You are on page 1of 11

Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No.

5: 668–678
DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arg043

Major histocompatibility complex genes,


symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in
men and women
Randy Thornhill,a Steven W. Gangestad,b Robert Miller,a Glenn Scheyd,b Julie K. McCollough,a and
Melissa Franklina
Departments of aBiology and bPsychology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,
NM 87131-1091, USA

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


Previous research indicates that the scent of developmental stability (low fluctuating asymmetry, FA) is attractive to women who
are fertile (at high-conception risk points in their menstrual cycles), but not to other women or men. Prior research also indicates
that the scent of dissimilarity in major histocompatibility complex (MHC) genes may play a role in human mate choice. We
studied the scent attractiveness to the opposite sex of t-shirts worn for 2 nights’ sleep. Our results indicate that the two olfactory
systems are independent. We repeated previous results from studies of the scent of symmetry. We repeated previous results from
MHC research in part; men, but not women, showed a preference for t-shirts with the scent of MHC dissimilarity. Women’s scent
ratings of t-shirts were uncorrelated with the wearer’s MHC dissimilarity and allele frequency, but positively correlated with the
wearer’s MHC heterozygosity. Fertile women did not exhibit any MHC trait preferences. Women’s preference for the scent of
men who were heterozygous for MHC alleles may be stronger in women who are at infertile cycle points. Men preferred the scent
of common MHC alleles, which may function to avoid mates with rare alleles that exhibit gestational drive. Men also preferred
the scent of women at fertile cycle points. The scent of facially attractive women, but not men, was preferred. Neither FA nor
facial attractiveness in either sex correlated with MHC dissimilarity to others, MHC heterozygosity, or MHC allelic rarity. Key words:
Homo sapiens, inbreeding, major histocompatibility complex, mate choice, parasites, pheromones, sexual selection. [Behav Ecol
14:668–678 (2003)]

he major histocompatibility complex (MHC) is a highly be at least partly mediated by body scent. In contrast, a study
T polymorphic gene complex (in humans often referred to
as human leukocyte antigens, HLA genes), that encodes cell-
of South American Indians (Hedrick and Black, 1997) and
a study of Japanese couples (Ihara et al., 2000) reported no
surface receptors that play a critical role in the initiation of evidence for MHC-dissimilar assortative pairing among
most immune responses. By binding and presenting peptides married couples (for review, see Penn, 2002).
derived from either endogenous or foreign proteins, the There are at least four adaptive explanations for MHC-
MHC plays a central role in the discrimination of self from dissimilarity scent preferences. First, these preferences may
non-self, thereby regulating recognition of infectious diseases. have the evolved function of inbreeding avoidance (see review
Host–pathogen coevolution is now generally thought to of literature on this hypothesis in Penn and Potts, 1999). The
explain much of MHC genetic polymorphism because of three remaining potential explanations of scent preference
MHC’s function in pathogen detection (Apanius et al., 1997; for MHC-dissimilarity are founded upon parasite-mediated
Hughes and Hughes, 1995; Penn and Potts, 1999). sexual-selection theory. The heterozygosity hypothesis posits
Increasing evidence indicates that MHC genes influence that the preference has evolved because it produces MHC-
body odor and mate choice based on body odor attractiveness. heterozygous offspring that have sound immunocompetence
Most investigations have involved house mice (Mus musculus) against more parasite types (owing to a wider array of
and humans. The mouse studies suggest a preference for pathogenic antigens recognized by the immune system, given
the scent of MHC dissimilarity (Penn, 2002; Penn and that MHC alleles show codominance; Brown, 1997; Wedekind
Potts, 1999). In humans, Wedekind et al. (1995) found that et al., 1995). Penn et al. (2002) found that heterozygous
normally ovulating women (e.g., not taking a contraceptive
house mice are more fit than homozygotes upon encounter-
pill) preferred the scent of men (on t-shirts worn for 2 nights’
ing multiple strains of pathogens. The rare-allele hypothesis
sleep) who had MHC genotypes dissimilar to their own. A
claims that the preference functions to obtain rare MHC
second study replicated the MHC-dissimilarity scent prefer-
alleles for offspring, which give defense against rapidly
ence in women not on the pill and, furthermore, found the
same scent preferences in men (Wedekind and Füri, 1997). evolving parasites that may escape recognition by immune
Interestingly, participants in these two studies also claimed systems containing common alleles or that may reduce the
that the scents of MHC-dissimilar individuals were reminis- risk of autoimmunity possibly associated with common alleles
cent of those of former or current mates. Indeed, Ober et al. (Penn and Potts, 1999). The rare-allele hypothesis implies
(1997) found that Hutterite married couples tend to be more preference for MHC dissimilarity because, to place the rare
MHC dissimilar than expected by chance, an effect that may MHC alleles in offspring by mate choice, mate choosers (most
of whom carry relatively common MHC alleles) must choose
the infrequent individuals who are dissimilar because of their
Address correspondence to R. Thornhill. E-mail: rthorn@unm.edu. rare MHC genotype. Wedekind and Füri (1997) found no
Received 16 January 2002; revised 25 September 2002; accepted 4 evidence that particular MHC genotypes were preferred, as
December 2002. the rare-allele hypothesis implies, but their study examined
Ó 2003 International Society for Behavioral Ecology responses to only six people’s scents. The diverse-genes
Thornhill et al.  Human scent attractiveness 669

