You are on page 1of 10

The Spanish Journal of Psychology Copyright 2009 by The Spanish Journal of Psychology

2009, Vol. 12, No. 2, 667-676 ISSN 1138-7416

Leadership in Nonprofit Organizations of Nicaragua and


El Salvador: A Study from the Social Identity Theory

Juan Antonio Moriano León1, Gabriela Topa Cantisano1,


and Jean-Pierre Lévy Mangin2
1Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
2Université du Québec (Canada)

This study follows the social identity model of leadership proposed by van Knippenberg
and Hogg (2003), in order to examine empirically the mediator effect of leadership
prototypicality between social identity, extra effort, and perceived effectiveness of group
members. The sample consisted of 109 participants who worked in 22 different work-
teams of non-profit organizations (NPO) from Nicaragua and El Salvador. The data
analysis was performed through structural equation modeling (SEM). The results show
that NPO membership is related to a high level of social identity. In addition, the results
confirmed that leadership prototypicality has a significant and positive mediator effect
in the relationship between the group identification and the group members’ extra effort
and the perceived effectiveness of leadership.
Keywords: leadership, social identity, prototypicality, extra effort, effectiveness.

El presente estudio toma como referencia el Modelo de la Identidad Social del Liderazgo
(SIMOL) de van Knippenberg y Hogg (2003), con el objetivo de contrastar empíricamente
el efecto mediador de la prototipicidad del líder entre la identificación con el grupo de
trabajo, y la eficacia percibida del líder y el esfuerzo extra de los miembros del grupo.
La muestra está formada por 109 personas que trabajaban en 22 grupos diferentes
pertenecientes a Organizaciones No Lucrativas (ONL) de Nicaragua y El Salvador. El
análisis de los datos se realizó mediante el Modelado de Ecuaciones Estructurales
(SEM). Los resultados revelan que la pertenencia a ONL de Nicaragua y El Salvador
promueve altos niveles de identificación grupal, y, asimismo, se confirma que la
prototipicidad del líder tiene un significativo efecto mediador entre la identificación grupal,
y la eficacia percibida del líder y el esfuerzo extra de los miembros del grupo.
Palabras clave: liderazgo, identidad social, prototipicidad, esfuerzo extra, eficacia.

This article was funded as part of the actions to aid the dissemination of the Research Promotion Plan of the UNED. The authors wish
to thank Alfonso Pérez for his contribution in collecting the data.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juan Antonio Moriano León, Dpto. de Psicología Social y de las
Organizaciones, Facultad de Psicología, UNED.C/ Juan del Rosal 10 (Ciudad Universitaria). Madrid, 28040 (Spain). Phone: +34-
913988251; E-mail: jamoriano@psi.uned.es

