You are on page 1of 19

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/5490027

Moral Disengagement in Ethical Decision Making: A Study of Antecedents and


Outcomes

Article  in  Journal of Applied Psychology · April 2008


DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.2.374 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
394 12,605

3 authors, including:

James R. Detert
Cornell University
28 PUBLICATIONS   3,070 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by James R. Detert on 25 November 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 93, No. 2, 374 –391 0021-9010/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.2.374

Moral Disengagement in Ethical Decision Making:


A Study of Antecedents and Outcomes

James R. Detert Linda Klebe Treviño


Cornell University The Pennsylvania State University

Vicki L. Sweitzer
Albion College

This article advances understanding of the antecedents and outcomes of moral disengagement by testing
hypotheses with 3 waves of survey data from 307 business and education undergraduate students. The
authors theorize that 6 individual differences will either increase or decrease moral disengagement,
defined as a set of cognitive mechanisms that deactivate moral self-regulatory processes and thereby help
to explain why individuals often make unethical decisions without apparent guilt or self-censure
(Bandura, 1986). Results support 4 individual difference hypotheses, specifically, that empathy and
moral identity are negatively related to moral disengagement, while trait cynicism and chance locus of
control orientation are positively related to moral disengagement. Two additional locus of control
orientations are not significantly related to moral disengagement. The authors also hypothesize and find
that moral disengagement is positively related to unethical decision making. Finally, the authors
hypothesize that moral disengagement plays a mediating role between the individual differences they
studied and unethical decisions. Their results offer partial support for these mediating hypotheses. The
authors discuss the implications of these findings for future research and for practice.

Keywords: moral disengagement, ethical decision making, empathy, trait cynicism, moral identity

Recent years have brought unrelenting news about unethical are able to engage in unethical behavior without apparent guilt or
behavior in virtually every sector of society (e.g., business, gov- self-censure.
ernment, education, military, sports, religious institutions). The Despite its potential importance for explaining unethical deci-
tales of executives who enrich themselves at shareholders’ expense sion making, our understanding of moral disengagement remains
(Horovitz, 2002; Samuelson, 2006), of corrupt government offi- at an early stage. Research thus far has focused primarily on the
cials (Seglin, 2000), of cheating athletes (Lewis, 2006), and more outcomes of moral disengagement, such as its positive relationship
raise an obvious and critically important question, “Why do people to aggression in children (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pas-
make unethical decisions?” Consistent with recent arguments that torelli, 1996; Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, & Rega-
the best explanations for unethical decision making may reside in lia, 2001; Bandura, Underwood, & Fromson, 1975) or its relation-
underlying psychological processes (Messick & Bazerman, 1996; ship to decisions to support military action (Aquino, Reed, Thau,
Tenbrunsel & Messick, 2004), we explore Bandura’s (1986) as- & Freeman, 2007). Most importantly, we know little about the
sertion that people make unethical decisions when moral self-
antecedents of moral disengagement. While it is logical to assume
regulatory processes that normally inhibit unethical behavior are
that individuals may differ in their propensity to morally disen-
deactivated via use of several interrelated cognitive mechanisms
gage, extant research has focused on simple demographics (e.g.,
collectively labeled moral disengagement. Bandura (1986) argued
age, nationality) as antecedents. For example, McAlister (2001)
that moral disengagement explains why otherwise normal people
found male subjects to be more morally disengaged than were
female subjects. Yet, if organizations knew more about whether
some individuals were more predisposed to moral disengagement
James R. Detert, Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell than others, perhaps they could target resources toward improving
University; Linda Klebe Treviño, Smeal College of Business, The Penn- these individuals’ decision making processes.
sylvania State University; Vicki L. Sweitzer, Economics and Management This study therefore seeks to contribute to knowledge about
Department, Albion College. moral disengagement by investigating the individual difference
We gratefully acknowledge the advice received from Michael Brown, antecedents of moral disengagement as well as the relationship
Ethan Burris, Donald Hambrick, and David Harrison. We are also indebted between moral disengagement and subsequent unethical decision
to Craig Crossland and Kristin Price for their research assistance and to the
making. Figure 1 illustrates the key relationships we address. First,
Smeal College of Business for their financial support of this research
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to James R. we theorize that, because of relatively stable individual differences
Detert, Cornell University, 318 Sage Hall, Ithaca, NY 14850. E-mail: associated with how individuals see others, events, and them-
jdetert@cornell.edu selves, some people will be more predisposed to moral disengage-

374
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 375

Antecedents Outcomes

Empathy
H1
Trait
Cynicism H2
Unethical
Moral H5
Decision
H 3a-c Disengagement Making
Locus of Control:
(Mediating hypotheses: H 6a-f)
• Internal
• Chance
• Power H4

Moral
Identity

Figure 1. Hypothesized antecedents and outcomes of moral disengagement. H ⫽ hypothesis.

ment than others. We hypothesize that, because of their likely Bandura (1986) suggested that moral self-regulation can be
facilitating or inhibitory influence on moral self-regulation pro- deactivated or disengaged via eight interrelated moral disengage-
cesses, the following individual differences will influence moral ment mechanisms: moral justification, euphemistic labeling, ad-
disengagement: empathy, trait cynicism, locus of control orienta- vantageous comparison, displacement of responsibility, diffusion
tions, and moral identity. We then investigate the relationship of responsibility, disregarding or distorting the consequences,
between moral disengagement and unethical decision making, dehumanization, and attribution of blame. The first three mecha-
defined as decisions to behave in ways that breach accepted moral nisms—moral justification, euphemistic labeling, and advantageous
norms or standards of behavior. Finally, we explore whether moral comparison—involve cognitive misconstrual of reprehensible behav-
disengagement plays a mediating role between the individual ior in a way that increases its moral acceptability (Bandura, 1986). In
difference antecedents we examine and unethical decision making. today’s society, for example, most people recognize that harming
others is wrong. However, with moral justification, individuals recon-
Theory and Hypotheses strue harm to others in ways that make it appear morally justifiable.
For example, hiring young children overseas may be justified by
Moral Disengagement Overview stating that without such work the children would have to engage in
Bandura (1999) developed the notion of moral disengagement other more dangerous or degrading forms of employment to help their
as an extension of social cognitive theory. Social cognitive theory desperate families. With euphemistic language, individuals use mor-
offers an agentic perspective on human behavior whereby individ- ally neutral language to make reprehensible conduct seem less harm-
uals exercise control over their own thoughts and behaviors ful or even benign. For example, lying to business competitors may be
through self-regulatory processes (Bandura, 1986). According to called “strategic misrepresentation” (Safire, 1979), and killing civil-
social cognitive theory, moral agency is governed by a self- ians in war may be referred to as “collateral damage” (Bandura,
regulatory system that includes self-monitoring of one’s conduct 1999). With advantageous comparison, unethical behaviors are com-
as well as self-reaction to that conduct in light of internal moral pared with even more harmful conduct, thus making the original
standards. According to the theory, most people have developed behavior appear acceptable. For instance, a student who asks another
personal standards of moral behavior that serve a self-regulatory student general questions about the content of an upcoming exam that
role. These standards guide good behavior and deter bad behavior the latter has inappropriately obtained in advance may favorably
because individuals use their personal standards to anticipate, compare his or her actions with the behavior of other students who
monitor, and judge their own actions. Behaving in ways that review the specific exam questions by noting that just asking about
counter these standards results in self-censure. Thus, individuals exam topics is less (or not) problematic compared with viewing the
usually behave in ways that are consistent with their internal moral entire exam.
standards because they anticipate their own positive and negative The next three moral disengagement mechanisms, displacement
evaluations of possible conduct choices. However, this self- of responsibility, diffusion of responsibility, and distortion of
regulatory function operates only if it is activated. Bandura (1999) consequences, occur when an individual obscures or distorts the
argued that moral self-regulation can be activated and deactivated effects of harmful actions (Bandura, 1986). When individuals view
selectively, and he proposed moral disengagement as the key their behaviors as a direct result of authoritative dictates (e.g., my
deactivation process. Through moral disengagement, individuals boss told me to do it), they may displace responsibility for their
are freed from the self-sanctions and the accompanying guilt that actions to the authority figure, negating any personal accountabil-
would ensue when behavior violates internal standards, and they ity for the unfavorable act. In addition, adverse group behavior
are therefore more likely to make unethical decisions. may trigger diffusion of responsibility because no one group
376 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

member feels personally liable for the collective’s destructive people are more prone to moral disengagement than others. We
behavior. These two moral disengagement tactics seem particu- therefore identify a set of individual differences that have been
larly applicable in work organizations where individuals feel com- previously linked to moral cognition and action (e.g., Andersson &
pelled to follow the orders of authority figures (e.g., to change the Bateman, 1997; Aquino & Reed, 2002; Miller & Eisenberg, 1988;
numbers in a report) and where responsibility for harmful out- Treviño & Youngblood, 1990) and link them in Hypotheses 1– 4 to
comes is often diffused to organizational teams or units. Individ- the moral self-regulation process via their proposed influences on
uals may also disconnect harmful activities from self-sanctions by moral disengagement. We will argue that these four individual
distorting the consequences associated with a given act. For ex- differences predispose individuals to see others, events, and them-
ample, customers may tell themselves that no one will be harmed selves in ways that should make moral disengagement more or less
by not reporting an error in their favor because “this little bit of likely.
money doesn’t affect anything in a huge company like X.” Empathy. Psychologists who study moral cognition and action
Finally, dehumanization and attribution of blame can disengage have highlighted the importance of imagining oneself in another’s
moral sanctions by reducing identification with the targets of place or taking the perspective of others (see Eisenberg, 1986;
harmful acts. Research has consistently shown the tendency of Kohlberg, 1969; Rest, 1986). Empathy is an individual difference
individuals to form groups and to quickly develop us-versus-them that describes the degree to which an individual notices and is
thinking based on group membership (Brewer, 1979; Gaertner & concerned about the needs or concerns of others (see Eisenberg &
Insko, 2000). Such recasting of others as out-group members Miller, 1987; Batson et al., 1989; Miller & Eisenberg, 1988). The
makes it more likely that harm will be perpetuated because internal affective approach to understanding empathy emphasizes the ob-
standards and self-sanctions are less likely to be activated once server’s feeling of the target’s emotions, while the cognitive ap-
others have been cast as worthy of derogation or even lacking in proach focuses on recognizing and understanding another’s
human qualities (Bar-Tal, 1989; Struch & Schwartz, 1989). Sim- thoughts and feelings—that is, cognitively “taking the place” of
ilarly, attribution of blame can exonerate the self by placing fault another or putting oneself in another’s shoes. Kohlberg (1969)
with the target of the harmful behavior. For example, torture may used the term role-taking to represent this cognitive process and
be blamed on terrorists by noting they have brought such outcomes argued that role-taking was essential to moral development and
on themselves. moral judgment. Rest (1986) similarly acknowledged the impor-
The ideas captured in Bandura’s moral disengagement frame- tance of role-taking to moral judgment, but also took a more
work are not completely new to psychological research, organiza- affective approach, suggesting that gut-level empathic feelings
tional behavior research, or behavioral ethics research. Others have often occur prior to moral judgment, increasing sensitivity to the
identified individual cognitive mechanisms that serve to discon- moral nature of the situation. Both approaches emphasize that
nect an act from its moral valence. For example, the tendency of those high in empathy are more likely to take into account the
individuals to displace responsibility for their actions onto pow- other person’s concerns. According to Bok (1998) “Empathy and
erful others (e.g., Diener, 1977) or to diffuse responsibility by fellow feeling form the very basis of morality . . . Without some
pointing to collective decision making (e.g., Kelman, 1973) is well rudimentary perception of the needs and feelings of others, there
documented. Likewise, much has been written about the processes can be no beginnings of felt responsibility toward them” (p. 70).
leading to moral exclusion and subsequent dehumanization (Opo- The dispositional view of empathy suggests that some individ-
tow, 1990; Staub, 1989) as well as the terrible consequences that uals are more predisposed than others toward vicarious empathic
can flow from these processes (e.g., Kelman, 1973). More re- experience and are more likely to engage in personalization and
cently, scholars have cited Bandura’s (1999) work when arguing “imaginative self-involvement” (Bandura, 1986, p. 314). Higher
that some of these same cognitive processes can foster unethical levels of such arousal motivate the helping of others in need and
action in organizations. For example, Anand, Ashforth, and Joshi reduce motivation to harm others. Although social psychologists
(2005) linked selected rationalization tactics (denial of responsi- have argued about whether such a predisposition toward empathy
bility, denial of injury, denial of the victim) and euphemistic exists (e.g., Batson, 1991), research has demonstrated support for
language to the facilitation of corruption. Similarly, Tenbrunsel the idea that individuals differ in their concern for others and that
and Messick (2004) identified euphemistic language as a key empathy emerges in childhood and is quite stable over time (Eisen-
self-deceptive tactic that allows individuals to behave unethically berg et al., 1999).
in organizations. We propose that being more acutely aware of the needs and
We view Bandura’s (1986) unique contribution to be the pro- feelings of others should inhibit moral disengagement. Disposi-
vision of a coherent theory that ties together all of these cognitive tional empathy should be negatively related to moral disengage-
mechanisms or tactics by explaining how they all serve to deacti- ment because individuals high on empathy are more likely to
vate moral self-regulatory processes. With colleagues, he has pre- vicariously experience the feelings of others and to be concerned
sented initial evidence that these cognitive processes operate as about those others’ needs. As a result, high empathy individuals
part of a single overarching moral disengagement construct that should be less likely to morally disengage through processes such
can be linked to outcomes such as childhood aggression or delin- as the moral justification of acts that would harm others or the
quency (Bandura et al., 1996, 2001). dehumanization of the targets of those acts. They should also be
less likely to distort the consequences (i.e., potential harm) of their
Individual Differences and Moral Disengagement actions or attribute blame to victims.

