You are on page 1of 4

REVIEW: NEUROSCIENCE

Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of the

Affective Dimension of Pain

Donald D. Price

intensity are likely the result of assurances of

The affective dimension of pain is made up of feelings of unpleasantness and emotions safety or the brevity of the 5-s stimuli. Ratios

associated with future implications, termed secondary affect. Experimental and clin- of affective to sensory ratings of brief exper-

ical studies show serial interactions between pain sensation intensity, pain unpleas- imental pain stimuli (5-s heat and electric

antness, and secondary affect. These pain dimensions and their interactions relate to shock) are systematically less than 1.0,

a central network of brain structures that processes nociceptive information both in whereas those of long-duration pain stimuli

parallel and in series. Spinal pathways to limbic structures and medial thalamic nuclei (ischemia and cold pressor) are 1.0 or greater

provide direct inputs to brain areas involved in affect. Another source is from spinal (5). Thus, systematic differences in ratios of

pathways to somatosensory thalamic and cortical areas and then through a cortico- affective to sensory ratings of nociceptive

limbic pathway. The latter integrates nociceptive input with contextual information stimulus intensity occur as a predicted con-

and memory to provide cognitive mediation of pain affect. Both direct and cortico- sequence of simple factors, such as stimulus

limbic pathways converge on the same anterior cingulate cortical and subcortical duration and presence or absence of assur-

structures whose function may be to establish emotional valence and response


ances. These differences provide evidence for

priorities.
separate dimensions of pain.

Two related experiments provide further

support for the uniqueness of the two pain

npleasant emotional feelings are inte- types of pain), spatial spread of sensation at dimensions and help establish the direction of

U gral components of pain because of


unique sensory qualities and because
these qualities often occur within a context
suprathreshold levels, spatial summation,
and unique sensory qualities, as implied by
words such as stinging, burning, and aching
causation between them ( 6 ) .For both, the left
hands of subjects were immersed in a mod-
erately painful 47OC water bath. Hypnotic
that is threatening, such as during disease or (1, 2). Sensory attributes dispose us to per- suggestions were alternately given for en-
physical trauma. Thus, pain contains both ceive pain as invasive and intrusive for hancing and then decreasing only pain un-
sensory and affective dimensions and is often both the body and consciousness (2). Both pleasantness in the first experiment and for
accompanied by desires to terminate, reduce, neural and psychological processes related enhancing and then decreasing pain sensa-
or escape its presence (1, 2). Part of the to pain-related sensation can be conceived tion intensity in the second. Only pain un-
affective dimension of pain is the moment- as important causal links in the production pleasantness ratings were changed in the
by-moment unpleasantness of pain, made up of pain-related emotional disturbance. The directions suggested in the first experiment,
of emotional feelings that pertain to the persistence of pain enhances unpleasant- whereas both pain intensity and unpleasant-
present or short-term future, such as distress ness over time. ness ratings changed in parallel in the sec-
or fear. Pain unpleasantness is often, although Nociceptive, exteroceptive (e.g., sight and ond. These results establish the direction of
not always, closely linked to the intensity of sound), and interoceptive sensory processes causation-pain sensation is in series with
the painful sensation. Another component of (e.g., startle and increased autonomic re- and is a cause of pain unpleasantness and
pain affect, "secondary pain affect," includes sponses) may provide parallel contributions not vice versa (Fig. 1). These results are
emotional feelings directed toward long-term to pain affect (2). Consistent with Damasio's consistent with studies showing that some
implications of having pain (e.g., "suffer- (3) neurological view of emotion mecha- psychological factors selectively influence
ing"). This review provides evidence for se- nisms, pain unpleasantness reflects the con- pain unpleasantness and others alter pain
rial interactions between sensory, unpleasant- tribution of several sources, including pain unpleasantness in response to changes in
ness, and secondary affective dimensions of sensation, arousal, autonomic, and somato- pain sensation (2, 4).
pain and their underlying neural mechanisms, motor responses, all in relation to meanings Unlike pain unpleasantness, secondary
of the pain and to the context in which pain pain affect is based on more elaborate reflec-
Psychological Mechanisms of presents itself. tion related to that which one remembers or
Pain Affect Psychophysical studies demonstrate that imagines. This involves meanings such as
Multiple factors contribute to pain unpleas- pain sensation and pain unpleasantness rep- perceived interference with one's life, diffi-
antness. Several sensory attributes of pain resent two distinct dimensions of pain that culties of enduring pain over time, and the
dispose unpleasant emotional feelings. The demonstrate reliably different relations to no- implications for the future (2, 4). Pain is
foremost among these is that sensations of ciceptive stimulus intensity and are separate- often experienced not only as a threat to the
pain are often more intense than other types ly influenced by various psychological fac- present state of one's body, comfort, or ac-
of somatic sensations. In addition, pain pre- tors. Psychophysical relations of both pain tivity but also to one's future well-being and
sents characteristics of slow adaptation (i.e., sensation intensity and pain unpleasantness life in general. The perceived implications
persistence), temporal summation (for some ratings to 45' to 51°C 5-s skin temperature that present distress holds for future well-
stimuli are power functions, yet the ratio of being and functioning support the link be-
- - -

