You are on page 1of 5

Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1155e1159

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Shorter communication

Extreme thinking in clinically depressed adolescents: Results from the Treatment


for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS)
Rachel H. Jacobs a, *, Mark A. Reinecke a, Jackie K. Gollan e, f, Neil Jordan a, g, Susan G. Silva b, c,
John S. March c, d
a
Division of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA
b
Duke University School of Nursing, Durham, NC, USA
c
Duke Clinical Research Institute, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA
d
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA
e
Stress and Depression Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA
f
The Northwestern University, Department of Psychology, Chicago, IL, USA.
g
Center for Management of Complex Chronic Care, Hines VA Hospital, Hines, IL, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The purpose of this report is to examine relations between extreme thinking, as measured by the
Received 14 June 2010 Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale, and the maintenance of gains among adolescents who participated in the
Received in revised form Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS). We examine extreme thinking among 327
2 August 2010
adolescents (mean age ¼ 14.56, 57% female, 75% White) who received cognitive behavior therapy (CBT),
Accepted 4 August 2010
fluoxetine (FLX), or a combination of CBT and FLX (COMB). Among those who met remission status on the
Children’s Depression Rating Scale e Revised (CDRS-R  28; 56 at week 12, 79 at week 18) extreme
Keywords:
thinking did not predict failure to maintain remission. This is in contrast to findings with depressed
Major depressive disorder
Adolescents
adults. Treatment influenced level of extreme thinking, and this appeared to be driven by greater
Recovery endorsement of positively valenced beliefs as opposed to a decrease in negatively valenced beliefs.
Cognitive therapy Developmental or investigation characteristics may account for the discrepancy in findings.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Cognitive models of depression (for a review see Clark & Beck, attitude) when compared to those with no extreme scores. Similar
1999) have generated a great deal of research. Recent work points results were reported by Beevers, Keitner, Ryan, and Miller’s (2003)
to the form, as opposed to the content, of dysfunctional thinking (i.e., study of relapse among hospitalized adults who received six
Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002) as increasing an individual’s months of outpatient treatment. In this study, poor cognitive change
susceptibility to depression. For example, higher levels of extreme (defined as one standard deviation below the mean) during depres-
thinking (defined as number of totally agree or totally disagree sion treatment predicted time to relapse of depressive symptoms.
responses) pre- and post-treatment have been found to predict Again, both extreme responses to positive and negative DAS items on
shorter time to relapse among adults (Teasdale et al., 2001). In fact, the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (Weissman & Beck, 1978) predicted
extreme responses represent substantial increased risk, as Teasdale a return of depressive symptoms.
et al. (2001) reported that relapse risk was more than 2.5 times Extreme thinking has not been explored among youth to date.
greater for those who reported just one extreme response (regardless The current investigation examines the effect of extreme thinking
of response valence, i.e. totally disagreeing with a dysfunctional on depression outcome across 36 weeks of treatment within
a large, representative sample of clinically depressed youth. We
hypothesized that pre-treatment extreme thinking scores on the
DAS would predict a return of clinically significant depressive
* Corresponding author. Present address: Columbia University Child Anxiety and symptoms after achieving remission, which we refer to as ‘failure to
Related Disorders, 3 Columbus Circle, New York, NY 10019, USA. Tel.: þ1 212 246 maintain remission status’ (Kennard et al., 2009). We examined this
5740. phenomenon across 36 weeks of treatment among adolescents
E-mail addresses: rj2310@columbia.edu (R.H. Jacobs), m-reinecke@northwestern. who met criteria for remission. We hypothesized that all partici-
edu (M.A. Reinecke), j-gollan@northwestern.edu (J.K. Gollan), neil-jordan@
northwestern.edu (N. Jordan), susan.silva@duke.edu (S.G. Silva), john.march@duke.
pants, regardless of remission status, would demonstrate a reduc-
edu (J.S. March). tion in extreme thinking over the course of treatment.

0005-7967/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.001
1156 R.H. Jacobs et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1155e1159

Methods compliance or adherence. Analyses were conducted on the partici-


pants who remitted from depression during the acute treatment phase
Study participants (n ¼ 56) and during the continuation treatment phase (n ¼ 79).

