You are on page 1of 26

J Bus Psychol

DOI 10.1007/s10869-016-9484-3

ORIGINAL PAPER

Moral Emotions Toward Others at Work and Implications


for Employee Behavior: a Qualitative Analysis Using
Critical Incidents
Michael T. Ford 1 & John P. Agosta 1 & Jingyi Huang 1 & Choe Shannon 1

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Abstract We propose a theoretical framework for and, via Morality can be defined broadly Bas system of rules that facil-
critical incidents, inductively investigate (a) the situations that itate and coordinate group living; as such it involves behav-
trigger moral emotions toward others at work and (b) worker ioral regulation so as to optimize our existence as social
responses to these situations. Critical incidents were collected beings^ (Janoff-Bulman and Carnes 2013; p. 219). There is
from a heterogeneous sample of 423 workers. Participants growing evidence that morality motivates a variety of actions
described an incident that caused each of four moral emotions (e.g., Crockett et al. 2014; Janoff-Bulman et al. 2009; Tabibnia
(gratitude, anger, admiration, and contempt) and their re- et al. 2008) and that emotions play an important role in how
sponse. Incidents and responses were coded, and frequencies, individuals perceive and respond in morally relevant situa-
differences across the moral emotions, and associations be- tions (Haidt 2001; Warren and Smith-Crowe 2008). While
tween incidents and responses were analyzed. Several morally much has been written about the process of moral reasoning
relevant situations at work were found to trigger the moral in organizations (e.g., Hannah et al. 2011), there has been little
emotions. Participants responded in ways that had implica- explicit attention to different types of moral emotions, the
tions for their performance and their own well-being. The situations that trigger these emotions, and how workers re-
incidents largely align with moral foundations theory (Haidt spond. Many work-related experiences such as
and Graham in Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116, 2007). (dis)respectful treatment, pay, and helping behavior trigger
Responses also show evidence for social exchange, social moral emotions toward others such as gratitude, admiration,
learning, and rational expectancy-based processes and behav- anger, and contempt. These emotions have potential to influ-
ior. The incidents specify several practices for managers and ence health and well-being (de Jong et al. 2015; Robbins et al.
organizations to encourage and avoid. This was the first qual- 2012) and play an important role in social exchange relation-
itative study of moral emotions at work and one of the few ships (e.g., Gordon et al. 2012). In this study, we use an in-
workplace studies to examine these emotions empirically. ductive approach to explore the work-related stimuli that elicit
Several understudied factors and behavioral responses moral emotions toward others at work and the ways workers
emerged. respond in these situations.
Moral emotions are the emotions that tend to be triggered
in situations that are morally relevant. From this perspective
Keywords Morality . Discrete emotions . Anger . Gratitude . and definition of morality cited previously (Janoff-Bulman
Admiration . Contempt and Carnes 2013), morally relevant situations occur at work
when individuals or organizations act in a way that influences
John P. Agosta and Jingyi Huang contributed equally to the paper. the well-being and rights of others. Morally relevant actions
typically have an agent and a recipient (Gray and Wegner
* Michael T. Ford 2009). The agent is the person or entity engaging in action
mford@albany.edu that has implications for the well-being or rights of others. The
recipient is the person or group whose well-being and/or rights
1
University at Albany, SUNY, Social Sciences 399, 1400 Washington are helped or harmed by the moral agent. So, for example, if
Avenue, Albany, NY 12222, USA an organization cuts the pay of its employees, this causes some
J Bus Psychol

harm to its employees. The agent in this case would be the exchange theory (Cropanzano and Mitchell 2005), social
organization, and a recipient would be an employee whose learning theory (Bandura 1977), and expectancy theory
pay was cut. Workers may direct their moral emotions in these (Vroom 1964) to develop propositions about the different
situations toward the agent or the recipient. Agent-directed ways workers may respond in these situations We also take
moral emotions include anger or contempt toward the person an inductive exploratory approach toward investigating what
or entity committing the transgression, whereas recipient- these responses are and their relative frequency across the
directed moral emotions might include sympathy or empathy moral emotions. Given the inductive and qualitative nature
toward the employees who were harmed. of this study, we will not directly test the moral emotions as
In this paper, we focus on the moral emotions that are mediators in the effects of work situations on behavior.
directed toward other agents at work, the situations that trigger However, our results will highlight the potential role of these
these moral emotions, and what workers do in response. emotions in eliciting responses to morally relevant (and per-
Moral emotions can be directed toward coworkers, supervi- haps some other) workplace situations.
sors or managers, customers/clients, and/or the organization
itself. Negative moral emotions, namely anger and contempt,
tend to be directed toward agents who violate moral obliga- Moral Emotions Toward Other Agents at Work
tions or standards, whereas the positive moral emotions of
gratitude and admiration tend to be directed toward agents In this investigation, we focus on four types of agent-directed
who display excellence (Haidt 2003). These emotions can be moral emotions that we expect to occur with some regularity
experienced when one is the recipient of the agent’s actions, in organizations, namely gratitude, anger, admiration, and
such as if one is treated disrespectfully, and when one is a contempt. We chose these four emotions to represent positive
third-party observer, such as when a worker watches someone and negative valence as well as different action tendencies,
else being treated disrespectfully. In both situations, the work- drawing heavily from Haidt’s (2003) discussion of moral
er can potentially feel anger or contempt toward the actor. emotion families. The discrete moral emotions and their dif-
Moral emotions also have the potential to influence worker ferences have begun to receive increased theoretical attention
well-being and behavior and thus are mechanisms through (Rudolph and Tscharakstschiew 2014). This work suggests
which morally relevant experiences may influence employee that these moral emotions are Belicited by and directed at
health and performance. actions and characteristics of others (Rudolph and
As Haidt (2003) noted, there are two features that distin- Tscharakstschiew 2014, p. 346) and play a functional role in
guish moral emotions from other core affective states such as facilitating behavior (e.g., Carver and Harmon-Jones 2009;
anxiety, sadness, depression, happiness, or serenity. First, the Rudolph and Tscharakstschiew 2014). These suggestions
moral emotions tended to be elicited by situations in which should apply in the workplace and deserve empirical attention
individuals or organizations influence the well-being of others and scrutiny in organizational settings.
in some way. Other emotions are less specific to these situa- Gratitude is a positive moral emotion that occurs in re-
tions. Second, moral emotions motivate prosocial action ten- sponse to helpfulness from others and inspires a desire to
dencies (Haidt 2003) such as a desire to correct a moral trans- reciprocate. Individuals tend to experience gratitude in re-
gressor, restore justice, or reciprocate acts of kindness. For sponse to helpfulness that is to their benefit and that goes
instance, gratitude can elicit a desire to do something kind in beyond obligatory standards (McCullough et al. 2001).
return, while anger can elicit a desire to punish, correct, or Opposite gratitude is anger, which tends to occur in response
compensate for the wrongdoing. Core affective states such to harm or other moral violations. Anger is often strongest
as depression or happiness are less likely to inspire such when one is harmed by an actor but may also occur in re-
justice-oriented activity. sponse to third-party observations in which moral standards
Considering the potential role of moral emotions in em- are violated (Haidt 2003). Much has been written about these
ployee well-being and behavior, this paper has two goals. two emotions in the general psychology literature (e.g., Carver
First, we take an inductive approach toward identifying the and Harmon-Jones 2009; McCullough et al. 2001; Haidt
types of experiences that lead to moral emotions toward others 2003), but they have received limited attention in empirical
at work. Second, we examine how workers respond to these organizational research.
situations. We use moral foundations theory (Graham et al. Another discrete moral emotion, which we will label admi-
2011; Haidt and Graham 2007) as an a priori framework for ration, represents the Bother-praising^ emotion family in the
the general types of experiences that should lead to the moral moral psychology literature and is elicited by exemplars that
emotions at work. However, using the critical incident method display the best of humanity’s nature (Haidt 2003).
(Flanagan 1954), we take an exploratory approach toward Admiration is the affection felt when observing excellence
investigating the specific types of workplace experiences regardless of its beneficiary. Research suggests that admira-
and their relative frequency. We then draw from social tion is a distinct experience from that of pure joy (Algoe and
J Bus Psychol

Haidt 2009). In situations where one is the beneficiary of any act by an organization or its members that harms or helps
morally commendable actions, one might feel both gratitude others could trigger moral emotions. This implicates working
and admiration toward the actor. For example, if a supervisor conditions, pay, benefits, and various forms of interpersonal
provides help on a work task, the recipient may feel gratitude treatment as potential antecedents to moral emotions.
toward the supervisor while also admiring the supervisor for
this behavior. As such, gratitude and admiration likely share Fairness and Reciprocity The second moral foundation iden-
some antecedents. However, admiration also tends to occur in tified by Graham et al. (2011) is fairness/reciprocity, which
situations in which one is not the beneficiary, such as if a refers to a concern for individual rights and the equality of
supervisor helps someone else. Admiration is therefore likely condition and opportunity. Rai and Fiske’s (2011) taxonomy
to have some antecedents that are distinct from the situations of moral motives also identifies equality of treatment, voice,
that lead to gratitude. opportunity, and expected contributions, along with propor-
The final moral emotion that we describe here is contempt. tionality among costs and rewards, as principles that constitute
Like anger, contempt occurs in response to violations of moral moral behavior from the perspective of fairness. The organi-
obligations (Rozin et al. 1999). However, contempt tends to zational justice constructs of distributive and procedural fair-
be more enduring and entity focused and elicits a distancing ness fall clearly within the moral domain from this perspective
behavioral orientation toward the moral transgressor, whereas (Colquitt et al. 2013a). Informational and interactional justice,
anger is more likely to occur in response to a discrete event which refer to transparency of decision-making processes, and
and tends to elicit an approach response (Fischer and the respect and dignity with which workers are treated
Roseman 2007). There is also evidence that continuous expo- (Colquitt et al. 2013a) also fall under this foundation to some
sure to anger-inducing stimuli from the same entity can turn degree. Constructs that reflect social exchange processes and
into contempt even as the anger recedes (Fischer and reciprocity also fall within the fairness/reciprocity moral foun-
Roseman 2007). An episode of disrespectful treatment from dation and thus may elicit moral emotions toward responsible
a coworker or supervisor may lead to anger temporarily, but if entities. Such constructs include leader-member exchange
such treatment became repeated and routine, this could lead to quality (Graen and Uhl-Bien 1995), which reflects the quality
more enduring feelings of contempt. Others have also argued and balance in the exchange of resources between leaders and
that anger and contempt have distinct social causes their subordinates; perceived organizational support, which
(Hutcherson and Gross 2011), suggesting that contempt de- reflects the extent to which the organization values the contri-
serves attention as a distinct moral emotion. There has been butions of its members and cares about their well-being
little empirical research on contempt among workers in orga- (Eisenberger et al. 1986); and psychological contract fulfill-
nizational settings (e.g., Samnani and Singh 2015; Sanders ment or breach (Conway and Briner 2002; Morrison and
et al. 2015), which we will explore in this study. Robinson 1997), which reflects the extent to which the em-
ployee perceives how the organization has met its obligations
as part of the employee-organization relationship. Studies
Situations that Trigger Moral Emotions at Work have indeed identified anger as a proximal outcome of injus-
tice (Weiss et al. 1999) and a partial mediator explaining ef-
Moral foundations theory (Haidt and Graham 2007) suggests fects of organizational injustice on emotional labor demands
four types of overlapping values that guide moral judgment, (Rupp et al. 2008; Rupp and Spencer 2006). Conversely, ad-
trigger moral emotions, and are prominent in organizational miration has been found to occur in response to fair treatment
life: harm/help, fairness/reciprocity, in-group loyalty, and re- and self-sacrificial behavior on the part of organizational
spect for others (Graham et al. 2011; Rai and Fiske 2011). leaders (Vianello et al. 2010).
Here, we use the theoretical and empirical work of Haidt and
Graham (2007), Graham et al. (2011), and Rai and Fiske (2011) In-group Loyalty This third moral principle, in-group loyal-
to guide our discussion of the types of workplace experiences ty, goes beyond traditional notions of organizational justice
that may trigger gratitude, anger, admiration, or contempt. that preserve the well-being of individuals and includes ac-
tions that influence the integrity and order of the group
Harming and Helping Behaviors, events, and conditions that (Graham et al. 2011; Rai and Fiske 2011). In organizational
involve harming and helping others constitute the most basic settings, this may include poor performance by coworkers,
and fundamental moral value involving the well-being of in- subordinates, or supervisors; a lack of citizenship from orga-
dividuals (Graham et al. 2011; Rai and Fiske 2011). People nizational members; counterproductive work behavior direct-
tend to be angry or contemptuous in response to actions that ed toward the organization; a lack of commitment on the part
harm themselves and others and, conversely, are grateful for of coworkers, subordinates, and supervisors; and withdrawal
and admire actions that help themselves and others in the behavior that hurts the group’s performance and integrity.
pursuit of pleasure and achievement. From this perspective, Excellent performance and citizenship, on the other hand,
J Bus Psychol

