You are on page 1of 26

Accepted Manuscript

Positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, and emotional


eating: The mediating role of stress

Hanwei Wang, Jie Li

PII: S1471-0153(16)30245-8
DOI: doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.12.012
Reference: EATBEH 1148
To appear in: Eating Behaviors
Received date: 15 September 2016
Revised date: 26 December 2016
Accepted date: 30 December 2016

Please cite this article as: Hanwei Wang, Jie Li , Positive perfectionism, negative
perfectionism, and emotional eating: The mediating role of stress. The address for the
corresponding author was captured as affiliation for all authors. Please check if
appropriate. Eatbeh(2017), doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.12.012

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As
a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The
manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before
it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may
be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the
journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, and emotional eating: The mediating role of

stress

Hanwei Wang
Assistant Professor
School of Business, Jiangnan University

PT
1800 Lihu Avenue, Wuxi, Jiangsu, 214122, China
Tel: +86-18762615972, Fax: +86-510-85913595

RI
Email: wanghw@jiangnan.edu.cn

SC
Jie Li
NU
(Corresponding author)
Assistant Professor
School of Management, Shanghai University
MA

99 Shangda Road, Shanghai, 200444, China


Tel: +86-13817923361, Fax: +86-21-66134284
Email: mgmtli@i.shu.edu.cn
E D
PT
CE
AC

1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, and emotional eating: The mediating role of

stress

Abstract

The current study examines the different impacts of positive perfectionism and

PT
negative perfectionism on individuals’ emotional eating, as well as stress as the proposed

RI
underlying mediator that explains the abovementioned relationships. Overall, 386 adults in

SC
China reported their levels of positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, perceived stress,

and emotional eating behaviors. Results demonstrate that positive perfectionism is negatively
NU
associated with emotional eating, while negative perfectionism is positively associated with
MA

emotional eating. In addition, stress mediates the relationship between perfectionism and

emotional eating. Specifically, positive perfectionism is indirectly related to emotional eating


D

through the mediation of stress, whereas negative perfectionism is related to emotional eating
E

directly and indirectly through the mediation of stress. Findings of the current study indicate
PT

that practitioners working with individuals who suffer from emotional eating problems
CE

should focus on ways to reduce negative perfectionism while finding approaches that enhance
AC

positive perfectionism. With this approach, individuals would experience less stress and,

therefore, would be less likely to be involved in emotional eating.

Keywords: positive perfectionism; negative perfectionism; stress; emotional eating

2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1. Introduction

Perfectionism is a personality style characterized by striving for excellence and

setting extremely high standards of performance, accompanied by concern over faults (Hewitt

PT
& Flett, 1991; Terry-Short, Owens, Slade, & Dewey, 1995). Previous research has provided

consistent evidence that perfectionism is connected to eating problems (Wade, O’Shea, &

RI
Shafran, 2015). For instance, perfectionists are more likely to develop anorexia, binge eating,

SC
overeating, etc. (Bardone, Vohs, Abramson, Heatherton, & Joiner, 2000; Machado, Gonçalves,
NU
Martins, & Hoek, 2014). However, studies have found different patterns of perfectionism

(Terry-Short et al., 1995). That is, perfectionism is not always detrimental. It also has
MA

beneficial aspects. Nevertheless, the roles that different aspects of perfectionism play in

eating problems are still underexplored. Moreover, the underlying mechanism that mediates
E D

the above associations should also be investigated.