If moral disengagement is an important precursor of unethical Hypothesis 1: Dispositional empathy is negatively related to
decision making, it seems essential to understand whether some moral disengagement.
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 377

Trait cynicism. Through early experience, individuals develop cally distinct external orientations— one in which life’s outcomes
philosophies of human nature that are difficult to change (Wrights- are largely attributed to chance, that is, seen as randomly deter-
man, 1992). One such philosophy of human nature is trait cyni- mined by fate or luck, and another wherein outcomes are seen as
cism, defined as a general attitude characterized by feelings of largely under the control of powerful others.
frustration and disillusionment as well as distrust of other persons, The chance external locus of control dimension describes how
groups, ideologies, social conventions, and institutions (Abraham, much an individual believes that life experiences and outcomes are
2000; Costa, Zonderman, McCrae, & Williams, 1985; Hochwarter, a result of fate or luck rather than personal initiative (Levenson,
James, Johnson, & Ferris, 2004). We propose that trait cynicism 1981). We propose that individuals with higher chance locus of
will facilitate moral disengagement because individuals who are control orientations are more prone to moral disengagement in part
high on trait cynicism have an underlying distrust of other people. because they see responsibility for outcomes as coming from
Thus, an individual who is high on trait cynicism will be more outside the self (Borrero-Hernandez, 1979). High chance locus of
likely to question the motives of others, including targets of harm, control individuals may also be more likely to disregard or distort
and will be more likely to think that such targets are deserving of consequences because they are more likely to think that an out-
their fate. Trait cynics should also be more likely to diffuse come could not be helped.
responsibility because they think everyone is engaged in selfish
acts; in addition, they should be more likely to displace responsi- Hypothesis 3b: Chance locus of control is positively related to
bility to others, especially leaders, because they see such others as moral disengagement.
lacking integrity or altruism (Kanter & Mirvis, 1989). Therefore,
we predict that those higher in trait cynicism will be predisposed The powerful others locus of control dimension refers to the
toward the types of morally disengaged reasoning that Bandura belief that, although the world is relatively predictable, powerful
argued are central to deactivation of moral self-regulation. others are in control of events (Levenson, 1981). Having a higher
powerful others locus of control orientation should facilitate moral
Hypothesis 2: Trait cynicism is positively related to moral disengagement because individuals who are high on this locus of
disengagement. control dimension should be more likely to displace responsibility
for their own actions onto authority figures. For example, when
Locus of control. Locus of control orientations relate to how powerful others in an organization decide to change the rules
individuals think about the events in their lives. These orientations arbitrarily, subordinates with higher powerful others locus of con-
are relatively stable dispositions that differentiate between people trol orientations may be particularly likely to go along with such
who believe they have personal control over the outcomes in their rules even if the changes have harmful effects on various stake-
lives and those who believe that such outcomes are controlled by holders because they reason that they have little personal control in
chance or powerful others (Lefcourt, 1966; Rotter, 1966). Individ- such situations. Individuals higher on the power locus of control
uals with strong internal locus of control orientations see clear dimension may also be more likely to use other moral disengage-
connections between their own behavior and the outcomes of that ment tactics, such as moral justification, because they are more
behavior (Levenson, 1981; Rotter, 1966). Treviño (1986) theoret- likely to merely parrot what they hear from authority figures rather
ically linked internal locus of control with ethical decision making, than question those in charge.
arguing that those who see a clear connection between their own
behavior and its outcomes would be more likely to take personal Hypothesis 3c: Power locus of control is positively related to
responsibility for that behavior. Treviño and Youngblood (1990) moral disengagement.
found empirical support for this link. This responsibility-taking, in
turn, activates moral norms (Schwartz, 1977). Thus, we propose Moral identity. Moral identity concerns how individuals think
that individuals with higher internal locus of control orientations about themselves. Moral identity has been defined in individual
should be less likely to morally disengage by displacing or diffus- difference terms as a relatively stable “self conception organized
ing responsibility for unethical actions because they look within around specific moral traits” (Aquino & Reed, 2002, p. 1,424) and
rather than to others or to normative conditions for explanation or one of a number of hierarchically organized identities that com-
justification of the relationship between their actions and conse- pose the sense of self. Because individuals have multiple identities
quences (Maqsud, 1980). Internal locus of control should also be (Markus & Kunda, 1986), those identities that are the most salient
negatively associated with moral disengagement because those to the self are expected to most strongly influence thoughts and
with stronger internal orientations are more likely to consider the feelings. For individuals with a highly self-important moral iden-
consequences of their actions and are less likely to attribute blame tity, moral concerns and commitments are central to their self-
for wrongdoing to others. definition and self-concept (Aquino & Reed, 2002). As a result,
Aquino and colleagues (Aquino et al., 2007) proposed that such
Hypothesis 3a: Internal locus of control is negatively related individuals are more likely to be concerned about the suffering of
to moral disengagement. others, including out-group members. We therefore hypothesize
that a highly self-important moral identity should inhibit moral
While Rotter (1966) treated locus of control as a single contin- disengagement processes such as those that minimize or miscon-
uum with internal locus of control at one end and external locus of strue harm to others (distortion of consequences) or dehumanize or
control at the other, Levenson (1974, 1981) asserted and found blame victims of harm (dehumanization, attribution of blame).
evidence that (a) internal and external orientations represent dis- Thus, we predict that individuals with highly self-important moral
tinct dimensions and (b) there are two conceptually and empiri- identities will be lower in moral disengagement.
378 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

Hypothesis 4: Moral identity is negatively related to moral We propose that the relationship between empathy and unethical
disengagement. decision making can be explained, in part, through moral disen-
gagement processes. Individuals higher in empathy are more likely
Moral Disengagement and Unethical Decision Making to be emotionally aroused by the needs of others and are more
likely to cognitively put themselves in others’ shoes. Thus, as
Moral disengagement has been proposed to increase unethical argued above, those higher in empathy should be less likely to
behavior because morally disengaged reasoning disconnects a con- morally disengage via processes such as dehumanization or victim
templated act from the guilt or self-censure that would otherwise blaming. This, in turn, should lead to a lower likelihood of making
prevent it. This break between internal standards and contemplated decisions that would harm others.
behavior reduces self-deterrents that would normally block indi-
viduals from carrying out unethical action (Bandura et al., 1996; Hypothesis 6a: Moral disengagement mediates the relation-
Duffy, Aquino, Tepper, Reed, & O’Leary-Kelly, 2005). Empirical ship between empathy and unethical decision making.
evidence supports this theoretical relationship. For example, Ban-
dura and colleagues found that moral disengagement decreased Trait cynicism, moral disengagement, and unethical decision
prosocial behavior (helpfulness, cooperativeness) and increased making. Consistent with arguments that people who are distrust-
antisocial behavior (aggression, delinquency) in children (Bandura ful of others are more likely to engage in unethical behavior
et al., 1996, 2001; Bandura et al., 1975). With adults, the primary (Rotter, 1980), research on cynicism in the workplace supports the
application of moral disengagement has been in the area of atti- notion that cynicism increases unethical behavior. For example,
tudes toward war and terrorism. McAlister (2001), for example, Andersson and Bateman (1997) found that trait cynics were sig-
found moral disengagement to be positively related to support for nificantly more likely to approve an advertising plan that made
military attacks against Iraq and Yugoslavia. Similarly, Aquino false claims. We propose that individuals high in trait cynicism
and colleagues (Aquino et al., 2007) found moral disengagement will be more likely to make unethical decisions that harm others in
mechanisms to be positively related to the choice of death rather part because of their moral disengagement. Such individuals are
than nonlethal options for the perpetrators of the 9/11 attacks. To less likely to trust others, and they are more likely to blame
our knowledge, Duffy and colleagues (2005) have conducted the potential victims and see them as deserving of bad outcomes. This,
only study on the relationship between moral disengagement and in turn, should make unethical decisions more likely.
adult unethical decision making or behavior. They found that
moral justification (a single moral disengagement mechanism) was Hypothesis 6b: Moral disengagement mediates the relation-
positively related to subsequent co-worker undermining (e.g., ship between trait cynicism and unethical decision making.
spreading rumors) among hospital workers. We propose here that
Locus of control, moral disengagement, and unethical decision
moral disengagement will have this same positive relationship
making. Several studies have found locus of control orientations
with undesirable outcomes in the realm of everyday decision
to be related to ethical/unethical decision behavior. Treviño and
making wherein individuals make decisions that involve cheating,
Youngblood (1990), for example, showed that those with higher
lying, stealing, and other unethical behaviors.
internal locus of control orientations made more ethical decisions.
Hypothesis 5: Moral disengagement is positively associated Likewise, Reiss and Mitra (1998) found that those higher on the
with unethical decision making. internal locus of control orientation were more likely to label a
variety of ethically ambiguous organizational actions as unaccept-
able. Research has also found that those with higher external locus
Moral Disengagement as a Mediator of control orientations behave unethically in experimental scenar-
Because some of the individual differences we hypothesized as ios (Hegarty & Sims, 1978), are willing to engage in insider
influences on moral disengagement (Hypotheses 1– 4) have also trading (Terpstra, Reyes, & Bokor, 1991), and are less likely to
been theorized and/or found to directly influence counterproduc- report ethical beliefs that conflict with perceived organizational
tive or unethical behaviors (e.g., Andersson & Bateman, 1997; interests (Mudrack & Mason, 1996).
Miller & Eisenberg, 1988; Treviño & Youngblood, 1990), we Here, we hypothesize that the direct relationships between locus
theorize that moral disengagement may play a mediating role of control orientations and unethical decision making will be
connecting stable individual differences to unethical decision mak- mediated by moral disengagement. For example, those with higher
ing. internal locus of control orientations should be less likely to
Empathy, moral disengagement, and unethical decision making. morally disengage through processes such as diffusion or displace-
Psychologists have argued that empathy should inhibit aggressive ment of responsibility. On the other hand, individuals with higher
or antisocial conduct toward others because individuals who ex- chance locus of control orientations may make more unethical
perience others’ negative feelings should be less inclined to con- decisions because they blame outcomes on fate or luck rather than
tinue harmful behavior or engage in it in the future (e.g., Eisen- recognize their own role in producing harmful outcomes. In addi-
berg, 1986). Miller and Eisenberg’s (1988) meta-analysis revealed tion, individuals with higher power locus of control orientations
that empathy was negatively related to antisocial actions such as may make more unethical decisions because it is easy for them to
verbal and physical aggression in childhood, aggressive criminal displace responsibility to powerful others. Thus, moral disengage-
offenses, and administration of shock in a learning task. However, ment processes should help to explain why these three locus of
the authors also noted that the processes underlying the link control orientations make unethical decision making more or less
between empathy and antisocial behavior are not well understood. likely.
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 379