helps the group and organization and thus may trigger admi- from others. Such behavior is not contingent on the expecta-
ration and/or gratitude. tion that one will be rewarded in the future. In fact, reciprocity
can lead to negative or harmful behavior toward others (e.g.,
Respect for Others The moral domain also includes respect negative reciprocity), which is arguably less likely to have
for authority, role relationships, and others in general (Graham positive consequences for the responder.
et al. 2011). Respect facilitates cooperative living by helping Social exchange theory has been successful in generating
to maintain order and hierarchy in social groups (Rai and many predictions about organizational behavior. Much of the
Fiske 2011). Respect is also instrumental to the integrity of work on reactions to fair and unfair treatment has used SET as
groups as the structure of social groups depends on ordered a framework, hypothesizing and finding that individuals react
roles and authority relationships. Based on this moral founda- to such treatment by reciprocating (e.g., Colquitt et al. 2013b).
tion, disobedient and counterproductive employee actions to- Social exchange theory has also inspired successful predic-
ward other workers that show disrespect for authority and tions about the target of recipient behavior such that individ-
other role relationships may trigger anger or contempt. uals act favorably toward supervisors when treated fairly by
Disrespectful treatment toward peers and subordinates, such those supervisors, while acting favorably toward the organi-
as the behaviors specified in measures of interactional injus- zation at large when helped by organizational level policies
tice (e.g., Roch and Shanock 2006) or models of incivility and procedures (e.g., Cropanzano et al. 2002; Wayne et al.
(Cortina et al. 2001; Lim et al. 2008), may also fall into this 2002).
category because such treatment threatens the quality of social Emotions such as gratitude were discussed in some of the
roles and relationships. Supporting this notion, Fitness (2000) early literature on social exchange and may play an important
found through interviews that disrespect and humiliation were role in positive exchanges between employees and their
among the most common anger-inducing events experienced coworkers, supervisors, and/or the organization at large.
by workers. Other experiences such as ostracism, abusive su- Blau (1964, p. 16) stated BGenerally, people are grateful for
pervision, and bullying are also relevant to this foundation, favors and repay their social debts, and both their gratitude
because they threaten the quality of role relationships. and their repayment are social reward for the associate who
has done them favors.^ Recent research in social psychology
Proposition 1: Events or conditions at work that involve has suggested that gratitude inspires further helping behavior,
the four moral foundations of help/harm, fairness/reci- facilitating strong social exchange relationships (Gordon et al.
procity, in-group loyalty, and respect for others trigger 2012; Grant and Gino 2010).
the moral emotions of gratitude, anger, contempt, and Alternatively, harmful treatment in organizations has been
admiration. shown to inspire negative reciprocity toward those responsible
(e.g., Mitchell and Ambrose 2007) and given the role of anger
in facilitating vengeful responses to harmful or immoral be-
havior, it seems likely that anger inspires some of this negative
Worker Responses to the Moral Emotions reciprocity. There is also evidence from basic emotions re-
search that anger is an approach emotion and evokes an effort
We draw from three theories to develop a priori propositions to correct the offender, change the offender’s behavior, or even
about how individuals respond to events that trigger these inflict pain or harm on the offender in order to restore justice
moral emotions. The first such theory, social exchange theory (Carver and Harmon-Jones 2009). Spector’s (1978)
(SET) (e.g., Blau 1964; Cropanzano and Mitchell 2005), frustration-aggression model and subsequent research (Fox
proposes that there are ambiguous but powerful rules for be- and Spector 1999) further supports this notion that anger in-
havior in situations in which there is an exchange of resources spires negative reciprocity, leading to counterproductive be-
such as time, money, energy, and other intangibles (for an havior. Other studies (e.g., Fitness 2000; Simon et al. 2015)
early review and critique, see Emerson 1976). Early perspec- have also found that individuals withdraw effort from their
tives on SET took a behavioral approach suggesting that rein- work and/or behave counterproductively when experiencing
forcement contingencies helped maintain the rules for social anger.
exchange relationships, implying that exchange-related be- Based on the general propositions of SET and the action
havior was rational (Blau 1964). For example, one might help tendencies associated with the moral emotions, we expect that
a coworker because he/she expects that the coworker will individuals sometimes respond to situations that trigger grati-
respond in kind, reinforcing the original help. These mutual tude, anger, and contempt by reciprocating. Situations that
expectations and interdependencies render it rational for an trigger gratitude at work should elicit behavior that helps the
individual to reciprocate. Others (e.g., Gouldner 1960) have original actor. This may be in the form of organizational citi-
noted that the norm of reciprocity creates a felt obligation to zenship behavior toward the source of the help (e.g., cowork-
respond in kind when receiving helpful or harmful treatment er, supervisor, or organization) and/or or more effort and better
J Bus Psychol

task performance if the source is the organization as a whole. psychology literature, social learning theory has largely been
Conversely, we expect that anger or contempt, which occurs used to develop and theorize about training programs (Latham
in response to moral violations, inspires workers to respond and Saari 1979; Taylor et al. 2005). However, social learning
with deviant or counterproductive behavior that either (a) principles also apply to naturally occurring events and situa-
takes revenge against the source of the harmful or immoral tions, such as those that trigger the moral emotions discussed
treatment or (b) withdraws from the perpetrator. Such behav- here. Evidence suggests that admiration and the observation of
ior is motivated by the natural justice restoration and recipro- moral excellence is related to nurturing behavior (Silvers and
cation tendencies that accompany moral emotions and create a Haidt 2008), altruistic behavior (Schnall et al. 2010), and
balanced and fair social exchange. The basic action tendencies self-reported organizational citizenship among workers
of anger and contempt would suggest that anger is more likely (Vianello et al. 2010). This suggests that workers sometimes
to elicit approach-oriented, vengeful acts, whereas contempt is copy or mimic exemplary moral acts, especially when
more likely to elicit avoidance and withdrawal from the source experiencing a positive moral emotion toward the actors. It
(Carver and Harmon-Jones 2009; Fischer and Roseman seems less likely that negative moral emotions would inspire
2007). Gratitude is likely to inspire acts that are helpful to the same kind of mimicry given that the negative moral emo-
the person or organization responsible, and these helpful acts tions would render the behavior unpleasant. Thus, we restrict
repay the responsible party that which it is justly due. our application of SLT to the workers’ responses to the posi-
Similarly, anger and contempt inspire justice restorative be- tive moral emotions.
havioral tendencies that punish the transgressor. Admiration
should be more likely than gratitude to occur in response to Proposition 4: Gratitude and admiration elicit attempts to
third-party observations of moral excellence (Haidt 2003) and, copy, mimic, or emulate the source that triggered the
as a result, may play less of a functional role in social ex- emotion.
change relationships. Thus, we are excluding admiration from
this set of propositions inspired by social exchange theory. In some situations, individuals are likely to consider the
potential outcomes of their actions when responding to events
Proposition 2: Gratitude elicits helpful behavior from that elicit moral emotions and, in turn, control their emotional
workers toward the source of the triggering event or impulses to reciprocate. Expectancy theory (Vroom 1964)
situation. helps explain behavior in these situations. This theory sug-
Proposition 3a: Anger and contempt elicit counterproduc- gests that work motivation is a function of (a) the extent to
tive or deviant behavior toward the source of the trigger- which the worker believes he/she can perform the behavior
ing event or situation. (i.e., expectancy), (b) the perceived effect of the behavior on
Proposition 3b: Anger is more likely than contempt to outcomes (i.e., instrumentality), and (c) the value placed on
elicit approach-oriented, vengeful responses toward the the outcomes (e.g., valence). In sum, this theory specifies
source of the triggering incident. rational decision-making bases for work behavior. In the con-
Proposition 3c: Contempt is more likely than anger to text of this study, individuals are more likely to engage in
elicit avoidance-oriented, withdrawal responses toward reciprocation or copying behavior when it is perceived to have
the source of the triggering incident. positive consequences. For this reason, workers are not al-
ways going to negatively reciprocate in response to anger or
While SET may help explain some responses to events and contempt. In many situations, negative reciprocation, which
situations that trigger moral emotions, there are some re- would be predicted by SET, is likely to result in negative
sponses for which SET principles may be insufficient. Social consequences for the worker. Expectancy theory suggests that
learning theory (SLT) (Bandura 1977) yields some additional workers do consider these consequences of their actions when
predictions. Social learning theory builds on Skinner’s (1963) choosing how to respond. As a result, a worker is unlikely to
principles of operant conditioning to suggest that individuals engage in negative reciprocation if it is toward an individual
are motivated to behave not only by reinforcement but also by with power to punish or toward a coworker with whom one
the observation of others. According to SLT, individuals learn must cooperate. For example, if a supervisor treats an employ-
in part by observing, remembering, and reproducing the ac- ee poorly, the employee may not reciprocate with counterpro-
tions of others. When others, sometimes termed behavioral ductive behavior toward the supervisor because this might
Bmodels^, act in ways that bring positive consequences, indi- result in negative consequences such as being fired or not
viduals are motivated to mimic the behavioral models and getting a raise. Instead, employees may engage in more con-
behave in similar ways. The pleasantness of gratitude and structive responses such as complaining through official chan-
admiration may reinforce and help individuals remember the nels or attempting to respectfully correct the behavior. They
eliciting events and further inspire individuals to copy the may also vent their frustration to others outside of the organi-
behavior of exemplars at work. In the organizational zation, who have less power to harm them. Indeed, research
J Bus Psychol

suggests that workers displace supervisor-directed anger by situations in their own words without imposing an a priori
undermining or acting abusively toward other individuals, theoretical structure on their responses. We developed catego-
such as subordinates or spouses, who have less power over ries of incidents and responses to these incidents through a
them (Hoobler and Brass 2006). Given the role of expectan- coding procedure to be described further in the following
cies in behavior in these situations, it is likely that individuals sections.
will engage in attempts to improve the situation when they We took a qualitative, inductive approach for two reasons.
have enough power to suggest their efforts will make a differ- First, these moral emotions, particularly gratitude, admiration,
ence. When individuals do not perceive they have the power and contempt, have received very little attention in the orga-
to change the situation, they will have little expectancy that nizational science literature. Thus, there is not much of an
they will be rewarded for attempts to change the situation and empirical research base to build on. Because of this lack of
instead engage in more emotion-focused responses such as empirical research, we aimed to better understand the types of
displacing and venting their anger. events and situations that trigger these potentially potent mor-
al emotions. A more deductive and quantitative approach in
Proposition 5a: When employees perceive that they have which we measured pre-defined variables would be limited to
the power to improve the situation, anger and contempt the constructs specified in previous theories. Our goal here
will trigger responses that involve attempting to improve was to potentially uncover new theoretical relationships be-
the situation constructively. tween specific categories of workplace situations and moral
Proposition 5b: When employees perceive that they do emotions. This approach also has potential to expand the pool
not have the power to improve the situation, their anger of variables and constructs to be studied in future research.
and contempt will trigger displacement or venting of their Second, the narrative descriptions that are common in in-
emotions toward others not involved in the situation. ductive qualitative research provide important details on
work-related experiences that can help build better theory
In summary, because no one theory is sufficient for gener- while also providing concrete examples that are of practical
ating propositions about the range of potential behavioral re- value. Recent work on work-family conflict (Shockley and
sponses to incidents that trigger moral emotions, we have Allen 2015) and affective work events (Ohly and Schmitt
chosen to discuss three different theoretical bases. These three 2015) has taken a narrative episodic approach in which par-
bases account for (a) rational motives for behavioral re- ticipants described the details of specific situations at work.
sponses, as specified by expectancy theory; (b) motives to These narrative descriptions can be useful for building and
reciprocate without necessarily considering rewards or pun- developing new theory while discovering important
ishments, as specified by social exchange theory; and (c) be- contextual variables and internal psychological states. As
havioral modeling, which accounts for behavior change another example, Taber and Deosthali (2014) were able to
through observation and social learning. We expect responses identify new motives for helping behaviors through a qualita-
to the moral emotions that are consistent with each of these tive, inductive approach based on narrative descriptions of
three theories. participants. We expect that an inductive, qualitative approach
As noted earlier, this study takes an inductive approach to studying incidents that trigger moral emotions and re-
toward understanding the types of situations that trigger moral sponses to these emotions will yield information about new
emotions toward others at work and responses to these situa- theoretical relationships and contribute to future theory build-
tions. Thus, we do not have any formal hypotheses. However, ing. Yet, it is still possible to link these findings to constructs
the basic tenets of moral foundations theory, SET, SLT, and specified in existing theory on moral emotions and behavior.
expectancy theory give rise to some general propositions. We We also believe that this approach will have practical impli-
anticipate that workers tend to reciprocate behavior that trig- cations for behaviors and practices to encourage and avoid in
gers positive and negative moral emotions, copy or emulate organizations, given the concrete nature of the narrative de-
behavior that triggers positive moral emotions, and also con- scriptions and examples.
sider the expected outcomes when acting on negative moral
emotions. Participants

