You are on page 1of 11

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

published: 11 June 2019


doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00563

Microstates as Disease and


Progression Markers in Patients With
Mild Cognitive Impairment
Christian Sandøe Musaeus 1* , Malene Schjønning Nielsen 2 and Peter Høgh 2,3
1
Department of Neurology, Danish Dementia Research Centre, Rigshospitalet, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen,
Denmark, 2 Regional Dementia Research Centre, Department of Neurology, Zealand University Hospital, Roskilde, Denmark,
3
Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark

Network dysfunction is well established in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and
has been shown to be present early in the disease. This is especially interesting in
Edited by:
Ashish Raj, patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) since they are more likely to develop
University of California, AD. In EEG, one type of network analysis is microstates where the EEG is divided
San Francisco, United States
into quasi-stable states and these microstates have been linked to networks found
Reviewed by:
Thomas Koenig, with resting state functional MRI. In the current exploratory study, we therefore wanted
University of Bern, Switzerland to explore the changes in microstates in MCI, and AD compared to healthy controls
Scott Edward Counts,
(HC) and whether microstates were able to separate patients with MCI who progressed
Michigan State University,
United States (pMCI) and those who remained stable (sMCI). EEGs were recorded at baseline
*Correspondence: for 17 patients with AD, 27 patients with MCI, and 38 older HC and the patients
Christian Sandøe Musaeus were followed for 3 years. To investigate whole-brain dynamics we extracted different
christian.sandoee.musaeus@
regionh.dk microstate parameters. We found that patients with MCI, and AD had significantly
higher occurrence (p-value = 0.028), and coverage (p-value = 0.010) for microstate
Specialty section: A compared to HC. However, we did not find any significant systematic deviation of the
This article was submitted to
Neurodegeneration, transition probabilities from randomness for any of the groups. No significant differences
a section of the journal were found between pMCI and sMCI but the largest difference in duration was found for
Frontiers in Neuroscience
microstate D. Microstate A has been linked to the temporal lobes in studies combining
Received: 25 February 2019
Accepted: 15 May 2019
EEG and fMRI and the temporal lobes are the most affected by AD pathology in the early
Published: 11 June 2019 stages of the disease. This supports our idea that microstate A may be the first affected
Citation: microstate in early AD. Even though not significant between pMCI and sMCI, Microstate
Musaeus CS, Nielsen MS and D has previously been shown to be associated with both frontal and parietal areas
Høgh P (2019) Microstates as
Disease and Progression Markers as measured with fMRI and may correspond to underlying pathological changes in the
in Patients With Mild Cognitive progression of MCI to AD. However, larger studies are needed to confirm these findings.
Impairment. Front. Neurosci. 13:563.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00563 Keywords: EEG, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer, Alzheimer’s disease, progression, stable, MCI, microstate

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS


Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative Recruitment, Inclusion Criteria, and
disease and patients with AD have shown changes in functional
Subjects
brain networks (Dickerson and Sperling, 2009). Studies have
The whole dataset or parts of the dataset have also been used
even suggested that alterations in networks are present very
for other studies (Engedal et al., 2015; Musaeus et al., 2018a,
early in the disease process (Selkoe, 2002; Cummings, 2004).
2019; Nielsen et al., 2018) including separate presentation of
Patients mild cognitive impairment (MCI), which is thought
results from spectral power analysis (Musaeus et al., 2018b)
of as mild objective cognitive deficits, are associated with
and for coherence and imaginary part of coherency analysis
later development of AD (Petersen et al., 1999; Petersen,
(Musaeus et al., 2019).
2004). While some patients with MCI progress (pMCI)
This prospective cohort study was conducted at two
others remain stable (sMCI) in their disease, which is in
Danish memory clinics at Zealand University Hospital and
large due to different etiological causes as for example
Rigshospitalet, respectively. Patients consecutively referred for
depression or vascular changes. However, for patients with
cognitive evaluation and diagnosed with either MCI or mild AD
MCI due to AD, there is also evidence of fast and slow
and at least a baseline Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
progression (Chui, 1987), which may be due to affection of
score of ≥22 were eligible for inclusion. The patient selection
different brain networks.
was defined using preexisting exclusion criteria: (1) no close
Multiple methods to investigate brain networks have
relatives who wished to participate, (2) if they were participating
been proposed with the most common being fMRI. But
in other intervention studies or (3) if they were suffering from
since network function are thought of as fast processes
other neurological, psychiatric, or other severe disease, (4) if
that changes over time, fMRI may not be able to capture
they received sedative medication due to a potential sedative
these. Electroencephalography (EEG) has a high temporal
effect, and (5) if they had any past or current addictions to
resolution and methods like microstate analysis has been
alcohol or medications.
able to show topographical maps that have been associated
The HC were all volunteers recruited trough public
with resting state networks (Van de Ville et al., 2010;
advertisements at the memory clinics, at local associations
Yuan et al., 2012). Microstates is a technique where the
for elders and through an online recruitment site for trial
multichannel resting-state EEG signal can be divided
subjects. Inclusion criteria were: (1) age between 50 and
into a number of distinct states (Lehmann et al., 1987).
90 years, (2) MMSE score ≥26, (3) ACE ≥85, (4) normal
Although these states occur in a time range of milliseconds
neurological and clinical examination, (5) normal or age-related
(ms), it has been shown that momentary stable spatial
brain atrophy measured on a computed tomography (CT)
patterns that last approximately 80–120 ms before rapidly
scan, (6) normal routine blood tests. Exclusion criteria
transitioning to a different microstate (Khanna et al., 2015).
were: (1) an inability to participate (including impaired
The majority of the studies have clustered the resting
vision or hearing), (2) presence of cognitive symptoms
EEG into four microstate classes, which has been found
including memory complaints, (3) signs of major neurological,
to be the optimal number according to cross-validation
psychiatric or other severe disease, which potentially could
criterion (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995; Koenig et al.,
elicit cognitive impairments including signs of major
2002) and a study found a high test-retest reliability
depression or a geriatric depression scale score >7, (4) be
(Khanna et al., 2014).
pregnant, (5) have undergone general anesthesia, (6) received
Only few studies have investigated alterations in microstates
electroconvulsive therapy in the past 3 months, (7) receive
in patients with AD (Ihl et al., 1993; Dierks et al., 1997;
sedatives, or (8) have any past or current addictions to
Strik et al., 1997; Stevens and Kircher, 1998; Nishida et al.,
alcohol or medications.
2013). Most studies found a shorter duration of the microstates
In total, we included 17 patients with AD, 27 patients with
compared to healthy elderly controls with one study finding
MCI, and 38 HC. The study was reported to and approved by
a longer duration of microstates. However, a more recent
the Danish Data Protection Agency and by the Regional Ethical
study has found no significant changes in either duration or
Committee according to Danish legislation.
occurrence in patients with AD compared to healthy controls
(Nishida et al., 2013). Moreover, none of the studies have
investigated the early changes in microstates by looking at Diagnostic Assessment
patients with MCI or whether microstates are able to differentiate The patients underwent a standardized diagnostic assessment
between pMCI and sMCI. including a full physical and neurological examination, routine
In the current exploratory study, we wanted to investigate blood analysis, brain CT or MRI scan as well as cognitive
the changes in microstates in patients with MCI compared to screening, i.e., MMSE, Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination
both AD, and healthy controls (HC). Furthermore, we wanted (ACE), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and Clinical
to investigate whether microstates can be used to separate Dementia Rating (CDR). Furthermore, as part of the diagnostic
pMCI from sMCI. Lastly, we wanted to investigate whether any assessment patients and relatives underwent NeuroPsychiatric
microstates parameters correlated with either cognitive scores Inventory (NPI), Major Depression Inventory (MDI), Activities
or AD biomarkers. of Daily Living Inventory (ADCS-ADL). The CT and MRI

