You are on page 1of 8

Psychobiology and Behavioral Strategies

Attitudes toward Obese Individuals among


Exercise Science Students
HEATHER 0. CHAMBLISS, CARRIE E. FINLEY, and STEVEN N. BLAIR
Centersfor Integrated Health Research, The Cooper Institute, Dallas, TX

ABSTRACT
CHAMBLISS. H. O.. C. E. FINLEY, and S. N. BLAIR. Attitudes toward Obese Individuals among Exercise Science Students. Med.
Sci. Sports Exerc., Vol. 36, No. 3, pp. 468-474, 2004. Purpose: The purpose of this research was to evaluate attitudes toward obese
individuals and to identify personal characteristics associated with antifat bias among students majoring in exercise science. Methods:
Undergraduate (N = 136) and graduate (N = 110) students (mean age 23.2 yr, 55% male, 77% Caucasian) completed a series of
questionnaires to assess attitudes toward obese individuals. Instruments included the Implicit Association Test (IAT), a timed self-report
assessment that measures automatic attitudes and stereotypes toward obese persons through word categorizations (good vs bad;
motivated vs lazy), and the Antifat Attitudes Test (AFAT). a self-report instrument that measures negative beliefs and attitudes toward
obese individuals. Participants also completed a general demographic questionnaire. Results: A strong bias was found for implicit
measures including good versus bad attitude (P < 0.0001) and motivated versus lazy stereotype (P < 0.0001). Characteristics
associated with greater bad bias included being female, Caucasian, and growing up in a less populated area (P < 0.05). Belief in greater
personal responsibility for obesity was associated with stronger lazy bias (P < 0.01). On the AFAT self-report measure, belief in less
personal responsibility for obesity, positive family history of obesity, and having an obese friend were associated with lower antifat
scores (P < 0.05). Conclusion: These results suggest that students in the field of exercise science possess negative associations and
bias toward obese individuals. These findings have important implications for health promotion, as antifat bias and weight discrim-
ination among exercise professionals may further contribute to unhealthy lifestyle behaviors and reduced quality of life for many obese
individuals who are at high risk for chronic disease. Key Words: STIGMA, BIAS, DISCRIMINATION, WEIGHT, OBESITY,
HEALTH PROMOTION