We contracted an online research support firm, Qualtrics.com,


Method to recruit participants from an online survey panel. Qualtrics
uses social network advertising, online banners, and direct
We used the critical incident technique (Flanagan 1954) to messaging to recruit individuals for their panels. These
collect data on the types of workplace situations that elicit panelists are compensated for participation in studies with
the moral emotions of gratitude, anger, admiration, and con- online gift certificates. This method gave us the chance to
tempt. This technique allows participants to describe these sample experiences from a variety of organizations,
J Bus Psychol

occupations, and locations, and offered greater anonymity what did you do?^ We asked these questions four times,
than more traditional recruitment methods through inserting the following emotions into each respective ques-
organizations. We hoped that the perceived anonymity tion: grateful or thankful, angry or furious, fondness or admi-
would result in greater candor and willingness to disclose ration, and contempt or disapproval. This procedure was to
negative information. Concerns about random responding in result in up to 1692 incidents, four per person.
online studies are also less of an issue with the critical incident We coded only the usable incidents. We did not code inci-
method than with close-ended responses, because random re- dents in which participants wrote Bnothing^ or some type of
sponses to open-ended questions are easily detected and random response or if participants indicated that they had
discarded. never experienced that emotion at work. Ultimately, there
Responses were collected from 423 participants. These par- were 398 incidents coded for gratitude, 335 incidents coded
ticipants were all working in the USA. Of these, 53.7% were for anger, 293 incidents coded for admiration, and 278 inci-
women, 7.9% identified themselves as Hispanic or Latino, dents coded for contempt. The number of words describing
83.2% were white, 8.4% were black or African American, the incidents for gratitude, anger, admiration, and contempt
7.1% were Asian, and 1.3% were of another race. The age were 5951, 5603, 4446, and 4748, respectively. Thus, the
of participants ranged from 18 to 65, with an average of number of words per usable incident was 14.95, 16.73,
39.8, and participants worked an average of 42.6 h per week 15.17, and 17.08. This is evidence that although the number
with a range of 30 to 84 and a standard deviation of 6.76 h per of responses decreased over the course of the study, the quality
week. of the answers, at least with respect to their length, did not
Participants held a variety of occupations. In order to gain decrease across the incidents.
information about the types of jobs that the participants held,
we asked participants to provide a brief narrative description Coding
of their job duties. We were able to categorize 352 of the
participants into a two-digit category using the Standard Our coding approach was problem focused (Krippendorff
Occupational Classification (SOC) system (http://www.bls. 2013) in that the analysis of texts was driven by an interest
gov/oes/current/oes_stru.htm). Of the participants who in the situations that trigger moral emotions and behavioral
provided usable information, 13.4% were in management; responses. The unit of analysis was the incident or response,
9.9% were in business/financial operations; 9.1% were in a and thus the units were defined prior to the analysis.
computer/mathematical occupation; 0.6% were in First, we developed categories of incidents inductively and
architecture/engineering; 2.3% were scientists; 6.5% were in iteratively as a research team. Then, we revisited the responses
social/community service, 0.3% were in a legal services oc- and placed them into the categories that had been developed,
cupation; 5.1% were educators; 1.7% were in arts and enter- allowing us to refine the category definitions that emerged
tainment; 5.1% were healthcare practitioners/technicians; 1.4 from the data and to identify potentially new categories that
% were in healthcare support; 2.3% were in protective service; we needed to create. This approach followed the approach
3.7% were in food preparation and serving; 1.4% were in used in critical incident analysis. Critical incident analysis
building and grounds maintenance; 0.9% were in personal typically involves assembling a set of analysts to review and
care; 8.2% were in a sales-related occupation; 15.3% were in categorize the incidents independently or as a group, develop-
office/administrative support; 0.3% were in farming/fishing; ing a common set of categories, and then developing a de-
3.1% were in construction/extraction; 2.0% were in installa- scription of each category (Anderson and Wilson 1997). To
tion, maintenance, and repair; 5.4% were in production; and align with the critical incident method as it is typically
2.0% were in a transportation and moving occupation. employed, we used the judgments of multiple human coders
rather than a computerized text analysis program.
Measures The incidents were first distributed to members of the first
author’s research team, including coauthors on the study and
Using the critical incident method for each moral emotion, other volunteer research assistants, all of whom were graduate
participants were instructed, BWe would like you to think of or undergraduate students in psychology. The research assis-
an event or situation at work that caused you to feel <insert tants and the first author attempted to develop categories that
emotion>. This could be something that happened just once or captured the essence of all of the responses. To this end, the
it could be something that is ongoing that still causes you to first author read all responses for each moral emotion and
feel this way.^ Then participants were asked, BWhat is/was the developed categories of incidents. Research assistants devel-
event or ongoing situation at work that caused you to feel oped categories of incidents independently by analyzing a
<insert emotion>? What happened to make you feel <insert subset of the incidents. All incidents were reviewed by at least
emotion>?^ Finally, participants were asked, BDid you do one of the research assistants in addition to the first author.
anything in response to feeling <insert emotion>, and if so, The first author then integrated these category lists by
J Bus Psychol

combining overlapping categories and adding unique catego- for contempt. These indices would be considered good or
ries to the overall list. These categories were developed induc- substantial agreement by the benchmarks referenced earlier
tively and not necessarily with respect to the moral founda- (Altman 1991; Landis and Koch 1977).
tions listed in proposition 1. Our categories were more con- As a final step in the coding process, the first three authors
crete and specific than the moral foundations. held group meetings and discussed every incident and re-
After these categories were established, the first author and sponse on which there was any disagreement in coding and
one of the three coauthors independently coded each incident categorization. Each incident on which there was any dis-
once again by reviewing each incident and selecting the cate- agreement was discussed, and a final coding decision was
gories to which the incident belonged. Extensional lists, which made by consensus as a group. Thus, even though the kappa
specify the types of situations and responses that define each values were in acceptable ranges, further discussion was used
of the categories (Krippendorff 2013), were created for this to resolve disagreements in coding and develop a reliable set
coding process. There were a total of four coders, the first of final codes on which either (a) the coders agreed upon
author and three coauthors, involved in this part of the process. independently or (b) the group agreed upon after discussion.
Incidents could belong to more than one category and in some
cases were assigned up to four or five categories. The vast
majority of incidents had one or two codes assigned to them. Results and Discussion
The first author ultimately combined the codes after reviewing
the coauthor’s codes and made a final coding decision. Most In generating incidents and response categories, we combined
of the incidents that did not fall into an existing category were the categories for the two positive moral emotions as well as
not work related. Examples of incidents that were not work the categories for the two negative moral emotions. There
related included events involving one’s own health or family. were almost no categories that were entirely exclusive to one
As an additional coding step, each of the incidents was of the moral emotions, and thus we analyzed the positive and
once again independently coded by one of the coauthors in negative moral emotions together while comparing the rela-
order to compute an interrater agreement index and to further tive frequency of each incident and response for each emotion.
refine the coding decisions. These codes were compared to the In Table 1, we report the categories of incidents that we de-
codes developed in the previous process in which the first veloped for the positive moral emotions alongside a brief de-
author made the final decision. We then computed the scription of the category, some illustrative quotes from partic-
Cohen’s kappa (Cohen 1960) for the original codes and the ipants, and the number and percentage of incidents for each
new codes. We used Cohen’s kappa because there were two moral emotion that were coded into that category. Table 2
sets of codes for each moral emotion: the codes assigned by provides the same information on the responses to the positive
the first author and the codes assigned by one of the coauthors. moral emotions. Tables 3 and 4 provide the same information
Thus, there were only two coders for each moral emotion, for the negative moral emotion incidents and responses.
rendering Cohen’s kappa the appropriate index for assessing Finally, in Tables 5 and 6, we report the relationship between
coding reliability. The Cohen’s kappa for usable incidents was the specific types of events and the responses when possible.
0.78 for gratitude, 0.54 for anger, 0.65 for admiration, and Rather than repeating all of the detailed results displayed in
0.59 for contempt. Cohen’s kappa values of around 0.60 have the tables, we instead provide a brief summary of the positive
been considered acceptable in coding qualitative data and negative moral emotion incidents and responses. Then,
(Schonfeld and Mazzola 2015). Furthermore, available bench- we discuss their implications for theory and practice.
marks suggest that kappa values of 0.41 to 0.60 demonstrate
moderate agreement and those from 0.61 to 0.80 demonstrate Positive Moral Emotion Incidents and Responses
good or substantial agreement (Altman 1991; Landis and
Koch 1977). We found 20 categories of incidents that triggered the positive
A similar process was used to code the behavioral re- moral emotions (see Table 1). Generally speaking, participants
sponses to the incidents. First, each set of responses to each reported gratitude and/or admiration in response to (a) receiv-
moral emotion was split such that one coauthor read half of the ing or observing recognition from the organization; (b) having
behavioral responses and another coauthor read the other half. a job with good working conditions, benefits, or opportunities
Each coder came up with categories of responses. The first for promotion and development; (c) good task-related experi-
author combined these categories into two lists, one for the ences such as being able to perform meaningful work and
positive and one for the negative moral emotions. Then, two having adequate resources, (d) the performance and success
coauthors independently coded each behavioral response and of colleagues, supervisors, and subordinates; (e) support from
interrater agreement was computed using the Cohen’s kappa leaders, coworkers, or subordinates; (f) the competence of
index. The Cohen’s kappa index for the usable responses was leaders and the organization; and (g) observations of moral
0.78 for gratitude, 0.67 for anger, 0.75 for elevation, and 0.71 excellence.
J Bus Psychol

Table 1 Categories, descriptions, examples, and frequencies of incidents for the positive moral emotions

Category Description Examples Gratitude Admiration Chi-


square
# % # %

Recognition
Non-pay recognition • Recognition for good performance that does BBeing recognized for the effort and time that 77 19.3a 40 13.7 3.89a
not involve pay. Examples included was put in analyzing some production data
receiving thanks, praise, or a nonmonetary that will be used going forward.^
reward BMy boss said I was doing a good job at work.^
Pay-based recognition • Any type of pay-based recognition, including BI am very thankful when I get a bonus at work, 46 11.6a 6 2.0 21.93a
raises, bonuses, tips, and gift cards this told me that my boss is happy with job I
am doing.^
BI received a bonus for a special project I
volunteered to help with. It was unexpected
and was very much appreciated. Made me
think a lot of the company I work for.^
Observing others being • Seeing others receive recognition in the BWhen management shows their appreciation 1 0.3 7 2.4a 6.74a
recognized organization for its employees and all their hard work by
buying lunch or having a BBQ.^
BSomeone does their job so well, they get
rewarded for it, recognition.^
Job and career related
Having a job • Having a job, not being laid off or furloughed BSimply having a job makes me thankful when 77 19.3a 6 2.0 47.78a
I read the news and see the difficulties others
are having in this world.^
BSeeing others laid off.^
Promotion and • Receiving a promotion, training, and/or BI got a promotion.^ 26 6.5 11 3.8 2.57
development challenging developmental tasks BI worked very hard on a spreadsheet and
opportunities numerous other assignments and I got a
promotion. I also got a raise, since I was
promoted, when my family needs it most.
Benefits and working • Having good working conditions and BThey let me off work to take care of my little 26 6.5a 6 2.0 7.69a
conditions, benefits. Examples include sick time, health girl when she got out of the hospital.^
non-task-related insurance, holidays, and flexible work BWe have something called floating holidays.
aspects of work arrangements This means we get one of the following off
in addition to regular vacation and sick time:
day after Thanksgiving, Christmas eve, or
New Year’s eve.^
Work task related
Desirable work tasks and • Getting to do something enjoyable, being able BBeing able to help someone whose home had 35 8.8a 9 3.1 9.27a
responsibilities to help people, and receiving desired been burned down.^
assignments BI feel grateful that I have a job that allows me
to be creative and try new things.^
Sense of • A feeling of accomplishment for major or BCompleted a major project.^ 27 6.8a 5 1.7 9.85a
accomplishment and difficult tasks. Examples include closing BWinning a case for a client.^
achievement business deals, helping others successfully, BSaved a 9-year-old from drowning.^
and improving one’s performance
Interactions with • Positive interactions with students, clients, or BI was helping my students study for a timed 40 10.1a 17 5.8 4.02a
external beneficiaries of other customers test and was able to give them strategies to
one’s work remember some of the facts they were
having trouble remembering. When they
took their timed test, they got all of the
questions right.^
BSpeaking with clients on a daily basis in
making a small difference in the world.^
Having the resources to • Having the necessary equipment, information, BNew equipment provided that made my job 7 1.8 2 0.7 1.52
do one’s work and technical support easier.^
BIt was a computer malfunction. The guy went
out of his way to get it fixed quickly.^
Performance and success of others
Performance and success • Observing peers succeeding, performing well, BA coworker was promoted for years on the job 4 1.0 38 13.0a 42.32a
of colleagues and/or being promoted and outstanding work habits.^
BAdmiration for a colleague who is juggling a
new baby, marriage, and work.^
Performance and success • Observing someone vertically higher in the BWhen the director at my job was promoted to 0 0.0 13 4.4a 18.00a
of one’s supervisor, organization succeeding, performing well, VP, I felt really proud of him. I really
mentor, and/or leader and/or being promoted
J Bus Psychol

