by a member of the high-status outgroup led recip-
ients to feel worse. This relatively negative affect may reflect the Method
greater threat to social identity under these conditions, leading to
efforts for positive ingroup distinctiveness (expressed in more Participants and Design
discrimination toward the outgroup in the unstable/help cell than Participants were 71 Arab-Israeli high school students in a school in
the stable/help cell). The parallel comparison in the no-help con- northern Israel, ages 16 and 17. The experiment was conducted on school
dition was not significant. The findings for perceived aggressive- premises and consisted of a 2 (Arab-Israeli vs. Jewish-Israeli helper) ⫻ 2
ness indicate that the outgroup was perceived as most aggressive (stable vs. unstable status hierarchy) between-participants design. The
by participants who had perceived status relations as unstable and proportion of male to female participants in each of the experimental cells
had received help from the high-status outgroup. This finding is was the same.
conceptually important and suggests that when help thwarted the
low-status group members’ motivation for equality (i.e., status Procedure
relations were perceived as unstable), recipients viewed the helper
as “forcing” his or her generosity on them. Finally, consistent with Participants took part in the experiment in small groups of 5– 6 individ-
research that threat to social identity leads to viewing the source of uals who were seated in individual booths. The experiment was portrayed
as an assessment of the validity of the Israeli psychometric tests, which are
threat as more homogeneous (Doosje et al., 1999), participants in
used as entrance examinations for institutions of higher learning in Israel.
the unstable status condition who had received help viewed the The test was said to include an assessment of verbal, quantitative, and
outgroup as more homogeneous than those who had not received social skills. The research was said to be sponsored by the National Testing
such help. Center in Israel and conducted nationwide. After making these short
The conclusions from the first experiment are limited by the introductory comments, the experimenter excused himself, saying that he
nature of the help that was studied and the type of groups that were must attend a prescheduled meeting with the school’s principal and that his
used. Regarding the nature of help, the participants received help coworker would administer the study.
without having asked for it (i.e., assumptive help; Schneider et al.,
1996). This raises the possibility that help from a high-status group Independent Variable Manipulations
when status relations are perceived as unstable poses a threat to the
recipient’s social identity only when it is imposed by the high- Stability of status relations. Participants were asked to read an infor-
mation page on psychometric testing, which was presented as part of the
status helper. We address this issue in the fourth experiment and
measurement of verbal skills. The information page ended with a statement
discuss it more extensively in the General Discussion section. on the achievements of different subgroups in Israeli society on psycho-
Concerning the type of groups used, this experiment used ad metric tests and noted that the average scores of Arab-Israeli students were
hoc, experimentally created groups. While this reliance on the lower than the average scores of Jewish-Israelis. In the stable status
minimal group paradigm increases confidence in the internal va- condition, the text stated that this gap had remained constant over the years,
lidity of the observed phenomena and places this research within and in the unstable status condition, the text stated that the gap was
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 103

consistently narrowing. Because it is commonly known that members of sociology student who was investigating how different groups in Israel
disadvantaged groups within Israeli society, including many Israeli Arabs, perceived each other. They all agreed and were asked to evaluate Israeli
score lower on these tests than members of advantaged groups, this Jews on seven 7-point bipolar adjective scales: wise/stupid, honest/dishon-
information did not constitute a deception. est, good/bad, devious/trustworthy, truthful/deceitful, egoistic/altruist, and
Participants were then asked to answer a few questions ostensibly materialistic/nonmaterialistic. The average of these items was used as an
assessing their understanding of the material they had just read. One of index of outgroup evaluation (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .91).
these questions comprised the manipulation check on the stability of status
relations.
Group affiliation of helper and help-giving. The experimenter intro- Results
duced himself, by using a different name, to half of the participants as an
Arab-Israeli and to the other half as a Jewish-Israeli. Participants were
asked to work on a sample of 12 psychometric problems, 6 of which were Manipulation Checks
extremely difficult and 6 of which were readily soluble. Participants were
given 15 min to solve all 12 problems and had been told that students can Perceived Stability of Status Relations
usually do so in about 10 min. Six minutes into their work, the experi-
menter, walking between the booths, discreetly pointed to the correct Participants in the stable status condition perceived the status
answer on 4 difficult problems (i.e., provided assumptive help). Subse- gap between Arabs and Jews in Israel as being more constant over
quently, participants were asked to fill out the dependent measures. The the years than did participants in the unstable status condition;
experimenter was blind to the stability condition to which participants means were 6.05 and 3.30, respectively, t(69) ⫽ 2.26, p ⬍ .001.
belonged.