T he increasing prevalence of obesity and associated


health risks have brought the issue of obesity pre-
nance of physical activity behaviors including embarrass-
ment, health concerns, disproportionate focus on weight
T vention and treatment to the forefront of the public loss, and previous negative experiences with exercise (1.9).
health agenda (19,20). Exercise has the important potential Additionally, an often-overlooked barrier to exercise for
to assist in weight management as well as alleviate many of obese children and adults is antifat bias and weight discrim-
the comorbidities associated with obesity (18). Because ination by health professionals and others (2,9).
physical inactivity and overweight are predominant among Obesity has been called the last acceptable form of prej-
American adults, these problems are primary objectives of udice, and discrimination toward obese individuals is prev-
current public health initiatives (19,20). alent throughout society including unfavorable employment
Individuals report diverse barriers to physical activity. practices, reduced education and housing opportunities, and
Common barriers include environmental factors such as negative portrayals in popular media (14). Bias is defined as
safety and proximity to facilities as well as personal factors "an inclination of temperament or outlook, especially a
such as time, self-efficacy, and enjoyment (12). Obese in- personal and sometimes unreasoned judgment" (11). Thus,
dividuals face additional barriers for adoption and mainte- antifat bias is an obesity prejudice in which the attribute of
being obese influences expectations about the individual,
often in terms of negative character assessments such as
Address for correspondence: Heather 0. Chambliss, Ph.D.. The Cooper laziness, lack of self-discipline, and incompetence (14).
Institute, 12330 Preston Road. Dallas, TX 75230: E-mail: hchambliss@ Many obese individuals give accounts of disparaging
cooperinst.org. remarks by health professionals and report discrimination
Submitted for publication January 2003. in facility and equipment access. Negative attitudes and
Accepted for publication October 2003.
stereotypes have been documented among various groups
0195-9131/04/3603-0468 of health professionals including dietitians (1 3), medical
MEDICINE & SCIENCE IN SPORTS & EXERCISE, students (22), physicians (6,8), nurses (10), and obesity
Copyright © 2004 by the American College of Sports Medicine specialists (15,16). Because antifat biases have been doc-
DOI: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000117115.94062.E4 umented among other groups of health professionals, it is
468
TABLE 1. Demographic characteristics.
Undergraduate Graduate
Men Women Men Women
Variable (N = 77) (N = 57) (N = 57) (N = 53)
Age (yr)
Mean (SD) 22.7 (2.9) 21.4 (2.9) 25.9 (4.8) 23.5 (2.4)
Range 18-37 18-33 20-41 20-30
BMI (kg-m 2)
Mean (SD) 26.3 (4.3) 23.3 (3.7) 26.3 (3.7) 23.3 (2.8)
Ethnicity [N (%)]
Caucasian 59 (76.6) 50 (87.7) 37 (66.1) 40 (78.4)
African-American 7 (9.1) 5 (8.8) 7 (12.5) 3 (5.9)
Hispanic 10 (13.0) 2 (3.5) 8 (14.3) 4 (7.8)
Other 1 (1.3) 0 (0.0) 4 (7.1) 4 (7.8)
Missing 1 2
Marital status (N(%)N
Single 59 (80.8) 49 (92.5) 35 (67.3) 42 (84.0)
Married 14 (19.2) 4 (7.6) 16 (30.8) 8 (16.0)
Divorced 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1(1.9) 0 (0.0)
Missing 4 4 5 3
Childhood environment [N (%)]
Rural area-small city 57 (74.0) 41(71.9) 34 (59.7) 38 (71.7)
Mid-sized city-large metropolitan area 20 (26.0) 16 (28.1) 23 (40.4) 15 (28.3)
Family history of obesity [N (%)]