Table 1 (continued)

Category Description Examples Gratitude Admiration Chi-


square
# % # %

admired all his hard work and his


dedication.^
BI admire this one supervisor I have because
she works hard at work and then goes home
and takes care of her kids by herself.^
Performance and success • Observing subordinates, trainees, and/or BWatching a young man I trained do a great 2 0.5 11 3.8a 9.67a
of subordinates students succeeding, performing well, job.^
and/or being promoted BWhen my blind student did well on a braille
test.^
Support from others and the organization
Supervisor, leader, • Receiving help and emotional support from BI have my immediate supervisor to be thankful 38 9.5 32 10.9 0.35
and/or mentor support supervisors, trainers, or leaders in the for because he listens to us as employees,
organization says thank you, and lets us know we are
doing a good job. If it wasn’t for him, I
would have quit.^
BMy boss asked me for a favor, she asked me to
take Friday off to take care of myself.^
Coworker and • Receiving help and emotional support from BSomeone volunteering to help me out on a 63 15.8 47 16.0 0.01
subordinate support coworkers, teammates, and/or subordinates project that was really burning me out. Most
people don’t volunteer to help, so I was very
thankful and blessed when this person did.^
BMy daughter passed away unexpectedly a few
years back. Of course, everyone was very
kind because they all knew her. The office
closed and everyone was allowed to attend
the funeral. Then throughout the shiva
period, a different person from the office
would come to my home to see us and one
morning the office actually sent over
breakfast for my whole family. It was very
thoughtful and much appreciated.^
Communication from • Receiving information from the organization BHaving my questions answered promptly and 2 0.5 2 0.7 0.10
the organization clearly.^
BOur CEO giving a quarterly update.^
Competence of others and the organization as a whole
Effective leadership on • Having leaders that are competent and BThe continuing dedication of my direct 6 1.5 22 7.5a 15.63a
task-related issues effective and that make sure that supervisor.^
subordinates keep their jobs BMy supervisor went to bat for our department
and refused to lay off anyone.^
BWatching my supervisor complete tasks.^
General positive • Generic positive feeling toward the BI admire the company I work for.^ 0 0.0 2 0.5 2.73
evaluation of the organization BMy company being a leader.^
organization
Observations of moral excellence
Third-party observations • Seeing others being helpful and supportive BWhen I watch people having a rough day and 8 2.0 40 13.7a 35.39a
of others’ moral someone makes them smile or laugh to cheer
excellence them up.^
BA coworker has stepped up and is leading a
drive to help the affected family.^ (referring
to a former coworker who had lost health
insurance)
BA coworker adopted a little special needs
boy.^
Socially responsible • Organization that is socially responsible BOur company hosted a huge fundraiser for 2 0.5 6 2.0 3.52
organization charity. We had such good results.^
BMy organization produces a lot of cutting edge
research to promote conservation.^

B#^ refers to the number of incidents that fell into the category
a
There was a significant difference across the moral emotions in the likelihood of discussing an incident that fell into the category. The superscript letter a
is placed under the moral emotion for which the incident was the most likely to appear
J Bus Psychol

Table 2 Categories, descriptions, examples, and frequencies of responses for the positive moral emotions

Category Description Examples Gratitude Admiration Chi-


square
# % # %

Reciprocation
Thanked the source • Said thanks or showed appreciation BI thanked my boss.^ 150 50.8a 74 34.4 13.63a
through body language BI wrote a thank you note.^
BI let them know how grateful I felt.^
Reciprocated beyond • Did something nice for the source, BI took one of his hardest tasks a few days later.^ 46 15.6 46 21.4 2.83
thanks usually BI always help them out in return because I feel grateful
in the form of personal favors or to them.^
gifts BGave a gift and a card to the person.^
Gave praise • Praised, congratulated, BComplimented him on how he handled the situation.^ 7 2.4 57 26.5a 66.03a
complimented, BI told them that I thought their idea was excellent.^
and/or encouraged the source BTold her how impressed I was.^
Spent more time with • Spent time with the source BContinued the relationship.^ 6 2.0 7 3.2 0.75
the source BHad a picnic with them.^
BI spoke with them for the time I was able to and got to
know them.^
Prosocial behavioral responses
Did something nice for • Did something nice for someone BThanked everyone and gave my staff a bonus.^ 21 7.1 10 4.7 1.33
others other than BI bring in treats for my officemates.^
the source. Examples included BTried to be nicer to people.^
helping family
members, coworkers, and friends;
Bpaying it forward^
Tried to copy or • Tried to behave more like the person BI tried to learn a lesson and inspire myself.^ 4 1.4 13 6.0a 8.49a
emulate the source who BI tried to be more like her, she is a great example.^
triggered the emotion BI try to emulate what they do in my interactions with
those I supervise.^
Performance-related responses
Increased effort toward • Increased work effort, citizenship, and BI put in extra effort on a side project that I was asked to 84 28.5 51 23.7 1.44
work commitment toward the group and assist my boss with.^
organization BI just come to work in a better mood and feel like I am
more productive because of it.^
BI worked harder to keep my projects on time.^
BI worked extra hours to show my appreciation.^
Emotion-focused responses
Savored the emotion • Enjoyed or celebrated the incident BCelebrated with a dear friend at a restaurant.^ 10 3.4 8 3.7 0.04
that triggered BTreated my family and coworker out for dinner.^
the emotion BWent out for drinks.^
Treated oneself • Treated oneself, usually with BWent out to eat with the first check.^ 9 3.1a 1 0.4 4.33a
resources provided BI spent the money on me.^
by the incident (e.g., money from a
raise)
Expressed positive • Expressed the emotion to others BI told them how happy and excited I was.^ 19 6.4 11 5.1 0.39
emotions verbally or nonverbally BI smiled and thanked them.^
(e.g., smiled, told someone how BI hugged her so tightly.^
happy they were)
Prayed • Prayed in response to the incident BI pray every day and go to church whenever I can.^ 10 3.4 2 0.9 3.28
BI thanked God.^

B#^ refers to the number of responses that fell into the category
a
There was a significant difference across the moral emotions in the likelihood of discussing a response that fell into the category. The superscript letter a
is placed under the moral emotion for which the response was the most likely to appear

We conducted chi-squared tests to determine whether an contrast, the performance and success of colleagues, leaders,
incident was more likely to be reported for one of the positive and subordinates; and third-party observations of moral excel-
moral emotions than the other. We found that non-pay- and lence were more common incidents for admiration than for
pay-based recognition, good benefits and working conditions, gratitude. This is consistent with the notion that gratitude is
desirable work tasks, a sense of accomplishment, and positive more common in response to situations that directly benefit
interactions with students, clients, or customers were more the individual, whereas admiration is more likely than grati-
common incidents for gratitude than for admiration. By tude to occur in response to situations with no direct benefit.
J Bus Psychol

Table 3 Categories, descriptions, examples, and frequencies of incidents for the negative moral emotions

Category Description Examples Anger Contempt Chi-


square
# % # %

Unfair treatment
Distributive or • Unfair decision outcomes and/or processes BI make the lowest level pay and have to do a 17 5.9 19 8.2 1.03
procedural injustice higher skilled job.^
BPeople making the same amount of money
as me and having to do less work.^
BBeing delegated to a task without my input
when I was already overloaded with
work.^
Third-party • Observing others treated unjustly BWatching someone get accolades for 6 2.1 9 3.9 1.47
observations of something they didn’t do.^
unfair treatment BTo see a coworker reprimanded after being
lied on by a student.^
Politics, general • Organizational politics, nepotism, and unspecified BAbuse of power.^ 3 1.0 7 3.0 2.65
unfairness unfairness BBackstabbing.^
BThose who are in the ‘IN’ crowd chose to
isolate me.^
False accusations • Being accused of doing something that one did not BI was accused of stealing cash.^ 11 3.8 4 1.7 2.03
do BReceiving blame for something I did not
do.^
BAnother employee made a mistake and I
was blamed for it before it was
investigated.^
Unethical behavior by • The supervisor swearing, taking credit for one’s BMy boss takes credit for my work.^ 8 2.8 13 5.6 2.63
the supervisor work, spreading rumors, and displaying a lack of BA supervisor went out of their way to
integrity prevent me from getting a promotion.^
BContempt for a boss who gave a—kissers
special privileges and other people were
not noticed just for their hard work.^
BIn several instances, I have discovered that
a supervisor has lied to me or to others in
our organization.^
Job/career-related issues
Terminations, layoffs,• Observing or experiencing terminations, layoffs, or BI felt disapproval when the business started 6 2.1 16 6.9a 7.31a
and furloughs furloughs firing people.^
BI disapproved of the way they handled the
layoff situation.^
BLayoffs, they laid off the wrong people.^
BThe government shutdown and possibility
of being furloughed.^
Compensation and • Not getting paid well, not getting raises, pay cuts, BNo annual raises given to anyone due to the 18 6.3 16 6.9 0.08
benefits unpaid overtime, and a lack of benefits bad economy.^
BCommission plan was changed in the
middle of the quarter, which did not
benefit me.^
BOur company wants us to work overtime
for free.^
BMy work is cheap. No raises and bosses
suck.^
BThey reduced my pay.^
Lack of promotion and • The denial of opportunities for promotion, growth, BI did not get a promotion.^ 8 2.8 4 1.7 0.64
development and development BHaving me train as a teller after being the
opportunities main person to bring in loans.^
BPromised a new position and never got it.^
Lack of support
Lack of support from • A perception that the organization does not value BWhen the company is selfish.^ 12 4.2 17 7.4 2.42
the organization employees’ contributions or care about employees’ BDisapproval of how employees are being
well-being treated by some management.^
BManagement forcing me to work with
injuries.^
Lack of supervisor • Not receiving help or emotional support from the 44 15.4 24 10.4 2.79
support supervisor; being Bthrown under the bus,^
J Bus Psychol

Table 3 (continued)

Category Description Examples Anger Contempt Chi-


square
# % # %

humiliated, or threatened by the supervisor; BI was reported by a supervisor because I


receiving a poor performance evaluation from the told her I was unable to perform the
supervisor work.^
BOne of our workers was called out by
management for making a mistake in
front of everyone.^
BMy boss second guesses my work and then
repeatedly lectures me.^
BWhen a manager was firing a new
employee after a few weeks on the job and
she held this employee up for ridicule in
front of other employees.^
BMy supervisor answered me in a snippy
way at our staff meeting.^
Poor performance and mismanagement
Poor organizational • Poor performance and management at the group or BManagement, they micromanage and make 27 9.4 23 10.0 0.04
performance and organizational level. This can include poor change everything much harder than it has to be.^
mismanagement implementation, micromanagement, bureaucracy, BDifficult projects that don’t seem to have
poor financial performance, bad strategic the ability to succeed.^
decisions, and poor decision-making processes BOur computers are always down and our
new ‘non personal’ way of doing business
makes our clients very upset.^
BMajor department change/move with no
regard to opinion of those affected.^
Supervisor • Poor supervision. Examples include receiving BSupervisors telling you you did something 35 12.2 20 8.7 1.72
incompetence and inappropriate task assignments, conflicting wrong when you came up with a
poor performance instructions, unrealistic expectations, supervisors solution.^
not doing their jobs, and supervisors not rewarding BOne coworker is constantly given tasks that
subordinates she can’t do.^
BWatching my boss not get work done.^
BManagement looking the other way when
subordinates leave early, report hours not
worked, etc., simply because they don’t
want to be bothered with extra work that
would keep them later.^
Poor coworker • Poor coworker task performance. Examples include BPeople not doing their work, and myself 72 25.2 48 20.8 1.39
performance coworkers not doing their job, coworkers doing a having to pick up their slack.^
poor job, coworkers being late or absence, BI had to fix something that another
coworkers not exerting effort, and coworkers employee that knows better did
making mistakes incorrectly.^
BPeople whose work I am dependent on
(before I can do mine they must do theirs)
continually miss their deadlines.^
Task-related issues
Undesirable work task • Undesirable work task situations (regardless of BWhen the boss recognizes that I am 53 18.5a 22 9.5 8.36a
situations fairness). Examples included having too much to overwhelmed and have too much to do,
do, having to work long hours, performing poorly, and will not find a way to get me some
obstacles to good performance on tasks, having to help, yet has no problem assigning more
do someone else’s work, and having to do tasks.^
something unethical BStressful deadline and not being able to take
breaks or lunches.^
BI had just finished planning a major event
and my coworker deleted the whole thing.
I had to stay up all night for a couple of
days to redo it.^
Lack of resources to • Not having the resources to perform the work. BWorking shorthanded.^ 12 4.2 8 3.5 0.18
perform the work Examples include equipment problems, not having BComputer went down and I lost my place
enough staff, and a lack of training on the test I was taking.^
BRecently a new version of a software
package was launched, but the
organization didn’t think it was important
J Bus Psychol

