You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/273420597

Learning the Language of Science

Article  in  International Journal of Learning · January 2008


DOI: 10.18848/1447-9494/CGP/v15i08/45903

CITATIONS READS

2 1,662

5 authors, including:

Amy Cassata Joyce Duckles


University of Chicago University of Rochester
20 PUBLICATIONS   60 CITATIONS    9 PUBLICATIONS   24 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Lucia French
University of Rochester
33 PUBLICATIONS   865 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

UX Applications in Educational Research View project

User Research - AP Computer Science Principles View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Amy Cassata on 11 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


The International
JOURNAL
ofLEARNING

Volume 15, Number 8

Learning the Language of Science

Amy Cassata-Widera, Yuko Kato-Jones,


Joyce Mahler Duckles, Kathleen Conezio
and Lucia French

www.learning-journal.com
THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING
http://www.Learning-Journal.com

First published in 2008 in Melbourne, Australia by Common Ground Publishing Pty Ltd
www.CommonGroundPublishing.com.

© 2008 (individual papers), the author(s)


© 2008 (selection and editorial matter) Common Ground

Authors are responsible for the accuracy of citations, quotations, diagrams, tables and maps.

All rights reserved. Apart from fair use for the purposes of study, research, criticism or review as
permitted under the Copyright Act (Australia), no part of this work may be reproduced without written
permission from the publisher. For permissions and other inquiries, please contact
<cg-support@commongroundpublishing.com>.

ISSN: 1447-9494
Publisher Site: http://www.Learning-Journal.com

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING is a peer refereed journal. Full papers submitted for
publication are refereed by Associate Editors through anonymous referee processes.

Typeset in Common Ground Markup Language using CGCreator multichannel typesetting system
http://www.CommonGroundSoftware.com.
Learning the Language of Science
Amy Cassata-Widera, University of Rochester, NY, UNITED STATES
Yuko Kato-Jones, University of Rochester, NY, UNITED STATES
Joyce Mahler Duckles, University of Rochester, NY, UNITED STATES
Kathleen Conezio, University of Rochester, NY, UNITED STATES
Lucia French, University of Rochester, NY, UNITED STATES

Abstract: The discourse in formal school settings involves the use of language to exchange information, a social practice
important in all cultures that allows learners to construct, debate, and elaborate shared knowledge. Familiarity with and
participation in this “academic social language” in the early years is important for ongoing school success. The use of
science discourse, marked by specific language structures and patterns of interaction, serves as a vehicle for language and
cognitive development, providing children new ways of representing, organizing and interpreting daily experiences. This
research project considers interactions within a single preschool classroom, using an inquiry-based science curriculum,
examining the forms discourse takes as young children “do science.” Transcripts from classroom videotapes were analyzed
in order to identify aspects of teacher-child talk during hands-on science activities as well as aspects of classroom context
that nurture children’s participation in and appropriation of science practices and discourse. Discourse analysis reveals
that the classroom teacher played a vital role in creating diverse participation structures, providing children sustained
opportunities for conversation, fostering a community of learners who authentically listen to one another, and bridging
home and school language practices. Our analysis demonstrates that within moment-by-moment interactions young children
engage in multiple science activity structures (including observing, predicting, and arguing) and are competent emerging
participants in school science discourse.

Keywords: Science Discourse, Scientific Genre, Language Development, Cognitive Development, Inquiry-based Instruction,
Early Childhood Education

Introduction
Science Practices
HIS RESEARCH HAS attempted to address

T the question, “Can young children do sci-


ence?” To answer this question, we must
examine our conceptions of “science” as well
as our beliefs about how young children learn. “Do-
Activity Structures
Prior research has identified a set of distinguishable
components characteristic of scientific thinking, in-
ing science” is inherently complex, involving inter- cluding asking questions, predicting, observing,
actions with conceptual structures, cognitive pro- testing, measuring, comparing, classifying, model-
cesses, epistemic frameworks, linguistic structures, building, and reflecting. These components have
and participation in social practices (Duschl, 2008). been collectively referred to as “inquiry skills”
In the present study, we will be foregrounding the (Worth & Grollman, 2003), “science process skills”
sociocultural processes, contexts, and understandings (Gelman & Brenneman, 2004; Lind, 1997; Peterson
involved when “doing science,” including character- & French, 2008), “cognitive processes” underlying
istic patterns of interaction, ways of speaking and general problem-solving (Li & Klahr, 2006), and
modes of representing ideas when engaged in scientif- “scientific practices” (Duschl, 2008). However, there
ic practices. We are interested in the ways meanings has been little agreement on how these individual
are negotiated between people in and through social components should be defined or how they might be
interaction (Gee & Green, 1998). In exploring the assessed (Lehrer & Schauble, 2006; Li & Klahr,
question, “Can young children do science?” we focus 2006).
on children’s participation in the interactions af- Our selection of the term activity structures to
forded within a single preschool classroom that uses represent the various aspects of scientific inquiry
an inquiry-based, hands-on science curriculum. reflects the theoretical position of science as a social
practice that is dynamic and situated. According to
Lemke (1990), an activity structure is a socially re-
cognizable sequence of actions that can be realized

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING,


VOLUME 15, NUMBER 8, 2008
http://www.Learning-Journal.com, ISSN 1447-9494
© Common Ground, Amy Cassata-Widera, Yuko Kato-Jones, Joyce Mahler Duckles, Kathleen Conezio, Lucia French, All Rights Reserved, Permissions:
cg-support@commongroundpublishing.com
142 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

