You are on page 1of 16

European Journal of Personality, Eur. J. Pers.

(2018)
Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.2134

A Five-Factor Theory Perspective on Causal Analysis

ROBERT R. MCCRAE1* and ANGELINA R. SUTIN2


1
Gloucester, MA USA
2
College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL USA

Abstract: Five-Factor Theory provides a broad but largely blank template for causal personality research. Within
Five-Factor Theory, there are three major categories of questions: (1) how do biological structures and functions lead
to trait levels? (2) how do traits and the environment give rise to acquired psychological institutions? and (3) how do
personality characteristics interact with specific situations to determine behaviours and reactions? Both practical and
ethical issues complicate the search for the causes of trait change. Causal explanations of the development of
characteristic adaptations are likely to be incomplete, because there are many different ways in which the same
adaptation may be acquired. Studies of the determinants of behaviour are usually left to social, educational, or clin-
ical psychologists—although personality psychologists may make distinctive contributions by emphasizing the role of
the individual in selecting and creating situations. A causal understanding of the functioning of the personality system
is possible through the integration of many lines of evidence, but it is likely to take a very long time. In the meanwhile,
personality psychologists may fruitfully pursue the identification of practical causes by which individuals with a given
set of traits can optimize their adaptation. Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology

Key words: development of personality; personality scales and inventories; philosophy of science

If we require truth in any strict sense, we must confine ourselves widely used because it is often an effective treatment for se-
to one entire state of the world. This will be the cause, and the next rious and refractory psychiatric conditions and thus qualifies
entire state will be the effect. There is much truth in this conclu- as a practical cause of improved mental health. But, as Prudic
sion, but it remains indefensible. and Duan (2017) euphemistically noted, ‘the mechanism of
F. H. Bradley, 1893/1966, p. 48
action of ECT remains an active area of hypothesis and re-
Psychologists concerned with causality ought perhaps to search’ (p. 3280)—that is, we have basically no idea how it
be required to read the brief chapter on ‘Causation’ in works.
Bradley’s Appearance and reality to have some appreciation Ideally, of course, the two kinds of causes are closely
of the complexities of the topic when approached rigorously. intertwined: explanatory causes should guide us to practical
Even in that simpler time, before the advent of relativity and interventions, and (in the hypothetico-deductive method)
quantum mechanics, causality posed serious issues of conti- the success of a practical cause (the experimental treatment)
nuity and change, infinite regress, and the role of what is taken as evidence for the validity of the explanation. It is
Bradley called ‘background’ and we would call context.1 perhaps less widely appreciated that the relation between ex-
Psychologists, and most other empirical scientists, have a planation and practice can be and often is extremely loose,
more pragmatic view of causation. Basically, they recognize especially in the early stages of a science. We could, for ex-
two kinds of causes: we will call them explanatory if they ample, provide a causal explanation for the efficacy of ECT:
provide a conceptual account of why a phenomenon occurs ‘Psychiatric disorders are dysfunctions of the brain; ECT af-
and practical if one can use them to make the phenomenon fects the brain, and somehow improves the situation.’
happen. These two are essentially independent. Five-Factor Readers who think such an explanation is completely mean-
Theory (FFT; McCrae & Costa, 2008) proposes an explana- ingless and empty should recall that in ECT the electric cur-
tory cause of lifespan trait development—intrinsic matura- rent is directed through the brain, not the liver. Trait
tion—but says nothing about how development can be explanations of behaviour are often loose as well as distal,
accelerated or modified. Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) is but this does not disqualify traits as real causes (McCrae &
Costa, 1995).
Statisticians have provided psychologists with sophisti-
cated tools for the quantitative evaluation of causal models
*Correspondence to: Robert R. McCrae, 90 Magnolia Avenue, Gloucester, (Pearl, 2009), but the value of these models depends entirely
MA 01930, USA. on the causal assumptions built into them. These assump-
E-mail: rrmccrae@gmail.com
1
For a more contemporary example of the complexities of causal analysis,
tions should ‘derive from prior studies, research design, sci-
readers might consider the discussion of functional explanation in the Inter- entific judgment, or other justifying sources’ (Bollen &
net Encyclopedia of Philosophy, http://www.iep.utm.edu/func-exp/#H2. Pearl, 2013, p. 307). Theories like FFT can be used as a

Handling editor: René Mõttus


Received 6 September 2017
Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Revised 6 November 2017, Accepted 6 November 2017
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

justifying source of causal assumptions if, in the judgment of height and blood type and eye colour—are innate and rela-
the scientist, the theories are plausible. tively enduring human potentials that vary quantitatively
across individuals, but are qualitatively universal.2
The distinction between BTs and CAs is easier to articu-
TRAITS IN FIVE-FACTOR THEORY late than to operationalize. There can be no direct measure-
ment of an abstract potential; traits and other BTs can only
Five-Factor Theory offers a framework for causal explana- be inferred from their consequences. The consequences of
tions in personality psychology, but does so in very general traits are CAs and, indirectly, the behaviours and experiences
terms. Biology causes basic tendencies (BTs—including per- that are generated by CAs. Extraversion, for example, might
sonality traits at all levels of the trait hierarchy); BTs in inter- show the following manifestations:
action with external influences (EIs) cause characteristic
• smiles at strangers,
adaptations (CAs—including habits, skills, and beliefs);
• vacations at popular beaches,
CAs in interaction with EIs cause behaviours and reactions.
• leads groups,
These assertions are so broad that they may at first seem
• acts energetically,
trivial. What merit FFT has depends on the conceptual value
• listens to loud music, and
of the distinction between BTs and CAs and the heuristic
• laughs often.
value of emphasizing certain causal pathways to the exclu-
sion of others. We first consider the conceptual distinction; One could assess such characteristics in several ways: by
in later sections, we will focus on the causal pathways direct surveillance, daily diary entries of the target, self-
allowed by FFT. reports, and observer ratings. Personality questionnaires
employing self-reports or informant ratings are the default
method in contemporary research, and although they are far
Traits as basic tendencies
from perfect, they both offer serviceable tools for trait assess-
Basic tendencies are abstract potentials, hypothetical psycho- ment. Such instruments essentially ask the respondent to es-
logical features of the individual that, over time and in spe- timate the frequency or intensity with which the target
cific situations, come to be manifested in concrete engages in the behaviour, relative to some implicit norm.
realizations, CAs. BTs include more than personality traits, Despite the vagueness of a statement such as ‘I [strongly
and the notion is perhaps more easily grasped in other areas. agree that, compared to other people, I] often lead groups’,
All healthy infants, for example, have a capacity for language scales composed of such items form reasonably valid trait
(BT); over time, they develop fluency in their native tongue measures (Funder, 1989).
(CA). Intelligence (BT) is a hypothetical aptitude for learning Note that such scales are implicitly adopted by FFT as
that can only be discerned when education leads to acquired operationalizations of traits. One essential rationale for
knowledge (CA). Musical genius (BT) must be inferred from FFT is the universality, stability, and heritability of traits,
the prodigious rate at which a child masters an instrument and the empirical basis for asserting those properties is re-
(CA). The central premise of FFT is that personality traits search conducted with self-reports and observer ratings on
are BTs: people low in Agreeableness (BT) develop sceptical personality scales. If BTs could not be operationalized by
attitudes (CAs); those low in Conscientiousness learn tricks personality scales, FFT would collapse. But they can: if
to get by with minimal effort. there is a trait (or a certain degree of a trait), then it would
There are many other ways of defining ‘trait’ (McCrae, in likely be manifest in particular CAs or behaviours that func-
press). Some theorists see traits as observable patterns of be- tion as what Tellegen (1991) called trait indicators. By in-
haviour (Pervin, 1994), some as neuropsychic structures that ductive reasoning, people who exhibit such trait indicators
are shaped by experience (Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & are likely to have the trait.
Wetzel, 1994), and some as distributions of personality states The difficulty—there are always difficulties with induc-
(Fleeson, 2001). FFT views traits as BTs because that pre- tive reasoning—is that trait indicators are inherently ambig-
mise allows causal explanations for a number of well- uous. The questions in a personality scale do not ask directly
replicated observations that are not easily accommodated about underlying dispositions, but about beliefs, values, be-
by other definitions. If traits were simply patterns of behav- haviours, and so on, and these may have many different
iour, we would expect them to vary widely across cultures causes. Why might one smile at strangers if one is not an ex-
(where behaviours surely vary), yet traits are universal travert? It might be the disarming defensive strategy of a
(McCrae, Terracciano, & Members of the Personality Pro- timid person, or a Boy Scout’s conscientious attempt to be
files of Cultures Project, 2005). Experience encoded in the friendly, as the Scout Law requires, or a politician’s ploy
brain may lead to new habits, interests, or relationships, but to win votes. Beyond the fact that a particular CA may re-
traits endure over decades (Ferguson, 2010) despite changing flect different traits (e.g. Extraversion, or timidity, or Con-
life circumstances. Distributions of personality states are sta- scientiousness, or ambition), it may also be chiefly the
tistical summaries of behaviour, and it is not immediately
2
clear why they would be similar in monozygotic (MZ) A study of a preliterate culture in Bolivia (Gurven, Von Rueden,
twins—yet traits are heritable (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Massenkoff, Kaplan, & Lero Vie, 2013) may suggest limits to the claim of
universality of personality structure, or it may simply suggest limits to the
Riemann, & Livesley, 1998). We can explain the universality, use of self-report personality questionnaires as a means of assessing
stability, and heritability of traits by assuming that they—like personality.