Table 3 (continued)

Category Description Examples Anger Contempt Chi-


square
# % # %

enough to offer supplemental training to


representatives like members of my group
who are sometimes called upon to work
the phone que in a back-up capacity.^
Counterproductive and/or disrespectful behavior
Coworker/subordinate • Poor or disrespectful interpersonal behavior from BWhen coworkers bring a negative attitude 46 16.1 29 12.6 1.28
interpersonal issues coworkers or subordinates. Includes unpleasant to the workplace.^
interactions, rumor spreading, yelling, and other BOne coworker spilled coffee all over my
forms of counterproductive interpersonal behavior notes before I had the time to write them
into a document. This coworker did not
ever apologize nor consider the time it
took to create the presentation without
notes.^
BThe way I was spoken to.^
Coworker • Actions by coworkers that harm the organization BOther staff did not follow rules.^ 14 4.9 28 12.1a 8.94a
counterproductive or and/or are unethical BCoworkers surfing the Internet on company
unethical behavior time.^
toward the BI caught a coworker fudging her work by
organization writing down false test results, without
actually doing the necessary tests.^
Poor interactions with • Poor interactions with the beneficiaries of one’s BWhen a customer blames me for their 31 10.8 15 6.5 2.98
beneficiaries work, including students, customers, and others shortcomings, gambling is a choice, not
outside of the organization. Also includes something we force people to do.^
observations of mistreatment among students or BWhen a client yelled and blamed me for
customers something that was truly their fault.^
BCustomer made unreasonable demands.^
Lack of recognition • A lack of recognition for one’s work. Includes when BI got all inventory sorted and a person went 15 5.2 12 5.2 0.00
others take credit for one’s work, others question behind me and took credit.^
one’s work, or others fail to show appreciation BBeing unappreciated and taken for
granted.^
BWhen my work is questioned.^
Disloyalty • Reneging on promises, lack of respect for seniority BRecently lost a coworker to a serious health 1 0.3 6 2.6a 4.83a
issue. I found out a few days later that
insurance coverage for the spouse and
children has been terminated and doctor’s
visits already scheduled would not be
covered.^
BAn employee was hired at a higher pay rate
than current employees with more
experience and years of service.^
BI was promised a new position and never
got it.^

B#^ refers to the number of incidents that fell into the category
a
There was a significant difference across the moral emotions in the likelihood of discussing an incident that fell into the category. The superscript letter a
is placed under the moral emotion for which the incident was the most likely to appear

The responses to incidents that triggered the positive Negative Moral Emotion Incidents and Responses
moral emotions (see Table 2) included (a) reciprocation,
such as thanking or doing something nice for the actor; We also found 20 categories of incidents that triggered the neg-
(b) general prosocial behavior; (c) increased effort and ative moral emotions (see Table 3). In general, participants re-
performance; and (d) focusing on and expressing the ported feeling anger or contempt in response to (a) unfair treat-
emotions in some way. Chi-squared tests revealed that ment; (b) poor job security, compensation, and career prospects;
gratitude was more likely to inspire workers to give (c) a lack of support from the organization and supervisors; (d)
thanks and treat themselves, whereas admiration was poor organizational, supervisor, or coworker performance; (e)
more likely to motivate workers to give praise and at- undesirable work task situations; and (f) counterproductive or
tempt to copy or emulate the source. disrespectful treatment from coworkers, beneficiaries (e.g.,
J Bus Psychol

Table 4 Categories, descriptions, examples, and frequencies of responses for the negative moral emotions

Category Description Examples Anger Contempt Chi-


square
# % # %

Complaints
Complained to management • Voiced concerns to someone with BElevate the matter to my superiors.^ 68 29.6 39 23.1 2.09
power, usually management BI was able to get the personnel office to correct the
problem.^
BI vented to my supervisors who are always willing to
listen.^
BI wrote the employee up.^
Complained to coworkers • Complained to and/or sought BI talked with a coworker/vented my emotions.^ 15 6.5 11 6.5 0.00
support from coworkers BI complained to another colleague.^
Complained to others outside • Complained to and/or sought BI kept my mouth shut and complained to my husband 17 7.4 5 3.0 3.67
of work support from others outside of when I got home.^
work, such as friends or family BCried and talked to a friend.^
members BSulked, felt frustrated, discussed with close friends,
tried to read up more to avoid such disasters in the
future.^
BYou bottle it up and then go home or to a bar, vent to
your friends that aren’t coworkers and forget about it
until Monday morning comes and you encounter
any new kinds of issues.^
Complained in general • Complained or vented to no one BYelled at someone else.^ 10 4.3 29 17.2a 18.13a
specific BSpoke my mind about it, stood up for myself.^
BComplained to other people.^
Actions focused on the person or entity responsible
Reciprocated • Reciprocated negatively, punished BI followed our discipline procedure and took away 16 7.0 10 5.9 0.17
the person some of his privileges.^
BI wrote that person up.^
BFiled a lawsuit against them.^
Avoided or disengaged from • Disengaged from the source of the BI generally avoid interacting with her.^ 12 5.2 18 10.7a 4.14a
the source emotion, such as by leaving the BI went outside and walked.^
premises or avoiding the person BI walked away.^
Confronted the source • Confronted and/or tried to correct BI told her what happened and to make sure it would not 69 30.0a 35 20.7 4.36a
the person causing the emotion happen again.^
BTried to discuss the matter.^
BI spoke to the coworker and explained the importance
of doing projects in a timely manner and asking for
help if they can’t finish on time.^
BTold my boss in private that I did not appreciate the
disrespect.^
Emotion-focused responses
Tried to control one’s • Attempted to control one’s BI just told myself to calm down and breathe and I 13 5.7 8 4.7 0.17
emotions emotions, such as by joking about drank some water to calm down.^
the situation or holding in one’s BBit my tongue and hoped that he would look like a
feelings. Often participants fool to everyone in the process.^
mentioned engaging in emotional BJoked about it with a coworker.^
labor BShut up and sat there. Fumed silently.^
Focused on work tasks • Focused on work tasks BI tried to do better.^ 5 2.2 4 2.4 0.02
BI worked harder.^
BI internalized the feelings and focused on next year.^
BOccupied my time by trying to stay busy.^
Substance use • Substance use (e.g., drinking BI walked out, took a smoke break, and went back to 4 1.7 0 0.0 2.97
alcohol, using cigarettes) work.^
BI drank more than usual.^
BHave an angry meeting with employees, smoke a
cigarette.^
Prayed • Prayed BPrayed and cried.^ 2 0.9 3 1.8 0.65
BI might pray for the person.^
BI pray that the truth would be revealed.^
Work performance
Exerted less effort • Exerted less effort and showed less BI went home early.^ 6 2.6 4 2.4 0.02
commitment at work BContinued my work at a slightly slower pace.^
BIt certainly did demotivate me when I see that nothing
is being done about those responsible for the
mistakes.^
J Bus Psychol

Table 4 (continued)

Category Description Examples Anger Contempt Chi-


square
# % # %

BDidn’t work as hard.^


Withdrawal • Looked into leaving the BI’ve been looking for a new job.^ 12 5.2 3 1.8 3.19
organization, left the BGot angry and left the department.^
organization, and/or left the job BChecked for other options in retirement benefits, even
situation (e.g., department) so far as considering changing career or job.^
BI quit my most recent former position.^
Constructive responses
Tried to fix the situation • Tried to fix, clean up, or diffuse the BI took care of most of the task by myself.^ 25 10.9 18 10.7 0.00
situation, and/or tried to help the BI tried to calm that person down, but it didn’t work.^
victim or recipient BI told them I did not think I could get it done in time,
but I would try.^
BThere was nothing I could do other than stand up for
the junior staff member in evaluation meetings.^
BI offered empathy to my coworker and suggestions on
steps to take.^
Attempted to prevent • Sought ways to prevent the BI work with these people to try and build a team 9 3.9 13 7.7 2.67
reoccurrence reoccurrence of the atmosphere.^
event/situation BSolve the problem and work to not let it happen
again.^
BI spoke to the supervisor and let him/her know that it
was wrong and they should be more truthful to staff
in the future if they want to retain the best and
brightest.^
BIt made me qualify myself more to others when asked
to do something I am unfamiliar with.^

B#^ refers to the number of responses that fell into the category
a
There was a significant difference across the moral emotions in the likelihood of discussing a response that fell into the category. The superscript letter a
is placed under the moral emotion for which the response was the most likely to appear

students, customers), and the organization. Chi-squared tests triggers more approach-oriented responses whereas contempt
revealed that undesirable work situations were more likely to triggers more avoidance-oriented responses. Still, both types of
lead to anger than contempt, whereas terminations, layoffs and responses were sometimes reported for each moral emotion.
furloughs, counterproductive or unethical behavior by co- Finally, we conducted chi-squared analyses testing whether
workers, and disloyalty from the organization were more likely a response was more likely to occur in response to a particular
to lead to contempt than to anger. These differences are not as incident. In Tables 5 and 6, we report on significant relation-
easy to interpret as those between gratitude and admiration, but ships for each of the moral emotions. We limited these analyses
there is some evidence that event-related problems such as work to incidents that appeared at least 20 times in order to improve
task issues are more closely tied to anger, whereas entity-focused the interpretability and reliability of the results. Some of these
organization-level issues such as terminations or disloyalty are results will be discussed further when delving into the theoret-
more likely to trigger contempt. Still, the differences between ical implications of the findings for the behavioral responses.
contempt and anger were not as clear, suggesting the anger and
contempt may co-occur and share many antecedents. Differences across Gender, Managerial Role, and Age
Responses to incidents that triggered anger or contempt (see
Table 4) included (a) complaining to management, coworkers, We also conducted analyses on the extent to which there were
and others outside of work; (b) avoiding, confronting, and/or differences in the incidents or responses across gender, age,
reciprocating toward the source of the anger or contempt; (c) and whether the individual was in a managerial role in the
focusing on controlling or managing one’s own emotions; (d) organization. First, we conducted chi-squared tests comparing
decreasing one’s effort and commitment; and (e) constructively men and women in the frequencies of all incident and response
attempting to fix the situation or prevent reoccurrence. The fre- categories. We report on results for which the Pearson
quency of most of the responses was similar across the two moral chi-squared p value was less than 0.05. Results indicate that
emotions, but anger was more likely to result in confronting the there were no differences in the type of incidents that triggered
actor whereas contempt was more likely to result in avoiding the gratitude. However, women were more likely than men (56.6
actor. This is consistent with the basic proposition that anger vs. 43.0%) to report thanking the source of the gratitude. Men
J Bus Psychol

Table 5 Significant relationships between positive incident categories and responses (based on chi-squared analyses)

Incident category Significant relationships with responses—gratitude Significant relationships with


responses—admiration

Non-pay recognition Thanked the source (+) Thanked the source (+), gave praise (−),
savored the emotion (+)
Pay-based recognition Reciprocated (−), treated oneself (+) N/A
Having a job Thanked the source (−), reciprocated (−), N/A
increased effort (−), prayed (+)
Promotion and development opportunities Reciprocated (−), increased effort (+), savored N/A
the emotion (+)
Benefits and working conditions, None N/A
non-tasked-related aspects of work
Desirable work tasks and responsibilities Thanked the source (−), did something nice N/A
for others (+), gave praise (+)
Sense of accomplishment and achievement Thanked the source (−), did something nice for N/A
others (+), gave praise (+), expressed
positive emotions (+)
Interactions with external beneficiaries Expressed positive emotions (+), spent more N/A
of one’s work time with the source (+)
Performance and success of colleagues N/A Thanked the source (−), gave praise (+)
Supervisor, leader, and/or mentor support Thanked the source (+) Increased effort (+), gave praise (−)
Coworker and subordinate support Reciprocated (+), increased effort (−) Thanked the source (+), increased effort (−),
spent more time with the source (+)
Communication from the organization N/A N/A
Effective leadership on task-related issues N/A Increased effort (+)
Third-party observations of others’ moral excellence N/A Did something nice for others (+), increased
effort (−), prayed (+); tried to copy the
source (+)