Yes 24 (31.2) 17 (29.8) 21(36.8) 27 (50.9)
No 53 (68.8) 40 (70.2) 36 (63.2) 26 (49.1)
Personal history of obesity [N (%)]
Yes 3 (3.9) 0 (0.0) 1 (1.8) 5 (9.4)
No 74 (96.1) 57 (100.0) 56 (98.3) 48 (90.6)
Belief in personal control of obesity [N (%)]
Yes 43 (55.8) 34 (59.7) 35 (61.4) 23 (43.4)
No 34 (44.2) 23 (40.4) 22 (38.6) 30 (56.6)

possible that prejudicial attitudes toward obese individ- attributes were chosen because they represent common an-
uals also exist among students and professionals within tifat stereotypes and have been examined in prior studies
the exercise and fitness fields. Therefore, the purpose of using the IAT (15-17). Participants complete the IAT by
the present study was to evaluate attitudes toward obesity classifying words into superordinate categories. Check
among students majoring in exercise science and to ex- marks are used to classify the words into the categories
amine individual characteristics associated with antifat indicated at the top of each page (Fig. 1). Participants have
bias. We hypothesized that students would demonstrate 20 s to complete each of the IAT tasks, and each measure is
strong implicit antifat biases but would not explicitly repeated with the superordinate pairings reversed. The IAT
endorse negative attitudes and stereotypes. is then scored by subtracting the number of words correctly
classified when the term fat people is paired with the neg-
ative attributes (i.e., bad and lazy) from the number of words
METHODS
correctly classified when the term fat people is paired with
Participants. Participants were 136 undergraduate and the positive attributes (i.e., good and motivated).
110 graduate students majoring in exercise science repre- People generally find the IAT tasks easier when the
senting three colleges and universities in Texas and Ala- category pairing matches their attitude (fat people paired
bama. Approximately 55% of participants were male. The with bad or lazy vs good or motivated) and are able to
sample was predominantly Caucasian (77%) with a mean correctly classify more words within the 20-s time. Thus, a
age of 23.2 yr. Written informed consent was obtained from positive difference score indicates a stronger automatic pref-
each participant, and The Cooper Institute Institutional Re- erence for the pairing of fat people with negative attributes,
view Board approved the study. Table I presents demo- or implicit antifat bias. Unlike traditional self-report ques-
graphic information for study participants. tionnaires, the IAT measures associations and automatic
Implicit Association Test (IAT). The IAT is a timed
assessment that measures automatic associations of a target TABLE 2. Categories and associated subordinate word stimuli for Implicit
construct with particular attributes. It has been used primar- Association Test (IAT) tasks.
ily to examine social prejudice against different groups (e.g., Stimuli to Be Classified
racial stereotypes) (5). The IAT has also been used to assess Target category labels
implicit antifat bias in various populations including stu- Fat people Fat Obese Large
Thin people Slim Thin Skinny
dents and community members (17) as well as health pro- Attribute category labels (Task 1)
fessionals (15,16). In the present study, all participants were Bad Terrible Nasty Horrible
Good Wonderful Joyful Excellent
given two IAT measures to assess the attributes of bad Attribute category labels (Task 2)
versus good and lazy versus motivated with the target cat- Lazy Slow Lazy Sluggish
egories of fat people and thin people (Table 2). These Motivated Determined Motivated Eager