Four hundred and thirty nine clinically depressed adolescents Children’s Depression Rating Scale e Revised (CDRS-R;
were originally enrolled in the Treatment for Adolescents with Poznanski & Mokros, 1995)
Depression Study (TADS). Details of consent and assent, IRB
approval, rationale, methods, and other aspects of the design are The CDRS-R is a well validated 17-item clinician-rated depres-
given in previous reports (TADS Team, 2003, 2005). The following sion severity measure that was completed by an Independent
analyses are conducted on the 327 youth randomized to an active Evaluator (IE) blind to treatment arm. Scores on the CDRS-R are
treatment arm. Only those youth randomized to active treatment based on interviews with the adolescent and parent and could
who were full or partial responders over Stage I (first 12 weeks of range from 17 to 113, with higher scores representing more severe
treatment) based on the treating clinician’s rating on the Clinical depression. Interrater reliability on the CDRS-R at baseline (intra-
Global Impression - Improvement score (CGI-I; Guy, 1976) class correlation coefficient of .95) and week 12 (intraclass corre-
continued in their randomized treatment arm (COMB, FLX, and lation coefficient of .98) was high (TADS Team, 2005).
CBT; n ¼ 242) during the subsequent six-week consolidation phase
(Stage II). This was followed by an 18 week maintenance phase Extreme thinking
(Stage III). To ensure compatibility with previous adult and pedi-
atric studies, full response was defined as a CGI-I score of 1 (very The DAS (Weissman & Beck, 1978) is a self-report rating scale
much improved) or 2 (much improved) at the end of Stage I; that assesses beliefs associated with vulnerability for depression.
whereas partial response was indicated by a CGI-I score of 3 The scale consists of 40 statements on a 7-point Likert scale.
(minimally improved). Fifty-seven percent of the sample was Although the DAS was originally developed within adult pop-
female and 87% were experiencing their first episode of major ulations, it has been widely used with adolescent samples. Internal
depressive disorder (MDD). The average age at the beginning of the consistency is good and stability is excellent among youth (Garber,
trial was 14.56 (SD ¼ 1.5). Seventy-five percent of participants Weiss, & Shanley, 1993). The extreme thinking variable was first
classified their race/ethnicity status as White; 10% as African computed as a sum of the number of extreme responses (i.e.,
American; 8% as Hispanic white; 2% as Hispanic black; 1% as Asian; “totally agree” or “totally disagree”) given on the DAS at baseline.
and 3% as Other. The modal family income was $50,000 to $74,000, This is the same as the computation methods used in two prior
with a range of less than $5000 to more than $200,000. The flow of studies among adults (Beevers et al., 2003; Teasdale et al., 2001),
participants for this study is outlined in Fig. 1. which included strong endorsement of positive as well as negative
Analyses were conducted on those participants who continued in beliefs (Teasdale et al., 2001). As these phenomena have not been
their assigned treatment arm through week 36 regardless of treatment explored among youth to date, we subsequently analyzed the
positive and negative beliefs separately.

A 439 randomized Definition of remission and return to clinically significant


symptomatology
Stage I Active Treatment Completers
COMB = 86 FLX = 80 CBT = 76
For the current analyses, remission was defined in line with
56 Acute Phase Treatment Completers Met Kennard et al.’s (2009) definition as achievement of a CDRS-R  28.
Remission Criteria
This definition is consistent with previous definitions of remission
FTM = 10 M = 17 FTM = 5 M = 11 FTM = 2 M = 11 in the child and adolescent psychiatry literature (e.g., Emslie et al.,
1997). A return to clinically significant symptomatology, failure to
Acute Phase Analysis Sample maintain remission status, was defined as a CDRS-R score  29 at
any one time point. We do not use the term relapse due to the
17 failed to 39 constrained nature of the assessment schedule maintained in the
maintain maintained TADS. IEs met with participants every six weeks, thus, the exact
timing of relapse was not captured.