PT

1.1. Perfectionism and emotional eating


CE

Perfectionism has long been considered a pathology-causing personality trait (Frost,


AC

Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991). Existing research has

demonstrated that perfectionism is a risk factor for eating disorders (Bardone-Cone et al.,

2007; Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011; Wade et al., 2015). Although most prior studies assume

that perfectionism is a negative trait (Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Terry-Short et al., 1995), recent

studies have found that perfectionism can be either negative or positive. Derived from

Hamachek’s (1978) model of normal and neurotic perfectionism, a dual process

3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

model—“positive perfectionism” and “negative perfectionism”—was developed (Slade &

Owens, 1998; Terry-Short et al., 1995). In the dual-process model, positive perfectionism is

described as perfectionistic behavior driven by the desire to achieve favorable outcomes,

whereas negative perfectionism is defined as perfectionistic behavior driven by the goal to

prevent failures. The dual-process model has provided a tool to better understand the impacts

PT
of perfectionism on people’s lives. Recent studies have reinforced the discrepancy between

RI
positive and negative perfectionism (Chan, 2007; Choo & Chan, 2013). With respect to eating,

SC
Choo and Chan (2013) suggested that positive perfectionism and negative perfectionism had

different influences on eating problems. Specifically, positive perfectionistic tendencies


NU
predicted lower total eating disorder index scores, while negative perfectionism was
MA

positively correlated with eating disorder index scores (Choo & Chan, 2013). Thus, we

conjecture that positive perfectionism is negatively related to emotional eating, whereas


D

negative perfectionism is positively related to emotional eating.


E
PT

1.2. Perfectionism and stress


CE

Positive perfectionists have superior adaptive skills. If they fail to achieve high

standards, they might change their goals or work harder to cope with failures (Haase,
AC

Prapavessis, & Owens, 2002; Hamachek, 1978). Moreover, they are more likely to have high

self-esteem and, therefore, are less dissatisfied with themselves (Stoeber & Otto, 2006).

Previous research has shown that positive perfectionism is negatively correlated with

emotions like shame and anxiety, while negative perfectionism is positively associated with

those emotions (Fedewa et al., 2005; Pirbaglou et al., 2013). Thus, we propose that positive

perfectionism is negatively related to stress.

4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

In contrast, numerous studies have shown that negative perfectionism often has

harmful effects on people’s daily lives. Individuals with high negative perfectionism set

unrealistically high standards and strive for impractically high goals, which often end in

failure (Burns, Dittmann, Nguyen, & Mitchelson, 2000; Hamachek, 1978). That is, negative

perfectionism often leads to negative affect, anxiety, depression, and less satisfaction with life.

PT
Therefore, we propose that negative perfectionism is positively related to stress.

RI
1.3. Stress and emotional eating

SC
Emotional eating is broadly defined as the propensity for eating in response to
NU
negative emotions (Hawks, Goudy, & Gast, 2003). Stress is widely believed to lead to

craving food or overeating (Greeno & Wing, 1994; Royal & Kurtz, 2010). For instance,
MA

female college students considered stress as the primary trigger for their emotional eating

behavior. When they became stressed, they ate more (Bennett, Greene, & Schwartz-Barcott,
E D

2013). A recent study found that both males and females who experienced high stress levels
PT

were more likely to suffer from emotional eating (Tan & Chow, 2014). People might cope
CE

with stress through eating because eating can distract them from thinking about things that

bother them, or eating something that tastes good per se is pleasant (Zellner et al., 2006).
AC

Therefore, we propose that stress is positively associated with emotional eating.

1.4. The mediating role of stress

As noted earlier, existing research has provided some evidence about the relationship

between perfectionism and eating behaviors. However, the underlying mechanism that

mediates the relationship has yet to be examined and the role of stress has been overlooked.

5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Prior research speculated that negative perfectionism is particularly related to eating

problems because negative perfectionists are concerned with the social evaluation of their

appearances (Bardone-Cone et al., 2007; Wade et al., 2015). To these individuals, minor flaws

in body image may bring about negative emotions. Consequently, they deal with these

feelings through eating.

