You are on page 1of 8

Child Abuse in Stepfamilies

Author(s): Jean Giles-Sims and David Finkelhor


Source: Family Relations, Vol. 33, No. 3, Remarriage and Stepparenting (Jul., 1984), pp. 407-413
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/584711
Accessed: 05-11-2015 12:52 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Family Relations.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Child Abuse in Stepfamilies*
JEAN GILES-SIMSAND DAVIDFINKELHOR**
There is an often repeated presumption that children are at increased risk of abuse
at the hands of stepparents. This paper tries to initiate a more formal examination of
the evidence and theory related to this idea. Although evidence from reported cases
suggests that stepparents are overrepresented among abusers, the available data are
inadequate to determine the nature of the relationship between the stepfamily struc-
ture and child abuse. Five theories that can or have been used to explain this pre-
sumed relationship are evaluated here: social-evolutionary, normative, stress, selec-
tion and resource theory.

Fairy tales such as Cinderella and Hansel father substitutes who were involved as per-
and Gretel have dramatized and perhaps per- petrators of child abuse. Similar findings have
petuated a widely held presumption that chil- been replicated in a number of other studies
dren are at high risk of abuse in stepfamilies. using reported cases (Burgess & Garbarino,
The idea seems almost accepted as fact, yet it 1983; Johnson, 1974; Justice & Justice, 1976;
has never been adequately tested. The dual Kimball, Stewart, Conger & Burgess, 1980;
purpose of this paper is to examine the data Maden, 1980; Martin & Walters, 1982; Phelan &
which can be marshalled to support the exis- Joyce, 1980; Wilson, Daly & Weghorst, 1980)
tence of a relationship between stepparenting and are widely cited by reviewers (Maden &
and child abuse, and to review theories that Wrench, 1977).
have been or could be forwarded to explain this These studies, unfortunately, are confined
relationship. entirely to officially reported cases. A recent
The earliest systematic inquiries into the study bearing similar findings has partly tran-
characteristics of child abusers implicated scended that limitation. The National Inci-
nontraditional type family structures, including dence (NI) Study (1981) advanced previous ef-
the single parent family and the stepfamily. In forts and collected national data on child
Gil's (1970) landmark survey of reported cases abuse, identifying cases which were officially
of abuse, for example, he found a large amount reported, and those that came to professional
of abuse by stepfathers. Stepfathers in that attention but not officially reported. It also
study constituted a third of the fathers or used a clear, rigorous and uniform definition of
child abuse, and the random selection of coun-
*The authors would like to thank Murray Straus, Gerald ties allowed for more accurate statistical
Hotaling and other members of the Family Violence Research generalization on a national level.
Seminar, UNH, for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this In this study of child abuse, data were
paper, and Ruth Miller, Shirley Parshley, and Elaine Hashem
for help in preparing the manuscript. Part of the work on this gathered in two ways. First, researchers made
paper was made possible by grants from the National Institute a careful audit of cases coming to the attention
of Mental Health (MH34109), National Center for Child Abuse of child protective agencies during a one year
and Neglect (90CA840), and Research Office of the University
of New Hampshire. This is one of a series of articles on family
period in each of the 26 counties chosen to be
violence written by associates of the Family Violence representative of all counties in the U.S.' Sec-
Research Program. A list of publications can be obtained by ond, other official investigatory agencies such
contacting the Program Administrator. as police departments and community profes-
**Jean Giles-Sims is Assistant Professor of Sociology,
Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX 76129. David sionals (e.g., teachers and public health
Finkelhor is Associate Director of the Family Violence workers) in each county were asked to partici-
Research Program, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH
03824.
'The 26 counties were selected in a five stage procedure.
The procedure stratified states and counties by region and size
Key Concepts: child abuse, resource theory, sexual abuse, to allow for the selection of 1 county from the 11 largest coun-
social-evolutionary perspective, stepfamilies, stress theory. ties in the country, 1 county from among the 28 next largest
counties, and 1 small, 1 medium and 1 large county from each
of 8 randomly chosen states (National Incidence Study, 1981,
(Family Relations, 1984, 33, 407-413.) pp. 3-4).