pain unpleasantness judgments to pain senso- tween pain unpleasantness and secondary
Departments o f Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery and
Neuroscience, University of Florida, Health Science
ry judgments is less than 1.0 for temperatures pain affect.
center, post office 100416, ~ ~ i ~ FL~ within
~ ~ i this
l l range
~ , (4). The lower ratings of Studies of pain patients show the distinc-
32610-0416,USA E-mall: dprice@dental.ufl.edu unpleasantness in companson with sensation tion between immediate pain unpleasantness

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 2 8 8 9 J U N E 2 0 0 0 1769


and secondary pain affect and their sequential patients (9). Both studies demonstrated that These pathways include a spinohypothalamic
interactions. Harkins et al. (7) assessed the neuroticism exerted its largest influences pathway (II), a spinopontoamygdaloid path-
influence of two personality traits, neuroti- not on early stages of pain sensory process- way (12), and a component of the spinotha-
cism and extraversion, on pain sensation in- ing and pain unpleasantness, but on second- lamic pathway that projects to specific mid-
tensity, pain unpleasantness, and secondary ary pain affect. line thalamic nuclei (13). The latter projects
pain affect, using Eysenck and Eysenck's (8) The sequential model of intensity-un- to limbic cortical areas such as anterior cin-
personality inventory. A group of 105 myo- pleasantness-secondary affect also was sup- gulate (ACC) and insular cortex (IC) (13).
fascia1 pain dysfunction (MPD) patients were ported by multivariate (linear structural rela- Individual neurons often project in more than
given validated scales to rate sensory and tions) analyses of ratings of these pain dimen- one of these pathways. Another component
affective dimensions of pain. First, the per- sions by 1008 chronic pain patients. The se- of the spinothalamic pathway projects to so-
sonality trait of neuroticism had no influence quential model scored high on several indices matosensory relay nuclei of the thalamus
on sensory ratings of experimental heat pain of goodness of fit (10). [ventroposterior lateral nucleus (VPL) and
or clinical pain. Second, neuroticism was as- ventroposterior inferior nucleus (VPI)] that
sociated with a small but statistically signif- Neural Mechanisms of Pain relay nociceptive information to somatosen-
icant enhancement of patients' unpleasant- Unpleasantness and Secondary Pain sory (S-1 and S-2) cortices (1,2,13). S-1 and
ness ratings of both experimental and clinical Affect S-2 are anatomically interconnected with a
pain. Third, high neurotic score patients rated Relations between these dimensions of pain ventrally directed cortico-limbic somatosen-
emotions of secondary pain affect (i.e., de- can be at least partly related to present un- sory pathway that integrates somatosensory
pression and anxiety) as much more negative derstanding of their underlying neural mech- input with other sensory modalities such as
in comparison with low neurotic score pa- anisms. Multiple ascending pathways project vision and audition and with learning and
tients. Extraverts and introverts did not dif- to several brainstem and cortical regions, as memory (14). This pathway proceeds from
fer in their ratings of any pain dimensions. shown in Fig. 2. Some of these pathways S-11s-2 to posterior parietal cortical areas and
The same overall pattern of results was project from the spinal cord dorsal horn di- to IC and from IC to amygdala, perirhinal
obtained in a study of 205 chronic pain rectly to brainstem and limbic system areas. cortex, and hippocampus (14). Importantly,
this system ultimately converges on the same
Fig. 1. A schematic used t o illus-
trate interactions between pain
sensation, pain unpleasantness,
NOCICEPTI!E INPUT
-.
*****-.
limbic and subcortical structures that are di-
rectly accessed by ascending spinal pathways
(Fig. 2). This dual convergence may be relat-
i
..*a
and secondary pain affect (solid *
O
.t
ed to a mechanism whereby multiple neural
arrows). Neural structures likely NOClCEPTlVE (S-1, S-2, PPC?, IC?)
t o have a role in these dimen- AROUSAL, SENSATIONS sources contribute to pain affect.
sions are shown by abbreviations AUTONOMIC AND Thus, several ascending pathways and
SOMATOMOTOR
in adjacent parentheses, and
their full names are given in the ACTIVATION
(RF, HYP, SMA,
...... PERCEIVED INTRUSION OR
brain regions are activated by nociceptive
input and participate in pain processing. The
legend of Fig. 2. Dashed arrows THREAT (PPC, IC) number and extent of activation of all of these
AMYG)
indicate nociceptive or endoge- regions are variable and controversial across
nous physiological factors that
influence pain sensation and un- neural imaging studies [see (15) for review].
IMMEDIATE PAlN UNPLEASANTNESS
pleasantness.
J
SECOND ORDER APPRAISALS (PFC)
(ACC)