in many ways. Activity structures are characterized instruction in science discourse in direct and situated
by their variability and creativity in enactment; they experiences. Many others (e.g., Lemke, 1990; Vare-
can be carried out through different sequences of las & Pappas, 2006; Warren, Ballenger, Ogonowsky,
actions, embedded within one another, repeated with Rosebery & Hudicourt-Barnes, 2001) propose that
minor variations, and interrupted and resumed later. science discourse is inherently “hybrid,” incorporat-
Viewing science practices as a set of enacted activity ing narrative and scientific genre features, allowing
structures highlights the inter-relationships and mu- diverse meanings and genres to be expressed, ex-
tual contingencies among components, acknow- plored and negotiated.
ledging the interplay between process and context. Researchers adopting this hybrid view of discourse
propose that it is essential for classroom teachers to
“Conversations of Inquiry” recognize and build on the knowledge, resources,
and familiar discourse practices that children bring
Duschl (2008) describes scientific endeavors as to school from their home cultures (Bransford,
“conversations of inquiry,” in which meaning-mak- Brown & Cocking, 2000; Duschl, 2008; Heath, 1983;
ing takes place through the use of language. In sci- Varelas, Pappas & Rife, 2005; Peterson, in press;
entific practice, a main function of language becomes Roth, 2005; Warren et al., 2001), bridging “points
seeking, communicating and sharing particular kinds of contact” between students’ ways of knowing and
of information with others. The kinds of information talking and scientific ways. Conceptualizing scientif-
important in science are those that enable participants ic language as hybrid allows for new understandings
to establish shared meaning with the scientific com- of science language which incorporate features of
munity, express abstract ideas and relationships, and lifeworld language, broadening opportunities for
talk about things that are not immediately present participation in science practices.
(Fang, 2005). Scientific language facilitates shared
meaning through use of a common, “specialized”
Science Learning in Early Childhood
vocabulary for referring to concepts and events, yet
also allows for the co-construction of local meanings Sociocultural approaches to development take into
through specific ways of representing and describing account that all learning outcomes are dependent on
experience (e.g., defining, classifying, and compar- and expressed in social and cultural contexts (Lehrer
ing). & Schauble, 2006). Learning science, in this view,
It is important for all young children to have ac- involves not only understanding and using know-
cess and opportunity to “learn the language of sci- ledge and strategies, but engaging in activities as
ence.” The demands of scientific language, such as they are embedded within social and cultural goals
communicating information in a structured format and practices. Within the sociocultural view, devel-
and engaging in abstract thinking, reflect practices opment is understood as a process of transformation
that all students need for school success (Lind, 1997). of participation in practices (Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu,
In later years, fluency in scientific language corres- Mosier, Chavajay & Heath, 2003). Meaning-making
ponds to higher education levels as well as authority among participants with respect to an activity, which
and control in the workplace, serving as a gatekeeper is constructed through language, becomes central to
to economic success and political power (Gee, 2004; learning science. The teacher assumes a role of
Schleppegrell, 2001). For these reasons, several re- providing the learner with the tools to participate in
searchers have advocated explicit, overt instruction meaning making activity and supporting participa-
in the use of scientific language within authentic tion, creating opportunities to participate as “legitim-
practices (Fang, 2005; Gee, 2004; Wellington & ized and authorized individuals” (Kovalainen &
Osborne, 2001). Kumpulainen, 2007, p. 142).
In the present study, we view children’s learning
Scientific and Lifeworld Languages of scientific language within a sociocultural frame-
work. We are interested in exploring the patterns of
In contrast to the abstract, formal, logical nature of interaction and participation that occur in a preschool
scientific language, lifeworld language is based in classroom that focuses on encouraging young chil-
particular instances or episodes, is colloquial in dren’s emerging participation in science discourse
nature, and follows a narrative construction (Gee, and practices. The participatory roles and practices
2004). While scientific talk refers to objects, events, of the children and teacher are examined by focusing
and their logical relationships, everyday language on interactional moves and sequences of interaction.
reflects expression of personal attitudes, interests, We also explore how children engage in multiple
and values (Lemke, 1990). Is there room for life- and evolving science activity structures through the
world language within scientific discourse? Gee moment-by-moment use of the language of science.
(2004) claims a dichotomy between lifeworld and
science languages, and calls for the need for overt
CASSATA-WIDERA, KATO-JONES, DUCKLES, CONEZIO, FRENCH 143

Study Design made final changes of the transcripts before analysis


(all children’s names are pseudonyms).
Background
An Early Reading First Grant from the Department Data Analysis
of Education to the University of Rochester funded A participation and practice framework (e.g.,
implementation of the ScienceStart! Early Childhood Kaartinen & Kumpulainen, 2001) framed our unit
Experiential Curriculum in 12 preschool classrooms of analysis as “ongoing interaction” in the sociocul-
in four urban parochial schools. The ScienceStart! tural activity of science practice. Because meanings
Curriculum consists of five content-based modules1 are negotiated through social interaction in “ongoing
which each span a ten-week period of instruction conversation” (Duschl, 2008), we chose to analyze
(Conezio & French, 2002; French, 2004). On a daily classroom discourse in order to understand children’s
basis, and throughout the day in various ways, sci- emerging participation in science practices.
ence activities are enacted according to a cycle of In the present study, we adopted a micro-level
scientific reasoning that includes phases of collabor- analysis that focuses first on message-by-message
ative reflection, asking questions, making predic- discourse patterns (Green & Wallat, 1981;
tions, planning and carrying out an experiment, Kovalainen & Kumpulainen, 2007). This approach
making observations, and reflecting on results. Ap- to analysis allows us to not only see moment-to-
plication of this inquiry cycle provides children with moment use of language in a conversation, but also
a predictable framework to organize their investiga- leads to a richer interpretation of message meaning
tions using a set of identifiable discourse patterns by also attending to nonverbal information. In order
(French & Peterson, in press). to see the systematic patterns of how message units
are tied to each other, we selected the interactional
Participants sequence as the second dimension of analysis
(Kovalainen & Kumpulainen, 2007; Mehan, 1979).
Margo, the classroom teacher, is European-American
Particularly, our interest was in analyzing how sci-
and had 15 years of experience as a preschool
ence discourse unfolds. This multistep analysis was
teacher at the beginning of the study, including 5
designed to review each message unit separately at
years of experience using the ScienceStart! Cur-
a micro-level, and then situate it into its larger con-
riculum. Participating children (N=17, Mean age=3.5
text of thematically tied conversational patterns.
as of mid-year 2006) included 10 males and 7 fe-
Through the process of grounded theory (Glaser
males, with ethnicities of 7 African-American, 8
& Holton, 2004; Glaser & Strauss, 1976), we coded
Hispanic, 1 inter-ethnic of African-American and
the transcribed data into several emergent categories.
Hispanic, and 1 Other. A majority of the children
The categories were refined into five major dimen-
were classified as living at or below the poverty
sions: 1) discursive moves, 2) communicative func-
level.
tions, 3) communities of practice, 4) science practice,
5) physical space and materials (See Appendix A).
Data Collection Although these categories were analyzed separately,
Large and small group science activities in Margo’s they are clearly inter-dependent. Together they
classroom were videotaped three times during the provide insights into the unique learning environment
2006-2007 school year. The science activities ana- created in this classroom. Margo consulted with us
lyzed were recorded on the same day mid-year, dur- on our process of data analysis, reviewed the video-
ing the “Color and Light” module, which involved tapes, and provided contextual information that we
manipulating and mixing colors using various media could not obtain directly from the videotapes.
and exploring properties of light. One of the authors Discursive moves: Discursive moves include initi-
reviewed and transcribed the videotaped data from ations, replies, and follow-ups. Initiations are the
this classroom. Authors selected two transcribed 40- message units that mark a new conversational
minute discourse segments for analysis, comprising “thread” that begins a new thematic interaction by
one large group activity and one small group activity. asking questions or making new comments
These particular segments were selected because (Kaartinen & Kumpulainen, 2001). Replies are
they are representative of this teacher’s practice and closed-ended responses that provide closure, finality,
reflect how she typically interacts with children in and/or conclusion of a particular topic. Follow-ups
the two different settings. The authors watched the are open-ended responses involving elaboration and
videotapes of the selected segments together, and revoicing of the preceding message unit, continuation
of thought, and providing space for continued con-
versation.