Hypotheses 6c– 6e: Moral disengagement mediates the rela- promised that the data would be treated confidentially and would
tionships between locus of control orientations (internal, bear no relation to their performance in the course.
chance, and power are Hypotheses 6c, 6d, and 6e, respec- The choice of a university sample was useful for testing this
tively) and unethical decision making. study’s hypotheses. First, our goal was to explore individual dif-
ferences and psychological mechanisms underlying ethics-related
Moral identity, moral disengagement, and unethical decision decision making. By college age, dispositions are presumably well
making. When moral identity is highly self-important, moral established. Conversely, while many individuals of college age
commitments are central to an individual’s self-concept. Such have some work experience, few have been fully socialized into
individuals are expected to behave ethically in order to be true to the world of work in ways that could be considered major influ-
their moral conception of themselves (Aquino & Reed, 2002). In a ences on their attitudes or behavior. Further, divorcing the study
predictive validity study, Aquino and Reed found that high self- from an employment context minimized the likelihood that respon-
importance of moral identity was positively associated with proso- dents would distort their answers out of concern for potential
cial behaviors such as food donations and volunteering to help career consequences. A university context was also well suited to
others. Taking up Aquino and Reed’s call for future study of the aspects of our research design. For example, we were able to (a)
relationship between moral identity and antisocial behaviors, we collect data on multiple constructs, (b) collect data from the same
propose here that moral identity should reduce unethical decision individuals on several occasions, thereby minimizing concern for
making. Further, we argue that this relationship can be explained, response bias in self-report data (Ostroff, Kinicki, & Clark, 2002),
in part, by the reduced use of moral disengagement mechanisms by and (c) obtain behavioral data to help validate our measure of
those whose moral identity is of higher self-importance. An indi- unethical decisions.
vidual whose morality is more central to the self should be more Survey 1 was designed primarily to collect the individual dif-
likely to activate moral cognitions (such as assessing the potential ferences data to be used as control and independent variables in
for harm), less prone to morally disengaged reasoning, and, there- our hypothesis testing. It was administered during the first 2 weeks
fore, less likely to make unethical decisions. of the semester in small sections of the orientation seminar re-
quired for all freshmen. Survey 2 was designed primarily to
Hypothesis 6f: Moral disengagement mediates the relation- measure moral disengagement and was administered in class sev-
ship between moral identity and unethical decision making. eral weeks later during the same seminar to the same sample of
business and education students. Survey 3, which contained the
In sum, we hypothesized that a set of individual differences will
items for the Unethical Decision Making scale, was administered
predict moral disengagement (Hypotheses 1– 4) and that moral
several weeks after Survey 2 during the first week of a new
disengagement, in turn, will predict unethical decision making
semester. The ordering and temporal spacing of the three surveys
(Hypothesis 5). We also hypothesized that moral disengagement
was designed to evaluate mediation and to minimize cognitive
will play a mediating role between the hypothesized individual
carryover of prior questions and responses, a major source of
difference antecedents and unethical decision making (Hypotheses
concern about common source variance (Harrison & McLaughlin,
6a– 6f). These hypotheses are summarized visually in Figure 1.
1993). None of the respondents had completed the business
school’s required course in business ethics (a possible source of
Method contamination) at the time of Surveys 1–3. A final behavioral
measure used to provide validation evidence for the Unethical
Research Design and Procedures Decision Making measure was obtained 10 weeks after Survey 3.

Testing the hypotheses outlined in Figure 1 required extensive Research Subjects


cooperation from individual respondents and a sponsoring institu-
tion. We therefore conducted a multi-wave survey study of busi- Because our data collection occurred across three survey admin-
ness and education undergraduates in the business and education istrations spanning two semesters, the final sample for these anal-
colleges of a large public research university in the Northeast. The yses came from the 307 respondents enrolled in colleges of busi-
institution enrolls approximately 40,000 students with an entering ness and education who completed and provided their correct ID
freshman class of approximately 6,500 students (of which approx- number on all three surveys. This is approximately 29% of all
imately 1,050 say they intend to major in business or education). freshmen who plan to major in business or education at the
To survey students multiple times in a short time frame, we university. As noted in Table 1, respondents in the final sample
obtained access to the students via 40 sections of a first year were primarily raised in the United States (96%), had an average
seminar offered in both the business and education colleges. All SAT score of 1214, and 54% were female students while 46%
surveys were administered by one of the authors or a trained were male students. Respondents were on average 18.4 years of
graduate assistant. Students were told by the administrator prior to age; were 87% Caucasian American, 5% Asian American, 3%
Survey 1 that it was the first of three surveys they would be asked African American, 2% Hispanic American, and 3% non-
to complete in class as part of a research study on decision making. American; and had an average of 19.3 months of paid full- or
Students were informed that their participation was voluntary and part-time work experience in 2.88 different jobs (3% reported no
that all parts of the study had been reviewed and approved by the work experience). The population of business and education fresh-
University’s institutional review board. They were also informed men at this university was similar on these dimensions, having
that their student ID was being requested on all surveys so that data only a slightly lower SAT average (M ⫽ 1166). The 307 respon-
could be matched across the multiple instruments, and they were dents who completed all surveys were also similar to the more than
380 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations and Intercorrelations of Study Variables

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
a
1. Gender .46 .50 —
2. Business vs. educationb .70 .46 .41*** —
3. SAT total 1,213.52 120.46 .23*** .19** —
4. Native countryc .96 .19 ⫺.02 ⫺.13* ⫺.06 —
5. Empathyd 3.86 .77 ⫺.34*** ⫺.26*** ⫺.09 ⫺.03 —
6. Trait cynicismd 4.80 1.02 .15* .11 ⫺.12 ⫺.03 ⫺.06
*

7. Internal locus of 4.28 .59 .15* .21*** .11 ⫺.05 ⫺.21*** .05 —
controle
8. Chance locus of 2.71 .75 ⫺.02 ⫺.12 ⫺.02 ⫺.01 .07 .19** ⫺.26*** —
controle
9. Power locus of controle 2.93 .73 .11 .19* .04 ⫺.04 ⫺.11 .18** ⫺.00 .47*** —
10. Moral identityf 4.58 .46 ⫺.16** .04 ⫺.01 .01 .22*** .01 .00 ⫺.04 ⫺.08 —
11. Moral disengagementf 2.11 .41 .30*** .02 .07 ⫺.10 ⫺.27*** .30*** .04 .20*** .14* ⫺.24*** —
12. Unethical decisionsd
3.30 1.00 .17**
.11 ⫺.04 .04 ⫺.21*** .31*** .08 .11 *
.15 ⫺.11
*
.34*** —
a
Dichotomous variable for gender: male ⫽ 1; female ⫽ 0.
b
Dichotomous variable for intended major: business ⫽ 1; education ⫽ 0.
c
Dichotomous variable for country raised: United States ⫽ 1; outside United States ⫽ 0.
d
Measure was assessed using a 7-point scale.
e
Measure was assessed using a 6-point scale.
f
Measure was assessed using a 5-point scale.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

500 others who completed (with correct ID) one or two but not all scales for internal, chance, and power locus of control in Survey 1.
surveys on the study’s focal variables (e.g., trait cynicism: M ⫽ The eight-item Internal Locus of Control scale measures the extent
4.80 for all surveys vs. 4.88 for some surveys; moral disengage- to which individuals believe that they have control over events in
ment: M ⫽ 2.11 for all surveys vs. 2.24 for some surveys). their own lives (e.g., items assess the belief that those with more
power determine whether one experiences personal or professional
Independent Variables success). The eight-item Chance Locus of Control scale measures
one’s perceptions of how probable it is that events occur because
Empathy. Empathy was measured in Survey 1 by using a
of fate or chance (e.g., items assess the belief that luck or fate
10-item scale from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP;
determine whether one experiences personal, professional, or
Goldberg, 2002). Labeled as sympathy on the IPIP, the items
physical well-being). The eight-item Power Locus of Control scale
represent a general disposition of empathy. According to Bierhoff
measures the extent to which individuals believe that their life
(2002), with few exceptions (Eisenberg, 2000) the term sympathy
outcomes are largely determined or controlled by powerful others
is an older term that is rarely used in current psychological dis-
(e.g., items assess the belief that one can make one’s plans happen
cussions of empathic responses. The items on the IPIP scale aim to
or protect one’s personal interests). All locus of control items were
tap an individual’s willingness to take others’ problems and emo-
assessed by using a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly
tions into consideration. Sample items illustrating the affective and
cognitive aspects of empathy are “I suffer from others’ sorrows” disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha for the Internal,
and “I am not interested in other people’s problems” (reverse Chance, and Power Locus of Control scales were .65, .78, and .77,
scored). The 10 items (␣ ⫽ .81) were assessed by using a 7-point respectively.
Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly Moral identity. Moral identity was measured by using
agree). Aquino and Reed’s (2002) Internalization subscale in Survey 2.
Trait cynicism. Trait cynicism was measured in Survey 1 by On this measure, subjects are first presented a set of nine
using a five-item scale adapted by Johnson and O’Leary-Kelly adjectives (e.g., caring, compassionate, fair, honest) along with
(2003) from Wrightsman’s (1992) subscale for cynicism as a the statement that these represent “some characteristics that
Philosophy of Human Nature (PHN). The items assessing cyni- might describe a person.” Subjects then rate, with a scale
cism stem from Wrightsman’s (1992) assumption that cynicism ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), five
reflects a “virulent assumption about human nature,” namely, an items intended to assess the degree to which these characteris-
individual’s “attributions of selfishness and fakery to others” (p. tics represent an important part of their own identity (sample
5). Therefore, the items assess the belief that people will refrain item: “Being someone who has these characteristics is an im-
from lying, cheating, or stealing only when it is easy to do so or portant part of who I am”; scale ␣ ⫽ .77). The Internalization
when they think they would get caught. The items were assessed subscale captures the degree to which a person’s moral identity
with a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 is rooted at the core of one’s being and has been found to be the
(strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha was .79. more robust predictor (compared with the Symbolization sub-
Locus of control: internal, chance, and power. To measure the scale) of ethics-related attitudes and behavior (Aquino & Reed,
three locus of control orientations, we used Levenson’s (1981) 2002; Aquino et al., 2007).
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 381