+SECONDARY PAlN AFFECT


A likely major source of this variability is
that the number of activated structures is a
function of pain intensity itself. Graded noci-
ceptive (46", 48", and 50°C) and control
(35°C) skin temperature stimuli were used to
Fig. 2. Schematic of determine stimulus-neural response relations
ascending pathways,
subcortical structures, for several brain areas just described (16).
and cerebral cortical Increases in magnitude and spatial distribu-
structures involved in tion of neural activation and additional re-
processing pain. PAC, cruitment of more brain structures in re-
periaqueductal gray; sponse to ascending temperatures rated as
PB, parabrachial nu- increasingly painful were observed. These
cleus of the dorso-
lateral pons; VMpo, stimulus-response relations were obtained for
ventromedial part of several functionally diverse brain areas, in-
the posterior nuclear cluding those likely involved in pain sensa-
complex; MDvc, ven- tion (S-1, S-2, and IC?), motor control (sup-
trocaudal part of the plementary motor area), affect, andlor atten-
medial dorsal nucleus; tion (ACC) (16). These results underscore the
VPL, ventroposterior
lateral nucleus; ACC, idea that precise coding of the intensity of
anterior cingulate cor- nociceptive stimulation is common to all
tex; PCC, posterior cin- functions associated with pain.
gulate cortex; HT, hy- How then is it possible to determine brain
pothalamus; S-1 and structures that are differentially involved in
S-2, first and second
somatosensory cortical
sensory and affective dimensions of pain? In
areas; PPC, posterior a recent positron emission tomography (PET)
parietal complex; SMA, study, hypnotic suggestions were used to se-
supplementary motor lectively increase or decrease unpleasantness
area; AMYG, amygdala; ratings of experimental pain (1 7). Significant
PF, prefrontal cortex. pain-related activation occurred in the so-