PT
Nevertheless, we propose that stress mediates the relationship between perfectionism

RI
and emotional eating. As mentioned earlier, negative perfectionists often set high standards

SC
that are not easy to achieve, so they are more likely to experience stress, which may lead to

emotional eating. The high goals that negative perfectionists set may not only be related to
NU
their appearance, they are probably also related to other domains of their lives (e.g., learning
MA

or sports; Gregersen & Horwitz, 2002). In contrast, positive perfectionists have better

adaptive skills, which signifies that they might change their goals or work harder to cope with
D

failures. Moreover, studies have found that stress mediates the relationship between
E

maladaptive perfectionism and psychological functioning (Chang, Banks, & Watkins, 2004),
PT

and the relationship between perfectionism and psychological well-being (Chang, 2006). That
CE

is, perfectionism has a negative influence on individuals’ lives, and stress mediates this
AC

relationship. Taking the above evidence together, we hypothesize that stress may also play a

mediating role in the relationship between perfectionism and emotional eating.

1.5. Current study and hypotheses

In this article, we propose a theoretical framework to examine how positive

perfectionism and negative perfectionism influence individuals’ emotional eating behaviors.

Specifically, we argue that positive perfectionism could ease stress levels, whereas negative

6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

perfectionism would raise stress levels, which in turn would lead to emotional eating. Given

the evidence discussed in the preceding paragraphs, we propose that (1) positive

perfectionism is negatively related to stress; (2) negative perfectionism is positively related to

stress; (3) stress is positively related to emotional eating; and (4) stress mediates the

relationships between both (a) positive perfectionism and emotional eating and (b) negative

PT
perfectionism and emotional eating.

RI
2. Method

SC
2.1. Participants NU
We recruited a group of respondents from a professional website offering paid online
MA

tasks (http://www.sojump.com). Three hundred and eighty-six participants completed the

online survey. Of these participants, 215 (55.70%) were female and 171 (44.30%) were male.
D

Participants ranged in age from 18 to 60 years. Most participants (83.68%) held a bachelor’s
E

degree or higher.
PT

2.2. Measures
CE

2.2.1. Perfectionism
AC

We adapted the self-report 40-item Positive and Negative Perfectionism Scale

(PANPS; (Terry-Short et al., 1995) to assess the levels of positive and negative perfectionism

(20 items each). The PANPS includes items such as, “When I am competing against others, I

am motivated by wanting to be the best” and “If I make a mistake I feel that the whole thing

is ruined.” Participants rated the items on a Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (strongly

disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for positive perfectionism

7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

and negative perfectionism were .91 and .89, respectively.

2.2.2. Stress

We measured participants’ stress levels using the 14-item Global Measure of

Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). The scale measures how

often participants found their lives to be unpredictable, overloaded, and uncontrollable during

PT
the last month. The scale consists of items such as, “In the last month, how often have you

RI
felt nervous and stressed?” and “In the last month, how often have you been able to control

SC
the way you spend your time?” Participants rated each item on a scale ranging from 0 (never)

to 4 (very often). Cronbach’s alpha for this measure was .86.


NU
2.2.3. Emotional Eating
MA

We used the 13-item emotional eating subscale of the Dutch Eating Behavior

Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) to measure
E D

participants’ emotional eating behaviors. The scale includes items such as, “Do you have a
PT

desire to eat when somebody lets you down?” and “Do you have a desire to eat when you are
CE

disappointed?” Participants rated each item on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very

often). Cronbach’s alpha for this measure was .93.


AC

All the scales used in this study were originally developed in English. The items for

the Perceived Stress Scale and the emotional eating subscale were translated following the

procedure recommended by Brislin and colleagues (1973). Two bilingual researchers

independently translated these scales into Chinese and the translated version of each scale

was back-translated to English by another bilingual researcher. We used the Chinese version

of the PANPS (Choo & Chan, 2013).

8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

2.3. Procedures

To control for order effects, we randomized the order of the three scales in the

questionnaire. Prior to completing the questionnaire, each individual was informed of the

broad nature of the research. To encourage participants to answer each question as honestly as

possible, we informed them that their responses were anonymous and there were no right or

PT
wrong answers. Having read the study information, participants then indicated their

RI
agreement with the study protocol and procedure by signifying their consent online.