N/A indicates that there were fewer than 20 incidents in that category for the given moral emotion. When there were fewer than 20 incidents in a category
for both moral emotions, that category was omitted from this table. A B(+)^ indicates that the response was more likely to occur for that incident category
than for other incident categories. A B(−)^ indicates that the response was less likely to occur for that incident category than for other incident categories

were more likely than women to report pay-based recognition admiration (9.6 vs. 2.2%). Women were more likely to report
as a source of admiration (4.9 vs. 0.0%). Women were more poor interactions with beneficiaries as a source of anger (15.5
likely to report attempting to copy or emulate the source of their vs. 6.1%), whereas men were more likely to report poor

Table 6 Significant relationships between negative incident categories and responses (based on chi-squared analyses)

Incident category Significant relationships with responses—anger Significant relationships with


responses—contempt

Lack of supervisor support Confronted the source (+) Disengaged from the source (+)
Poor organizational performance and mismanagement Complained to management (+), withdrawal (+), None
confronted the source (−)
Supervisor incompetence and Exerted less effort (+), avoided the source (+) None
poor performance
Poor coworker performance Withdrawal (−) Complained to management (+)
Undesirable work task situations Withdrawal (+), substance use (+) Tried to prevent reoccurrence (+),
focused on work tasks (+)
Coworker/subordinate interpersonal issues Confronted the source (+) Disengaged from the source (+)
Coworker counterproductive or unethical behavior N/A Complained to management (+),
reciprocated (+)
Poor interactions with beneficiaries Complained to management (−), reciprocated (+), N/A
tried to control emotions (+); tried to fix the situation (+)

N/A indicates that there were fewer than 20 incidents in that category for the given moral emotion. When there were fewer than 20 incidents in a category
for both moral emotions, that category was omitted from this table. A B(+)^ indicates that the response was more likely to occur for that incident category
than for other incident categories. A B(−)^ indicates that the response was less likely to occur for that incident category than for other incident categories
J Bus Psychol

coworker performance (36.0 vs. 16.2%) and poor organization- success of colleagues (r = .11) and working conditions and
al performance and mismanagement (13.2 vs. 6.1%) as a source benefits (r = .12) as sources of gratitude. There were no dif-
of anger. Men were more likely than women to report substance ferences across age in responses to gratitude. Older workers
use (4.3 vs. 0.0%) in response to anger, whereas women more were more likely to report effective task-oriented leadership as
likely to report complaining to coworkers (10.1 vs. 2.1%). a source of admiration. There were no differences across age
There were no differences between men and women with re- in responses to admiration. Older workers were less likely to
spect to incidents that triggered contempt, but women were report a lack of recognition as a source of anger (r = −.14) and
more likely to report responding to contempt by complaining more likely to report third-party observations of unfair treat-
to others outside of work (5.6 vs. 0.0%). ment as a source of anger (r = .13). Older workers were less
We conducted the same analysis comparing managers to likely to report withdrawal (r = −.16) or complaining to others
non-managers in the frequency of each of the incidents and outside of work (r = −.15) in response to anger and were more
responses. We used the occupation codes generated earlier to likely to report praying (r = .16) in response to anger. Age was
determine if the individual was in a managerial role. We note uncorrelated with the incidents that triggered contempt.
that, using the SOC coding system, individuals who were Finally, older workers were less likely to report focusing on
first-line supervisors were not considered in Bmanagement^ tasks and channeling their emotion in response to contempt
occupations. They instead were considered as working in their (r = −.16) and were more likely to report praying in response
particular functional area. There were 47 respondents in man- to contempt (r = .19).
agement occupations and 305 in non-management occupa-
tions. Results indicated that non-managers were more likely
than managers to report supervisor/leader/mentor support as a Theoretical Implications of the Critical Incidents
source of gratitude (11.4 vs. 0.0%). Managers were more like-
ly than non-managers to report coworker or subordinate sup- The results for the critical incidents that trigger the moral
port as a source of gratitude (28.6 vs. 14.3%). There were no emotions warrant further discussion in light of the first prop-
differences between managers and non-managers in their re- osition, which was based on moral foundations theory. We
ported responses to gratitude. Managers were more likely than proposed that based on moral foundations theory and other
non-managers to report a general positive evaluation of the theoretical work on moral principles (Graham et al. 2011;
organization (3.1 vs. 0.0%), non-pay-based recognition of Rai and Fiske 2011), incidents that involved harming or help-
others (9.4 vs. 2.1%), and third-party observations of moral ing, fairness or reciprocity, loyalty to the group, and respect
excellence (34.4 vs. 13.8%) as sources of admiration. for others would fall within the moral domain and trigger
Managers were also more likely to report savoring the emo- moral emotions toward responsible individuals or entities. In
tion in response to admiration (13.8 vs. 1.7%). Managers were Tables 7 and 8, we link each of the incident categories to the
more likely than non-managers to report a lack of promotion moral foundation or foundations to which each seems most
and development opportunities as a source of anger (10.0 vs. closely aligned.
2.2%), whereas non-managers were more likely to report poor The most fundamental of the moral foundations, harming
interactions with beneficiaries as a source of anger (12.7 vs. and helping, was implicated in many of the positive incidents
0.0%). There were no significant differences between man- that involved receiving or observing help from others or the
agers and non-managers in their reported responses to anger. organization at large. Pay-based recognition (e.g., raises, bo-
Managers were more likely to report poor organizational per- nuses) is helpful to the recipient, as are some other forms of
formance and mismanagement as a source of contempt (22.2 recognition. Having a job, promotion and development oppor-
vs. 8.1%). There were no significant differences between man- tunities, and good benefits and working conditions are helpful
agers and non-managers in their reported responses to con- and thus triggered gratitude and admiration. Having pleasant
tempt. The lack of a difference between managers and non- and enjoyable work tasks, having the resources to perform the
managers and managers in their responses to the negative work, and feeling a sense of accomplishment with work com-
moral emotions is inconsistent with propositions and 5a and pletion are all helpful to the worker and were found to be
5b, suggesting that these need to be revisited. common antecedents to gratitude in particular. Coworker, sub-
Finally, we analyzed age by computing the point-biserial ordinate, and leader support, usually in the form of instrumen-
correlation between age and whether each incident or response tal or socio-emotional assistance, were also common anteced-
category was reported. We report results for which the p value ents to gratitude and admiration. Finally, third-party observa-
was less than .05. Results indicate that older workers were less tions of helping behavior by others or the organization as a
likely to report promotion and development opportunities whole were found as antecedents to admiration. As a whole,
(r = −.17), pay-based recognition (r = −.18), and communica- these results suggest that behavior and conditions that help
tion from the organization (r = −.11) as sources of gratitude, workers or their colleagues are the most common and salient
whereas they were more likely to report the performance and antecedents to gratitude and admiration in the workplace.
J Bus Psychol

Table 7 Moral foundations and


associated incidents for the Category Harm/help Fairness/ Loyalty to Respect for
positive moral emotions reciprocity the group others

Non-pay recognition X X
Pay-based recognition X X
Observing others being recognized X
Having a job X
Promotion and development opportunities X X
Benefits and working conditions, X
non-task-related aspects of work
Desirable work tasks and responsibilities X
Sense of accomplishment and achievement X
Interactions with external beneficiaries X
of one’s work
Having the resources to do one’s work X
Performance and success of colleagues X
Performance and success of one’s supervisor, X
mentor, and/or leader
Performance and success of subordinates X
Supervisor, leader, and/or mentor support X X
Coworker and subordinate support X X
Communication from the organization X
Effective leadership on task-related issues X
General positive evaluation of the organization
Third-party observations of others’ X
moral excellence
Socially responsible organization X

BX^ indicates that the incident category represents situations that reflect the given moral foundation

The other moral foundations were less prominent among embedded in the responses. Fairness and reciprocity was often
the positive moral emotions incidents but were sometimes implicit and sometime explicit in incidents involving

Table 8 Moral foundations and


associated incidents for the Category Harm/ Fairness/ Loyalty to the Respect for
negative moral emotions help reciprocity group others

Distributive or procedural injustice X


Third-party observations of unfair treatment X
Politics, general unfairness X
False accusations X
Unethical behavior by the supervisor X X
Terminations, layoffs, and furloughs X
Compensation and benefits X
Lack of promotion and development opportunities X X
Lack of support from the organization X
Lack of supervisor support X X
Poor organizational performance and X
mismanagement
Supervisor incompetence and poor performance X
Poor coworker performance X X
Undesirable work task situations X
Lack of resources to perform the work X
Coworker/subordinate interpersonal issues X
Coworker counterproductive or unethical behavior X
toward the organization
Poor interactions with beneficiaries X
Lack of recognition X
Disloyalty from the organization X

BX^ indicates that the incident category represents situations that reflect the given moral foundation
J Bus Psychol

recognition and promotion and development opportunities. The in-group loyalty foundation was represented by inci-
Participants might feel they deserve a raise, recognition, and/ dents in which the organization, supervisors, and coworkers
or a promotion opportunity based on principles of equity or performed poorly or engaged in counterproductive behavior,
fairness, and thus it is possible that fair distribution and a bal- hurting the effectiveness, integrity, and well-being of the
anced reciprocal relationship between the employee and the group and organization as a whole. It is worth noting that poor
organization are another factor in gratitude and admiration. coworker performance was by far the most common type of
The foundation of in-group loyalty is represented in the dedi- incident that participants reported as causing them to feel an-
cation and task effectiveness of supervisors, coworkers, and ger or contempt. In some cases, the poor performance of co-
subordinates to the work group and organization. The respect workers also caused the participant to have to perform more
for others foundation is reflected in the incidents involving work, creating an unfair situation while also harming the
support from supervisors and coworkers as well as in positive group. Finally, the respect for others foundation was promi-
interactions with beneficiaries and communication from the nent among the negative incidents. Unethical behavior by the
organization. Each of these incidents involves being treated supervisor, lack of supervisor support, and poor interactions
with respect and dignity by others. It is also noteworthy that with coworkers and beneficiaries all involve situations in
admiration sometimes occurred in response to the competence which one is not treated with respect and dignity.
of others. Competence may in some cases lead to better out- Some overall trends are notable when examining the links
comes for the group, thus having implications for the well- between the incident categories and moral foundations. First, it
being of the group and representing the in-group loyalty moral appears that helping is the most prominent foundation among the
foundation. However, in some cases, the participants’ descrip- incidents triggering the positive moral emotions of gratitude and
tions suggested the admiration had more to do solely with the admiration. Actions in which individuals or the organization are
success and competence of others than with the effect on the helpful are the most likely to trigger gratitude and/or admiration.
work group. As such, admiration may in some cases occur in By contrast, fairness and reciprocity, in-group loyalty, and re-
response to competent behavior and performance, which does spect for others seem to be more prominent among the incidents
not necessarily fit into any of the aforementioned moral associated with the negative moral emotions of anger and con-
foundations. This was also noted by Algoe and Haidt (2009) tempt. This suggests that fairness and reciprocity, in-group loy-
in their discussion of admiration. alty, and respect for others tend to represent moral obligations
With respect to the negative incidents, the harming and that trigger negative moral emotions when not met but are less
helping foundation was represented in a number of situations, likely to trigger positive moral emotions when they are met. For
including terminations, layoffs and furloughs, poor compen- example, a worker may feel angry or contemptuous when treated
sation and benefits, a lack of promotion and development disrespectfully or unfairly because these are violations of oblig-
opportunities, a lack of support from the organization and atory moral treatment; however, the worker may not necessarily
supervisors, having undesirable work tasks, and not having feel gratitude or admiration toward someone who is fair and
the resources to perform the work. In each of these types of respectful as this only meets expectations and does not represent
situations, the recipient is harmed in some way and/or does not excellence. Helping, on the other hand, often involves going
receive the help and resources needed. The harmful nature of beyond obligations and may represent a moral ideal that triggers
these experiences would trigger anger and/or contempt ac- gratitude and admiration. This parallels the distinction between
cording to moral foundations theory. obligations and ideals in Higgins’s influential regulatory focus
Additionally, the fairness/reciprocity foundation was (1997) and self-discrepancy (1987) theories.
prominent among the negative incidents, more so than It is also notable that many of the incident categories, while
among the positive incidents. Incidents involving distribu- more closely linked to one particular foundation, touch on
tive or procedural injustice, third-party observations of un- multiple moral foundations. For example, unfair treatment is
fair treatment, politics, false accusations, a lack of deserved sometimes also harmful and disrespectful. Pay raises or pro-
recognition, and unethical behavior by the supervisor typi- motion opportunities can be both helpful while also fulfilling
cally included specific references to unfair decisions or principles of fairness. Counterproductive or poor coworker
treatment that violate principles of equity or equality of performance creates unfair situations while also potentially
treatment. A lack of promotion or development opportuni- harming others by forcing them to work more and fix mis-
ties was sometimes discussed as leading to anger or con- takes. Further research might consider more directly assessing
tempt, because these opportunities had been promised, sug- the extent to which the moral foundations are fulfilled or vio-
gesting a lack of psychological contract fulfillment and per- lated by the organization, supervisors, and coworkers in order
ceived violation of the norm of reciprocity. Incidents of to tease apart the critical features of these events.
disloyalty also often involved a failure to keep promises, Finally, it was notable that there were some incidents that
causing a violation of the expected employee-organization reflected structural conditions and ongoing situations whereas
relationship. other incidents reflected discrete episodes. For example, some
J Bus Psychol

