You are on page 1of 13

www.elsevier.

com/locate/ynimg
NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437 – 449

Pre-reflective and reflective self-reference: A spatiotemporal


EEG analysis
Michaela Esslen,a,1 Sibylle Metzler,a,1 Roberto Pascual-Marqui,b and Lutz Jancke a,⁎
a
Department of Neuropsychology, University of Zurich, Switzerland
b
The KEY Institute for Brain-Mind Research, University Hospital of Psychiatry, Zurich, Switzerland
Received 30 January 2007; revised 21 January 2008; accepted 24 January 2008
Available online 14 February 2008

Functional imaging studies consistently support the role of the medial ‘self’ (Ghin, 2003). The self is regarded as a multidimensional
prefrontal cortex to be a part of a functional network of reflective self- phenomenon (Gallagher, 2000; Damasio, 1998, 2003; James,
awareness. The current study introduces a new linguistic task which (1) 1890; Neisser, 1988). Consequently, different perspectives and
directly compares self-reference and other-reference, and (2) separates definitions on the self exist, and therefore, different tasks have been
pre-reflective from reflective aspects of self-awareness. Twenty-six
constructed to unravel the neuronal basis of the self. Despite the
healthy volunteers evaluated trait adjectives in reference to the self or a
close friend while a continuous 30-channel EEG was recorded. Low- different material used to induce self-referential processing, many
resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) was used for studies point consistently to cortical midline structures as brain
statistical brain imaging. As expected, the direct comparison of self- regions essential for representing, monitoring, evaluating, and
reference to other-reference revealed the MPFC to be significant and integrating stimuli that are experienced as strongly related to one's
strongly more active during the self-reference condition. Pre-reflective own person (Northoff et al., 2006; Gillihan and Farah, 2005). In
aspects of self seem to be implemented to a greater degree in the ventral, particular, several fMRI and PET studies corroborated activation of
reflective aspects of the self in dorsal parts of the MPFC. In the pre- the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), while subjects reflected on
reflective self condition, brain areas that receive homeostatic afferents their own personality traits or evaluated them as opposed to doing
from somatic and visceral sensation of the body such as the insula and the same for another person (Gusnard et al., 2001; Kjaer et al.,
the somatosensory cortex were strongly activated as early as 134 ms after
2002; Fossati et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2002; Kelley et al., 2002;
stimulus onset. The right inferior parietal lobe seems to be involved in
self-referential processing in general. Kircher et al., 2002; Schmitz et al., 2004).
© 2008 Published by Elsevier Inc. According to William James, any discussion of the self needs to
include the distinction between the self as object (the “Me”) and
Keywords: Self-reference; Medial prefrontal cortex; EEG (electroencepha- the self as subject (the “I”) of experience (James, 1890; Northoff
lography); ERP (event related potential); LORETA et al., 2006). Legrand (2003) states that there is an “obvious but
crucial difference between being reflectively conscious of one's
own perspective on the world and being aware of the world from
one's own perspective”. The subjective first-person perspective
(FPP) is thought to be needed to interact with and reflect on objects
Introduction or other persons. This aspect of consciousness is thought to occur
pre-reflectively and is the basis upon which any other type of self-
The capacity to reflect on and evaluate oneself and others is an consciousness evolves. In contrast, many imaging studies on
important component of human consciousness. Our daily experi- evaluation of trait adjectives focus on the self as object, in other
ences make us feel that we are someone who is experiencing the words, on the self evaluating itself from the third person
world. We commonly refer to this phenomenon by speaking of the perspective (TPP). However, it is possible that while being in the
TPP, reflecting on one's own traits, processes related to FPP may
co-occur. That is, the mental flow, when engaged in higher level
⁎ Corresponding author. Institute of Psychology, Department of Neurop- self-consciousness may involve a rapid change from the pre-
sychology, University of Zurich, Binzmühlestrasse 14, 8050 Zurich,
reflective FPP to the reflective TPP. This difference on the
Switzerland. Fax: +41 44 635 74 09. phenomenological or cognitive level must be related to specific
E-mail address: neuropsychologie@psychologie.unizh.ch (L. Jancke). neuronal activation.
1 Gallagher (2000) defines a pre-reflective aspect of the self
The first and second authors have contributed equally to this publication.
Available online on ScienceDirect (www.sciencedirect.com). which he calls ‘minimal self’. This minimal self enables us to
1053-8119/$ - see front matter © 2008 Published by Elsevier Inc.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.060
438 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

