You are on page 1of 9

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience


journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/dcn

Review

The developing brain: From theory to neuroimaging and back


Eveline A. Crone a,b,c,∗ , K. Richard Ridderinkhof c,d
a
Leiden University, Institute for Psychological Research, The Netherlands
b
Leiden Institute for Brain & Cognition, The Netherlands
c
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
d
Cognitive Science Center Amsterdam, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Surprisingly little headway has been made towards understanding how brain growth maps
Received 17 September 2010 onto mental growth during child development. This review aims at bridging and integrat-
Received in revised form 2 December 2010
ing recent human neuroscientific brain maturation findings with the conceptual thinking
Accepted 4 December 2010
of theorists in the behavioural tradition of studying cognitive development. Developmen-
tal research in the field of internal control and self-regulation serves as a reference point
Keywords:
for understanding the relation between brain maturation and mental growth. Using several
Brain maturation
recent neuroimaging findings as points in case, we show how a deeper appreciation of struc-
Neuroimaging
Cognitive development tural and functional neural development can be obtained from considering the traditional
Neuroconstructivism conceptual frameworks, and vice versa. We conclude that paradigmatic progress in devel-
Working memory opmental neuroscience can rely more on knowledge from developmental experimental
psychology, and that developmental models of cognitive development can be constrained
and articulated with more precision on the basis of knowledge of differential structural and
functional brain maturation.
© 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
2. Brain maturation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102
3. Cognitive development theories vis-à-vis brain development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
3.1. Developmental theories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
3.2. Brain development supporting changes in working memory and control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104
4. Merging developmental theory with developmental imaging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106
5. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108

1. Introduction
behaviour. Textbooks on cognitive development are now
Developmental neuroimaging studies have had a great incorporating brain development as further explanations
impact on thinking about developmental changes in for developmental improvements in a wide area of skills
(e.g., Blakemore and Frith, 2005; Goswami, 2008), and neu-
roscientists are speculating about how brain development
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Developmental Psychology, results in changes in cognitive function (e.g., Shaw et al.,
Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333AK Leiden, The Netherlands. 2006). Despite this mutual interest, the two research areas
E-mail address: ecrone@fsw.leidenuniv.nl (E.A. Crone). (developmental psychology and neuroscience) are still seg-

1878-9293/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.12.001
102 E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109

regated and a gap remains between our knowledge of brain confirmed, however, for instance by longitudinal studies
development and cognitive development. Developmental showing consistent relations between individual differ-
neuroimaging studies tend to be data-driven rather than ences in brain growth and cognitive development.
theory-driven; that is, these studies tend to be inspired Here, we set out to revive and rejuvenate this general
more by the prospect of finding differential maturational approach by relating recent neuroimaging findings using
trajectories of specific structures and functions than by pre- cognitive paradigms to the theoretical bases which have
dictions derived from theoretical perspectives on mental shaped the thinking of developmental scientists. Given that
growth (see also Johnson, 2001, 2010). It is not uncom- functional developmental neuroimaging studies to date
mon that developmental changes in cognitive function are have focused mainly childhood and adolescence (ages 6–7
interpreted as the simple maturation of a single brain area and older), we will restrict the review to this age period.
or circuit, ignoring the long history of cognitive theorizing This focus allows us to make more solid inferences on the
about the development of thought and behaviour. Like- relation between brain developmental and cognitive devel-
wise, it is not uncommon that developmental changes in opment. We further restrict our review to the functional
domains such as working memory are explained in terms of domain of internal control and working memory develop-
