You are on page 1of 10

JSGE Vol. XV, No. 2, Winter 2004, pp. 70–79. Copyright ©2004 Prufrock Press, P.O.

9. Copyright ©2004 Prufrock Press, P.O. Box 8813, Waco, TX 76714


The Journal of Secondary
Gifted Education

A Synthesis of Research
on Psychological Types
of Gifted Adolescents

Ugur Sak
University of Arizona

In this study, the author synthesizes results of studies about personality types of gifted adolescents. Fourteen studies were coded with
19 independent samples. The t otal number of identified participants in original studies was 5,723. The m ost common personality
types among gifted adolescents were “intuitive” and “perceiving.” They wer e higher on the Introversion, Intuition, Thinking, and
Perceiving dimensions of the personality scales of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) when comp ared to general high school
students. Also, gifted adolescents differed within the group by gender and by ability. Based on the findings, the author discusses teach-
ing practices for gifted students according to their personality preferences.

T
he personality characteristics of highly able youth have tions of perception and judgment. Jung’s theory differentiates
been investigated extensively (Chiang, 1991; Cord re y, b e t ween two typological categories: attitude-related types and
1986; Ga l l a g h e r, 1987; Ge i g e r, 1992; Hawkins, 1997; function-related types. Jung port r a yed the two attitude types in
Jackson, 1989; McCarthy, 1975; McGinn, 1976; Mills, 1984, terms of directions or orientations in behaviors and intere s t s
Mills & Parker, 1998). In these studies, gifted adolescents were of people tow a rd the material world. These orientations bring
found to be different from the general adolescent population, about two attitude types: extraversion and introversion.
as well as different among themselves in personality types as In relation to the extraversion-introversion dimension, the
measured by the Mye r s - Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). relationship between individual and environment is to be
Personality dimensions have also been shown to be associated i n vestigated. Ex t r a ve rted types develop a strong awareness of
with academic achievement and intelligence. For instance, their environment for stimulation. The typical extravert has a
Myers (1980) asserted that the possibility of one’s being intu- s t rong propensity to influence others, but is likely to be influ-
itive- introverted increases as academic giftedness incre a s e s . enced by others, as well. Extraverts usually seem confident,
One might anticipate, then, that a high introve rt or intuitive accessible, and expansive in the manner in which they build
type may be related to high intellectual capacity and high aca- relationships with others (Jung, 1971; Lawrence, 1984; Spoto,
demic achievement in one or more areas. 1995). In t rove rts, on the contrary, are somewhat more inde-
pendent and idea-oriented than the extraverts, as they usually
get their excitement from the inner world. They may some-
Psychological Type Theory times seem lost in thought or maybe somewhat inaccessible in
the way they move around the world (Lawrence; Spoto).
In the 1920s, Jung developed the theory of psyc h o l o g i c a l The second typological category, function-related types,
types to elucidate natural differences in human behaviors. He refers to the specific manner or means of adaptation that pro-
postulated that apparently random behaviors of an individual duces a consciously differentiated psychological function. Jung
could be understood in terms of his or her use of the func- put forw a rd four possible functions: “sensation, intuition,

