You are on page 1of 4

Music2605_01 5/8/09 6:28 PM Page 397

EDITORIAL

S PECIAL I SSUE —M USICAL M OVEMENT AND S YNCHRONIZATION

Music and movement are inseparable. Human music provide a forum for exchanging ideas on the scientific
making requires the body—and often an instrument investigation of body movements that accompany solo
that serves as an extension of the body—to move in and ensemble music performance and dance. Sixteen
order to produce sound energy. When listening to researchers were invited to present their discoveries
music, moreover, most individuals experience the urge about these topics, and this special issue of Music
to move. Indeed, dance and music are blissfully wed in Perception comprises a reviewed and edited selection of
most cultures. Even if overt movement is inhibited, eight articles based on these presentations. The organi-
music perception recruits motor-related brain areas. zation of this collection of articles is tripartite: A quar-
Thus, some of the core cognitive and motor processes tet of articles on musical timing and kinematics is
that underlie music performance are involved in the followed by two duets, one on movement coordination
even more common activity of music listening. in space and time, and the other on links between
From a historical perspective, the synergy of musical movement and social communication.
sound and bodily movement was the subject of early Music is characterized by the precise temporal rela-
systematic but highly subjective investigations by tion of sounds both within and between separate
Becking (1928) and Truslit (1938). Empirical music instrumental parts. The perceptual and motor processes
research targeted auditory processing in the ensuing that enable such precision are addressed in research
decades (eventually well-nourished by the fruition of reported in the first four articles, which examine move-
cognitivism in psychology) until sparks of renewed ment timing and kinematics during musical activity. In
interest in the 1970s (e.g., Clynes, 1977) led to the the first contribution, Rankin, Large, and Fink focus on
emergence of rigorous qualitative and quantitative mechanisms involved in pulse prediction. To synchro-
approaches to musical movement (see Davidson, 2005; nize movements with music, it is necessary to predict
Gabrielsson, 1987; Shove & Repp, 1995; Todd, 1999). the timing of upcoming sounds. Music typically con-
An undercurrent to these developments is provided by tains both unintentional and intentional fluctuations in
research on sensorimotor synchronization (typically in local tempo, with the latter being used by performers to
the guise of finger tapping studies), which yielded communicate structural and expressive information to
sophisticated computational models of sensorimotor co-performers and audience members. Rankin, Large,
timing control by the mid-1990s (see Large, 2000; and Fink employed a sensorimotor synchronization
Pressing, 1999; Repp, 2005; Vorberg & Wing, 1996). task to study adaptation to tempo fluctuations in
Most recently, theoretical and empirical approaches expressive performances of piano music. Analyses of
that focus on links between music cognition and action the relation between tap timing and expressive timing
have addressed the impact of motor processes on the revealed that listeners are sensitive to long-range serial
perception of musical structure, as well as reciprocal correlation (timing in an early portion of a sequence
effects of auditory information on motor control in the piece is similar to timing in a later portion) and fractal
context of music performance in healthy individuals as scaling (timing at fast time scales is proportionally sim-
well as in clinical settings (e.g., Bangert et al., 2006; ilar to timing at slower time scales) in the expressive
Drost, Rieger, & Prinz, 2007; Leman, 2007; Pfordresher, timing profiles of the music. Results indicated that lis-
2008; Phillips-Silver & Trainor, 2008; Repp & Knoblich, teners use these types of regularities to predict tempo
2007; Thaut, 2005). changes.
To further our knowledge about such links, a work- In the next contribution, Loehr and Palmer describe
shop on ‘Musical Movement and Synchronization’ was a study that was designed to tease apart auditory and
held at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive motor contributions to processes that underlie musical
and Brain Sciences in Leipzig on the 3rd and 4th of May synchronization. In ensemble performance, musicians
2008. The main catalyst for this event was a visit to typically produce different rhythms that mesh to form
Leipzig by workshop co-organizer Caroline Palmer in a coherent Gestalt. Thus, performers routinely syn-
October 2007. A specific aim of the workshop was to chronize with each other under conditions where the