classic stage-wise development while ignoring the signifi- ment, because this domain is well described in both the
cant improvements that have been made in relating these information-processing theories of cognitive development
functions to brain maturation, which could constrain the and in recent neuroimaging studies. Many of the recom-
currently available developmental theories. mendations we present following this review, however,
The goal of this review is to identify initial steps towards are also applicable to other domains of cognitive develop-
understanding recent neuroscientific brain development ment where links are made with brain development, such
findings in the context of classic developmental theories as inhibition, reasoning, self-regulation, and mentalizing
of cognitive development. We identify several problems functions (e.g., theory of mind).
which occur when neuroimaging studies are designed
without taking into account the prior findings from classic 2. Brain maturation
developmental theories.
Before the advent of developmental neuroimaging, sev- Biological models of brain development have made
eral endeavors have been undertaken towards integrating great progress in understanding the development of brain
mental development and brain maturation (e.g., Crnic structure, especially since the use of magnetic resonance
and Pennington, 1987; Dawson and Fischer, 1994; Schore, imaging (MRI). In the early days of developmental neuro-
1994; Segalowitz and Rose-Krasnor, 1992; for an extensive science, our knowledge of brain structure and development
review see van der Molen and Ridderinkhof, 1998). Initial were mostly based on post-mortem studies, which demon-
attempts focused on measures of brain size. In a series of strated that the size of the brain increases until age
studies, Epstein (1974a,b) derived his phrenoblysis hypoth- 9–10 and by that time the size and weight of the brain
esis (the Greek word “phreno” stands for skull or mind does not change that much anymore until senescence
while “blysis” refers to welling-up of matter), suggesting (Huttenlocher, 1979; Huttenlocher et al., 1983). These
that the brain grows in spurts with peaks in growth rate studies reported on two main changes which occur in the
occurring between 6 and 8, 10 and 12, and 14 and 17 years developing brain. First, expansion of the layer of myelin
of age. The correlated patterns of peaks and troughs in brain around the axon of developing neurons continues into ado-
growth and mental growth (as indexed by intelligence lescence, especially for the frontal regions of the brain.
tests) suggested to Epstein (1978) a link with Piagetian the- Second, synaptic density, or the number of synapses in a
ory. The spurts in brain growth would precede and prepare certain volume of brain tissue, increases dramatically in
stage transitions in cognitive development. The phrenobl- early in post-natal development, followed by pruning dur-
ysis hypothesis has met with considerable methodological ing maturation.
criticism, however. The rise of in vivo brain scanning methods in the last
Other attempts focused on the spectral analysis of 20–30 years, including MRI, allowed researchers to exam-
brain electrical activity, as measured using the electro- ine changes in brain structure on a much broader scale
encephalogram (EEG). Matousek and Petersén (1973) and in much more detail. Brain structure changes could
reported developmental changes in spectral power that now be studied across time within the same individuals,
showed periods of rapid growth alternating with peri- and several reports have confirmed that there are impor-
ods of slow growth, taken to support stage theories of tant changes in grey and white matter structure until
brain maturation (Hudspeth and Pribram, 1992), echoing late adolescence. The development of white-matter tracks
Epstein’s hypothesis formulated almost 20 years earlier. has recently been studied by the use of diffusion tensor
Thatcher (1994) suggested that EEG coherence, a spectral imaging (DTI), which provides sensitive measures of the
measure that provides an index of the functional coupling changes in microstructure of white matter in the brain that
of neural generators, revealed growth spurts in cortical occurs across childhood and adolescence. These studies
organization are repeated during three major develop- have reported a steady increase in white-matter den-
mental cycles, with transitions at approximately 6 and sity and myelination across childhood, and despite some
10 years. These findings were interpreted to provide sup- reports showing region-specific changes, the majority of
port for neo-Piagetian views of cognitive development. The studies propose that these changes are wide-spread across
hypothesis of stage-wise cognitive development driven by brain regions (Pfeffenbaum et al., 1994; Paus et al., 1999;
iterative and sequential brain-growth cycles remains to be Giorgio et al., 2008). In contrast to white-matter devel-
E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109 103