ATTITUDES TOWARD OBESE INDIVIDUALS Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise® 469
Pairing A Pairing B of the lecture time. Students were informed that they were
Thin People Fat People Fat People Thin People volunteering to participate in a study to examine the rela-
Motivated Lazy Motivated Lazy
tionships between individual characteristics and attitudes
/
obese obese and to examine the psychometric properties of question-
sluggish V sluggish /
V naires used in prior research. Questionnaires were coded
V slim slim /
before administration and linked by unique numbers so that
I/ eager eager no identifying information was provided on any of the
large I/ / large
questionnaires. Research packets were distributed to stu-
lazy / lazy I
dents who were instructed to complete the demographic and
fat v / fat AFAT questionnaires and then to stop and wait for direc-
V motivated / motivated tions. The IAT was then administered to the class as a whole
/ thin thin I/
using standardized instructions and timing. After complet-
/ determined determined ing the IAT, participants were asked to provide explicit
I/ skinny skinny I/
ratings for the attributes assessed in the IAT. The order of
instrument administration was selected to avoid contaminat-
FIGURE I-Sample portions of two completed IAT tasks measuring
implicit associations among fat and thin people with lazy and motivated
ing the AFAT self-report questionnaire with the experience
descriptors. Pairing A is more congruent for people who have implicit of completing the IAT. Because the IAT is a task-oriented
antifat bias compared with pairing B. Antifat bias is indicated by a measure, it is unlikely that knowledge regarding the purpose
higher number of correct responses on part A relative to part B.
of the study or completing the AFAT would unduly influ-
ence IAT responding. After the assessments were completed
preferences that exist beyond conscious evaluation, thereby and collected, students were debriefed on the purpose of the
providing a measure of bias of which people may be un- study, and the problem of antifat bias within health promo-
aware or unwilling to report (5). tion settings was discussed.
Explicit ratings. Explicit attitudes representing com- Statistical analysis. Questionnaire data were entered
mon obesity stereotypes were measured using a 7-point by machine scanning and verified. The IAT data were ex-
semantic differential scale. Participants rated their beliefs amined to identify outliers. Participants who completed
about fat people and thin people for the scale very stupid to fewer than four items per page or who had an error rate
very smart and very lazy to very motivated to yield four above 35% (i.e., incorrectly classified or missing items)
explicit ratings. Explicit scores were then calculated by were excluded. This guideline identifies people who did not
subtracting the value on the 7-point measure forfat people understand the task or were not paying attention and ensures
from the value on the scale for thin people. Thus, a score the quality of the data in a group administration. Using this
greater than zero indicated antifat bias for the measured guideline resulted in deleting 14% (35 subjects) from the
attribute (stupid, lazy). In contrast to the IAT, which mea- good/bad analysis and 6% (15 subjects) from the lazy/
sures unconscious associations, the purpose of the explicit motivated analysis. This strategy and percentage of dele-
scale is to measure attitudes directly, allowing a comparison tions is based on previous research using similar methods
between automatic or unconscious attitudes and conscious (16). Scoring of the AFAT measure followed the procedures
attitudes that people are willing to report. This explicit published by Lewis and colleagues (7) in the development
rating scale has been used as a comparison for implicit and validation of the questionnaire.
scores in other studies using the IAT (15,16). Descriptive statistics were calculated for implicit and
Antifat Attitudes Test (AFAT). The AFAT is a ques- explicit IAT scores and AFAT composite and subscale
tionnaire that measures negative attitudes toward obese in- scores. One-sample t-tests were conducted to test whether
dividuals using a traditional self-report format. The instru- the implicit and explicit IAT scores were significantly dif-
ment consists of 47 statements about fat people, and items ferent from zero, which indicates an antifat bias. Analysis of
are rated using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from strongly variance was used to assess differences in IAT and AFAT
disagree, 1, to strongly agree, 5, with some items reverse scores by several demographic characteristics including sex,
scored so that higher scores reflect greater antifat bias. The race, and education as well as social and environmental
questionnaire yields scores for three subscales including influences. Pearson correlations were used to determine the
social/character disparagement, physical/romantic unattrac- associations between implicit and explicit IAT scores and
tiveness, and weight control/blame as well as a total com- AFAT scores. All data analyses were performed using SAS
posite score (7). software, Version 8. All reported P values are two-tailed.
Demographic questionnaire. The demographic Sample size provided a statistical power of 0.80 for detect-
questionnaire documented individual characteristics includ- ing moderate effect sizes at an alpha of P < 0.05 for
ing age, sex, race, and education, as well as social and academic level, gender, and collapsed race for IAT analyses.
environmental influences such as personal experience with
obesity, family history of obesity, and beliefs about personal
RESULTS
responsibility for obesity.
Procedure. The instruments were administered to Implicit attitudes and beliefs. There was a signifi-
groups of exercise science students in the classroom as part cant antifat bias on both attribute categories, bad/good
470 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine http:Hwww.acsm-msse.org
C positive attribute greater personal control of obesity (F( 1,24 2) = 7.3, P =
35 l negative attribute
0.007) and lack of family history of obesity (F(1,2 4 2 ) = 8.7,
30 P = 0.004) were associated with higher antifat composite
scores as well as higher scores on the social/character dis-
t 25 paragement (F(I 242) = 9.5, P = 0.002 and F (1.242) = 7.2, P
= 0.008, respectively) and weight control/blame subscales
00 20
(F(1,242) = 3.8, P = 0.0524 and F(1,242 ) = 5.5, P = 0.002,
0
. 15 respectively). Individuals reporting no obese friends had
E higher scores on the composite score and all subscales than
Z 10 individuals reporting having obese friends (social/character:
F( 242) = 5.9, P = 0.02; physical/romantic: F(L2 42 ) = 5.7 P
5 = 0.02; weight control/blame: F(,, 2 42 ) = 7.2, P = 0.008;
o - I
total score: F(,, 24 2 ) = 7.8, P = 0.0064). Being an ethnic
PC .0001
Good Bad Motivated Lazy
minority was associated with less antifat bias on the phys-
ical/romantic unattractiveness subscale (F (1.212) = 5.2, P =
FIGURE 2-Number of IAT items correctly classiried when the fat 0.02). Eleven AFAT items had a mean score >3.0 for at
people target category was paired with positive and negative attributes.
least one comparison group, indicating antifat bias (Table
4). No statistically significant relationships were observed
between AFAT scores and BMI (P > 0.29) or implicit
(mean = 11.8, SD = 6.5, t(2 1 0 ) 26.28, P < 0.0001) and
- attitudes (P > 0.11).
laz)y¼lnotivated(mean = 9.2, SD 6.1, t(230), P < 0.0001).
-