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

product of the environment. Smiling at strangers may be a in comparison with multi-item scales are twofold: first, even
general cultural norm or a form of courtesy instilled by if they are conceptually purer, single global items are prone
one’s mother. to considerable random error of measurement in comparison
Good personality items—the kinds that survive rigorous with multi-item scales; and second, the implicit ‘items’ that
item analyses (Clark & Watson, 1995)—have a high likeli- underlie a global trait rating are unknown and likely to vary
hood of actually indicating the intended trait and a lower widely across different respondents. (It is, of course, an em-
likelihood of indicating something else. But items are never pirical question whether and to what degree these disadvan-
perfect. tages limit the criterion-related validity of global trait
assessments.)
The Structure Postulate of FFT states that ‘Traits are or-
The duality principle
ganized hierarchically from narrow and specific to broad
Conceptually, there is another issue. Personality items inevi- and general dispositions’ (McCrae & Costa, 1996, p. 72).
tably have a dual nature: they are simultaneously instantia- When that postulate was first formulated, a two-level hierar-
tions of both BTs and CAs, something that FFT calls the chy of domains and facets was the focus of attention; now,
duality principle (Costa & McCrae, 2017). A fondness for there are levels between these two (DeYoung, Quilty, &
heavy metal music is at once an acquired taste and (perhaps) Peterson, 2007), above domains (Digman, 1997) and below
a manifestation of Excitement Seeking. Psychologists are fa- facets (McCrae, 2015). At the lowest level are nuances,
miliar with this notion of duality in studies of intelligence. which correspond roughly to single items. Mõttus, Kandler,
Each of the items on a vocabulary test assesses specific Bleidorn, Riemann, and McCrae (2017) showed that most
knowledge that must have been acquired through formal of the items of the NEO Inventories function as traits: they
education or informal exposure, yet scores on the test are ex- are consensually valid, longitudinally stable, and substan-
cellent indicators of general (and heritable) intellectual tially heritable. This is true not only of the items themselves
ability. Is a vocabulary test an aptitude test or an achievement (which is unsurprising, because they must have those proper-
test? Clearly, it is both; FFT asserts that something similar is ties to serve as valid indicators of facets and domains) but
true of personality scales. also of the residual variance specific to each item when var-
Some readers might imagine that the problem of duality iance due to facets and domains has been statistically re-
can be avoided by items that ask about tendencies rather moved. There is some meaning peculiar to each individual
than their manifestations. Is not the item ‘I am very extra- item that corresponds to a specific BT; the biological basis
verted’ a pure measure of an underlying disposition? On of traits must be exquisitely complex.
the surface it is, but in fact it is only duality once removed. It is clear that the problem of duality is most acute for nu-
If we asked a respondent what she meant by ‘I am very ex- ances. At this level of the hierarchy, the trait is essentially
traverted’, she might say, ‘Oh, you know, I have lots of isomorphic with its manifestation, and this poses special
friends, and I’m fun-loving, and I like to keep active, and I problems for causal analysis. Suppose, for example, there is
talk a lot, and …’ (she does talk a lot). As this example illus- a nuance of the Angry Hostility facet assessed by the item,
trates, people learn the meaning of trait terms, including ‘I lose my temper a lot.’ If we identified a set of individuals
their indicators, as part of basic language acquisition; and who scored high on this item and trained them to count to 10
they learn which terms apply to them by reflecting on their before responding to a provocation, we might find that they
own patterns of thinking, feeling, and behaving. They incor- subsequently scored lower on that item. If we wished to
porate their conclusions into their self-concept (an important know if we had altered the Angry Hostility trait, we should
CA) and draw on it when asked to describe themselves. At examine their responses to other items in the facet scale
least according to FFT, people have no direct, intuitive ap- (cf. Nicholls, Licht, & Pearl, 1982), dealing perhaps with bit-
prehension of their inner nature; they discover it only terness or with disgust at incompetent people. If these other
through experience. items also showed a reduction (and if it endured for months
There is, however, one way in which one can approxi- or years, and if it was corroborated by independent ob-
mate a pure measure of BTs: by aggregating over a wide servers), we could argue that our intervention had success-
range of trait indicators. Each item in a scale may be about fully caused the facet to change (cf. Mõttus, 2016). But
some particular habit or value or belief; but if many different suppose the other items did not change; could we sustain
items are summed, the conceptual interpretation of the sum the narrower claim that we had at least changed the BT cor-
must become more general. Just as statistical error variance responding to the quick temper nuance? Or had we merely
tends to cancel out when items are summed to form a scale, changed the CA used to assess the nuance by instilling a
leaving a higher proportion of true score variance, so specific new habitual response to provocation?
CA meanings cancel each other, leaving a purer indicator of Nuances, like facets and domains, are transcontextual
the underlying trait. traits (McCrae & Costa, 1984), so it should in principle be
One might argue that global trait assessments (e.g. ‘I am possible to identify different forms in which they are mani-
very extraverted’) implicitly aggregate over a range of spe- fested. Perhaps the item ‘I get really frustrated when things
cific indicators (e.g. number of friends, enjoyment of fun, ac- don’t work’ taps the same nuance as ‘I lose my temper a
tivity, and talkativeness) and thus offer purer trait lot.’ If so, it could be used as a test of the hypothesis that
assessments than more behavioural items. However, from a our intervention had altered the quick temper nuance. Of
psychometric perspective, the disadvantages of global ratings course, identifying a set of alternative items to assess a

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

nuance to allow such causal tests would require a programme environment jointly lead to behaviours. All high school
of scale development and validation as for any other trait. students might be required to take the same courses, but
Fortunately, most personality psychologists are more con- what and how much they learn, and whether they develop
cerned with broader traits. new interests, will be (in part) a reflection of their abilities
The conceptual distinction between BTs and CAs is fun- and traits. Without exposure to, say, Byzantine history,
damental for FFT. It provides a basis for asserting that man- few students would ever develop an interest in the topic;
ifestations that change over time, or vary across situations, but without a native curiosity, even students exposed to
can be indicators of enduring and transcontextual traits. the topic would probably remain indifferent. The develop-
The value of this distinction has been implicitly acknowl- ment of CAs requires both traits and experiences—both
edged by other theorists (e.g. Church, 2017; McAdams & motive and opportunity, as it were. The relative impor-
Pals, 2006), who have adopted the term characteristic adap- tance of these two contributing causes may vary. Every
tations to refer to acquired features that express the individ- child learns the times tables by rote and willy-nilly; ele-
ual’s personality. mentary education is a strong situation. Conversely, math-
ematically gifted children (like Gauss) may teach
themselves even when opportunities for formal education
CAUSAL PATHWAYS IN FIVE-FACTOR THEORY are limited.
• According to FFT, the major personality processes are uni-
The personality system in operation directional, with a clear distinction between causes and ef-
fects. Behaviours do not create CAs, and CAs do not
Figure 1 provides a simplified version of the personality sys-
modify traits. One apparent exception to this is indicated
tem as portrayed by FFT. The present article focuses on the
by the curved arrow in Figure 1, pointing from CAs to
solid arrows, which represent dynamic processes and indi-
CAs, and identified as an accommodative process. This ar-
cate the causal pathways specified by the theory. Several
row refers to the obvious fact that CAs build on previously
points need to be made about these processes.
established CAs. A new exercise is added to an established
• The most important pathways (there are other pathways routine; the role of assistant professor segues into associ-
not discussed here) fall into three categories: developmen- ate; knowledge of trigonometry is a prerequisite for learn-
tal, accommodative, and assimilative (Figure 1). Develop- ing the calculus. But the causal ordering is still basically
mental processes give rise to BTs, accommodative linear, provided one distinguishes between CA1 (the
processes to CAs, and assimilative processes to behaviour cause) and CA2 (the effect).
and experience and, over time, to the cumulative objective • Crucially, the arrows in Figure 1 refer collectively to all
biography. the processes that operate on a given pathway, of which
• These three operate on different time scales. Developmen- there are presumably a very large number (McCrae,
tal processes often require years; accommodative pro- 2016). In part, this is because there are many quite differ-
cesses may take days or months; and assimilative ent sorts of CAs. The processes needed to learn knitting
processes occur in the moment. are clearly different from those involved in bonding with
• Accommodative and assimilative processes are inherently a new pet or forming an opinion about legalizing mari-
interactive. That is, both traits and the environment jointly juana. Further, for any given CA, there are often a wide va-
lead to the emergence of CAs, and both CAs and the riety of different causal routes: one may learn knitting by

Figure 1. A simplified representation of the Five-Factor Theory personality system. Core components are in rectangles; interfacing components are in ellipses;
solid arrows represent causal pathways on which dynamic processes operate. Adapted from McCrae & Costa, 1996.