Dependent Variables 6 (highly likely). Just prior to the first scenario, students were
reminded that “there are no ‘incorrect’ answers” and that the
Moral disengagement. Moral disengagement was assessed on survey would have value to themselves and others only if they
the second survey with a measure similar to the one developed and gave “truthful responses, not those [they] think might seem more
used in multiple studies by Bandura and others (Bandura et al., desirable.” Respondents were also assured (on all three surveys)
1996, 2001; Pelton et al., 2004). However, because Bandura’s that their responses would be confidential and not used to evaluate
32-item scale was developed for use with children and young them personally in any way. Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was
adolescents, we adapted it to fit the population of our study (e.g., .74.
“It is unfair to blame a child who had only a small part in the harm We conducted three independent exercises to provide evidence
caused by the group” was changed to “You can’t blame a person regarding the construct validity of the Unethical Decision Making
who plays only a small part in the harm caused by a group”; “If measure. Before administering the measure, we contacted 20 busi-
kids fight and misbehave in school it is their teacher’s fault” was ness ethics experts via e-mail and asked them to provide ratings on
eliminated and replaced with “People are not at fault for misbe- a 5-point Likert scale on whether the behavior in each of the eight
having at work if their managers mistreat them”). Similar to scenarios composing the Unethical Decisions Making scale repre-
Bandura’s measure, the items were designed to equally tap the sents a violation of one or more ethical principles (e.g., virtue,
eight subcomponents of the overarching moral disengagement rights, justice). The experts were also asked to rate five randomly
construct (see Appendix A for all items and the subcomponents interspersed scenarios written by the authors to not represent
they are designed to tap). The items were assessed on a 5-point unethical behavior. Sixteen experts responded. Findings indicated
Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly that the panel of ethics experts found the eight scenarios used in
agree). the scale to clearly represent violations of ethical principles: The
We randomly split the 828 responses to the moral disengage- grand mean for the items used in our Unethical Decision Making
ment items in half and conducted exploratory factor analyses scale was 4.66 (SD ⫽ .35). Further, this mean was significantly
(maximum-likelihood estimation with direct oblimin rotation) by different, t(15) ⫽ 15.3, p ⬍ .001, than the grand mean for the five
using 424 responses. Consistent with the theory, statistical criteria other scenarios (M ⫽ 1.68, SD ⫽ .62) rated by the experts but not
(e.g., scree test examination, eigenvalues) suggested that an eight included in our scale. Appendix B provides additional details on
factor solution was most appropriate. The pattern of item loadings the expert rating exercise.
for the eight factor model, however, suggested that several items To demonstrate that our Unethical Decision Making scale is
should not be retained because they did not load on their expected correlated with actual unethical behaviors, we conducted two
factor at ⬎.30 and/or had cross-loadings ⬎.30 (Fabrigar, Wegener, additional validation exercises. Both are described in full in Ap-
MacCallum, & Strahan, 1999). To keep the Moral Disengagement pendix B. In one of these exercises, we obtained behavioral data
scale balanced across its eight components and as parsimonious as from a sample of 59 subjects that we then correlated with the
possible, we selected for retention the 3 best-fitting items for each closest individual item on our Unethical Decision Making scale.
moral disengagement subcomponent. The 24 items selected (see This exercise presented subjects with an actual opportunity to keep
Appendix A) all loaded on their expected factor at greater than .40 or return $8 received in the mail that did not belong to them, a
and had no cross-loadings greater than .25. We next conducted choice somewhat similar to the scenario from the Unethical De-
confirmatory factor analysis with Lisrel 8.53 on the hold-out cision Making scale that involved keeping or returning $10 in extra
sample (n ⫽ 424), estimating a model with the 24 items loading change from a coffee shop. The polychoric correlation coefficient
onto eight first order factors and those factors loading onto a single of .34 between subjects’ actual behavior (keeping the cash) and
second order factor. The fit statistics for this maximum-likelihood their response to the related scenario was significant at p ⬍ .01.
estimated model indicate that the model fits the data well (e.g., In the second exercise, we conducted two surveys of business
root-mean-square error of approximation [RMSEA] ⫽ .05; non- students at the same university. Students first provided responses
normed fit index [NNFI] ⫽ .95; comparative fit index [CFI] ⫽ .96; to our eight ethical decision making scenarios (␣ ⫽ .80). Two
root-mean-square residual [RMR] ⫽ .06), suggesting the validity weeks later, these same students were asked to indicate how often
of Bandura and colleagues’ (Bandura et al., 1996) claim that moral they had engaged in each of 13 cheating, lying, or stealing behav-
disengagement should be measured as a single higher order con- iors (e.g., “taking small amounts of money from my parent’s wallet
cept. We therefore created an overall measure (␣ ⫽ .87) of moral without their permission,” “copying from another student on a
disengagement by averaging the responses to the 24 items. test”; ␣ ⫽ .80). We were able to match data for 58 respondents. At
Unethical decision making. We created a measure of unethical .61 ( p ⬍ .001), the correlation between these scales indicates that
decision making by averaging responses to eight ethically charged those who have engaged in an array of unethical behaviors are
scenarios presented to subjects in Survey 3. Five scenarios were significantly more likely to say that they intend to behave uneth-
created specifically for this study; three were adapted from Seg- ically as measured by our Unethical Decision Making scale.
lin’s Ethics Survey (n.d.). A sample scenario is “You work as an
office assistant for a department at [University X]. You’re alone in Control Variables
the office making copies and realize you’re out of copy paper at
home. You therefore slip a ream of paper into your backpack.” For To account for variance in the dependent variables (moral
this and the other seven scenarios (see Appendix B, Scenarios disengagement for tests of Hypotheses 1– 4 and unethical decisions
1– 8), respondents were asked to read the scenario and evaluate for Hypotheses 5– 6f) that might be explained by factors other than
“How likely is it that you would engage in the behavior de- the hypothesized variables, we first entered four control variables
scribed?” using a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all likely) to into each of our regression models. We coded male subjects as 1
382 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

and female subjects as 0 on a gender dummy variable. Although the midpoint of the 0 – 6 scale) and standard deviation (1.0) for
often perceived to be different in their ethics (Schminke, Ambrose, unethical decision making suggests reasonably high variability in
& Miles, 2003), small or nonsignificant differences have been subjects’ willingness to say that they would engage in unethical
found between male subjects and female subjects on ethical rea- actions.
soning and outcomes. When differences were found, female sub- Ordinary least squares regression analyses were used to test the
jects demonstrated slightly higher moral reasoning or more ethical hypotheses. Results are shown in Table 2. We first tested the
action (Ambrose & Schminke, 1999; Gephart, Harrison, & hypotheses involving individual difference influences on moral
Treviño, 2007; O’Fallon & Butterfield, 2005; Rest, Thoma, Moon, disengagement (Hypotheses 1– 4) and then tested the hypothesis
& Getz, 1986). Second, students with an intended major in busi- that moral disengagement will predict subsequent unethical deci-
ness were assigned a 1 and education a 0 on an intended major sion making (Hypothesis 5). Finally, we tested our hypotheses
dummy variable because students with different intended majors about moral disengagement as a mediator of the individual
may systematically differ in their propensity to morally disengage differences– unethical decision making relationships (Hypotheses
or behave unethically due to other unmeasured individual charac- 6a– 6f).
teristics (McCabe & Treviño, 1995). We also entered each sub- To account for the variance in moral disengagement explained
ject’s total SAT score (obtained with permission from university by the control variables, we first entered only respondents’ gender,
records) into each regression equation because moral reasoning is intended major, total SAT score, and native country into a regres-
a cognitive task and has been associated with mental ability (Rest, sion model. As shown in Model 1 of Table 2, 11% of the variance
1986). Finally, we used a native country dummy variable indicat- in respondents’ moral disengagement was accounted for by the
ing whether respondents had spent the majority of their first 18 control variables. Male subjects were more likely to be morally
years in the U.S. (1) or elsewhere (0) as a final control variable in disengaged than were female subjects (B ⫽ .29; 95% confidence
all analyses because the native country of individuals may influ- interval [CI] ⫽ .19, .39; p ⬍ .001), while those intending to major
ence their ethical considerations. For example, national K–12 in business (vs. education) were less likely to be morally disen-
educational approaches may have systematic effects on ethical gaged (B ⫽ –.12; 95% CI ⫽ –.23, –.02; p ⬍ .05). The other control
awareness in young adults; alternatively, national culture differ- variables, SAT score and native country, were nonsignificant.
ences may alter individuals’ perceptions of right and wrong in In Model 2, we entered the control variables, followed by the six
particular situations (McAlister, 2001). independent variables to test the empathy, trait cynicism, locus of
control (internal, chance, and power), and moral identity influ-
Results ences on moral disengagement (Hypotheses 1– 4). The inclusion of
these individual difference variables explained an additional 16%
To assess construct independence among the eight independent of the variance in moral disengagement, F(6, 300) ⫽ 10.3, p ⬍
and dependent variables, we conducted confirmatory factor anal- .001. As predicted in Hypothesis 1, empathy (B ⫽ –.10; 95% CI ⫽
ysis on the items measuring empathy, cynicism, the three locus of –.15, –.04; p ⬍ .01) was negatively related to moral disengage-
control orientations, and moral identity, moral disengagement, and ment, indicating that those more likely to empathize with others
unethical decision making. The hypothesized eight factor structure are less likely to morally disengage. Conversely, trait cynicism
for the 76 items fit the data well (RMSEA ⫽ .04, CFI ⫽ .93, (B ⫽ .10; 95% CI ⫽ .06, .14; p ⬍ .001) was positively related to
NNFI ⫽ .92, RMR ⫽ .06). We also fit a number of other moral disengagement, lending support to the hypothesis (Hypoth-
models, including (a) a seven factor model that assessed how esis 2) that the higher in trait cynicism an individual is, the more
the data fit a model with the moral disengagement and unethical likely he or she is to morally disengage. However, only one of the
decision making items as a single factor and (b) a six factor three locus of control orientations (chance) was significant.
model that loaded moral disengagement and unethical decision Chance locus of control (B ⫽ .09; 95% CI ⫽ .02, .15; p ⬍ .01) was
making onto a single factor as well as both externally oriented positively related to moral disengagement, supporting the hypoth-
locus of control dimensions (power and chance) onto a different esis (Hypothesis 3b) that those who see life’s events as due to fate
distinct factor. The intent of these alternative models was to or luck are more likely to morally disengage. Neither the internal
rule out more parsimonious models as providing equally good (B ⫽ .01; 95% CI ⫽ –.06, .09; ns) nor power (B ⫽ .09; 95% CI ⫽
or better fit to the data. For each model with fewer than eight –.07, .06; ns) locus of control orientations were related to moral
factors, all fit statistics suggested a significantly worse fit to the disengagement. Thus, Hypotheses 3a and 3c were not supported.
data. Additionally, all chi-square differences per degrees of Finally, moral identity (B ⫽ –.13; 95% CI ⫽ –.22, –.04; p ⬍ .01)
freedom between the eight factor model and the models with was negatively related to moral disengagement, providing support
fewer factors were significant at p ⬍ .001. Thus, we found for the hypothesis that moral identity reduces the propensity to
strong support for our hypothesized eight factor structure for morally disengage (Hypothesis 4).
this study’s independent and dependent variables. As shown in Model 3 (Table 2), we next tested the hypothesis
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and intercor- that moral disengagement would significantly predict unethical
relations of the study variables. The bivariate correlations between decision making (Hypothesis 5). To account for the variance in
the individual difference variables, moral disengagement, and un- unethical decision making explained by alternative potential
ethical decision making are nearly all in the predicted direction and causes of unethical decision making, we first estimated a model
most are statistically significant. The mean (2.11) and standard with this study’s 4 control variables and the 6 independent
deviation (.41) for moral disengagement, assessed on a 5-point variables. Collectively these 10 variables explain 16% of the
scale, suggests most respondents are not highly prone to moral variance in unethical decision making, but only empathy (B ⫽
disengagement, while the mean (M ⫽ 3.30, which is slightly above –.19; 95% CI ⫽ –.34, –.04; p ⬍ .05) and trait cynicism (B ⫽
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 383