70
Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents

thinking, and feeling” (Spoto, 1995, p. 33). Jung used “judg- types more than general high school students do. For instance,
i n g” to describe the polarity of thinking-feeling dimensions, re s e a rchers (De l b r i d g e - Pa rker & Robinson, 1989; Ga l l a g h e r,
which reflects an individual’s preference between two different 1990; Hoehn & Bi reley, 1988) reported that about 50% or
types of judgment. Feeling types usually value harmony and m o re of the gifted population is introve rted compared to the
human relationships in their judgments. They make decisions general population, whose pre f e rence for introversion is 25%.
subjectively with a consideration of society’s values. On the Si l verman (1985) found that 34% of 61 graduate students we re
other hand, Jung (1971) designated “thinking” as an opposite e x t r a ve rts, while 66% we re introve rts. Howe ver, some other
function to “feeling.” In contrast to feeling types, thinking studies have revealed different results about gifted adolescents’
types emphasize logic and objectivity in reasoning. This pre f- p re f e rences on the extrave r s i o n - i n t roversion dimension. For
e rence suppresses values and uses impersonal feelings in deci- example, Williams (1992) found that extraverts we re more
sion making (Spoto). frequent than introve rts in the gifted population. Yet,
Jung (1971) believed that “sensation and intuition” consti- C s i k s zentmihalyi (1997) has argued that cre a t i ve people have
tuted two perceiving types. Sensing types rely mostly on the both traits at the same time, while the general population tends
five senses while they perceive information, which makes them to be one or the other.
factual and observant. Sensing types usually approach a pro b- Research also reveals that most gifted adolescents are intu-
lem in a carefully deliberate way; hence, they perc e i ve appar- itive, as opposed to the general population, most of whom
ent aspects of the issue (Jung; Lawrence, 1984; Spoto, 1995). (70%) prefer sensing (Gallagher, 1990; Hawkins, 1997; Hoehn
Spoto stated that, unlike sensing types, intuitive types look at & Bi re l e y, 1988; Mills, 1983; Myers & Mc C a u l l e y, 1985a,
things holistically and critically to get a sense of the whole over 1985b; Ol s zewski-Kubilius & Kulieke, 1989; Williams, 1992).
the parts; hence, they are usually imaginative, speculative, and Since intuitive types are better at abstraction, symbols, theory,
analytical, and they can be more cre a t i ve. They are able to see and possibilities, they outperform sensing types on aptitude
abstract, theoretical, and global relationships. tests. For example, when MBTI types of 3,503 high school
Mo re ove r, Myers extended Jung’s theory, adding a perc e i v- male students in a college-preparatory curriculum we re com-
ing-judging polarity, which she considered to be connected with pared with the students’ IQ scores, all intuitive types had
the extraversion and introversion polarity (Spoto, 1995). Judging higher scores than sensing types (Myers & McCaulley, 1985b).
and perceiving refer to the process a person uses in dealing with Also, De l b r i d g e - Pa rker and Robinson examined the MBTI
the outer world. A judging type is well organized, systematic, and p re f e rences of 72 gifted junior high students who we re final-
orderly and has a planned way of life, while a perception type is ists in the Duke Talent Identification Program and found that
spontaneous, receptive, and understanding and has a flexible way the gifted students showed strong pre f e rences for intuition
of life (Myers & McCaulley, 1985a). (75%).
Fu rt h e r m o re, thinking and feeling functions seem to vary
in the pre f e rences of gifted adolescents. Bi reley (1991) has
Giftedness and Psychological Type asserted that gender and age can explain some of this variance.
For example, most females tend to prefer feeling in their judg-
Myers and McCaulley (1985b) proposed that psychologi- ments, while most males prefer thinking. Also, developmental
cal type is related to aptitude and achievement. People who t rends in thinking can bring about differences. For example,
p re f e r red introversion and intuition showed greater academic Bi reley stated that the adolescent movement tow a rd the more
aptitude than those who pre f e r red extraversion and sensing. logical and objective style may re flect the shift from a feeling to
Thinking types are thought to be better at some tasks that a thinking type. Se veral studies have demonstrated distribu-
re q u i re logical analysis, while feeling types are better at tasks tions of pre f e rences of gifted adolescents on the thinking-feel-
that re q u i re understanding of human relations. Mo re over, ing scale. For instance, Hoehn and Bi reley (1988) found that
Myers and McCaulley found that judging types perform bet- 67.5% of their gifted sample pre f e r red feeling, while there we re
ter on applications, which are thought to be related to higher important differences between elementary and secondary stu-
grades, while perceiving types outperform judging types on dents’ personality types. Most elementary students pre f e r re d
aptitude measures. There f o re, it might be hypothesized that feeling, while most secondary students preferred thinking.
gifted adolescents should prefer introve rted-intuitive thinking In addition, re s e a rchers (Ga l l a g h e r, 1990; Hawkins, 1997;
types, as they are precocious in intellectual deve l o p m e n t . Hoehn & Bi reley, 1988; Mills, 1984; Myers & Mc C a u l l e y,
Howe ver, their preference for judging-perceiving can show 1985b; Williams, 1992) have re p o rted that gifted learners gen-
more variance. erally have a stronger pre f e rence for perceiving over judging.
Although gifted adolescents demonstrate all personality Howe ve r, the Atlas of Type Tables (Ma c Daid, Kainz, &
types as measured by the MBTI, they tend to prefer certain McCaulley, 1986) indicates that most of the general population