667
668 MORIANO, TOPA, AND LÉVY

The approach of Social Identity Theory (hereafter, SIT) This perspective of leadership led Reicher et al. (2005)
developed originally as a theory about intergroup relations to consider the relation between leaders and followers as
(Turner, 1982), but, in recent years, there has been increasing one of partners and collaborators in the transformation of
interest in its application to group processes in organizations the social reality. According to these authors, the traditional
(Hogg, 2001). Identification with the work group can lead models consider leadership as a kind of zero-sum game,
individuals to adopt as their own the interests and goals of in which the influence of the leader is achieved at the
the collective, so that people would be mainly interested expense of the followers’ influence and vice versa. In
in the group goals instead of their own individual interests, contrast to this viewpoint, leadership is a process of
which would be less relevant, as revealed in empirical studies collective social influence, in which leaders and followers
that provide evidence of the relation between identification are partners. Thus, the salient social identity makes leadership
and efforts for the benefit of the group (Ashforth & Mael, possible and, in turn, leaders’ actions reproduce and promote
1989; van Knippenberg, 2000). the social identity and, therefore, make their own
With regard to the process of leadership, SIT argues that leadership—feasible (van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003),
prior theories of leadership have not granted importance to The SIT proposes the group prototypes are configured
the fact that leaders are also members of the groups they to maximize the proportion or ratio of between-group and
direct and that, therefore, the characteristics of the leaders within-group differences within a particular comparison
as members of a group, and more specifically, their higher context (Hogg, 1993; Hogg & Terry, 2000). Thus, they
or lower prototypicality, affects the effectiveness of the group underline what “we” share and what makes us different
(van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003). Thus, leadership is not from “the others,” to maximize the extent to which the group
only an issue of leaders, or even of leaders and followers. is a clear and different entity. So, the process of influence
Rather, it involves the relation between leaders and followers is related to the concept of prototypicality and is explained
within a social group (Reicher, Haslam, & Hopkins, 2005). by membership or belonging to the group, by means of
From these initial proposals, the goal of this study was which, the members let themselves be influenced by the
to contrast empirically, in various nonprofit organizations group prototype and, in turn, the prototype is influenced
(NPOs) of Nicaragua and El Salvador, the predictions of by the group members and the context.
the SIT and, more specifically, the Social Identity Model In groups with high identification, the members are very
of Leadership (hereafter, SIMOL) of van Knippenberg and sensitive to prototypicality as the basis for self-perception
Hogg (2003) concerning group identification and the effects and perception of other group members. In fact, they notice
of prototypicality in the perceived effectiveness of leadership and respond to subtle differences in the degree of
and the group members’ extra efforts. prototypicality of the members that form the group (Hogg,
2001). The essential prediction of the SIMOL is that the
Leadership from the theory of social identity higher the identification of the members with their group,
the more their perception, assessment, and approval of the
Leadership is a phenomenon that occurs in all human leader will be influenced by his prototypicality and, in turn,
groups and that has captured the interest of thinkers and writers the more likely the prototypical members will emerge as
from all cultures for hundreds of years. Currently, four leaders (Hogg, 2001; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003).
important aspects of leadership are acknowledged (Molero, Ultimately, in groups with high group identification, the
2004): (a) that leadership is not a trait or static characteristic prototypical members will become effective leaders more
residing only in the leader, but a process that takes place easily and the leaders will be more effective if they use
over time, in which the leader, the followers, and the situation their prototypical credentials. Following these considerations,
in which leadership is exercised are interactively involved; we suggest the following hypothesis:
(b) leadership involves influence, that is, what characterizes
leaders is their ability to influence the other group members; Hypothesis 1a: the identification of the members with the
(c) leadership is a group phenomenon that loses its meaning group is positively related to the leader’s prototypicality.
outside of this context; and (d) the leader’s influence aims to The effectiveness of leadership has been assessed in
obtain a goal or common objective of the group. diverse studies through two main variables: perceived
From the SIT perspective, leadership depends on the efficacy and extra effort (Bass & Avolio, 2000). Firstly,
existence of a social identity shared by the group members. perceived efficacy refers to the followers’ perception of
Without such an identity, nothing unites leaders and followers the leader’s efficacy to conduct and represent the group,
and, therefore, there would not be sufficient consensus for as well as to meet the requirements and expectations of
a leader to represent the group. Thus, the SIMOL underlines the organization. According to SIT perspective, leadership
the fact that leaders are not only leaders of groups of people, is understood as a group phenomenon that appears when
but also members of these groups, emerging complementarily, the members identify with the group. When people identify
and that group membership has a strong impact on the with the group, they are more easily influenced and are
attitude and behavior of the group members. more motivated to cooperate with the leader, because the
LEADERSHIP IN NONPROFIT ORGANIZATIONS 669

leader’s behavior is mainly aimed at achieving group goals


and at representing the group (Hogg, Hains, & Mason, 1998).
In contrast, when people do not identify with the group,
but act independently, then the leader’s role may be less
important, because it is no longer a matter of achieving
group goals, but individual goals. According to this
reasoning, we propose the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 1b: group identification is positively related to