SC
3. Results NU
3.1. Correlations among study variables
MA

Table 1 describes the correlations among positive perfectionism, negative

perfectionism, stress, and emotional eating. Positive perfectionism was negatively correlated
D

with stress (r = -.46, p < .01) and emotional eating (r = -.18, p < .01). In contrast, negative
E

perfectionism was positively correlated with stress (r = .50, p < .01) and emotional eating (r
PT

= .44, p < .01). The correlation between positive perfectionism and negative perfectionism
CE

was not significant (r = -.03, n.s.). In addition, stress was positively correlated with emotional

eating (r = .40, p < .01).


AC

9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

TABLE 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations between the variables

Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

T
1. Negative perfectionism 2.92 .55 --
2. Positive perfectionism
3. Stress
3.91
2.48
.47
.50
-.03
.50**
--
-.46** --
IP
4. Emotional eating 2.47 .78 .44** -.18** .40** --
R
SC
5. Gender 1.56 .50 -.07 .03 .04 .08 --
6. Age 3.54 .90 -.04 .11* -.16** -.19** -.21* --
7. Education 2.92 .56 -.09 .15

N U
-.04 .05 .00 -.18** --

Notes: N = 386.
Gender: 1 = male, 2 = female.
M A
Age: 1 = under 18, 2 = 18  25, 3 = 26  30, 4 = 31  40, 5 = 41  50, 6 = 51  60, 7 = 61 or older.

Post-graduate.
E D
Education: 1 = Less than high school graduate or high school graduate, 2 = Some college, but no degree, 3 = College graduate, 4 =
*
p < .05; ** p < .01

P T
C E
A C

10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

3.2. Measurement model

Before testing our hypotheses, we used a series of confirmatory factor analyses

(CFAs) to assess the properties of the 4-factor measurement model (positive perfectionism,

negative perfectionism, stress, and emotional eating) using Amos 22 software. Since the

PT
positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, stress, and emotional eating scales comprised

too many items, we parceled these items to form four indicators for each construct (Little,

RI
Cunningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002).

SC
The proposed four-factor baseline model provided best fit indices (χ2 = 287.37, df =
NU
98; CFI = .96; TLI = .95; RMSEA = .07) (Browne & Cudeck, 1992). To examine whether the

four constructs could be distinguished, we combined the two latent variables with the highest
MA

correlation coefficient (in this case, negative perfectionism and stress) into one factor. The
D

resulting indices (χ2 = 826.74, df = 101; CFI = .84; TLI = .81; RMSEA = .14) were much
E

worse than for the baseline model, and further combination would yield even poorer fit
PT

indices. Therefore, these results indicated adequate discriminant validity for the scales used in
CE

the present study.


AC

3.3. Structural model

To examine the hypotheses, we compared a series of structural models. The baseline

model, Model 1, is a partial mediation model including paths from positive perfectionism and

negative perfectionism to stress, paths from stress to emotional eating, and paths from

positive perfectionism and negative perfectionism to emotional eating. In Model 2, we

omitted the direct path from positive perfectionism to emotional eating. In Model 3, we

11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

omitted the direct path from negative perfectionism to emotional eating. Model 4 is a full

mediation model with the omission of two direct paths from positive perfectionism and

negative perfectionism to emotional eating. In Model 5, stress only mediates the effect of

negative perfectionism on emotional eating. In Model 6, stress only mediates the effect of

positive perfectionism on emotional eating. In Model 7, we tested a model in which positive

PT
perfectionism and negative perfectionism directly influence emotional eating. The results are

RI
presented in Table 2.

SC
The results show that the baseline model (Model 1) provides the best fit for the data
NU
(χ2 = 287.37, df = 98). However, in Model 1, the path from positive perfectionism to

emotional eating was not significant (t = -1.38, n.s.). Moreover, when we removed the direct
MA

path from positive perfectionism to emotional eating, χ2 increased (χ2 = 289.25, df = 99).