participants mentioned situations such as low pay, poor orga- exchange theory and with past research on negative recipro-
nizational performance, or a supportive or unsupportive su- cation (Mitchell and Ambrose 2007), individuals sometimes
pervisor as triggering moral emotions. These are ongoing sit- reciprocated negative treatment by attempting to punish the
uations that do not change much from day to day but instead actor and confronting the actor about the incident. Our find-
indicate structural conditions and/or impressions that are ings highlighted the role of anger and contempt in these situ-
based on accumulated experience. By contrast, some partici- ations. In addition, respondents often reported increasing ef-
pants mentioned discrete episodes such as particular instances fort and commitment when feeling grateful and decreasing
of poor coworker performance or being passed over for a effort and commitment when feeling angry or contemptuous.
specific promotion. This distinction between structural situa- It may be that the moral emotions play an affective motiva-
tional features and discrete episodes aligns with the general tional role in facilitating these forms of reciprocation.
distinction between work environment features and work Furthermore, when looking at the relationship between the
events specified in affective events theory (Weiss and incidents and the responses (see Tables 5), it is apparent that the
Cropanzano 1996). More recent work has distinguished participants were more likely to behaviorally reciprocate toward
chronic from episodic forms of anger and gratitude (Fehr coworkers or subordinates, whereas when receiving recognition
et al. 2017; Gibson and Callister 2010), and we would expect and support from leaders, they were more likely to just express
that chronic forms of these emotions would tend to be trig- appreciation. It may be that there are more formal rules for
gered by ongoing situations and structural features, whereas behavioral interactions with leaders and more discretion for
the episodic forms of these emotions would tend to be trig- interactions with coworkers, creating greater ambiguity in
gered by discrete episodes. coworker-coworker exchange relationships that amplify posi-
tive social exchange processes. Currently, the literature on so-
Theoretical Implications of Responses to the Moral cial exchange theory does not specify these types of differences.
Emotions More research is needed on these coworker-coworker relations
to better understand these positive exchange processes.
Drawing on social exchange theory, social learning theory, This study also sheds light on the role of emotions in social
and expectancy theory, we proposed that individuals would learning and behavioral modeling at work, which has receive
respond to incidents that trigger moral emotions by recipro- little empirical attention in organizational research outside of
cating positive and negative behavior, copying or emulating the formal training realm (Taylor et al. 2005). Based on social
positive behavior, and in some cases engaging in rational be- learning theory (Bandura 1977), we proposed that individuals
havior in response to negative moral emotions when recipro- would copy or emulate the source when experiencing grati-
cation could lead to negative consequences. The results pro- tude or admiration. We found this to the be the case in that
vide some support for these propositions, but there were some individuals sometimes reported trying to be more like the ac-
responses that did not align with these predictions. tor when experiencing gratitude or admiration. Individuals
Consistent with social exchange theory, individuals often also sometimes tried to do something nice for others, Bpaying
reciprocated positive treatment when experiencing gratitude it forward^ in a way. Such behavior is consistent with work
or admiration. Despite the extensive discussion of social ex- suggesting that generalized reciprocity is in part driven by
change theory in the organizational psychology literature affect (Gray et al. 2014). Analyses (see Table 5) also suggest
(Cropanzano and Mitchell 2005), there has been very little that individuals were more likely to attempt to copy the source
direct investigation of the role of emotions in social exchange when observing a third party performing an exemplary act.
processes. Recent meta-analytic work has considered the role This suggests that third-party observations of helpful or sup-
of generic positive and negative affect in facilitating responses portive behavior can inspire admiration and attempts to copy
to fair treatment (Colquitt et al. 2013b), while work in other the behavior. These results indicate that responses to gratitude
domains of psychology has found that gratitude is instrumen- and admiration go beyond those predicted by social exchange
tal to social exchange relationships (Gordon et al. 2012). We theory and may be further understood through principles of
found qualitative evidence that the moral emotions, which social learning and behavioral modeling, which up to this
have a specific target, occur in response to morally relevant point have received limited research attention.
treatment and often facilitate action tendencies toward justice In addition, based on expectancy theory, we proposed that
restoration (Haidt 2003), which play a strong role in social individuals would respond to negative acts in ways that would
exchange processes between the employee and the organiza- have helpful consequences or that would avoid harmful con-
tion. Gratitude in particular was found to facilitate positive sequences. In particular, we were expecting that individuals
reciprocation. Examples of positive reciprocation included would often avoid reciprocating negative treatment when it
saying thanks, doing something nice or helpful in return, could ultimately be harmful to themselves. Indeed, direct at-
praising or congratulating the individual, and spending more tempts to punish the transgressor, while represented in the
time with the individual. Also, consistent with social responses, were not as frequent as were attempts to reciprocate
J Bus Psychol

positive treatment. Instead, participants often reported negative events that some participants reported involved fo-
complaining to coworkers or others outside of work about cusing more on their work. Participants sometimes reported
negative events. While consistent with expectancy theory, trying to work harder even though they were angry about
such complaining behavior is consistent with other past re- something. This sort of resilience to the negative effects of
search suggesting that individuals often engage in emotion- anger or contempt is also interesting but not easily understood
focused coping strategies such as venting (e.g., Day and through the aforementioned theories.
Livingstone 2001). Participants also often tried to avoid the
source by walking away or avoiding interaction in general,
consistent with other work on withdrawal responses to poor Practical Implications
treatment (e.g., Mawritz et al. 2014). In addition, participants
often reported active attempts to control their moral emotions, This study has some important implications for practice in
using phrases such as Bbit my tongue^ and Bbottle it up and organizations. First, this study highlights several managerial
then go home…^ These attempts are likely a rational response practices to be encouraged. Non-pay recognition emerged as a
driven by the expectation that expressing those emotions frequent predictor of gratitude and admiration. Examples in-
would bring harmful consequences. Still, participants often cluded receiving thanks, being praised for one’s work, or even
complained to management as another way to deal less direct- receiving a birthday card. These indicators of appreciation are
ly with the source of the anger. In other cases, they confronted not expensive, but they communicate that an employee is
the source in attempt to correct the behavior, even if not to valued, apparently having a positive effect on many of the
punish the person. Participants also sometimes reported at- participants in this study. Managers should be made aware
tempts to be constructive by fixing the situation and of the usefulness and meaningfulness of individually recog-
preventing it from occurring again. Analyses even suggested nizing their employees in a genuine way from time to time.
that respondents were more likely to confront their supervisors Offering benefits and good working conditions such as flexi-
in response to a lack of support than they were to confront ble work arrangements or floating holidays also were appre-
others (see Table 6). ciated and may inspire greater performance and commitment,
On the whole, these latter responses to negative treatment helping confirm some of the conclusions from a recent meta-
suggest that workers exert a great deal of top-down control analysis by Butts et al. (2013), which found positive relation-
over their emotional impulses. Such findings are consistent ships between work-family support policies and employee
with the extensive work on emotional labor in organizations, attitudes.
which shows that workers often suppress their felt emotion in Another prominent factor that emerged from this study was
favor of an emotion that fulfills the emotional display rules of the performance of other coworkers and management in gen-
the situation (Grandey and Gabriel 2015). We suspect that this eral. Participants often reported being grateful for and admir-
self-control is driven to a large degree by expectancies for the ing others who performed well, while poor coworker perfor-
outcomes of their behavior. As noted earlier, anger has been mance was the most common reported antecedent to anger
conceptualized as a fundamentally approach-oriented emo- and contempt. This suggests that effective performance man-
tion, while contempt has been discussed as more of an agement plays an important role in employee moral emotions,
avoidance-oriented emotion. Yet, individuals often acted con- which in turn are associated with effort and commitment.
structively when experiencing anger and contempt. It would Furthermore, the results suggest that good performance is con-
be interesting to further study the role of expectancies on this tagious and support among coworkers is reciprocated through
type of behavior more directly, while also considering the role thanks, praise, and helpful acts in return. This suggests that
of more constructive anger and contempt responses to team supportive behavior and good performance are contagious and
conflict resolution. may emerge at a collective level. On the other hand, it appears
Finally, some responses were not easily predicted by the that poor performers can be toxic and that organizations need
aforementioned theories but warrant further consideration. In to pay special attention toward dealing with their performance
responses to the positive events, individuals often reported in some way.
expressing the gratitude or admiration to others or savoring Finally, there is some work suggesting that interventions
the emotion by celebrating in some ways. This type of coping can be used to induce positive moral emotions such as grati-
with positive events has received little emphasis in the re- tude, and that these interventions can improve well-being.
search literature but might be useful in extending the psycho- Workers taking part in these interventions typically reflect
logical benefits of pleasant situations such as those that trigger on aspects of work that they are grateful for (e.g., Kaplan
gratitude or admiration. Other work has also suggest that pos- et al. 2014) or on positive work events in general (Bono
itive self-reflection may have similar benefits (Bono et al. et al. 2013). Interventions such as these, in which workers
2013), indicating that positive event coping warrants further bring gratitude-inducing events and conditions to conscious
research and theoretical attention. Another response to the awareness, may be useful.
J Bus Psychol

In a more general sense, these results suggest that managers (e.g., Bcoworker or subordinate support^). This subjectivity
and organizations should consider the moral principles of and variability may have caused the moderately low Cohen’s
helping and harming, fairness and reciprocity, the integrity kappa values of 0.54 and 0.59 on the coding agreement indi-
and performance of the group, and respect for others through- ces for anger and contempt incidents. This represents a limi-
out their practices. Many specific manifestations of these gen- tation of the paper. With that said, the coding process was
eral principles emerged from this inductive study, with con- iterative and an attempt was made to ensure all relevant cate-
crete examples listed in the quotes on Tables 1 and 3. gories were included in this final paper. All incidents and
responses were coded independently by two individuals, and
Limitations all discrepancies were resolved through group discussion and
consensus judgment. Follow-up research with more standard-
There are some notable caveats and limitations that should ized responses can help verify and expand on the findings
be noted in considering the findings of our analysis. First, from this study. Furthermore, computerized text analysis
our method in collecting qualitative data was to allow might be a useful way to augment subjective coding
participants to choose an event or situation that triggered procedures.
the given moral emotion. It is possible that there is some There are some other limitations that are worth noting
variance in what the moral emotions, and words used to as well. This research was cross sectional in that all data
describe them, mean to participants. The words that we were collected in a single setting. Thus, we cannot draw
listed in order to priming responses may have triggered strong empirically based causal inferences about the rela-
different emotions than were intended. In a minority of tionships among the event, emotions, and behavioral re-
cases, participants might have felt admiration or contempt sponses. Additionally, data were based on retrospective
when they themselves were admired or held in contempt, self-reports. It is possible that individuals forgot elements
rather than when they admired or held in contempt some- of the situations they were recalling or did not correctly
one else. Furthermore, by not standardizing the responses, remember how they felt. Future research might consider
it is possible that the frequency of responses in each cat- collective qualitative, narrative data on episodes as they
egory does not necessarily represent their relative influ- are occurring to see if the results are different. Finally, all
ence on the moral emotion; it may just be that these situ- data were collected from the USA. It is possible that the
ations are most salient to workers. Some factors that in- types of events that yield these moral emotions and the
fluence these moral emotions may be outside of the em- behavioral responses would differ across individuals from
ployee’s awareness, and this method fails to capture such different cultural backgrounds. Further research is needed
processes being that it relies on the employee’s memory to develop and test cross-cultural hypotheses about the
and introspection. moral emotions at work.
A second limitation is that there were some missing data.
As participants went through the survey, some provided un-
usable responses and these became more common throughout
the survey. Thus, the results for contempt, which were the last Conclusion
moral emotion we asked about, yielded fewer incidents than
the results for anger or gratitude. The nature of the online The goals of this paper were to (a) present a theoretical
panel method for collecting data likely contributed to this framework for moral emotions toward others at work
limitation. However, one advantage of our open-ended re- and responses to situations that elicit these moral emo-
sponse method was that participants could not randomly give tions and (b) collect and analyze qualitative data on the
answers to questions without us detecting them as such an- types of experiences and responses associated with the
swers were quite obvious. Random responses to close-ended moral emotions of gratitude, anger, elevation, and con-
surveys, on the other hand, are more difficult to detect as they tempt among workers. The results point to several types
do not necessarily stand out. Thus, although we did have some of experiences that lead to these moral emotions, some
missing data, we are confident that the data we collected are of which have received limited attention in organization-
not based on random responding but rather the actual experi- al research. These experiences generally align with mor-
ences of the workers we sampled. al foundations of harming/helping, fairness/reciprocity,
Third, as with most qualitative analyses, this one involved a in-group loyalty, and respect for others. The results also
subjective coding process. Although several individuals were call attention to several types of meaningful responses
involved in coding, it is possible that some different categories to these situations, with implications for theory and or-
would emerge among a different set of coders. Furthermore, ganizational practice. Still, further research is needed on
the codes varied in their specificity, with some codes being these moral emotions, their causes, and their implica-
very specific (e.g., Bjob status^) and others being more general tions for the employee’s well-being and performance.
J Bus Psychol