the leader’s perceived efficacy. Figure 1. Summary of the study hypotheses. H = Hypothesis.
Secondly, extra effort is operationalized through extra-
role (undetermined) behaviors that the diverse group Depersonalization leads people to enter fully into collective
members are prepared to perform to achieve the group goals. behavior, as their perception and performance are no longer
To the degree to which people identify more with their work those of a mere singular person, but of a person who feels
group, it is more likely for them to adopt the interests and like a member of a group with which he or she identifies
goals of the group as their own. This implies that people (Morales, 2002). Following these arguments, we expect that
will be more interested in the group goals than in their own the leader’s prototypicality mediates partially the relationship
personal concerns and that the latter would be more between group identification and leadership effectiveness
irrelevant, as revealed in empirical studies that provide (consisting of the group members’ perception of the efficacy
evidence of the relation between identification and efforts of their leader and their extra effort to achieve the group
for the benefit of the group (Doosje, Spears, & Ellemers, goals). Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses:
2002; Mael & Ashforth, 1992). According to this proposal,
we suggest the following hypothesis: Hypothesis 2a: The relation between group identification
and the leader’s perceived efficacy is partially mediated
Hypothesis 1c: group identification will be positively by the leader’s prototypicality.
related to the members’ extra effort.
The most essential prediction of SIT is that the more Hypothesis 2b: The relation between group identification
the members identify with the group, the higher leadership and the extra effort of the group members is partially
effectiveness will be based on the degree of the leaders’ mediated by the leader’s prototypicality.
prototypicality (Hogg & Terry, 2000; van Knippenberg & In Figure 1 is displayed the structural model that
Hogg, 2003). This has been confirmed in laboratory studies represents the relations predicted in this study. In this model,
of emerging leadership (Hains, Hogg, & Duck, 1997; Hogg there is one exogenous latent variable (group identification)
et al., 1998). According to Hogg and Terry (2000), this and three endogenous latent variables (leader’s prototypicality,
process is explained by three aspects: leader’s perceived efficacy, and group members’ extra effort).
1) Prototypicality becomes a seedbed for attitudinal and
behavioral depersonalization when the members
identify with the group. Thus, the leader becomes Method
the personification of the aspirations, attitudes, and
behaviors of the group. Participants
2) The more prototypical members are more socially
attractive than the less prototypical members. This The sample of this study consisted of 109 people, who
implies that the leader is socially attractive to the carried out tasks in 22 work groups with a mean of 5
group members and, therefore, they tend to agree members in each group. The participants were of three
with and to try to please the leader. nationalities: 10 Spaniards, 74 Nicaraguans, and 25
3) Group members tend to attribute the social attraction Salvadorans. Of the sample, 49.5% were men, with a mean
and influence that leaders exert over the group to age of 32.06 years (SD = 8.17), and a mean duration in
the leaders’ personality characteristics, rather than the organization of approximately 3 years (M = 30.43
to their prototypical position within the group. When months, SD = 28.02 months) and 2 years membership in
this attribution error is maintained over time, the their work group (M = 24.40 months, SD = 21.47).
leader is considered to have a charismatic personality. In this study participated a total of 14 NPOs from
Such prototypicality-based leadership can be very effective Nicaragua and 8 from El Salvador. The types of
and mutually satisfactory for leaders and followers, because organizations were as follows: Spanish NGOs in El
there is no need to be coercive, as the change in attitude Salvador (22%), Salvadoran NGOs (16.5%), municipal
and the influence occur automatically by means of the process town halls (10.1%); Nicaraguan NGOs (33.9%), and others
of depersonalization associated with group identification. – basically Nicaraguan schools (17.4%). Lastly, the
670 MORIANO, TOPA, AND LÉVY