However, the change was not significant (Δχ2 = 1.88, n.s.). Thus, we selected Model 2, in
E D

which stress fully mediated the effect of positive perfectionism on emotional eating and
PT

partially mediated the effect of negative perfectionism on emotional eating.


CE
AC

12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

TABLE 2

Comparisons of structural equation models

Model specifications χ2 df Δχ2


P
CFIT TLI RMSEA
1: NP + PP → stress → EE & NP + PP → EE ab
2: NP + PP → stress → EE & NP → EE
287.37
289.25
98
99
--
1.88
R I
.96
.96
.95
.95
.07
.07
3: NP + PP → stress → EE & PP → EE
4: NP + PP → stress → EE
311.51
313.22
99
100
S C
24.14**
25.85**
.95
.95
.94
.94
.08
.07
5: NP → stress → EE & PP → EE
6: PP → stress → EE & NP → EE
432.79
428.66
100
N
100
U145.42**
141.29**
.93
.93
.91
.91
.09
.09
7: NP + PP → EE 527.77

M A 101 240.40** .90 .89 .11

Notes: N = 386.
Δχ2 is the change in χ2 compared with the baseline model.
E D
*

a
p < .05. ** p < .01.
Baseline model.
P T
b
E
NP = negative perfectionism; PP = positive perfectionism; EE = emotional eating

C
A C

13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

FIGURE 1

Path coefficients of the hypothesized model

P T
R I
Positive Perfectionism

S C
-.51**
N U
M
Stress A .27** Emotional Eating

.56**
E D
P T .31**

Negative Perfectionism

C E
Notes. N = 386. A C
Standardized path coefficients are reported here.
**
p < .01.

14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Figure 1 presents the path coefficients of our final selected model. The paths from

both positive perfectionism to stress ( = -.51, p < .01) and negative perfectionism to stress (

= .56, p < .01) were significant, supporting Hypotheses 1 and 2, respectively. The path from

stress to emotional eating was significant ( = .27, p < .01), supporting Hypothesis 3. Finally,

the mediating effects of stress proposed in Hypothesis 4 were supported. The direct path from

PT
negative perfectionism to emotional eating was also significant ( = .31, p < .01). Therefore,

RI
these results indicate that stress partially mediated the path from negative perfectionism to

SC
emotional eating. Path coefficients for each proposed model can be found in the supplemental

material.
NU
To further verify Hypothesis 4, we adopted a bootstrapping approach to evaluate the
MA

significance of the mediating role of stress in our hypothesized model (Mackinnon,

Lockwood, & Williams, 2004). Table 3 shows the direct effects, indirect effects, and their
D

95% confidence intervals. As demonstrated in Table 3, the direct effects of positive


E

perfectionism and negative perfectionism on stress, stress on emotional eating, and negative
PT

perfectionism on emotional eating were all significant. The indirect effects of positive
CE

perfectionism and negative perfectionism on emotional eating through stress were also
AC

significant.

15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 3
Direct and indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals

Estimated effect 95% CIa


Direct effects
Positive perfectionism → stress -.51** [-.59, -.41]
Negative perfectionism → stress .56** [.48, .64]
Stress → emotional eating .27** [.15, .39]

PT
Negative perfectionism → emotional eating .31** [.18, .44]
Indirect effects

RI
Positive perfectionism → stress → emotional eating -.14** [-.21, -.07]
Negative perfectionism → stress → emotional eating .15** [.09, .23]

SC
Notes. N = 386.
**
p < .01.
NU
a
CI = confidence interval.