perceive ourselves as the immediate subject of experience, intention was to design a new experimental approach which would
unextended in time. In the verbal domain, this corresponds to the allow us to study the old questions about different aspects of self
use of the personal pronoun “I” (Bermúdez, 1998). Gallagher, awareness in the context of modern cognitive neuroscience. We
referring to Shoemaker (1984), suggests that the access to oneself intended to exclusively focus on the true separation between the
in FPP is immediate and non-observational; that is, it does not perception of the self as subject or object.
involve a perceptual or reflective act of consciousness. In this In this study we will concentrate on these two different aspects
sense, the immediate self that is referred to by saying or reading “I” of the self, the pre-reflective and the reflective self, which are both
is the pre-reflective point of origin for action, experience and thought to operate at different time points and to be controlled by
thought. Besides this pre-reflective self, many authors assume a different neural networks. Our subjects viewed sequentially
reflective self, which Gallagher denotes as ‘narrative self’. The presented personal pronouns and trait adjectives in three-word
narrative self comprises all aspects of our personality and it allows sentences under self-reference and other-reference conditions.
us to understand ourselves in the continuity of time. Gallagher's When reading the personal pronouns, subjects performed a pre-
conception of the narrative self is akin to Damasio's (1999) reflective, minimal reference to the self, and when reading the trait
‘autobiographical self’ while his definition of the minimal self adjectives, they performed a reflective, narrative self-reference.
seems to be related to the “physical self” of William James (1890) Using conventional brain imaging methods such as fMRI or
or Damasio's “proto” or “core self”. A description of the different PET, it would be nearly impossible to disentangle these different
characteristics of the pre-reflective and reflective self is presented aspects, especially the different time course, because of the rather
in Table 1 (for more information about self as a subject or object coarse time resolution of these methods. Therefore, we designed a
see also Legrand, 2007). high time resolution EEG-experiment in combination with
In short, the pre-reflective and reflective self are two aspects of functional electric brain tomography, allowing the calculation of
the self that might interact, but still are very distinct. Both are the intracerebral sources of EEG activity in the millisecond range.
needed to make an individual feel her- or himself as a unique Most fMRI experiments published so far trying to disentangle
personality. While the pre-reflective (minimal) self might also be cortical activations to the perception of the self have not
present in lower species (as they might feel somehow existing), the distinguished between the two perspectives (the self as object or
reflective self can only exist in at least partially self-conscious the self as subject). There are only few fMRI studies (e.g., Ochsner
species, i.e. perhaps in some primates, if not only in humans above et al., 2005) taking the same approach. However, the new aspect of
very early childhood. our study is to continue the research endeavor to distinguish these
We are aware of the fact that our considerations about the self two perspectives by use of electrical tomography, in order to
and its different aspects are somewhat “philosophical”. Indeed, enhance the time resolution of cortical mapping. Our hypothesis is
there is a long-standing discussion within the philosophical but that by use of this technique, neural activations associated with the
also psychological literature whether a difference between these processing of the self as subject or object will occur at different
distinct view-points (early emphasized by William James) really time epochs and brain areas.
exists. Yet, the literature strongly distinguishes the psychological Based on previous brain imaging experiments, we hypothesize
processes associated with the perception of stimuli representing the that the MPFC will be the core region for processes associated with
self as subject or as object as it can also be experienced by the control and generation of the reflective self. With respect to the
ourselves. Each reflection and cognition related to the perception pre-reflective self, we hypothesize that the insula and somatosen-
of the self as subject changes the perspective of the self sory areas will be involved, reminiscent to brain areas which are
immediately to the one of an object: as soon as the “I” reflects engaged in the control of the proto-self as proposed by Damasio
about itself it changes to “me”. In other words, we cannot (Damasio, 2003).
contemplate our “self“ as subject but only as an object, since the The aim of this paper was to study the dynamic activation of
perspectives of subject and object exclude each other. This is the brain areas involved in pre-reflective and reflective self-reference.
theoretical assumption on which this study is based. When talking about self-reference, we are assuming a self capable
We are aware of the fact that there are currently no specific data of linguistic communication—i.e., the person is capable of using
available which are exactly supporting our idea. However, our the first-person pronoun (Gallagher, 2000). But because the
mechanisms of the first-person perspective and the reference to a
minimal self remain largely pre-reflective and not accessible to
Table 1 language, the concept seems to lie between what Gillihan and Farah
The different characteristics of the pre-reflective and reflective self (2005) call “physical self” and “psychological self”. Therefore, we
Pre-reflective self Reflective self hypothesized that the MPFC would be activated in both self-
reference induced by personal pronouns and trait adjectives. In
Minimal self Narrative self
particular, we suggested that dorsal and lateral parts of this brain
“I” “me”
area, which constitute the more cognitive part of the prefrontal
first person perspective (FPP) third person perspective (TPP)
Unextended in time (dateless), “Me” in the continuity of time as a cortex, would be activated in the trait-adjective condition and more
immediate subject of experience, subject with a personal autobiography, ventral parts in the personal pronoun condition (Damasio, 2003,
point of origin for action, comprises all characteristics of 1999; Ochsner et al., 2005).
experience and thought the personality, Since we are using a brain imaging method, which allows
Other denotations: proto self, Other denotations: autobiographical cortical mapping of psychological functions in the milliseconds
core self, physical self self range, we are able to examine the dynamic evolvement of cortical
Explored in this study when Explored in this study when subjects activations in the context of self-consciousness. Based on
subjects are reading personal are reading trait adjectives Damasio's theory of what he calls a proto-self, we hypothesize
pronouns (I / he / she)
that the brain activations during the generation of the pre-reflective
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 439