Winter 2004, Volume XV, Number 2 71


Sak

p refers judging. Pi i rto (1990) found that 95% of 50 creative ado- Method
lescents we re intuitive - p e rc e p t i ve. De l b r i d g e - Parker and
Robinson (1989) compared type preferences of 72 gifted junior Sample
high students to those of 1,001 National Merit Finalists and
found that the percentage of the types in both groups were alike. Original studies constituted the sample in this re s e a rch
Myers and McCaulley (1985b) stated that, because perc e p t i ve synthesis (the studies included in the research synthesis are
types are more open to new information, they score higher on m a rked with an asterisk in the re f e rences). These studies were
aptitude measures, whereas judging types can be slightly higher re p o rted in published articles, books, technical re p o rts, and
in grades because they are well organized and focused. unpublished dissertations and re p o rts related to psychological
types of gifted adolescents as measured by the MBTI (see Table
1). Fourteen studies with 19 independent samples were coded.
Rationale for the Research Synthesis The reason for including unpublished re s e a rch was to avoid
missing valuable data. The norm group was composed of high
T h e re have been many studies about personality charac- school students in 11th and12th grades. Data for the norm
teristics of gifted adolescents. A substantial number of these group were adapted from the Atlas of Type Tables (MacDaid,
studies used the MBTI as a tool to explore personality types of Kainz, & McCaulley, 1986).
p recocious youth. Although the findings of most studies are
similar, some researchers found somewhat different re s u l t s Data Collection
about personality preferences of gifted adolescents in some
scales of the MBTI. In addition to differing results, the type The literature review was done by means of the online ver-
of data re p o rted in original studies varies. Although some of the sion of the Educational Re s o u rce Information Center (ERIC)
studies used just percentiles, others used continuous score s and D i s s e rtation Abstracts Intern a t i o n a l. Currently, ERIC con-
and self-selection ratio to re p o rt data. The studies also tains either abstracts, full texts of studies, or both indexed from
e m p l oyed different base populations or norm groups ava i l a b l e 1966 to the present. Keywords used in the search with various
in the manual of the MBTI and in the Atlas of Type Tables. combinations were gifted, talented, personality, personality char-
This caused va rying results in the difference between the psy- acteristics, personality types, psychological types, Myers-Briggs Type
chological types of the gifted adolescents and the general high Indicator, and MBTI. Four hundred and twe l ve studies either
school population. There f o re, lack of unity among processes in full-text or in abstract format we re found. After an exami-
and findings of the studies have caused difficulties in inter- nation of each abstract, 63 studies we re selected for further
p reting the results. Another problem arises from studies not re v i ew. The rest of the studies were excluded from further
re p o rting enough data by ability level, sex, age, and grade of the i n vestigation for three possible reasons: They we re completely
participants, even though it is well known that these va r i a b l e s irrelevant to this research, they did not use the MBTI, or they
help us to understand better the diversity of the gifted popula- were not original research.
tion. After 63 studies we re obtained, including articles, reports,
Therefore, an integration of the findings of these studies is books, and dissertations, they were coded in identification forms
essential to understanding the psychological types of gifted ado- for further review, which indicated that only 14 of them had
lescents. The purpose of this study was to empirically investigate enough data for inclusion. Each study had to report either the
personality types specific to gifted adolescents as measured by number of participants falling into each type, the eight basic per-
the MBTI. This investigation invo l ved re s e a rch integration for sonality types of the participants, or both to be included in this
the purpose of creating generalizations in four dimensions of the research synthesis. The 14 studies yielded 19 independent sam-
eight basic types—Ex t r a ve r s i o n - In t roversion (EI), Sensing- ples because some of them had more than one sample. Also, mul-
Intuition (SN), Thinking-Feeling (TF), and Judging-Perceiving tiple studies by an author we re carefully reviewed to avo i d
(JP)—and in 16 personality types, which re p resent combina- duplication in the synthesis. When sample characteristics
tions of the basic types: ISTJ, ISFJ, INFJ, INTJ, ISTP, ISFP, matched in different studies by an author that were published in
INFP, INTP, ESTP, ESFP, ENFP, ENTP, ESTJ, ESFJ, ENFJ, d i f f e rent journals and at different times, the one that had more
and ENTJ. The following questions guided this study. data about findings and sample characteristics was included in the
1. How do psychological types of gifted adolescents differ synthesis. Only two studies of one author (Mills, 1984; Mills &
from those of the general high school students as measure d Parker, 1998) were included because there were 14 years between
by the MBTI? these two studies and the sample characteristics were significantly
2. How do psychological types of gifted adolescents differ different. The 19 samples were then coded in sample character-
among themselves as measured by the MBTI? istics forms and type distributions forms for inclusion.

72 The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education


Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents

Table 1 comparison gro u p. Conceptually, it is the ratio of the observe d


frequency to the expected frequency. If the index is greater than
Sources of the Coded Studies 1.00, there are more participants in that type than expected
from their numbers in the base population. On the other hand,
Source Sample* Study** Sample % continuous scores are a linear transformation of pre f e rence
s c o res such that the midpoint is established at 100 and pre f e r-
Journal 5 3 26 ence scores for E, S, T, and J are subtracted from 100, while
Doctoral dissertation 7 5 37 pre f e rence scores for I, N, F, and P, are added to 100.
Book 4 4 21 For this study, statistical integration of the data was done
Unpublished report 3 2 16 through a pooling technique as opposed to the traditional
effect-size model (Cooper & Hedges, 1994; Glass, 1976; Gl a s s ,
Total 19 14 100 McGaw, & Smith, 1981; Rosenthal, 1978) because most stud-
Note. * Number of samples taken from each source; ** Number of studies taken from each source.
ies either did not provide any comparative data or did not
re p o rt enough data to estimate effect sizes. First, the number of
Coding Forms and Code Book participants of the studies in a particular type was pooled. This
p ro c e d u re was carried out for each type. This resulted in the
In order to code studies, the author developed several cod- total number of participants falling into each type. Then, fre-
ing forms and a code book. The code book provided informa- quencies we re distributed across the types according to subjects’
tion necessary to code data from primary studies into coding gender, age, and ability level. The number of subjects in each
forms. It contains names, labels, and code values of the va r i- type was divided by the total subjects, and the result was mul-
ables in data sets and explains abbreviations. There we re three tiplied by 100. This provided the percentage of subjects in each
major coding forms used in this study: identification, sample type by total group, age, gender, and ability level.
characteristics, and type distributions. In order to test statistical significances, the z-test of statis-
The identification form helped to identify whether or not tical significance was employed at the p < .05 significance lev-
a study would be further investigated and included in the els. The z- value was obtained for each basic type re p o rted for
re s e a rch synthesis. The initial 63 studies we re coded using these the samples used in the studies and weighted in order to test
forms. The following pieces of information we re coded: an statistical significance between groups. In addition to compar-
identification number for each study, year of publication, isons between the gifted population and the general high
author(s), title of the study, source of data, and a decision of school population and comparisons within the gifted popula-
whether or not the study was to be coded furt h e r, and reason for tion by gender and ability, this integration also provided a
not coding if the study was to be excluded (Rosenthal, 1978), gifted base population or a gifted norm group by means of the
and date of coding. Also, sample characteristics we re coded as pooling technique.
age, grade, sex, and ability level, with specific domains coded
as verbal and math through using a sample characteristics forms. Instrument
This section was completed for each sample re p o rted in each
study. The last of the coding forms was type distributions. It The MBTI is a forced-choice, self-re p o rt inventory that
helped to code findings of each study according to the person- discriminates among dimensions of personality types as
ality types that characterize each sample. All data, the perc e n t- described by the theory of Carl Jung (Devito, 1989; Myers &
age of each type in a sample, and the number of subjects Mc C a u l l e y, 1985a; Spoto, 1995). The purpose of the MBT I
preferring each type were coded. The 19 samples extracted fro m is to identify people’s basic preferences in relation to their per-
the final 14 studies were coded using these last two forms. ceptions and judgments. It generates four dichotomous pref-
erences or eight basic personality types: EI (Extraversion-
Data Analysis Introversion), SN (Sensing-Intuition), TF (Thinking-Feeling),
and JP (Judging-Perception). Combinations of these 8 types
The MBTI provides three methods to report data and yield 16 personality types.
extract meaning from these data: percentile scores, self-selection The EI index illustrates whether a person is extrave rt (E)
index or self-selection ratio, and continuous scores. The MBT I or introve rt (I). The SN index shows one’s pre f e rence for
p e rcentile scores indicate the pro p o rtion of people in a sample either sensing (S) or intuition (N). The TF index indicates
who prefer a particular MBTI personality type. The self-selec- o n e’s pre f e rences for either thinking (T) or feeling (F). The
tion index (SSI) compares the number or percentage of part i c i- JP index illustrates one’s pre f e rence for either judging (J)or
pants in a type to those in the base population or in a p e rceiving (P).