The numbers of items correctly classified in each IAT


DISCUSSION
condition are presented in Figure 2. This illustrates the
discrepancy between the numbers of items correctly classi- The exercise science students surveyed in the present
fied when fat people was combined with each adjective, study exhibited strong implicit antifat bias, an effect that has
with higher scores indicating a stronger antifat bias. A been observed among other groups of health professionals
moderate positive correlation was observed between the two (15,16). In the present study, being Caucasian was associ-
implicit attribute categories (r = 0.52, P < 0.001). When the ated with more negative attitudes on the implicit good/bad
influence of individual characteristics was examined, measure when compared with non-Caucasian participants.
women had stronger implicit bias on the good/bad measure However, one limitation of the study is that the sample is
(F (1.207) = 4.8. P = 0.03) but not on the lazy/motivated predominately Caucasian, and we combined minority
measure when compared with men. Being Caucasian or groups for analyses. Therefore, we are unable to determine
growing up in a more rural environment was also associated how antifat bias differs across various ethnic groups within
with more negative attitudes on the good/bad measure the population of exercise science students. Women and
(F( 1 203) = 6.5, P = 0.01 and F(, 209 ) = 7.1, P = 0.008, individuals growing up in a more rural environment exhib-
respectively). Belief in greater personal control of obesity ited greater antifat bias for the goodlbad global attribute,
was associated with antifat bias for the lazy stereotype whereas people who endorsed a belief in greater personal
(F(, 22 9 ) = 7.9, P = 0.006). A small but significant corre- control of obesity exhibited greater bias on the lazy stereo-
lation was observed between implicit bias on the good/bad type. The reasons for these findings are unknown, but it may
attribute and BMI, with individuals with a lower BMI hav- be that implicit biases are susceptible to specific repeated
ing higher bias scores (r = -0.19, P = 0.006). associations over time. As defined by Greenwald and Banaji
Explicit attitudes. Compared with thin people,fatpeo- (4), an implicit attitude is the unconscious trace of past
ple were rated higher on the lazy attribute (mean = 1.30, SD experience or prior exposure that influences responses.
= 1.4, t(2 3 6 ) = 14.0, P < 0.0001), but there was no signif- Thus, individuals may hold stronger implicit biases for
icant antifat bias for the very smart to very stupid attitude particular attributes based on personal experience.
scale (t(2 3 6 ) = -1.3; P = 0.19). A significant correlation As expected, participants did not exhibit high overall bias
was found between the lazy1¼notivatedimplicit measure and scores on the explicit measure or traditional self-report
the very motivated to very lazy explicit scale (r = -0.20, P questionnaire (AFAT). However, certain antifat beliefs and
= 0.003), but other relationships to implicit measures were stereotypes were endorsed, most often in the area of phys-
not significant (P > 0.44). Explicit attitudes for the lazy ical unattractiveness and weight blame. Of particular inter-
attribute were positively correlated to composite and sub- est for this selected group of exercise science students are
scale AFAT scores (composite: r = 0.39; social/character: r the negative attitudes that relate to lifestyle behaviors in-
- 0.32; physical/romantic: r = 0.36: weight control/blame: cluding assumptions regarding junk food, control of weight
r = 0.42; P < 0.0001 for all). No significant correlations were loss, and physical coordination. Participants were also more
observed between explicit scores for the stupid attribute and willing to endorse a lazy stereotype in the explicit ratings.
AFAT composite or subscale AFAT scores (P > 0.07). These types of prejudicial attitudes have potential to inhibit
Antifat attitudes test. Mean item scores for AFAT the effectiveness of lifestyle counseling and wellness activ-
subscales and composite are presented in Table 3. Belief in ities provided by health and exercise science professionals.
ATTITUDES TOWARD OBESE INDIVIDUALS Medicine & Science in Sports &Exercises 471
TABLE 3. Comparison of Antifat Attitudes Test (AFAT) scores of undergraduate and graduate students by gender.
Undergraduate Sludents Graduate Students PUG vs G PUG vs G PUG vs G
AFAT Scales Men Women P Men Women P (Men) (Women) (All)
I. Social/Character Disparagement (15 items)
Mean 1.85 1.69 0.11 1.65 1.59 0.62 0.06 0.31 0.03
(SD) (0.59) (0.58) (0.66) (0.43)
II. Physical/Romantic Unattractiveness (10 items)
Mean 3.01 2.75 0.02 2.74 2.77 0.80 0.03 0.84 0.10
SD (0.61) (0.69) (0.82) (0.57)
Ill. Weight Control/Blame (9 items)
Mean 2.91 2.72 0.10 2.72 2.62 0.46 0.13 0.40 0.08
SD (0.59) (0.68) (0.82) (0.64)
Composite Score (47 items)
Mean 2.40 2.20 0.04 2.19 2.12 0.52 0.05 0.44 0.04
SD (0.52) (0.58) (0.68) (0.46)
UG, undergraduate; G, graduate.