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

observing one’s grandmother, or reading a book, or attend- To the extent that BTs are underlying causes of CAs, FFT
ing a class at the Senior Center; the physicist P. A. M. Di- must be described as a correspondence model; the observed
rac is alleged to have ‘invented’ knitting by topological covariance of trait indicators is considered to be a direct (if
analysis. imperfect) reflection of the structure of the traits themselves.
• Note that these alternative routes are themselves likely to But, as noted previously, the processes involved in translat-
express personality. For example, introverts will likely pre- ing a BT into a CA are variable across persons and occa-
fer to learn knitting by reading a book rather than by at- sions. Further, the same processes (e.g. habit formation and
tending a class; closed people may focus on mastery of rational choice) might contribute to the development of many
basic stitches, whereas open people may try to skip ahead unrelated CAs. It is not immediately clear to what the ‘struc-
to more elaborate and interesting patterns. FFT expresses ture of dynamic processes’ would refer in FFT, but it seems
this in the Differential Dynamics Postulate, which claims likely that the relation between such a structure and the struc-
that ‘Some dynamic processes are differentially affected ture of trait indicators would be one of emergence.
by basic tendencies of the individual’ (McCrae & Costa,
1996, p. 75).
TRAITS AS EFFECTS: DEVELOPMENTAL
It is the ubiquitous equifinality of accommodative pro-
PROCESSES
cesses that constrains causal explanation within FFT. We
cannot specify how one learns to act agreeably or conscien-
‘Development’ usually refers to normative changes after
tiously, because there are many different and equally possi-
birth that lead to a mature, fully functioning organism. In
ble routes, some or all of which, on different occasions,
the case of non-human animals, development is assumed
may have contributed to the establishment of CAs that ex-
to have been shaped by natural and sexual selection to in-
press those traits. This does not mean that research into spe-
crease the animal’s fitness. We will use development in a
cific processes is impossible or unimportant; indeed, that
broader sense here, to refer to anything that leads to endur-
remains a major task for personality psychology (McCrae,
ing changes in trait levels. Such development need not be
2016). But one can understand how personality will be
evolved, need not stop with the attainment of adulthood,
expressed without considering the causal mechanisms in
and need not be functional. Alzheimer’s disease (AD), for
detail.
example, leads to late-life changes in Conscientiousness
(Siegler et al., 1991) that are neither normative nor func-
tional, but it can be understood as trait development in the
Correspondence and emergence
broad sense of trait change.
Baumert et al. (in press) have outlined a research agenda As Figure 1 illustrates, the Origin and Development Pos-
intended to integrate personality structure, process, and de- tulates of FFT assert that traits are endogenous BTs that
velopment. Briefly, they argued that observed traits (behav- change (largely in adolescence and early adulthood) through
iour patterns) are caused by personality processes and point processes of intrinsic maturation. Genetics plays a role, but
out that there are two different ways in which the organiza- any biological process that affects the brain, from intrauterine
tion of processes (the causal structure) can be related to the environment to traumatic brain injury, can also shape person-
organization of traits (the phenotypic structure). If distinct ality traits.
sets of processes are implicated in the creation of each dis- Although FFT’s biological bases include much more
tinct set of related traits (e.g. Agreeableness-generating pro- than genetics, it is reasonable to expect that traits should be
cesses produce facets of Agreeableness), then there will be substantially heritable, and there is currently some contro-
correspondence between the causal structure and the pheno- versy about just how heritable traits are. In particular, recent
typic structure. If, however, the same processes contribute in genomic studies of personality traits have suggested only
various ways to the development of a wide range of possibly modest heritability. Lo et al. (2016) reported significant her-
unrelated traits, then the causal structure will bear no neces- itability for all five factors, but with all h2 ≤ .18. Hill et al.
sary resemblance to the phenotypic structure, which can be (2017), using a sample of related individuals and a different
ascribed to emergence. analytic approach, found heritabilities of .30 and .13 for Neu-
It is possible to draw some rough parallels between the roticism and Extraversion, respectively. A meta-analysis of
model of the personality system of Baumert et al. (in press) behaviour genetic studies (Vukasović & Bratko, 2015) found
and FFT’s. Their traits (‘relatively stable inter-individual dif- considerably larger values, with a mean value of .40. How-
ferences in the degree/extent/level of coherent behaviors, ever, that figure combined results from family and adoption
thoughts, feelings’; p. xxx) seem to refer to CAs and the be- studies (where mean h2 = .22) and twin studies (where mean
haviours they give rise to; their personality processes appear h2 = .47). Vukasović and Bratko (2015) attributed this dra-
to parallel FFT’s dynamic processes. However, their model matic difference to the presence of non-additive genetic ef-
does not seem to have a direct equivalent to BTs. In their fects that are detectable only in twin designs. That might
model, processes are the basic causal units, whereas in account for the relatively low heritabilities in some genomic
FFT, processes are intermediaries between BTs and CAs studies.
(or CAs and behaviour). The study of personality processes Further, all these studies rely on self-reports and thus re-
is fundamental for the agenda of Baumert et al. (in press), fer to the heritability of observed scores. All personality as-
whereas it is incidental for FFT. sessments include substantial error, both random and

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

systematic, so these are necessarily lower limits of the heri- Correlational studies have been useful for identifying as-
tability of true scores. Multi-method studies, which allow es- sociations of traits with a range of biological factors, includ-
timates of true score heritability, typically find heritabilities ing brain structures (DeYoung et al., 2010), inflammatory
of .5 to .7 (Mõttus et al., 2017; Riemann, Angleitner, & and other physiological markers of health (Luchetti, Barkley,
Strelau, 1997). Stephan, Terracciano, & Sutin, 2014; Sutin et al., 2010), and
Conspicuously absent from Figure 1 is an arrow from EIs chronic diseases (Chapman, Lyness, & Duberstein, 2007).
to BTs: traits are relatively immune to effects from the psy- These studies, however, are limited in the way that all corre-
chological environment. This is the most radical and unpop- lational studies are limited: they provide information about
ular tenet of FFT, and it is almost certain that there will be an association but no indication of which variable is cause
exceptional circumstances in which it is wrong. But, as ar- and which consequence.
gued elsewhere (McCrae, De Bolle, Löckenhoff, & Longitudinal studies are more informative than cross-
Terracciano, in press), a purely biological basis for traits is sectional studies because they can sometimes help tease apart
consistent with a wide range of findings3 from comparative the temporal ordering of the variables. For example, Van
(King, Weiss, & Sisco, 2008), cross-cultural (McCrae et al., Scheppingen et al. (2016) reported that participants who be-
2005), longitudinal (Costa, McCrae, & Arenberg, 1980), came parents during the course of a longitudinal study did
and behaviour genetic (Jang et al., 1998) studies, whereas ev- not change any more than a matched set of participants
idence for important effects on personality traits from the who did not become parents on any of the Five-Factor Model
psychological environment is not yet compelling. domains. Thus, having children—which is a profound bio-
Five-Factor Theory’s claim that the environment does not logical as well as psychological event—does not appear to
affect trait development could easily be misunderstood by be a cause of personality change. However, Van
psychologists and developmentalists (e.g. Gottleib, 2007). Scheppingen et al. (2016) did find that closed extraverts were
In fact, no one imagines that the human brain develops in a more likely to become parents, suggesting a possible causal
vacuum; cognitive and personality development presuppose role of personality in the decision to have children.
a rich social environment with which the organism interacts. Several longitudinal studies have found that biological
An embryo raised to maturity in a sensory deprivation tank— factors are associated with change in personality traits, but
if it survived at all—would not be a human being. two-point data often provide insufficient information for
Developmentalists are naturally interested in the details of strong causal inferences. For example, physiological dysreg-
the organism/environment transactions that allow develop- ulation, as indexed by cardiovascular, metabolic, and im-
ment to unfold, but these need not concern the personality mune markers of allostatic load (or stress), is associated
psychologist, because the psychological environment—at with subsequent declines in Extraversion and Conscientious-
least the average expectable environment of Hartmann ness four years later (Stephan, Sutin, Luchetti, &
(1939)—has no differential effect on trait development. Ac- Terracciano, 2016) and may be a contributing cause to per-
cording to FFT, any psychological environment will lead, sonality change. However, without serial data on personality
in the long run, to the same levels of personality traits. This traits prior to the baseline measure of allostatic load, one can-
phenomenon was most famously illustrated in the Minnesota not rule out the alternative possibility that the physiological
studies of twins raised apart (Tellegen et al., 1988). dysregulation was itself a consequence of an ongoing decline
in Extraversion and Conscientiousness. Likewise, young
adults who quit smoking decline in Neuroticism and impul-
Biological influences on development sivity (Littlefield & Sher, 2012). Nicotine is a powerful drug
with well-established pharmacological effects on the brain,
If the psychological environment has no distinctive effect on so these findings are plausible from the perspective of FFT.
trait development, it makes sense to focus on the causal anal- But even though smoking cessation occurred before the
ysis of biological influences on personality. Because most change in Neuroticism was measured, it is not possible to
psychologists agree that biology has some effect on traits, know whether cessation caused the decline in Neuroticism
this approach ought to be uncontroversial … as far as it goes. or whether naturally occurring declines in Neuroticism led
There are several possible approaches to understanding the individual to quit smoking.
the biological underpinnings of trait development across the Evidence for a causal role of biology on personality also
lifespan, ranging from correlational to experimental. Where comes from the change in personality traits that occurs with
analogous developmental processes are seen in other species the onset of AD. Numerous studies have found that individ-
(e.g. King et al., 2008), even animal models might be uals with dementia have dramatic increases in Neuroticism
exploited. Each approach is informative in contributing a and decreases in Conscientiousness compared with their
piece of evidence on the biological origins of personality de- premorbid personality (Robins Wahlin & Byrne, 2011).
velopment, yet each also has limitations that prohibit strong There are profound changes in the brain due to AD, and
claims about causality. the changes in personality that occur with AD are likely a re-
sult of these changes in biology. How these alterations in the
brain directly lead to the observed changes in personality are
3
Other, non-biological explanations are also possible, although they are un- not yet known. Still, change in personality with the onset of
likely to be as parsimonious. Stability of the environment might account for
stability of traits, but it could not explain the similarity of personality struc- dementia is so common that it is enshrined in the clinical
ture across cultures. criteria for AD (McKhann et al., 2011).