Manipulation Checks Group Affiliation of Helper


Stability of status relations. Following the status stability manipula-
All of the participants correctly remembered the group affilia-
tion, participants were asked to answer a number of questions on the
material they had just read. One of the questions asked them to rate their tion of the experimenter (i.e., the helper), and 97% (69 partici-
agreement, on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 pants) also indicated that he had helped them.3
(strongly agree), with the statement that “I think that the gaps between
Arab-Israelis and Jewish-Israelis in Israeli society have remained constant
over the years.” Dependent Measures
Group affiliation of helper. Toward the end of the experiment, partic-
ipants were asked to report their impressions of the session they had just Affect
participated in. These included questions asking them for the group affil-
iation of the test administrator and whether or not he had helped them. A 2 (Arab-Israeli vs. Jewish-Israeli helper) ⫻ 2 (stable vs.
unstable status) ANOVA on affect scores revealed a main effect
Dependent Variables for helper’s group affiliation, F(1, 67) ⫽ 5.15, p ⬍ .05, which was
qualified by a two-way interaction, F(1, 67) ⫽ 3.97, p ⬍ .05. The
As in Study 1, here we also assessed affective, behavioral, and attitudinal
reactions (i.e., affect, ingroup favoritism, and outgroup evaluation, respec- interaction was due to the finding that affect scores in the Jewish-
tively) to the receipt of help from the high-status outgroup, as influenced Israeli helper/unstable status condition were significantly lower
by different levels of perceived stability of status relations. than affect scores in the Jewish-Israeli helper/stable status condi-
Affect. Participants were asked to rate their affect on the same nine tion; means were 4.26 and 5.48, respectively, t(67) ⫽ 3.70, p ⬍
7-point bipolar adjectives that were used in Study 1, and the average of .001. The comparable difference in the Arab-Israeli helper condi-
these items was used as an affect score (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .88).
tion was not significant; means were 5.24 and 5.54, respectively,
Ingroup favoritism. This measure was presented as assessing social
decision making. Participants were asked to imagine that they had just
t(67) ⬍ 1.
learned that high schools in their area (i.e., the Haifa–Nazareth region in
northern Israel) needed funds to operate a special educational program. The
schools were arranged in four pairs of schools, each consisting of an Arab Ingroup Favoritism
and a Jewish school (identified by name). Participants were asked to
allocate funds by dividing 1,000 NIS between the two schools in each pair A 2 (Arab-Israeli vs. Jewish-Israeli helper) ⫻ 2 (stable vs.
by choosing one out of seven possible allocation possibilities. Three unstable status) ANOVA revealed a significant two-way interac-
represented ingroup favoritism (50/950 NIS, 200/800 NIS, or 350/650 NIS tion, F(1, 67) ⫽ 7.27, p ⬍ .01. This interaction is due to the finding
to the Jewish-Israeli and the Arab-Israeli school, respectively), one repre- that participants who had received help from a Jewish-Israeli under
sented equal division (500 NIS to each school), and three represented conditions of an unstable status hierarchy displayed greater in-
outgroup favoritism (950/50 NIS, 800/200 NIS, or 650/350 NIS to the
group favoritism than those who had received help from a Jewish-
Jewish-Israeli and Arab-Israeli school, respectively). The correlations be-
tween choices made on each of these four pairs of Arab-Israeli/Jewish-
Israeli under conditions of a stable status hierarchy; means were
Israeli schools were significant and high (ranging from .57 to .77). Ingroup 554.2 and 252.8, respectively, t(67) ⫽ 2.92, p ⬍ .01. When the
favoritism scores were computed by subtracting the average amount allo- helper was an Arab-Israeli, the ingroup favoritism scores were not
cated to the ingroup (an Arab-Israeli school) from the average amount significantly different when status relations were perceived as
allocated to the outgroup (a Jewish-Israeli school). Thus, positive scores
reflect ingroup favoritism and negative scores reflect outgroup favoritism.
3
The higher the score, the greater the amount of ingroup favoritism. The analyses on the ratings of all 71 participants yielded the same
Outgroup evaluation. At the end of the experiment, we asked partic- effects as analyses using only the 69 participants who correctly remem-
ipants if they would be willing to fill out a scale for a separate study by a bered having been helped.
104 NADLER AND HALABI

Table 2
Means (and Standard Deviations) of Affect Scores, Amount of Funds Allocated to the Ingroup,
and Evaluation of the High-Status Outgroup as a Function of Helper’s Group Affiliation and
Status Stability (N ⫽ 71): Study 2

Experimental condition

Jewish-Israeli helper Arab-Israeli helper

Dependent measure Unstable Stable Unstable Stable

Affect 4.26 5.48 5.24 5.54


(0.67) (0.99) (1.30) (0.78)
Amount given to the ingroup 554.20 252.80 295.60 200.00
(334.50) (281.00) (368.30) (234.90)
Evaluation of the outgroup (Jewish-Israelis) 2.75 4.33 4.32 4.55
(0.98) (1.38) (1.51) (0.93)