Table 2
Results of Ordinary Least Squares Regression Analysis: Individual Differences, Moral Disengagement, and Unethical Decisions

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

DV ⫽ moral DV ⫽ moral DV ⫽ unethical decision DV ⫽ unethical


disengagement disengagement making decision making

Variable B 95% CI B 95% CI B 95% CI B 95% CI

Control variable
Gendera .29*** .19, .39 .18*** .09, .28 .13 ⫺.11, .38 .03 ⫺.22, .28
Business vs. educationb ⫺.12* ⫺.23,⫺.02 ⫺.13* ⫺.23,⫺.02 .05 ⫺.23, .32 .12 ⫺.15, .39
SAT total .00 .00, .00 .00 .00, .00 .00 ⫺.00, .00 .00 ⫺.00, .00
Native countryc ⫺.22 ⫺.45, .00 ⫺.21 ⫺.42, .00 .27 ⫺.28, .81 .38 ⫺.16, .92
Independent variable
Empathy ⫺.10** ⫺.15, .04 ⫺.19* ⫺.34,⫺.04 ⫺.13 ⫺.28, .02
Trait cynicism .10*** .06, .14 .26*** .16, .37 .21*** .10, .32
Internal locus of control .01 ⫺.06, .09 .08 ⫺.11, .28 .08 ⫺.11, .26
Chance locus of control .09** .02, .15 .09 ⫺.08, .27 .05 ⫺.13, .22
Power locus of control ⫺.01 ⫺.07, .06 .05 ⫺.12, .22 .05 ⫺.12, .22
Moral identity ⫺.13** ⫺.22,⫺.04 ⫺.15 ⫺.38, .09 ⫺.07 ⫺.31, .16
Mediator
Moral disengagement .56*** .27, .86

R2 .11*** .27*** .16*** .20***


Adjusted R2 .10 .24 .13 .17
⌬R2 .16*** .04***
df (regression, residual) (4, 302) (10, 296) (10, 296) (11, 295)
a
Dichotomous variable for gender: male ⫽ 1; female ⫽ 0.
b
Dichotomous variable for intended major: business ⫽ 1; education ⫽ 0.
c
Dichotomous variable for native country: United States ⫽ 1; outside United States ⫽ 0.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

.26; 95% CI ⫽ .16, .37; p ⬍ .001) were significantly related to directly affect moral disengagement (the mediator). The Sobel test
this dependent variable. In Model 4, we added moral disen- statistics are significant at p ⬍ .05 for all four of the tested
gagement to the model as a predictor of unethical decision independent variables: empathy (z ⫽ –2.50, p ⫽ .012), trait cyn-
making. As predicted (Hypothesis 5), moral disengagement icism (z ⫽ 2.91, p ⫽ .004), chance locus of control (z ⫽ 2.16, p ⫽
(B ⫽ .56; 95% CI ⫽ .27, .86; p ⬍ .001) was significantly and .031), and moral identity (z ⫽ –2.33, p ⫽ .020). Thus, we find
positively related to unethical decision making. The inclusion support for the hypotheses that moral disengagement plays a
of moral disengagement explained an additional 4% of the mediating role between empathy (Hypothesis 6a), cynicism (Hy-
variance in unethical decision making, F(1, 305) ⫽ 14.1, p ⬍ pothesis 6b), chance locus of control (Hypothesis 6d), and moral
.001. Thus, the results provide support for our hypothesis identity (Hypothesis 6f) and the dependent variable— unethical
(Hypothesis 5) that individuals who are more morally disen- decisions. Conversely, results do not support the mediation hy-
gaged are more likely to make unethical decisions. potheses involving the internal (Hypothesis 6c) and power (Hy-
Hypotheses 6a– 6f proposed that moral disengagement will me- pothesis 6e) locus of control orientations.
diate relationships between the focal individual differences and
unethical decisions. The traditional method for assessing media- Discussion
tion over the past 2 decades has been the multi-step process
Our goal in this research was to better understand the anteced-
outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986). However, recent research
ents and outcomes of moral disengagement, a set of cognitive
has suggested that mediation can be established without significant
mechanisms that are thought to deactivate moral self-regulation
direct relationships between independent and dependent variables
and allow individuals to make unethical decisions more easily
(the first step in the Baron–Kenny method; MacKinnon, Lock-
(Bandura, 1999). We found that individual differences in empathy,
wood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; Shrout & Bolger, 2002).
trait cynicism, chance locus of control, and moral identity predict
We therefore tested our mediation hypotheses with the Sobel test
moral disengagement and that moral disengagement predicts un-
(Sobel, 1982), a method for assessing indirect effects that does not
ethical decision making. Further, we found support for the idea
require significant main effects for the independent variables and
that moral disengagement mediates the relationships between these
that provides better balance between Type I and Type II errors.
individual differences and unethical decisions.
(MacKinnon et al., 2002).
We conducted the Sobel tests for moral disengagement as a
Contributions and Implications for Future Research
mediator of relationships between empathy, trait cynicism, chance
locus of control, and moral identity, excluding tests for the internal Our study contributes to knowledge about moral disengagement
and power locus of control orientations because they did not on several fronts. First, we identified four types of individual
384 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

differences that had been previously associated with moral cogni- For example, parenting style has been found to influence moral
tion and action and that could be theoretically linked with the disengagement in children (Pelton et al., 2004). Also, in the
facilitation or inhibition of moral disengagement. We proposed organizational literature, researchers have theorized that an ethical
that these individual differences would predispose individuals to environment can reduce employee use of corruption-inducing psy-
see others, events, and themselves in ways that make moral dis- chological tactics (Anand et al., 2005; Tenbrunsel & Messick,
engagement more or less likely. We found that four individual 2004). Thus, future research should investigate the possibility that
differences predict moral disengagement, explaining 16% of the contextual factors (e.g., leadership style, ethical climate or culture)
variance in moral disengagement beyond control variables. have independent and interactive (with individual difference fac-
Consistent with our theorizing that empathy would make it tors) influences on moral disengagement. Adding contextual fac-
harder, for example, to discount or dismiss harm to others, we tors to future studies is likely to increase the amount of variance in
found that those who are more empathic are less likely to morally moral disengagement that researchers are able to explain and
disengage. This finding is consistent with previous research and provide additional practical insights for those seeking to reduce
theory that has emphasized the importance for ethical decision unethical decision making.
making of seeing a situation from another’s perspective both With regard to the consequences of moral disengagement, we
cognitively and affectively (Eisenberg, 2000; Kohlberg, 1969). We found higher levels of moral disengagement to be positively asso-
also found that trait cynicism facilitates moral disengagement. ciated with increased unethical decision making, providing addi-
Rather than heightening sensitivity to the plight of others as tional support beyond research previously conducted on aggres-
empathy does, such thoughts appear to be suppressed in trait sion in children (Bandura et al., 1996, 2001) and beyond research
cynics. We theorized that this is because of trait cynics’ distrust of that found a relationship between a single moral disengagement
others, which should allow them to more easily distance them- mechanism (moral justification) and coworker undermining
selves from and diffuse responsibility to others as well as blame or (Duffy et al., 2005). Moral disengagement explained 4% of the
dehumanize victims. variance in unethical decision making beyond that explained by an
We developed and tested another set of hypotheses proposing array of individual differences. This finding suggests that moral
that locus of control orientations would inhibit or facilitate moral disengagement can help us understand why unethical decision
disengagement. We found that chance locus of control orientation making occurs. Because Bandura’s theory applies to transgressive
was positively related to moral disengagement, suggesting that behavior more generally (Bandura, 1999), we speculate that our
those who believe life experiences and outcomes are due to forces findings on moral disengagement’s influence on unethical deci-
outside their control are more likely to morally disengage (perhaps sions should extend to a wider variety of behaviors that violate
by displacing or diffusing responsibility). However, our hypothe- societal or organizational norms and are therefore of great interest
ses regarding internal locus of control and power locus of control to managers—for example, antisocial, counterproductive, and de-
were not supported. The nonsignificant finding for internal locus viant behaviors (Bennett & Robinson, 2003; Vardi & Weitz,
of control orientation may be attributable to the relatively low 2003).
reliability obtained for the measure in our sample. The nonsignif- Given previous research suggesting direct effects on unethical
icant finding for the powerful others locus of control orientation decisions for the individual differences we studied as antecedents
may be because our decision making scenarios did not generally of moral disengagement, we also asked whether moral disengage-
highlight authority relationships. Future research should continue ment might help to explain why these relationships exist. We
to examine how locus of control orientations relate to moral found that moral disengagement plays a mediating role between
disengagement. unethical decision making and individual differences in empathy,
Finally, we argued from an identity perspective that an individ- trait cynicism, chance locus of control, and moral identity. Thus,
ual who thinks of the self in terms of moral concerns and com- one of the reasons less empathic people and those with a less
mitments (Aquino & Reed, 2002) will be less likely to morally self-important moral identity are more likely to make unethical
disengage in ways that minimize or misconstrue harm to others. decisions is that such individuals are more likely to morally
Our findings support this hypothesis, namely that higher self- disengage. Similarly, distrustful cynics and those high on chance
importance of moral identity was negatively related to moral locus of control may be more likely to make unethical decisions in
disengagement. part because they disconnect from self-censure mechanisms by
In sum, our research is among the first to propose and find that morally disengaging.
moral disengagement is facilitated or inhibited by a number of Although our research contributed to knowledge about the an-
individual differences that reflect the ways individuals think of tecedents and outcomes of moral disengagement, much remains to
others, events, and themselves. Although our hypotheses were be understood about the temporal aspects of moral disengagement.
based on solid theorizing, our study did not measure the proposed While Bandura argued that moral disengagement precedes uneth-
cognitive mechanisms associated with these individual differences, ical behavior (e.g., “People do not ordinarily engage in reprehen-
such as perspective taking, trust, or responsibility taking. Future sible conduct until they have justified to themselves the rightness
work should explicitly measure these mechanisms. For example, of their actions”; Bandura, 1996, p. 335), and while our Moral
researchers might employ Davis’s (1980) Interpersonal Reactivity Disengagement measure did precede the Unethical Decision Mak-
Index to more directly assess perspective taking. ing measure, our study did not attempt to access the internal
Beyond dispositional influences, social cognitive theory (Ban- cognitive processing that immediately precedes or follows specific
dura, 1986) and interactionist theories of ethical decision making unethical decisions. Others have noted that processes similar to
behavior (Treviño, 1986) suggest that social context antecedents moral disengagement can also be invoked retrospectively (Anand
are also likely to be important predictors of moral disengagement. et al., 2005). For example, individuals queried later about their
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 385