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

Because AD patients may not be able to complete self- perhaps biological processes, that may account for the ob-
report measures, research on the personality changes that served change.
occurs with AD relies primarily on retrospective reports Researchers guided by FFT might choose to study natural
from knowledgeable loved ones. Such reports are presum- experiments that focus on potential biological processes,
ably biased to some extent because the informant may exag- comparing populations that do or do not experience famine,
gerate the difference between premorbid and post-morbid epidemics, or climate change. One problem with all such de-
personality and may rely on stereotypes of dementia when signs is that the independent variable (earthquake, famine,
rating their loved one’s personality. There is some evidence, etc.) typically consists of an entire complex of biological,
however, that supports these findings even in the presence psychosocial, and political influences, and it will rarely be
of bias. First, the stability of self-reported traits is much possible to single out the effective cause of any observed
lower among individuals with cognitive impairment changes.
(Terracciano, Stephan, Luchetti, & Sutin, 2017). Over a In medicine, randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are the
four-year follow-up, for example, the mean stability coeffi- gold standard for establishing causality. Such studies are
cient for individuals with dementia was .43 compared with difficult in the context of personality research for both
.70 for individuals of similar age without dementia. By con- practical and ethical reasons. Still, some RCTs suggest that
trast, the internal consistency of the scales was similar biological interventions can change personality. Experimen-
across individuals with and without dementia, which sug- tal studies with psilocybin, a common hallucinogen, for ex-
gests that lower stability coefficients were not merely a re- ample, indicate that Openness increases with exposure to
flection of lower reliability in the self-reports of those with this drug: individuals who received high doses increased
dementia. Second, there is little relation between baseline in Openness following administration and maintained this
personality and AD neuropathology. Terracciano et al. higher Openness at least one year later (MacLean, Johnson,
(2013) reported an association between Neuroticism and & Griffiths, 2011). A short-term (two week) follow-up
the Braak stage of neurofibrillary tangles in the brain at au- study replicated this effect with lysergic acid diethylamide
topsy, but not neuritic plaques; others haves found no rela- (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016). In both cases, this effect was
tion between either tangles or plaques and personality specific to Openness. Further evidence suggested that
(Wilson, Schneider, Arnold, Bienias, & Bennett, 2007). changes in brain function accounted for this effect: lysergic
Premorbid personality thus does not predict the amount of acid diethylamide may increase Openness through greater
neuropathology that accumulates in the brain. Instead, the entropy in the brain (i.e. changes in brain signal that are re-
brain changes associated with AD presumably account for lated negatively to predictability of a time series; Lebedev
the significant changes that occur in personality with the et al., 2016). RCTs, however, still face limitations that com-
disease. The mechanisms through which AD leads to promise the ability to draw conclusions about causality. For
changes in personality traits, however, have yet to be example, there is significant selection bias both in who
identified. chooses to participate and who completes the study. In
Natural experiments provide a quasi-experimental ap- the aforementioned examples, hallucinogens increased
proach to identifying factors that have causal effects on per- Openness among participants who were willing to accept
sonality traits. Natural experiments occur when an the possibility of being given a hallucinogen as part of a
intervention happens in the ‘real world’ not under the con- study; whether the same effect would occur in individuals
trol of the investigator. In psychology, natural experiments unwilling to take a hallucinogen in the first place is
often examine the effect of environmental stressors on psy- unknown.
chological outcomes, including traits. A recent example Even with evidence from RCTs, it is difficult to evaluate
comes from the 2011 earthquake in Christchurch, New whether the intervention changed the underlying BT or the
Zealand. This massive 6.3 magnitude earthquake killed CA that measures it. As previously discussed in the example
nearly 200 people and destroyed much of the city’s infra- of Angry Hostility, if all or at least many items within the
structure. Milojev, Osborne, and Sibley (2014) used this oc- trait or facet changed in response to the intervention, then
casion to test whether personality changed as a result of a there would be some evidence for change in the BT. If, how-
major natural disaster. With data collected a year before ever, only one or just a few items showed change in response
and after the earthquake, the researchers found that partici- to the intervention, it would be more difficult to differentiate
pants in Christchurch increased slightly in Neuroticism from whether the change reflected a change in the BT or the CA
before to after the quake, whereas participants from similar used to assess it.
areas of New Zealand that were not affected by the earth- The methodology of RCTs provides a rigorous test of
quake had a slight decline in Neuroticism over the same how a biological intervention may or may not change person-
time period; there were no changes in any of the other traits ality. Such approaches, however, may be less useful for iden-
for either group, and there were no differences in pre-quake tifying the pure biological determinants of traits. If
personality between the two groups. The random selection administration of a drug known to effect a specific structure
of the inhabitants of Christchurch subjected to a major or function of the brain also leads to change in personality,
earthquake suggests—contra FFT—that exposure to a sig- compared with controls, then one can infer that the structure
nificant natural disaster increases Neuroticism. It is not yet or function effected by the drug is also implicated in person-
known for how long this difference will persist, and this de- ality. As such, it would provide insight into some of the bio-
sign cannot identify underlying mechanisms, including logical processes responsible for personality but not

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

necessarily the trait’s entire biological basis. There are other but writing a grammatical sentence would not generally be
limitations of RCTs that limit their usefulness (Deaton & considered a personality-related behaviour.
Cartwright, 2016), and for any biological process that may Five-Factor Theory asserts that traits, as BTs, are distal
contribute to personality development, no one methodology causes of behaviour, their effects mediated by CAs. For ex-
is likely to lead to conclusions about causality. Rather, it will ample, extraverts are better group leaders (Judge, Bono,
take converging evidence from diverse approaches to iden- Ilies, & Gerhardt, 2002), and Extraversion is associated with
tify plausible explanatory causes. social skills (Riggio, 1986); it is plausible to argue that so-
Neither diseases nor drugs nor earthquakes are likely to cial skills are a proximal cause of good leadership and Ex-
be able to account for personality trait development in the traversion a distal cause. Similarly, investigative vocational
narrow sense, such as the gradual normative decrease in Neu- interests might be a proximal cause of career choices, and
roticism and increases in Agreeableness and Conscientious- Openness the distal cause (Costa, McCrae, & Holland,
ness that have been called personality maturation (e.g. 1984); a nicotine addiction the proximal cause of smoking
Roberts & Mroczek, 2008), or the gradual erosion of rank- behaviour, and Neuroticism a distal cause (Terracciano &
order stability over decades of life (Conley, 1984). FFT as- Costa, 2004). Personality psychologists can extend the
cribes such developmental pathways to intrinsic maturation, causal chains of educational, industrial/organizational, and
due presumably to genetically controlled processes of ageing health psychologists back—in part—to enduring
and to accumulated random changes in brain structure. Be- dispositions.
cause ageing cannot be manipulated, direct tests of these hy- The causal mediation between traits and behaviours pos-
potheses are probably impossible. Twin studies speak to the ited by FFT does not translate directly into simple statistical
heritability of differential rates of trait change, but not to mediation. One might, for example, assess Extraversion, so-
the possible genetic basis of universal trait changes. Indeed, cial skills, and leadership behaviour and find that, statisti-
the best evidence for the intrinsic maturation hypothesis cally, social skills partially mediate the effect of
comes from ruling out alternatives (McCrae et al., in press; Extraversion on leadership4—leaving, in the usual interpreta-
Van Scheppingen et al., 2016). tion of the data, an additional direct effect of Extraversion
that FFT would not predict. FFT would explain this anomaly
by arguing that other mediating CAs related to Extraversion
TRAITS AS CAUSES: ASSIMILATIVE PROCESSES (e.g. social network size) account for the remaining variance.
A full model, in which all relevant CAs were included,
Many psychologists would claim that the chief task of psy- would be expected to show no direct effect of traits on
chology is to understand, predict, and control behaviours. behaviours.
Cognitive psychologists focus on how behaviours are Conversely, empirical studies might show that particu-
learned and how actions are planned or chosen. Social psy- lar CAs offer stronger causal accounts of outcomes than
chologists emphasize situational determinants of behaviour, do the traits that underlie them. For example, social skills
although sometimes in conjunction with individual differ- might be much better predictors of leadership than Extra-
ences in goals and needs (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & version itself is. Such a result might be due to the indirect
Jarvis, 1996). Personality psychologists also predict specific influence of other, unmeasured BTs such as intelligence
behaviours (e.g. Kolar, Funder, & Colvin, 1996), although (Riggio, 1986), or to EIs such as formal education or life
they have learned that traits are better predictors of aggre- coaching that increase social skills and thus leadership
gated than of single behaviours (Epstein, 1979). ability.
Social psychological explanations of behaviour are usu- Five-Factor Theory insists that the effects of traits on
ally tested with experimental designs in which participants behaviour must be mediated by CAs, but for practical pur-
are randomly assigned to conditions. Personality psycholo- poses, it may suffice to know that traits predict an outcome
gists understand that situations do not occur randomly in real of interest, without any consideration or assessment of
life; instead, people select situations (Buss, 1987), often on CAs. Some links in the causal chain will be missing, but
the basis of their personality traits. Personality psychologists some links in any causal chain are inevitably missing. It
may be able to provide more naturalistic accounts of behav- is useful to know that conscientious job applicants will
iour because they acknowledge the contribution of the person outperform others, even if one does not know whether to
(as well as the situation; Funder, 2016). attribute this to their perfectionist attitudes, their habits of
To justify the imperialistic claim that personality psychol- punctuality, or their methodical working styles. Of course,
ogy is the study of the whole person, one might note that if one knew which specific CAs were relevant to the out-
CAs, a central part of the personality system, are involved come, for practical purposes it might make sense to assess
in the generation of all behaviours—except perhaps uncondi- them and ignore the underlying traits. But a small number
tioned responses—and thus that personality provides part of of domain-level and facet-level personality traits are associ-
the causal explanation of virtually all psychological phenom- ated with a myriad of CAs, so systematic exploration of
ena. In practice, personality psychologists are concerned
with a more limited range of behaviours and experiences that
4
are tied, at least conceptually, to traits or to CAs (such as In any such design, care should be taken to assess the BT with indicators
that are not confounded with the CAs of interest. For example, the Extraver-
values, interests, or the self-concept) that are substantially re- sion scale in this example should probably omit items related to
lated to traits. Knowledge of basic English grammar is a CA, assertiveness.