unstable versus stable; means were 295.6 and 200.0, respectively, was conducted with real groups, and (b) the first study tested the
t(67) ⬍ 1.4 hypothesis by comparing responses between the stable and unsta-
ble status cells in the help and no-help conditions, whereas the
Outgroup Evaluation second study tested the hypothesis by comparing between the
stable and unstable cells in the ingroup and high-status outgroup
A 2 (Arab-Israeli vs. Jewish-Israeli helper) ⫻ 2 (stable vs.
helper conditions. The support for the major experimental hypoth-
unstable status) ANOVA revealed a main effect for the helper’s
esis, given these differences, raises our confidence in the theoret-
group affiliation, F(1, 67) ⫽ 9.47, p ⬍ .01, which is qualified by
ical logic that underlies the phenomena under study. The findings
an interaction between status stability and the helper’s group
affiliation, F(1, 67) ⫽ 5.24, p ⬍ .05. The interaction is due to the of the second experiment, by themselves, are open to the alterna-
finding that the evaluation of the outgroup (i.e., Israeli Jews) was tive interpretation that these discrepancies reflect the difference
lower when status relations were perceived as unstable than when between receiving help from an ingroup versus an outgroup helper,
they were perceived as stable; means were 2.75 and 4.33, respec- irrespective of the outgroup’s status. Yet, the agreement with the
tively, t(67) ⫽ 3.80, p ⬍ .001. The parallel difference between the findings of the first experiment, and the conceptually consistent
stable and unstable status conditions when the helper was an effects of the perceived stability of status relations, makes this
Arab-Israeli was not statistically significant; means were 4.32 and alternative interpretation less plausible.
4.55, respectively, t(67) ⬍ 1 (see Table 2). Reactions to threat to social identity are determined by level of
ingroup identification. In the face of threat to social identity, high
Discussion identifiers attempt to positively distinguish their group from the
source of threat (i.e., the outgroup) more so than low identifiers
The main findings of the second study are consistent with our (Ellemers et al., 1999). Applied to the context of intergroup help-
predictions and the findings of the first study. The greater threat to
ing, this suggests that the threat posed by help from the high-status
social identity was evident on affective, behavioral, and attitudinal
outgroup will be higher for high than low identifiers. High iden-
reactions to receiving help from the high-status outgroup. Partic-
tifiers are expected to report worse affect when receiving help
ipants who had been helped by the high-status outgroup helper
(i.e., a Jewish-Israeli) felt worse under conditions of unstable from a high-status outgroup and to discriminate against and de-
status relations than participants who had received help from the value the source of threat (i.e., the outgroup helper) more so than
high-status outgroup helper under conditions of stable status rela- those whose ingroup identification is relatively low. The third
tions, and worse also than those who had been helped by an study examined this hypothesis.
ingroup helper (i.e., an Arab-Israeli). The depressed affect in the
unstable status/outgroup helper cell is consistent with the sugges-
tion that these conditions present the greatest level of threat to 4
Another index of ingroup favoritism is the value that is derived from
social identity. Congruent with the idea that this threat will result the choice that participants make on the 7-point scale, where 1 represents
in increased efforts to attain positive ingroup distinctiveness, par- extreme ingroup favoritism (i.e., 950 and 50 NIS to the ingroup and
ticipants in this cell displayed a higher level of discrimination outgroup, respectively), 4 represents equality (i.e., 500 NIS to each), and 7
against the outgroup and evaluated it less positively than partici- represents an extreme outgroup allocation choice (i.e., 50 and 950 NIS to
pants who had received help from the high-status outgroup under the ingroup and outgroup, respectively). Decisions on the four allocation
items were internally consistent (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .87) and summed to
the stable status conditions. Variations in the perceived stability of
obtain a single index of ingroup favoritism. A 2 (stable vs. unstable
status relations did not affect the discrimination and evaluation of status) ⫻ 2 (Arab vs. Jewish helper) ANOVA revealed a significant
the outgroup by participants who had been helped by an ingroup interaction, F(1, 67) ⫽ 6.29, p ⬍ .01. The pattern of findings was similar
member (i.e., an Arab-Israeli). to those obtained on the measure of average funds allocated to the ingroup,
There are two important differences between Studies 1 and 2: as reported in the Results section (i.e., the Jewish helper/unstable status cell
(a) The first study was a minimal group experiment and the second had the highest ingroup favoritism score).
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 105

STUDY 3 Dependent Measures


Method Affect
A 2 ⫻ 2 ANOVA on the affect measure revealed a main effect
Participants and Design
for the group affiliation of the helper, F(1, 60) ⫽ 5.66, p ⬍ .05,
Sixty-four high school students from an Arab-Israeli high school in due to the lower affect scores of participants who had been helped
northern Israel, ages 16 and 17, participated in a 2 (high identifiers vs. by a member of the outgroup than by an ingroup helper (means
control) ⫻ 2 (ingroup vs. outgroup helper) between-participants experi- were 4.74 and 5.38, respectively). The two-way interaction failed
mental design. The proportion of male to female participants in each of the to exceed an acceptable level of significance, F(1, 60) ⫽ 1.92, p ⬍
four experimental cells was the same.
.17; however, to assess the a priori hypothesis that only in the high
identification condition would recipients of help from the high-
Procedure status outgroup experience lower affect than recipients of help
The procedure was identical to the one used in Study 2. The experiment
from an ingroup helper, we conducted planned comparisons be-
was presented as an investigation of three dimensions in psychometric tween relevant cell means. Consistent with the hypothesis, partic-
tests: verbal, quantitative, and social skills. The first part was described as ipants in the high identification/Jewish-Israeli helper group had
an assessment of verbal skills and consisted of an ingroup identification lower affect than participants in the high identification/Arab-
manipulation. Israeli helper cell; means were 4.34 and 5.39, respectively, t(60) ⫽
2.70, p ⬍ .01. There was no significant difference between the
Independent Variable Manipulations affect scores in the Arab-Israeli and Jewish-Israeli helper cells in
the control condition; means were 5.36 and 5.14, respectively,
Ingroup identification. In the high identification condition, participants t(60) ⬍ 1.
were asked to read half a page on the contributions of Arab culture to
humankind in medicine, mathematics, and the arts. In the control condition,
participants read a neutral section. Following the reading, participants were Ingroup Favoritism
asked to answer a few questions on the section they had just read. One of A 2 (high identification vs. control) ⫻ 2 (Arab-Israeli vs.
these questions was a check on the ingroup identification manipulation (see Jewish-Israeli helper) between-participants ANOVA revealed a
Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1997, for a similar manipulation).
significant two-way interaction, F(1, 60) ⫽ 4.09, p ⬍ .05. Planned
Group affiliation of helper and help-giving. The experimenter pre-
sented himself to half of the participants as an Israeli Jew and to the other
comparisons between relevant cell means indicate that this inter-
half as an Israeli Arab. Subsequently, and in an identical manner to the action is due to the outcome that high identifiers who had received
procedures in the second experiment, participants began working on the help from the high-status outgroup (i.e., from a Jewish-Israeli
quantitative part of the assessment. The experimenter provided assumptive helper) discriminated against the outgroup more than high identi-
help on 4 out of the 20 sequences of geometric problems that the partici- fiers who had received help from an ingroup helper; means were
pants were working on. 436.0 and 291.0, respectively, t(60) ⫽ 2.06, p ⬍ .05. The compa-
rable difference for participants in the control condition was not
Manipulation Checks significant; means were 230.0 and 318.0, respectively, t(60) ⬍ 1.5