Music Perception VOLUME 26, ISSUE 5, PP. 397–400, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312 © 2009 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA . ALL
RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS ’ S
RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS WEBSITE , HTTP :// WWW. UCPRESSJOURNALS . COM / REPRINTINFO. ASP. DOI:10.1525/MP.2009.26.5.397
Music2605_01 5/8/09 6:28 PM Page 398

398 Editorial

temporal intervals produced by one performer are sub- observed for piano playing. This buttresses the generality
divided by those produced by another performer. Loehr of the claim that tactile feedback plays an important role
and Palmer investigated the effects of such subdivisions in timing control during music performance.
on synchronization accuracy by employing a MIDI The second thematic cluster of the special issue con-
piano and a motion capture system to analyze the tains two articles on spatial and temporal aspects of
timing and movement characteristics of pianists as movement coordination. Consider the following scene:
they performed melodic sequences in time with a A man in a black lycra bodysuit sashays across the silent,
metronome. In separate conditions, melody tones were darkened room, halts, arches his back, then performs a
subdivided by auditory information (heard tones), motor circular jump as another man in a red shirt and a
production (produced tones), both, or neither. The pres- woman in a purple dress swoop toward him. For some,
ence of subdivisions increased asynchronies in a manner dance as an artform may be a perplexing activity to
suggesting that pianists were sensitive to the timing of engage in either as performer or audience member. The
both heard and produced subdivisions. However, kine- contribution by Stevens, Schubert, Wang, Kroos, and
matic analyses indicated that movement trajectories were Havlovic demonstrates, nevertheless, that dance is not
influenced only by produced subdivisions. only “an ancient and ubiquitous form of human expres-
Ensemble musicians coordinate their actions with sion and communication” (Stevens et al., this volume,
one another under conditions of varying auditory and p. 451) but also an activity ideally suited to study time
visual feedback, and these conditions can influence the keeping, synchronization, and memory for movement
roles that performers assume in the musical interplay. sequences. Stevens et al. used a motion capture system
Goebl and Palmer investigated these issues by examin- to record the movements of one dancer from a trio as
ing timing and movement characteristics in pianists the group performed the same contemporary work
who were assigned roles as leaders and followers during twice; first without and then with accompanying music.
duet performance. The analysis of pianists’ key stroke It was found that the silent performance was shorter.
timing revealed that, under ideal circumstances (i.e., Time-series analyses designed to distinguish between
full auditory feedback), pianists adopted a cooperative temporal scaling (uniformly faster or slower timing)
performance style (with bidirectional adjustments) irre- and lapsing (omitted or inserted material) suggested
spective of the assigned roles. Analysis of head move- that this duration difference was more likely due to
ments indicated that visual cues from co-performers memory lapses than a miscalibrated internal time-keep-
were of greatest importance to coordination when ing mechanism.
auditory information was absent. The second contribution in this section addresses the
The articles described thus far illustrate how our coordination of movements with a conductor’s ges-
understanding of musical timing and kinematics has tures. Luck and Sloboda investigated the spatio-temporal
profited from the study of piano performance. The final cues that mediate this visually mediated form of syn-
contribution in this section is relevant to the question chronization in a study that required finger tapping in
whether results obtained with the piano generalize to time with dynamic point-light displays of beat gestures
other musical instruments. It is known from previous produced by an experienced conductor. Analyses of the
studies of piano performance that timing accuracy is tapping data examined the effects of factors such as
influenced by sensory information associated with the beat clarity and tempo on the kinematic features that
finger’s arrival at the piano key: Performances that con- participants spontaneously selected as synchronization
tain kinematic landmarks indicative of high accelera- targets. Points of high absolute acceleration were dom-
tion—and therefore force—at initial key contact display inant cues to target time points for synchronization
more accurate timing (Goebl & Palmer, 2008). Of course, across differing levels of beat clarity and tempi.
finger acceleration also is used to control the intensity The final section of this special issue includes two
(loudness) of tones in piano playing. To investigate the articles that deal with the relationship between move-
contribution of tactile feedback to temporal accuracy ment and emotional and social aspects of musical
while sidestepping this potential confound, Palmer, behavior. Although movement is an essential ingredient
Koopmans, Loehr, and Carter examined clarinet playing in musical experience, movement per se is not typically
in the current article. In contrast to the piano, clarinetists’ the primary motivation for engaging in musical activity.
finger movements affect the timing of tones, but not Music is by and large a social phenomenon; a medium
their intensity (which is controlled by airflow). Palmer for affective communication. Vocal music is particularly
et al. found a similar relationship between kinematic interesting in this regard not only because it seems rea-
landmarks and temporal accuracy to that previously sonable to assume that the voice is a phylogenetically
Music2605_01 5/8/09 6:28 PM Page 399