4. Discussion
MA

In this article, we examined the different influences of positive and negative


D

perfectionism on emotional eating and the mediating role of stress between perfectionism and
E

emotional eating. First, we found that positive perfectionism and negative perfectionism have
PT

different impacts on stress and emotional eating. Correlational analyses indicated that
CE

positive perfectionism was negatively correlated with stress and emotional eating, whereas

negative perfectionism was positively correlated with stress and emotional eating. Results
AC

also showed that individuals who experienced more stress were more likely to engage in

emotional eating. Furthermore, path analysis supported our hypotheses that both positive

perfectionism and negative perfectionism were associated with stress, which in turn

aggravated emotional eating behavior. Most importantly, path analysis demonstrated that

stress mediated the relationship between both (a) positive perfectionism and emotional eating,

and (b) negative perfectionism and emotional eating.

16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

This article contributes to the literature in several ways. First, this study extends the

understanding of the effect of perfectionism on eating behaviors. Prior research has mainly

focused on the impact of maladaptive perfectionism on eating behavior. Whether positive and

negative perfectionism play different roles when influencing emotional eating had not yet

been fully examined. The current study filled this gap by indicating that positive

PT
perfectionists are less vulnerable to emotional eating, whereas negative perfectionists are

RI
more likely to be involved in emotional eating. In addition, the mediating effect of stress

SC
should be noted. Prior research speculated that perfectionists could not tolerate the

imperfections or shortcomings in their body appearance. Tiny flaws in their body appearance
NU
may remind them of their failure to achieve perfection, which in turn, results in disordered
MA

eating (Bardone-Cone et al., 2007; Wade et al., 2015). However, our results show that stress

mediates the effects of positive perfectionism, negative perfectionism, and emotional eating.
D

This finding implies that perfectionism is correlated with emotional eating because it is not
E

only related to satisfaction with body appearance, but it also influences individuals’ overall
PT

stress levels. Finally, this article contributes to the literature on eating behaviors by
CE

examining the determinants and process for alleviating emotional eating. A high level of
AC

emotional eating may lead to ineffective weight control (Blair, Lewis, & Booth, 1990), binge

eating (Waters, Hill, & Waller, 2001), and other kinds of health problems. However, there is a

dearth of research investigating the different effects of positive and negative perfectionism on

emotional eating and how they impact emotional eating. Therefore, the current research also

provides useful insight into exploring how to reduce individuals’ stress and emotional eating

levels.

17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Our findings also provide implications for the prevention and treatment of emotional

eating problems. Emotional eating is of great importance to individuals’ psychological and

physical well-being. Research has shown that emotional eating may lead to health problems

such as overweight status (Bennett et al., 2013; Geliebter & Aversa, 2003; Koenders & van

Strien, 2011) and eating disorders (Allen, Byrne, La Puma, McLean, & Davis, 2008; Masheb

PT
& Grilo, 2006). Moreover, previous research found that emotional eating could also reduce

RI
psychological well-being (Hawks et al., 2003; Masheb & Grilo, 2006).

SC
Our study supported the duality of perfectionism. Negative perfectionism intensified

stress levels, which often resulted in emotional eating. On the other hand, positive
NU
perfectionism predicted lower stress levels. This duality indicates that practitioners who work
MA

with individuals beset by emotional eating problems should focus on ways to reduce negative

perfectionism while finding approaches that enhance positive perfectionism. For instance,
D

practitioners can help individuals to set realistic goals that are more likely to be achieved.
E

Moreover, practitioners could also enhance individuals’ self-esteem and help them develop
PT

better adaptive skills. With these qualities, individuals would cope with failures more
CE

effectively and experience less stress. As stress levels decease, people are less likely to
AC

engage in emotional eating behaviors.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. First, the cross-sectional

nature of the present study design did not allow us to explore causality among the variables.

Second, the current study relied solely on self-reported behavior, but did not measure actual

behavior. Additionally, the partial mediation effect of stress on perfectionism and emotional

eating suggests that other variables also influence this effect. The validity of the Chinese

18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

versions of the scales requires further examination. Finally, the data in the current study was

collected from a sample in China, which limits the generalizability of the results.