Winter 2004, Volume XV, Number 2 73


Sak

The MBTI manual (Myers & McCaulley, 1985a) reports Table 2


the reliability and validity of the data. Internal consistency is
obtained by means of the split-half technique and stability via Frequency of Participants
test-retest correlations. Correlations are high when the time Classified by Age and Gender
interval between tests is short (Devito, 1989). As re p o rted in
the MBTI manual, coefficient alpha ranges from .76 to .83. Age Frequency Percent
The test-retest reliability coefficient ranges from .87 (7 weeks)
to .48 (14 months). 12–16 2,738 48
In addition, criterion validity was established in many 17 and above 1,029 18
studies in education, counseling, management, and occupa- Not specified 1,956 34
tions. For example, as re p o rted in the MBTI manual (Myers
& McCaulley, 1985a), correlations between the MBTI extra- Total 5,723 100
version dimension and business interest and drama interest on
the Kuder Occupational Interest Survey (Kuder, 1968) are .37 Gender Frequency Percent
and .30 (p < .01) and between the MBTI introversion, intu-
ition, and thinking dimensions and engineering interest are Female 2,661 46.5
.25, .33, and .34 (p < .01), respectively. Construct validity was Male 1,798 31.5
established by correlating scores of the MBTI with those of Not specified 1,264 22
similar personality measurements. For example, the correlation
b e t ween the MBTI extrave r s i o n - i n t roversion scale and Jungian Total 5,723 100
Type Su rvey (Wheelwright, Wheelwright, & Buehler, 1964)
extraversion-introversion scale is .79 (p < .01), between sens- sample we re pooled, which resulted in a Verbal mean score of
ing-intuition scales is .58 (p < .01), and between thinking feel- 501.71 and a Math mean score of 544.87. The majority of par-
ing scales is .60 (p < .01). Mo re validity and reliability studies ticipants we re eighth graders from the talent search at Jo h n s
are available in the MBTI manual. Hopkins University.
T h e re are three MBTI forms in current use: Form G is
the standard form for general use; Form F has additional Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents
u n s c o red re s e a rch items; and Form AV is the abbreviated self-
scoring version. In this research synthesis, three of the original Comparisons between gifted adolescents and general high
studies used Form G, four studies used Form F, and seven of school students. Gifted adolescents we re significantly more
the studies did not report which form was used. introverted than the normative group (n = 5,723; z = 3.85; p<
.01). The data analyzed in this investigation re vealed that
51.3% of the gifted adolescents we re extraverts and 48.7%
Results we re introve rts (Table 3). Comparative l y, 64.85% of the nor-
mative group was reported to prefer extraversion and 35.15%
Descriptive Data was reported to prefer introversion in the Atlas of Type Tables
( Ma c Daid, Kainz, & McCaulley, 1986). Also, the analysis indi-
Table 1 shows the distributions of studies integrated into cated that gifted adolescents we re significantly more intuitive
this re s e a rch synthesis. While all 19 samples provided infor- than the general high school population (z = 12.71; p < .01).
mation for the estimation of z scores of the eight basic person- While 71.60% of the gifted adolescents pre f e r red intuition, the
ality types, 16 of them supplied data for the determination of normative group showed a pre f e rence of 31.90% in this scale.
16 personality types. There we re 5,723 participants classified Further, this integration of research results revealed significant
by gender and age as shown in Table 2. However, 34% of par- d i f f e rences between the gifted samples and the normative in the
ticipants’ ages we re not specified by the original studies, nor thinking dimension (z =1.72; p < .05; one-tailed). While
we re 22% of part i c i p a n t s’ gender. Because grades we re specifie d 53.80% of the gifted adolescents pre f e r red thinking, the pre f-
b roadly by the original studies, it was impossible to constru c t erence of the normative group was 47.50% in this dimension.
categories or determine the number of participants falling into Moreover, the gifted adolescents were significantly higher than
each grade. Howe ver, all the participants we re within grades the general high school population in the perceiving dimension
6–12, with an overwhelming majority in the 8th grade and (z = 4.96; p < .01). They preferred perceiving over judging,
above. El e ven of the original studies provided mean SAT scores contrary to the normative group. The percentage of the gifted
of the samples (3,624 participants). Mean SAT scores of each adolescents preferring perceiving was 60.10%, while the per-