Editorial 399

and ontogenetically early mediator of group music music in emotional communication, and in therapeu-
making, but also because vocal sounds effortlessly carry tic and educational contexts.
acoustic traces of an individual’s real or imagined emo- The full program of the Musical Movement and
tional state. Furthermore, facial expressions when Synchronization workshop can be found on the Internet
singing convey emotional information in a similar man- (www.cbs.mpg.de/news/workshops/1). Other publica-
ner to how they function in everyday nonmusical inter- tions by workshop participants that are relevant to this
actions. In a study of emotional communication in special issue address expressive body movements in
singing, Livingstone, Thompson, and Russo employed music performance (Davidson, 2009; Nusseck &
motion capture and electromyography to monitor the Wanderley, 2009), anticipatory movements in drum-
facial expressions of singers as they viewed (and heard) ming and piano performance (Dahl, 2006; Dalla Bella
audiovisual recordings of musical phrases that had to be & Palmer, 2006; Palmer, 2006), the effects of the firm-
subsequently imitated. These phrases had been recorded ness of the tapping surface on timing and kinematics
by a model singer with the intention to sound happy, during sensorimotor synchronization (Pfordresher &
sad, or emotionally neutral. Expressive facial activity Palmer, 2008), the use of adaptively timed pacing sig-
that differentiated between target emotional states was nals to interrogate temporal error correction processes
observed during all stages of the task—perception, plan- (Repp & Keller, 2008), and the cognitive/motor skills
ning, production, and post-production—suggesting that underlie musical ensemble performance (Keller,
that cognition, action, and affect are tightly interwoven 2008). Together with the articles published in this spe-
in emotional singing. cial issue, these studies exemplify the rich diversity of
The final article in the special issue offers a broad lit- questions that have been tackled in research on musical
erature review and theoretical discussion of music movement and synchronization. In short, this vibrant
making and listening as inherently social activities. field holds great promise for deepening our understand-
Indeed, it is well documented that even individuals who ing of music cognition, action, and human interaction.
seem relatively disinterested in conventional social The metamorphosis of workshop presentations into
interaction, such as people with autism, find a compan- journal articles is not a self-organizing process. We con-
ion in music. Overy and Molnar-Szakacs present a clude by thanking the team of expert reviewers who pro-
model that posits that the affective power of music vided constructive comments to the authors of the
derives from the way in which it supports imitation, articles published in this special issue. Their dedication—
synchronization, and shared experience. They argue that together with the support of Editor Lola Cuddy and
music does so by activating a network of brain areas— Managing Editor Christine Koh from Music
including the so-called ‘mirror neuron system’—that Perception—has made possible the timely delivery of
play a role in action simulation, action understanding, this compilation of fresh discoveries about musical
and intention attribution. As a consequence, music movement and synchronization.
conveys a sense of agency, arousing in the listener the
experience of empathically perceiving another’s P ETER E. K ELLER & M ARTINA R IEGER
actions and affective states. Overy and Molnar-Szakacs Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive
propose that this accounts for the effectiveness of and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany

References

B ANGERT, M., P ESCHEL , T., S CHLAUG , G., R OTTE , M., motor control and the brain (pp.125-138). Oxford, UK: Oxford
D RESCHER , D., H INRICHS , H., ET AL . (2006). Shared University Press.
networks for auditory and motor processing in professional DALLA B ELLA , S., & PALMER , C. (2006). Personal identifiers in
pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage, 15, musicians’ finger movement dynamics. Journal of Cognitive
917-926. Neuroscience, 18, Supplement, G84.
B ECKING , G. (1928) Der musikalische Rhythmus als Erken- DAVIDSON , J. W. (2005). Bodily communication in musical
ntisquelle [Musical rhythm as a source of insight]. Augsberg: performance. In D. Miell, D. J. Hargreaves, & R. Macdonald
Benno Filser. (Eds.), Musical communication (pp. 215-237). New York:
C LYNES , M. (1977). Sentics: The touch of emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Doubleday/Anchor. DAVIDSON , J. W. (2009). Movement and collaboration
DAHL, S. (2006). Movements and analysis of drumming. In E. in musical performance. In S. Hallam, I. Cross,
Altenmüller, M. Wiesendanger, & J. Kesselring (Eds.), Music, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music
Music2605_01 5/8/09 6:28 PM Page 400

400 Editorial

psychology (pp. 364-376). Oxford, UK: Oxford P FORDRESHER , P. Q., & PALMER , C. (2008). The role of tactile
University Press. feedback in synchronization tapping. Abstracts of the
D ROST, U. C., R IEGER , M., & P RINZ , W. (2007). Instrument Psychonomic Society, 13, 75.
specificity in experienced musicians. The Quarterly Journal P HILLIPS -S ILVER , J., & T RAINOR , L. J. (2008). Vestibular
of Experimental Psychology, 60, 527-533. influence on auditory metrical interpretation. Brain and
G ABRIELSSON , A. (E D.) (1987). Action and perception in Cognition, 67, 94-102.
rhythm and music. Stockholm: Royal Swedish Academy P RESSING , J. (1999). The referential dynamics of cognition and
of Music. action. Psychological Review, 106, 714-747.
G OEBL , W., & PALMER , C. (2008). Tactile feedback and timing R EPP, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of
accuracy in piano performance. Experimental Brain Research, the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12,
186, 471-479. 969-992.
K ELLER , P. E. (2008). Joint action in music performance. In R EPP, B. H., & K ELLER , P. E. (2008). Sensorimotor
F. Morganti, A. Carassa, & G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting inter- synchronization with adaptively timed sequences. Human
subjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective to the study of Movement Science, 27, 423-456.
interactions (pp. 205-221). Amsterdam: IOS Press. R EPP, B. H., & K NOBLICH , G. (2007). Action can affect
L ARGE , E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music. auditory perception. Psychological Science, 18, 6-7.
Human Movement Science, 19, 527-566. S HOVE , P., & R EPP, B. H. (1995). Musical motion and
L EMAN , M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation performance: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. In
technology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. J. Rink (Ed.), The practice of performance (pp. 55-83).
N USSEK , M., & WANDERLEY, M. M. (2009). Music and Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
motion—How music-related ancillary body movements T HAUT, M. H. (2005). Rhythm, music, and the brain: Scientific
contribute to the experience of music. Music Perception, 26, foundations and clinical applications. New York: Routledge.
335-353. TODD, N. P. M. (1999). Motion in music: A neurobiological
PALMER , C. (2006). The nature of memory for music perspective. Music Perception, 17, 115-126.
performance skills. In E. Altenmüller, M. Wiesendanger, T RUSLIT, A. (1938). Gestaltung und Bewegung in der Musik
& J. Kesselring (Eds.), Music, motor control and the brain [Shaping and motion in music]. Berlin: Christian Friedrich
(pp. 39-53). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Vieweg.
P FORDRESHER , P. Q. (2008). Auditory feedback in music VORBERG , D., & W ING , A. (1996). Modeling variability and
performance: The role of transition-based similarity. Journal dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.),
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Handbook of perception and action, Vol. 2 (pp. 181-262).
Performance, 34, 708-725. London: Academic Press.

You might also like