1
Measurement and Mapping, Movement and Machines, Color and Light, Properties of Matter, and Neighborhood Habitats
144 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

Communicative Functions: This category elabor- T: It’s…what?


ates properties of discursive moves, which can con- C: Green.
tain various meanings depending upon contexts of T: Mmmmm…
discourse and the way message units are connected C: Melting.
to each other. For example, following-up can involve
either revoicing or affirming, depending upon pros- As illustrated in the selected excerpt, Margo’s initi-
odic, nonverbal, and contextual cues within a mes- ations require the children to think and take an active
sage unit. role in the discussion. Margo does not use “known-
Communities of practice: Our analysis highlights information” questions (e.g., Mehan, 1979) in initi-
specific classroom practices that Margo and the ating, but rather calls for children to make observa-
children engage in that facilitate participation in tions and predictions. Margo, by directing children’s
evolving classroom and science discourse through attention accompanied by a call to observe (“let’s
building a community of practice. This category de- just watch”), implicitly invites children to talk about
scribes how a teacher and students co-construct the what they see. In addition, Margo frequently talks
community of science practice together while parti- about what is happening (“narrating”) and invites
cipating in science activities and discussions. children to do the same (“It’s turning”).
Science Practice: This category is central to the In the same excerpt, follow-ups trigger children’s
present study. Science practice elaborates the way attention and encourage the children to continue to
activity structures involved in science inquiry (i.e., express their opinions to the group. Margo’s follow-
observing, predicting) occur in a sequential manner, ups are often open-ended and refrain from evaluation,
and describes how scientific language plays a role leaving room for children to explore the topic them-
in unfolding discourse. selves. When the child in the above excerpt calls the
Physical space and materials: In the three-year- Jello “water,” Margo follows up with a comparison
old classroom, science activities involve a variety of (“This is like water”), providing information while
hands-on materials, such as Jello and eyedroppers. continuing the conversation on the topic put forth by
Physical space not only supports the special relation- the child. Likewise, when children offer their opin-
ship among the participants but also symbolizes the ions of what is happening to the Jello, Margo’s re-
sense of classroom community they build and share. sponses (“It’s what? / Mmmmm…”) invite continued
discussion and input from the children without indic-
ating that a given answer is “right” or “wrong.” This
Findings differs from typical “Initiation-Response-Evaluation”
practice in which children are guessing an already
Discursive Moves
known answer, and evaluation consists of a reply
In Margo’s classroom, discursive moves reveal the without the intention to extend conversation or pro-
ways meaning is negotiated through the co-construc- voke further questions or thoughts (e.g., Mehan,
tion of specific conversational patterns. Our analysis 1979).
identified instances of Margo and children initiating, In the small group, Margo takes the role of super-
responding, and following-up on the responses of vising small-group activities occurring simultan-
others, as well as the functions these moves served eously at two tables and managing children’s inter-
in conversation and the different discursive patterns actions with the materials. Because her attention is
that developed within large group and small group divided, she is not the primary initiator of conversa-
settings. tional topics at either table. In the small group, Margo
In the large group setting, Margo commonly initi- does not attempt to guide discussion toward a partic-
ates conversational sequences that are typically fol- ular topic or goal. Rather, topics of conversation
lowed by a child response. In the following excerpt, emerge from the children’s own comments as they
Margo and the children are exploring the clear Jello participate in the hands-on activity with frozen clear
that they had made the day before. Margo regularly Jello and food coloring.
considers and addresses children’s responses, some-
times taking this as an opportunity to segue into a C: It’s melting.
new initiation. T: It is melting. ((touches Jello)) Yup.
C: See?
T: Ok let’s just watch and see what happens to T: Yup.
that, OK? OK. C: See the ice?
T: ((holds up the dish of clear Jello)) Um…
C: That’s water. In the above excerpt, the child initiates the topic us-
T: Well, this is like water. ing similar discourse patterns to those modeled in
T: What’s happening to this? It’s what? the large group (establishing joint attention, calling
C: It’s turning for and making observations). The child initiating
CASSATA-WIDERA, KATO-JONES, DUCKLES, CONEZIO, FRENCH 145

conversation is establishing a shared understanding Encouraging distributed expertise. Just as she