opment, grey-matter development seems to follow an describes the periods of major change in cognitive pro-
inverted U-shaped pattern of region-specific developmen- cesses that support abstract thinking (Piaget, 1952a,b,
tal changes. Thus, whereas white matter develops steadily 1965; Piaget and Inhelder, 1974). Before he started to
and linearly across brain areas, grey-matter density follows describe his observations of developing children, cognitive
progressive and regressive changes, which follow a differ- developmental psychology was hardly if at all established
ent time course depending on the specific brain region. The as a discipline in its own right. His thinking about devel-
latest changes are observed in the prefrontal cortex, pari- opment was based on questions which have inspired
etal cortex and superior temporal cortex, with grey-matter philosophers for centuries, such as ‘Where does knowledge
peaks around puberty. In prefrontal and parietal cortex come from?’ and ‘How does intelligence develop?’ In addi-
grey matter peaks around age 12 for males and around tion, Piaget’s early interest in cognitive development grew
age 11 for females, whereas in temporal cortex grey mat- out of his interest in biology. Inspired by the likes of Dar-
ter peaks around age 17 (Gogtay et al., 2004; Sowell et al., win, Piaget was interested in questions such as ‘How do
2004). people and knowledge evolve?’ In this sense, the theory
Despite the large body of evidence about structural of Piaget had already a strong link with brain develop-
brain development, much less is known about how these ment, because it was based on assumptions of interaction
changes map onto the development of cognitive functions between preprogrammed biological systems and chang-
which are observed across childhood and adolescence. A ing environmental demands, which together produce rapid
handful of studies have examined how structural brain changes in development. Piaget’s theory does nowadays no
development is predictive of cognitive functioning, as longer accommodate for the full range of findings derived
expressed in for instance IQ, and these studies show that from developmental experimental psychology, particularly
region-specific changes in synaptogenesis in prefrontal with respect to its description of mental changes in infancy.
cortex can predict whether children are low, middle or Much research in recent years with young infants and
high in IQ (Shaw et al., 2006). It is also believed that toddlers has shown that Piaget’s approach to studying cog-
critical or sensitive periods in learning, which are time nitive development may have underestimated cognitive
points in development during which children learn new abilities in younger children by using complicated designs
skills that cannot be learned with the same precision and and methods. For example, there is evidence that shows
success during other periods of life, are the result of synap- that preoperational toddlers will pass a false-belief task, if
tic changes which allow a great speed of assimilation to it is designed appropriately (e.g., Southgate et al., 2010),
currently changing environmental demands (Blakemore or that 9-month-old infants exhibit predictive motor acti-
and Choudhury, 2006; Blakemore, 2008; Johnson, 2001, vation when perceiving grasping movements (Southgate
2010). The exact relation between structural brain develop- et al., 2009). Despite the controversial nature of his detailed
ment and cognitive development, however, is still largely theoretical claims, Piaget’s theory can provides an example
unknown. to illustrate the kinds of ideas and concepts that originated
A method which maps cognitive functioning to brain from psychology and that developmental neuroscience
function more directly is functional MRI (fMRI). Using this needs to tackle.
approach, it is possible to examine brain functioning in vivo Perhaps the most influential of Piaget’s ideas are his
in developing populations while they perform a certain developmental stages, propelled by dynamic processes of
cognitive task (Casey et al., 2005). Despite the limitations assimilation and accommodation. These ideas could be said
that co-occur with this technique, such as the correla- to be reminiscent of sensitive periods in brain develop-
tive nature of the data or the commonly applied use of ment, as indicated by synaptogenesis and synaptic pruning.
reversed inferences (Poldrack, 2005), the cumulating num- For example, Piaget suggested that a child cannot reach a
ber of developmental MRI studies is most likely the most new stage before mastering the old one (Brainerd, 1978;
promising approach for integrating knowledge of brain Flavell, 1963, 1971), which has similarities with the idea
development with classic developmental theories of cog- of a hierarchical development of conscious control levels
nitive development. The promising steps in this direction (Zelazo, 2004). Interestingly, synaptic density studies sug-
are indicative of interpretable changes in brain function gest that changes in grey matter develop at different rates
and behaviour, although it should be noted at the outset for different brain regions (Huttenlocher et al., 1983; see
that the developmental imaging studies to date do not yet also Gogtay et al., 2004), and the change in grey matter in
allow for a direct test of cognitive developmental theo- a higher-order brain region would not contribute to cog-
ries. These shortcomings are mostly related to the use of nitive function if the grey-matter changes in a supporting
wide age ranges and broad experimental manipulations, brain region were not yet completed (Casey et al., 2005).
and these limitations will also be highlighted during this Whether the changes occur suddenly or through slow accu-
review. mulation of knowledge (which is suddenly observable)
has remained unclear (Fisher and Bidell, 1991; Siegler,
3. Cognitive development theories vis-à-vis brain 1981), but theories of brain maturation and of stage-wise
development mental development agree that changes in cognitive skills
occur through an interplay between biological programs
3.1. Developmental theories and accumulating environmental input (see also Karmiloff-
Smith, 2006).
The work of Jean Piaget has probably been most influ- Information-processing theories, which often build
ential in our thinking about cognitive development, as it upon the classic Piagetian framework, have empha-
104 E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109