74 The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education


Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents

Table 3 Table 4

Frequency and Percentage of the Eight Percentage of Eight Personality Types


Personality Types of the Total Gifted Among the Gifted Adolescents
Samples and the Normative Group by Gender

Gifted* Norm** Scale Female % Male %

Scale N % N % Extraversion 53.40 45.83


Introversion 46.60 54.17
Extraversion 2,988 51.3 6,044 64.8 Sensing 23.75 29.28
Introversion 2,836 48.7 3,276 35.2 Intuition 76.25 70.72
Sensing 1,643 28.4 6,350 68.1 Thinking 41.04 69.29
Intuition 4,165 71.6 2,970 31.9 Feeling 59.96 30.71
Thinking 3,128 53.8 4,432 47.5 Judging 43.31 39.71
Feeling 2,688 46.2 4,888 52.5 Perceiving 56.69 60.29
Judging 2,342 39.9 5,091 54.6
Perceiving 3,485 60.1 4,229 45.4
the norm group showed ESFP, ENFP, ESTJ, and ESFJ as the
Note. * Number of samples included in this study is 19.
** The norm group is composed of high school students in 11th–12th grades. Data for the norm group is adapted most common types. INFP, INTP, ENFP, and ENTP types
from the Atlas of Type Tables (Macdaid, Kainz, & McCaulley, 1986). constituted almost 50% of the whole gifted sample compared
with 19% of the normative group.
centage of the normative group in this dimension was 45.40%.
Variations in gender. Table 4 shows gender comparisons.
The gifted females we re significantly higher in extraversion Discussion and Conclusion
when compared to the gifted males (53.40% vs. 45.83%; z =
3.05; p < .01). Although the gifted females we re higher than The most common type among gifted adolescents is intu-
the gifted males in the intuition direction of the intuition-sens- ition. The high pre f e rence of gifted adolescents for intuition
ing scale (76.25% vs. 70.72%), the seeming difference was compared to general high school students in this study is con-
not statistically significant. Yet, gifted females were significantly sistent with what Myers and McCauley (1985b) wrote about
higher than gifted males in the feeling dimension (59.96% vs. the connection between the psychological type theory and aca-
30.71%; z = 8.5; p < .01). There also was a nonsignificant dif- demic aptitude. They stated that people showing high score s
f e rence between the gifted females and gifted males in the judg- on introversion (I) and intuition (N) show greater academic
ing-perceiving scale. The gifted males we re higher than the aptitude than those who score high on extraversion (E) and
gifted females in the perceiving dimension (60.29% vs. sensing (S). While sensing types almost always fall below the
56.69%). mean in IQ, intuition types are mostly above the mean.
Variations in ability. The high verbal group was statistically Indeed, IN types with P or J usually have the top scores in the
s i g n i ficantly higher than the high math group in the intuition comparisons of students’ SAT, IQ, and Florida Eighth Gr a d e
dimension of the sensing-intuition scale (math n = 460, verbal Test in the manual of the MBTI. Howe ve r, according to
n = 66; z = 4.98; p < .01). Conve r s e l y, the high math group was McCauley and Myers, this is not necessarily related to intelli-
significantly higher than the high verbal group (65% vs. 45%) gence; rather, it is related to the match between the academic
on the thinking dimension of the thinking-feeling scale (z = characteristics of IN types and the content of aptitude tests.
5.33; p < .01). Although the high math group was higher than When gifted adolescents are compared to general high school
the high verbal group in the introversion dimension of the students according to their pre f e rence for intuition, they are
extrave r s i o n - i n t roversion scale (z = .095; p = .47) and in the m o re likely to enjoy solving new problems and dislike doing
judging dimension of the judging-perceiving scale (z = .080; p the same thing repeatedly. They also are conclusive, impatient,
= .50), the differences were not statistically significant. and interested in complicated situations. They might be more
Concerning the 16 personality types, there we re differ- interested in novelty according to the type theory.
ences between the gifted and the general high school students. Although gifted adolescents may not be as introve rted as
The most common personality types were INFP, INTP, ENFP, p reviously believed, almost half of them show a pre f e rence for
and ENTP among the gifted adolescents (see Table 5), while i n t roversion. When this pre f e rence is compared to the pre f e r-