B 439 randomized Statistical analyses


Stage I Active Treatment Completers
Descriptive statistics
COMB = 86 FLX = 80 CBT = 76
79 Continuation Phase Treatment Completers
General Linear Models (GLMs) with a posteriori t-tests were
Met Remission Criteria
employed to compare the treatment arms on key baseline clinical
FTM = 12 M = 27 FTM = 8 M = 16 FTM = 2 M = 14
Table 1
Type 3 effects in logistic regression models for acute phase failure to maintain
Continuation Phase Analysis Sample
remission.

Model Wald c2 p AIC


22 failed to 57
maintain maintained Treatment only 1.84 .40 72.63

Treatment and extreme thinking


Treatment 2.20 .33
Fig. 1. (a) Sample size for acute phase analyses. (b) Sample size for continuation phase
Extreme thinking 1.53 .22
analyses. Note. This sample consists of treatment completers. FTM ¼ failure to maintain
Extreme thinking by treatment 0.78 .68 75.97
remission; M ¼ maintained remission; COMB ¼ fluoxetine and cognitive behavior
therapy; CBT ¼ cognitive behavior therapy; FLX ¼ fluoxetine. R square ¼ 0.08.
R.H. Jacobs et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1155e1159 1157

Table 2 18
Type 3 effects in logistic regression models for continuation phase failure to main- 17
tain remission. 16

Mean Extreme Thinking Scores


Model Wald c2 p AIC 15
14 COM B
Treatment only 2.21 .33 96.75
Treatment and extreme thinking 13 FLX
Treatment 3.42 .18 12 CBT
Extreme thinking 1.62 .20 11
Extreme thinking by treatment 2.69 .26 99.55 10

R square ¼ 0.07. 9
8
7
characteristics. When the assumptions of this test were not met,
6
non-parametric KruskaleWallis or WilcoxoneManneWhitney
5
tests were used. Base Wk6 Wk12 Wk24 Wk36
Assessment

Predictor analyses Fig. 2. Extreme thinking by treatment group across 36 weeks. Note. Predicted score
trajectories generated from RRM model.
As a first step, the remission rates reported by Kennard et al.
(2006) were replicated. Generalized Estimating Equations (GEEs)
were employed to compare between-treatment differences in 9.31 (SD ¼ 9.19). Thirteen individuals were missing baseline
remission rates among the 327 adolescents randomized to active extreme thinking scores and the median score was imputed in
treatment and to estimate the probabilities of remission over time these instances. There were no significant differences on extreme
for each treatment arm. Logistic regression analyses were used to thinking scores by gender (c2 ¼ 1.52, p ¼ .22), age (F ¼ 0.31, p ¼ .58),
assess the effects of baseline extreme thinking on failure to main- family income (c2 ¼ 9.71, p ¼ .56), baseline CDRS-R depression
tain remission at subsequent assessment points among two severity (F ¼ 0.74, p ¼ .39), CGAS functioning (F ¼ 0.39, p ¼ .53), or
samples: acute phase (Stage I) remitters and continuation phase presence of comorbidities (c2 ¼ 3.61, p ¼ .46).
(Stage II) remitters.