Belief in greater personal control of obesity and being a in terms of access to facilities and the professional-client
male undergraduate student were associated with higher relationship. For example, a study examining physician at-
AFAT scores. In addition, individuals without family his- titudes toward case reports of patients differing only in
tory of obesity or reporting no obese friends had higher weight found that physicians reported that they would feel
antifat scores. Thus, personal experience with friends and more negatively toward overweight "patients," would spend
family who are obese may lessen negative attitudes, and it less time with them, but would order more tests (6). Facility
may be that efforts to enhance sensitivity and understanding and equipment access is another area in which obese pa-
among health and fitness professionals may help reduce bias tients face discrimination in general health care, as standard
toward obese individuals. blood pressure cuffs, hospital gowns, and wheelchairs are
Our results are consistent with previous research that has often too small to accommodate larger individuals.
demonstrated negative attitudes toward obese individuals Antifat bias and weight discrimination by health profes-
among health professionals. Two recent studies adminis- sionals may, in turn, result in a decrease in utilization of
tered the IAT to groups of health professionals who treat
health and weliness services. For example, obese women
obesity and found strong implicit negative attitudes (good!
have been found to be less likely to seek breast and gyne-
bad) and stereotypes (motivated/lazy) associated with obese
cological screening and exams relative to normal weight
persons (15,16). Similarly, a survey of family practice phy-
women (3,21), and part of this effect may be due to attitudes
sicians found that a significant number of physicians held
projected by health care professionals. It is likely that the
negative beliefs toward obese persons and described obese
patients as lazy, sad, and lacking self-control (8). wellness field parallels other segments of healthcare. How-
Stigmatization and discrimination toward obese persons ever, further research is needed to determine the extent to
within the health community may negatively affect quality which implicit antifat biases held by fitness professionals
of life for many obese individuals in terms of psychosocial may influence adoption and maintenance of physical activ-
effects, reduced quality of care, and decreased utilization of ity. For example, a recent study examined weight criticism
services including wellness activities. It is unknown whether during physical activity among schoolchildren and found
the implicit antifat bias observed in the present study trans- that children who reported greater weight criticism also
lated to discriminatory behavior, as behavior was not mea- reported less sports enjoyment compared with peers (2). To
sured. However, reports from other health professions indi- our knowledge, no studies have examined the impact of
cate that antifat bias can result in differential treatment, both antifat bias and weight discrimination on exercise percep-

TABLE 4. Antifat Attitudes Test (AFAT) items with a mean rating indicating antifat bias.
Overall Undergraduate Graduate
Sample Men Women Men Women
Antifat Attitudes Test Item (N = 244) (N = 77) (N = 57) (N = 57) (N = 53)
There's no excuse for being fat. 3.05 (1.03) 3.10 (0.97) 3.11 (0.90) 3.07 (1.27) 2.89 (0.99)
If I were single, I would date a fat person.' 3.83 (1.02) 3.96 (1.06) 3.56 (1.02) 3.93 (1.05) 3.83 (0.91)
Jokes about fat people are funny. 2.55 (1.21) 3.06 (1.21) 2.35 (1.09) 2.63 (1.14) 1.94 (1.10)
Most fat people buy too much junk food. 3.33 (0.96) 3.55 (0.85) 3.35 (0.94) 3.25 (1.15) 3.09 (0.88)
Fat people are physically unattractive. 3.22 (1.16) 3.36 (1.18) 2.91 (1.07) 3.32 (1.15) 3.25 (1.19)
Fat people shouldn't wear revealing clothes in public. 4.05 (1.16) 4.09 (1.14) 4.30 (0.93) 3.68 (1.36) 4.11 (1.12)
If fat people really wanted to lose weight they could. 3.62 (1.14) 3.81 (1.00) 3.58 (1.24) 3.51 (1.35) 3.53 (0.95)
The existence of organizations to lobby for the rights of fat people in our society isa good idea.* 2.97 (1.13) 3.04 (0.95) 3.02 (1.04) 3.02 (1.45) 2.75 (1.07)
I don't understand how someone could be sexually attracted to a fat person. 2.77 (1.25) 3.04 (1.14) 2.68 (1.20) 2.44 (1.38) 2.83 (1.25)
People who are fat have as much physical coordination as anyone.* 3.25 (1.23) 3.51 (1.14) 3.19 (1.27) 3.21 (1.41) 2.98 (1.03)
Fat people should be encouraged to accept themselves the way they are.* 2.75 (1.17) 3.01 (1.06) 2.75 (1.18) 2.58 (1.41) 2.57 (0.97)
Values are means (SD).
* Item reverse scored.
Item scores > 3.0 indicate antifat bias.