educational and professional levels were relatively high, circles). Secondly, a Group Identification Scale (GIS), made
no doubt because of the minimal demands requested in up of 7 items, ranging from 0 (completely disagree) to 4
these organizations to have access to a remunerated job. (completely agree), adapted from the scales of Henry, Arrow,
Specifically, 55% had university studies and 88% had and Carini (1999), and van Dick, Wagner, Stellmacher, and
studies equal to or higher than high school and professional Christ (2004), was also used. The reliability of this scale
training. With regard to participants’ professional level in was moderately satisfactory with a Cronbach alpha value
the study, 50.5% had jobs that were equal to or higher of .66. The items that decreased the internal consistency
than Area and/or Projects Technician and medium level were deleted and a new scale made up of 3 items was
professionals. obtained, with satisfactory reliability (α = .70). The items
were the following: (1) “I feel identified as a member of
Measures the work group,” (2) “I like to work for my group,” and
(3) “I enjoy dealing with the members of this work group.”
Below are described the diverse scales and measures These sentences focus mainly on the affective aspects of
included in the questionnaire used in this study. identification and make up a complementary measurement
Bibliographical data. In the first part of the questionnaire, of the GGIS.
participants were asked their age, sex, nationality, type of Leader prototypicality. As with social identity, we used
organization, educational and professional level. two different types of scales to assess the leader’s
Group identification. Identification with the work group prototypicality. Firstly, we used a Graphic Scale of Leader’s
is considered a specific form of social identity in work Prototypicality in the Group (GLP) that was specifically
spheres. The degree of social identity with the work group developed for this research (see Figure 3). Secondly, a
was assessed with two scales. Firstly, a Graphic Group new scale was developed to measure the leader’s
Identification Scale (GGIS), adapted from the graphic scale prototypicality according to the SIMOL. On this scale,
of Shamir and Kark (2004), was used to assess the degree participants indicated, on a 5-point Likert-scale ranging
of the person’s identification with the work group with a from 0 (not at all) to 4 (completely), the degree to which
single item (see Figure 2). Participants were asked to indicate the person in charge of their work group: (a) represents
the square which best reflected their degree of identification the most characteristic traits of the group, (b) would be
with their work group on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging elected by the group members to represent the group before
from 1 (completely separate circles) to 7 (overlapping others; (c) is well-liked by the group members, and (d)

Figure 2. Graphic Group Identification Scale (Shamir & Kark, 2004).

Figure 3. Graphic Scale of Leader Prototypicality in the Group


Note. The instructions of this scale were: “In the following chart, the degree to which the person in charge of the work group represents
the group is assessed. Below, you will find 7 rectangles with 2 circles: one represents the person in charge (R) and the other represents the
group you belong to (G). In the first rectangle, the circles are very far apart and they represent a situation in which the person in charge is
very remote from the group and, therefore, does not represent group identity. In contrast, in the last rectangle, the person in charge is at
the center of the group and, therefore, is the most representative member. Your task is to choose from the 7 rectangles the one that best
reflects the degree to which you consider that the person in charge represents the characteristics of the group”.
LEADERSHIP IN NONPROFIT ORGANIZATIONS 671

personifies the most positive values of the group. The Results


reliability of this scale was satisfactory, as it reached a
Cronbach alpha of .89. Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Perceived leadership effectiveness. The scales of the
leader’s perceived efficacy and followers’ extra effort of Confirmatory factor analysis was carried out to assess
the Multi-factor Leadership Questionnaire, short form 5x reliability and validity of the scales used in this research.
(MLQ; Bass & Avolio, 2000), were used. The scale of the Firstly, we analyzed convergent validity, which is
leader’s perceived efficacy was made up of 3 items and its determined by the statistical significance of the factor
reliability was satisfactory (α = .79). Likewise, the scale loadings of the indicators of each construct. This means
of the followers’ extra effort was made up of 3 items and that for a probability of .05, the critical ratio (CR) should
obtained satisfactory reliability (α = .82). be higher than 1.96. Moreover, the standardized factor
loadings of each indicator in the latent variable on which
Procedure it saturates should be higher than .40 (Hair, Anderson,
Tatham, & Black, 2005). As shown in Table 1, all the
Once the questionnaire had been elaborated and taking standardized loadings and CRs exceed the recommended
into account that it would be administered to a different minimum levels of .40 and 1.96, respectively.
cultural sample than the Spanish one, a pilot study was Secondly, the composite reliability coefficient was
carried out. In this pilot study, we found that participants calculated because it is more adequate than the Cronbach
were concerned about their anonymity, because some items alpha, as it does not depend on the attributes associated with
were focused on their superiors and they feared the latter each concept. Normally, it is recommended that this statistic
might have access to the results of the questionnaire. This reach a value of .70, although some authors maintain that
situation led us to include the following paragraph in the values over .60 are sufficient (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988). As shown
introduction of the questionnaire: “The data you provide in Table 1, all the composite reliability coefficients exceed
is completely confidential and anonymous, therefore, do .70. However, the average variance extracted (AVE) reflects
not be concerned about responding sincerely.” the total quantity of variance of the indicators captured by
We should clarify that although the sample was made the latent construct. The higher their value, the more
up of people who belonged to the work groups, the level representative are the indicators of the construct on which
of analysis was individual, as there were no within-group they load. In general, it is suggested that its value should
measures. not exceed .50 (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988). In this study, the