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

scans were examined by a neuro-radiologist. The majority Fujirebio Europe, Ghent, Belgium]. AD biomarkers analyses
also had a lumbar puncture (except two patients with from both clinics were all carried out at one central laboratory.
MCI and six HC) performed to measure AD biomarkers
(Amyloid-β42 , total tau, and phosphorylated tau), and for Preprocessing of EEG
routine parameter analysis. If diagnostically relevant, the patients The EEG data were imported to MATLAB (Mathworks, v2016a)
also had a neuropsychological evaluation undertaken by a using the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Only
clinical neuropsychologist, but these were individualized for segments with EC were selected either using markers placed
each patient with varying overlap and therefore not included doing recording or from the first 10 min of recording if markers
in the current study. Diagnoses were settled by consensus of were not present. The electrodes were computationally located on
a multidisciplinary team based on all examination results. The the scalp using the dipfit toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011) with
included MCI patients fulfilled the Winblad consensus criteria the standard 10–20 electrode model. The excessive channels were
(Winblad et al., 2004) and AD patients fulfilled the NIA-AA removed, and the data were bandpass filtered from 1 to 70 Hz
criteria (McKhann et al., 2011). using the pop_firws function in MATLAB with a filter order of
At inclusion, all HC underwent the standardized diagnostic 2 and the Kaiser window parameter beta was estimated using a
assessment, which included cognitive tests (ACE, MMSE, DSST), maximum passband ripple of 0.001. Furthermore, the data were
MDI and analysis of CSF was performed on almost all HC. At the bandstop filtered from 45 to 55 Hz using the same settings as
baseline visit all HC were referred for a standardized EEG. The described previously. Afterward, the data were down sampled
EEG recordings were not used in the diagnostic assessment. to 200 Hz. Then, the data were divided into 1 s epochs and the
EEGs were visually inspected and epochs with excessive noise or
Study Design artifacts were removed. Channels with excessive noise, drift, or
The patients were recruited within 6 months after the diagnosis bad connection were interpolated using spherical interpolation.
and all tests were repeated at inclusion. Follow-up visits were The EEG had to have ≤ three electrodes with excessive artifact,
carried out on a yearly basis, with serial cognitive tests, i.e., otherwise the EEG was excluded from the analysis. Afterward, the
MMSE and ACE and the NPI, MDI, ADCS-ADL, and CDR EEGs were re-referenced to average and independent component
scales. Clinical progression of MCI to AD was determined analysis (ICA) was performed using the extended infomax
based on whether the patient clinically fulfilled the NIA-AA algorithm (Lee et al., 1999) for each file and components that
criteria (McKhann et al., 2011). If the patient progressed to contained eye blinks, eye movement, or specific line noise
another diagnosis, they were excluded from the comparison artifacts were removed manually. Lastly, the EEGs were inspected
between pMCI and sMCI. visually again and epoch with excessive noise or artifacts were
The primary investigator performing the tests was blinded for removed. The investigator who performed the preprocessing was
the results of the EEG, imaging and CSF analysis during the study blinded to the diagnosis. Due to excessive artifacts, we excluded
period. This was done for the investigator to be blinded for the the following number of EEGs: two from patients with AD, two
potential presence of underlying AD pathology. from patients with MCI, and one from HC. When comparing
pMCI, and sMCI, one EEG from MCI was excluded due to
clinical progression to vascular dementia.
Electroencephalography Recording
The EEG recordings were performed at the two participating
Microstate Analysis
centers and the EEG recordings were performed using
Before performing the microstate analysis, we first lowpass
NicoletOne EEG Systems (Natus ) with a sampling rate of
R