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 407

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 1.
Stepparents and Natural Parents as Perpetrators In Three Types of Child Abuse
% of cases involvinga
Physical Sexual Emotional All Forms
Perpetrator Abuse Abuse Abuse of Abuse
Stepfather 15 30 21 18
Father 45 28 38 40
Stepmother 3 1 13 6
Mother 66 43 71 64
Source: National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (1981).
aColumns sum to more than 100 because many cases of abuse involve more than one perpetrator.

pate. Researchers left forms at various agen- outnumbered natural father abusers (30% vs.
cies that could be completed anonymously by 28/%).3
workers, or workers could call a toll free WATS Unfortunately, the information in Table 1 is
line number to report a case of child abuse. Na- not adequate in itself to answer the question of
tional estimates were generated from the 26 whether stepparents are disproportionately
representative counties through an elaborate represented among child abusers. One also
weighting procedure. needs to know how many stepparents we
Despite its name and its elaborate method- would expect in such statistics if stepparents
ology, however, this study is still not a true "in- were no more abusive than other parents.
cidence" study in the most usual sense of that Glick's (1979) estimate from census data sug-
word. The study did not include cases known gests that in 1978 1 in 10 children lived with a
only to people in the community other than stepparent, in most cases a stepfather. This
professionals, or cases known only to the child suggests that if stepparents were no more like-
and the perpetrator. This is a critical limitation ly to abuse their children than natural parents,
because estimates from general population approximately 10% of stepparents would be
surveys suggest that the majority of child identified in the abuse statistics presented in
abuse is of this sort, i.e., unreported to profes- Table 1, and that more stepfathers would be im-
sionals (Straus, Gelles & Steinmetz, 1980). plicated in abuse than stepmothers. Inspection
Cases not coming to professional attention of the actual figures indicates that stepfathers
probably differ in systematic ways from those were significantly overrepresented in abuse
known to professionals, injecting possible bias cases, and that more stepfathers were per-
into the study. Another problem with the study petrators of abuse than stepmothers.
was that in some counties, researchers were However, problems arise with using these
unable to elicit full professional cooperation.2 estimates as a basis for comparison. First,
For these reasons, data from the NI study can- Glick (1979) only counted stepparents who
not be said to portray the true nature of child were living with the stepchildren. Many chil-
abuse with any confidence. However, as the dren have stepparents with whom they do not
best available data to date on the incidence of live but who still may abuse them. Including
child abuse in stepfamilies, the findings from these noncustodial parents in the estimate
this study are worth presenting here in more would raise the comparison figure above 10%.
detai 1. Second, using Glick's estimates fails to take
The National Incidence Study findings into account social class and its relation to
reported in Table 1 show the percent of step- child abuse. Divorce rates are higher at lower
parents and natural parents identified as abuse socio-economic statuses (Norton & Glick,
perpetrators by type of abuse. (The percent- 1979) as are fertility rates (U.S. Bureau of the
ages sum to more than 100% because in many Census, 1974). Also, persons with lower educa-
cases of abuse more than one parent was in- tion are most likely to remarry following a
volved.) As can be seen, stepfathers were in- divorce (Spanier & Glick, 1980). Together, these
volved in 18% of the cases and stepmothers in findings suggest that the proportion of chil-
6%. Stepfathers were particularly common dren living with a stepparent is significantly
among sexual abusers, where they actually higher at lower socio-economic status levels
than the 10% estimated for the total popula-
tion. Suppose that instead of 10%, the propor-
0Overall,87% of the agencies which were solicited did parti-
cipate in the study, but this figure hides some glaring omis-
sions. For example, one of the largest hospitals in one of the 'The seemingly high proportionof mothers involved in sex-
largest cities-an institution that receives many reports of ual abuse, 43%, stems from a decision-in our view mis-
abuse-did not participate.Moreover,in some urbanareas, up taken-to cite mothers as sexual abuse perpetrators if they
to a third of all children attended privateschools, which were contributed to the abuse by knowingly putting the child in a
not included in the study (National Incidence Study, 1981, p. high risk situation. For more discussion on this point, see
72). Finkelhorand Hotaling, 1983.