Table 1
Results of the confirmatory factor analysis
Construct / Indicator Standardized factor loading CR Reliability Extracted variance
1. Group identification .73 .40
GGIS .64 —
II .60 5.07
I2 .60 5.06
I3 .69 5.49
2. Prototypicality .89 .63
P1 .89 —
P2 .87 12.57
P3 .75 9.56
P4 .80 10.70
GLP .64 7.78
3. Extra effort .82 .61
EE1 .86 —
EE2 .80 9.02
EE3 .68 7.24
4. Perceived efficacy .81 .58
PE1 .86 —
PE2 .65 7.13
PE3 .77 8.80
672 MORIANO, TOPA, AND LÉVY

variance of prototypicality, perceived efficacy, and extra effort a first model is assessed, with only the influence of group
exceeded .50, whereas the variance of group identification identification on the followers’ perception of efficacy and
only reached .40 (see Table 1). extra effort, represented in Figure 1, which includes the
To assess discriminant validity between the constructs, mediation of the leader’s prototypicality. In both models,
the square root of the mean extracted variance should be control variables such as nationality and duration in the
higher than the correlation between constructs (Fornell & work group were also included.
Larcker, 1981; Lévy-Mangin, 2003). Table 2 presents the The AMOS 6.0 program was used because it offers the
correlations between the constructs and, along the diagonal, possibility of assessing diverse relations simultaneously
the square root of the mean extracted variance. In view of and is particularly adequate for small samples (Arbuckle
these data, the constructs assessed in the model have & Wothke, 1999). The analyses were carried out using the
discriminant validity, although they are all closely related. matrix of the original data as input and the maximum
likelihood procedure. Diverse indexes were suggested to
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations test the goodness of fit of the model (Lévy-Mangin, 2003),
such as the chi-square statistic, c2, the value of which should
The results of the study show high levels of identification be nonsignificant (p >.05) in order to indicate that the
of the members with their work group and high prototypicality proposed model fits the data. Due to the sensitivity of the
of the group leader (see Table 2). Specifically, a mean score chi-square statistic to sample size and the deviations from
of 3.20 for group identification and of 2.61 for prototypicality normality of the data, other absolute fit indexes were
(for response intervals from 0 to 4) was found. The proposed, such as c2/degrees of freedom, where values lower
correlations between the variables of this study provide support than 2 indicate a good fit, and the root mean square error
for the hypotheses proposed in this work. As shown in Table of approximation (RMSEA), which is a measure of model
2, group identification had a high and significantly positive discrepancy by degrees of freedom, whose values should
correlation both with prototypicality and with perceived be lower than .05 to indicate a good fit. Likewise, we used
efficacy and extra effort. Likewise, the prototypicality showed the relative comparative fit index (CFI), whose threshold
high, positive, and significant correlations with perceived value is .95 to consider that the proposed model has a good
efficacy and extra effort. fit (Hu & Bentler, 1995). Table 3 shows the fit statistics of
both models proposed in this study.
Structural Equation Modeling The first model, which only shows the direct influence
of group identification, had adequate goodness-of-fit statistics,
Structural equation modeling was used to contrast the but it only explained 27% of the variance of perceived efficacy
second hypothesis of this work. For this purpose, the fit of and 39% of the variance of extra effort. The second model,

Table 2
Descriptive statistics, correlations among the variables and AVE
M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Nationality — —
2. Duration in the group 24.40 21.47 –.15 —
3. Group identification 3.20 0.56 .02 .07 .63
4. Prototypicality 2.61 0.92 –.06 .02 .53** .79
5. Extra effort 2.47 0.91 –.14 .06 .47** .59** .78
6. Perceived efficacy 2.69 0.89 –.11 –.02 .39** .69** .66** .76

Note. Response scale, ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (completely); along the diagonal of the correlations is the square root of the
average variance extracted (AVE).
** p < .01.