filtered the data at 20 Hz with the same settings as mentioned


either 500 or 1000 Hz. Nineteen electrodes were positioned
above. Afterward, we concatenated the epochs for each subject,
according to the International 10–20 system. Most EEGs were
i.e., ending up having one continuous EEG file. We performed
recorded with alternating eyes closed (EC) and eyes open periods
the microstates analysis using the Microstate EEGlab Toolbox
for 3 min each but some of the recording only had EC segments.
(Poulsen et al., 2018). Here, we first extracted the global field
The participants were alerted if they became visibly drowsy, since
power (GFP) peaks and the settings were a minimum peak
drowsiness influences recording. The neurophysiology assistant
distance of 10 ms, the number of GFP peaks per subject that enter
recording the EEG made marks in the EEG when the participant
the segmentation was set at 1000, and GFP peaks that exceeded
closed and open their eyes. After the recording, the files were
2 times the standard deviation of the GFPs of all maps were
exported as raw EEGs without any filtering.
excluded. All the GFP peaks from all subjects were aggregated
into one file before segmentation with the goal to maximizing
Collection and Analysis of Cerebrospinal the similarity between the microstates they would be assigned
Fluid to. For segmentation, we used the modified K-means algorithm
The lumbar puncture was performed between the L3/L4 or L4/L5 since it ignores the polarity of the EEG topography (Lehmann,
intervertebral space and the CSF was collected in polypropylene 1971; Wackermann et al., 1993; Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995).
tubes. Analysis of the CSF included routine parameters and Here, we predefined the number of microstates as four, which
the core AD biomarkers, i.e., Aβ42 , T-tau, and P-tau. The AD previously has been reported as the most common (Khanna et al.,
biomarkers were quantified with sandwich ELISAs [INNOTEST 2014) and reproduceable (Khanna et al., 2015). The number of
amyloid-β42 , hTau, and Phospho-Tau (181P), respectively; repetitions were set at 50 and maximum number of iterations

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

Statistics
MATLAB (vR2016a) was used for all statistical analyses. When
comparing demographics, number of epochs, average GEV
and cognitive scores for AD, MCI, and HC, we performed
one-way ANOVAs. Independent t-tests were used to compare
baseline cognitive scores between pMCI and sMCI. Since the
FIGURE 1 | The global maps that were calculated based the aggregated microstate results (duration, occurrence, coverage, and syntax)
dataset from all participants and were back-fitted to each of the EEG were non-normally distributed, we log-transformed the data.
recordings. The labels (A–D) are according to the previous literature in the Afterward, we performed an ANCOVA with age, gender,
field.
education, and current medication (see Table 1) as covariates.
If we found a significant difference (p-value < 0.05), we
performed independent t-tests (without covariates) between AD
were set at 1000. The global maps (see Figure 1) were then vs. HC, MCI vs. HC, and AD vs. MCI. For the microstate
back-fitted to each of the EEG files by labeling each of EEG results from baseline EEG between pMCI vs. sMCI we used an
segments with the class of microstates it is most familiar. Since ANCOVA with the same covariate as mentioned above. The
resting state EEG is noisy, it happens that consecutive time division of the baseline EEGs into either pMCI and sMCI was
frames are labeled different by change. To avoid this, we rejected determined on progression after 2nd year follow-up. We used
microstate segments shorter than 30 ms. The labels of time frames the data before long-transformation for display in the tables.
in small segments were changed to the next most likely microstate For the post hoc analyses between microstate features, we also
class, as measured by global map dissimilarity (Poulsen et al., calculated effect size measured with Cohen’s d, which is defined
2018). After back-fitting the global maps, we calculated global as the difference between two means divided by the standard
explained variance (GEV), duration, occurrence, coverage, and deviation of the data.
the syntax for EEG files. Furthermore, we performed Spearman’s correlation using
As a post hoc examination of the transition probabilities, the values from significant differences between AD, MCI, and
we performed the same analysis as previously described in HC (coverage, and occurrence for microstate A). We chose to
detail (Lehmann et al., 2005; Nishida et al., 2013). In short, we correlate these values with amyloid, total tau, phosphorylated
calculated the observed transitions based on all transitions and tau, MMSE, and ACE.
then the expected transitions based on the occurrence of the
microstates for each subject separately. Afterward, these values
(4 × 4 − 4 = 12) were averaged across subjects for each group, RESULTS
and the difference was assessed using the chi-square distance. To
statistically test the difference, we performed a permutation test Demographics, Cognitive Tests, and EEG
with 5000 repetitions where the labels “expected” and “observed” Length
were randomly assigned to the subjects’ sets of the 12 transition Characterization of the patients including cognitive test scores
probabilities, and the chi-square distance was computed. The and EEG length is shown in Table 1. For the performance on
underlying hypothesis of this test was that if transitions from cognitive tests for each visit see Table 2. For the comparison
one state into the next occurred randomly, observed transition between demographics, baseline cognitive scores, and CSF
values would be proportional to the relative occurrence of the biomarkers for pMCI and sMCI see Table 3. Flow diagram of the
microstate classes. included patients is illustrated in Figure 2.