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

potential predictors of some outcome is probably easier at self-report. But a follow-up assessment collected a few days
the trait level. later showed that the original self-concept had been restored.
Psychotherapists are well aware that the progress made in
one session may have evaporated by the next.
TRAITS AS CAUSES: ACCOMMODATIVE
PROCESSES
Core and surface characteristics
Most of the vast literature on personality correlates consists Kandler, Zimmerman, and McAdams (2014) have discussed
of studies linking traits to CAs: health habits, values, prej- a distinction found in the personality literature between core
udices, personality disorders, relationships, parenting and surface characteristics—a distinction they liken to that
styles, virtues, music preferences, and so on. Although between BTs and CAs. Core characteristics (roughly, traits)
these are correlational studies, the usual (and reasonable) are thought to be stable, heritable, and causally prior to sur-
assumption is that traits are the cause and their correlates face characteristics. The latter, including self-related schema,
the effect. Such studies are valuable in charting the causes attitudes and beliefs, and personal strivings, would generally
of behaviour and cataloguing the consequences of person- be classified by FFT as CAs. Kandler et al. (2014) argued
ality traits, and they have been greatly facilitated by the that if they are really distinct from core characteristics, sur-
widespread adoption of the Five-Factor Model, which pro- face characteristics should be substantially less stable and
vides a framework for organizing systematic research and less heritable.
meta-analyses. Five-Factor Theory does not make specific predictions
Five-Factor Theory claims that in these cases, an associ- about the stability of CAs. Some of them are quite time lim-
ation is found because traits respond to the demands and op- ited (e.g. the goal of finishing a term paper), whereas others
portunities of the environment to create new intrapsychic endure for decades (e.g. knowledge of one’s native tongue).
structures that can become relatively enduring features of Some CAs are malleable (the route one takes to work),
the individual—CAs. People are not born with a distrust of whereas others resist change (cigarette smoking). Thus, sta-
foreigners, but some are born with (or develop over time) a bility is generally expected for BTs, but it is not
disposition to be wary and sceptical. The specific objects of contraindicative for CAs.
mistrust are learned—through personal experience, model- Kandler et al. (2014) reviewed a series of studies on the
ling one’s peers, or formal indoctrination. As noted previ- heritability of general interests, attitudes, and life goals in
ously, many different mechanisms may be involved, conjunction with personality traits. The most straightforward
including self-selection into like-minded groups. interpretation of FFT would predict that interests, attitudes,
Five-Factor Theory expects that the end result of a and goals ought to be heritable only insofar as they are re-
lifetime of experience is more-or-less fixed by the traits lated to heritable traits. In fact, Kandler et al. (2014) typically
the individual brings to it; the stability of individual found that these CAs were sensibly related to traits and that
differences over periods of up to 40 years (Terracciano, part of their heritability could be accounted for by the herita-
Costa, & McCrae, 2006) is consistent with the idea that bility of traits—but that some could not. Interests, attitudes,
BTs provide a kind of set-point around which CAs hover. and goals appear to have some unique biological basis be-
A prediction that follows from this is that effects yond that captured by the traits assessed by the Revised
produced by one set of experiences (including deliberate NEO Personality Inventory (Ostendorf & Angleitner,
interventions) may stimulate compensating effects 2004). Kandler et al. (2014) argued that this means that the
elsewhere. This phenomenon was made famous in the conceptual distinction between traits and other attributes
psychoanalytic notion of symptom substitution: cure one (e.g. interests) offered by FFT (and some other personality
problem in an individual high in Neuroticism, and another theories) needs revision.
will pop up (cf. Ellis, 1987). It is also the basis of the However, it is possible to reconcile the findings of
therapeutic strategy of channelling aggression in socially Kandler et al. (2014) with standard FFT (Costa & McCrae,
acceptable ways, rather than trying to reduce it. FFT 2017). FFT insists on the distinction between BTs and
would anticipate similar effects for all traits: for example, CAs, and it claims that the personality sphere is essentially
natural introverts who are required by their profession to exhausted by the five factors and their definers. From this
interact extensively with others may seek out new oppor- perspective, Kandler et al. (2014) have documented (i) that
tunities for solitude (Long & Averill, 2003). Psychologists the duality principle applies to attitudes, interest, and goals
evaluating interventions intended to modify trait-related as well as to NEO Inventory traits and (ii) that many
CAs should perhaps also enquire about unintended attitudes, interests, and goals may be regarded as facets
compensatory consequences that might be considered side or nuances of one or more of the five factors. This is
effects of the intervention. not a radical innovation: the NEO Inventories already in-
Characteristic adaptations develop over time, in the con- clude facets that tap attitudes (Openness to Values and
text of enduring dispositions. One implication is that experi- Tender Mindedness), interests (Openness to Aesthetics
mental research on modifying CAs may be misleading. and Ideas), and goals (Excitement Seeking and Achieve-
Swann (1983) provided a nice illustration of this fact. He ment Striving).
showed that fairly simple false feedback could alter a Kandler et al. (2014) argued that the catalogue of core
person’s self-concept, as assessed by a post-intervention characteristics should be broadened beyond ‘stylistic and

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

regulatory patterns of thoughts, feelings, and actions’ to in- dictate these attitudes and behaviours in the way that instinct
clude ‘interests, motives, attitudes and values’ (p. 240). At guides a squirrel to conceal a stash of acorns. Drug use per se
least in theory, current views of the Five-Factor Model al- is heritable in the statistical sense that if one twin uses drugs,
ready do. Costa and McCrae (2017) discussed the fact that the other has a greater probability of doing so. In a mechanis-
all broad traits are expressed in a variety of ways; the items tic sense, genes must affect the brain in such a way as to cre-
in personality questionnaires tend to be a mix of emotional, ate a disposition to acquire drug-use attitudes and behaviours
behavioural, and cognitive indicators of the trait (Pytlik —CAs that are developed through life experience in a partic-
Zillig, Hemenover, & Dienstbier, 2002). One of the early ular social context. FFT would interpret this heritable dispo-
demonstrations of the comprehensiveness of the Five-Factor sition as a trait we might call drug-use proneness. It might
Model showed that its factors could be found in an inventory turn out that drug-use proneness is merely another name for
of Murray’s needs (Costa & McCrae, 1988) and the NEO Extraversion; if so, the more extraverted group in a discor-
Inventories have been described as assessing ‘emotional, dant twin study should show higher rates of drug use. What
interpersonal, experiential, attitudinal, and motivational if the study showed that (true score) Extraversion and drug
characteristics’ (Costa & McCrae, 2014, p. 231). use were not completely yoked? Because drug-use proneness
From the perspective of FFT, the issue is not whether at- is a trait associated with, but not equivalent to, Extraversion,
titudes, values, and interests are core characteristics, but FFT would interpret it as a facet of Extraversion—perhaps
whether they are BTs or CAs, and the answer to that depends Excitement Seeking or perhaps some new and distinct facet.
entirely on how one looks at them. If one focuses on the con- Extraversion would then be one of the contributing causes of
crete content of a need or interest or value, it is a CA; if one drug-use proneness (in the ‘factorial’ sense; see below) and a
focuses on the general tendency underlying the specific in- distal cause of drug use.
stantiation, it is likely a BT. Aspiring to be President is a The same argument, of course, can be applied to any
CA that would have been meaningless in most cultures other CAs—like the interests, motives, and values of Kandler
throughout most of history. Having a predisposition to seek et al. (2014)—that are statistically heritable and substantially
social dominance is a BT that is presumably universal. related to one or more of the five factors: they must corre-
spond to (and be indicators of) BTs somewhere in the per-
sonality trait hierarchy. That a CA is a personality
A lesson from discordant MZ twins
correlate, that it is statistically heritable, and that some por-
Turkheimer, Pettersson, and Horn (2014) present an interest- tion of its heritability is not shared with Five-Factor Model
ing approach to causal analysis exploiting differences be- factors—these are empirical findings. That a personality-
tween monozygotic twins. In their example, drug use is related CA has some corresponding BT is merely a logical
hypothetically associated with Extraversion, which would deduction from the premises of FFT, but it facilitates a whole
normally be interpreted as evidence of an accommodative series of hypotheses on the characteristic’s stability, univer-
process: Extraversion, a BT, leads to the development of sality, and development.
drug addiction, a CA. Turkheimer et al. (2014) suggest an-
other possibility: perhaps some of the same genes affect both
Extraversion and drug use, so the cause of drug addiction is TRAITS AS CAUSES: ‘FACTORIAL’ PROCESSES
not Extraversion, but a shared set of genes. Discordant twins
provide a way of testing this hypothesis. Because twins are Statistical purists argue for a sharp distinction between factor
never identical, we can expect that one MZ twin in each pair analysis and component analysis: principal component anal-
will be more extraverted than the other,5 and we can compare ysis is said merely to describe the correlational structure of
the rate of drug addiction in the more extraverted twins with variables, whereas factor analysis explains it by revealing
the rate in the more introverted twins. If Extraversion per se the causal connection between underlying factors and their
is the causal factor, then the former group should have a manifestation in observed variables. In fact, however, causal
higher rate of abuse. If, however, there is no difference, inferences are not made by statistical manipulations, but by
Turkheimer et al. (2014) would conclude that Extraversion scientists who consider a pattern of evidence (including, on
is not a cause of drug abuse; instead, Extraversion and drug occasion, factor or component loadings).
use are common effects of shared genes. That might appear With this understanding, it is appropriate to ask whether
to imply that biological bases are direct causes of CAs—a the broader traits identified by factor or component analysis
conclusion that is not consistent with FFT. can be considered causes of the narrower traits that define
A proponent of FFT would argue that drug use is a suite them. This is the default view in personality psychology;
of attitudes and behaviours that might include finding a we tend to assume that warmth and cheerfulness and activity
dealer, buying the drug and paraphernalia, smoking, covary because they share some common cause that we call
snorting, or injecting the drug, concealing the stash, and so Extraversion. Similarly, items about laughing, feeling joy,
on. Surely Turkheimer et al. (2014) do not believe that genes and being optimistic covary because they share a facet-level
cause called Positive Emotions.
5
Note that such differences might reflect the influence of the non-shared en- We might imagine that there is an Agreeableness centre
vironment on the trait, as Turkheimer et al. (2014) assume, but they might in the brain, with neural connections to centres controlling
also result in part or in whole from random and systematic measurement er-
ror. Fruitful implementation of this design would probably require multi- Trust, Straightforwardness, Altruism, Compliance, Modesty,
method trait assessment and large samples. and Tender Mindedness. If the Agreeableness centre is