unethical behavior may find themselves resorting to attribution of the plausibility of the theory and logic we have provided rather
blame, diffusion of responsibility, and similar tactics to justify a than conclusive empirical evidence.
prior unethical act. Future research should explore these cognitions Our college student sample raises concerns about generalizabil-
and their timing more directly. Research might also investigate ity (Gordon, Slade, & Schmitt, 1986). However, we believe our
whether and how post hoc rationalizations for an act at Time 1 feed sample was appropriate and useful given our primary focus on a
pre-act moral disengagement in future periods. In other words, is psychological process and our desire to collect data from individ-
there a self-reinforcing cycle of moral disengagement? uals on multiple constructs at multiple time periods. Prominent
Future research can also help to determine where moral disen- researchers (e.g., Greenberg, 1987) have also argued that student
gagement fits within the ethical decision making process and about samples are not necessarily more problematic than many nonrep-
the level of consciousness involved. For example, Rest’s (1986) resentative adult samples and that student samples may be partic-
commonly cited four-stage model asserts that ethical decision ularly useful for understanding psychological processes. Others
making involves the sequence of moral sensitivity, judgment, have demonstrated quite convincingly (e.g., Locke, 1986) that
intention, and action. It is currently unclear whether moral disen- research findings on psychological processes such as goal setting
gagement is associated with moral sensitivity, moral judgment, or conducted with student and adult samples are quite similar. None-
both. Moral sensitivity involves the initial recognition that the theless, we recognize that future studies using diverse samples are
individual is facing a decision with moral overtones. It seems quite needed to help establish generalizability.
possible that moral disengagement mutes moral sensitivity by When designing the study, we also considered the issue of social
reducing moral issue recognition. This would suggest that moral desirability bias, always a concern in ethics-related research (Ran-
disengagement occurs below consciousness—that individuals us- dall & Fernandes, 1991). As noted in the Method section, prior to
ing moral disengagement would not be aware that they are using completing all surveys, respondents were told that there were no
such cognition to completely ignore the ethical nature of a situa- incorrect answers and that the research would have value only if
tion. Alternatively, moral disengagement might instead be more they gave truthful responses, not those they might think seem more
related to the next step, moral judgment, the process of deciding desirable. They were also assured on each survey that their re-
what is right or wrong in a situation. If moral disengagement is sponses would be kept completely confidential and would not be
associated primarily with moral judgment, it would suggest that used to evaluate them in any way. We did not use a measure of
moral disengagement operates at a more conscious level and social desirability bias in our research because, having discussed
involves rationalizing away recognized moral concerns. Much ethical issues with similar students in our classes for years, we
more research will be required to situate moral disengagement have found them to be surprisingly open about their willingness to
within these stages of ethical decision making and to determine the engage in unethical actions such as lying, cheating, and stealing.
level of conscious thought involved. When discussing their reasons with them, we noticed a pattern of
Finally, researchers may also wish to explore the theoretical and responses that indicated moral disengagement. This experience
empirical differences between moral disengagement and moral does not rule out the possibility that social desirability bias influ-
engagement. We speculate that the two may be separate constructs enced our results. However, to the extent that it did, the effect is
rather than opposite ends of a single continuum. That is, an likely to attenuate rather than inflate the significance of our find-
individual who does not routinely morally disengage need not be ings. Nevertheless, future researchers should consider including a
engaged in active reflection about ethical issues. For example, measure of social desirability bias in their studies, particularly with
managers may passively accept extreme pay differentials between samples and in settings where respondents may be more likely to
executives and average workers without using moral disengage- fear being identified.
ment to justify these differentials but also without actively engag- We also acknowledge that, despite its frequent use in behavioral
ing the potential moral aspects of the pay gap issue. Some addi- ethics research (Treviño & Weaver, 2003; Weber, 1992) and
tional cognitive effort may be required in order for an individual to evidence that relationships between individual differences and
morally engage. Here, research on the moral intensity of the ethical unethical intentions and behaviors are similar in direction and
issue (Jones, 1991) and moral awareness (e.g., Butterfield, significance (Gephart et al., 2007), a scenario-based measure can
Treviño, & Weaver, 2000; Flannery & May, 2000; May & Pauli, only simulate unethical decision making behavior. We did, how-
2002) seems relevant. For example, moral intensity of an issue ever, take substantial steps to validate the Unethical Decision
depends largely on the extent of the harm involved. A situation that Making measure, designing two validation exercises to demon-
is likely to produce serious harm should be more likely to get strate a significant statistical link between our measure and behav-
attention and instantiate moral standards (Jones, 1991). ior. In the “keep the cash” validation exercise, the significant
correlation was of modest absolute size (.34). However, the results
are within the range that might be expected considering that (a) the
Strengths and Limitations of the Research two items that correlated (a single scenario from the scale and a
related behavioral indicator) were designed to be only roughly
We conducted a carefully designed study in a professionally equivalent to avoid priming effects (Feldman & Lynch, 1988), and
oriented sample of young adults, in which we collected data over (b) modest effect sizes in the .30 range may be near the upper limit
multiple time periods in order to demonstrate mediation via the of what is possible when explaining relationships between com-
ideal condition of having the independent, mediator, and depen- plex variables that are likely to be determined by multiple factors
dent variables collected sequentially over time. Nonetheless, in the (Swann, Chang-Schneider, & McClarty, 2007). In the “cheat–lie–
absence of an experimental design, judgments about the causal steal” validation exercise, we correlated responses to our Unethical
ordering among our key constructs must be rendered according to Decision Making measure with respondents’ self-reported lying,
386 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

cheating, and stealing behavior (r ⫽ .608, p ⬍ .001). Thus, the high on predictors such as empathy, for placement into ethically
evidence from these validation exercises shows significant statis- sensitive positions.
tical relationships between our intentions-based Unethical Deci- Organizations may also attempt to influence existing employees
sion Making measure and actual behaviors. who are found to be more prone to moral disengagement. Training
These validation exercises were not designed to provide evi- could be developed to help such individuals uncover the ethical
dence of a relationship between moral disengagement and behav- dimensions of their work. This might include helping employees
ior. However, we also regressed the “keep the cash” binary out- recognize the most common euphemisms, distortions, and external
come variable on moral disengagement and the other individual attributions associated with their particular job, company, or in-
difference variables in a logistic regression analysis (n ⫽ 59).1 The dustry. Training could also be designed, in part, to integrate our
results were nonsignificant but trended in the hypothesized direc- findings on specific individual differences that predict moral dis-
tion (e.g., the odds ratio for moral disengagement was 1.79). These engagement. For example, training could aim to enhance empathy
nonsignificant results could reflect a mismatch between the very for certain types of stakeholders (e.g., customers, distant employ-
broad nature of the independent variables and the very narrow ees). Research will be required to determine if specific interven-
single behavior outcome (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977; Fisher, 1980; tions have their intended effects, but others have found that cog-
Harrison, Newman, & Roth, 2006), the likely meager reliability nitive biases and distortions can be significantly reduced via
associated with a single behavioral item (Wanous & Hudy, 2001), training interventions (c.f. Bazerman, 1994).
and/or the low power associated with a logistic regression using a Organizations might also be able to guard against moral disen-
small sample with a nonoptimal base rate of the phenomenon gagement by instituting decision making systems that are designed
(Agresti, 2002; Long, 1997; Tosteson, Buzas, Demidenko & Kara- to explicitly surface and address ethical issues. For example,
gas, 2003). In regard to the first possibility, we also examined the decision makers could be required to have multiple stakeholders
correlation between moral disengagement and a broader range of review certain types of decisions, or a decision making group
unethical behaviors. Specifically, as part of the third exercise to could assign an “ethical decision making advocate” (someone who
validate our Unethical Decision Making measure (discussed in the is not prone to moral disengagement) to listen specifically for signs
Method section and in Appendix B), we included this study’s of moral disengagement (e.g., “it’s just part of the game,” “it
24-item measure of moral disengagement and then computed the doesn’t hurt anyone,” “it’s their own fault”). Standardized lists of
correlation between respondents’ moral disengagement and their questions might also be developed to help individuals and groups
self-reported cheating, lying, and stealing behaviors (collected 2 identify the potential moral blind spots in their decision processes.
weeks later). The statistically significant correlation (r ⫽ .389, p ⬍
.01) provides support for a relationship between moral disengage- Conclusions
ment and unethical decisions, but clearly more research is needed
to definitively link moral disengagement to a range of unethical The current management environment places a premium on
behaviors that cause harm to organizations and society. understanding the drivers of ethical decision making. We therefore
conducted a study focusing on moral disengagement as a set of
sociocognitive thought processes influenced by individual differ-
Practical Implications ences and associated with unethical decision making. Clearly, we
have a long way to go toward understanding these mechanisms and
The subtext underlying this study rests on our normative belief their antecedents and consequences as well as how they interact
that moral disengagement should be reduced because it leads to with other individual differences and contextual influences. How-
unethical decisions. Moral disengagement is likely to be particu- ever, this research provides a useful starting point, and the results
larly important in organizations because bureaucratic structures suggest the potential value of further efforts to better understand
and the division of labor seem to lend themselves to moral disen- moral disengagement processes so we can test interventions that
gagement mechanisms such as the diffusion and displacement of might counter their negative effects.
responsibility (Bandura, 1986). Euphemistic labeling is also com-
mon in organizations, such as when managers refer to layoffs as
“rightsizing.” Also, with victims out of sight, globalization makes
1
it easier to ignore or distort the harmful consequences of business This regression was conducted at the request of an anonymous re-
actions. Thus, moral disengagement seems highly relevant to un- viewer.
derstanding unethical behavior in 21st century organizations.
Our individual difference results suggest that organizations can References
identify individuals who are prone to moral disengagement. For
example, job applicants determined as a result of selection assess- Abraham, R. (2000). Organizational cynicism bases and consequences.
ments to be high on trait cynicism and chance locus of control Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 126, 269 –292.
orientation or low on moral identity and empathy should be con- Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude– behavior relations: A theoret-
sidered at risk for moral disengagement. Given the relationship
ical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin,
between moral disengagement and unethical decisions, organiza- 84, 888 –918.
tions may choose to avoid hiring individuals with higher propen- Ambrose, M. L., & Schminke, M. (1999). Sex differences in business
sity to morally disengage, particularly for jobs that are unsuper- ethics: The importance of perceptions. Journal of Managerial Issues, 11,
vised or ethically sensitive. Organizations might also explicitly 454 – 474.
search for individuals who are low on moral disengagement, or Anand, V., Ashforth, B. E., & Joshi, M. (2005). Business as usual: The
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 387