TABLE 1 | Table showing the characteristics of the participants included in the analysis.

HC (n = 37) AD (n = 15) MCI (n = 25) p-value

Mean age (SD), years 65.7 (6.9) 70.1 (7.8) 71.4 (6.0) 0.006
Female gender, n 17 8 6 0.119
Education, years (SD) 12.7 (3.6) 12.1 (4.0) 10.6 (3.4) 0.105
MMSE, mean (SD) 29.1 (1.0) 26.3 (3.2) 27.6 (1.5) 0.001∗
Antidepressants 1 1 4 0.161
Cholinesterase Inhibitors 0 8 1 0.001∗
Pain killers 2 0 2 0.553
CSF amyloid, mean (SD) 997.5 (320.2) 550.7 (141.2) 782.3 (319.8) 0.001∗
CSF total tau, mean (SD) 303.3 (144.7) 618.4 (186.0) 419.6 (173.9) 0.001∗
CSF phosphorylated tau, mean (SD) 68.5 (103.4) 93.0 (33.3) 59.4 (21.5) 0.384
EEG length, mean seconds (SD) 177.5 (62.1) 147.1 (19.6) 153.6 (44.6) 0.078

HC, healthy controls; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; SD, standard deviation; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid.
∗ Indicates significant p-value (< 0.05).

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

TABLE 2 | The cognitive scores, number of participants that dropped out, and number of patients with MCI that progressed doing follow-up for year 2.

Baseline 2nd year follow-up t-value p-value

HC Dropout/total (n) 0 1/37


Progression/no-progression NR NR
MMSE, mean (SD) 29.08 (0.98) 29.36 (0.83) −1.312 0.194
ACE, mean (SD) 94.70 (3.28) 95.58 (3.32) −1.139 0.259
MDI, mean (SD) 3.62 (2.87) 4.06 (3.30) −0.600 0.551
MCI Dropout/total (n) 0 6/25
Progression/no-progression NR 12/13
MMSE, mean (SD) 27.60 (1.50) 26.00 (3.33) 2.138 0.038∗
ACE, mean (SD) 84.13 (8.17) 79.67 (11.59) 1.464 0.151
MDI, mean (SD) 7.13 (5.91) 10.22 (7.75) −1.450 0.155
NPI, mean (SD) 3.38 (3.49) 5.24 (2.49) −1.844 0.073
ADL, mean (SD) 70.71 (4.84) 66.59 (9.87) 1.544 0.133
AD Dropout/total (n) 0 7/15
Progression/no-progression NR NR
MMSE, mean (SD) 26.27 (3.17) 23.50 (5.53) 1.537 0.139
ACE, mean (SD) 77.60 (12.87) 67.14 (18.85) 1.532 0.141
MDI, mean (SD) 5.67 (4.70) 4.17 (4.62) 0.664 0.515
NPI, mean (SD) 1.5 (1.24) 5.00 (2.45) −4.235 < 0.000∗
ADL, mean (SD) 70.86 (8.16) 67.38 (8.67) 0.942 0.358
Missing values (%) 6.78 26.21

In addition, the percentage of missing values for the cognitive scores can be seen. All cognitive scores have been compared over time using a paired t-test. ∗ Indicates
significant p-value (< 0.05).

TABLE 3 | Demographics, baseline cognitive scores, and CSF results for stable mild cognitive impairment (sMCI) and progressed mild cognitive impairment (pMCI).

Baseline – sMCI Baseline – pMCI p-value


(n = 13) (n = 11)

Mean age (SD), years 72.38 (6.06) 70.27 (6.63) 0.424


Female gender, n 4 2 0.500
Education, years (SD) 10.69 (3.84) 10.55 (3.36) 0.922
CSF amyloid, mean (SD) 820.08 (348.64) 695.75 (309.90) 0.419
CSF total tau, mean (SD) 398.25 (162.10) 461.56 (206.29) 0.440
CSF phosphorylated tau, mean (SD) 60.54 (24.54) 59.89 (19.28) 0.948
MMSE, mean (SD) 27.92 (1.38) 27.09 (1.58) 0.182
ACE, mean (SD) 87.54 (6.08) 79.00 (8.36) 0.010∗
MDI, mean (SD) 8.67 (6.89) 6.00 (4.14) 0.297
NPI, mean (SD) 3.09 (3.96) 3.00 (2.24) 0.952
CDR, mean (SD) 0.50 (0) 0.56 (0.17) 0.281
ADL, mean (SD) 70.60 (6.06) 70.86 (2.73) 0.918

T-tests were performed to compare the two groups for each score separately. ∗ Indicates significant (p-value < 0.05) difference. One patient with MCI showed up during
follow-up to fulfill the criteria for vascular dementia and was not included in the comparison between pMCI and sMCI.