References impact on health? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6,


348–360. doi:10.1037//1076-8998.6.4.348.
Eisenberger, R., Huntington, R., Hutchison, S., & Sowa, D. (1986).
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: the Perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology,
‘other praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. 71, 500–507. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.71.3.500.
Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 105–127. doi:10.1080 Emerson, R. M. (1976). Social exchange theory. Annual Review of
/17439760802650519. Sociology, 2, 335–362. doi:10.1146/annurev.so.02.080176.002003.
Altman, D. G. (1991). Practical statistics for medical research. London: Fehr, R., Fulmer, A., Awtrey, E., & Miller, J. (2017). The grateful work-
Chapman and Hall. place: a multilevel model of gratitude in organizations. Academy of
Anderson, L., & Wilson, S. (1997). The critical incident technique. In D. Management Review. doi:10.5465/amr.2014.0374.
L. Whetzel & G. R. Wheaton (Eds.), Applied measurement methods Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: the
in industrial psychology (pp. 88–112). Palo Alton, CA: Davies- characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal
Black. of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 103–115. doi:10.1037
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: /0022-3514.93.1.103.
Prentice Hall. Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: an emotion script approach to
Blau, P. (1964). Exchange & power in social life. New Brunswick: anger episodes between workers and their superiors, co-workers,
Transaction Publishers. and subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 147–
Bono, J. E., Glomb, T. M., Shen, W., Kim, E., & Koch, A. J. (2013). 162 doi: 0.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<147::AID-
Building positive resources: effects of positive events and positive JOB35>3.0.CO;2-T.
reflection on work stress and health. Academy of Management Flanagan, J. C. (1954). The critical incident technique. Psychological
Journal, 56, 1601–1627. doi:10.5465/amj.2011.0272. Bulletin, 41, 237–358. doi:10.1037/h0061470.
Butts, M. M., Casper, W. J., & Yang, T. S. (2013). How important are Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (1999). A model of work frustration-aggression.
work-family support policies? A meta-analytic investigation of their Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 915–931. doi:10.1002
effects on employee outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, /(SICI)1099-1379(199911)20:6<915::AID-JOB918>3.3.CO;2-Y.
1–25. doi:10.1037/a0030389. Gibson, D. E., & Callister, R. R. (2010). Anger in organizations: review
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related and integration. Journal of Management, 36, 66–93. doi:10.1177
affect: evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183– /0149206309348060.
204. doi:10.1037/a0013965. Gordon, A. M., Impett, E. A., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2012).
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. To have and to hold: gratitude promotes relationship maintenance in
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46. intimate bonds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103,
doi:10.1177/001316446002000104. 257–274. doi:10.1037/a0028723.
Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., Rodell, J. B., Long, D. M., Zapata, C. P., Gouldner, A. (1960). The norm of reciprocity. American Sociological
Conlon, D. E., & Wesson, M. J. (2013a). Justice at the millennium, a Review, 25, 161–178 Retrieved from: http://www.jstor.
decade later: a meta-analytic test of social exchange and affect-based org/stable/2092623.
perspectives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 199–236. Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to
doi:10.1037/a0031757. leadership: development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory
Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2013b). What is of leadership over 25 years: applying a multi-level multi-domain
organizational justice? A historical overview. In J. Greenberg & J. perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219–247. doi:10.1016/1048-
A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 3–58). 9843(95)90036-5.
New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H.
Conway, N., & Briner, R. B. (2002). A daily dairy study of affective (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and
responses to psychological contract breach and exceeded promises. Social Psychology, 101, 366–385. doi:10.1037/a0021847.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 287–302. doi:10.1002 Grandey, A. A., & Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads:
/job.139. where do we go from here? Annual Review of Organizational
Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001). Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2, 323–349.
Incivility in the workplace: incidence and impact. Journal of doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400.
Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 64–80. doi:10.1037/1076- Grant, A. M., & Gino, F. (2010). A little thanks goes a long way:
8998.6.1.64. explaining why gratitude expressions motivate prosocial behavior.
Crockett, M. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Siegel, J. Z., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 946–955.
(2014). Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision doi:10.1037/a0017935.
making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Moral typecasting: divergent percep-
17320–17325. doi:10.1073/pnas.1408988111. tions of moral agents and moral patients. Journal of Personality and
Cropanzano, R., & Mitchell, M. S. (2005). Social exchange theory: an Social Psychology, 96, 505–520. doi:10.1037/a0013748.
interdisciplinary review. Journal of Management, 31, 874–900. Gray, K., Ward, A. F., & Norton, M. I. (2014). Paying it forward: general-
doi:10.1177/0149206305279602. ized reciprocity and the limits of generosity. Journal of Experimental
Cropanzano, R., Prehar, C. A., & Chen, P. Y. (2002). Using social ex- Psychology: General, 143, 247–254. doi:10.1037/a0031047.
change theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuition-
Group & Organization Management, 27, 324–351. doi:10.1177 ist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–
/1059601102027003002. 834. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814.
de Jong, J., Clinton, M., Rigotti, T., & Bernhard-Oettel, C. (2015). Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, &
Nonlinear associations between breached obligations and employee H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–
well-being. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 30, 374–389. 870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1108/JMP-06-2012-0171. Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposed justice: conser-
Day, A. L., & Livingstone, H. A. (2001). Chronic and acute stressors vatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social
among military personnel: do coping styles buffer their negative Justice Research, 20, 98–116. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z.
J Bus Psychol

Hannah, S. T., Avolio, B. J., & May, D. R. (2011). Moral maturation and Roch, S. G., & Shanock, L. R. (2006). Organizational justice in an ex-
moral conation: a capacity approach to explaining moral thought change framework: clarifying organizational justice distinctions.
and action. Academy of Management Review, 36, 663–685. J o u r n a l o f M a n a g e m e n t , 3 2 , 2 9 9 – 3 2 2 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7
doi:10.5465/amr.2010.0128. /0149206305280115.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad
Psychological Review, 94, 319–340. doi:10.1037/0033-295 hypothesis: a mapping between the three moral emotions (contempt,
X.94.3.319. anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divin-
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, ity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–586.
52, 1280–1300. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574.
Hoobler, J. M., & Brass, D. J. (2006). Abusive supervision and family Rudolph, U., & Tscharakstschiew, N. (2014). An attributional analysis of
undermining as displaced aggression. Journal of Applied moral emotions: naïve scientists and everyday judges. Emotion
Psychology, 91, 1125–1133. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.1125. Review, 6, 344–352. doi:10.1177/1754073914534507.
Hutcherson, C. A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The moral emotions: a social- Rupp, D. E., & Spencer, S. (2006). When customers lash out: the effects
functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of of customer interactional injustice on emotional labor and the medi-
Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 719–737. doi:10.1037 ating role of discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
/a0022408. 971–978. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.971.
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Carnes, N. C. (2013). Surveying the moral land- Rupp, D. E., McCance, A. S., Spencer, S., & Sonntag, K. (2008).
scape: moral motives and group-based moralities. Personality and Customer (in)justice and emotional labor: the role of perspective
Social Psychology Review, 17, 219–236. doi:10.1177 taking, anger, and emotion regulation. Journal of Management,
/1088868313480274. 34, 903–923. doi:10.1177/0149206307309261.
Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S., & Hepp, S. (2009). Proscriptive versus Samnani, A.-K., & Singh, P. (2015). Workplace bullying: considering the
prescriptive morality: two faces of moral regulation. Journal of interaction between individual and work environment. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 521–537. doi:10.1037 Business Ethics. Advanced online publication. doi. doi:10.1007
/a0013779. /s10551-015-2653-x.
Kaplan, S., Bradley-Geist, J. C., Ahmad, A., Anderson, A., Hargrove, A. Sanders, S., Wisse, B. M., & Van Yperen, N. W. (2015). Holding others in
K., & Lindsey, A. (2014). A test of two positive psychology inter- contempt: the moderating role of power in the relationship between
ventions to increase employee well-being. Journal of Business and leaders’ contempt and their behavior vis-à-vis employees. Business
Psychology, 29, 367–380. doi:10.1007/s10869-013-9319-4. Ethics Quarterly, 25, 213–241. doi:10.1017/beq.2015.14.
Krippendorff, K. (2013). Content analysis: an introduction to its Schnall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2010). Elevation leads to
methodology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. altruistic behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 315–320.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agree- doi:10.1177/0956797609359882.
ment for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. doi:10.2307 Schonfeld, I. S., & Mazzola, J. J. (2015). A qualitative study of stress in
/2529310. individuals self-employed in solo businesses. Journal of Occupational
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1979). Application of social-learning Health Psychology, 20, 501–513. doi:10.1037/a0038804.
theory to training supervisors through behavioral modeling.
Shockley, K. M., & Allen, T. D. (2015). Deciding between work and
Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 239–246. doi:10.1037/0021-
family: an episodic approach. Personnel Psychology, 68, 283–318.
9010.64.3.239.
doi:10.1111/peps.12077.
Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup
Silvers, J. A., & Haidt, J. (2008). Moral elevation can induce nursing.
incivility: impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied
Emotion, 8, 291–295. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.291.
Psychology, 93, 95–107. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95.
Simon, L. S., Hurst, C., Kelley, K., & Judge, T. A. (2015). Understanding
Mawritz, M. B., Dust, S. B., & Resick, C. J. (2014). Hostile climate,
cycles of abuse: a multimotive approach. Journal of Applied
abusive supervision, and employee coping: does conscientiousness
Psychology, 100, 1798–1810. doi:10.1037/apl0000031.
matter? Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 737–747. doi:10.1037
/a0035863. Skinner, B. F. (1963). Operant behavior. American Psychologist, 18, 503–
McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. 515. doi:10.1037/h0045185.
(2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127, Spector, P. E. (1978). Organizational frustration: a model and review of
249–266. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.249. the literature. Personnel Psychology, 31, 815–829. doi:10.1111
Mitchell, M. S., & Ambrose, M. L. (2007). Abusive supervision and /j.1744-6570.1978.tb02125.x.
workplace deviance and the moderating effects of negative reciproc- Taber, T. D., & Deosthali, K. (2014). Analysis of self-reported motives for
ity beliefs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1159–1168. task-related helping: implications for an integrated theory of help-
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.1159. ing. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29, 343–366. doi:10.1007
Morrison, E. W., & Robinson, S. L. (1997). When employees feel /s10869-013-9327-4.
betrayed: a model of how psychological contract violation develops. Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side
Academy of Management Review, 22, 226–256. doi:10.5465 of fairness: preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and
/AMR.1997.9707180265. disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry).
Ohly, S., & Schmitt, A. (2015). What makes us enthusiastic, angry, feel- Psychological Science, 19, 339–347. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
ing at rest or worried? Development and validation of an affective 9280.2008.02091.x.
work events taxonomy using concept mapping methodology. Taylor, P. J., Russ-Eft, D. F., & Chan, D. W. L. (2005). A meta-analytic
Journal of Business and Psychology, 30, 15–35. doi:10.1007 review of behavior modeling training. Journal of Applied
/s10869-013-9328-3. Psychology, 90, 692–709. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.4.692.
Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regu- Vianello, M., Galliani, E. M., & Haidt, J. (2010). Elevation at work: the
lation: moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportion- effects of leaders’ moral excellence. Journal of Positive Psychology,
ality. Psychological Review, 118, 57–75. doi:10.1037/a0021867. 5, 390–411. doi:10.1080/17439760.2010.516764.
Robbins, J. M., Ford, M. T., & Tetrick, L. E. (2012). Perceived unfairness Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
and employee health: a meta-analytic integration. Journal of Applied Warren, D. E., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). Deciding what’s right: the role
Psychology, 97, 235–272. doi:10.1037/a0025408. of external sanctions and embarrassment in shaping moral
J Bus Psychol

judgments in the workplace. Research in Organizational Behavior, Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: a the-
28, 81–105. doi:10.1016/j.riob.2008.04.004. oretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of af-
Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., Bommer, W. H., & Tetrick, L. E. (2002). The fective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior,
role of fair treatment and rewards in perceptions of organizational 18, 1–74.
support and leader-member exchange. Journal of Applied Weiss, H. M., Suckow, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Effects of justice
Psychology, 87, 590–598. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.590. conditions on discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,
786–794. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.786.

You might also like