Table 3
Measures of the fit of the model
Model χ2 (d.f.) p CMIN/DF RMSEA CFI
Model with only direct effects on identity 53.2 (46) .217 1.15 .04 .98
Model with effects mediated by prototypicality 128.4 (107) .078 1.20 .04 .97

Note. CMIN/DF = χ2/degrees of freedom; RMSEA = Root mean square error of approximation; CFI = Comparative fit index.
LEADERSHIP IN NONPROFIT ORGANIZATIONS 673

where prototypicality acts as a mediating variable, not only members’ extra effort, explaining, respectively, 62 and 50%
presented adequate goodness of fit, but in addition, the of the variance of these variables. Moreover, the leader’s
percentage of explained variance in each of the criteria was prototypicality acts as a total mediating variable, because
higher. In Figure 4, group identification is observed to the effects of group identification on perceived efficacy and
significantly affect the leader’s prototypicality and it explained extra effort were no longer significant in this second model.
48% of its variance, so that, the higher the degree identification Lastly, we confirmed that the control variables, nationality
of the members with their group, the more the leader is and duration in the group, had no significant relation with
considered to represent the group. In turn, prototypicality group identification, nor did they significantly affect the
significantly affected perceived efficacy and the group leader’s prototypicality, extra effort, and perceived efficacy.

Figure 4. Standardized estimations for the proposed models.


674 MORIANO, TOPA, AND LÉVY

Discussion sample. Secondly, it would be appropriate to have a broader


array of measures to test a more complete model. Among
The results of this study reveal that group identification such measures could be considered diverse focal points of
positively affects the leader’s perceived efficacy and the identification, such as the complete organization, the
extra effort of the members of NPOs’ work groups in department, the work group, or the profession. In this sense,
Nicaragua and El Salvador. These results provide empirical one could explore whether these different ways of
support to the predictions of the SIT about the leadership identification significantly affect leadership effectiveness
process (Hogg, 2001; Reicher et al., 2005; van Knippenberg in any way, as well as followers’ extra effort. Regarding
& Hogg, 2003), as they indicate that group identification leader prototypicality, it should be taken into account that
leads individuals to adopt as their own the goals of the it is a new measurement developed specifically for this
group, so that people make an extra effort for the benefit study and, therefore, it is necessary to carry out studies
of the group and they acknowledge the leader’s efficacy that allow generalizing its use.
to lead and represent the group.
The findings also show that the influence of group Implications of the Study
identification on leadership effectiveness is totally mediated
by the group leader’s prototypicality. Thus, the more the The results of this study shows that promoting and
members identify with their group, the more leadership facilitating high work group identification and high leader
effectiveness is based on the degree to which the leader is prototypicality can be a great investment and a highly
perceived as the group prototype. This particular result pertinent decision for the management of any type of NPO.
exceeds our hypotheses, because we only expected to find Specifically, team leaders can promote high identification
a partial mediation of the leader’s prototypicality. The with the work group through the establishment of
explanation may be found in the disproportionate influence collective goals. As noted by van Knippenberg and
that the prototypical leader has on the group members Ellemers (2004), the individual systems of incentives not
through diverse mechanisms such as social attraction, only have a limited capacity to induce employers’
attribution of charisma, etc. (Hogg, 2001; van Knippenberg performance but, in addition, they reduce the investments
& Hogg, 2003). However, the leaders do not fulfill a passive of the group members in behaviors that serve group
function when they are defined as such by the processes interest, because the members focus on merely individual
of social identity, but instead they intervene actively creating concerns and goals.
and redefining the contents of the group identity, which With regard to increase leader’s prototypicality, van
produces a transformation in their followers. In fact, the Knippenberg and Hogg (2003) describe how the leaders
leaders’ activity is not limited to words and ideas, but also try to seem more like a team member, and to describe
extends to the initiation of structures that can make those themselves as prototype of their groups. For this purpose,
ideas manifest (Reicher et al., 2005). leaders may use emblems and symbols that represent the
With regard to other theories of leadership, the results group, and focus their work on group goals, even going so
of this study support the idea that the influence of far as to make personal sacrifices for the group. For example,
transformational leadership on the efficacy of work teams they dress like the rest of the team members, they eat at
(Lowe, Kroeck, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996; Molero, the same place, park their car in the same parking lot, and
Cuadrado, Navas, & Morales, 2007), especially their participate in activities with the rest of the group outside
charisma factor, is partially due to the leader’s prototypicality of working hours.
(Hogg, 2001; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003). In fact, However, group identification and leader prototypicality
using the measures of leadership effectiveness proposed cannot be conceived as simple “technical” tools in Human
by Bass and Avolio (2000), the results of this study show Resources management without further critical analyses or
that the leader’s prototypicality explains one half of the contextualizations. For example, groups can develop
variance of extra effort and 62% of the variance of the counterproductive rules when there is a conflict between
leader’s perceived efficacy. the leader and members and/or when the organization is
perceived as exploitive or unproductive (Topa & Morales,
Limitations of the Study 2006). In another context, the implicit rule in a certain
environment may be to do as little as possible and leave
The results of this study are limited by several factors. as soon as possible and, in this case, the higher the group
Firstly, the participants were not selected by random identification, the less productivity (van Knippenberg, 2000).
procedures and they only belonged to NPOs from Nicaragua Moreover, leaders are not the exclusive owners of social
and El Salvador, which limits the ability to generalize the identity nor are the followers passive consumers of the
results of this study to other kinds of organizations and leader’s prototypicality. Followers frequently reject leaders
countries. Also, the sample size was relatively small, so who have violated or broken the contents of their own social
this study should be replicated with a larger and more diverse identity (Haslam & Platow, 2001).
LEADERSHIP IN NONPROFIT ORGANIZATIONS 675