Microstates Results Between HC, MCI, for occurrence we found a significant difference between
and AD AD, and HC (p-value = 0.0395, t-value = 2.1142, Cohen’s
d = 0.6471) and between MCI, and HC (p-value = 0.0411,
The average GEV was not significantly different between HC t-value = 2.0874, Cohen’s d = 0.5404). For coverage,
(mean = 0.54, SD = 0.08), MCI (mean = 0.53, SD = 0.10), and AD we found a significant difference between AD, and HC
(mean = 0.56, SD = 0.03), (p-value = 0.3624, F-value = 1.0290). (p-value = 0.0066, t-value = 2.8359, Cohen’s d = 0.8681) and
See Figure 1 for global maps of the microstates that between MCI, and HC (p-value = 0.0077, t-value = 2.7575,
were used for back-fitting. Between AD, MCI, and HC, we Cohen’s d = 0.7139).
found significantly different occurrence (p-value = 0.0277, For the syntax analysis, we found patients with MCI and AD
F-value = 3.7807, degrees of freedom = 68) and coverage were significantly more likely to transition from microstates
(p-value = 0.0101, F-value = 4.9237, degrees of freedom = 68) C to A, and for AD from and D to A compared to HC
for microstate A, see Table 4. For the post hoc t-test when only looking at the observed transition percentages

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

FIGURE 2 | Flow diagram showing the number of participants recruited and drop-out over 3 years. Figure reproduced from Musaeus et al. (2018b).

for each microstate separately, see Figure 3. However, Correlation


when we performed the permutation test between observed No significant correlations were found between coverage,
and expected percentage of transitions, we did not find and occurrence for microstates A and amyloid, total tau,
any systematic deviation of transition from randomness phosphorylated tau, MMSE, or ACE.
(p-value > 0.05). See Table 6 for observed and expected
percentage of transitions.
DISCUSSION
Microstates Between pMCI and sMCI
No significant differences (p-value < 0.05) were found In the current exploratory study, we found that patients with
between pMCI and sMCI for duration, occurrence, MCI, and AD compared to HC had significantly higher
or coverage. The largest difference in duration was occurrence and coverage of microstate A. In addition,
found for microstate D between pMCI and sMCI both microstates C and D transitioned significantly more
(see Table 5). to microstate A in patients with AD compared to HC,
We also performed syntax analysis, but no significant and microstate C transitioned more to microstate A in
differences were found between pMCI and sMCI. MCI compared to HC. However, we did not find evidence

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

p-value
that there was any systematic deviation of transition

0.578
0.307
0.01
0.26
probabilities from randomness for any of the groups.
Between pMCI, and sMCI, we did not find any significant
differences but the largest difference in duration was

0.24 (0.04)
0.21 (0.07)

0.29 (0.08)
0.26 (0.8)
found for microstate D. Lastly, no correlations were

AD
found between microstate A and either biomarkers or

TABLE 4 | Table showing the mean, standard deviation (SD), and p-value for comparisons between the three groups (HC, MCI, and AD) for microstates A–D for duration, occurrence, and coverage.
neuropsychological tests.

Coverage
Previous studies have investigated patients with AD (Ihl

0.23 (0.06)
0.24 (0.08)
0.25 (0.15)
0.28 (0.09)
et al., 1993; Dierks et al., 1997; Strik et al., 1997; Stevens and

MCI
Kircher, 1998; Nishida et al., 2013) but the majority found a
shorter duration of the microstates in patients suffering from
AD (Dierks et al., 1997; Strik et al., 1997; Stevens and Kircher,
1998) compared to healthy older controls. In the early studies

0.19 (0.06)
0.21 (0.09)
0.27 (0.11)
0.34 (0.13)
(Ihl et al., 1993; Dierks et al., 1997; Strik et al., 1997; Stevens

HC
and Kircher, 1998) adaptive segmentation was used, which may
have given rise to different results. However, a more recent study
using clustering analysis (Nishida et al., 2013) did not find any

p-value

0.028
0.422
0.256
0.304
significant differences between patients with AD and HC, which
could be due to low sample size or as previously suggested
temporal disorganization in patients with AD (Koenig et al.,
2005; Nishida et al., 2013). However, in the current study we

2.83 (0.45)
2.62 (0.68)
3.01 (0.56)
3.09 (0.41)
found longer duration in both patients with MCI, and AD with

AD
significant increased occurrence and coverage in microstate A

Occurrence
compared to HC, see Table 4. The main reason for our finding
compared to the recent study not finding any significant results

2.78 (0.55)
2.81 (0.67)
2.61 (0.59)
2.93 (0.66)
between AD, and HC (Nishida et al., 2013) may be differences

MCI
in methods. Here, we compiled GFP peaks for all participants
before segmentation or it could be due to differences in the
recruitment. However, significantly increased occurrence and

2.45 (0.65)
2.56 (0.87)
2.98 (0.76)
3.16 (0.63)
coverage for microstate A has not previously been reported

HC
and the underlying reason may be underlying AD pathology
in the temporal lobes, which has been shown in pathological
studies using Braak staging (Braak and Braak, 1991; Thal et al.,
2002) and studies using follow-up data on the deposition of
p-value

0.059
0.082
0.725
0.55
amyloid with PiB-PET (Okello et al., 2009; Villemagne et al.,
2011). The increased coverage and occurrence of microstate A
may therefore be due to underlying pathological changes in the
83.19 (11.05)

87.12 (16.95)
94.32 (22.69)
78.62 (9.15)

temporal lobes and thereby disruption of the underlying neuronal


networks. Interestingly, no statistically significant differences
AD

were found between MCI, and AD, which may indicate that the
majority of the included patients with MCI had an underlying
Duration

AD pathology. Supportive of this assumption, is the observation


82.23 (11.05)
85.45 (11.48)
90.76 (42.38)
92.26 (16.32)

that more than half of the MCI cohort progressed significantly


MCI

clinically over 2 years follow-up. However, we did not find


any significant changes in the microstate B between AD, MCI,
and HC, which may be due the topographical map did not
involve as large a region of the temporal region as microstate
75.88 (10.05)
78.10 (12.42)
89.08 (23.19)
106.02 (41.73)