acceptance and perpetuation of corruption in organizations. Academy of Diener, E. (1977). Deindividuation: Causes and consequences. Social
Management Executive, 19, 9 –23. Behavior and Personality, 5, 143–156.
Andersson, L. M., & Bateman, T. S. (1997). Cynicism in the workplace: Duffy, M. K., Aquino, K., Tepper, B. J., Reed, A., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M.
Some causes and effects. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 449 – (2005, August). Moral disengagement and social identification: When
469. does being similar result in harm doing? Paper presented at the annual
Aquino, K., & Reed, A. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. meeting of the Academy of Management, Honolulu, HI.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440. Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Hills-
Aquino, K., Reed, A., Thau, S., & Freeman, D. (2007). A grotesque and dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
dark beauty: How moral identity and mechanisms of moral disengage- Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual
ment influence cognitive and emotional reactions to war. Journal of Review of Psychology, 51, 665– 697.
Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 385–392. Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland,
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of prosocial
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 70, 1360 –1372.
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the preparation of inhuman- Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial
ities. Personal and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193–209. and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91–119.
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J.
Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psycholog-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 364 –374. ical research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299.
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C. Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G. (1988). Self-generated validity and other
(2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgres- effects of measurement on belief, attitude, intention, and behavior.
sive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 125– Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 421– 435.
135. Fisher, C. D. (1980). On the dubious wisdom of expecting job satisfaction
Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of to correlate with performance. Academy of Management Review, 5,
aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of 607– 612.
victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9, 253–269. Flannery, B. L., & May, D. R. (2000). Environmental ethical decision
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator distinction making in the U.S. metal finishing industry. Academy of Management
in social psychology: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consider- Journal, 43, 642– 662.
ations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1171–1182. Gaertner, L., & Insko, C. A. (2000). Intergroup discrimination in the
Bar-Tal, D. (1989). Stereotyping and prejudice: Changing conceptions. minimal group paradigm: Categorization, reciprocation, or fear? Journal
New York: Springer-Verlag. of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 77–94.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social– Gephart, J., Harrison, D., & Treviño, L. K. (2007, August). The who, when,
psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and where of unethical choices: Meta-analytic answers to fundamental
Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, S., Brandt, J. R., ethics questions. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy
Sprengelmeyer, P., & Bayly, M. J. (1989). Negative-state relief and the of Management, Philadelphia, PA.
empathy–altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Goldberg, L. R. (2001). International Personality Item Pool: A scientific
chology, 56, 922–933. collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of personality
Bazerman, M. (1994). Judgment in managerial decision making. New traits and other individual differences. Retrieved December 6, 2007, at
York: Wiley. http://ipip.ori.org/
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2003). The past, present, and future of Gordon, M. E., Slade, L. A., & Schmitt, N. (1986). The “science of the
workplace deviance research. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Organizational sophomore” revisited: From conjecture to empiricism. Academy of Man-
behavior: The state of the science (2nd ed., pp. 247–282). Mahwah, NJ: agement Review, 11, 191–207.
Erlbaum. Greenberg, J. (1987). The college sophomore as guinea pig: Setting the
Bierhoff, H. (2002). Prosocial behavior. New York: Taylor & Francis. record straight. Academy of Management Review, 12, 157–159.
Bok, S. (1998). Mayhem: Violence as public entertainment. New York: Harrison, D. A., & McLaughlin, M. E. (1993). Cognitive processes in
Perseus Publishing. self-report responses: Tests of item context effects in work attitude
Borrero-Hernandez, A. (1979). Unidimensional and multidimensional measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 129 –140.
measures of locus of control and their relationships to selected person- Harrison, D. A., Newman, D. A., & Roth, P. L. (2006). How important are job
ality variables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas A & M Uni- attitudes? Meta-analytic comparisons of integrative behavioral outcomes
versity. and time sequences. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 305–325.
Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A Hegarty, W. H., & Sims, H. P. (1978). Some determinants of unethical
cognitive–motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307–324. decision behavior: An experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63,
Butterfield, K. D., Treviño, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2000). Moral aware- 451– 457.
ness in business organizations: Influences of issue-related and social Hochwarter, W. A., James, M., Johnson, D., & Ferris, G. R. (2004). The
context factors. Human Relations, 53, 981–1018. interactive effects of politics perceptions and trait cynicism on work out-
Costa, P., Zonderman, A., McCrae, R., & Williams, R. (1985). Cynicism comes. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 10, 44 –57.
and paranoid alienation in the Cook and Medley Ho scale. Psychoso- Horovitz, B. (2002). Scandals grow out of CEO’s warped mind-set. Re-
matic Medicine, 48, 283–285. trieved on February 14, 2007, from http://www.usatoday.com/money/
Daniel, L. G., Blount, K. D., & Ferrell, C. M. (1991). Academic miscon- companies/management/2002.html
duct among teacher education students. Research in Higher Education, Johnson, J. L., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M. (2003). The effects of psycholog-
32, 703–725. ical contract breach and organizational cynicism: Not all social ex-
Davis, M. A. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differ- changes are created equal. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24,
ences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 627– 647.
10, 85. Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organiza-
388 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

tions: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16, Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003).
366 –395. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the
Kanter, D. L., & Mirvis, P. H. (1989). The cynical Americans. San literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 88, 879 –903.
Kelman, H. C. (1973). Violence without moral restraint: Reflections on the Randall, D. M., & Fernandes, M. F. (1991). The social desirability re-
dehumanization of victims and victimizers. Journal of Social Issues, 29, sponse bias in ethics research. Journal of Business Ethics, 10, 805– 817.
25– 61. Reiss, M. C., & Mitra, K. (1998). The effects of individual difference
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive– developmental factors on the acceptability of ethical and unethical workplace behaviors.
approach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization Journal of Business Ethics, 17, 1581–1593.
theory and research (pp. 347– 480). Chicago: Rand McNally. Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory.
Lefcourt, H. M. (1966). Internal versus external control of reinforcements: New York: Praeger.
A review. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 206 –220. Rest, J., Thoma, S. J., Moon, Y. L., & Getz, I. (1986). Different cultures,
Levenson, H. (1974). Activism and powerful others: Distinctions within sexes, and religions. In J. Rest (Ed.), Moral development: Advances in
the concept of internal– external control. Journal of Personality Assess- research and theory (pp. 89 –132). New York: Praeger.
ment, 38, 377–383. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external
Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality, powerful others, control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Ap-
and chance. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control plied, 80, 1–28.
construct: Vol. 1. Assessment methods (pp. 15– 63). New York: Aca- Rotter, J. B. (1980). Interpersonal trust, trustworthiness, and gullibility.
demic Press. American Psychologist, 35, 1–7.
Lewis, G. (2006). Why do athletes cheat? Retrieved on February 13, 2007, Safire, W. (1979, May 13). The fine art of euphemism. San Francisco
from http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/14234026.html Chronicle, p. 13.
Locke, E. A. (1986). Generalizing from laboratory to field: Ecological Samuelson, R. J. (2006). Delinquency of the CEOs. Retrieved on February
validity or abstraction of essential elements. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), 13, 2007, from http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/
Generalizing from laboratory to field settings (pp. 3–9). Lexington, MA: article.2006.html
Lexington Books Schminke, M., Ambrose, M. L., & Miles, J. A. (2003). The impact of
Long, S. J. (1997). Regression models for categorical and limited depen- gender and setting on perceptions of others’ ethics. Sex Roles, 48,
dent variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 361–375.
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berkowits
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221–
intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. 279). New York: Academic Press.
Maqsud, M. (1980). Locus of control and stages of moral reasoning. Seglin, J. L. (2000, October 15). When bribery is lost in translation. The
Psychological Reports, 46, 1243–1248. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com
Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the self- Seglin, J. L. (n.d.) Ethics Survey. Retrieved December 19, 2007, from
concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 858 – 866. http://www.mbajungle.com/monthlysurvey/ethicssurvey.cfm
May, D. R., & Pauli, K. P. (2002). The role of moral intensity in moral Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-
decision-making. Business and Society, 41, 84 –117. experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho-
McAlister, A. L. (2001). Moral disengagement: Measurement and modi- logical Methods, 7, 422– 445.
fication. Journal of Peace Research, 38, 87–99. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in
McCabe, D., & Treviño, L. K. (1993). Academic dishonesty: Honor codes structural equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological method-
and other contextual influences. Journal of Higher Education, 64, 520 – ology (pp. 290 –312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
538. Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group
McCabe, D., & Treviño, L. K. (1995). Cheating among business students: violence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
A challenge for business leaders and educators. Journal of Management Struch, N., & Schwartz, S. H. (1989). Intergroup aggression: Its predictors
Education, 19(2), 205–218. and distinctness from in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social
Messick, D. M., & Bazerman, M. H. (1996). Ethical leadership and the Psychology, 56, 364 –373.
psychology of decision making. Sloan Management Review, 37, 9 –22. Swann, W. B., Chang-Schneider, C., & McClarty, K. L. (2007). Do
Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggres- people’s self-views matter? American Psychologist, 62, 84 –94.
sion and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (2004). Ethical fading: The role of
324 –344. self-deception in unethical behavior. Social Justice Research, 17, 223–236.
Mudrack, P. E., & Mason, E. S. (1996). Individual ethical beliefs and Terpstra, D. E., Reyes, M. G. C., & Bokor, D. W. (1991). Predictors of
perceived organizational interests. Journal of Business Ethics, 15, 851– ethical decisions regarding insider trading. Journal of Business Ethics,
861. 10, 699 –710.
O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical Tosteson, T., Buzas, J. S., Demidenko, E., & Karagas, M. (2003). Power
ethical decision-making literature: 1996 –2003. Journal of Business Eth- and sample size calculations for generalized regression models with
ics, 59, 375– 413. covariate measurement error. Statistics in Medicine, 22, 1069 –1082.
Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Jour- Treviño, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person–
nal of Social Issues, 4, 1–20. situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11,
Ostroff, C., Kinicki, A. J., & Clark, M. A. (2002). Substantive and 601– 617.
operational issues of response bias across levels of analysis: An example Treviño, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2003). Managing ethics in business
of climate-satisfaction relationships. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, organizations: Social scientific perspectives. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford
355–368. University Press.
Pelton, J., Gound, M., Forehand, R., & Brody, G. (2004). The moral Treviño, L. K., & Youngblood, S. A. (1990). Bad apples in bad barrels: A
disengagement scale: Extension with an American minority sample. causal analysis of ethical decision-making behavior. Journal of Applied
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 31–39. Psychology, 74, 378 –385.
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 389

Vardi, Y., & Weitz, E. (2003). Misbehavior in organizations. Englewood Weber, J. (1992). Scenarios in business ethics research: Review, critical
Cliffs, NJ: Erlbaum. assessment, and recommendations. Business Ethics Quarterly, 2, 137–160.
Wanous, J. P., & Hudy, M. J. (2001). Single-item reliability: A replication Wrightsman, L. S. (1992). Assumptions about human nature: Implications
and extension. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 361–375. for researchers and practitioners (2nd ed.). London: Sage.