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

highly active, it will cause activity in all the related centres; factors would predict, significant effects were also found
in a population with varying levels of activity in the for five of the six Openness facets.
Agreebleness centre, the facets of Trust, Straightforwardness, Although these results are generally consistent with the
and so on will covary to define the phenotypic factor. Or per- hypothesis of a factorial cause, they unfortunately do not rule
haps Agreeableness reflects the operation of a set of genes, out a network explanation. Antidepressant medications, for
and some or all of the same genes contribute to each of its example, may alter the level of the Depression facet, and
facets (McCrae, 2015). In these scenarios, the biological ba- its network connections may then create changes in Anxiety,
sis of the factor contributes to the biological bases of the Self-consciousness, and Vulnerability facets. In principle, it
facets, and in this sense, the factor causes the facets. might be possible to decide between the two alternatives by
An alternative view has recently been promoted (e.g. assessing trait levels repeatedly: if all trait changes are simul-
Cramer et al., 2012): in network models, traits covary be- taneous, then a factorial explanation is favoured; if changes
cause they are functionally linked to each other; facets essen- in one facet regularly precede changes in other facets, it
tially cause each other. Some aspects of this account are would suggest the unfolding of network processes. At pres-
plausible. For example, we might suppose that people who ent we know almost nothing about the time scale on which
are cheerful make more friends, and people with many network processes work (if they do in fact work), so a good
friends learn good social skills, and people with social skills deal of exploratory research would need to precede a con-
become leaders, and leaders enjoy the prerogatives of leader- vincing test.
ship and become cheerful. Thus Positive Emotions, Gregari- It is also possible to consider natural experiments, of
ousness, and Assertiveness will tend to co-occur, and we which the most convenient is ageing (Mõttus, 2016; Mõttus
label that syndrome Extraversion. In this network model, et al., 2015). With age, adults show regular, though very
the factor emerges from the facets; it is effect, not cause. gradual, changes in all personality factors. In general, from
Throughout this article, we will discuss a simplified, pure age 30 to age 70 years, Neuroticism and all of its facets de-
network model in contrast to a pure factorial model; it is, cline, whereas Agreeableness and Conscientiousness and
however, clearly possible that both contribute to the observed their facets increase (McCrae, Martin, & Costa, 2005;
covariance of traits. Terracciano, McCrae, Brant, & Costa, 2005), consistent with
Normally, if there is a question about which of two re- the notion of unitary and unifying factors. But the develop-
lated phenomena is cause and which effect, researchers mental courses of the facets of Extraversion are not consis-
may try to manipulate each and see if the other responds. If tent. In both self-reports (Terracciano et al., 2005) and
we could increase the level of Neuroticism, we could see if observer ratings (McCrae et al., 2005), Warmth increased
all its facets increased, too. In our present state of ignorance, slightly with age, whereas Excitement Seeking declined dra-
this approach (even if it were ethical) is not feasible; we have matically. Part of the explanation for this difference is that
not identified practical causes of trait change. these two facets of Extraversion have secondary loadings
There are, however, manipulations that may provide on Agreeableness; the positive loading of Warmth on Agree-
some hints. When administered antidepressant medications, ableness may explain its increase (because Agreeableness in-
many patients improve (although others do not). For those creases); the negative loading of Excitement Seeking on
who respond to the drug, there are significance changes in Agreeableness helps explain its rapid decline.
trait profiles. Costa, Bagby, Herbst, and McCrae (2005) But even if the influence of all five factors is statistically re-
showed that recovered patients decreased on total Neuroti- moved from each of the facets, there are developmental trends
cism and four of its facets: Anxiety, Depression, Self- in the remaining specific variance that are consistent across
consciousness, and Vulnerability; the declines in Angry Hos- cultures (McCrae et al., 1999). We cannot easily tease apart
tility and Impulsiveness did not reach significance. One the causal influence of factors on facets through a study of age-
might reasonably conclude from these findings that antide- ing, because age has effects on both: any deviation from the
pressants lower Neuroticism and thereby its facets. However, pattern predicted by the factor-as-cause model could be due
trait levels also changed for Extraversion, Openness, and to developmental effects on the specific portion of the facet.
Conscientiousness, and seven of their facets. Antidepressant Researchers might have more luck testing predictions
medications (nine different drugs were used in this clinical from the network approach. Here, traits must be malleable;
study, at the discretion of the treating psychiatrist) do not otherwise, they would not be able to coalesce into a network.
seem to provide a clear and selective way to manipulate Manipulation of any single facet should have ramifications
Neuroticism. for all the other facets in the network. We might, for exam-
A more specific manipulation may have been found for ple, assign individuals low in Gregariousness to jobs that re-
Openness. As noted earlier, MacLean et al. (2011) adminis- quire extensive human contact. According to a network
tered the ‘magic mushroom’ drug, psilocybin, in a con- analysis, increased social interaction ought to promote in-
trolled setting, and found significant increases in creased social skills, cheerfulness, assertiveness, and so on
Openness, but none of the other factors. A subgroup of —the entire network of Extraversion-related traits. Placed
30 (of 52 total) participants had mystical experiences dur- in a new social environment, these individuals would achieve
ing the drug session; these could be considered ‘re- a new network equilibrium that elevates all facets of
sponders’. Within this subgroup, increases in Openness of Extraversion.
about 1/2 standard deviation were seen and were sustained Note that such predictions from network models are ex-
after a one-year follow-up. As the causal interpretation of actly the opposite of those made by FFT for CAs. According

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

to FFT, assigning introverts to jobs requiring extensive so- The factor is less heritable than its component facets.
cialization should lead them to seek more solitary free-time Now, consider the factor-as-cause model. Here, we as-
activities—what Little (1996) has called a restorative niche sume the components of variance are the heritable specific
—in compensation. It might be possible to conduct experi- variance, the heritable common variance, G, (i.e. the causa-
ments—or find natural ones—that allow one to determine tive underlying factor), and error:
whether changes to one facet (or its CAs) lead to proportional
X ¼ aGX þ bG þ cE X
or, on the contrary, to compensatory changes in others.
Similar ideas have been proffered by Little (2008) in his Y ¼ aGY þ bG þ cE Y
concept of free traits. Individuals need not be forced to act
in unaccustomed ways by therapeutic interventions or exper- The correlation between traits, rXY, is entirely due to G
imental manipulations; they may themselves freely choose to and is equal to b2 = .3. The heritability of X (or Y) is due
enact a trait that runs counter to their own natural disposition. to both GX (or GY) and G and is equal to a2 + b2 = .4. Thus,
These free traits are patterns of behaviour chosen to advance a2 must equal .4  .3 = .1, and c2 = .6. The factor score is
one’s personal projects. An introvert might take a position in given by
sales because this is the best job available; to keep it, he or ðX þ YÞ ¼ aGX þ aGY þ 2bG þ cE X þ cE Y
she would adopt an outgoing manner. Although free traits
and the heritability of the domain is due to all heritable com-
can advance one’s agenda, they require effort and, without
ponents:
restorative resources, can lead to lowered well-being (Little,
2008). Presumably free traits are discarded as soon as they  
h2 ðXþYÞ ¼ a2 þ a2 þ 4b2 = a2 þ a2 þ 4b2 þ c2 þ c2
cease to be useful.
¼ ð:1 þ :1 þ 1:2Þ=ð:1 þ :1 þ 1:2 þ :6 þ :6Þ
¼ :54 > :4:
The heritability of facets and factors
Evidence on the heritability of facets and factors can shed Here, the factor is considerably more heritable than the
some light on the causal status of factors. We know that both facets.
broad and narrow traits are heritable (e.g. Mõttus et al., Both these models are greatly simplified, omitting, for ex-
2017), so traits at some level of the hierarchy must have ample, method variance that itself may or may not be herita-
some genetic basis. But network and factorial models have ble. But they illustrate the general point that if facets share a
quantitatively different implications about the heritability of common genetic influence, that influence will be
higher-order factors. Consider a simplified example of a fac- compounded when they are added together and tend to pro-
tor with two facets, X and Y. From a network perspective, we duce higher heritability estimates for factors than for facets.
might identify three sources of variance that contribute to an Conversely, if there is no common genetic influence—as
individual’s score: genetic components specific to each trait pure network models assume—then the sum of the facets, al-
that accounts for their heritability (GX and GY), a network though still heritable, will show a diluted genetic effect, be-
component (N) that accounts for the observed covariation cause the compounded network influence, N, increases as a
of X and Y, and error (EX and EY). If we assume that these proportion of the variance in the sum.
are combined in the proportion of a : b : c (where Which model better fits the data? Jang et al. (1998) re-
a2 + b2 + c2 = 1) and that the proportions are identical in both ported that the mean heritability of NEO Inventory facets
traits, we can say that was .39, whereas the mean heritability of the five factors was
.48. Those data support the underlying-factor-as-cause model.
X ¼ aGX þ bN þ cEX
It might be argued that the simplified network model we
Y ¼ aGY þ bN þ cE Y have discussed is a straw man. In their figure 7, Cramer
et al. (2012) described a possible network model in which a
The correlation between traits, rXY, is entirely due to the set of genes affect more than one indicator of Neuroticism,
shared component, N, and is equal to b2. The heritability of X as well as some of the connections between the indicators.
(or Y) is entirely due to GX (or GY) and is equal to a2. To Certainly, this is a possible scenario, but such a set of genes
choose illustrative values, let rXY = b2 = .3 and would necessarily contribute to the covariation of items that
h2 = a2 = .4. Then c2 must equal 1  .3  .4 = .3. define a Neuroticism factor, and FFT would argue that the
Now, consider the factor formed by summing X and Y. set of genes is in fact a (partial) biological basis of Neuroti-
Clearly, cism itself. In that sense, Neuroticism is a factorial cause of
its facets. If we must choose between a pure network and a
ðX þ YÞ ¼ aGX þ aGY þ 2bN þ cE X þ cE Y pure factorial model, the data on heritability favour the latter,
but they do not rule out a mixed model.
The heritability of (X + Y) is due entirely to GX and GY
and is given by
CONCLUSIONS
 
h2 ðXþYÞ ¼ a2 þ a2 = a2 þ a2 þ 4b2 þ c2 þ c2 Personality psychologists are concerned with discovering
¼ ð:4 þ :4Þ=ð:4 þ :4 þ 1:2 þ :3 þ :3Þ ¼ :31 < :4: both explanatory and practical causes for the phenomena