For the manipulation check on ingroup identification, participants were


Outgroup Evaluation
asked to rate their agreement with the statement “I am proud to be an Arab”
on a 7-point scale. The manipulation check for group affiliation of helper A 2 ⫻ 2 ANOVA revealed a similar two-way interaction, F(1,
was identical to Study 2. 60) ⫽ 4.41, p ⬍ .05. Planned comparisons indicate that high
identifiers who received help from the Jewish-Israeli helper rated
Dependent Measures the outgroup lower than did high identifiers who had received help
from an Arab-Israeli helper; means were 3.68 and 4.62, respec-
As in the first two experiments, we again assessed the affective, behav-
tively, t(60) ⫽ 2.02, p ⬍ .05. The parallel difference in the control
ioral, and attitudinal reactions to receiving help from the high-status
condition was not significant; means were 4.55 and 4.20, respec-
outgroup. Measures of affect, ingroup favoritism, and outgroup evaluation
were identical to those used in Study 2. tively, t(60) ⬍ 1 (see Table 3).

Discussion
Results
Study 3 indicates that the threat to social identity inherent in
Manipulation Checks receiving help from the high-status outgroup depends on ingroup
Ingroup Identification
5
Here, too, an index of ingroup favoritism was created by summing the
The difference between the high identification and control con-
choices made on the four allocation items (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .79; i.e., scores
ditions was highly significant, t(62) ⫽ 4.97, p ⬍ .0001 (means
ranging from 1 to 7, where 1 represents the highest level of ingroup
were 6.62 and 4.18, respectively). favoritism). A 2 (high vs. low identification) ⫻ 2 (Jewish vs. Arab helper)
ANOVA on these scores revealed a significant interaction, F(1, 59) ⫽
Helper’s Group Affiliation 3.97, p ⬍ .05. This interaction reflects a similar pattern of findings as those
obtained on the average allocation of funds, as reported in the Results
All of the participants correctly remembered the group affilia- section (i.e., the highest ingroup favoritism score was found in the high
tion of the helper, and 98% reported that he had helped them. identification/Jewish helper cell).
106 NADLER AND HALABI

Table 3
Means (and Standard Deviations) of Affect Scores, Amount of Funds Allocated to the Ingroup,
and Evaluation of the High-Status Outgroup as a Function of Helper’s Group Affiliation and
Ingroup Identification (N ⫽ 64): Study 3

Experimental condition

Jewish-Israeli helper Arab-Israeli helper

High ingroup Low ingroup High ingroup Low ingroup


Dependent measure identification identification identification identification

Affect 4.34 5.14 5.39 5.36


(0.85) (1.32) (1.04) (0.98)
Amount given to the ingroup 435.90 229.70 290.60 318.70
(387.70) (273.10) (202.80) (248.70)
Evaluation of the outgroup 3.68 4.55 4.20 4.62
(Jewish-Israelis) (1.50) (1.38) (1.11) (1.19)

identification. High Arab-Israeli identifiers had lower affect after identification. Thus, it represents a full examination of the predic-
receiving help from the dominant outgroup than after receiving tions of the intergroup helping model (Nadler, 2002), together with
help from an ingroup helper, but affect for participants in the extensions suggested by the findings of Study 3 (i.e., the role of
control condition did not differ in these two cells. This lower affect ingroup identification). This examination was carried out using a
suggests that receiving help from a high-status outgroup is more behavioral measure of willingness to seek help.
threatening for high identifiers than for participants in the control
condition who had not undergone a manipulation to increase Method
ingroup identification. Similar to the findings in the first two
experiments, this greater threat to social identity led to greater Participants and Design
efforts for positive ingroup distinctiveness, which found expres-
sion in more discrimination and devaluation of the source of threat Fifty-six students from a high school in a midsize town in northern Israel
participated in the study. The design was a 2 (high ingroup identification
(i.e., the high-status outgroup). It may be noted that the manipu-
vs. control) ⫻ 2 (stable vs. unstable status relations) between-participants
lation of ingroup identification (i.e., praising Arab contributions to design. Each of the four experimental cells included a similar number of
global art and culture) might be construed as having led to per- male and female students. The dependent measures consisted of the fre-
ceptions of higher ingroup status. Yet, we maintain that the effects quency that participants chose to (a) not seek help, (b) seek dependency-
are attributable instead to increased identification. This interpreta- oriented help, and (c) seek autonomy-oriented help. As in Studies 2 and 3,
tion is supported by the consistency of the findings with the results this experiment also used real groups. However, unlike the previous
of Study 4, which are based on a different manipulation of iden- experiments, which used national group affiliation, the present experiment
tification (i.e., informing high school students that graduates of used high school affiliation as the intergroup context.
their school are committed to their school).
The theoretical model guiding the present research program also Procedure
predicts that low-status group members will avoid seeking help
During the first part of the experiment the experimenter, who had been
from the dominant outgroup when dependency on the high-status
introduced as an employee of the Israeli Ministry of Education, described
group poses a threat to social identity (Nadler, 2002). Taken
the study as an assessment of alternative forms of psychometric testing.
together with the findings of the previous experiments, this model Participants were told that they would be working on tasks assessing verbal
suggests that high identifiers will be most reluctant to seek help abilities and interactive analytical abilities. Following this general intro-
from the high-status outgroup when status relations are perceived duction, participants were told that the study was being carried out in
as unstable. This reluctance is likely to be evident only when the different geographical areas in the country and that their school and another
assistance is dependency-oriented (i.e., a full solution is provided). school had been chosen to represent northern Israel. The other school was
When assistance is autonomy-oriented (i.e., consisting of a partial said to be a prestigious and reputable school in the city of Haifa (i.e., a
solution or hints), seeking help from the high-status outgroup is not high-status outgroup).
likely to pose a threat for social identity, and high-identifying
members of low-status groups are likely to seek such help even Independent Variables Manipulation
when perceived status relations are unstable. These predictions are
Stability of status relations. In the stable status condition, participants
the focus of the fourth study. learned that a comparative analysis had revealed that over the last 5 years
their school’s overall performance on various criteria was consistently
STUDY 4 lower than that of the higher status school (e.g., as measured by admission
to selective university programs). In the unstable status condition, partic-
The fourth experiment examined the willingness of low-status ipants were told that over the last 5 years the gap between their school and
group members to seek autonomy- or dependency-oriented help the high-status school had grown consistently narrower.
from a high-status outgroup as a function of the interaction be- Ingroup identification. Following the manipulation of status stability,
tween (a) perceived stability of status relations and (b) ingroup the manipulation of ingroup identification was introduced. In the guise of
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 107