sized how biologically based growth of internal control, 3.2. Brain development supporting changes in working
self-regulation, working memory and automatization memory and control
allows children to progressively increase processing lim-
its (Demetriou et al., 1993; Fisher, 1980; Halford, 1993, Functional neuroimaging studies have built upon the
1995). For example, the development of working memory idea that working memory development increases across
capacity has been explained as an age-related increase in this specific age period, by comparing brain activation in
processing space, in which the absolute capacity of work- 8–17-year-old children relative to adults. Neuroimaging
ing memory does not change, but the capacity functions studies in adults have shown that regions within prefrontal
more efficiently with advancing age. Early in development, cortex (PFC, in particular lateral PFC) and parietal cor-
children are thought to rely more on working memory stor- tex are important for the maintenance and manipulations
age space, but the processes are less efficient. In contrast, of information in working memory (Smith and Jonides,
across development, children increase in processing abil- 1999; Passingham et al., 2000; Bunge et al., 2003; Olesen
ity, and consequently, there is a decrease in the necessary et al., 2004, see also Fig. 1). The first developmental neu-
storage space (Case, 1992). These changes in capacity may roimaging studies have focused on the pure maintenance
be influenced by a general increase in processing speed of information in memory, by instructing participants to
which could underlie performance enhancements in a wide maintain verbal or spatial information online over the
variety of domains, including working memory (Kail, 1991, course of several seconds, followed by a probe demanding a
2007). button press. These studies have shown that the increased
More recently, and influenced by findings from patients ability to maintain information online between ages 8 and
with prefrontal damage, it is assumed that internal 12 and young adulthood coincides with increased activa-
control and working memory capacity are associated tion in lateral PFC and parietal cortex (Klingberg et al.,
with the emergence of executive functions (Dempster, 2002; Luna et al., 2001; Kwon et al., 2002; O’Hare et al.,
1993; Diamond, 2002). Executive functions refer to the 2008; Thomason et al., 2009). A comparison of fMRI data
ability to control our thoughts and actions with the and DTI data revealed that increased fractional anisotropy
purpose of achieving future goals. Changes in executive in fronto-parietal white matter – suggestive of increased
functions could account for developmental improve- strength of anatomical connectivity between these regions
ments in a variety of higher-order processing domains, – is positively correlated with BOLD activation in the lat-
although it is still debated whether these are co-occurring eral PFC and parietal cortex and with visuospatial working
but separate developments (Huizinga et al., 2006) or memory capacity (Olesen et al., 2003). Thus, these find-
whether a general reflective level of processing under- ings seem to be in favour of the hypothesis that processing
lies these improvements (Zelazo, 2004). For example, an capacity increases over the course of child development.
information-processing theory referred to as the levels- Prior behavioural research, however, postulated that
of-consciousness theory, postulates that the development working memory depends on the interplay between stor-
of the levels of consciousness goes via hierarchical func- age capacity and processing efficiency (Case, 1992). In this
tional system changes, where young children may master sense, the developmental studies which have focused on
one level of processing (e.g., keeping rules active in mind) pure maintenance may only have tapped improvements in
but not another level of processing (e.g., flexibly switch- storage capacity with stable processing demands. Subse-
ing between competing rules). Once the highest level of quent studies have examined these processing demands
consciousness is reached, children can solve the most com- in more detail by asking children to manipulate informa-
plex problems, which can explain the observed age-related tion in working memory (Crone et al., 2006a,b; Jolles et al.,
improvements in executive functions (Zelazo, 2004). in press; Fig. 1). These studies differentiated between pure
Based on these behavioural models, it is generally maintenance and manipulation and found evidence for dif-
predicted that two or more brain systems work closely ferent neural systems which support these functions. That
together when performing complex tasks, but the way in is, ventrolateral PFC was active only for maintenance tri-
which they work together is predicted to be different. For als, whereas dorsolateral PFC was additionally recruited
example, according to Case’s conceptualization of working for manipulation trials. The finding of slower development
memory development, there should be one system which is of dorsolateral PFC could be taken as evidence for delayed
important for storage of information, and a second system processing development. According to Case’s terminology,
which is important for processing information (or execu- especially working memory manipulation should result in
tive control of stored information, Baddeley, 1992, 2003), increased demands on processing capacity, whereas work-
and the relative contribution of these systems changes ing memory maintenance should rely more on storage
during development. In contrast, the levels of conscious capacity. Therefore, the brain imaging results could be
processing account suggests that executive function and taken to suggest that storage capacity can remain stable (as
reflective consciousness is dependent on the maturation of indicated by stable levels of ventrolateral PFC activation),
additional brain regions in prefrontal cortex (see also Bunge and processing capacity may independently increase with
and Zelazo, 2006). The challenge is now to use neuroimag- advancing age (as indicated by increased levels of dorso-
ing methods to directly test the developmental theories of lateral PFC activation when processing demands increase).
working memory and executive function. For this purpose, Whereas the neuroimaging studies described above
we will summarize neuroimaging research which focused have mainly reported change in terms of increases and
on different types of working memory and control func- decreases of task-relevant areas, qualitative developmen-
tions. tal differences have been reported as well. For example,
E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109 105