This study can also contribute to the previous research Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. (1997). Self-
on the extra-role behaviors, in particular about organizational categorization and leadership: Effects of group prototypicality
citizenship behavior (hereafter, OCB). The leader’s and leader stereotypicality. Personality and Social Psychology
prototypicality may affect the group members’ OCB through Bulletin, 23, 1087-1100.
various mechanisms. Firstly, the leader’s prototypicality Hair, J.F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (2005).
will cause the group members’ most significant and Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
important behaviors to become characteristic and Prentice Hall
prototypical, because they fit the general framework of Haslam, S. A., & Platow, M. J. (2001). The link between leadership
group performance and reinforce the feeling that the group, and followership: How affirming social identity translates
as a whole, is advancing towards a common goal (Organ, vision into action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 2006). Secondly, leader 27, 1469-1479.
prototypical can provide the group members with an Henry, K. B., Arrow, H., & Carini, B. (1999). A tripartite model
appropriate model that will promote the OCBs in various of group identification: Theory and measurement. Small Group
ways. On the one hand, the leader becomes a model of Research, 30, 558-581.
behavior and thus serves to establish the rules or expectations Hogg, M. A. (1993). Group cohesiveness: A critical review and
of appropriate work behavior, increasing the probability some new directions. European Journal of Social Psychology,
that the employees will feel obliged to display such civic 4, 85-111.
behaviors (Organ et al., 2006). On the other hand, as Hogg, M. A. (2001). A social identity theory of leadership.
mentioned, prototypical leaders can promote cooperative Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 184-200.
behaviors, which are part of the OCBs, because they Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C., & Mason, I. (1998). Identification
encourage employees to work together towards a common and leadership in small groups: Salience, frame of reference,
goal. Thus, we increase the perception of a shared identity and leader stereotypicality effects on leader evaluations. Journal
and the probability that efforts aimed at individual interests of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1248-1263.
will be given up in order to devote one’s efforts toward Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social identity and self-
more altruistic behaviors, which are forms of OCBs. categorization processes in organizational contexts. The
An interesting aspect to explore in future studies is the Academy of Management Review, 25, 121-140.
probability that trust is the key mediator in the relationship Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H.
between leader prototypicality and the group members’ Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling. Concepts, issues
OCB. Some studies on the paradigm of transformational and applications. (pp. 77-100). London: Sage.
leadership suggest interesting results along these lines (Pillai, Lévy-Mangin, J. P. (2003). Análisis multivariable para las ciencias
Schriesheim, & Williams, 1999; Podsakoff, MacKenzie, sociales. Madrid.
Moorman, & Fetter, 1990). Lowe, K., Kroeck, K., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996).
Effectiveness correlates of transformational and transactional
leadership: A meta-analytic review. The Leadership Quarterly,
References 7, 385-425.
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. (1992). Alumni and their alma mater: A
Arbuckle, J. L., & Wothke, W. (1999). Amos 4.0 User’s Guide. partial test of the reformulated model of organizational
Chicago: Small Waters. identification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 103-
Ashforth, B., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the 123.
organization. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20-39. Molero, F. (2004). El liderazgo. In C. Huici (Ed.), Psicología de
Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural grupos I (pp. 141-169). Madrid: UNED.
equation models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Molero, F., Cuadrado, I., Navas, M., & Morales, J. F. (2007).
16, 74-94. Relations and effects of transformational leadership: A
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (2000). MLQ-Multi-Factor Leadership comparative analysis with traditional leadership styles. The
Questionnaire, technical report. Redwood City, CA: Mind Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 358-368.
Garden. Morales, J. F. (2002). Nuevos desarrollos en identidad social. In
Doosje, B., Spears, R., & Ellemers, N. (2002). Social identity as J. F. Morales, D. Paez, A. L. Kornblit, & D. Asún (Eds.),
both cause and effect: The development of group identification Psicología social (pp. 387-395). Buenos Aires: Pearson.
in response to anticipated and actual changes in the intergroup Organ, D., Podsakoff, P., & MacKenzie, S. (2006). Organizational
status hierarchy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, citizenship behavior. Its nature, antecedents, and consequences.
57-76. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation Pillai, R., Schriesheim, C., & Williams, E. (1999). Fairness
models with unobservable variables and measurement error: perceptions and trust as mediators for transformational and
Algebra and statistics. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, transactional leadership: A two-sample study. Journal of
39-50. Management, 25, 897-933.
676 MORIANO, TOPA, AND LÉVY