Predictor analyses
Effect of treatment on extreme thinking
At week 12 remission rates were as follows: 41% COMB; 26%
Random coefficients regression models (RRMs) were employed
FLX; 19% CBT. At week 18 the estimated rates for COMB, FLX, and
to test the effect of treatment on extreme thinking across the 36
CBT were 60%, 42%, and 24%, respectively, whereas at week 36 these
week treatment period. The RRM for this analysis included both
rates were 64%, 60%, and 60%.
fixed (treatment, natural log of time, time-by-time, treatment-by-
Seventeen out of 56 acute phase (Stage I) remitters failed to
time, treatment-by-time-by-time, site) and random (patient,
maintain their remission status across Stages II and III. Twenty-two
patient-by-time, patient-by-time-by-time) effects. Trajectories of
out of 79 of the continuation phase (Stage II) remitters failed to
levels of extreme thinking across the entire 36 week treatment
maintain their remission status over Stage III. Baseline extreme
period were generated.
thinking1 did not predict failure to maintain remission among
All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.1 using commands
either acute phase or continuation phase remitters, as detailed in
such as PROC LOGISTIC, PROC GENMOD, and PROC MIXED. The level
Tables 1 and 2.
of significance was set at .05 for each statistical test. A posteriori
paired comparisons were conducted only if the omnibus test was
significant at the .05 level for the treatment or treatment-by-time Effect of treatment on extreme thinking
effect. As these analyses are considered exploratory, no adjust-
ments were made for the number of statistical tests. The treatment-by-time interaction significantly predicted levels
of extreme thinking across the 36 weeks (F ¼ 4.95, df ¼ 259,
Results p < .01), as did the quadratic treatment-by-time-by-time interac-
tion (F ¼ 3.07, df ¼ 253, p < .05), and site (F ¼ 2.31, df ¼ 312, p < .05).
Descriptive statistics All other effects were not significant. Table 3 details treatment
contrasts and Cohen’s d effect sizes. Fig. 2 depicts change in
At baseline, extreme thinking scores ranged from 0 to 40, with extreme thinking over the course of treatment.
a median score of 6. The mean baseline extreme thinking score was When analyzed separately, positive extreme thinking scores
were also predicted by a treatment-by-time interaction (F ¼ 4.73,
Table 3 df ¼ 324, p ¼ .01), a time-by-time-by-treatment interaction
Effect of treatment on extreme thinking.
(F ¼ 4.20, df ¼ 324, p ¼ .02), and site (F ¼ 3.21, df ¼ 315, p < .01).
Comparison F p d Negative extreme thinking scores were not predicted by a quadratic
Wk12 COMB vs. CBT 19.56 <.01 .69 time-by-time-by-treatment interaction and this term was removed
Wk12 COMB vs. FLX 6.94 <.01 .36 from subsequent models. Negative extreme thinking was predicted
Wk12 FLX vs. CBT 3.41 .07 .31
by time (F ¼ 62.66, df ¼ 324, p < .01) and the interaction of treat-
Wk24 COMB vs. CBT 15.88 <.01 .71
Wk24 COMB vs. FLX 8.13 <.01 .31 ment-by-time (F ¼ 5.81, df ¼ 324, p < .01). These results suggest
Wk24 FLX vs. CBT 1.30 .25 .37 that the increase in extreme thinking across treatment was driven
Wk36 COMB vs. CBT 8.38 <.01 .40
Wk36 COMB vs. FLX 6.09 .01 .34
Wk36 FLX vs. CBT 0.17 .68 .06 1
We also examined baseline DAS total scores as potential predictors and these
Note. COMB ¼ combination of fluoxetine (FLX) and cognitive behavior therapy (CBT). were not significant.
1158 R.H. Jacobs et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1155e1159

Positive Negative
11 4

10
Mean Extreme Thinking Scores

9 3

8
COM B
7 2 FLX
6 CBT

5 1

3 0
Base Wk6 Wk12 Wk24 Wk36 Base Wk6 Wk12 Wk24 Wk36

Fig. 3. Extreme endorsement of positive and negative items on the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. Note. Predicted score trajectories generated from RRM model.