Fig. 1. Working memory related activation in children and adults. In the working memory task, participants were required to remember a sequence of
objects in forward order (maintenance condition) or backward order (manipulation condition) during a delay of 6 s. After the delay period, one object was
presented and participants had to indicate the position of the object in the forward or backward sequence. This figure shows delay-period activation when
three objects had to be remembered. The images are overlaid on axial and sagittal slices (z = 60 and x = −42) of a standard anatomical image. The left side
of the image is the right side of the brain. Results are thresholded at p < .05, cluster corrected, using clusters determined by z > 2.3 (orange) and at p < .001,
uncorrected (red). Maint. = maintenance; Manip = manipulation (based on Jolles et al., 2010; Developmental Science). (For interpretation of the references
to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

Scherf et al. (2006) demonstrated that 8–12-year-old chil- What theoretical framework can we use to interpret
dren and 13–17 years old show a qualitatively different the neuroimaging findings? There is probably not one the-
activation pattern when performing a working memory ory available which serves this goal, but there are theories
maintenance task. This study showed that 8–12-year-old available which allow for a direct mapping between cog-
children failed to recruit the working memory network nitive theories and brain development trajectories. The
that is core to working memory performance in adults functional development of the human brain could, for
(lateral PFC and parietal cortex), but instead rely on a dif- example, be related to developmental theories in a testable
ferent region within PFC to perform the task (ventromedial way using the neural maturation accounts put forward by
PFC). In contrast, adolescents aged 13–17 years recruited Johnson (2001, 2010) and Johnson et al. (2009). This the-
lateral PFC, just like adults did, but failed to activate this ory was developed to provide a biological framework for
region to a similar extent. The results by Scherf et al. (2006) the development of early attention systems. According to
could be interpreted as a shift in storage or processing Johnson (2001, 2010), cognitive abilities emerge through
capacity between ages 8–12 and 13–17 (see also Ciesielski three possible routes of neuroanatomical development:
et al., 2006 for a similar shift between children and adults, (1) maturational progress (the maturation of additional
although in a different network). The results also indicate brain areas), (2) interactive specialization (changes in inter-
that adult levels of working memory are not yet reached actions between brain areas that were already partially
in adolescence, and that there is a refinement of control active) and (3) skill learning (the patterns of activation
abilities that cannot always be observed on the basis of of cortical regions change during the acquisition of new
behaviour only (see also Kwon et al., 2002; Klingberg et al., skills). Even though this model was developed to account
2002). In this sense, the neuroimaging findings extend for postnatal and experience-dependent changes in the
the results from information-processing theories by show- first two years of life, these trajectories of neuroanatom-
ing that the underlying network which is important for ical development could also be used as a starting point
abstract thought is still improving across adolescent devel- for studying more advanced levels of cognitive develop-
opment. ment, such as working memory. Fig. 2 provides an example
Taken together, prior studies which have used working of how these maturation accounts can be translated in
memory paradigms have reported that the brain regions activation patterns observed in ventrolateral PFC, dor-
that are important for these functions in adults are differ- solateral PFC and superior parietal cortex. For example,
entially engaged in childhood and adolescence. However, the additional recruitment of dorsolateral PFC in work-
the paradigms are often relatively simple and do not ing memory manipulation studies could be interpreted in
allow for a direct test of changes in storage capacity, the terms of the maturational account (as predicted by theo-
processing of information in working memory, and the ries of executive function development), but also in terms
relation with competing information in working memory. of the interactive specialization account (as predicted by
Therefore, a challenge for future research is to develop classic working memory information-processing theories).
experimental paradigms based on developmental theory In contrast, the qualitative differences in brain activation
which allows for the test of different neural development between children and adults support the skill learning
accounts. account. Judicious task manipulations should allow for
106 E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109