408 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
tion of children with a stepparent in lower Weghorst, 1980). There are, however, at least
socio-economic statuses was as high as two problems with this explanation of physical
20-30%. In this case, the figures in Table 1 abuse of stepchildren.
would not necessarily reflect an overrepresen- First, this theoretical perspective cannot ac-
tation of stepfathers among child abusers. The count for abuse of natural children except in
high percentages for stepfathers would stem situations of scarce resources where certain
from the overrepresentation of stepparents children in the family are genetically or in other
among lower socio-economic groups. The ma- ways inferior. Second, and more importantly,
jority of child abuse reports come from lower the argument about parental investment is in-
socio-economic groups (Gil, 1970; Johnson, ternally contradictory as applied to physical
1974). Adequate testing of whether children are abuse. If parents physically abuse stepchil-
at an unusually high risk of abuse from step- dren, unless the stepchild dies-and few do-
parents requires data on rates of abuse in dif- the injuries resulting from the abuse are likely
ferent family structures controlling for many to lead to an increased demand on parental
socio-demographic characteristics, including resources. Stepparents, theoretically, would
household size and income levels. Until this is more likely avoid behavior which increases the
done, the question of whether stepfamilies are burden the child places on them. This argu-
high risk environments for child abuse cannot ment might be more applicable to the issue of
be definitely settled. neglect of stepchildren than to physical abuse.
The social-evolutionary perspective also has
Theories of Abuse in Stepfamilies
an argument to explain the greatervulnerability
Considering many researchers have noted of children to sexual abuse by stepparents. Ac-
the high rates of abuse in stepfamilies, it is cording to the theory, inbreeding reduces
curious that the relationship has attracted little fitness. Thus the process of evolution has lead
theoretical attention. Even when theoretical to the survival of individuals living in social
arguments have been applied, such attempts groups which have some mechanism that in-
have lacked rigor. Therefore, in addition to hibits such inbreeding. One of the theoretical
reviewing explicitly articulated theories, our mechanisms suggested is an incest aversion
review here extends to theoretical arguments that is "pre-cultural and involves negative im-
which have been forwarded only by inference, printing on intimate associates during a critical
but provide promise for further exploration. period of early childhood (between roughly two
Some of the theories of the connection be- and six years of age)" (van den Berghe, 1983).
tween stepfamilies and abuse pertain primarily Evidence for this mechanism rests on studies
to physical abuse and others primarily to sex- of sexual avoidance among co-reared kibbutz
ual abuse. Although physical and sexual abuse children (Shepher, 1971) and co-reared adop-
are often lumped together as child abuse, they tive daughter-in-law marriages in Taiwan (Wolf,
are very different phenomena with different 1966). Because stepparents would be less like-
causes. Here, we will address how the theoreti- ly to be present in the child's life during the
cal arguments pertain to each type of abuse critical period, the negative imprinting would
separately. less likely occur, and thus incest with a step-
parent becomes more probable.
Social-Evolutionary Theory
There are a number of problems with this
The social-evolutionary (sometimes referred theory. First, all the evidence about the
to as socio-biological) perspective predicts presumed imprinting mechanism-meager as
that stepchildren are more likely to be both it is-comes from studies of sexual inhibition
physically and sexually abused than natural among peers. There is no evidence that such a
children, using different arguments to explain mechanism operates in a situation where one
each type of abuse. The prediction about physi- partner is a child and the other an adult. More-
cal abuse derives from the theoretical hypothe- over, the inhibition is proposed to operate on
sis concerning parental investment, a concept the child during the critical period (age 2-6),
central to the social-evolutionary perspective. not the adult. What it says in effect is that
Parental investment has been defined as "any daughters would initiate less, and resist sexual
investment by the parent in an individual off- encounters more with natural fathers than
spring that increases the offspring's chance of stepfathers. However, the research on father-
surviving (and hence reproductive success) at daughter incest does not make this a plausible
the cost of the parent's ability to invest in other explanation of the frequency of incest with
offspring" (Trivers, 1971, p. 139). stepfathers. Incest is almost always initiated
This group of researchers argues that step- by the parent, and generally evokes discomfort
parents have lesser investment in and there- and confusion, if not outright revulsion in the
fore are more likely to physically abuse nonbio- child toward whom it is directed (Herman,
logical children (particularly when resources 1981). Even the possibility that natural daugh-
are scarce), because those children do not ters might resist more strenuously does not
carry on their genes (Burgess & Garbarino, seem convincing in view of the fact that so
1983; Daly & Wilson, 1980; Wilson, Daly & much incest occurs in spite of children's resis-