A. However, in a previous paper using the same data for


spectral power analysis, we found that the changes are more
HC

pronounced on the left side (Musaeus et al., 2018b). This effect


may correspond to previous MR studies showing atrophy being
more pronounced on the left side (Killiany et al., 2000; Baron
Microstate C, (SD)
Microstate D, (SD)
Microstate B, (SD)
Microstate A, (SD)

et al., 2001) or a previous study showing more pronounced


hypometabolism in the left temporal region using SPECT (Hogh
et al., 2004). On the other hand, this may also simply be due
to low sample size and thereby individual differences affecting
the results. For the syntax analysis, we found that patients

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

FIGURE 3 | Significant results for the syntax analysis between HC, MCI, and AD. The first column is for HC, second for MCI, and third for AD. The values represent
the percentage of times when microstates C, and D transitioned to the other microstates. The figure shows that both microstates C, and D were more likely to
transition to microstate A in patients with AD and in patients with MCI microstate C transitioned significantly more to microstates A.

TABLE 5 | Table showing the mean, standard deviation (SD), and p-value for comparisons between pMCI, and sMCI for microstates A-D for duration, occurrence,
and coverage.

Duration Occurrence Coverage

pMCI sMCI p-value pMCI sMCI p-value pMCI sMCI p-value

Microstate A, (SD) 75.32 (8.98) 76.22 (10.48) 0.594 2.41 (0.67) 2.47 (0.53) 0.587 0.18 (0.06) 0.19 (0.05) 0.547
Microstate B, (SD) 79.32 (16.66) 76.29 (8.49) 0.296 2.41 (0.96) 2.62 (0.84) 0.654 0.20 (0.12) 0.20 (0.08) 0.463
Microstate C, (SD) 81.59 (18.26) 95.06 (22.59) 0.673 2.61 (0.74) 3.31 (0.48) 0.708 0.22 (0.11) 0.31 (0.07) 0.655
Microstate D, (SD) 116.90 (33.91) 93.08 (26.15) 0.235 3.30 (0.58) 3.15 (0.75) 0.292 0.39 (0.14) 0.30 (0.10) 0.238

with both AD, and MCI were more likely to transition from studies (Britz et al., 2010; Van de Ville et al., 2010; Yuan
microstate C to A, and AD from D to A, see Figure 3. et al., 2012). One study has associated microstate A with
However, we did not find any systematic deviation of transition BOLD activations in the superior and middle temporal gyri
probabilities from randomness, which strongly indicates that the as well as the left middle frontal gyrus (Britz et al., 2010).
transitions were in large part due to the increased occurrence Other studies extracted 13 (Yuan et al., 2012) and 10
of microstate A. (Musso et al., 2010) microstates, respectively, and any direct
The microstate classes have also been associated with BOLD comparisons were therefore very difficult. By visual inspection,
signal and resting state networks obtained with fMRI in multiple it is possible that microstate A may correspond to microstate

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

TABLE 6 | Observed and expected percentage of transitions.

HC MCI AD

Observed Expected Observed Expected Observed Expected

D to C 0.118 0.111 0.084 0.084 0.1 0.096


D to A 0.083 0.088 0.089 0.089 0.094 0.092
D to B 0.087 0.089 0.088 0.089 0.076 0.081
C to D 0.119 0.108 0.085 0.083 0.099 0.095
C to A 0.072 0.079 0.078 0.079 0.082 0.086
C to B 0.076 0.081 0.077 0.078 0.08 0.079
A to D 0.084 0.08 0.088 0.087 0.092 0.088
A to C 0.074 0.074 0.077 0.077 0.084 0.084
A to B 0.062 0.066 0.085 0.085 0.069 0.073
B to D 0.086 0.082 0.089 0.087 0.079 0.077
B to C 0.075 0.076 0.079 0.078 0.076 0.076
B to A 0.065 0.067 0.082 0.085 0.07 0.072