Fig. 2. Neural basis of development accounts, based on Johnson (2001), applied to development of working memory. Brain regions implicated in working
memory function are ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and superior parietal cortex (SUPPAR). The maturational
account poses that children become better at performing manipulation trials because of the maturation of additional brain areas, such as DLPFC and SUPPAR.
The interactive specialization account suggests that the performance increases are associated with a refinement of connectivity between brain regions
(VLPFC, DLPFC and SUPPAR). The skill learning account suggests that children may first perform a working memory task based on reliance on a lower-level
system (such as VLPFC) but, when older, they rely on different system (such as DLPFC and SUPPAR).

the test of these different accounts. Even though all three about ways of improving our experimental designs based
theoretical possibilities can account for observed changes on theoretical knowledge of cognitive development. Here,
in brain activation, Johnson (2010) argues in favour of we summarize several problems and possible solutions for
the interactive specialization. Future studies should test achieving the integration of these fields (see also Table 1).
whether this developmental trajectory accounts for devel- One of the main problems with current neuroimag-
opmental data better than the maturational and skill ing evidence which could support or falsify developmental
learning account. theories lies in the selection of age groups. Whereas
information-processing theories argue that the most
4. Merging developmental theory with prominent changes in problem solving and working
developmental imaging memory are seen between ages 7 and 12, almost all devel-
opmental neuroimaging studies have collapsed across
In the prior sections we showed that the interpretation 7–12 years old. Even though the studies report differences
of brain imaging results is limited by the relative absence in behaviour between children and adults, it remains to
of developmental theories. Despite the advances which be determined whether these changes are predominantly
have been accomplished in the field of developmental neu- driven by the youngest children within the selected age
roimaging, it is now time to also consider the limitations of group or occur across the whole child group. In future
the approaches that have been used so far, and to think studies, it will be important to carefully select age groups
E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109 107

Table 1
Guidelines for neuroimaging research based on developmental theory.

Suggested guidelines for neuroimaging research

1 Selection of age groups should be theory-driven


2 Compute brain-behaviour correlations and be aware of differences in effect sizes
3 Maturation and change can only be examined using longitudinal designs
4 Training and intervention studies can demonstrate the malleability of cognitive functions and underlying neural mechanisms
5 Compensatory brain activation should be interpreted with caution
6 Differential brain activation can be caused by strategy differences

based on theoretical predictions about when the changes publication). These results indicate that the developmental
are expected to occur. differences in brain activation are not fixed and can be mod-
A second problem concerns the differences in brain and ified by instruction or training. These preliminary findings
behaviour effect sizes. The correlations between perfor- do not fit with a purely maturational account and concomi-
mance and brain activation are usually modest across all tant structural brain development. Instead they fit in with a
participants, but poor when the analysis only includes one skill learning model. More work is necessary to understand
age group. This difficulty with interpreting brain-behaviour how brain function is sensitive to training or interventions.
correlations across age groups is associated with the dif- One pervasive problem in studies of brain develop-
ficulty in distinguishing between effects of maturation ment is that the different control functions have often
versus individual differences in performance. It is possible been studied in isolation. Even though consistent findings
that these processes cannot be dissociated at all, that is, per- are reported across studies, these are often limited to the
formance is usually correlated with maturational changes. observation that a certain brain area which is important for
In addition, the poor correlations with performance within behaviour in adults is not yet activated to the same level
age groups are usually associated with smaller effect sizes in children. Most of these studies, however, report that
for either the brain or the behavioural measures. Again, a children activate several other brain areas in the critical
straightforward way to address these problems involves contrast that are not seen in adults. What do these acti-
using a theoretically based selection of participants for vation patterns indicate? Currently, we can only interpret
age-related comparisons, and by using tasks which vary these post hoc, based on reverse inference about the func-
the construct under study at different levels. A combi- tion of these unexpectedly activated brain areas in adults.
nation between functional development and structural For example, prior studies have suggested that when chil-
development indices (such as developmental DTI studies) dren activate the left rather then right hemisphere more
is necessary to further expose the extent to which men- in a certain cognitive task, this can possibly be interpreted
tal growth corresponds to growth in (white-matter) brain to reflect that they used a more verbally guided approach,
connectivity. such as rehearsal, because in adults language functions
A third problem is that little is known about test–retest tend to be left-lateralized (Bunge et al., 2002). Along a simi-
effects or longitudinal changes. There is much varia- lar line of reasoning, it has been suggested that 15-year-old
tion within individuals and the time at which spurts in adolescents may have larger compensatory skills because
development occur may change between individuals and besides PFC, they additionally recruit the hippocampus
depend on external factors. In order to track developmen- when they perform a memory task (Finn et al., 2010).
tal changes over time, it is important to use multi-level Finally, the differential activation patterns hint towards
models of change in longitudinal designs (see also Ferrer another important explanation, that is, children may use
et al., 2009). A recently completed 3-year longitudinal different strategies when performing a task. Here, we may
study tested 26 participants between ages 8 and 28 on a again learn from classic developmental theories, which
performance monitoring task using fMRI (Koolschijn et al., have differentiated between different types of memory
submitted for publication). This study showed that, even function, including storage of verbal information, storage of
though the network of brain regions which was activated spatial information, and an executive system which works
was highly similar at both measurements, age was a poor with information in working memory. Consider, for exam-
predictor of test–retest effects. Instead, performance was ple, the differentiation of verbal versus spatial working
a very good predictor for change; those individuals who memory and the inhibition of irrelevant information. In
improved most from measurement 1 to measurement 2, adults, presenting distracting spatial information impairs
also showed the largest change in brain activation. These their spatial working memory capacity, but not their verbal
findings indicate that using only age as a predictor for working memory capacity. In contrast, presenting dis-
change and ignoring performance changes can result in tracting verbal information impairs their verbal working
false or incomplete conclusions. memory capacity, but not their spatial working memory
Based on this stable brain-behaviour relation, we rea- capacity. These results suggest a strong interplay between
soned that performance can increase with training, and working memory and inhibition, which is modality spe-
that brain activation should change accordingly. To test this cific. In 8-year-old children, distracting spatial information
hypothesis, we performed a developmental training study also impairs their verbal working memory performance
and demonstrated that reduced activation in lateral PFC and distracting verbal information also impairs their spa-
in children aged 11–12 years can be improved by exten- tial working memory performance (Hale et al., 1997). How
sive working memory training (Jolles et al., submitted for to interpret these changes? Apparently, in early childhood
108 E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109