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 409

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
tance. An explanation of the high frequency of that norms and sanctions differ for parents and
stepfather incest from a social-evolutionary stepparents, a major question has yet to be ful-
theory needs to propose a mechanism that ly explored: Do normative taboos against sex-
creates lower inhibition in the father, not the ual, or even physical, abuse of children differ
child. for parents and stepparents? If so, how do they
Because the social-evolutionary perspective differ?
is in the early stages of development and little
Stress Theory
concrete evidence is available to support the
theoretical propositions suggested, it is diffi- The theory that higher stress levels are
cult to assess the contributions of this theory responsible for the increased risk of abuse in
to explaining child abuse in stepfamilies. Other stepfamilies has both theoretical and empirical
theories yet to be discussed offer explanations support (Justice & Justice, 1976; Straus et al.,
of child abuse in stepfamilies that are more 1980). Although the relationship between
parsimonious and amenable to empirical in- stress level and child abuse within stepfami-
vestigation. At the same time these theories lies has not been specifically tested, it is well
may not be incompatible with the basic documented that stepfamilies experience
premises of the social-evolutionary perspec- stress, and that stress is related to child abuse.
tive. Higher rates of both physical and sexual
abuse among stepfamilies could be related to
Normative Theory the unique stresses present in those families.
A common theoretical argument to explain Remarried families containing children from
the higher rates of sexual abuse by stepfathers prior marriages must solve problems un-
suggests that stepfathers-as nonblood rela- familiar to other types of families (Cherlin,
tions-are subject to weaker normative taboos 1978). There are few institutionalized guide-
against incest. Since incest is a relatively lines for solving the unique problems of this
strong prohibition in human societies, the family structure including appropriate kinship
taboo would appear to be stronger for blood terms, authority to discipline stepchildren, and
relations than for nonblood kin (Tierney & Cor- family loyalty (Nelson & Nelson, 1982). Step-
win, 1983). The relative weakness of taboos parents also experience contradictory
against incest with a stepchild could be par- pressures to assume some aspects of the
ticularly critical, because stepparents (like parental role and to refrain from others
parents) are in roles which provide an authority (Cherlin, 1978; Fast & Cain, 1966; Keshet, 1980;
position and opportunities for exploitation of Maddox, 1975; Rallings, 1976). Without estab-
children (Garbarino & Gilliam, 1980). lished guidelines, there are more opportunities
The normative theory of sexual abuse by for disagreements and stress associated with
stepparents is consistent with a social-evolu- conflicting values and beliefs (Visher & Visher,
tionary perspective which argues that the 1979).
justification of incest taboos is to avoid The unique problems stepfamilies face may
genetic degradation. However, the normative create stress levels higher than those in intact
argument does not require a biologically based families. Pasley and Ihinger-Tallman (1982) in a
assumption. Incest with nonblood kin may be recent review postulated three main causes of
less taboo in many cultures for any number of stress in remarried families: (1) clashing of
reasons. family cultures, (b) differing perceptions of
The normative theory has common sense how the family's time, energy, material goods
validity for many people. Yet, there is little em- and affections should be shared, and (c) feel-
pirical evidence to support it. In many Ameri- ings of loyalty to current and prior family mem-
can jurisdictions, laws proscribing incest do bers. High stress levels may act as an interven-
not apply to steprelations (Wulkan & Bulkey, ing variable to explain high reported abuse
1981), suggesting that the taboo is absent or rates in stepfamilies. There is empirical evi-
weaker. However, no attitude survey exists, to dence from the national survey of violence in
the authors' knowledge, showing that the American families (Straus et al., 1980) linking
taboo on sex with dependent children is stress and the physical abuse of children.
weaker for stepparents. Even if such attitudes Justice and Justice (1976) also found a positive
could be documented, it is not certain they association between crises or major life
would explain the observed high frequency of changes and child abuse. The connection be-
stepfather incest. A recent study demon- tween stress and sexual child abuse has not
strated little correlation between the strength been empirically demonstrated to the same ex-
of taboos against various forms of incest and tent, but it is frequently mentioned in the litera-
the relative frequency of occurrences ture (Tierney and Corwin, 1983).
(Finkelhor & Redfield, 1982). For example, Stress theory accounts for more of the corre-
father-daughter incest is considered more lates of child abuse than either social-evolu-
taboo by American respondents than is tionary or normative theory. Specifically, it
mother-son incest, yet it is more frequent. helps explain the correlation between child
Although there are theoretical suggestions abuse and low family income, large family size,