5 and 13 in a previous publication (Yuan et al., 2012) not correct for multiple comparisons due to the exploratory
and thereby be associated with the default mode network. nature of the study. However, these changes suggest that
These findings suggest that microstate A is associated with larger studies will be able to use microstates as a classifier
temporal connectivity and may even be related to the of disease even at an early stage. In addition, the follow-
default mode network. up time was short and according to previous studies, annual
In patients with pMCI and sMCI, we did not find clinical progression rate is 15% (Petersen et al., 1999; Saxton
any significant changes but the largest difference in et al., 2009), which means that only 30% of the patients
duration was found for microstate D, see Table 5. with MCI should have progressed to AD. However, we found
Microstate D has previously been associated with that 48% progressed, which may in part be due to the
BOLD changes in the frontal and parietal areas patients with MCI being at a more advanced stage of the
measured with fMRI (Britz et al., 2010) and may disease at inclusion. Furthermore, we included patients receiving
reflect underlying pathological changes in patients with medication in the analysis, which may have affected the EEG.
MCI who progress to AD. However, larger studies are Nevertheless, our findings in this small pilot study with affected
needed to test whether microstate D is in fact different microstate A in patients with MCI and possible affection of
between pMCI and sMCI. microstate D in the transition from MCI to AD may be able to
Previous studies have found an inverse correlation between guide larger studies.
microstate lifespan and degree of cognitive impairment (Dierks
et al., 1997; Strik et al., 1997). In the current study, we
did not find any correlation between occurrence or coverage CONCLUSION
and either biomarkers or neuropsychological tests. This may
be due to the low sample size or the values extracted In the current exploratory study, we found that patients
based on the global maps. Larger studies are needed to with MCI, and AD compared to HC had significantly
investigate whether microstate changes are associated with higher occurrence and coverage of microstate A. The
neuropsychological findings. changes may correspond to the previous literature of
In the current study, we choose to extract four microstates pathological changes in the temporal regions in patients
since this is the most commonly reported and these have with AD and microstate A may correspond to temporal
been shown to be reliable (Khanna et al., 2014). However, regions measured with BOLD fMRI. Furthermore, between
the GEV was not significantly different between the three pMCI, and sMCI, no significant differences were found
groups but was low (average GEV = 54%) compared to but a tendency of a prolonged duration of microstate D in
other studies with most commonly reporting a GEV >70% patients with pMCI was seen. Larger studies are needed to
(Michel and Koenig, 2018). The low GEV may be due confirm these findings.
to either broad filter settings (2–20 Hz) or simply due to
patient data being noisier. In the current analysis, we included
only the first 1000 GFP peaks to the segmentation and DATA AVAILABILITY
thereby avoided problems in terms of more contributions from
larger EEG files. The datasets supporting the conclusions of this manuscript
The study indicates that microstate A could be an early disease will be made available by the authors to any qualified
marker in patients with MCI, but it has some limitations. Firstly, researcher. However, due to regulations, we are not able to
we acknowledge the relatively small sample size and we did share the EEG files.

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

ETHICS STATEMENT experiments. CM conducted the data analyses and drafted


the manuscript. PH, MN, and CM contributed to
This study was carried out in accordance with the revising the manuscript.
recommendations of the Regional Committee on Health
Research Ethics with written informed consent from all subjects.
All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with FUNDING
the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the
Regional Committee on Health Research Ethics. This work was funded by the Velux Foundation.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS


PH, MN, and CM conceived the project idea of We would like to thank the study nurses for all their help
using quantitative EEG. PH and MN conducted the conducting this study.

REFERENCES Koenig, T., Studer, D., Hubl, D., Melie, L., and Strik, W. K. (2005). Brain
connectivity at different time-scales measured with EEG. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
Baron, J. C., Chetelat, G., Desgranges, B., Perchey, G., Landeau, B., de la Sayette, Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1015–1023.
V., et al. (2001). In vivo mapping of gray matter loss with voxel-based Lee, T. W., Girolami, M., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1999). Independent component
morphometry in mild Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage 14, 298–309. doi: 10. analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed subgaussian
1006/nimg.2001.0848 and supergaussian sources. Neural Comput. 11, 417–441. doi: 10.1162/
Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related 089976699300016719
changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/bf00308809 Lehmann, D. (1971). Multichannel topography of human al pha EEG fields.
Britz, J., Van De Ville, D., and Michel, C. M. (2010). BOLD correlates of Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 33, 439–449. doi: 10.1016/0013-
EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. NeuroImage 52, 4694(71)90165-9
1162–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.052 Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Galderisi, S., Herrmann, W. M., Kinoshita, T., Koukkou,
Chui, H. C. (1987). The significance of clinically defined subgroups of Alzheimer’s M., et al. (2005). EEG microstate duration and syntax in acute, medication-
disease. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 24, 57–68. naive, first-episode schizophrenia: a multi-center study. Psychiatry Res. 138,
Cummings, J. L. (2004). Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 56–67. 141–156. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.05.007
Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for Lehmann, D., Ozaki, H., and Pal, I. (1987). EEG alpha map series: brain
analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component micro-states by space-oriented adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003. Neurophysiol. 67, 271–288. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90025-3
10.009 McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr.,
Dickerson, B. C., and Sperling, R. A. (2009). Large-scale functional brain network Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease:
abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease: insights from functional neuroimaging. recommendations from the national institute on aging-alzheimer’s association
Behav. Neurol. 21, 63–75. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2009-0227 workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers
Dierks, T., Jelic, V., Julin, P., Maurer, K., Wahlund, L. O., Almkvist, O., et al. Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
(1997). EEG-microstates in mild memory impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Michel, C. M., and Koenig, T. (2018). EEG microstates as a tool for studying the
possible association with disturbed information processing. J. Neural Transm. temporal dynamics of whole-brain neuronal networks: a review. NeuroImage
104, 483–495. doi: 10.1007/bf01277666 180(Pt B), 577–593. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.062
Engedal, K., Snaedal, J., Hoegh, P., Jelic, V., Bo Andersen, B., Naik, M., et al. Musaeus, C. S., Engedal, K., Hogh, P., Jelic, V., Morup, M., Naik, M., et al. (2018a).
(2015). Quantitative EEG applying the statistical recognition pattern method: EEG theta power is an early marker of cognitive decline in dementia due to
a useful tool in dementia diagnostic workup. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. 40, alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 64, 1359–1371. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180300
1–12. doi: 10.1159/000381016 Musaeus, C. S., Nielsen, M. S., Osterbye, N. N., and Hogh, P. (2018b). Decreased
Hogh, P., Madsen Sjo, N., Gade, A., and Waldemar, G. (2004). Temporal lobe parietal beta power as a sign of disease progression in patients with mild
hypoperfusion in isolated amnesia with slow onset: a single photon emission cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 65, 475–487. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180384
computer tomography study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. 18, 15–23. doi: 10. Musaeus, C. S., Nielsen, M. S., and Hogh, P. (2019). Altered low-frequency EEG
1159/000077730 connectivity in mild cognitive impairment as a sign of clinical progression.
Ihl, R., Dierks, T., Froelich, L., Martin, E. M., and Maurer, K. (1993). Segmentation J. Alzheimers Dis. 68, 947–960. doi: 10.3233/JAD-181081
of the spontaneous EEG in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychobiology Musso, F., Brinkmeyer, J., Mobascher, A., Warbrick, T., and Winterer, G. (2010).
27, 231–236. doi: 10.1159/000118986 Spontaneous brain activity and EEG microstates. A novel EEG/fMRI analysis
Khanna, A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Farzan, F. (2014). Reliability of resting-state approach to explore resting-state networks. NeuroImage 52, 1149–1161.
microstate features in electroencephalography. PLoS One 9:e114163. doi: 10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.093
1371/journal.pone.0114163 Nielsen, M. S., Simonsen, A. H., Siersma, V., Hasselbalch, S. G., and Hoegh, P.
Khanna, A., Pascual-Leone, A., Michel, C. M., and Farzan, F. (2015). Microstates in (2018). The diagnostic and prognostic value of a dual-tasking paradigm in a
resting-state EEG: current status and future directions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. memory clinic. J. Alzheimers Dis. 61, 1189–1199. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161310
49, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.010 Nishida, K., Morishima, Y., Yoshimura, M., Isotani, T., Irisawa, S., Jann, K.,
Killiany, R. J., Gomez-Isla, T., Moss, M., Kikinis, R., Sandor, T., Jolesz, F., et al. (2013). EEG microstates associated with salience and frontoparietal
et al. (2000). Use of structural magnetic resonance imaging to predict who networks in frontotemporal dementia, schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease.
will get Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 47, 430–439. doi: 10.1002/1531- Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1106–1114. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.01.005
8249(200004)47:4<430::aid-ana5>3.3.co;2-9 Okello, A., Koivunen, J., Edison, P., Archer, H. A., Turkheimer, F. E., Nagren, K.,
Koenig, T., Prichep, L., Lehmann, D., Sosa, P. V., Braeker, E., Kleinlogel, H., et al. et al. (2009). Conversion of amyloid positive and negative MCI to AD over
(2002). Millisecond by millisecond, year by year: normative EEG microstates 3 years: an 11C-PIB PET study. Neurology 73, 754–760. doi: 10.1212/WNL.
and developmental stages. NeuroImage 16, 41–48. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1070 0b013e3181b23564