Podsakoff , P., MacKenzie, S., Moorman, R., & Fetter, R. (1990). Van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Stellmacher, J., & Christ, O. (2004).
Transformational leader behavior and their effects on followers’ The utility of a broader conceptualization of organizational
trust in leader, satisfaction and organizational citizenship identification: Which of its aspects really matters? Journal of
behaviors. Leadership Quarterly, 1, 107-142. Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77, 171-191.
Reicher, S., Haslam, S. A., & Hopkins, N. (2005). Social identity van Knippenberg, D. (2000). Work motivation and performance:
and the dynamics of leadership: Leaders and followers as A social identity perspective. Applied Psychology: An
collaborative agents in the transformation of social reality. International Review, 49, 357-371.
The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 547-568. van Knippenberg, D., & Ellemers, N. (2004). Social identity and group
Shamir, B., & Kark, R. (2004). A simple graphic scale for the performance. identification as the key to group-oriented effort. In
measurement of organizational identification. Journal of A. Haslam, D. van Knippenberg, M. J. Platow, & N. Ellemers
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 77, 115-123. (Eds.), Social identity at work. Developing theory for organizational
Topa, G., & Morales, J. F. (2006). Identificación organizacional practices (pp. 29-59). New York: Psychology Press.
y proactividad personal en grupos de trabajo: Un modelo van Knippenberg, D., & Hogg, M. (2003). A social identity model
de ecuaciones estructurales. Anales de Psicología, 22, of leadership effectiveness in organizations. Research in
234-242. Organizational Behavior, 25, 243-295.
Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social
group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup Received April 8, 2008
relations. (pp. 15-40). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Revision received January 7, 2009
Press. Accepted January 19, 2009

You might also like