taking the verbal part of the psychometric test, participants were asked to Ingroup Identification
read a short article that they would later be questioned on as part of the
“verbal abilities” section of the psychometric test. In the high ingroup Participants in the high identification condition identified with
identification condition, the short article recounted the history of their high their group more than participants in the control condition, F(1,
school, praising the school and noting the commitment that students and 52) ⫽ 14.90, p ⬍ .001 (means were 6.01 and 4.77, respectively).
graduates felt for it. The article was said to have been taken from the local
community newspaper. In the control condition, participants read an article
of similar length that focused on environmental issues. After they had Dependent Measures
finished reading the articles, participants were asked to answer a few
Avoidance of Help-Seeking
questions on what they had just read. This short questionnaire included
checks on the status stability and ingroup identification manipulations. A 2 (stable vs. unstable status) ⫻ 2 (high identification vs.
control) ANOVA on the avoidance of help-seeking scores revealed
Manipulation Checks a significant Stability ⫻ Identification interaction, F(1, 52) ⫽ 6.15,
p ⬍ .01. This indicates that for participants in the control condi-
Stability of status relations. Participants were asked to respond to two tion, scores for avoidance of help-seeking did not differ between
questions on a 7-point scale: the chances that a graduate of their high the stable and unstable status conditions; means were 0.67 and
school and a graduate of the high-status school would be admitted to 0.50, respectively, t(52) ⫽ 0.80, ns, whereas high identifiers ex-
prestigious university departments, and how many years it would be before
hibited significantly higher avoidance when status relations were
the scholastic achievements of the two schools would become relatively
equal. Responses to these two items were significantly correlated (r ⫽
perceived as unstable than when they were perceived as stable;
⫺.55, p ⬍ .001), and ratings were summed to obtain a single index of means were 1.31 and 0.36, respectively, t(52) ⫽ 2.40, p ⬍ .05.
perceived status stability.
Ingroup identification. Participants answered two questions on a Dependent Help-Seeking
7-point scale: the degree to which they had a sense of belonging to their
school and whether they would recommend enrolling at their school to Consistent with our hypothesis, the incidence of help-seeking in
others. Answers to these two questions were highly correlated (r ⫽ .80, the unstable status/high identification condition was zero, render-
p ⬍ .001), and ratings were summed to obtain a single index of ingroup ing the use of ANOVA impossible. To counter this problem, we
identification. conducted two t tests for independent samples in which equal
variance is not assumed; these compared the amount of
Dependent Measures: Avoidance of Help-Seeking, dependency-oriented help sought in the stable versus unstable
status cells, within the high identification and control conditions
Dependency-Oriented Help-Seeking, and Autonomy-
separately. In the control condition, the amount of dependency-
Oriented Help-Seeking
oriented help did not differ between the stable and unstable status
Participants were told that the next part of the study would assess cells; means were 1.20 and 1.43, respectively, t(26.51) ⫽ 0.52, ns.
interactive analytic thinking. Participants were given five index cards, each In the high identification condition, participants in the stable status
of which displayed an arithmetic problem that had to be solved within 60 s. cell sought more dependency-oriented help than those in the un-
Two of the problems were readily soluble and were solved by all partici- stable status cell; means were 1.21 and 0.00, respectively,
pants. The other three cards contained insoluble problems. After it became t(13.00) ⫽ 3.82, p ⬍ .01.
clear that participants could not solve the three problems, and in prepara- Because students had to choose among three alternatives, there
tion for the upcoming “interactive” part of the session, the students were were two degrees of freedom on this measure. We expected
asked to indicate one of three choices for each problem they had been
significant Stability ⫻ Identification interaction on the avoidance
unable to solve: (a) not receiving any assistance from a student from the
other high school, who was said to be working on the same task (i.e.,
index, and a similar interaction, but in an opposite direction, on the
avoidance of seeking help); (b) receiving the solution to the unsolved dependent help-seeking index. We expected, and found, that high
problem from the other high school student (i.e., seeking dependency- identifiers will have the highest avoidance scores when status
oriented help); or (c) receiving a hint from the other high school student relations were unstable and lowest dependency help-seeking
that might help them find the solution on their own (i.e., seeking autonomy- scores when relations were unstable. Under these conditions, the
oriented help). The indices for avoidance of help-seeking, dependent ANOVA on the third measure (i.e., autonomy-oriented help-
help-seeking, and autonomous help-seeking consisted of the average num- seeking) could not have produced any significant effects. We
ber of times that participant chose either of these three alternatives. Thus, therefore did not conduct an ANOVA on this index.
scores for either of these options could range from 0 (i.e., never choosing
this option) to 3 (i.e., choosing that option on all three problems).
Discussion