410 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
and family disruption. For these reasons, the factors may be more important in explaining
testing of the implications of stress theory the high divorce rate for remarrieds (see
should actively focus on the following: Is child Cherlin, 1978; Furstenberg, 1979; Goetting,
abuse in stepfamilies associated with com- 1982). At this point, however, no empirical evi-
paratively high levels of stress following a re- dence is availabe to test whether any common
marriage? How do some stepfamilies manage antecedent condition exists which explains the
to minimize stress? Where stress is minimized, association between stepfamilies and reported
does it decrease the likelihood of chiid abuse? cases of abuse. Further work should take
selection factors into consideration.
Selection Factors
Resource Theory
Still another theoretical possibility is that
the association between stepfamilies and A final theoretical approach to explaining
cases of child abuse is spurious, and that there higher rates of child abuse by stepparents is
are common antecedent conditions to both. resource theory. According to this theory, the
Besides the socio-demographic variables men- more resources a person can command, the
tioned earlier, it may be that the same factors more power and authority that person has at
which make people prone to divorce and remar- his/her disposal to regulate a social system.
riage also make them prone to abuse children. Therefore the less likely he/she is to resort to
For example, people with individual character- physical force to get his/her way (Goode, 1971).
istics such as a tendency toward violence or A person who contributes needed resources to
sexual deviance may be overrepresented a system is granted authority. In contrast,
among stepparents, because such characteris- when a person lacks resources, the social
tics played a part in the termination of their system often resists granting him/her authori-
previous marriage. Such characteristics would ty. Then violence is more likely to be used to
also contribute to their high rates of child gain desired needs. This is particularly true
abuse in the new marriage. when a person feels that he/she has a right to
Research has indicated that physical authority, but resources are insufficient to gain
abusers are often emotionally disturbed, have it voluntarily (Gelles & Straus, 1979).
difficulty in dealing with aggressive impulses, This theory, when applied to physical abuse
and have low self-esteem and/or a tendency to of stepchildren, focuses on the legitimacy of
be rigid and domineering (Elmer, 1967; John- the power position of stepparents and at the
son & Morse, 1968; Spinetta & Rigler, 1972; dynamics within stepfamilies which may block
Wasserman, 1967). Sexual abusers are cited as a stepparent from a position of authority.
persons who have impulse control problems, Authority may come from many types of re-
are authoritarian, have alcohol problems, have sources, e.g., economic resources or parenting
difficulty empathizing, and have low self- skills. Theoretically, the more valued resources
esteem (Meiselman, 1978). Such persons are the stepparent brings into the family, the more
likely to have difficulty in their marital relation- likely it is that he/she will be granted authority.
ships which may in turn lead to higher rates of Examples of this would include a stepparent
divorce and remarriage. Thus, people entering who significantly improves the overall well-
remarriage and forming stepfamilies after being of the new family with money, kindness,
divorce may be more abusive people than generosity or love. On the other hand, when a
those who do not divorce. stepparent does not bring new resources,
The idea that people available for remarriage either financial or interpersonal, to the family,
may be more violence prone is supported by the stepparent is less likely to be granted a
studies of divorcing couples. These couples position of authority. Even when a stepparent
report that violence occurred in a large number does bring resources, a family may refuse to
of their marriages (Levinger, 1966). Survey data recognize his/her authority for other reasons
from the general population indicate that per- (Keshet, 1980). This blocking of authority may
sons who have been violent to a spouse are be particularly frustrating for stepfathers
also likely to use violence in child rearing because of the expectation that fathers should
(Straus et al., 1980). This tendency toward be the head of household. Resource theory
abusiveness could conceivably have been suggests that when a stepparent wants
something learned and reinforced in the first authority but does not have the resources to
marriage or prior to that marriage. earn it, he or she may resort to the use of
An argument made to support the theory of violence to gain control (Goode, 1971).
selection factors is that remarriages are also Although the resource theory of power has
subject to high rates of divorce because people been applied to the use of violence in general,
who are unstable or ill suited for family life are it has not, to our knowledge, been directly
overrepresented among the remarried. How- tested in regard to stepfamilies. The theory
ever, this argument has been criticized be- does lead to several relevant and important
cause it overlooks the fact that divorce and re- research questions: Does the rate of child
marriage may be an indication of desire for a abuse in stepfamilies vary with the authority
better family life situation. Nonpathological granted to the stepparent? Does the rate of