self will antecede those activations accompanying the generation of Services, Germany) according to the international 10-10 system,
the reflective self. with the following locations: Fp1/2, F7/8, F3/4, FCz, Fz, FT7/8,
FC3/4, T7/8, C3/4, Cz, TP7/8, CP3/4, CPz, P7/8, P3/4, Pz, O1/2
Materials and methods and Oz. Two electrooculogram (EOG) channels were placed below
the outer canthi of each eye. Recording reference was FCz, with
Subjects off-line re-referencing to average-reference. Digital sampling rate
was 500 Hz, on-line filtering 0.1–100 Hz, off-line filtering 0.5–
Twenty-six healthy volunteers (13 females, 13 males, mean age 30 Hz, impedance was kept below 10 kOhm. Subjects were sitting
25, SD 2.8), took part in the study and gave their written informed on a comfortable chair with their head on a customizable head rest
consent. All subjects were consistently right-handed (Annett to minimize head motion. The recording room was dimmed and the
handedness questionnaire, Annett, 1970) and native speakers of subjects were wearing earplugs during the entire recording.
Swiss German. None of the subjects reported any neurological or Before the actual experiment, 5 min resting EEG and 1 min
psychiatric disease, prior head trauma, sensory impairment or checkerboard stimulation (flickering in 2 Hz between black and
subjective cognitive impairment. Besides these exclusion criteria, white) was recorded in order to inspect spontaneous EEG and the
subjects were required not to consume alcohol, drugs or any visual evoked potential. For the comparison of the personal
medication (except contraception) 48 h before the experiment. pronouns, a control condition was recorded in order to correct for
After completion of EEG data analysis, two subjects were excluded the different physical features of the words Ich, Sie and Er (I/she/
from group analysis due to excessive motion artifacts. he), due to the different number of letters. The aim was to ensure
that any difference in brain activation was due to different semantic
Experimental task meaning of the word and not the different visual stimulation.
Therefore, participants were stimulated in randomized order with
Participants were shown 3-word German sentences, word by the following letter strings on the center of the computer screen:
word, on a computer screen. They had to make a yes/no decision on “hcI” (for “Ich”), “iSe” (for “Sie”) and “rE” (for “Er”) (exchanged
the third word of each sentence. All third words were randomized letter condition, EL). Each letter string was presented for 200 ms.
trait adjectives in two different conditions: self-reference (SR) (e.g. Subjects had to state if he or she had realized that the exchanged
“I am happy”), and other-reference (OR) (“She/He is happy”). In the letters were contrasts of the personal pronouns, and were excluded
SR condition participants were required to evaluate if the presented from further analysis if this was the case. Afterwards the
word was descriptive to themselves, and in the OR condition if they participants were stimulated with the three words German
were descriptive to two close, pre-defined, and equally well known sentences (see Experimental task).
friends or relatives, one female and one male, whereas romantic All recorded EEG epochs were carefully and individually
relations were excluded. During the entire experiment subjects were checked for artifacts (e.g. eye blinks, horizontal and vertical eye
requested to do the evaluation in the OR condition in reference to the movements, muscle artifacts). Epochs that were associated even
same male and female person. Participants were informed that the with small changes in the horizontal or vertical EOG channel within
investigator was blind to their actual response. the test interval were rejected (amplitude minimum, − 100 μV;
Fifty trait adjectives were drawn from the German multi- amplitude maximum, 100 μV).
dimensional questionnaire “Eigenschaftswörterliste” (EWL-K, Event-related potentials (ERPs) of 700 ms duration were
Janke and Debus, 2006). The adjectives describe, covering a calculated after the letter strings of the EL condition, and under the
broad spectrum, the momentary mental and physical condition of a two conditions SR and OR, after each personal pronoun (pre-
person in the following six fields: performance-related activity reflective self), and each adjective (reflective self).
(e.g. eager), inactiveness, extra-/introversion (e.g. social), well- Before the actual main comparisons were carried out, the
being (e.g. happy), emotional bad temper and fear (e.g. nervous). activation due to the exchanged letters was subtracted from the one
Twenty-five adjectives of each, positive and negative valence, were due to the personal pronouns, thus creating a “corrected” ERP con-
chosen. The same set of 50 trait adjectives was presented in three trast used in the statistical analyses. In the trait adjective condition,
different types of sentences, either starting with the personal brain activation followed by exactly the same visual stimuli but
pronoun “I”, or “he” or “she”, followed by a corresponding verb, different instructions was compared and therefore no control con-
and ending with one of the 50 possible adjectives, which resulted dition was needed. For the OR condition, ERPs following words with
in 150 different sentences (e.g. I am shy; she is intelligent). Each male and female reference were summed and averaged.
word was presented in the center of the screen for 200 ms, Several steps of analysis were performed:
followed by a fixation cross presented for 500 ms. After the last
fixation cross a question mark appeared until the subject responded 1) Analysis of time-average LORETA images: In order to compare
by a key-press on a computer keyboard (right for yes, left for no; source localization based on EEG data with fMRI findings,
response keys alternated for each experiment). Response and which do not have the fine time resolution in the millisecond
reaction time were recorded. Subjects were asked to not give the range of the EEG, we compared the SR and OR conditions,
answer before the appearance of the question mark to avoid based on the respective time averages over 350 instantaneous
confounding of the ERPs with early motor components. LORETA images from 0 to 700 ms post stimulus (i.e. starting
with the onset of the word), using statistical non-parametric
EEG recording and analysis mapping methodology (Nichols and Holmes, 2002).
2) High time resolution analysis: In order to follow the dynamic
A 30-channel EEG was recorded (BrainAmp, BrainProducts, time course of brain activation within the 700 ms time interval, a
Germany). Silver/silver-chloride-electrodes placement was aided topographic analysis of variance (TANOVA, Strik et al., 1998)
by an application cap (Easy Cap System, FMS Falk Minow was applied to compare ERP maps obtained under different
440 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

Fig. 1. ERP potentials of the four different pre-reflective conditions over 700 ms. Black line: ERP potentials of the pre-reflective self condition (i.e. the averaged
ERPs over all subjects when reading the word Ich (I)); green line: ERP potentials of the pre-reflective others condition (i.e. the averaged ERPs over all subjects
when reading the words Sie and Er (she and he)); red line: ERP potentials of the correction condition “hcI” as ERP over all subjects; blue line: ERP potentials of
the correction condition “rE and iSe” as ERP over all subjects at three midline electrodes Fz, Cz, and Oz. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this
figure legend,the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 441

Fig. 2. ERP potentials of the two reflective self conditions when reading adjectives regarding the subject him-/herself (reflective self condition) and the imagined
others (reflective others condition) over 700 ms. Black line: reflective self condition; red line: reflective others condition. (For interpretation of the references to
colour in this figure legend,the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

conditions. On a time-frame by time-frame basis, this procedure a current density value (A/m²) based on the recorded scalp electric
tests for differences between scalp potentials (i.e. maps, potential differences (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994). LORETA
topographies). As explained in detail in Strik et al. (1998), these reveals an estimated solution of the inverse problem based on the
statistical comparisons are carried out using the non-parametric assumption that the smoothest of all possible activities is the most
randomization methodology, which corrects for multiple testing plausible one. This assumption is supported by electrophysiology,
(time and electrodes). Periods of ERP maps that differed sig- where neighbouring neuronal populations show highly correlated
nificantly between SR and OR were denoted as ‘time segments’. activity (Haalmann and Vaadia, 1997). Due to the low spatial
3) For all time segments longer than 10 ms, statistical analyses of resolution property of this imaging method, it should be kept in
functional LORETA images were performed. This step is mind that localization results might suffer from some uncertainty in
justified because different scalp potentials can only be due to terms of spatial extent.
different neuronal source distributions. The version of LORETA employed in this study implies a
three-shell spherical head model registered to the digitized
LORETA Talairach and Tournoux atlas (Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal
Neurological Institute). The registration between spherical and
LORETA is a method that computes a three dimensional Talairach and Tournoux head geometry used the realistic EEG
distribution of electrically active neuronal generators in the brain as electrode coordinates reported by Towle et al. (1993). Source
442 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

Fig. 3. LORETA statistical images comparing self reference to other reference in the pre-reflective self condition. 0–700 ms (time-average). Cortex shown in gray
scale, increased activity (p b 0.01) in the SR condition compared to OR is labeled red. A, anterior; P, posterior; S, superior; I, inferior; L, left hemisphere, R, right
hemisphere. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend,the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

solution space was limited to cortical gray matter and hippocampi, dimension: 7 mm3). It has been shown in numerous studies that the
according to the digitized probability atlas provided by the method is capable of correct localization with fairly low errors
Montreal Neurologic Institute (number of voxels: 2394, voxel (Menendez et al., 2001; Phillips et al., 2002), and has received

Fig. 4. LORETA statistical images comparing self reference to other reference in the reflective self condition. 0–700 ms (time-average). Legends and color
coding as in Fig. 3.
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 443