with Margo about what is happening to the ice. In creates a discursive space for children to voice their
this sequence, Margo supports an observation, ideas or opinions, Margo also encourages distributed
“testing” the claim “It’s melting” by gathering evid- expertise by directly calling for children’s responses
ence and touching the Jello herself, demonstrating and accepting a variety of opinions and interpreta-
coordination of a nonverbal (tactile) observation with tions, illustrated in the excerpt below. Acts of
a verbal expression of the observation. The second agreeing and disagreeing are both welcome and
“yup” by Margo indicates that she shares understand- “correct answers” are trumped by participation. She
ing with the child, who concludes the sequence “See accomplishes this through asking for repetition, using
the ice?” with a summative statement that describes oral cloze patterns, and using open-ended responses.
and creates a shared verbal representation of the
activity. T: ((holding out a pie plate filled with frozen
Jello)) Anya, do you think this is gonna fa..fall
out of here if I turn this upside down?
Communities of Practice
C: Uh-uh
Examining the patterns of interactive moves becomes T: You don’t think…
more interesting when considered in conjunction T: Uh, Lucas. Do you think this is gonna come
with the analytic category “building communities of out if I turn this upside down?
practice.” We found that many of the discursive C: Yeah
moves served not only communicative functions in T: You do?
maintaining and extending conversations, but also T: How about you Markell? You think it’s
(as in the example “See the ice?” above) created and gonna fall out?
sustained a community of practice. C: ((nodding))
Inclusive language. Our analysis uncovered how
both the teachers and children in our classroom build Using lifeworld language. As children share their
a repertoire of classroom practices through a variety opinions and elaborate their views within this socially
of interactional moves that evidence the desire to shared activity, we also found evidence of them using
build and sustain a learning community. As we ana- their lifeworld language in conversation.
lyzed the discursive moves of both teachers and
children we found that “initiations” often involve T: ((tipping a bowl of Jello on its side)) Oh,
inclusive language, accomplished through the use what’s gonna happen if I keep doing that?
of collective “we” (“Well, we’re going to do some- C: It’ll mess it up.
thing with colors”) and by activating familiar routines T: It’s gonna what?
(“cause that’s what we usually do next”). C: Melt
Creating discursive spaces. Margo’s initiations C: Spill
and follow-ups often create a discursive space for C: Fall down!
the children, taking the form of open-ended re-
Providing opportunities for children to incorporate
sponses, affirming, revoicing, and oral cloze. These
lifeworld language is important, as research emphas-
participation structures are taken up by the children
izes that pedagogy directed toward supporting stu-
in this classroom as they respond by providing their
dents’ active participation in science learning must
own ideas, using their own lifeworld language, and
be sensitive to children’s learning histories and cul-
offering their own interpretations.
tural identities (Kaartinen & Kumpulainen, 2001;
T: Hmm, look at this…((showing children Jello Varelas et al, 2005; Warren et al, 2001). Kaartinen
in container)) and Kumpulainen (2001) note that, “the histories and
C: Glitter. That’s glitter. identities of students can work as a resource for the
T: Hmm. learning community, and, conversely, the interaction
C: It’s shiny. of the learning community can work as a resource
T: It’s shiny. for the individual student to negotiate and renegotiate
his/her position and interpretation” (p. 156).
Within this short exchange, Margo provides neutral Our analysis provides evidence of ways that both
responses and open-ended statements to create space children and adults are involved in creating a local
for children to provide their own interpretations. In community of practice and points to pedagogical
addition, rather than providing evaluative responses, strategies that support multiple modes of participa-
the teacher revoices “it’s shiny” and expresses appre- tion in science and classroom practices. Our next
ciation of the child’s opinion. These strategies are section explores in more detail how the science
used to create the discursive space for multiple forms practices take form in this classroom within dis-
of participation. course, through sequences of interactional moves
146 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

and participation structures made available within by pointing at a material, exclaiming, “look at it!”
the community of practice. in order to call for an observation, or by narrating
her own action, “I’m making blue.”
Science Practices Both scientific and lifeworld languages are ob-
served in this activity structure. Observation state-
Peterson and French (2008) have demonstrated that ments are classified into three categories of scientific
scientific explanations in preschool settings are ac- language: naming, describing, and narrating. State-
complished across utterances in extended discourse. ments such as “color” while looking at food coloring
Our current analysis reveals that, in addition to ex- and “Jello” while pointing at a box of Jello mix are
planation, several other science discourse forms may examples of observation through the naming of ma-
also be co-constructed among multiple speakers in terials. When unfamiliar with the names of materials
preschool classrooms, comprising activity structures they encounter in the classroom, children participate
important for inquiry such as observation, prediction, in conversation by using lifeworld language. For
testing, and argumentation. We will illustrate three example, children make comments such as
prominent activity structures occurring in selected “squishes” and “doesn’t it get medicine?” while re-
discourse segments: observation, prediction, and ar- ferring to an eyedropper, relying on their prior exper-
gumentation sequences, highlighting the ways these iences.
forms of inquiry are co-constructed through lan- Observation statements are also indicated by de-
guage. scriptions of objects or events, for example, by stat-
Observation sequences. In this classroom, obser- ing, “it’s shiny” while looking at glitter reflecting
vation sequences are initiated in three general pat- light, or commenting “it’s melting” while looking at
terns. First, Margo frequently begins an observation frozen Jello melting in a dish. The act of “narrating,”
sequence by asking observation questions, as indic- or talking about what is happening while engaged in
ated in the following excerpt. an activity, is a common form of observation used
by both Margo and the children. When children are
T: Can you…Can you see through this? participating in hands-on science activities during
((holds up frozen Jello in a metal dish)) small group, they are especially likely to express
Children: No. their observation through narrating their actions,
T: Can you see me? ((holds up the dish in front such as “look what I made - circle” and “I’m making
of her face)) blue like you.”
Children: No. Prediction sequences. Prediction sequences begin
T: Noooo. with Margo asking explicit questions that grasp
children’s attention to call for their predictions. Two
Asking specific questions about the object of interest
different types of prediction-provoking questions
guides children to attend to specific attributes of the
were identified in this analysis: closed-ended ques-
materials, such as whether the metal dish is transpar-
tions beginning with a “do you think…” clause, and
ent. Second, Margo encourages children to make
open-ended questions typically composed of a
observations, including both explicit (i.e., “look at
“what’s gonna happen…” clause. With a closed-
this”) and implicit (i.e., “hmm”) comments.
ended prediction question, children are expected to
T: Hmm, look at this. ((shows Jello in a contain- reply with “yes-no” answers, but even so, Margo’s
er)) reply often allows space for the child to elaborate
Kaitlin: Glitter. That’s glitter. further.
T: Hmm.
T: Do you think this one if I turn it ((dish)) up-
Sara: It’s shiny.
side down, do you think this ((Jello)) is gonna
In this excerpt, Margo provides less direction about come out?
which details to observe, and children make two C: Yeah.
different observations based on the perceived qualit- T: You do?
ies of Jello. Kaitlin sees glitter on the surface of the C: Yeah.
Jello, and Sara instead focuses on the light-reflecting
Although children are encouraged to verbally express
property of the glitter. Margo’s comment “hmm” in
their opinions to the group, they sometimes provide
response to Kaitlin’s observational statement is cre-
nonverbal replies by nodding (“yes”), or shaking
ating discursive space for other children to expand
their head (“no”). On the other hand, with an open-
on Kaitlin’s observation. Third, children’s own initi-
ended prediction question, children reply to the
ations of observation sequences are also apparent,
question in various ways, using lifeworld language
typically observed during the small group science
(Gee, 2004).
activity. A child may begin the observation sequence
CASSATA-WIDERA, KATO-JONES, DUCKLES, CONEZIO, FRENCH 147