Winter 2004, Volume XV, Number 2 75


Sak

Table 5

Frequency and Percentage of Sixteen Personality Types


of the Gifted Samples and the Normative Group

Gifted Norm* Gifted Norm*

Type N % % Type N % %

ISTJ 330 6.83 6.92 ESTP 155 3.21 6.52


ISFJ 132 2.73 6.82 ESFP 127 2.63 9.37
INFJ 231 4.78 1.79 ENFP 746 15.45 7.60
INTJ 364 7.53 2.62 ENTP 548 11.35 4.89
ISTP 156 3.23 4.16 ESTJ 188 3.89 14.97
ISFP 104 2.15 5.40 ESFJ 160 3.31 13.97
INFP 503 10.41 3.89 ENFJ 220 4.55 3.61
INTP 582 12.05 3.54 ENTJ 282 5.84 3.93
Note. Number of samples in each type included in this study is 16;
* The norm group (n = 9,320) is composed of high school students in 11th-12th grades. Data for the norm group is adapted from the Atlas of Type Tables (Macdaid, Kainz & McCaulley, 1986).

ence of the general high school students, they are ove r re p re- their psychological types. They differ within themselves as
sented on this dimension. This finding implies that introverted much as they differ from the general high school population
gifted adolescents prefer quiet learning environments and indi- concerning their perceptions and judgments. Gifted females
vidual work to group work. a re significantly higher than gifted males in the extrove r s i o n
This re s e a rch synthesis provided evidence that gifted ado- and intuition dimensions, but insignificantly higher in the feel-
l e s c e n t s’ preference of thinking is slightly higher than feel- ing and judging dimensions. The significant differences in
ing, which contradicts some of the studies included in this extraversion and feeling preferences between gifted females and
re s e a rch synthesis. This might be, on the one hand, because gifted males could be partially accounted for by the general
some studies with a much larger number of part i c i p a n t s sex differences in these scales according to the type theory.
found gifted adolescents to prefer the thinking type. A re a- Re g a rding type differences in ability groups, the Hi g h
son might be because developmental trends could have some Verbal Group is higher than the high math group in intuition;
influence on gifted adolescents’ judgments tow a rd more log- conversely, the high math group is higher than the high verbal
ical thinking. In other words, as Bi reley (1991) suggested, g roup in thinking. This finding indicates that both groups have
gifted adolescents might become thinking-oriented earlier d i f f e rent pre f e rences in perceiving information and making
than the general population. Gifted adolescents’ pre f e re n c e judgments. Verbally gifted students can be more interested in
for thinking in their judgments is also higher when compared and adept at comprehending the global aspect of a phenome-
to the preference of the general high school population. The non than mathematically gifted students, who can be more
implication of this finding might be that gifted adolescents i n t e rested in and adept at analyzing critical parts of the phe-
p refer analysis and putting things into logical order and are nomenon. However, the findings about the personality prefer-
m o re impersonal, fair, and firm-minded when compared to ences of these two ability groups should be interpreted with
general high school students. caution because the majority of the participants who were
Unlike general high school students, who usually pre f e r identified as mathematically or verbally gifted based on their
judging to perceiving, most gifted adolescents prefer perceiving SAT scores we re eighth graders and the youngest part i c i p a n t s .
to judging in planning their lives. Consequently, this pre f e r- It is a question of whether or not differences in abilities may
ence can make them more open to alternatives and more curi- account for differences in type pre f e rences. Also, whether or
ous about new situations. They also can have difficulties in not differences in psychological types account for differences in
finishing projects because perceiving types are usually unorga- specific abilities is a question to be further investigated.
nized according to the type theory. There f o re, future research to study relationships between per-
Significant trends were found in gender and ability groups sonality characteristics and intellectual abilities would prov i d e
in gifted adolescents in the intuition-sensing and thinking-feel- n ew insights into understanding the unique characteristics of
ing scales. Gifted adolescents are not homogeneous in re g a rdto gifted adolescents.

76 The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education


Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents

Implications for Teaching-Learning Situations to be unorganized and late on assignments, an atmosphere of