Future research may confirm causal relationships between perfectionism, stress, and

emotional eating with experimental or longitudinal data. Additional empirical research should

examine whether other factors influence the relationships found in the current study.

PT
Moreover, the validity of the Chinese versions of the scales used in this study is subject to

RI
further examination. Finally, it would be useful if more studies could replicate the results

SC
from the current study in other cultures to strengthen the generalizability of the pattern found

in this article.
NU
In conclusion, we explored how positive perfectionism and negative perfectionism
MA

influence individuals’ emotional eating behaviors. Specifically, the results showed that stress

mediated the relationship between perfectionism and emotional eating. Specifically, positive
D

perfectionism was related to emotional eating indirectly through the mediation of stress,
E

whereas negative perfectionism was related to emotional eating directly and indirectly
PT

through the mediation of stress. The findings extend the understanding of the roles that
CE

positive and negative perfectionism play in eating behaviors and provide valuable insights
AC

into exploring how to decrease levels of emotional eating.


References
Allen, K. L., Byrne, S. M., La Puma, M., McLean, N., & Davis, E. A. (2008). The onset and

course of binge eating in 8- to 13-year-old healthy weight, overweight and obese children.

Eating Behaviors, 9(4), 438–446.

Bardone-Cone, A. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Frost, R. O., Bulik, C. M., Mitchell, J. E., Uppala,

S., & Simonich, H. (2007). Perfectionism and eating disorders: Current status and future

19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

directions. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(3), 384–405.

Bennett, J., Greene, G., & Schwartz-Barcott, D. (2013). Perceptions of emotional eating

behavior. A qualitative study of college students. Appetite, 60(1), 187–192.

Blair, A. J., Lewis, V. J., & Booth, D. A. (1990). Does emotional eating interfere with success

in attempts at weight control? Appetite, 15(2), 151–157.

PT
Bardone, A. M., Vohs, K. D., Abramson, L. Y., Heatherton, T. F., & Joiner, T. E. (2000). The

RI
confluence of perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, and low self-esteem predicts bulimic

SC
symptoms: Clinical implications. Behavior Therapy, 31(2), 265-280.

Brislin, R. W., Lonner, W. J., & Thorndike, R. M. (1973). Cross-cultural research methods.
NU
New York, NY: John Wiley.
MA

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological

Methods & Research, 21(2), 230–258.


D

Burns, L. R., Dittmann, K., Nguyen, B., & Mitchelson, J. (2000). Academic procrastination,
E

perfectionism, and control: Associations with vigilant and avoidant coping. Journal of
PT

Social Behavior and Personality, 15(5), 35–46.


CE

Chan, D. W. (2007). Positive and negative perfectionism among Chinese gifted students in
AC

Hong Kong: Their relationships to general self-efficacy and subjective well-being.

Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 31(1), 77–102.

Chang, E. C. (2006). Perfectionism and dimensions of psychological well-being in a college

student sample: A test of a stress-mediation model. Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 25(9), 1001-1022.

Chang, E. C., Watkins, A., & Banks, K. H. (2004). How Adaptive and Maladaptive

20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Perfectionism Relate to Positive and Negative Psychological Functioning: Testing a

Stress-Mediation Model in Black and White Female College Students. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 51(1), 93-102.

Choo, S. Y., & Chan, C. K. Y. (2013). Predicting eating problems among Malaysian Chinese:

Differential roles of positive and negative perfectionism. Personality and Individual

PT
Differences, 54(6), 744–749.

RI
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A Global Measure of Perceived Stress.

SC
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396.

Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: A
NU
clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(2), 203–212.
MA

Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of

perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(5), 449–468.


D

Geliebter, A., & Aversa, A. (2003). Emotional eating in overweight, normal weight, and
E

underweight individuals. Eating Behaviors, 3(4), 341–347.