Table 2
Detailed LORETA results of significant differences in brain electrical activity in the time-average analysis (0–700 ms) during self reference compared to other
reference in the pre-reflective (upper part of Table 1) and reflective (lower part of Table 1) self conditions
Time segment t-threshold Activation in left hemisphere Activation in right hemisphere Activation in medial areas
in ms Cerebral region Brodmann Cerebral region Brodmann areas Cerebral region Brodmann
areas (xyz-coordinates) [t-value] (xyz-coordinates) [t-value] areas (xyz-coordinates) [t-value]
Pre-reflective self condition
1–700 15.5 Insula 13 (−38, − 11, 15) [22.0] Superior frontal gyrus 6 (25, 10, 57) [18.3] –
Superior frontal gyrus 10 (− 10, 66, 22) [21.3]
Precentral gyrus 6 (−59, − 4, 29) [18.1]
Inferior frontal gyrus 11 (− 24, 38, −20)
[15.6]

Reflective self condition


1–700 11.00 Superior frontal gyrus 10 (− 17, 59, 29) [15.5] Precentral gyrus 4 (60, -− 4, 29) [15.1] Medial frontal gyrus
Superior frontal gyrus 6 (− 10, 24, 64) [11.7] Insula 13 (46, − 11, 15) [12.4] 10 (− 10, 59, 8) [13.2]
Superior temporal gyrus 21 (− 66, − 18, −13) Postcentral gyrus 43 (53, − 11, 15) [12.4]
[11.6] Superior frontal gyrus 8 (11, 52, 43) [11.9]
Superior frontal gyrus 6 (18, 17, 64) [11.8]
Numbers in parentheses give MNI coordinates (x, y, z) in mm, for p b 0.01. Brodmann areas correspond to MNI coordinates corrected to Talairach coordinates.

important validation from studies combining LORETA with precuneus. In the third time segment (418–450 ms post
functional MRI (Vitacco et al., 2002) and with PET (Pizzagalli stimulus), significant differences were found in the right
et al., 2004; Zumsteg et al., 2005). inferior parietal lobe, inferior frontal gyrus and right
medial prefrontal cortex. In the left hemisphere, regions in
Results the frontal and temporal lobe including the fusiform gyrus
were activated. In the fourth time segment (458–476 ms
Figs. 1 and 2 show the basic ERP data used in the analyses. post stimulus), the medial frontal cortex, including parts
of the anterior cingulum, as well as right frontal and
(1) Time-average LORETA analysis: In both the pre- precentral–parietal regions and the insula showed sig-
reflective and the reflective self-conditions, self-reference nificant activation. For details see Fig. 5 and Table 3.
induced consistently stronger activation than other- Interestingly, in the reflective self condition the TANOVA
reference. Figs. 3 and 4 reveal the qualitative similarity analysis revealed only one time segment with signifi-
of the activation in the two self-referential tasks: Fig. 3 cantly different activation, lasting from 254 to 264 ms
illustrates the LORETA image for the pre-reflective self post stimulus, with significant difference in the right
condition, and Fig. 4 for the reflective self condition. inferior parietal lobe. For details see Fig. 6 and Table 4.
In the pre-reflective self condition, SR compared to OR
was characterized by significant differences in the left Discussion
insula, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), the
ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), the right The results of the present study shed new light on the time
course and the different neuronal networks involved in the
dorsolateral frontal cortex and in a small area on the left
implementation of the pre-reflective and the reflective self
precentral gyrus. In the reflective self condition, SR
reference. Both are implemented at least partially in distinct brain
compared to OR revealed significant activation of the areas. During the entire first 700 ms after stimulus presentation,
DMPFC, spreading from the right precentral gyrus to the activation in the pre-reflective self condition was located in the left
frontal pole, with the left frontal cortex slightly stronger insula, the dorsal and ventral MPFC, and left precentral gyrus.
activated than the right. Superior and medial frontal gyri on During the reflective self condition, activation was located in the
the right frontal lobe and the underlying insula exhibited dorsal MPFC, right insula, right precentral gyrus, and right
activation. Additionally, the left medial temporal gyrus superior and medial frontal gyri. Furthermore, EEG high time
showed significant activation. Table 2 provides the details resolution revealed different activation in the pre-reflective self
of significant activations and their locations. condition, as early as 134 ms post stimulus, repeatedly involving
(2) LORETA high time resolution analysis: In the pre- the insulae and the right inferior parietal lobe.
The pre-reflective self is thought to consist of two closely
reflective self condition, as early as 134–170 ms post
related aspects: the sense of agency and the sense of ownership,
stimulus, several regions of the brain were activated
e.g. the sense that I am thinking my own thoughts or that it is my
including the left and right frontal and temporal cortex body that is moving. Sense of agency has been investigated by
and insula and midline structures in the anterior cingulum. manipulation of experienced agency, imagery of actions or
In the second significantly activated time segment (198– imitation of other persons (Blakemore and Frith, 2003). Farrer
234 ms post stimulus), activation was located in the right and Frith (2002) revealed that being aware of causing an action is
inferior parietal lobe, right medial frontal gyrus, and the associated with activation in the anterior insula, whereas attributing
444 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

Fig. 5. LORETA statistical images for the time-dependant comparisons of self reference to other reference in the pre-reflective self condition. First row: 134–
170 ms. Second row: 198–234 ms. Third row: 418–450 ms. Fourth row: 458–476 ms. Legends and color coding as in Fig. 3.