1770 9 JUNE 2000 VOL 288 SCIENCE www.sciencen


SCIENCE'S COMPASS

matosensory area I (S-1), ACC area 24, and which would be that related to appreciation of structures (ACC, IC, and amygdala) that re-
IC during control conditions. Hypnotic sug- the intensive and qualitative aspects of pain ceive direct input from spinal pain pathways.
gestions resulted in much larger unpleasant- sensation. S-1 and S-2 somatosensory corti- As shown in Fig. 2, somatosensory input
ness ratings during the "high unpleasantness" cal areas would be involved at this level. related to pain proceeds from S-1 and S-2
condition as compared with the "low unpleas- However, higher levels of processing also through posterior parietal cortex to IC and
antness" condition with no differences in pain occur within posterior parietal and insular finally to ACC (22). Convergence at the level
sensation ratings. Consistent with these regions that integrate somatosensory nocicep- of ACC would be consistent with a mecha-
changes, activity in the posterior sector of tive input with other contextual inputs to nism in which somatic perceptual and cogni-
ACC area 24 was much greater in the high as provide an overall sense of intrusion and tive features of pain would be integrated with
compared with the low unpleasantness con- threat to the physical body and self (2, 3, 14, attentional and rudimentary emotion mecha-
dition, yet no differences occurred for S-1. A 20-22). Studies showing that sensory and nisms. On the basis of neurological evidence
separate regression analysis, controlling for unpleasantness dimensions of pain are in se- (27), the ACC may have a complex pivotal
factors such as pain sensation intensity rat- ries are consistent with this mechanism (4, role in interrelating attentional and evaluative
ings, showed that pain unpleasantness ratings 6). functions with that of establishing emotional
were significantly associated with activity in Evidence for this mechanism derives from valence and response priorities. Response
the posterior sector of ACC area 24 (R = a study that found that some neurons of the priorities would be closely related to premo-
0.55, P < 0.001). A subsequent study that infraparietal cortical area 7b in the monkey tor functions that are integrally related to
used hypnosis to modify pain sensation inten- responded to nociceptive stimuli (44°C to motivation and emotions and may be associ-
sity showed corresponding changes in S-1 47°C) and yet were enhanced by antecedent ated with immediate efforts to cope with,
cortex (18). or concurrent visual stimuli (23). However, escape, or avoid the pain and pain-evoking
A PET study confirmed results of Rain- this enhancement only occurred if the target situation. In this view, cortical areas control-
ville et al. (1 7) by using a different experi- location or direction of motion within the ling sensory, attentional, premotor, and affec-
mental approach to induce selective variation visual receptive field was spatially aligned tive functions of pain are largely in series, an
in pain unpleasantness (19). With the use of with the cutaneous receptive field. The en- interpretation supported by both psychologi-
noxious heat and four successive experimen- hancement was much greater for mild noci- cal (4, 6 ) and brain imaging studies (1 7-19)
tal pain trials, lower ratios of pain unpleas- ceptive stimuli (44" to 45°C) than for stron- described above.
antness to pain sensation intensity were ger stimuli (47°C) (23). The neural organiza- Response priorities change over an ex-
evoked on the first two trials and higher ratios tion of this region of the posterior parietal tended period of time. Pain unpleasantness
were evoked on the second. Regression anal- cortex appears to be that of integrating noci- endured over time engages prefrontal cor-
ysis showed that although several brain struc- ceptive inputs with other sensory inputs in a tical areas involved in reflection and rumi-
tures were activated during pain, only pain manner that conveys information about the nation over the future implications of a
unpleasantness was encoded in ACC area 24 overall degree of threat presented to an or- persistent pain condition. The ACC may