July1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 411

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
child abuse by stepparents vary with the argues that abuse of stepchildren is ultimately
amount of resources contributed by the step- caused by an evolutionary process that favors
parent? Is child abuse related to resistance in expending resources on natural children over
the natural parent-child subsystem to accept- stepchildren to increase the chances of those
ing the authority of the stepparent? children passing on their genes. Stress theory
and resource theory emphasize that the link be-
Synthesizing Theories of Child Abuse in
tween resources and child abuse can be
Stepfamilies
demonstrated within a variety of family struc-
The theories discussed here are those which tures. These theories also emphasize that
might help explain the differences in reported higher stress levels within stepfamilies and the
child abuse rates between stepfamilies and in- limitation of resources and rejection of the
tact families. There are other relevant theories authority of stepparents explain the higher
of child abuse (Gelles & Straus, 1979) not in- rates of child abuse in stepfamilies.
cluded here because they are less germane to Stress theory, resource theory and social-
explaining these differences. evolutionary theory are not necessarily con-
The theories presented are not necessarily tradictory. According to each of these theories,
competing theories, nor are they mutually ex- child abuse in stepfamilies can be reduced by
clusive. Therefore, it is not necessarily appro- altering the resources of the family as a whole
priate to devise research with the goal of vali- or that of individual family members. However,
dating one theory at the expense of another. A the implications of the social-evolutionary per-
more productive approach may be to try to syn- spective for elimination of child abuse among
thesize the concepts from among theories to stepparents are less optimistic. If the ultimate
create the most effective explanation of child cause is biologically inherited, it is hard to af-
abuse in stepfamilies. Our review suggests two fect change by altering present conditions.
priorities. Also, it is important to evaluate the heuristic
First, priority in future research should be value of different theories. The hypotheses of
given to gathering data from the general normative theory, stress theory and resource
population which can be analyzed to deter- theory are more amenable to empirical investi-
mine: (a) the actual rates of child abuse in step- gation than those of social-evolutionary theory.
families compared to other families, and (b) Also, they are more fruitful for further specifi-
selection factors which explain both the rates cation of the processes that lead to children
of remarriage and rates of child abuse. Al- being abused in stepfamilies. Therefore, these
though many studies have reported higher perspectives are more likely to identify goals
rates of physical and sexual abuse in stepfami- for constructive intervention. For example, if
lies, all of the available studies are based on weak norms against sexual abuse by step-
reported cases of abuse and none have pro- parents create a potential for violations as
vided adequate data to assess the possible predicted by normative theory, then norms and
confounding of socio-demographic factors and their legal sanctions can be changed. More-
rates of child abuse. In addition, the personal over, if stress caused by conflicting expecta-
history of violence and individual character- tions is at the root of some child abuse, the
istics of abusers need to be studied to deter- families can be counseled to define their ex-
mine if they also are confounded with family pectations more clearly and to act in consis-
structure, thus producing higher reported rates tent ways to reduce overall stress levels. As
of child abuse in stepfamilies. the number of stepfamilies increase, expecta-
Second, research priority should be given to tions for stepparents should become more
testing certain empirical questions derived clearly defined. If conflicts over authority play
from the theories reviewed here. Special em- a part in child abuse then stepparents can be
phasis should be given to four factors: (a) the counseled to avoid the frustration of wanting a
objective and the relative level of economic position of authority too soon, and finding it
resources available to the family, (b) the nature blocked by other family members.4
of norms for the stepparent role and the In conclusion, no one factor or set of factors
strength of sanctions associated with those may be sufficient to explain child abuse in
norms, (c) the differential use of resources to stepfamilies. Further research on the combina-
benefit natural children over stepchildren, par- tion of empirical questions suggested by all of
ticularly when overall resources are scarce, these theories is most likely to further our
and (d) the link between the extent of and ac- understanding of this issue. We suggest that
ceptance of the stepparent's authority and those factors most amenable to manipulation
child abuse. and alteration through intervention receive
Stress theory, resource theory and the priority attention.
social-evolutionary theory all emphasize the
link between resources and child abuse. How-
ever, stress theory and resource theory do not
require the assumption of a biologically based 4See D. Mills in this special issue for a discussion of this
cause. In contrast, social-evolutionary theory idea.