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

with which they are concerned. The discovery of explanatory REFERENCES


causes is particularly challenging for personality psycholo-
gists, because personality traits are not easily manipulated. Baumert, A., Schmitt, M., Perugini, M., Johnson, W., Blum, G.,
With our still-limited understanding of the brain, specifying Borkenau, P., … Wrzus, C. (in press). Integrating personality
the developmental processes through which biology shapes structure, personality process, and personality development.
European Journal of Personality.
BTs is likely to require decades—if, indeed, it can ever be Bollen, K. A., & Pearl, J. (2013). Eight myths about causality and
done at all: biological structures and personality dispositions structural equation models. In S. L. Morgan (Ed.), Handbook
occupy different conceptual levels, and seeking the mecha- of causal analysis for social research (pp. 301–328).
nisms by which the brain produces thoughts, feelings, or en- Dordrecht: Springer, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-
during dispositions is like asking for the mechanisms by 6094-3_15.
Bradley, F. H. (1966). Appearance and reality: A metaphysical es-
which a printing press creates poetry. say (2nd ed.). London: Oxford University Press (Original work
Causal explanations for assimilative processes—to allow published 1897).
the prediction of behaviours—are more tractable, but we Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation, and manipulation. Jour-
normally leave the problem of discovering how attitudes nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1214–1221.
lead to votes, or learning styles to test scores, or irrational https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1214.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G.
thoughts to suicide attempts to specialists in social, educa- (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The
tional, or clinical psychology. Personality psychologists are life and times of individuals differing in the need for cognition.
more likely to be concerned with providing causal explana- Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/
tions for the accommodative processes that create CAs. But 0033-2909.119.2.197.
here equifinality complicates the task: there are many very Carhart-Harris, R. L., Kaelen, M., Bolstridge, M., Williams, T. M.,
Williams, L. T., Underwood, R., … Nutt, D. J. (2016). The par-
different routes by which the antagonistic person may ac- adoxical psychological effects of lysergic acid diethylamide
quire sceptical attitudes, inflated self-regard, and hostile (LSD). Psycholgical Medicine, 46, 1379–1390. https://doi.org/
relationships. 10.1017/S0033291715002901.
In the near future, personality psychologists may be Chapman, B. P., Lyness, J. M., & Duberstein, P. (2007). Personality
better advised to concentrate on seeking practical causes. and medical illness burden among older adults in primary care.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 277–282. https://doi.org/10.1097/
There equifinality is not a problem. Any intervention they PSY.0b013e3180313975.
find that can reliably rechannel anger, or promote better Church, A. T. (Ed) (2017). The Praeger handbook of personality
dietary habits, or increase teamwork is useful. The search across cultures: Vol. 2: Cultures and characteristic adaptations.
for interventions to modify CAs can, of course, be guided Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
by knowledge of personality traits and their typical expres- Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic is-
sues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment,
sion (McCrae, 2011). And personality psychologists 7, 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309.
should take every opportunity to see if people with differ- Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Wetzel, R. D.
ent traits respond differently to particular interventions, (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A
using the classic Trait × Treatment design (McCrae & guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Author.
Sutin, 2007). Conley, J. J. (1984). The hierarchy of consistency: A review and
model of longitudinal findings on adult individual differences in
Traits are plausible explanatory causes of many psycho- intelligence, personality and self-opinion. Personality and Indi-
logical characteristics and behaviour patterns, but because vidual Differences, 5, 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869
traits are not easily modified, these causal explanations (84)90133-8.
do not offer a direct path for intervention. Instead, trait Costa, P. T. Jr., Bagby, R. M., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae, R. R.
psychology is perhaps most widely used in the contexts (2005). Personality self-reports are concurrently reliable and
valid during acute depressive episodes. Journal of Affective Dis-
of selection and diagnosis, where personality assessment orders, 89, 45–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.010.
can be used to predict many desirable occupational and Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalog to classifica-
marital outcomes and many forms of maladjustment. The tion: Murray’s needs and the Five-Factor Model. Journal of Per-
I/O psychologist often has the luxury of choosing the can- sonality and Social Psychology, 55, 258–265. https://doi.org/
didate with an optimal set of personality traits (Costa, 10.1037/0022-3514.55.2.258.
Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2014). The NEO Inventories. In
McCrae, & Kay, 1995). The clinical psychologist must R. P. Archer, & S. R. Smith (Eds.), Personality assessment (2nd
deal with clients’ traits as given, but being able to antici- ed., pp. 223–255). London: Routledge.
pate their strengths and weaknesses can facilitate the pro- Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2017). The NEO Inventories as
cess of therapy (Miller, 1991). instruments of psychological theory. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Ox-
ford handbook of the Five-Factor Model (pp. 11–37). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Costa, P. T. Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Arenberg, D. (1980). Enduring
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS dispositions in adult males. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 38, 793–800. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.
Preparation of this publication was supported by the National 38.5.793.
Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Costa, P. T. Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Holland, J. L. (1984). Personality
and vocational interests in an adult sample. Journal of Applied
Award Number R01AG053297. The content is solely the re- Psychology, 69, 390–400. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.
sponsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent 69.3.390.
the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Robert Costa, P. T. Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Kay, G. G. (1995). Persons,
R. McCrae receives royalties from the NEO Inventories. places, and personality: Career assessment using the Revised

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Career Assessment, 3, personality differences and development. European Journal of
123–139. https://doi.org/10.1177/106907279500300202. Personality, 28, 231–243. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1952.
Cramer, A. O. J., Van der Sluis, S., Noordhof, A., Wichers, M., King, J. E., Weiss, A., & Sisco, M. M. (2008). Aping humans: Age
Geschwind, N., Aggen, S. H., … Borsboom, D. (2012). Dimen- and sex effects in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and human
sions of normal personality as networks in search of equilibrium: (Homo sapiens) personality. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
You can’t like parties if you don’t like people. European Journal 122, 418–427. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013125.
of Personality, 26, 414–431. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1866. Kolar, D. W., Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1996). Comparing the
Deaton, A., & Cartwright, N. (2016). Understanding and misunder- accuracy of personality judgments by the self and knowledgeable
standing randomized controlled trials. National Bureau of Eco- others. Journal of Personality, 64, 311–337. https://doi.org/
nomic Research Working Paper No. 22595. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00513.x.
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Lebedev, A. V., Kaelen, M., Lövdén, M., Nilsson, J., Feilding, A.,
Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from per- Nutt, D. J., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). LSD-induced entropic
sonality neuroscience. Brain structure and the Big Five. Psycho- brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Human
logical Science, 21, 820–828. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Brain Mapping, 37, 3203–3213. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.
0956797610370159. 23234.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between Little, B. R. (1996). Free traits, personal projects and idio-tapes:
facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Three tiers for personality research. Psychological Inquiry, 8,
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880–896. https://doi. 340–344.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. Little, B. R. (2008). Personal projects and free traits: Personality and
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal motivation reconsidered. Social and Personality Psychology
of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246–1256. https:// Compass, 2, 1235–1254. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1246. 2008.00106.x.
Ellis, A. (1987). The impossibility of achieving consistently good Littlefield, A. K., & Sher, K. J. (2012). Smoking desistance and per-
mental health. American Psychologist, 42, 364–375. https://doi. sonality change in emerging and young adulthood. Nicotine &
org/10.1037/0003-066X.42.4.364. Tobacco Research, 14, 338–342. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/
Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most ntr219.
of the people much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Lo, M.-T., Hinds, D. A., Tung, J. Y., Franz, C., Fan, C.-C., Wang,
Psychology, 37, 1097–1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514. Y., … Chen, C.-H. (2016). Genome-wide analyses for personal-
37.7.1097. ity traits identify six genomic loci and show correlations with
Ferguson, C. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of normal and disordered psychiatric disorders. Nature Genetics, 49, 152–156. https://doi.
personality across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social org/10.1038/ng.3736.
Psychology, 98, 659–667. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018770. Long, C. R., & Averill, J. R. (2003). Solitude: An exploration of
Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated benefits of being alone. Journal for the Theory of Social Behav-
view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Jour- iour, 33, 21–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00204.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. https:// Luchetti, M., Barkley, J. M., Stephan, Y., Terracciano, A., & Sutin,
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011. A. R. (2014). Five-Factor Model personality traits and inflamma-
Funder, D. C. (1989). Accuracy in personality judgment and the tory markers: New data and a meta-analysis.
dancing bear. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psy- Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 181–193. https://doi.org/10.
chology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 210–223). 1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.014.
New York: Springer-Verlag, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978- MacLean, K. A., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2011). Mysti-
1-4684-0634-4_16. cal experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to
Funder, D. C. (2016). Taking situations seriously: The situation increases in the personality domain of Openness. Journal of Psy-
construal model and the Riverside Situational Q-sort. Current Di- chopharmacology, 25, 1453–1461. https://doi.org/10.1177/
rections in Psychological Science, 25, 203–208. https://doi.org/ 0269881111420188.
10.1177/0963721416635552. McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new Big Five: Fundamen-
Gottleib, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Sci- tal principles for an integrative science of personality. American
ence, 10, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007. Psychologist, 61, 204–217. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-
00556.x. 066X.61.3.204.
Gurven, M., Von Rueden, C., Massenkoff, M., Kaplan, H., & Lero McCrae, R. R. (2011). Personality traits and the potential of positive
Vie, M. (2013). How universal is the Big Five? Testing the Five- psychology. In K. M. Sheldon, T. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.),
Factor Model of personality variation among forager–farmers in Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward
the Bolivian Amazon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- (pp. 193–206). New York: Oxford University Press, DOI: https://
ogy, 104, 354–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030841. doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.003.0013.
Hartmann, H. (1939). Ego psychology and the problem of adapta- McCrae, R. R. (2015). A more nuanced view of reliability: Specific-
tion. New York: International Universities Press. ity in the trait hierarchy. Personality and Social Psychology Re-
Hill, W. D., Arslan, R. C., Xia, C., Luciano, M., Amador, C., view, 19, 97–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314541857.
Navarro, P., … Penke, L. (2017). Genomic analysis of family McCrae, R. R. (2016). Integrating trait and process approaches to
data reveals additional genetic effects on intelligence and person- personality: A sketch of an agenda. In U. Kumar (Ed.), Wiley
ality. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/106203. handbook of personality assessment (pp. 3–18). Hoboken, NJ:
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Wiley, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119173489.ch1.
Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a McCrae, R. R. (in press). Defining traits. In V. Ziegler-Hill, & T.
cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of Shackelford (Eds.), Sage handbook of individual differences.
personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Los Angeles: Sage.
1556–1565. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1556. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1984). Personality is
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. (2002). Per- transcontextual: A reply to Veroff. Personality and Social Psy-
sonality and leadership: A qualitative and quantitative review. chology Bulletin, 10, 175–179. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 765–780. https://doi.org/ 0146167284102002.
10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1995). Trait explanations in per-
Kandler, C., Zimmerman, J., & McAdams, D. P. (2014). Core and sonality psychology. European Journal of Personality, 9,
surface characteristics for the description and theory of 231–252. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410090402.