the differentiation between functions and modalities is References


much less segregated than in adults, and this theoretical
knowledge should be taken into account when designing Baddeley, A., 1992. Working memory. Science 255, 556–559.
Baddeley, A., 2003. Working memory: looking back and looking forward.
neuroimaging experiments, such that the developmental Nature Review Neuroscience 4, 829–839.
comparisons are meaningful for restricting current models Blakemore, S.J., Frith, U., 2005. The Learning Brain: Lessons for Education.
of cognitive development (see also Johnson et al., 2009). Blackwell Publishing, Oxford.
Blakemore, S.J., 2008. Development of the social brain during adolescence.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61, 40–49.
Blakemore, S.J., Choudhury, S., 2006. Development of the adolescent brain:
5. Conclusions implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 47, 296–312.
Brainerd, C.J., 1978. The stage question in cognitive developmental theory.
In this review, we presented a critical view on the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1, 173–213.
parallels between brain maturation and developmental Bunge, S.A., Dudukovic, N.M., et al., 2002. Immature frontal lobe contribu-
theories. We proposed that the selection of paradigms and tions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fMRI. Neuron 33
(2), 301–311.
age groups can be more readily based on knowledge from Bunge, S.A., Kahn, I., et al., 2003. Neural circuits subserving the retrieval
decades of research in developmental psychology, and we and maintenance of abstract rules. Journal of Neurophysiology 90 (5),
argued that to date conclusions of brain-imaging findings 3419–3428.
Bunge, S.A., Zelazo, P.D., 2006. A brain-based account of the development
often rely on post hoc interpretation. We presented the of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science
example of a major developmental theory, Piaget’s con- 15, 118–121.
cepts of stage-like development, and saw that it can be Case, R., 1992. The Mind’s Staircase: Exploring the Conceptual Under-
pinnings of Children’s? Thought and Knowledge. Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
related to current data-based approaches in structural and NY.
functional brain imaging Casey, B.J., Tottenham, N., et al., 2005. Imaging the developing brain: what
We are enthusiastic about the use of psychophysio- have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 9 (3), 104–110.
logical measures, including brain-imaging analysis, which
Ciesielski, K.T., Lesnik, P.G., Savoy, R.A., Grant, E.P., Ahlfors, S.P., 2006.
can provide a more solid basis for relating developmental Developmental neural networks in children performing a categorical
changes in performance on tasks to actual brain matura- n-back task. Neuroimage 33, 980–990.
tion, and the results from prior fMRI studies provide the Crnic, L.S., Pennington, B.F., 1987. Developmental psychology and the neu-
rosciences: an introduction. Child Development 58, 533–553.
building blocks for starting this new direction. One exam- Crone, E.A., Donohue, S.E., et al., 2006a. Brain regions mediating flexi-
ple of a how these building blocks can be implemented is by ble rule use during development. Journal of Neuroscience 26 (43),
using the neural basis of development accounts proposed 11239–11247.
Crone, E.A., Wendelken, C., et al., 2006b. Neurocognitive development
by Johnson (2001, 2010). This theory provides a principled of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Pro-
division of possible processes in brain development. Even ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States
though this theory was originally developed to account for of America 103 (24), 9315–9320.
Dawson, G., Fischer, K.W. (Eds.), 1994. Human Behavior and the Develop-
changes observed in young children (ages 0–2 years), the ing Brain. Guilford Press, New York.
concepts of maturation, interactive specialization and skill Demetriou, A., Efklides, A., Platsidou, M., 1993. The architecture and
learning can readily be applied to functional neuroimaging dynamics of the developing mind. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development 58 (5/6), Serial No. 234.
studies in children and adolescents. The accounts pro- Dempster, F.N., 1993. Resistance to interference: developmental changes
vide direct starting points for understanding differences in a basic processing mechanism. In: Howe, M.L., Pasnak, R. (Eds.),
in brain recruitment and to relate these to developmental Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development: Foundations, vol. 1.
Springer, New York, pp. 3–27.
theory.
Diamond, A., 2002. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth
Taken together, we suggest that paradigmatic progress to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy and biochemistry.
in developmental neuroscience can rely more on knowl- In: Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, S.A.
edge from developmental experimental psychology, and Knight, London, pp. 466–503.
Dosenbach, N.U.F., et al., 2010. Predicting individual brain maturity using
that developmental models of cognitive development can fMRI. Science 329, 1358–1361.
be articulated further on the basis of anatomical and Epstein, H.T., 1974a. Phrenoblysis: special brain and mind growth periods.
functional differentiation of target brain regions. Much I. Human brain and skull development. Developmental Psychobiology
7, 207–216.
progress is currently made in the technological possibilities Epstein, H.T., 1974b. Phrenoblysis: special brain and mind growth periods.
of brain imaging techniques, which allows, for example, II. Human brain and skull development. Developmental Psychobiol-
for the estimation of age based on brain connectivity ogy 7, 217–224.
Epstein, H.T., 1978. Growth spurts during brain development: implica-
indices (Dosenbach et al., 2010). This is a very impor- tions for educational policy and practice. In: Chall, J.S., Mikky, A.F.
tant development for a better understanding of changes (Eds.), Education and the Brain: The Seventy-seventh Yearbook of the
in brain function. The task for the future, however, is to National Society for the Study of Education, Part II. Chicago University
Press, Chicago, pp. 343–370.
gain converging evidence from developmental theory, task Ferrer, E., O’Hare, E.D., Bunge, S.A., 2009. Fluid reasoning and the develop-
performance and brain activity to account for developmen- ing brain. Frontiers in Neuroscience 3 (1), 46–51.
tal changes and individual differences in various aspects of Finn, A.S., Sheridan, M.A., Hudson Kam, C.L., Hinshaw, S., D’Esposito, M.,
2010. Longitudinal evidence for functional specialization of the neural
internal control and self-regulation.
circuit supporting working memory in the human brain. Journal of
Neuroscience 30, 11062–11067.
Fisher, K.W., Bidell, T.R., 1991. Constraining nativist inferences about cog-
Acknowledgements nitive capacities. In: Carey, S., Gelman, R. (Eds.), The Epigenesis of
Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Fisher, K.W., 1980. A theory of cognitive development: the control
The research by both authors was supported by NWO and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review 87,
VIDI (E.A.C.) and VICI (K.R.R.) grants. 477–531.
E.A. Crone, K. Richard Ridderinkhof / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 101–109 109