by increases in extreme endorsement of positive, as opposed to many analyses examining such possibilities (e.g., Rohde et al.,
negative, beliefs as illustrated in Fig. 3. 2008), a differential sieve effect in the larger TADS study cannot
be ruled out. In addition, the generalizability of our findings are
uncertain as it has yet to be determined whether these findings are
Discussion
specific to depressotypic cognition or whether they would gener-
alize to extreme thinking as assessed by any self-report measure.
The fact that extreme thinking did not predict failure to main-
Last, we believe that these findings are too preliminary to guide
tain remission among adolescents treated for depression is
clinical practice; however, replication and extension of this line of
surprising given previous evidence that extreme thinking contrib-
work may support clinical application.
utes to relapse for depression among adults (Beevers et al., 2003;
In sum, extreme thinking did not predict a return to clinically
Teasdale et al., 2001).
significant symptoms among a sample of adolescents who were
Differences between the current and previous investigations may
treated for and remitted from depression. This is in contrast to
account for the different result. First, the studies reviewed focused on
reports with adult samples (Beevers et al., 2003; Teasdale et al.,
adults with chronic depression exclusively. For example, Beevers
2001; see Ching & Dobson, 2009 for a failure to replicate). Future
et al.’s (2003) study evaluated adults who were hospitalized for
research examining the effect of medication on cognition is espe-
depression and who had experienced an average of three previous
cially warranted. For example, innovative work has examined how
episodes. They note that “reductions in dysfunctional thought content
the administration of d-Fenfluramine impacts dysfunctional beliefs.
may thus have a more important role in mitigating depression
A study of adults documented a decrease in DAS scores in response
susceptibility among severely depressed people compared to people
to d-Fenfluramine when compared to placebo (Meyer et al., 2003).
with more mild forms of the disorder” (Beevers et al., 2003, p. 493).
Overall, a broad understanding of mechanisms of treatment
Similarly, Teasdale’s (2001) investigation studied adults with residual
response and how they may function across development will be
depressive chronicity, wherein participants had completed a success-
critical in the adequate prevention and amelioration of depression
ful trial of antidepressant medication and were randomized to clinical
that occurs among youth.
management alone or with cognitive therapy. It is possible that indi-
viduals with chronic depression may demonstrate extreme thinking
patterns more frequently. Moreover, evidence suggests that chronic Acknowledgements
depression differs from less chronic manifestations in clinical
presentation and classification (e.g., Mondimore et al., 2007). Perhaps TADS was supported by contract N01 MH80008 from the
extreme thinking is a phenomenon that predicts relapse among National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to Duke University
individuals who have experienced a highly chronic and relentless Medical Center (John S. March, Principal Investigator). Preparation
form of depression. of this manuscript was supported by NIMH fellowship F31
Interestingly, extreme endorsement of negative DAS items MH075308 to Rachel H. Jacobs. The TADS Team Authors: The TADS
decreased over the course of treatment, whereas extreme endorse- is coordinated by the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
ment of positive DAS items increased within the COMB treatment arm. Sciences and the Duke Clinical Research Institute at Duke Univer-
Negative and positive beliefs were not evaluated separately in previous sity Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina, in collaboration with
studies. Thus it is difficult to determine whether developmental the NIMH. The Coordinating Center principal collaborators are John
phenomena may be at play. Some researchers have noted that all-or- S. March, Susan Silva, Stephen Petrycki, John Curry, Karen Wells,
none thinking is normative in childhood (Harter,1999). Further work is John Fairbank, Barbara Burns, Marisa Domino, and Steven McNulty.
needed to determine whether increased endorsements of positively The NIMH principal collaborators are Benedetto Vitiello and Joanne
valenced extreme beliefs are adaptive or maladaptive among youth. Severe. Principal investigators and coinvestigators from the clinical
Our investigation is limited by the small number of adolescents sites are as follows: Charles Casat, Jeanette Kolker, and Karyn Riedal
who failed to maintain their remission status. Moreover, it is (Carolinas Medical Center, Charlotte, North Carolina); Norah Feeny,
possible that the current investigation represents a manifestation Robert Findling, Sheridan Stull, and Susan Baab (Case Western
of the differential sieve effect (Jacobson & Hollon, 1996); wherein, Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio); Elizabeth B. Weller, Michele
despite randomization, participants may have varied in respect to Robins, Ronald A. Weller, and Naushad Jessani (The Children’s
pre-existing characteristics. For example, if different types of Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania); Bruce Was-
patients are likely to complete or respond to a particular inter- lick (Baystate Health/Tufts University, Springfield, Massachusetts),
vention, the acute treatment period may act as a differential sieve Michael Sweeney, and Randi Dublin (Columbia University, New
and produce systematic differences in the sets of patients from the York, New York); John Walkup, Golda Ginsburg, Elizabeth Kastelic,
different treatment groups who continue in the study. Despite and Hyung Koo (The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore,
R.H. Jacobs et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1155e1159 1159