472 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine http://vvvvvv.acsm-msse.org


tions and participation in obese adults, representing an im- in this population. In terms of education and certification,
portant next step for future research. courses should address both client-centered strategies to pro-
Given the prevalence of obesity and the current initiatives mote empathy as well as practical recommendations to accom-
on obesity prevention and treatment, the problem of antifat modate potential health limitations and physical needs of spe-
bias and weight discrimination within health promotion cial populations, including large individuals. On a basic level,
settings warrants focused attention. Individuals who work in the knowledge that automatic antifat biases and unconscious
health and fitness fields are exposed to the same cultural stereotypes exist can help professionals become aware of their
messages as the rest of society, but additional factors may own predispositions toward obese individuals. These effects
also contribute to negative perceptions and stereotypes. Stu- may be most pronounced among students, where professional
dents pursuing an exercise science degree are often drawn to inexperience may further contribute to misconceptions.
the field because of an interest in athleticism, health, and The present study has several limitations. First, the sam-
physical function, which may contribute to a view of obesity ple included a limited number of students from a few ex-
as unacceptable. In addition, some of the academic curricula ercise science programs. We cannot determine how repre-
in the field of exercise science with an emphasis on health sentative this sample is compared with other students
and ideal body weight may contribute to automatic negative majoring in exercise science, and results pertaining to spe-
associations with obesity, thereby extending to obese people cific sample characteristics should be interpreted with cau-
in general. Health and fitness professionals are keenly aware tion given the small sample size in these analyses. Second,
of the health risks associated with obesity, and it is unknown we did not measure discriminatory behavior, and it is un-
how that knowledge and the observance of medical comor- known how implicit antifat bias might influence interper-
bidities influence personal attitudes and beliefs toward in- sonal interactions with obese individuals within exercise
dividuals who are overweight or obese. Furthermore, little settings. Finally, we did not conduct follow-up assessments
formal instruction in degree or continuing education pro- to determine whether consciousness raising by research
grams is available to provide training in physical activity participation attenuated antifat bias.
promotion and exercise prescription for obese individuals. It is important that fitness and wellness services are
Consciousness raising has been suggested as a strategy available, accessible, and acceptable to obese persons.
for avoiding discrimination and reducing implicit bias (4). However, antifat bias and weight discrimination among
Thus, bringing attention to the existence of antifat bias and exercise professionals may serve as barriers for physical
weight discrimination in the area of exercise and wellness activity participation for some obese individuals. Therefore,
may help improve access to physical activity and quality of programs involved in the training of health and fitness
services provided by fitness professionals. For example, the professionals should consider raising awareness of antifat
International Council on Active Aging recently issued a bias and weight discrimination in health and wellness set-
checklist to rate fitness facilities on "age-friendliness," tings to promote empathy among professionals and better
which addresses both the emotional and physical needs of care for all individuals.
older individuals participating in physical activity (http:H/
www.icaa.cc/Facilitylayouts/ICAA%2OFacility%2OTest.pdf). We thank the Rudd Institute for funding this research and Melba
Similar methods initiated by professional organizations could Morrow for her assistance in preparing the manuscript. We also
appreciate the time and participation of the students and instructors
be used to identify the needs of obese individuals and create at the participating institutions.
healthy and friendly environments to promote physical activity Dr. Blair is a consultant for Jenny Craig International.