PT

Greeno, C. G., & Wing, R. R. (1994). Stress-induced eating. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3),
CE

444–464.
AC

Gregersen, T., & Horwitz, E. K. (2002). Language learning and perfectionism: Anxious and

non-anxious language learners’ reactions to their own oral performance. The Modern

Language Journal, 86, 562–570.

Haase, A. M., Prapavessis, H., & Owens, R. G. (2002). Perfectionism, social physique anxiety

and disordered eating: A comparison of male and female elite athletes. Psychology of

Sport and Exercise, 3(3), 209–222.

21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism. Psychology,

15(1), 27–33.

Hawks, S. R., Goudy, M. B., & Gast, J. A. (2003). Emotional eating and spiritual well-being:

a possible connection? American Journal of Health Education, 34(1), 30–33.

Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts:

PT
conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of

RI
Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 456–470.

SC
Koenders, P. G., & van Strien, T. (2011). Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior,

drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. Journal of Occupational


NU
and Environmental Medicine, 53(11), 1287–1293.
MA

Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to

parcel: exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling: A
D

Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233–255.


E

Machado, B. C., Gonçalves, S. F., Martins, C., Hoek, H. W., & Machado, P. P. (2014). Risk
PT

Factors and Antecedent Life Events in the Development of Anorexia Nervosa: A


CE

Portuguese Case‐Control Study. European Eating Disorders Review, 22(4), 243-251.


AC

Mackinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect

effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral

Research, 39(1), 99.

Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Emotional overeating and its associations with eating

disorder psychopathology among overweight patients with binge eating disorder.

International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(2), 141–146.

22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Pirbaglou, M., Cribbie, R., Irvine, J., Radhu, N., Vora, K., & Ritvo, P. (2013). Perfectionism,

anxiety, and depressive distress: evidence for the mediating role of negative automatic

thoughts and anxiety sensitivity. Journal of American College Health, 61(8), 477–83.

Slade, P. D., & Owens, R. (1998). A dual process model of perfectionism based on

reinforcement theory. Behavior Modification, 22(3), 372–390.

PT
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of

RI
perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology

SC
Review, 10, 295–319.

Tan, C. C., & Chow, C. M. (2014). Stress and emotional eating: The mediating role of eating
NU
dysregulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 66, 1–4.
MA

Terry-Short, L. A., Owens, R. G., Slade, P. D., & Dewey, M. E. (1995). Positive and negative

perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(5), 663–668.


D

Van Blyderveen, S., Lafrance, A., Emond, M., Kosmerly, S., O'Connor, M., & Chang, F.
E

(2016). Personality differences in the susceptibility to stress-eating: The influence of


PT

emotional control and impulsivity. Eating Behaviors, 23, 76-81.


CE

Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior
AC

Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating

behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5(2), 295–315.

Wade, T. D., O’Shea, A., & Shafran, R. (2015). Perfectionism and eating disorders. In F. M.

Sirois & D. S. Molnar (Eds.), Perfectionism, health, and well-being (pp. 205–222).

Springer International Publishing.

Waters, A., Hill, A., & Waller, G. (2001). Bulimics’ responses to food cravings: Is

23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

binge-eating a product of hunger or emotional state? Behavior Research and Therapy,

39(8), 877–886.

Zellner, D. A., Loaiza, S., Gonzalez, Z., Pita, J., Morales, J., Pecora, D., & Wolf, A. (2006).

Food selection changes under stress. Physiology and Behavior, 87(4), 789–793.

PT
RI
SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC

24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Highlights

• Positive perfectionists are less likely to feel stressed.


• Negative perfectionists are more likely to feel stressed.

PT
• Positive perfectionism is negatively related to emotional eating.

RI
• Negative perfectionism is positively related to emotional eating.
• Stress mediates the effect of positive and negative perfectionism on emotional eating.

SC
NU
MA
E D
PT
CE
AC

25

You might also like