Appendix A

Items Used to Assess Moral Disengagement

1. It is alright to fight to protect your friends. [MJ] not be blamed if other students go ahead and do it.
[DIFR]
2. It’s ok to steal to take care of your family’s needs.
[MJ] 19. If a group decides together to do something harmful,
it is unfair to blame any one member of the group for
3. It’s ok to attack someone who threatens your fami- it. [DIFR]
ly’s honor. [MJ]
20. You can’t blame a person who plays only a small
4. It is alright to lie to keep your friends out of trouble. part in the harm caused by a group. [DIFR]
[MJ]
21. It is ok to tell small lies because they don’t really do any
5. Sharing test questions is just a way of helping your harm. [DC]
friends. [EL]
22. People don’t mind being teased because it shows
6. Talking about people behind their backs is just part interest in them. [DC]
of the game. [EL]
23. Teasing someone does not really hurt them. [DC]
7. Looking at a friend’s homework without permission
is just “borrowing it.” [EL] 24. Insults don’t really hurt anyone. [DC]
8. It is not bad to “get high” once in a while. [EL] 25. If students misbehave in class, it’s their teacher’s fault.
[AB]
9. Damaging some property is no big deal when you
consider that others are beating up people. [AC] 26. If someone leaves something lying around, it’s their
own fault if it gets stolen. [AB]
10. Stealing some money is not too serious compared to
those who steal a lot of money. [AC] 27. People who are mistreated have usually done things
to deserve it. [AB]
11. Not working very hard in school is really no big deal when
you consider that other people are probably cheating. [AC] 28. People are not at fault for misbehaving at work if
their managers mistreat them. [AB]
12. Compared to other illegal things people do, taking
some things from a store without paying for them is 29. Some people deserve to be treated like animals.
not very serious. [AC]
[DEH]
13. If people are living under bad conditions, they can-
30. It is ok to treat badly someone who behaved like a
not be blamed for behaving aggressively. [DISR]
“worm.” [DEH]
14. If the professor doesn’t discipline cheaters, students
31. Someone who is obnoxious does not deserve to be
should not be blamed for cheating. [DISR]
treated like a human being. [DEH]
15. If someone is pressured into doing something, they
32. Some people have to be treated roughly because they
shouldn’t be blamed for it. [DISR]
lack feelings that can be hurt. [DEH]
16. People cannot be blamed for misbehaving if their
Notes. Items in bold compose the 24-item scale used in the study.
friends pressured them to do it. [DISR]
Items not in bold represent items dropped based on factor analysis.
17. A member of a group or team should not be blamed MJ ⫽ moral justification; EL ⫽ euphemistic labeling; AC ⫽ advan-
for the trouble the team caused. [DIFR] tageous comparison; DISR ⫽ displacement of responsibility; DIFR ⫽
diffusion of responsibility; DC ⫽ distortion of consequences; AB ⫽
18. A student who only suggests breaking the rules should attribution of blame; DEH ⫽ dehumanization.

(Appendixes continue)
390 DETERT, KLEBE TREVIÑO, AND SWEITZER

Appendix B

Development and Validation of the Unethical Decision Making Scale

The Unethical Decision Making Scale additional scenarios (see below) created to represent behaviors that
were not thought to be unethical. Responses (on a 5-point Likert
Study respondents were asked “How likely is it that you scale) were used to determine whether the ethics experts viewed
would engage in the behavior described?” Each of the following the eight scenarios used in our scale as a violation of one or more
scenarios was rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all ethical principles and whether the grand mean for “violates one or
likely) to 6 (highly likely). Cronbach’s alpha for the eight-item more ethical principles” for the 8 scenarios used in our Unethical
scale ⫽ .74. Decision Making scale differed significantly from the grand mean
on the other 5 scenarios. Findings indicated that the ethics experts
1. You work in a fast-food restaurant in downtown [City X]. It’s agreed that the 8 scenarios used in the scale represent violations of
against policy to eat food without paying for it. You came straight ethical principles: The grand mean for the items used in our
from classes and are therefore hungry. Your supervisor isn’t Unethical Decision Making scale (Items 1– 8) was 4.66 (SD ⫽ .35;
around, so you make something for yourself and eat it without with a range of 4.44 –5.00 for the means of the individual scenar-
paying. ios). Conversely, the grand mean for the 5 scenarios not included
2. You work as an office assistant for a department at [Univer- in our scale (Items 9 –13 below) was 1.68 (SD ⫽ .62) with a range
sity Y]. You’re alone in the office making copies and realize for the means of the individual scenarios from 1.25 to 2.06. The
you’re out of copy paper at home. You therefore slip a ream of difference between these means (M ⫽ 4.66 vs. 1.68) was statisti-
paper into your backpack. cally significant, t(15) ⫽ 15.3, p ⬍ .001.
3. You’re preparing for the final exam in a class where the
professor uses the same exam in both sections. Some of your 9. You and your roommates draw straws for choosing bedrooms
friends somehow get a copy of the exam after the first section. in the 3 bedroom house you’re renting. You draw the longest straw
They are now trying to memorize the right answers. You don’t and get first choice so you choose the biggest room.
look at the exam, but just ask them what topics you should focus 10. Your professor in a large lecture class requires that your
your studying on. assignments be submitted in hard copy before the end of class.
4. You’ve waited in line for 10 minutes to buy a coffee and When you get to class on Thursday, you realize that you forgot to
muffin at Starbucks. When you’re a couple of blocks away, you bring your assignment. So, you decide to skip class, rush home,
realize that the clerk gave you change for $20 rather than for the print the assignment and rush back to turn it in before class is over.
$10 you gave him. You savor your coffee, muffin and free $10. 11. You need 3 more general education credits. After asking
5. You get the final exam back from your professor and you around, you narrow your choice to two. You’ve heard that one is
notice that he’s marked correct three answers that you got wrong. very valuable but hard. The other is less valuable but an easy A.
Revealing his error would mean the difference between an A and You choose the easy A.
a B. You say nothing. 12. Your professor asks you to complete a voluntary survey, but
6. Your accounting course requires you to purchase a software you don’t because you’re just too busy.
package that sells for $50. Your friend, who is also in the class, has 13. You start college in September and receive a scholarship that
already bought the software and offers to lend it to you. You take requires you to perform 40 hours of community service within 12
it and load it onto your computer. months. Since you already committed to volunteering at your
7. Your boss at your summer job asks you to get confidential hometown church next summer, you decline other community
information about a competitor’s product. You therefore pose as a service opportunities, figuring that the service at your church will
student doing a research project on the competitor’s company and be enough.
ask for the information.
8. You are assigned a team project in one of your courses. Your Construct Validation Exercise 2
team waits until the last minute to begin working. Several team
members suggest using an old project out of their fraternity/ Sixty subjects were randomly selected from the final sample of
sorority files. You go along with this plan. 307 subjects. Each subject was sent a letter from a strategy
doctoral candidate in the authors’ departments in which the doc-
Construct Validation Exercise 1 toral candidate explained that, as part of his dissertation research
on strategic decision making, he had recently conducted a short lab
Twenty business ethics experts were contacted via e-mail and study (on make, buy, or ally decisions in international expansions)
asked to rate the scenarios based on whether the behavior in each for which $8 had been promised for participation. The doctoral
scenario represents a violation of one or more ethical principles candidate’s letter further informed the subjects that his computer
(e.g., utilitarianism, rights, justice). Sixteen experts responded. All had crashed and, as a result, his list of subjects had been lost. This
of the respondents hold doctoral degrees and have an average of left him with only a paper copy of the original list of all potential
26.6 years of business ethics-related teaching experience and an subjects (from which his actual subjects had been selected). The
average of 55 ethics-related publications. The experts were given doctoral candidate’s letter continued by stating that he was keeping
a total of 13 scenarios to rate—the 8 composing the Unethical his end of the bargain by sending all of the potential subjects the
Decision Making scale (see above) plus 5 randomly interspersed money but requesting that nonsubjects return the money because
MORAL DISENGAGEMENT IN ETHICAL DECISION MAKING 391

getting money back was critical for completing the study. Thus, 4. Using a false excuse to delay taking an exam or turning
the doctoral candidate’s mailing to each of the students contained in an assignment
his letter, $8 cash, and a return-addressed envelope to be sent using
free campus mail. The return envelope and bills (a $5 and three 5. Claiming to have turned in an assignment when I have
$1s) were marked, using an ultraviolet pen, with a unique identifier not
so that the behavior (keep or return the cash) could be linked to
responses on a similar unethical decision behavioral intention 6. Taking low-cost items from a retail store
scenario (see below).
7. Taking small amounts of money from my parents’ wal-
In reality, the doctoral student had conducted no such lab study
let without their permission
and therefore none of the 60 recipients of the $8 was actually
entitled to it. The real purpose of the exercise was to see which
8. Copying from another student on a test
students would behave unethically by keeping the money. (All
subjects were subsequently thoroughly debriefed, and those who 9. Collaborating or receiving substantial help on an assign-
had returned the cash were given the $8 to keep.) This scenario ment when the instructor asked for individual work
was designed to be similar to, yet not entirely overlapping with (to
avoid cognitive priming or memory effects; Feldman & Lynch, 10. Helping someone else to cheat on a test
1988), the “keep the extra change at Starbucks” scenario from our
Unethical Decision Making measure (see Item 4 in this appendix) 11. Copying material and turning it in as your own work
so that a correlation between a stated behavioral intention and an
actual behavior could be computed. To be consistent with the 12. Asking another student who has previously taken a quiz
unethical decision scenarios (where unethical behavior was indi- or exam for the questions or the answers prior to taking
cated by responses at the high end of the scale), we coded “kept the the test
cash” in the bogus lab study as 1 and “returned the cash” as 0.
Thirty-nine of 59 recipients (one letter was returned as undeliver- 13. Changing a response after a paper or exam is returned
able) kept the cash despite the ease of returning it and the fact that and then reporting a grade error to the instructor
they had not earned it. Because neither of the variables is normally
To decrease respondents’ propensity to underreport their own
distributed, we computed a polychoric correlation coefficient be-
behavior, they were first asked to rate how frequently they ob-
tween the two variables (the behavioral intention regarding the
served others engaging in each of the 13 behaviors shown below
extra change at Starbucks and the behavioral choice regarding the
while in high school, using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1
$8 in the bogus lab exercise). At .342, this correlation coefficient
is statistically significant at p ⬍ .01. (never) to 5 (many times). They were then presented the same 13
items and asked to indicate how frequently they engaged in each
Construct Validation Exercise 3 behavior. Because 25 students were not at least age 18 at the time
of the first survey and because 2 did not provide a unique ID
On the first day of class, 85 students enrolled in a freshman required for matching the two surveys, the final sample for this
business class were invited to complete two surveys during class
validation exercise was n ⫽ 58. Estimated reliabilities for the
time. Survey 1, administered by one of the authors during Class 1,
Moral Disengagement, Unethical Decision Making, and Cheat–
included the eight scenarios composing this study’s Unethical
Lie–Steal (own behavior) scales were .81, .80, and .80, respec-
Decision Making scale (see above) and the 24 items used in the
tively.
final Moral Disengagement scale (see Appendix A). Two weeks
To assess whether reported intentions to engage in the eight
later, one author again visited the class to administer a second
unethical behaviors indicated in this study’s Unethical Decision
survey that asked students to report on their own and others’
Making scale were related to actual unethical behavior engaged in
cheating, lying, and stealing behaviors while in high school. Our
by the same respondents, we computed the correlation between the
intent in developing the Cheat–Lie–Steal scale was to collect data
Unethical Decision Making and Cheat–Lie–Steal scales (which
on multiple types of unethical behavior relevant to subjects this
age. Thus, the 13 items composing the Cheat–Lie–Steal scale had been collected with 2 weeks separation to reduce potential
(shown below) represent a combination of items drawn from the common method bias—Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff,
extant literature (4 items were used or adapted from McCabe and 2003). At r ⫽ .61 ( p ⬍ .001), results indicate that those who have
Treviño, 1993, and 3 from Daniel, Blount, and Ferrell, 1991) and actually engaged in a variety of unethical cheating, lying, and
items we developed for this study. stealing behaviors were significantly more likely to report their
intention to behave unethically on the scenarios composing this
1. Lying to my parents about my school performance study’s dependent variable.

2. Exaggerating my accomplishments on my college ap-


plication Received September 20, 2006
Revision received August 29, 2007
3. Lying about my age Accepted November 1, 2007 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like