412 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Maddox, B. (1975). The half-parent: Living with other people's
children. New York: Evans.
Maden, M. (1980). Disposition of reported child abuse. Sara-
REFERENCES toga, CA: Century 21 Press.
Maden, M. F., & Wrench, D. F. (1977). Significant findings in
Burgess, R. L., & Garbarino,J. (1983). Doing what comes child abuse research. Victimology, 2(2), 196-224.
naturally?An evolutionary perspective on child abuse. In Martin, M. J., & Walters, J. (1982). Familial correlates of
D. Finkelhor, R. J. Gelles, G. T. Hotaling, & M. A. Straus selected types of child abuse and neglect. Journal of Mar-
(Eds.), The dark side of families: Currentfamily violence riage and the Family, 44, 267-276.
research (pp.88-101). Beverly Hills: Sage. Meiselman, K. (1978). Incest. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cherlin, A. (1978). Remarriageas an incomplete institution. National Incidence Study on Child Abuse and Neglect. (1981).
AmericanJournal of Sociology, 84(3),634-650. Washington: National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1980). Discriminative parental solici- Nelson, M., & Nelson, G. K. (1982). Problems of equity in the
tude: A biological perspective. Journal of Marriageand the reconstituted family: A social exchange analysis. Family
Family,42(2),277-288. Relations, 31(2), 223-231.
Elmer, E. (1967). Children in jeopardy: A study of abused Norton, A., & Glick, P. (1979). Marital instability in America:
minors and their families. Pittsburgh: University of Pitts- Past, present, and future. In G. Levinger, & 0. C. Moles
burgh Press. (Eds.), Divorce and separation: Context, causes, and conse-
Fast, I., &Cain, A. C. (1966).The stepparent role: Potential for quences (pp. 6-19). New York: Basic Books.
disturbances in family functioning. American Journal of Pasley, K., & Ihinger-Tallman, M. (1982). Stress in second fami-
Othopsychiatry,36, 485-491. lies. Family Perspective, 16, 81-86.
Finkelhor,D., & Hotaling,G. T. (1983).Sexual abuse in the Na- Phelan, R., & Joyce, B. (1980). The CSATP program model:
tional Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect. Final Clients, treatment and family growth. Unpublished report of
reportto the National Center for Child Abuse and Neglect. an inquiry into the Child Sexual Abuse Treatment Program
Finkelhor,D., & Redfield, D. (1982).Public definitions of child through interviews with counselors. Palo Alto, CA: The
sexual abuse. Unpublished paper, Universityof New Hamp- Scripps Center.
shire. Rallings, E. M. (1976). The special role of stepfather. The Fami-
Furstenberg, F. F. (1979). Recycling the family. Marriageand ly Coordinator, 25, 445-449.
Family Review, 2(3), 12-22. Shepher, J. (1971). Mate selection among second generation
Garbarino, J., & Gilliam, G. (1980). Understanding Abusive Kibbutz adolescents and adults. Archives of Sexual Behav-
Families. Lexington, MA:Lexington Books. ior, 1, 293-307.
Gelles, R. J., &Straus, M.A. (1979).Determinantsof violence in Spanier, G. B., & Glick, P. C. (1980). Paths to remarriage. Jour-
the family:Towarda theoretical integration. In W. R. Burr, nal of Divorce, 3(3), 283-298.
R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.),Contemporarytheories Spinetta, J. J., & Rigler, D. (1972). The child abusing parent: A