T: Oh, what’s gonna happen if I keep doing In this excerpt from a small group science activity,
that. ((tilts Jello bowl)) Isaac uses the teacher’s feedback (“no, you are
C: It’ll mess it up making green”) as evidence and attempts to make
T: It’s gonna what? an argument against Tara (“you making, you making
C: Melt. green”). Through the disagreement, Isaac presents
C: Spill. an alternative viewpoint of what color they are ma-
C: Fall down. nipulating. Tara agrees that she is making green (“it’s
T: It’s gonna fall down. green”) and Damian who overheard the interaction
between Tara and Isaac completes the sequence by
Incorporating lifeworld language into their predic- drawing a conclusion, “we all green.” This is an in-
tions, children bring their various funds of knowledge triguing example of the extent to which three-year-
from everyday experience to the science practice in olds can build a preliminary yet strong argumentation
school (Varelas & Pappas, 2006). In addition to discourse with little guidance from an adult.
lifeworld language, instances of scientific language In this classroom, children appear to be comfort-
are also seen in prediction activity structure. When able disagreeing with one another, enabling construct-
Margo asks prediction questions, she frequently uses ive argumentation. This comfort with expressing
an “if-then” causal statement, such as “if I turn it multiple perspectives may be attributed to their sci-
upside down, do you think this is gonna come out?” ence practice in the large group, where Margo models
or “what’s gonna happen if I keep doing that?” This building argumentation by asking multiple children
“if-then” causal statement establishes a specific dis- the same questions and commenting, in a non-evalu-
course pattern governing many variations of predic- ative manner, on the differences in their opinions.
tion sequences. Summary of science activity structures. Both
In Margo’s classroom, science activities are organ- Margo and the children in her classroom use scientif-
ized around hands-on exploration of materials in or- ic language in activity structures, particularly inform-
der to make science experience concrete. Therefore, ative language that communicates information about
any prediction questions Margo asks children are ideas and events to others (Halliday, 2004; Pinnell,
always paired with a presentation of materials. For 1975). Specifically, making an observation often in-
example, when Margo asks children a question (e.g., volves observational statements including naming,
“what’s gonna happen if I keep doing that?”), she describing, and narrating when referring to objects
tilts the Jello bowl at the same time, providing chil- and events. The act of making predictions utilizes
dren a visual clue of what to predict. In this both “if-then” causal statements and prediction
classroom discourse, making an observation of ma- statements (e.g., “do you think”), and the argument-
terials is a prerequisite for making a prediction, ation sequence involves narrating and drawing con-
situating the act of observation within a prediction clusions. Together, these smaller activity structures
discourse pattern. This pattern of embedding smaller comprise a larger process of scientific inquiry. In
activity structures within larger, more complex addition to elements of scientific language, lifeworld
structures was frequently observed in the discourse language is also incorporated into the science dis-
(See Appendix B). course of this classroom. This integration, or hybrid-
Argumentation sequence. We observed argument- ization (Varelas & Pappas, 2006), of science and
ation in this classroom as a conversation in which lifeworld languages allows children to make connec-
alternative viewpoints were expressed. Children tions between their ideas and scientific thought.
construct evidence within the argumentation se-
quence based on their concrete observations of ma-
terials, providing another example of embedded Conclusion
activity structures. Our initial research task was to look for evidence of
the science “process skills” enacted through the sci-
Tara: I’m making blue like you, Isaac. ence cycle implemented in the preschool classrooms
((points at Isaac, as mixing Jello water mixture using the ScienceStart! Curriculum. We found that
with green food coloring)) these “skills” are always embedded within ongoing
Isaac: I’m blue? activity and social exchanges. They can be conceptu-
T: ((to Isaac)) No, you are making green. alized as complex interactional sequences or activity
Isaac: ((to Tara)) You making, you making structures that occur within quick succession, often
green. overlap, and occur on the micro-level of discourse.
T: ((to Tara)) Ok, put it in the dish. This finding, along with our grounding in sociocul-
Tara: It’s green. tural theories and our evolving understanding of
T: Um…that’s enough. science as a social practice, led us to a multi-dimen-
Damian: ((from the adjacent table)) We all sional analysis of classroom discourse. The analysis
green.
148 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