it to another person is associated with activation in the inferior processes and the latter in complex cognitive operations (for
parietal cortex. Furthermore, simulation of one's own actions from review see Drevets and Raichle, 1998; Kringelbach and Rolls,
the first versus third person point of view led to activation in the 2004). This suggestion is supported by neuroanatomical findings,
inferior parietal cortex, insula and somatosensory cortex in the left revealing dense interconnections of the VMPFC with brain areas
hemisphere (Ruby and Decety, 2001). In a fMRI study of Vogeley associated with emotional and motivational processes (the nucleus
et al. (2001), subjects read short stories written either from a first accumbens, the amygdalae and the basal ganglia). The dorsal areas
person perspective or from a third person perspective and were lack these strong links to emotional and motivational areas.
afterwards asked to ascribe adequate behavior, attitudes, or According to (Ochsner et al., 2005) this pattern of neuroanatomical
perceptions according to the given plot. Self-perspective led to interconnection might facilitate the re-representation of the
enhanced neuronal activity in the right temporoparietal area and the information in the DMPFC in a symbolic (emotion-free form)
anterior cingulum. The current study extends these findings and is supporting explicit reasoning about these states. In the current
the first one that introduces a linguistic task in order to activate the study, the reflective self condition produced a robust activation in
networks underlying both, the sense of agency aspect of the pre- the DMPFC, showing no activation in the VMPFC, in the time-
reflective self and the reflective self. average condition as well as in the time-dependant condition. In
A major finding of the present study is the differential contrast, the pre-reflective self condition activated large parts of the
involvement of the ventral and dorsal parts of the PFC. In the VMPFC, and lesser activations in the DMPFC. Our findings
reflective self condition, only dorsal parts of the MPFC were support the involvement of the DMPFC in more cognitively
activated, while the pre-reflective self condition showed strong oriented tasks such as evaluation of trait adjectives, as well as the
involvement of the ventral MPFC. Imaging studies and neuroana- involvement of ventral parts of the MPFC in more intuitive,
tomical findings suggest a functional distinction between the affective and thus pre-reflective forms of self-referential proces-
ventromedial (VMPFC) and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex sing. The concept of the pre-reflexive minimal self is defined as
(DMPFC). The first is more involved in emotional and affective ‘consciousness of oneself as an immediate subject of experience,
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 445

Table 3
Detailed LORETA results of the high time resolution analysis with significant differences in brain electrical activity during self reference compared to other
reference in the pre-reflective condition
Time segment t-threshold Activation in left hemisphere Activation in right hemisphere Activation in medial areas
in ms cerebral region Brodmann areas cerebral region Brodmann areas cerebral region Brodmann areas
(xyz-coordinates) [t-value] (xyz-coordinates) [t-value] (xyz-coordinates) [t-value]
134–170 4.58 Inferior temporal gyrus Insula Anterior cingulate
20 (−45, − 25, − 27) [7.1] 13 (39,-4,8) [6.3] 24/32 (-3,24,22) [5.6]
Superior frontal gyrus Middle temporal gyrus Cingulate gyrus
10 (−24, 45, 29) [6.6] 20 (53,-4,-34) [5.3] 24/32 (-10,31,29) [5.6]
Precentral gyrus Middle frontal gyrus Anterior cingulate
6 (−59, 3, 22) [5.6] 6/10 (39,-4,57)[4.8] 24/32 (-10,45,-6) [5.4]
Superior frontal gyrus Superior frontal gyrus Medial Frontal Gyrus
8 (−24, 17, 43) [5.6] 6/10 (18, 59, 15) [4.8] 10 (11,38,-6) [5.4]
Fusiform gyrus Postcentral gyrus Medial Frontal Gyrus
20 (−52, − 46, 22) [5.5] 1 (67, − 25, 36) [4.6] 10 (-3,45,-6) [5.4]
Inferior frontal gyrus Cingulate gyrus
10/11 (−38, 52, 1) [4.9] 24 (-3,-11,29) [5.1]
Insula Superior parietal lobe
13 (−45, − 4, 8) [4.6] 7 (-10,-74,57) [5.0]
198–234 4.61 – Inferior parietal lobe Precuneus
40 (46, − 60, 43) [5.1] 7 (4,-60,50) [5.1]
Middle frontal gyrus
46 (46, 17, 29) [5.1]
418–450 4.53 Inferior temporal gyrus Inferior parietal lobe Medial frontal gyrus
20 (−59, − 25, − 27) [5.5] 40 (60, − 39, 36) [7.1] 11 (11, 66, -13) [5.2]
Fusiform gyrus 6 (60, − 4, 36) [5.1]
37 (−59, − 53, − 20) [5.4] Inferior frontal gyrus
Superior temporal gyrus 47 (46, 31, −13) [4.4]
22 (−52, − 18, − 6) [4.7]
Inferior frontal gyrus
47 (−17, 24, − 20) [4.7]
Superior temporal gyrus
22 (−52, − 18, − 8) [4.7]
Inferior frontal gyrus
46 (−52, 31, 15) [4.6]
Supramarginal gyrus
40 (−59, − 60, 36) [4.6]
Precentral gyrus
3 (−66, −11, 29) [4.6]
Precuneus
19 (−38, − 81, 36) [4.6]
458–476 4.62 – Precentral gyrus Anterior cingulate
6 (60, − 4, 36) [6.9] 32 (-3, 38, -6) [8.0]
Middle frontal gyrus Medial frontal gyrus
6 (32, − 4, 50) [5.5] 11 (4, 45, -13) [8.0]
Superior frontal gyrus
10 (25, 66,15) [5.4]
Insula
13 (46, − 11,8) [5.0]
Superior parietal lobe
7 (18, − 74, 57) [4.7]
For details see legend of Table 1 and text.

unextended in time’ (Gallagher, 2000). Thus, we suggest that the comparison with others. Activation during the task was found in
concept of a minimal self is associated to a large extent with the MPFC, ventral anterior cingulate and posterior cingulate
neuronal activation in the VMPFC and could be distinguished from cortex. The authors distinguish between neural circuits for
the reflective and conscious narrative self, associated with neuronal cognitive aspects of self-reflection, i.e. the personal relevance of
activation in the DMPFC. information with activation in MPFC, and emotional aspects of
Activation of the MPFC seems to be essential for self- self-reflection with activation in the ventral anterior cingulate
referential processing. Moran and colleagues (2006) found cortex. Since we did not consider the valence of the presented
interesting results in a fMRI study comparing affective and stimuli in our study, we cannot distinguish if activation found in
cognitive aspects of self reflection. Their subjects had to decide if our study in MPFC and anterior cingulate cortex are a result of
adjectives would describe themselves or not, but there was no cognitive or emotional processes during self-reflective processes.
446 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

Fig. 6. LORETA statistical images for the time-dependant comparison of self reference to other reference in the reflective self condition. 254–264 ms. Legends
and color coding as in Fig. 1.