about the family (pp. 549-559). New York:The Free Press. psychological review. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 296-304.
Gil, D. (1970). Violence against children:Physical child abuse Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind
in the United States. Cambridge:HarvardUniversityPress. closed doors: Violence in the American family. Garden City,
Glick, P. C. (1979).Childrenof divorced parents in demographic NY: Doubleday/Anchor Press.
perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 35(4), 170-182. Tierney, K. J., & Corwin, D. L. (1983). Exploring intra-familial
Goetting, A. (1982). The six stations of remarriage:Develop- child sexual abuse: A systems approach. In D. Finkelhor,
mental tasks of remarriageafter divorce. Family Relations, R. J. Gelles, G. T. Hotaling, & M. A. Straus (Eds.), The dark
31, 213-222. side of families: Current family violence research, (pp.
Goode, W.J. (1971).Force and violence in the family.Journalof 102-116). Beverly Hills: Sage.
Marriageand the Family,33, 624-636. Trivers, R. L. (1971). Parental investment and sexual selection.
Herman,J. (1981).Father daugher incest. Cambridge:Harvard In B. Campbell, (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of
UniversityPress. man: 1871-1971 (pp. 136-179). Chicago: Aldine.
Johnson, B., & Morse, H. A. (1968). Injuredchildren and their U. S. Bureau of the Census. (1974, July). Current Population Re-
parents. Children,15, 147-152. ports, Series p-23 No. 48.
Johnson, C. L. (1974). Child abuse irnthe Southeast. Athens, van den Berghe, P. (1983). Human inbreeding avoidance: Cul-
GA: Regional Institute of Social Welfare Research, Univer- ture in nature. The Behavior and Brain Sciences, 6, 91-123.
sity of Georgia. Visher, E. B., & Visher, J. S. (1979). Stepfamilies: A guide to
Justice, B., &Justice, R. (1976).Theabusing family. New York: working with stepparents and stepchildren. New York: Brun-
HumanSciences Press. ner Mazel.
Keshet, J. K. (1980).Fromseparation to stepfamily. Journalof Wasserman, S. (1967). The abused parent of the abused child.
FamilyIssues, 1(4),517-532. Children, 14, 175-179.
Kimball,W. H., Stewart, R. B., Conger, R. D., & Burgess, R. L. Wilson, M., Daly, M., & Weghorst, S. J. (1980). Household com-
(1980).A comparison of family interaction in single versus position and the risk of child abuse and neglect. Journal of
two parent abusive, neglectful and normal families. In Biosocial Science, 12, 333-340.
R. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, &A. Sostek (Eds.),High risk Wolf, A. P. (1966). Childhood association, sexual attraction and
infants and children:Adultand peer interactions (pp.43-59). the incest taboo: A Chinese case. American Anthropologist,
New York:AcademicPress. 50, 383-398.
Levinger,G. (1966).Sources of maritalsatisfaction among ap- Wulkan, D., & Bulkey, J. (1981). Analysis of incest statutes. In
plicants for divorce. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, J. Bulkey, (Ed.), Child sexual abuse and the law (pp. 52-80).
36, 804-806. Washington: American Bar Association.

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 413

This content downloaded from 130.56.64.29 on Thu, 05 Nov 2015 12:52:30 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like