presented in this paper has added to our understand- by others. Our analysis thus informs curriculum and
ing of children’s emerging participation in the lan- pedagogy by providing insights into how children’s
guage and practices of science and has substantiated emerging participation in science discourse and
the claim that preschoolers can “do science.” practices can be nurtured in classrooms by creating
Varelas and Pappas (2006) call into question re- a range of participation structures that encourage and
commendations of science standards that limit sci- support engagement of diverse students in science
ence for young children to observing and describing. discourse and practices. The heuristic we have cre-
Our analysis demonstrates that young children en- ated to analyze ongoing interaction from several
gage in multiple science activity structures in addi- analytic dimensions can also be used in multiple
tion to observing, such as predicting and arguing, contexts to examine relationships between discursive
and are actively appropriating science discourse. In sequences, participation structures, and the activity
addition, our micro-analysis has highlighted some structures that children and teachers enact through
of the patterns of interaction that nurture diverse moment-by-moment interaction.
modes of participation in science discourse and As Edwards (1993) suggests, as we explore chil-
practices. Discursive sequences are created and dren’s learning we have the option of “heading for
maintained as Margo uses responses and follow-ups the laboratory” or of taking discourse more seriously
to extend conversations. Participation structures are and asking specific questions of it (p. 213). Analysis
made available for active participation in a com- of discourse can reveal how science activity struc-
munity of practice by establishing joint attention, tures are enacted locally – at how observing, predict-
creating discursive spaces, distributing expertise, ing, and arguing take form in the moment by moment
and building on children’s lifeworld language. Margo interaction of members of an evolving community
allows the space for science discourse in this of practice, even when most of the members of that
classroom to take many forms. It is truly hybrid, in- community are three years old.
corporating narrative as well as paradigmatic fea-
tures, and bringing together lifeworld language, home
Acknowledgement
discourse patterns and school “science talk.”
This work adds to our understanding of the mul- This Research Project was supported by Grant #
tiple repertoires of practice and discourse that young S359B040093 from the US Department of Education,
children can engage in. Margo’s classroom is a spe- “Early Reading First in an Integrated Content-based
cific community of practice in which young chil- Curriculum.” Special thanks are given to the
dren’s participation takes form. While these modes classroom teacher and students in our participating
of participation are particular to this classroom, they classroom.
point to pedagogical practices that can be adopted

References
Bransford, J.D., Brown, A.L., & Cocking, R.R. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Conezio, K, & French, L. A. (2002, September). Science in the preschool classroom: capitalizing on children’s fascination
with the everyday world to foster language and literacy development. Young Children, 12-18.
Duschl, R. (2008). Science education in three-part harmony: Balancing conceptual, epistemic, and social learning goals.
Review of Research in Education, 32, 268-291.
Edwards, D. (1993). But what do children really think? Discourse analysis and conceptual content in children’s talk.
Cognition and Instruction, 11 (3 & 4), 201-225.
Fang, Z. (2005). Scientific literacy: A systemic functional linguistics perspective. ScienceEducation, 89(2), 335-347.
French, L. (2004). Science as the center of a coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 19, 138-149.
French, L. A., & Peterson, S. M. (in press). Learning language through preschool science. In N. Scheuer, M. Puy Pérez
Echeverría, C. Andersen, & E. Teubal (Eds.), Representational systems and practices as learning tools in different
fields of knowledge. Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers.
Gee, J.P. (2004). Language in the science classroom: Academic social language as the heart of school-based literacy. In E.
W. Saul (Ed.), Crossing borders in literacy and science instruction; perspectives on theory and practice (pp.10-
32). Newark, DE: International Reading Association; Arlington.
Gee, J.P., & Green, J.L. (1998). Discourse analysis, learning, and social practice: A methodological study. Review of Research
in Education, 23, 119-169.
Gelman, R. & Brenneman, K. (2004). Science learning pathways for young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
19, 150-158.
Glaser, B.G. & Holton, J. (2004, March). Remodeling grounded theory. Forum:
Qualitative Social Research, 5 (2), Article 4. Retrieved June 17, 2008, from http://217.160.35.246/fqs-texte/2-
04/2-04glaser-e.pdf
CASSATA-WIDERA, KATO-JONES, DUCKLES, CONEZIO, FRENCH 149

Glaser, B.G. & Strauss, A.L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago, IL:
Aldine.
Green, J.L. & Wallat, C. (1981). Mapping instructional conversations – A sociolinguistic ethnography. In J.L. Green & C.
Wallat (Ed.), Ethnography and language in educational settings (pp. 161-205). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Halliday, M.A.K. (2004). Learning on how to mean. In J. Webster (Ed.), Collected works of M.A.K. Halliday, Volume 4:
The language of early childhood (pp.28-59). London: Continuum.
Heath, S.B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Kaartinen, S., & Kumpulainen, K. (2001). Negotiating meaning in science classroom communities: Case across age levels.
Journal of Classroom Interaction, 36(2), 4-28.
Kovalainen, M., & Kumpulainen, K. (2007). The social construction of participation in an elementary classroom community.
International Journal of Educational Research, 46, 141-158.
Lehrer, R. & Schauble, L. (2006). Scientific thinking and science literacy. In W. Damon, R.M. Lerner, K.A. Renninger &
I.E. Sigel (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Volume 4, Child Psychology in Practice, 6 th ed. (pp.153-196).
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Lemke, J.L. (1990). Talking science: Language, learning, and values. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Li, J. & Klahr, D. (2006). The psychology of scientific thinking: Implications for science teaching and learning. In J. Rhoton
& P. Shane (Eds.), Teaching science in the 21 st century. National Science Teachers Association and National
Science Education Leadership Association: NSTA Press. Retrieved June 24, 2008, from
http://www.psy.cmu.edu:16080/lessonplans/NSTA%20Li%20Klahr.pdf
Lind, K.K. (1997). Science in the developmentally appropriate integrated curriculum. In D. Burts, C. Hart, and R. Charlesworth
(Eds.), Integrated curriculum and developmentally appropriate practice. Albany, NY: State University of New
York Press.
Mehan, H. (1979). “What time is it, Denise?”: Asking known information questions in classroom discourse. Theory into
Practice, 18, 285-294.
Peterson, S. M. (in press). Narrative and paradigmatic explanations in preschool science discourse. Discourse Processes.
Peterson, S. M., & French, L. A. (2008). Supporting young children’s explanations through inquiry science in preschool.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 395-408.
Pinnell, G.S. (1975). Language in primary classrooms. Theory into Practice, 14 (5), 318-327.
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., Mosier, C., Chavajay, P., & Heath, S.B. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity
by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society in Child Development, 53 (8), i-179.
Roth, W.M. (2005). Telling in purposeful activity and the emergence of scientific language. In R. Yerrick & W.M. Roth
(Eds.), Establishing scientific classroom discourse communities: Multiple voices of teaching and learning research
(pp. 45-71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schleppegrell, M.J. (2001). Linguistic features of the language of schooling. Linguistics and Education 12(4), 431-459.
Varelas, M., & Pappas, C. (2006). Intertexuality in read-alouds of integrated science-literacy units in urban primary classrooms:
Opportunities for the development of thought and language. Cognition and Instruction, 24(2), 211-259.
Varelas, M., Pappas, C., & Rife, A. (2005). Dialogic inquiry in an urban second-grade classroom: How intertexuality shapes
and is shaped by social interactions and scientific understandings. In R. Yerrick & W.M. Roth (Eds.), Establishing
scientific classroom discourse communities: Multiple voices of teaching and learning research (pp.139-174).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Warren, B., Ballenger, C., Ogonowsky, M., Rosebery, A.S., & Hudecourt-Barnes, J. (2001). Rethinking diversity in learning
science: The logic of everyday sense-making. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(5), 529-552.
Wellington, J., & Osborne, J. (2001). Language and literacy in science education. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Worth, K. & Grollman, S. (2003). Worms, shadows, and whirlpools: Science in the early childhood classroom. Portsmouth,
NH: Heinemann.
150 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