REFERENCES
I. BALL, K., D. CRAWFORD, and N. OWEN. Too fat to exercise? 8. LooMis, G. A., K. P. CONNOLLY, C. R. CLINCH, and D. A. DIURIC.
Obesity as a barrier to physical activity. Aust. N. Z J. Public Attitudes and practices of military family physicians regarding
Healtli 24:331-333, 2000. obesity. Mil Med. 166:121-125, 2001.
2. FAITH, M. S., M. A. LEONE, T. S. AYERS, M. HEO, and A. PIETRO- 9. LYONS, P., and W. C. MILLER. Effective health promotion and clinical
EELLI. Weight criticism during physical activity, coping skills, and care for large people. Med. Sci. Sports E&erc. 31:1141-1146, 1999.
reported physical activity in children. Pediatrics I 10:e23, 2002. 10. MARO.NEY. D.. and S. GOLUB. Nurses' attitudes toward obese persons
3. FONTAINE, K. R., M. S. FAITH, D. B. ALLISON, and L. J. CHESKIN. and certain ethnic groups. Percept. Mot. Skills 75:387-391, 1992.
Body weight and health care among women in the general popu- 11. Merriam-Webster Dictionary [On-line]. Merriam-Webster, 2003.
lation. Arch. Fam. Med. 7:381-384, 1998. 12. NAPOLITANO, M. A.. and B. H. MARCUS. Breaking barriers to
4. GREENWALD, A. G., and M. R. BANAJI. Implicit social cognition: increased physical activity. Physician Sportsmed. 28:88-93, 2000.
attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psvchol. Rev. 102:4-27, 13. OBERRIEDER, H., R. WALKER, D. MONROE, and M. ADEYANJu. At-
1995. titude of dietetics students and registered dieticians toward obe-
5. GREENWALD, A. G., D. E. McGHEE, and J. L. SCHWARTZ. Measuring sity. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 95:914-916. 1995.
individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit associa- 14. PUHL, R., and K. D. BROWNELL. Bias, discrimination and obesity.
tion test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74:1464-1480, 1998. Obes. Res. 9:788-805, 2001.
6. HEEL. M. R. and J. Xu. Weighing the care: physicians' reactions 15. SCHWARTZ, M. B., H. 0. CHAMBLISS, K. D. BROWNELL, S. N. BLAIR,
to the size of a patient. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 25: and C. BILLINGTON. Weight bias among health professionals spe-
1246-1252. cializing in obesity. Obes. Res. 11:1033-1039, 2003.
7. LEWis, R. J., T. J. CASH, L. JACOBI, and C. BUBB-LEWIS. Prejudice 16. TEACHMAN, B. A., and K. D. BROWNELL. Implicit anti-fat bias
toward fat people: the development and validation of the Antifat among health professionals: is anyone immune? Int. J. Obes.
Attitudes Test. Obes. Res. 5:297-307, 1997. Relat. Metab. Disord. 25:1525-1531, 2001.

ATTITUDES TOWARD OBESE INDIVIDUALS Medicine &Science in Sports &Exercises 473


17. TEACHMAN, B. A., K. D. GAPINSKI, K. D., BROWNELL, and M. 20. U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES. The Surgeon
RAWLINS.Demonstrations of implicit anti-fat bias: the impact of General's call to action to prevent and decrease overweight and
providing causal information and evoking empathy. Health Psy- obesity. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human
chol. 22:68-78, 2003. Services. Public Health Service, Office of the Surgeon General,
18. U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES. Physical 2001, pp. 1-39.
Activity Fundamental to Preventing Disease. Washington, DC: 21. WEE, C. C., E. P. MCCARTHY. R. B. DAVIS, and R. S. PHILLIPS.
U. S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2002, pp. Screening for cervical and breast cancer: is obesity an unrecognized
1-19. barrier to preventive care? Ann. /Intent. Med. 132:697-704, 2000.
19. U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES. Healthy People 22. WIGTON, R. S., and W. C. McGAGHIE. The effect of obesity on
2010, 2nd Ed., Vol 1. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing medical students' approach to patients with abdominal pain.
Office, 2000, pp. 28-29. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 16:262-265, 2001.

474 Official Joumal of the American College of Sports Medicine hftp:Hwww.acsm-msse.org


COPYRIGHT INFORMATION

TITLE: Attitudes toward Obese Individuals among Exercise


Science Students
SOURCE: Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 36 no3 Mr
2004
PAGE(S): 468-74
WN: 0406201727016

The magazine publisher is the copyright holder of this article and it


is reproduced with permission. Further reproduction of this article in
violation of the copyright is prohibited.

Copyright 1982-2004 The H.W. Wilson Company. All rights reserved.

You might also like