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
A Five-Factor Theory perspective

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1996). Toward a new genera- Pervin, L. A. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psy-
tion of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the Five- chological Inquiry, 5, 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Factor Model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The Five-Factor Model s15327965pli0502_1.
of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 51–87). New York: Prudic, J., & Duan, Y. (2017). Electroconvulsive therapy. In B. J.
Guilford. Sadock, V. A. Sadock, & P. Ruiz (Eds.), Kaplan & Sadock’s com-
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2008). The Five-Factor Theory of prehensive textbook of psychiatry (10th ed., pp. 3280–3298), 2.
personality. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer.
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., Pytlik Zillig, L. M., Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (2002).
pp. 159–181). New York: Guilford. What do we assess when we assess a Big 5 trait? A content anal-
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., de Lima, M. P., Simões, A., ysis of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive processes repre-
Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., … Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Age sented in Big 5 personality inventories. Personality and Social
differences in personality across the adult lifespan: Parallels in Psychology Bulletin, 28, 847–858. https://doi.org/10.1177/
five cultures. Developmental Psychology, 35, 466–477. https:// 0146167202289013.
doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.466. Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and envi-
McCrae, R. R., De Bolle, M., Löckenhoff, C. E., & Terracciano, ronmental influences on personality: A study of twins reared to-
A. (in press). Toward an adequate theory of personality: gether using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal
Lifespan developmental processes. In J. F. Rauthmann (Ed.), of Personality, 65, 449–475. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
Handbook of personality dynamics and processes. Amsterdam: 6494.1997.tb00324.x.
Elsevier. Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of
McCrae, R. R., Martin, T. A., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2005). Age trends Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 649–660. https://doi.
and age norms for the NEO Personality Inventory-3 in adoles- org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.649.
cents and adults. Assessment, 12, 363–373. https://doi.org/ Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in
10.1177/1073191105279724. adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,
McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. R. (2007). New frontiers for the Five- 31–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x.
Factor Model: A preview of the literature. Social and Personality Robins Wahlin, T. B., & Byrne, G. J. (2011). Personality changes in
Psychology Compass, 1, 423–440. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review. International Journal
j.1751-9004.2007.00021.x. of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26, 1019–1029. https://doi.org/10.1002/
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the Personality gps.2655.
Profiles of Cultures Project (2005). Universal features of person- Siegler, I. C., Welsh, K. A., Dawson, D. V., Fillenbaum, G. G.,
ality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Earl, N. L., Kaplan, E. B., & Clark, C. M. (1991). Ratings of per-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547–561. sonality change in patients being evaluated for memory disorders.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 5, 240–250. https://
McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., doi.org/10.1097/00002093-199100540-00003.
Jack, C. R., Kawas, C. H., … Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis Stephan, Y., Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., & Terracciano, A. (2016).
of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from Allostatic load and change in personality: A 4-year study. Psy-
the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association chosomatic Medicine, 78, 302–310. https://doi.org/10.1097/
workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. PSY.0000000000000281.
Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7, 263–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Sutin, A. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., Uda, M., Ferrucci, L., Schlessinger, D.,
jalz.2011.03.005. & Terracciano, A. (2010). Personality and metabolic syndrome.
Miller, T. (1991). The psychotherapeutic utility of the Five-Factor Age, 32, 513–519. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-010-9153-9.
Model: A clinician’s experience. Journal of Personality Assess- Swann, W. B. Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality
ment, 57, 415–433. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5703_ into harmony with the self. In J. Suls, & A. G. Greenwald
3. (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self
Milojev, P., Osborne, D., & Sibley, C. G. (2014). Personality (pp. 33–66), 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
resilience following a natural disaster. Social Psychology and Tellegen, A. (1991). Personality traits: Issues of definition, evidence
Personality Science, 5, 760–768. https://doi.org/10.1177/ and assessment. In W. Grove, & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Thinking
1948550614528545. clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul Everett Meehl
Mõttus, R. (2016). Towards more rigorous personality trait– (pp. 10–35), 2. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
outcome research. European Journal of Personality, 30, Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal,
292–303. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2041. N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins
Mõttus, R., Kandler, C., Bleidorn, M., Riemann, R., & McCrae, R. reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social
R. (2017). Personality traits below facets: The consensual valid- Psychology, 54, 1031–1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
ity, longitudinal stability, heritability, and utility of personality 3514.54.6.1031.
nuances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, Terracciano, A., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2004). Smoking and the Five-
474–490. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000100. Factor Model of personality. Addiction, 99, 472–481. https://
Mõttus, R., Realo, A., Allik, J., Esko, T., Metspalu, A., & Johnson, doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00687.x.
W. (2015). Within-trait heterogeneity in age group differences in Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2006).
personality domains and facets: Implications for the development Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality and Social
and coherence of personality traits. PLoS One, 10(): e0119667, Psychology Bulletin, 32, 999–1009. https://doi.org/10.1177/
e0119667. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119667. 0146167206288599.
Nicholls, J. G., Licht, B. G., & Pearl, R. A. (1982). Some dangers of Terracciano, A., Iacono, D., O’Brien, R. J., Troncoso, J. C., An, Y.,
using personality questionnaires to study personality. Journal of Sutin, A. R., … Resnick, S. M. (2013). Personality and resilience
Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 572–580. to Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology: A prospective autopsy
Ostendorf, F., & Angleitner, A. (2004). NEO- study. Neurobiology of Aging, 34, 1045–1050. https://doi.org/
Persönlichkeitsinventar, revidierte Form, NEO-PI-R nach Costa 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.08.008.
und McCrae [Revised NEO Personality Inventory, NEO-PI-R of Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Brant, L. J., & Costa, P. T. Jr.
Costa and McCrae]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. (2005). Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of NEO-PI-R
Pearl, J. (2009). Causality: Models, reasoning, and inference (2nd scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology
ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, DOI: https://doi. and Aging, 20, 493–506. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.
org/10.1017/CBO9780511803161. 20.3.493.

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per
R. R. McCrae and A. R. Sutin

Terracciano, A., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., & Sutin, A. R. (2017). Investment Theory. Social Psychological and Personality Sci-
Cognitive impairment, dementia, and personality stability among ence, 7, 452–462. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616630032.
older adults. Assessment. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/ Vukasović, T., & Bratko, D. (2015). Heritability of personality: A
10.1177/1073191117691844, 107319111769184. meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychological Bulletin,
Turkheimer, E., Pettersson, E., & Horn, E. E. (2014). A phenotypic 141, 769–785. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000017.
null hypothesis for the genetics of personality. Annual Review of Wilson, R. S., Schneider, J. A., Arnold, S. E., Bienias, J. L., & Ben-
Psychology, 65, 515–540. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev- nett, D. A. (2007). Conscientiousness and the incidence of
psych-113011-143752. Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Archives of
Van Scheppingen, M. A., Jackson, J. J., Specht, J., Hutteman, R., General Psychiatry, 64, 1204–1212. https://doi.org/10.1001/
Denissen, J. J. A., & Bleidorn, W. (2016). Personality trait de- archpsyc.64.10.1204.
velopment during the transition to parenthood: A test of Social

Copyright © 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology Eur. J. Pers. (2018)


DOI: 10.1002/per

You might also like