flexibility in the classroom may help them in their learning. For
Different preferred learning experiences exist for each psy- instance, Be t t s’ (1985) Autonomous Learner Model and
chological type (Myers & Mc C a u l l e y, 1985b; Sternberg, Tre f fin g e r’s (1975) Se l f - Di rected Learning would be good ways
1999). Sternberg asserted that cooperative learning (group to motivate perceptive types since these models help to deve l o p
work), for example, is more likely to appeal to external people intrinsic motivation and autonomous learning skills and habits.
than internal people because externals enjoy working in groups, The findings of this study suggest the effectiveness of
while internals enjoy working individually. There is a com- teachers who use a variety of methods in their teaching, par-
mon belief about the preference of gifted students for individ- ticularly in high schools. If they rely solely on a single
ual learning. Interestingly, in this study, both types are method, such as lecture, they may exclude certain students.
distributed almost equally in gifted adolescents. There f o re, it is T h e re f o re, modification of teaching-learning strategies based
likely that gifted students can benefit from both group pro- on personality-learning styles, as well as academic abilities of
jects and individual projects to a maximum extent provided gifted adolescents, might improve their learning. At least,
that teachers have the flexibility to teach to different styles of some instruction should match gifted students’ styles of
thinking. Howe ve r, Sternberg has cautioned about the possi- thinking in order for them to benefit maximally from the
bility that gifted students might spend their time in group instruction.
work teaching less able children, rather than learning. Finally, this study also has some limitations that are char-
Mo re ove r, Myers and McCaulley (1985b) suggested that acteristic of most compilation analytical studies. First, any lim-
intuitive-introve rted types prefer self-paced learning and courses itations of the original studies are also limitations of this study
that enable them to study on their own initiative. In the same to some degree. In most of the original studies, there was no
line of thinking, projects encourage students to branch out and information about the socioeconomic status of the partici-
create their own work (Sternberg 1999; Sternberg & pants. Also, age and ability were reported broadly or not spec-
Grigorenko, 2000). Hence, project-based learning is more likely ified in some studies. Another limitation comes from the fact
to be preferred by intuitive - i n t rove rted students because they can that, although the MBTI is appropriate for adolescents and
h a ve opportunities to stru c t u re tasks that they like to do. They adults, some part i c i p a n t s’ ages we re as young as 12 in some
also can benefit more from less stru c t u red and inductive studies. Howe ve r, the authors of the original studies claimed
approaches. In addition, an integration of a structured teaching that gifted students reach the adolescent stage cognitively and
model into a less stru c t u red model would provide new, exciting emotionally earlier than those who show a normal deve l o p-
ways in education of intuitive - i n t rove rted gifted students. For mental pattern. Because of these limitations, the findings of
example, an integration of Renzulli’s (1977) unstru c t u red Type this study should be interpreted with special caution to the
III Enrichment model and Parnes’ (1988) stru c t u red Cre a t i ve sample and the instrument characteristics.
Problem Solving would fit into learning characteristics of gifted
students who prefer introversion and intuition, for these mod-
els foster analytical, cre a t i ve, and practical thinking thro u g h References
self-paced learning and group and individual projects.
The preference for objectivity and logical order of thinking References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
types shows in their pre f e rences for small-group discussions, research synthesis.
thought-based questioning (Sternberg, 1999; Sternberg &
Grigorenko, 2000), and carefully structured courses with clear Betts, G. (1985). The Autonomous Learner Model for the gifted
goals (Myers & Mc C a u l l e y, 1985b). Because the majority of and talented. Greeley, CO: Autonomous Learning
mathematically gifted adolescents prefer thinking style in their Publications and Specialists.
judgments, a well-organized and individually paced program of Bireley, M. (1991). Learning styles: One way to help gifted
instruction might encourage them in schools. For example, adolescents understand and choose life styles. In M.
Taba’s Teaching Strategies Program (Schieve r, 1991) would be Bireley & J. Genshaft (Eds.), Understanding the gifted ado-
a useful teaching technique for thinking types because it is lescent: Educational, developmental, and multicultural issues
sequentially structured on analytical and dialectical brain- (pp. 189–200). New York: Teachers Collage Press.
storming about causes and consequences of events, organiza- Chiang, G. C. T. (1991). Student ratings of teachers, teacher
tion of information based on logical and illogical associations, p s ychological types, and teacher classroom behavior: An
and generalization of assumptions. exploratory study in gifted education. (Doctoral dissert a-
Most gifted adolescents are perc e p t i ve types according to tion, No rthwestern Un i ve r s i t y, 1991). Dissertation
this re s e a rch synthesis. Because perc e p t i ve types are more likely Abstracts International, 52, 183.*