Results The findings of Study 4 support the hypotheses. Participants


were most reluctant to seek help from a member of the high-status
Manipulation Checks outgroup when they had been induced to identify with the ingroup
and viewed intergroup status relations as unstable. It is important
Perceived Stability of Status Relations to note that this reluctance was evident only in the case of
dependency-oriented help. In fact, not a single participant in the
Participants in the stable status group perceived the gap between unstable status/high identification condition sought dependency-
the ingroup and the outgroup as more stable than did participants oriented help. We interpret this to reflect that under these condi-
in the unstable status condition, F(1, 52) ⫽ 9.29, p ⬍ .01 (means tions, seeking help conflicted with the motivation of high identi-
were 3.11 and 2.04, respectively). fiers to elevate the status of their ingroup and attain equality with
108 NADLER AND HALABI

the outgroup. When assistance was framed as autonomy-oriented, the needs of the low-status group. The low-status group’s recep-
high identifiers sought similar amounts of help from the outgroup tiveness to such help serves as a behavioral acknowledgment of its
as participants in the control condition, regardless of the stability inferior position. This is captured in Mauss’s (1907/1957) state-
of status relations. ment that by accepting gifts without returning them in kind, the
While the first three studies examined segments of the theoret- recipients “become client and subservient” (p. 72). Such social
ical links suggested by the present analysis (i.e., the stability of situations are likely to have characterized relations between racial
power relations in Studies 1 and 2, and ingroup identification in and gender groups in past centuries (e.g., African Americans and
Study 3), the fourth experiment assessed all three conceptual European Americans, males and females) and in societies in which
elements in tandem. It found support for the joint effects of (a) the power structure is relatively rigid (such as tribal societies). It is
structural characteristics of intergroup power relations (i.e., per- interesting to note that such a unidirectional flow of assistance
ceived stability), (b) characteristics of the person in need of help from the strong to the weak is also evident in nature, in which a
(i.e., ingroup identification), and (c) the nature of the help (i.e., less dominant member in the community of birds that was studied
autonomy- or dependency-oriented) on help-seeking. Also, the rarely gives food to the more dominant member of its group
study extends the generalizability of the earlier findings in two (Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997). This idea is consistent with the argument
major aspects. First, it allows for the generalization of the theo- made by Worchel (1984) that helping may be a way for dominant
retical principles from the realm of reactions to help (as in the first groups to maintain their supremacy and with analyses of paternal-
three experiments) to actual help-seeking behavior. Second, while ism in gender and race relations (e.g., Jackman, 1994; Pratto &
Study 1 focused on experimentally created ad hoc groups and Walker, 2001; van der Berghe, 1967). In the same line, Leach,
Studies 2 and 3 on relations between Israeli Arabs and Jews, Study Snider, and Iyer (2002) argued that one of the consequences of
4 examined status differences between different high schools. The feeling secure in one’s privileged social position is benevolence
overall similarity in the pattern of findings across these different toward the disadvantaged. They wrote: “The security of the for-
kind of groups and operationalizations reinforces our confidence tunates’ advantage allows a somewhat benevolent reaction to the
that the phenomenon under study represents a basic phenomenon disadvantaged in the form of pity” (p. 7). In the same way that
of intergroup relations. receptivity to help from the high-status outgroup is a behavioral
affirmation of the disadvantaged position of the low-status group,
GENERAL DISCUSSION refusing help from the advantaged group may signify a challenge
to the existing status quo. As our data indicate, this is more likely
The above four experiments support the hypotheses of the when status relations are perceived as unstable.
intergroup helping model (Nadler, 2002). Taken together, these The empirical support for our hypotheses is underscored by the
studies indicate that members of low-status groups are least recep- differences that existed between the four experiments, as reflected
tive to help from a high-status outgroup when status relations are in the types of groups studied, the status dimensions used, the
perceived as unstable and help is dependency-oriented. Further, comparisons used to assess the main hypotheses, and the depen-
this unwillingness to seek or receive help from the high-status dent measures that were assessed. Regarding the types of groups
group is particularly characteristic of high ingroup identifiers. We studied, the first study was a minimal group experiment, whereas
interpret this to indicate that under the abovementioned conditions, the other three experiments studied real groups. Regarding the
dependency on a high-status helper is inconsistent with the moti- dimension of status, the second and third experiments studied
vation of the low-status group for social equality and that such help low-status groups within a given society (i.e., Arab citizens in
poses a threat to social identity. Israel), whereas the fourth experiment focused on status differ-
Although the overall empirical picture is consistent with this ences between two high schools. The central comparisons across
interpretation, the studies contain some methodological ambigu- the four experiments also varied (i.e., receiving vs. not receiving
ities that require discussion. In the second and fourth experiments, help in Study 1, and reactions to receiving help from a high-status
the perceived instability of status relations was induced by inform- outgroup and ingroup helper in Studies 2 and 3). Furthermore, both
ing participants that the gap between their group and the high- help-seeking behavior and reactions to help were predicted by the
status group was narrowing over time. It is possible that such a same theoretical logic. In closing, it may be noted that future
manipulation could have resulted in the perception of higher research should analyze the mediating role of relevant psycholog-
ingroup status or may have led to greater identification with the ical motivations (e.g., positive ingroup distinctiveness) and emo-
ingroup. Our interpretation that these effects are attributable to the tions (e.g., feeling humiliated by dependency) on the observed
different perceptions of the stability of status relations is reinforced relationships.
on a number of grounds. First, this manipulation is consistent with
previous manipulations of perceived status stability and instability Direct or Indirect Means to Maintain or Challenge
in this context (Boen & Vanbeselaere, 2000; Federico, 1998; Intergroup Status Relations
Turner & Brown, 1978). Second, our manipulation checks showed
that participants perceived the manipulation as intended. Third, the The logic guiding the present research has implications for both
effects of status stability in Studies 2 and 4 are consistent with high-status groups and help-giving behavior. It suggests that high-
those in the first experiment, which was a minimal group experi- status groups may try to retain a jeopardized position of dominance
ment and in which a different manipulation of stability was used by providing dependency-oriented help to the source of threat (i.e.,
(i.e., the likelihood that participants would be assigned to the the low-status group). This leads to an important question: When
low-status group in the future). will a high-status group try to maintain dominance by providing
In social systems in which the power hierarchy is perceived as help, and when will it do so by the more direct means of asserting
stable and legitimate, it is the privileged group’s duty to cater to its superiority? The same question also applies to the low-status
INTERGROUP HELPING RELATIONS 109