(19). Both studies suggest that ACC may be ganism. This integration is especially critical serve this function by coordinating somato-
more proximate to the production of pain at the low end of the nociceptive stimulus sensory features of pain with prefrontal
affect than somatosensory cortical areas (1 7, range, wherein an organism must make a cerebral mechanisms involved in attaching
19). However, the latter also contributes to behaviorally relevant decision about the ex- significance and long-term implications to
pain affect. tent of threat presented by an object. Process- pain, a function associated with secondary
The functional role of ACC and related ing of pain requires an evaluation of sensa- pain affect. Thus, ACC may be a region
limbic structures could be further clarified by tion in relation to its overall context, an eval- that coordinates inputs from parietal areas
consideration of how they fit into the general uation that may help link sensation with af- involved in perception of bodily threat with
circuitry associated with pain processing. The fect. This function would require integration frontal cortical areas involved in plans and
general ways in which multiple ascending of somatosensory input with other sensory response priorities for pain-related behav-
pathways and brain regions could participate modalities and with memory. ior. Both functions would help explain ob-
in the various components of pain affect are This interpretation is consistent with ef- servations on patients with prefrontal lobot-
evident in Fig. 2. Nociceptive pathways orig- fects of lesions to this area or to IC that omy and patients with pain asymbolia as
inating from the spinal cord dorsal horn di- receives input from S-217b. Focal damage to described above. The former have deficits
rectly activate brain structures involved in S-217b in the monkey results in an absence of in spontaneous concern or rumination about
rudimentary aspects of autonomic system ac- escape responses to painful temperatures de- their pain but can experience the immediate
tivation, escape, motoric orientation, arousal, spite preservation of the ability to detect the threat of pain once it is brought to their
and fear (2, 11-13). These structures include offset of noxious thermal stimuli (24). An attention (28). In contrast, asymbolia pa-
medullary and midbrain reticular formation area that receives input from areas S-217b is tients appear incapable of perceiving the
nuclei, deep layers of the superior colliculus, IC (14). When the latter is damaged in hu- threat of nociceptive stimuli under any cir-
central gray, amygdala, hypothalamus, and mans, a resultant syndrome of pain asymbolia cumstances (25, 26).
s~ecificmedial thalamic nuclei. Activation of results wherein patients no longer appreciate
these structures likely occurs during the early the destructive significance of pain and do A Parallel-Serial Model of Pain Affect
phase of pain, wherein fear, defensive behav- not withdraw from nociceptive stimuli or This view of pain affect mechanisms is that
ior, and autonomic responses would take threatening gestures (25, 26). This deficit of a central network of brain structures and
place. These responses would occur some- occurs despite their capacity to detect sensory pathways that contains both serial and paral-
what automatically and involve a minimum features of pain. lel connections (Fig. 2). Direct spinal inputs
amount of cognition. Posterior parietal cortical areas that inte- to lower brainstem and limbic structures may
A second general mechanism could result grate somatosensory input with other sensory contribute to rudimentary aspects of pain af-
from activation of somatosensory cortices modalities and with learning and memory are fect, such as arousal, autonomic, and somato-
and subsequent activation of brain structures at the origin of a ventrally directed cortico- motor activation. Spinothalamic pathways to
involved in perceptual and cognitive aspects limbic pathway (14). This pathway converg- medial thalamic nuclei provide direct input
of pain processing, the most rudimentary of es on the same cortical and subcortical limbic regions involved in monitoring the overall