Winter 2004, Volume XV, Number 2 77


Sak

Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (1994). The handbook of research Atlas of Type Tables. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
synthesis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Psychologists Press.*
Cordrey, L. J. (1986). A re p o rt on former MGM-GATE students Ma c Daid, G. P., McCaulley, M. H., & Kainz, R. I. (1986b).
at Fullerton College. Fullerton, CA: Fu l l e rton College.* High school students in Florida Fu t u re Scientist Pro g ram in
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychol- Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: Atlas of Type Tables. Palo Alto,
ogy of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins. CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.*
De l b r i d g e - Pa rk e r, L., & Robinson, D. C. (1988). Type and Ma c Daid, G. P., Kainz, R. I., & Mc C a u l l e y, M. H. (1986).
academically gifted adolescents. Jo u rnal of Ps yc h o l o g i c a l Mye r s - Briggs Type Indicator: Atlas of type tables. Palo Alto,
Type, 17, 66–72.* CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Devito, A. J. (1989). Re v i ew of Mye r s - Briggs Type Indicator. Mc C a u l l e y, M. H. (1985). The selection ratio type table.
In J. V. Mitchell (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements Journal of Psychological Type, 10, 45–56.
yearbook ( p p. 1030–1032). Lincoln: University of McCarthy, S. V. (1975). Differential V-Q ability: Twenty years
Nebraska Press. later. Review of Educational Research, 45, 263–282.
Gallagher, S. A. (1987). An analysis of visual-spatial ability, Mc Ginn, P. V. (1976). Verbally gifted youth: Selection and
intellectual efficiency, and learning style on mathematics description. In D. P. Keating (Ed.), Intellectual talent:
achievement of gifted male and gifted female adolescents. Re s e a rch and deve l o p m e n t. Ba l t i m o re: Johns Ho p k i n s
(Doctoral dissertation, The Un i versity of North Caro l i n a University Press.*
at Chapel Hill, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts In t e rn a t i o n a l, Mills, C.J. (1983, April). Personality characteristics of the gifted
48, 134. * adolescents and their parents: Comparisons and implications
Gallagher, S. A. (1990). Personality patterns of the gifted. for achievement and counseling. Paper presented at the
Understanding Our Gifted, 3, 11–13. annual meeting of the American Educational Research
Ge i g e r, R. W. D. (1992). Personality characteristics of highly Association, Montreal.
academically talented adolescents variations with ve r b a l Mills, C. J. (1984, April). Sex differences in self-concept and
or mathematical ability. (Doctoral dissertation, Georgia self-esteem for mathematically precocious adolescents. Paper
University, 1992). D i s s e rtation Abstracts In t e rnational, 53, presented at the annual meeting of the American
156.* Educational Research Association, New Orleans.*
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary and meta-analysis of Mills, C. J., & Pa rk e r, W. D. (1998). Cognitive-psychological
research. Educational Researcher, 5, 3–8. p rofiles of gifted adolescents from Ireland and the U.S.:
Glass, G. V., McGaw, B., & Smith, M. L. (1981). Meta analy- Cross-societal comparisons. In t e rnational Jo u rnal of
sis in social research. London: Sage. International Relations, 22, 1–16.*
Hawkins, J. (1997). Giftedness and psychological types. Myers, I. B. (1980). Gifts differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 9, 57–67.* Psychologists Press.
Hoehn, L., & Birely, M. K. (1988). Mental processing pre f e r- Myers, I. B., & McCaulley, M. H. (1985a). Manual: A guide to
ences of gifted children. Illinois Council for the Gifted the development and use of the Mye r s - Briggs Type Indicator.
Journal, 7, 28–31. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Jackson, C. M. (1989). A comparative study of Mye r s - Br i g g s Myers, I. B., & Mc C a u l l e y, M. H. (1985b). Gifted students
personality type, learning-style factors, and locus of con- from Arlington County: A guide to the development and use
trol of middle school achieving and underachieving intel- of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA:
lectually gifted students. (Doctoral dissert a t i o n , Consulting Psychologists Press.*
Un i versity of Alabama, 1989). D i s s e rtation Ab s t ra c t s Ol s zewski-Kubilius, P. M., & Kulieke, M. J. (1989). Personality
In t e rnational, 50, 123.* dimensions of gifted adolescents: A re v i ew of the empirical
Jung, C. G. (1971). Ps ychological types. In H. Read, M. literature. Gifted Child Qu a rt e rly, 32, 347–352.
Fordham, G. Ad l e r, & W. Mc Gu i re (Eds.), The collected Parnes, S. J. (1988). Visioning: State of the art processes for
w o rks of C. G. Jung, 6. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University encouraging innova t i ve excellence. East Au rora, NY: D.O.K.
Press. Piirto, J. (1990). Pro files of cre a t i veadolescents. Understanding
Ku d e r, G. F. (1968). Kuder Occupational In t e rest Su rvey: Our Gifted, 2, 10–12.
Manual. Chicago: Science Research Associates. Renzulli, J. S. (1977). The enrichment triad model: A guide for
Lawrence, G. (1984). A synthesis of learning style re s e a rch d e veloping defensible programs for the gifted and talented.
involving the MBTI. Jo u rnal of Ps ychological Type, 8, 2–15. Wethersfield, CT: Creative Learning Press.
Ma c Daid, G. P., Mc C a u l l e y, M. H., & Kainz, R. I. (1986a). Rosenthal, R. (1978). Combining results of independent stud-
Gifted high school seniors in Mye r s - Briggs Type In d i c a t o r : ies. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 185–193.

78 The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education


Psychological Types of Gifted Adolescents

Schiever, S. (1991). A compre h e n s i veapproach to teaching think- Tre f finger, D. J. (1975). Teaching for self-directed learning. A
ing. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. priority for the gifted and talented. Gifted Child Quarterly,
Si l verman, L. K. (1985). Personality and learning styles of the 19, 46–59.
gifted children. In J. Van Ta s s e l - Baska (Ed.), Excellence in Wheelwright, J. B., Wheelwright, J. H., & Buehler, H. A.
educating the gifted and talented learners ( p p. 29–65). (1964). Jungian type theory: The Gray-Wheelwright Test
Denver: Love. (16th ed). San Francisco: Society of Jungian Analysts of
Spoto, A. (1995). Ju n g’s typology in perspective ( Re v. ed). Northern California.
Wilmette, IL: Chiron. Williams, R. (1992). Personality characteristics of talented
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Thinking styles. Cambridge: and gifted students as measured by the Myers-Briggs Type
Cambridge University Press. Indicator and Murphy-Meisgeir Type Indicator for chil-
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Teaching for suc- d ren. (Doctoral dissertation, East Texas State University,
cessful intelligence. Arlington Heights, IL: SkyLight. 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 111.*

Winter 2004, Volume XV, Number 2 79

You might also like