Interestingly, we found weaker activation on the right-sided of the current study activated during 134–170 ms post stimulus
MPFC in the self reflective condition compared to the pre- large parts of the brain besides the VMPFC, including the anterior
reflective self condition. Stuss et al. (2001) showed that patients cingulate gyrus, the left lateral prefrontal cortex, right temporal pole
with right hemispheric MPFC lesions were incapable of discover- and the left temporoparietal junction. In an interesting LORETA
ing the deception of a protagonist, a task requiring attribution of study, Ofek and Pratt (2005) presented particular first names as
mental state. Similarly, Vogeley et al. (2001) showed in their fMRI subjectively significant stimuli to their subjects. They could
study that first and third person perspective activated overlapping demonstrate the involvement of a neural network for emotion
regions in the right prefrontal cortex. Yet, in the present study, no processing when hearing subjectively significant names, including
prominent right-sided activation in the MPFC could be found. the prefrontal cortex, with distinct brain activity as early as 200 ms
Possibly, the right MPFC was activated during SR and OR, and the after stimulus onset. Hearing the word “I/he/she” or the first name of
direct comparison resulted in an elimination of the activation. oneself or another important person might to some extent generate
Comparison to a baseline condition might have answered the the same brain activation, concurrently pre-reflective and uncon-
question of ‘shared representations between self and other’ (Decety scious, as well as emotionally highly important.
and Sommerville, 2003; Lawrence et al., 2006). Possible failings in internal validity should be taken into con-
In the pre-reflective self condition, the temporal analyses sideration. As described above, the VMPC has been associated
showed that the VMPFC was repeatedly activated very early at with modality and task independent emotional processing (Phan
134–170 ms post stimulus, later at 418–450 ms post stimulus and et al., 2002). Thus, the activity in the VMPFC might be due to a
finally at 458–476 ms post stimulus. Kawasaki et al. (2001) greater emotional involvement during self-reference. Although the
uncovered comparable response latencies as early as 120–170 ms participants were assured of confidential treatment of their
after onset of an emotional stimuli by using intracranial single-cell responses, the sincere evaluation of their own traits might have
recordings during emotional processing in human ventral pre- produced more arousal and anxiety than the evaluation of their
frontal cortex. By contrast, lateral prefrontal regions and more friends’ traits. It is even possible that the emotional processing
posterior located brain regions showed delayed responses of 300– started, irrespective of the following trait adjective, as soon as the
800 ms (Schore, 2003). Schore (2003) suggested a close relation- subjects read “I” as initial word of a sentence.
ship between the VMPFC and the self, considering this structure as Of particular interest is the repeated bilateral activation of
the “entrance door to consciousness”. He further assumed that the somatosensory areas in the self-reference condition. In the current
convergence of the interoceptive and exteroceptive stimuli is not study, the brain response to the pre-reflective self produced
processed consciously, but rather pre-consciously. Additionally, activation in the insula during the time segment over 700 ms, while
later processing in other brain regions is needed for the self- the time dependant analysis revealed activation in the right insula at
referential processing to become conscious. This might involve 134–170 ms and at 458–476 ms. These structures have been
cortical midline structures as proposed by Northoff and Bermpohl associated with what has been called “bodily self” or “proto-self”
(2004), like the dorsal anterior cingulum, DMPFC and posterior (Damasio, 1999; Craig, 2002; Panksepp, 2005). The insula receives
cingulum. In agreement with these findings, self-referential stimuli homeostatic afferents from several modalities, including tempera-
ture, pain, proprioception, and viscera and, as a result, is involved in
the mapping of body sensations (Craig, 2002). The insula seems to
Table 4 constitute a primary interoceptive image of homoeostatic afferents
Detailed LORETA results of the high time resolution analysis with and generates together with subcortical midline regions a “virtual
significant differences in brain electrical activity during self reference body” (Northoff et al., 2006) which serves as platform for the
compared to other reference in the reflective self condition emergence of additional higher levels of the self. Phan et al. (2004)
Time t- Activation in Activation in Activation in report bilateral anterior insula activation during a self-referential task
segment threshold left hemisphere right hemisphere medial areas to be even stronger when stimuli were perceived as emotionally
in ms Cerebral region cerebral region cerebral region more intense. The human ability to experience the self as a distinct
Brodmann areas Brodmann areas Brodmann areas entity, that is, to be self-aware, seems to be associated with the ability
(xyz-coordinates) (xyz-coordinates) (xyz-coordinates) of introspection (Craig, 2002), while the homeostatic needs of our
[t-value] [t-value] [t-value]
body manifest as feelings in our consciousness. As Damasio (1993)
254–264 4.80 – Inferior parietal – proposed, consciousness seems to be more than simple awareness of
lobe 40 (67, − the environment. To be able to experience the feeling of “mineness”
32, 36) [5.3] (Metzinger, 2003) of one's own body, and experience a cognitive
For details see legend of Table 1 and text. continuity over one's own mind, we need a self-awareness that
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 447