group, which at times may directly challenge existing inequality by low-status group: The effects of stability, permeability and individual
confronting the high-status group with a demand for equal power, ability. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 3, 41– 63.
and at other times less directly by refusing dependency on the Bourhis, R. Y., & Gagnon, A. (2003). Social orientations in the minimal
more dominant group. Although the direct and less-direct forms of group paradigm. In R. Brown & S. Gaertner (Eds.), Intergroup processes
maintaining intergroup power relations are not mutually exclusive, (pp. 89 –112). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
the question of when groups are more likely to use one or the other Brickman, P., Rabinowitz, V. C., Karuza, J., Coates, D., Cohn, E., &
Kidder, L. (1982). Models of helping and coping. American Psycholo-
method remains open. One possible determinant is the social and
gist, 37, 368 –384.
normative context within which intergroup relations take place. If,
Doosje, B., Spears, R., Ellemers, N., & Koomen, W. (1999). Perceived
for example, the social context is confrontational (e.g., groups are group variability in intergroup relations: The distinctive role of social
in open conflict), they may be likely to use more explicit and direct identity. European Review of Social Psychology, 10, 41–73.
methods to maintain or challenge existing status relations. If, Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1997). Sticking together or falling
however, the social context discourages open confrontation, apart: In-group identification as a psychological determinant of group
groups may be likely to use less explicit and confrontational means commitment versus individual mobility. Journal of Personality and
to maintain or challenge power relations. Helping relations are Social Psychology, 72, 617– 626.
such means. Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (Eds.). (1999). Social identity:
Context, commitment, content. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Intergroup and Interpersonal Helping Relations Ellemers, N., Wilke, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (1993). Effects of the
legitimacy of low group or individual status on individual and collective
Although the present analysis focused on intergroup helping, we identity enhancement strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
theorize that the same processes apply to the analysis of helping chology, 64, 766 –778.
relations between differentially powerful individuals (e.g., manag- Federico, C. (1998). The interactive effects of social dominance orienta-
ers and employees). Consistent with self-categorization theory, the tion, group status, and perceived stability on favoritism for high-status
analysis of the helping interaction will be informed by variables groups. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 2, 119 –143.
that are relevant to this level of analysis (e.g., ingroup identifica- Halabi, S., (2003). Reactions to receiving assumptive help in or out-group
tion) when interactants’ social identity is salient. However, when member: The moderating effects of task difficulty. Unpublished master’s
thesis, Tel Aviv University, Israel.
the interactants’ individual self is salient, the helping interaction
Jackman, M. R. (1994). The velvet glove: Paternalism and conflict in
may be construed as an interpersonal interaction, requiring con-
gender, class, and race relations. Berkeley: University of California
sideration of variables that are relevant to this level of analysis
Press.
(e.g., self-esteem). In both cases, however, the pattern of helping Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1996). Intergroup norms and
relations depends on the perceived stability and legitimacy of intergroup discrimination: Distinctive self-categorization and social
existing power relations between the parties and the dependency or identity effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
autonomy orientation of the help. 1222–1233.
Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1997). Strength of identifica-
Social Change and Helping Relations tion and intergroup differentiation: The influence of group norms. Eu-
ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 603– 609.
Times of social change give rise to motivational conflicts be- Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1999). Group distinctiveness
tween more and less socially dominant groups, and this may be and in-group discrimination. In N. Ellemers, R. Spears, & B. Doosje
expressed in the framework of helping relations as well. Whereas (Eds.), Social identity (pp. 107–126). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
a stronger party may seek to defend its social advantage by Leach, C. W., Snider, S. L., & Iyer, A. (2002). “Poisoning the conscience
providing dependency-oriented help, the less dominant party may of the fortunate”: The experience of relative advantage and support for
likely be motivated to refuse such help. This may fuel tension social equality. In I. Walker & H. J. Smith (Eds.), Relative deprivation:
between the two parties. In this scenario, one can imagine the more Specification, development and integration (pp. 1–14). New York: Cam-
dominant group feeling baffled and angry at the low-status group’s bridge University Press.
resistance to its generosity, and the less dominant party viewing Lederach, J. P. (1997). Building peace: Sustainable reconciliation in
the generosity of its counterpart as a manipulative effort to retain divided societies. Washington, DC: U.S. Institute of Peace.
Mauss, M. (1954). The gift: Forms and functions of exchange in archaic
social advantage. These processes are described as operating in
societies. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published 1907)
affirmative action programs (Pratkanis & Turner, 1996), and sim-
Mullen, B., Brown, R., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of
ilar barriers have been underscored on the road to peace-building, salience, relevance, and status: An integration. European Journal of
wherein the stronger party’s efforts to assist its former adversary Social Psychology, 22, 103–122.
may be spurned out of hand as mechanisms to retain dominance Mummendey, A., Klink, A., Mielke, R., Wenzel, M., & Blanz, M. (1999).
(Lederach, 1997; Nadler & Saguy, 2004). The fact that helping is Socio-structural characteristics of intergroup relations and identity man-
both an expression of caring and a demonstration of superiority agement strategies: Results from a field study in East Germany. Euro-
makes it an especially effective instrument of dominance in the pean Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 259 –287.
hands of a more advantaged group. As our data show, members of Nadler, A. (1987). Determinants of help seeking behavior: The effects of
low-status groups are sensitive to this danger and under certain helper’s similarity, task centrality and recipient’s self esteem. European
conditions resent such help and are unwilling to seek it. Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 57– 67.
Nadler, A. (1991). Help-seeking behavior: Psychological costs and instru-
mental benefits. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social
References
psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 290 –312). New York: Sage.
Bierhoff, H. (2002). Prosocial behavior. New York: Psychology Press. Nadler, A. (1997). Autonomous and dependent help seeking: Personality
Boen, F., & Vanbeselaere, N. (2000). Responding to membership of a characteristics and the seeking of help. In B. Sarason, I. Sarason, & R. G.
110 NADLER AND HALABI