www.sciencernag.org SCIENCE VOL 2 8 8 9 JUNE 2000 1771


S C I E N C E ' SC O M P A S S
state of the body (IC), directing attention 15. K. D. Davis, C. L. Kwan, A. P. Crowley, D. j. Mikulis,
References and Notes
1. R. Melzack and K. L. Casey, in The Skin Senses, D. R. I. Neurophysiol. 80, 1533 (1998).
(ACC), and assigning response priorities 16. R. C. Coghill. C. N. Sang, j. M. Maisog, M. j. ladarola,
Kenshalo, Ed. (Thomas, Springfield, IL, 1968), pp. 423-
(ACC). However, ACC receives a major se- 439. 1. Neurophysiol. 82, 1934 (1999).
rial input from a ventrally directed somato- 2. D. D. Price, Psychological Mechanisms of Pain and 17. P. Rainville, G. H. Duncan, D. D. Price, B. Carrier, M. C.
sensory-limbic pathway that contributes Analgesia (I.A.S.P. Press, Seattle, WA, 1999). Bushnell, Science 277, 968 (1997).
3. A. Damasio, Descartes Error (Avon, New York, 1994). 18. M. C. Bushnell et dl., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci U.S.A. 96,
varying degrees of cognitive evaluation to 4. D. D. Price, S. W. Harkins, C. Baker, Pain 28, 291 7705 (1999).
pain affect (Fig. 2). This pathway is consis- (1987). 19. T. R. Tolle, T. Kaufmann, T. Seissmeier, Ann. Neurol.
tent with evidence that pain unpleasantness is 5. P. Rainville, J. S. Feine, M. C. Bushnell, G. H. Duncan, 45, 40 (1999).
in series with sensory aspects of pain and is Somatosens. Mot. Res. 9, 265 (1992). 20. B. A. Vogt, D. L. Rosene, D. N. Pandya, Science 204,
6. P. Rainville, B. Carrier, R. K. Hofbauer, G. H. Duncan, 205 (1979).
cognitively mediated (Fig. 1). Additional par- M. C. Bushnell, Pain 82, 159 (1999). 21. J. L. Rubins and E. D. Friedman,Arch. Neurol. Psychi-
allel contributions to pain affect could in- 7. S. W. Harkins, D. D. Price, J. Braith, Pain 36, 209 atr. 60, 554 (1948).
clude arousal and consequences of autonom- (1989). 22. M. M. Musulam and E. J. Mufson, 1. Comp. Neurol.
8. H. J. Eysenck and S. B. G. Eysenck, The Manual of 212, 38 (1982).
ic1somatomotor activation and would be con- Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Hodder & Stough- 23. W. K. Dong, E. H. Chudler, K. Sugiyama, J. Neuro-
sistent with both psychological (Fig. 1) and ton, London, 1975). physiol. 72, 542 (1994).
neuroanatomical (Fig. 2) evidence. The ACC 9. j. B. Wade, L. M. Dougherty, R. P. Hart, D. D. Price,
24. W. K. Dong, V. J. Roberts, T. Hayashi, Pain 64, 579
Pain 51, 67 (1992).
is a pivotal area that receives multiple inputs 10. J. B. Wade, L. M. Dougherty, C. R. Archer, D. D. Price.
(1996).
and is more closely associated with pain un- 25. E. A. Weinstein, R. L. Kahn, W. H. Slate, Arch. Neurol.
Pain 62, 1 (1996).
11. R. Burstein, K. D. Cliffer. G. I. Giesler, J. Neurosci. 7. Psychiatr. 74, 235 (1955).
pleasantness than are cortical structures and 26. M. Berthier, S. Starkitein. R. Leiguarda, Ann. Neurol.
4159 (1987).
ascending pathways that project there. Sec- 12. J. F. Bernard and j. M. Besson, J. Neurophysiol. 63, 24, 41 (1988).
ondary pain affect is sustained by pain un- 473 (1990). 27. 0. Devinsky, M. j. Morrell, B. A. Vogt, Brain 118 (part
pleasantness and may depend on ACC-pre- 13. A. D. Craig, in Forebrain Areas Involved in Pain Pro- I ) , 279 (1995).
cessing, ].-M. Besson, G. Guildbaud, H. Ollat, Eds. 28. 1. D. Hardy. H. C. Wolff, H. Goodell, Pain Sensations and
frontal cortical interactions that add further (Eurotext, Paris. 1995), pp. 13-25. Reactions (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, 1952).
cognitive evaluation to emotions associated 14. D. P. Friedman, E. A. Murray, j. B. O'Neill, J. Comp. 29. This work was supported by NIH grant R01
with pain. Neurol. 252, 323 (1986). 295527912.

POWERSURGE

NEW! Science Online9sContent Alert Service


Knowledge is power. If you'd like more of both, there's only
one source that delivers instant updates on breaking science
news and research findings: Science's Content Alert Service.
This free enhancement to your Science Online subscription
delivers e-mail summaries of the latest news and research
articles published each Friday in Science- instantly. To sign
up for the Content Alert service, go to Science Online - but
make sure your surge protector is working first.

Science
www.sciencemag.org
For more information about Content Alerts go to www.sclencemag.org.
Click on Subscription button, then c l ~ c kon Content Alert button

1772 9 JUNE 2000 VOL 288 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org

You might also like