Appendix A

Table A:1 Large Group Prediction Excerpt


Line Transcribed Moves1 Communicative Communities of Science Practices Space and
Discourse Function Practice Materials
T: Now, do initiating, engaging establishing joint asking for a prediction Margo holds
you think, if I others attention, using in- (“do you think”), us- up flashlight
put the flash- clusive language ing scientific language in one hand,
26 light on… TI (collective “you”), (causal statement bowl of Jello
encouraging parti- “if…then”), naming in other hand
cipation, distribut- (“flashlight”)
ing expertise
T: Oh what’s initiating, engaging establishing joint asking for a prediction tips Jello
gonna happen others, open-ended attention, creating a (“what’s gonna hap- bowl
27 if I keep doing TI question discursive space, pen”), using scientific
that? encouraging parti- language (causal
cipation statement “if…then”)
C: It’ll mess it Responding using lifeworld lan- asking a prediction referring to
28 up CR guage, expressing using a lifeworld lan- Jello
opinion to group guage
1
Codes for Discursive Moves: TI (teacher initiations), TR (teacher responses)
CI (child initiations), CR (child responses), CF (child follow-ups)

Table A:2 Small Group Argumentation Excerpt


Line Transcribed Moves Communicative Communities of Science Practices Space and Ma-
Discourse Function Practice terials
Tara: I’m mak- Initiating, Narrating expressing opinion making a comparis- referring to ma-
ing blue like to a peer, using in- on, naming terials: bowl
7 you, Isaac CI clusive language (“blue”), making an with colored
((points at (“like you”) observation water, Jello,
Isaac)) eyedropper
Isaac: I’m questioning, continu- negotiating meaning building argumenta- referring to ma-
8 CF
blue? ing, revoicing tion terials
T: ((to Isaac)) disagreeing, narrat- participating in building argumenta- referring to ma-
No, you are ing child-initiated con- tion, making an ob- terials
9 TR
making green versation servation, naming
(“green”)
Isaac: ((to extending, narrat- distributing expert- using argumenta- referring to ma-
Tara)) You ing, revoicing ise (many view- tion, making an ob- terials
10 CF
making, you points are ex- servation, naming
making green pressed) (“green”)
CASSATA-WIDERA, KATO-JONES, DUCKLES, CONEZIO, FRENCH 151

Appendix B

Table B:1 Science Activity Structures


Line Transcribed Discourse
1 T: Ok. Before we..before we go to snack. Before we go have our snack. ‘Cause that’s what
we usually do next. We’re gonna do something a little different.
2

Shaun: Color.

3 T: Well, we’re gonna do something with colors.


4 Kaitlin: Jello-O. Let’s make Jell-O.
5 T: Hmm, look at this. ((shows Jell-O in a container))
6 Kaitlin: Glitter. That’s glitter.
7 T: Hmm.
8 Sara: It’s shiny.
9 T2: It is shiny.
10 T: It’s...It’s... ((points at Sara))
11 Children: ((talking))
12 T: No, no. Look at Sara. Look at Sara.
13 T: (to Sara) Say it again..again Sara, it’s what?
14 Sara: Shiny.
15 T: It’s shiny, she said.
16 C: I don’t like that.
17 T: Do you think if we turn the lights out…
18

C: I like it.

19 T: Do you think if we turn the lights out, this would still be shiny?
20 Children: Yeah.
21 T: Ok
22 T: Ah, Craig. Can you turn the lights out?
23

Craig: ((stands up to turn the light off))

24 T: Is it shiny?
152 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LEARNING, VOLUME 15

25 Some children: Yeah.

About the Authors


Amy Cassata-Widera
Amy Cassata-Widera is a PhD candidate in Education at the University of Rochester, Warner School of Education
and Human Development. With a research concentration in Human Development in Educational Contexts, she
examines the ways classroom talk, teaching practices and the use of mediating tools facilitate the development
of cognitive and metacognitive skills in young children.

Yuko Kato-Jones
Yuko Kato-Jones is a research assistant for ScienceStart! She investigates how preschoolers learn science
activity structures through interaction and participation in a classroom community.

Joyce Mahler Duckles


Joyce M. Duckles is a PhD student in Human Development at the Warner School at the University of Rochester
where she teaches and conducts research on families as contexts for learning and development.