Maryland); Christopher Kratochvil, Diane May, Randy LaGrone, and children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 785e792.
Brigette Vaughan (University of Nebraska, Omaha); Anne Marie
Garber, J., Weiss, B., & Shanley, N. (1993). Cognition, depressive symptoms, and
Albano (Columbia University), Glenn S. Hirsch and Elizabeth Pod- development in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 47e57.
niesinki (New York University, New York); Mark Reinecke, Bennett Guy, W. (1976). ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology (2nd ed.).
Leventhal, Gregory Rogers, and Rachel Jacobs (University of Chi- Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York:
cago, Chicago, Illinois, and Northwestern University, Evanston, Guilford Press.
Illinois); Sanjeev Pathak, Jennifer Wells, Sarah Arszman, and Arman Jacobson, N. S., & Hollon, S. D. (1996). Cognitive-behavior therapy versus pharma-
Danielyan (Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincin- cotherapy: Now that the jury’s returned its verdict, it’s time to present the rest
of the evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 74e80.
nati, Ohio); Paul Rohde, Anne Simons, James Grimm, and Stephenie Kennard, B., Silva, S., Vitiello, B., Curry, J., Kratochvil, C., Simons, A., et al. (2006).
Frank (University of Oregon, Eugene); Graham Emslie, Beth Ken- Remission and residual symptoms after short-term treatment in the Treatment
nard, Carroll Hughes, and Taryn L. Mayes (The University of Texas of Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS). Journal of the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 1404e1411.
Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas); David Rosenberg, Nili Kennard, B. D., Silva, S. G., Tonev, S., Rohde, P., Hughes, J. L., Vitiello, B., et al. (2009).
Benazon, Michael Butkus, and Marla Bartoi (Wayne State Univer- Remission and recovery in the Treatment for Adolescents with Depression
sity, Detroit, Michigan); and Kelly Posner, for the Columbia Study (TADS): acute and long-term outcomes. Journal of the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 186e195.
University Suicidality Classification Group. James Rochon (Duke Meyer, J. H., McMain, S., Kennedy, S. H., Korman, L., Brown, G. M., DaSilva, J. N., et al.
Clinical Research Institute, Durham) is statistical consultant. (2003). Dysfunctional attitudes and 5-HT2 receptors during depression and
self-harm. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 90e99.
Mondimore, F. M., Zandi, P. P., MacKinnon, D. F., McInnis, M. G., Miller, E. B., Crowe, R. P.,
Disclosure
et al. (2007). Familial aggregation of illness chronicity in recurrent, early-onset
major depression pedigrees. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1554e1560.
Susan Silva is a consultant with Pfizer. John March is a consultant Poznanski, E. O., & Mokros, H. B. (1995). Children’s depression rating scale-revised
or scientific advisor to Pfizer, Lilly, Wyeth, GSK, Jazz, and MedAvante manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Rohde, P., Silva, S. G., Tonev, S. T., Kennard, B. D., Vitiello, B., Kratochvil, C. J., et al.
and holds stock in MedAvante; he receives research support from (2008). Achievement and maintenance of sustained response during TADS
Lilly and study drug for an NIMH-funded study from Lilly and Pfizer. continuation and maintenance therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65,
The other authors have no financial relationships to disclose. 447e455.
Segal, Z. F., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy
for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Guilford Press.
References TADS Team. (2003). Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS):
rationale, design, and methods. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Beevers, C. G., Keitner, G. I., Ryan, C. E., & Miller, I. W. (2003). Cognitive predictors of Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 531e542.
symptom return following depression treatment. Journal of Abnormal TADS Team. (2005). The Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS):
Psychology, 112, 488e496. demographic and clinical characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of
Ching, L. E., & Dobson, K. S. (2009). An investigation of extreme responding as Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 28e40.
a mediator of cognitive therapy for depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, Teasdale, J. D., Scott, J., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., & Paykel, E. S. (2001).
48, 266e274. How does cognitive therapy prevent relapse in residual depression? Evidence
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory of from a controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 347e357.
depression. New York: John Wiley. Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978, November). Development and validation of the
Emslie, G. J., Rush, J. A., Weinberg, W. A., Gullion, C. M., Rintelmann, J., & dysfunctional attitude scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper presented at the
Hughes, C. W. (1997). Recurrence of major depressive disorder in hospitalized Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago.

You might also like