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 10 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563


Musaeus et al. EEG Microstates in MCI

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Van de Ville, D., Britz, J., and Michel, C. M. (2010). EEG microstate sequences in
open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive healthy humans at rest reveal scale-free dynamics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/ 107, 18179–18184. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007841107
2011/156869 Villemagne, V. L., Pike, K. E., Chetelat, G., Ellis, K. A., Mulligan, R. S., Bourgeat,
Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., and Lehmann, D. (1995). Segmentation of P., et al. (2011). Longitudinal assessment of abeta and cognition in aging and
brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation. IEEE Alzheimer disease. Ann. Neurol. 69, 181–192.
Trans. Biomed. Eng. 42, 658–665. doi: 10.1109/10.391164 Wackermann, J., Lehmann, D., Michel, C. M., and Strik, W. K. (1993).
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Int. Med. Adaptive segmentation of spontaneous EEG map series into spatially defined
256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x microstates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 14, 269–283. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(93)
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and 90041-m
Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al.
outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56, 303–308. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus:
Poulsen, A. T., Pedroni, A., Langer, N., and Hansen, L. K. (2018). Microstate report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment.
EEGlab toolbox: an introductory guide. bioRxiv J. Intern. Med. 256, 240–246.
Saxton, J., Snitz, B. E., Lopez, O. L., Ives, D. G., Dunn, L. O., Fitzpatrick, Yuan, H., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Drevets, W. C., and Bodurka, J. (2012).
A., et al. (2009). Functional and cognitive criteria produce different rates of Spatiotemporal dynamics of the brain at rest–exploring EEG microstates as
mild cognitive impairment and conversion to dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. electrophysiological signatures of BOLD resting state networks. NeuroImage 60,
Psychiatry 80, 737–743. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.160705 2062–2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.031
Selkoe, D. J. (2002). Alzheimer’s disease is a synaptic failure. Science 298, 789–791.
Stevens, A., and Kircher, T. (1998). Cognitive decline unlike normal aging is Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
associated with alterations of EEG temporo-spatial characteristics. Eur. Arch. conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 248, 259–266. doi: 10.1007/s004060050047 be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Strik, W. K., Chiaramonti, R., Muscas, G. C., Paganini, M., Mueller, T. J., Fallgatter,
A. J., et al. (1997). Decreased EEG microstate duration and anteriorisation of Copyright © 2019 Musaeus, Nielsen and Høgh. This is an open-access article
the brain electrical fields in mild and moderate dementia of the Alzheimer type. distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
Psychiatry Res. 75, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(97)00054-1 The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
Thal, D. R., Rub, U., Orantes, M., and Braak, H. (2002). Phases of a beta-deposition original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
in the human brain and its relevance for the development of AD. Neurology 58, publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No
1791–1800. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.12.1791 use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 11 June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 563

You might also like