Flavell, J.H., 1971. Stage-related properties of cognitive development. Cog- Luna, B., Thulborn, K.R., et al., 2001. Maturation of widely distributed
nitive Psychology 2, 421–453. brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage 13 (5),
Flavell, J.H., 1963. The Developmental Psychology of Jean Piaget. Van Nos- 786–793.
trand, New York. Matousek, M., Petersén, I., 1973. Frequency analysis of the EEG in normal
Giorgio, A., Watkins, K.E., et al., 2008. Changes in white matter microstruc- children and adolescents. In: Kelleway, P., Petersén, I. (Eds.), Automa-
ture during adolescence. Neuroimage 39, 52–61. tion of Clinical Electroencephalography. Raven Press, New York, pp.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., et al., 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical 75–102.
development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings O’Hare, E.D., Lu, L.H., Houston, S.M., Bookheimer, S.Y., Sowell, E.R.,
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2008. Neurodevelopmental changes in verbal working memory load-
101 (21), 8174–8179. dependency: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 42, 1678–1685.
Goswami, U., 2008. Cognitive Development: The Learning Brain. Psychol- Olesen, P.J., Nagy, Z., et al., 2003. Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data
ogy Press: Taylor & Francis, Hove and New York. reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal
Hale, S., Bronik, M.D., Fry, A.F., 1997. Verbal and spatial working memory network. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 18 (1), 48–57.
in school-age children: developmental differences in susceptibility to Olesen, P.J., Westerberg, H., et al., 2004. Increased prefrontal and parietal
interference. Developmental Psychology 33, 364–371. activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience 7 (1),
Halford, G.S., 1993. Children’s Understanding: The Development of Mental 75–79.
Models. Erlbaum, Hillsdale. Passingham, R.E., Toni, I., et al., 2000. Specialisation within the prefrontal
Halford, G.S., 1995. Learning processes in cognitive development: a cortex: the ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning. Exper-
reassessment with some unexpected implications. Child Develop- imental Brain Research 133 (1), 103–113.
ment 38, 295–301. Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., et al., 1999. Structural maturation of neural
Hudspeth, W.J., Pribram, K.H., 1992. Psychophysiological indices of cogni- pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science 283,
tive functioning. International Journal of Psychophysiology 12, 19–29. 1908–1911.
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C.V., Van der Molen, M.W., 2006. Age-related change Pfeffenbaum, A., Mathalon, D.H., et al., 1994. A quantitative magnetic
in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable response imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy
analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2017–2036. to late adulthood. Archives of Neurology 51, 874–887.
Huttenlocher, P.R., 1979. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex- Piaget, J., 1952a. The Child’s Concept of Number. W.W. Norton, New York.
developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research 163, Piaget, J., 1952b. The Origin of Intelligence in Children. International Uni-
195–205. versities Press, New York.
Huttenlocher, P.R., De Courten, C., et al., 1983. Synaptic development Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., 1974. The Child’s Construction of Quantities. Rout-
in human cerebral cortex. International Journal of Neurology 16–17, ledge, London.
144–154. Piaget, J., 1965. The Moral Judgment of the Child. New York, The Free Press.
Johnson, M.H., 2001. Functional brain development in humans. Nature Poldrack, R.A., 2005. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimag-
Reviews Neuroscience 2, 472–483. ing data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 59–63.
Johnson, M.H., Grossmann, T., Cohen Kadosh, K., 2009. Mapping func- Scherf, K.S., Sweeney, J.A., et al., 2006. Brain basis of developmental change
tional brain development: building a social brain through interactive in visusospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
specialization. Developmental Psychology 45, 151–159. 18, 1045–1058.
Johnson, M.H., 2010. Interactive specialization: a domain-general frame- Schore, A.N., 1994. Affect Regulation and the Origin of Self: The Neurobi-
work for human functional brain development? Developmental ology of Emotional Development. Erlbaum, New York.
Cognitive Neuroscience 1, 7–21. Segalowitz, S.J., Rose-Krasnor, L., 1992. The construct of brain maturation
Jolles, D.D., Kleibeuker S.W., Rombouts, S.A.R.B. & Crone, E.A. (in press). in theories of child development. Brain & Cognition 20 (1), 1–7.
Developmental Differences in Prefrontal Activation During Working Siegler, R.S., 1981. Developmental sequences within and between con-
Memory Maintenance and Manipulation for Different Memory Loads. cepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
Developmental Science. 46 (2) (Whole No. 189).
Jolles D.D., Van Buchem, M. A., Rombouts, S. A. & Crone, E. A. (submitted for Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., et al., 2006. Intellectual ability and cortical devel-
publication). The potential of the developing brain: Practice reduces opment in children and adolescents. Nature 440 (7084), 676–679.
developmental differences in brain activation. Smith, E.E., Jonides, J., 1999. Storage and executive processes in the frontal
Jolles, D.D., Grol, M.J., Van Buchem, M.A., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., Crone, E.A., lobes. Science 283 (5408), 1657–1661.
2010. Practice effects in the brain: Changes in cerebral activation after Southgate, V., Johnson, M.H., Osborne, T., Csibra, G., 2009. Predictive motor
working memory practice depend on task demands. Neuroimage 15 activation during action observation in human infants. Biology Letters
(2), 658–668. 5, 769–777.
Kail, R., 1991. Developmental change in speed of processing during child- Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., Csibra, G., 2010. Seventeen-month-olds
hood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin 109, 490–501. appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication.
Kail, R.V., 2007. Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed Developmental Science 13 (6), 907–912.
and working memory enhances children’s reasoning. Psychological Sowell, E.R., Thompson, P.M., et al., 2004. Longitudinal mapping of cortical
Science 18, 312–313. thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuro-
Karmiloff-Smith, A., 2006. The torturous route from genes to behavior: science 24 (38), 8223–8231.
a neuroconstructivism approach. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Thatcher, R.W., 1994. Cyclic cortical reorganization: origens of cognitive
Neuroscience 6 (1), 9–17. development. In: Dawson, G., Fischer, K.W. (Eds.), Human Behavior
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., et al., 2002. Increased brain activity in frontal and the Developing Brain. Guilford, New York, pp. 232–266.
and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working Thomason, M.E., Race, E., Burrows, B., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Glover, G.H.,
memory capacity during childhood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Gabrieli, J.D., 2009. Development of spatial and verbal working mem-
14 (1), 1–10. ory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Koolschijn, P. C. M. P., Schel, M. A., de Rooij, M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B. & Crone, 21, 316–332.
E. A. (submitted for publication). A 3-year longitudinal fMRI study of van der Molen, M.W., Ridderinkhof, K.R., 1998. The growing and aging
performance monitoring and test-retest reliability from childhood to brain: life-span changes in brain and cognitive functioning. In:
early adulthood. Demetriou, A., Doise, W., van Lieshout, C.F.M. (Eds.), Life-Span Devel-
Kwon, H., Reiss, A.L., et al., 2002. Neural basis of protracted develop- opmental Psychology: A European Perspective. Wiley, New York, pp.
mental changes in visuo-spatial working memory. Proceedings of the 35–100.
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99 (20), Zelazo, P.D., 2004. The development of conscious control in childhood.
13336–13341. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (1), 12–17.

You might also like