Pierce (Eds.), Handbook of personality and social support (pp. 258 – stigma and the potential costs of assumptive help. Personality and Social
302). New York: Plenum Press. Psychology Bulletin, 22, 201–209.
Nadler, A. (1998). Relationship, esteem and achievement perspectives on Schroeder, D. A., Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., & Piliavin, J. A. (1995).
autonomous and dependent help seeking. In S. A. Karabenick (Ed.), The psychology of helping and altruism: Problems and puzzles. New
Strategic help seeking: Implications for learning and teaching (pp. York: McGraw-Hill.
61–95). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Steele, C. (1992, April). Race and the schooling of Black Americans.
Nadler, A. (2002). Inter-group helping relations as power relations: Main- Atlantic Monthly, 68 –78.
taining or challenging social dominance between groups through help- Suleiman, R. (2004). The planned meeting as a microcosmos. In R. Halabi
ing. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 487–502. (Ed.), Israeli and Palestinian identities in dialogue (pp. 47– 67). Pisca-
Nadler, A., & Fisher, J. D. (1986). The role of threat to self-esteem and taway, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
perceived control in recipient reactions to aid: Theory development and Tajfel, H. (1978). Interindividual behavior and intergroup behavior. In H.
empirical validation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiations between social groups: Studies in the
social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 81–123). New York: Academic Press. social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 27– 45). New York:
Nadler, A., & Saguy, T. (2004). Trust building and reconciliation between Academic Press.
adversarial groups: A social psychological perspective. In H. Langholtz Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup
& C. E. Stout (Eds.), The psychology of diplomacy (pp. 29 – 46). New conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of
York: Praeger. intergroup relations (pp. 33– 47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Onorato, R. S., & Turner, J. C. (2004). Fluidity in the self-concept: The Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup
shift from personal to social identity. European Journal of Social Psy- behavior. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology
chology, 34, 257–278. of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Turner, J. C., & Brown, R. (1978). Social status, cognitive alternatives and
Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspective. Annual Review of Psychol- intergroup relations. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social
ogy, 56, 365–392. groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp.
Pratkanis, A. R., & Turner, M. E. (1996). The proactive removal of 201–234). London: Academic Press.
discriminatory barriers: Affirmative action as effective help. Journal of Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S.
Social Issues, 52, 111–132. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory.
Pratto, F., & Walker, A. (2001). Dominance in disguise: Power, benefi- Oxford, England: Blackwell.
cence and exploitation in personal relationships. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & Turner, J. C., & Reynolds, K. J. (2001). The social identity perspective in
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The use and abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on inter-group relations: Theories, themes and controversies. In R. Brown
the causes of corruption (pp. 93–112). Washington, DC: Psychology & S. Gaertner (Eds.), Intergroup processes (pp. 133–153). Oxford,
Press. England: Blackwell
Roccas, S. (2003). Identification and status revisited: The moderating role Van der Berghe, P. L. (1967). Race and racism. New York: Wiley.
of self-enhancement and self-transcendence values. Personality and Worchel, S. (1984). The darker side of helping: The social dynamics of
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 726 –736. helping and cooperation. In D. Bar-Tal, J. Karylowski, & J. Reykowski
Rothgerber, H. (1997). External intergroup threat as an antecedent to (Eds.), The development and maintenance of prosocial behavior (pp.
perceptions in ingroup and outgroup homogeneity. Journal of Person- 379 –397). New York: Plenum.
ality and Social Psychology, 73, 1206 –1212. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece
Sachdev, I., & Bourhis, R. (1987). Status differentials and intergroup of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 277–293.
Sachdev, I., & Bourhis, R. (1991). Power and status differentials in
minority and majority group relations. European Journal of Social Received April 26, 2005
Psychology, 21, 1–24. Revision received October 29, 2005
Schneider, M. E., Major, B., Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1996). Social Accepted November 4, 2005 䡲

You might also like