“engenders the fundamental image of the physical self as a feeling 2007). Yet, most of the regions found activated in this study were
(sentient) entity” (Damasio, 1993). We therefore suggest that the described to be involved in self-processing. Another reason for
activation in the insula is crucial for processes of pre-reflective self different results might lie in the new design of this study. During the
reference such as self perception or the fundamental feeling of being. subjects’ task, two different self-related functions could be identified:
Reading the personal pronoun “I” in the current study lead to pre-reflective and reflective processing. In this study, we could
repeated activation of the inferior parietal lobe during 198–234 ms identify different brain areas and time courses for the two functions.
and 418–450 ms post stimulus. Previous neuroimaging studies Nevertheless, it is a potential limitation of the study that pre-reflective
investigating pre-reflective, minimal aspects of self such as agency self processing, as the more basic function, might always be present in
(Decety and Sommerville, 2003) and perspective taking (Ruby and all self-related processing, thus also potentially during the reflective
Decety, 2001; Vogeley et al., 2001) reported the inferior parietal processing. This implies that the two processes are inseparably
lobe to be involved in self related processing, irrespective of the correlated on the higher level (i.e. reflective self), although not always
different cognitive demands of the tasks. Berlucchi and Aglioti causally, i.e. the pre-reflective processing is not necessarily occurring
(1997) suggested that the implementation of the body schema before the reflective, since the processes might alternate. The more
occurs in the right inferior parietal lobe. Besides, the experience of basic pre-reflective processes on the other hand, can occur
a normal self seems to include the experience of the self as residing independently.
within the limits of one's body (Blanke, 2005). Such an experience Although the pre-reflective and reflective conditions of this study
has been shown to be disturbed in a patient suffering spontaneous differ considerably, there is no order confound in the results, since
out-of-body experiences caused by epileptic seizures originating in these conditions were never compared directly. The order of the
the right temporoparietal junction (Blanke et al., 2002). A recent words in the sentences cannot be exchanged, i.e. the adjective cannot
evoked potential study showed the selective activation of this area be presented before the pronoun. Not only that the sentences would
during 330–400 ms after healthy subjects imagined themselves in be very unusual, but also when reading e.g. “Happy am I”/ “Happy is
the position which people generally reported when experiencing he/she”, the subject would be involved in unwanted tasks: 1) the
themselves to be out of their body (Blanke and Arzy, 2005). This subject would not know how to reflect on the adjective (happy),
leads to the assumption that a reference to the self is accompanied because he/she has no information yet about the labeling (does the
by a representation of the body in its “egocentric reference frame” adjective describe him/her self or the others?) and thus we could only
(Vogeley et al., 2001). Contrary to our expectation, evaluation of test language processing and to some extent expectations of the
trait adjectives also produced activation in the right inferior parietal upcoming labeling task; 2) reading the verb (am/is) would im-
lobe during 254–264 ms post stimulus. Hence this area seems to be mediately reveal the upcoming pronoun (am for I / is for he/she) and
in general, i.e. modality independent, involved in the processing of therefore immediately trigger self-referencing processes before the
self-referential stimuli (Lou et al., 2004). actual pronoun (I /he/she) is presented. Reading the pronoun would
Finally, we also found stronger intracerebral activations within then lead to pre-reflective and reflective processes at the same time
the left fusiform gyrus during the presentation of the expression “I” and confound them.
The fusiform area is known to code the skill level in perceiving
objects, the so called expertise effects (Gauthier et al., 2000). Conclusion
Gauthier and colleagues tested bird and car experts with in a fMRI
study during tasks with faces, familiar objects, cars and birds and This study replicates and extends, in an EEG-LORETA design,
found significant expertise effects, i.e. enhanced activation in the previous findings of the MPFC as being crucially involved in self-
fusiform gyrus and occipital lobe. On this basis, it might be argued referential processing. Additionally, we show that pre-reflective
that reading the word “I” evokes stronger activations than reading and reflective aspects of the self recruit different neuronal
adjectives describing a person because humans are to some extent substrates, and we therefore suggest that in forthcoming studies a
“experts” about themselves and are instantaneously and directly careful analysis of the phenomenal requirements of self-referential
affected by the word “I”. In line with this interpretation are the tasks be performed. Furthermore, our data strongly supports the
findings of another very interesting study on self information involvement of structures that do not lie on the cortical midline,
processing by Kircher et al. (2000). In their fMRI study, they found such as the insulae and the right inferior parietal lobe, in the
prominent activation in the left fusiform gyrus when comparing self neuronal network of structures underlying the multidimensional
versus non-self judgments based on pictures of faces and personality phenomenon of self awareness.
trait words describing themselves. The left fusiform gyrus was
activated irrespective of stimulus modality (faces or words), which Acknowledgment
demonstrates that “information matching the internal self concept is
being implicitly recognized as such, at an early processing level. This work is partly funded under the European Union FET
Thus, the modality independent signal changes in the left fusiform project PRESENCCIA (Contract Number 27731).
gyrus for self referent information” (Kircher et al., 2000, p. 141). We
assume that very similar processes were also present when our References
subjects would read the word “I”, since they knew immediately that
the upcoming task would relate to their very own person. Annett, M., 1970. A classification of hand preference by association
analysis. Br. J. Psychol. 61, 303–321.
In this study, a distinct network of regions was identified as
Berlucchi, G., Aglioti, S., 1997. The body in the brain: neural bases of
compared to previous studies. One possible explanation is our use of
corporeal awareness. Trends Neurosci. 20, 560–564.
high time resolution EEG imaging instead of measurements of Bermúdez, J.L., 1998. The Paradox of Self-Consciousness. MIT Press,
metabolic changes (fMRI and PET). It should not be surprising that Cambridge, MA.
these two different modalities yield non-identical results, since they Blakemore, S.J., Frith, C., 2003. Self-awareness and action. Curr. Opin.
are complimentary but often times non-related (see e.g. Lachaux et al., Neurobiol. 13, 219–224.
448 M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449