Kathleen Conezio
Kathleen Conezio is a doctoral candidate in Education at the University of Rochester, Warner School of Education
and Human Development. She holds a Bachelor of Science degree in Elementary Education and a Master of
Science degree in Elementary Education. Her current research focuses on early literacy development within the
context of inquiry based science activities in preschool classroom, especially focusing on the role of co-constructed
writing activities as a tool for understanding the ways that literacies can be developed within preschool. She
has worked on grants from the National Science Foundation and the U.S. Department of Education developing
curriculum, designing and implementing professional development in the areas of science inquiry and early
literacy learning, and has led a team of classroom mentors for the Early Reading First grant. She has recently
presented her work at the National Science Teachers Conference and the NAEYC Professional Development
Institute.

Lucia French
Dr. French investigates the relationship between cognitive and language development. As a Fulbright Scholar
in South Korea, she investigated how the language experiences of Korean preschoolers prepare them for aca-
demic success. She led the research team that created ScienceStart!, which has fostered the development of a
rich knowledge base, vocabulary, and cognitive skills for hundreds of preschoolers.
EDITORS
Bill Cope, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA.
Mary Kalantzis, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA.

EDITORIAL ADVISORY BOARD


Michael Apple, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA.
David Barton, Lancaster University, UK.
Mario Bello, University of Science, Technology and Environment, Cuba.
Robert Devillar, Kennesaw State University, USA.
Manuela du Bois-Reymond, Universiteit Leiden, Netherlands.
Ruth Finnegan, Open University, UK.
James Paul Gee, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA.
Kris Gutierrez, University of California, Los Angeles, USA.
Anne Hickling-Hudson, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Australia.
Roz Ivanic, Lancaster University, UK.
Paul James, RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia.
Carey Jewitt, Institute of Education, University of London, UK.
Andeas Kazamias, University of Wisconsin, Madison, USA
Peter Kell, University of Wollongong, Australia.
Michele Knobel, Montclair State University, New Jersey, USA.
Gunther Kress, Institute of Education, University of London.
Colin Lankshear, James Cook University, Australia.
Daniel Madrid Fernandez, University of Granada, Spain.
Sarah Michaels, Clark University, Massachusetts, USA.
Denise Newfield, University of Witwatersrand, South Africa.
Ernest O’Neil, Ministry of Education, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia.
José-Luis Ortega, University of Granada, Spain.
Francisco Fernandez Palomares, University of Granada, Spain.
Ambigapathy Pandian, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Penang, Malaysia.
Miguel A. Pereyra, University of Granada, Spain.
Scott Poynting, University of Western Sydney, Australia.
Angela Samuels, Montego Bay Community College, Montego Bay, Jamaica.
Juana M. Sancho Gil, University of Barcelona, Spain.
Michel Singh, University of Western Sydney, Australia.
Helen Smith, RMIT University, Australia.
Richard Sohmer, Clark University, Massachusetts, USA.
Pippa Stein, University of Witwatersrand, South Africa.
Brian Street, King's College, University of London, UK.
Giorgos Tsiakalos, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
Salim Vally, University of Witwatersrand, South Africa
Gella Varnava-Skoura, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece.
Cecile Walden, Sam Sharpe Teachers College, Montego Bay, Jamaica.
Nicola Yelland, Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia.
Wang Yingjie, School of Education, Beijing Normal University, China.
Zhou Zuoyu, School of Education, Beijing Normal University, China.

Please visit the Journal website at http://www.Learning-Journal.com


for further information about the Journal or to subscribe.
THE UNIVERSITY PRESS JOURNALS

International Journal of the Arts in Society


Creates a space for dialogue on innovative theories and practices in the arts, and their inter-relationships with society.
ISSN: 1833-1866
http://www.Arts-Journal.com
International Journal of the Book
Explores the past, present and future of books, publishing, libraries, information, literacy and learning in the information
society. ISSN: 1447-9567
http://www.Book-Journal.com
Design Principles and Practices: An International Journal
Examines the meaning and purpose of ‘design’ while also speaking in grounded ways about the task of design and the
use of designed artefacts and processes. ISSN: 1833-1874
http://www.Design-Journal.com
International Journal of Diversity in Organisations, Communities and Nations
Provides a forum for discussion and builds a body of knowledge on the forms and dynamics of difference and diversity.
ISSN: 1447-9583
http://www.Diversity-Journal.com
International Journal of Environmental, Cultural, Economic and Social Sustainability
Draws from the various fields and perspectives through which we can address fundamental questions of sustainability.
ISSN: 1832-2077
http://www.Sustainability-Journal.com
Global Studies Journal
Maps and interprets new trends and patterns in globalization. ISSN 1835-4432
http://www.GlobalStudiesJournal.com
International Journal of the Humanities
Discusses the role of the humanities in contemplating the future and the human, in an era otherwise dominated by
scientific, technical and economic rationalisms. ISSN: 1447-9559
http://www.Humanities-Journal.com
International Journal of the Inclusive Museum
Addresses the key question: How can the institution of the museum become more inclusive? ISSN 1835-2014
http://www.Museum-Journal.com
International Journal of Interdisciplinary Social Sciences
Discusses disciplinary and interdisciplinary approaches to knowledge creation within and across the various social
sciences and between the social, natural and applied sciences.
ISSN: 1833-1882
http://www.Socialsciences-Journal.com
International Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Change Management
Creates a space for discussion of the nature and future of organisations, in all their forms and manifestations.
ISSN: 1447-9575
http://www.Management-Journal.com
International Journal of Learning
Sets out to foster inquiry, invite dialogue and build a body of knowledge on the nature and future of learning.
ISSN: 1447-9540
http://www.Learning-Journal.com
International Journal of Technology, Knowledge and Society
Focuses on a range of critically important themes in the various fields that address the complex and subtle relationships
between technology, knowledge and society. ISSN: 1832-3669
http://www.Technology-Journal.com
Journal of the World Universities Forum
Explores the meaning and purpose of the academy in times of striking social transformation.
ISSN 1835-2030
http://www.Universities-Journal.com

FOR SUBSCRIPTION INFORMATION, PLEASE CONTACT


subscriptions@commonground.com.au

View publication stats

You might also like