Blanke, O., Arzy, S., 2005. The out-of-body experience: disturbed self- Kringelbach, M.L., Rolls, E.T., 2004. The functional neuroanatomy of the
processing at the temporo-parietal junction. Neurosci. 11, 16–24. human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neurop-
Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., Seeck, M., 2002. Stimulating illusory sychology. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 341–372.
own-body perceptions. Nature 419, 269–270. Lachaux, J.P., Fonlupt, P., Kahane, P., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D., Bertrand,
Craig, A.D., 2002. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the O., Baciu, M., 2007. Relationship between task-related gamma
physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev., Neurosci. 3, 655–666. oscillations and BOLD signal: New insights from combined fMRI and
Damasio, A.R., 1993. Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human intracranial EEG. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1368–1375.
Brain. Putnam, New York. Lawrence, E.J., Shaw, P., Giampietro, V.P., Surguladze, S., Brammer, M.J.,
Damasio, A.R., 1998. Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human David, A.S., 2006. The role of 'shared representations' in social
Brain. Avon, New York. perception and empathy: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 29, 1173–1184.
Damasio, A.R., 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in Legrand, D., 2007. Subjectivity and the body: Introducing basic forms of
the Making of Consciousness. Heinemann, London. self-consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 577–582.
Damasio, A., 2003. Feelings of emotion and the self. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. Legrand, D., 2003. How not to find the neural signature of self-consciousness.
1001, 253–261. Conscious. Cogn. 12, 544–546.
Decety, J., Sommerville, J.A., 2003. Shared representations between self and Lou, H.C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J.P., Nowak, M., Kjaer, T.W.,
other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 527–533. Sackeim, H.A., Lisanby, S.H., 2004. Parietal cortex and representation
Drevets, W.C., Raichle, M.E., 1998. Suppression of regional cerebral blood of the mental Self. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 6827–6832.
during emotional versus higher cognitive implications for interactions Menendez, R.G.D., Andino, S.G., Lantz, G., Michel, C.M., Landis, T., 2001.
between emotion and cognition. Cogn. Emot. 12, 353–385. Noninvasive localization of electromagnetic epileptic activity. I. Method
Farrer, C., Frith, C.D., 2002. Experiencing oneself vs another person as descriptions and simulations. Brain Topogr. 14, 131–137.
being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of Metzinger, T., 2003. Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity.
agency. NeuroImage 15, 596–603. MIT Press, Cambridge.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S.J., Graham, S.J., Grady, C., Keightley, M.L., Craik, F., Moran, J.M., Macrae, C.N., Heatherton, T.F., Wyland, C.L., Kelley, W.M.,
Mayberg, H., 2003. In search of the emotional self: an FMRI study 2006. Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective
using positive and negative emotional words. Am. J. Psychiatr. 160, components of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1586–1594.
1938–1945. Neisser, U., 1988. Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philos. Psychol. 1, 35–59.
Gallagher, S., 2000. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for
cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 14–21. functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15,
Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J.C., Anderson, A.W., 2000. Expertise for 1–25.
cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nat. Northoff, G., Bermpohl, F., 2004. Cortical midline structures and the self.
Neurosci. 3, 191–197. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 102–107.
Ghin, M., 2003. Book Review: Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H.,
Subjectivity by Thomas Metzinger. Human Nat. Rev. 3, 450–454. Panksepp, J., 2006. Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-
Gillihan, S.J., Farah, M.J., 2005. Is self special? A critical review of analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage 31, 440–457.
evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Ochsner, K.N., Beer, J.S., Robertson, E.R., Cooper, J.C., Gabrieli, J.D.,
Psychol. Bull. 131, 76–97. Kihsltrom, J.F., D'Esposito, M., 2005. The neural correlates of direct and
Gusnard, D.A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G.L., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Medial reflected self-knowledge. NeuroImage 28, 797–814.
prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a Ofek, E., Pratt, H., 2005. Neurophysiological correlates of subjective
default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, significance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2354–2362.
4259–4264. Panksepp, J., 2005. Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in
Haalmann, I., Vaadia, E., 1997. Dynamics of neuronal interactions: relation animals and humans. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 30–80.
to behavior, firing rates, and distance between neurons. Hum. Brain Pascual-Marqui, R.D., Michel, C.M., Lehmann, D., 1994. Low resolution
Mapp. 5, 249–253. electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical
James, W., 1890. The Principles of Psychology. Holt, New York. activity in the brain. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 18, 49–65.
Janke, W., Debus, G., 2006. Die Eigenschaftswörterliste (EWL-K) - Ein Phan, K.L., Taylor, S.F., Welsh, R.C., Ho, S.H., Britton, J.C., Liberzon, I.,
Verfahren zur Erfassung der Befindlichkeit. Verlag für Psychologie, 2004. Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a
Hogrefe, Zürich. trial-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 21, 768–780.
Johnson, S.C., Baxter, L.C., Wilder, L.S., Pipe, J.G., Heiserman, J.E., Phan, K.L., Wager, T., Taylor, S.F., Liberzon, I., 2002. Functional
Prigatano, G.P., 2002. Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125, neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies
1808–1814. in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage 16, 331–348.
Kawasaki, H., Kaufman, O., Damasio, H., Damasio, A.R., Granner, M., Phillips, C., Rugg, M.D., Friston, K.J., 2002. Anatomically informed basis
Bakken, H., Hori, T., Howard III, M.A., Adolphs, R., 2001. Single- functions for EEG source localization: combining functional and
neuron responses to emotional visual stimuli recorded in human ventral anatomical constraints. NeuroImage 16, 678–695.
prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 15–16. Pizzagalli, D.A., Oakes, T.R., Fox, A.S., Chung, M.K., Larson, C.L.,
Kelley, W.M., Macrae, C.N., Wyland, C.L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., Heatherton, Abercrombie, H.C., Schaefer, S.M., Benca, R.M., Davidson, R.J., 2004.
T.F., 2002. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn. Functional but not structural subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities
Neurosci. 14, 785–794. in melancholia. Mol. Psychiatry 9, 325, 393–325, 405.
Kircher, T.T., Senior, C., Phillips, M.L., Benson, P.J., Bullmore, E.T., Ruby, P., Decety, J., 2001. Effect of subjective perspective taking during
Brammer, M., Simmons, A., Williams, S.C., Bartels, M., David, A.S., simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat. Neurosci.
2000. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of 546–550.
faces and words. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 133–144. Schmitz, T.W., Kawahara-Baccus, T.N., Johnson, S.C., 2004. Metacognitive
Kircher, T.T., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., David, evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage
A.S., 2002. The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self 22, 941–947.
processing. Neuropsychologia 40, 683–692. Schore, A.N., 2003. Affect Regulation and the Repair of the Self. W.W.
Kjaer, T.W., Nowak, M., Lou, H.C., 2002. Reflective self-awareness and Norton, New York.
conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal Shoemaker, S., 1984. Identity, cause, and mind. Cambridge University
core. NeuroImage 17, 1080–1086. Press, Cambridge.
M. Esslen et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 437–449 449

Strik, W.K., Fallgatter, A.J., Brandeis, D., Pascual-Marqui, R.D., 1998. Three- Vitacco, D., Brandeis, D., Pascual-Marqui, R., Martin, E., 2002. Correspondence
dimensional tomography of event-related potentials during response of event-related potential tomography and functional magnetic resonance
inhibition: evidence for phasic frontal lobe activation. Electroencephalogr. imaging during language processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 4–12.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 406–413. Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happe, F., Falkai, P.,
Stuss, D.T., Gallup Jr., G.G., Alexander, M.P., 2001. The frontal lobes are Maier, W., Shah, N.J., Fink, G.R., Zilles, K., 2001. Mind reading: neural
necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain 124, 279–286. mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage 14,
Towle, V.L., Bolanos, J., Suarez, D., Tan, K., Grzeszczuk, R., Levin, D.N., 170–181.
Cakmur, R., Frank, S.A., Spire, J.P., 1993. The spatial location of EEG Zumsteg, D., Wennberg, R.A., Treyer, V., Buck, A., Wieser, H.G., 2005. H2
electrodes: locating the best-fitting sphere relative to cortical anatomy. (15)O or 13NH3 PET and electromagnetic tomography (LORETA)
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 86, 1–6. during partial status epilepticus. Neurology 65, 1657–1660.

You might also like