hypothesis views the function of the preference as obtaining chosen as an EPC partner (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997a).
MHC alleles for offspring that are different from the parents’ Moreover, men’s symmetry predicts a relatively high frequency
own (independent of both production of heterozygous of their sexual partners’ copulatory orgasms (Thornhill et al.,
offspring and offspring with rare alleles) as well as from one 1995). Female copulatory orgasm may be a mechanism of
another because pathogens adapted to parents and other cryptic female choice by selective ejaculate retention in con-
close kin will then be less adapted to offspring (Penn and texts in which women mate with multiple partners (see Baker
Potts, 1999). and Bellis, 1995; Thornhill et al., 1995).
A prediction related to the the diverse-genes hypothesis is The effect of symmetry on sexual attractiveness appears to
that individuals should avoid mating with MHC homozygotes. be stronger in men than in women, and it is only in men
Homozygotes have offspring who vary less from each other that symmetry predicts the components of mating success
and the mate, which increases the risk of a within-family mentioned above (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997a). This
disease epidemic. The diverse genes hypothesis is supported difference is not surprising, as sexual selection has been
by a study of sticklebacks. Females preferred males with many stronger in males than females in human evolutionary history
MHC alleles over males with fewer (Reusch et al., 2001). (Buss, 1994; Geary, 1998; Symons, 1979).
According to the first two parasite-mediated sexual selec- Several lines of evidence indicate that olfactory cues or

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


tion hypotheses, the heterozygosity and rare-allele hypothe- pheromones may play a major role in the human sexual
ses, genotypic variation in MHC should be associated with selection system. First, though adults of both sexes report that
interindividual phenotypic condition. Specifically, individuals body scent of others significantly affects their sexual interest,
with MHC heterozygosity or rare MHC alleles are predicted to women report a stronger effect than men (Franzoi and
be of relatively high phenotypic quality as a result of their Herzog, 1987; Herz and Cahill, 1997; Regan and Berscheid,
greater resistance to parasites or, in the latter case, reduced 1995). Second, evidence suggests that the importance of male
autoimmunity. The outbreeding hypothesis gives no reason to scent in women’s sexual interest and arousal varies across the
expect such associations in the case of humans. In our largely menstrual cycle. Women’s olfactory sensitivity to androstenol
outbred human sample (see below), MHC homozygosity and related chemicals appears to be enhanced before
should not be a reliable marker of being inbred and thus of ovulation, near the peak of fertility in the menstrual cycle
low phenotypic quality. (Doty, 1981; Vierling and Rock, 1967; but see Amoore et al.,
A potentially important phenotypic marker of sound 1975; see also Pause et al., 1996). Androstenol, a chemical
phenotypic condition and underlying high genetic quality is precursor of androstenone, is an important contributor to
low fluctuating asymmetry (FA). FA is nondirectional de- body odor, and its production is highly sexually dimorphic;
viation from perfect bilateral symmetry in traits that are, on men excrete three times more androstenol in urine than
average, bilaterally symmetrical. FA reflects ability to deal with do women (Brooksbank, 1962; Brooksbank and Haslewood,
stresses, both genetic and environmental, during ontogeny. 1961).
Individual FA, then, is developmental maladaptation or Women’s sexual desire for men other than their primary
instability owing to the individual’s inability to achieve perfect partner and rate of EPCs also change across the menstrual
bilateral symmetry given perturbations during traits’ ontogeny cycle, peaking during the midfollicular to ovulatory phases
(review in Møller and Swaddle, 1997; Polak, 2003). (e.g., Baker and Bellis, 1995; Gangestad et al., 2002). When
Genetic perturbations known to generate FA in traits coupled with findings on cycle-related scent preferences,
include mutations, homozygosity, and genetic disturbance these shifts suggest a third connection between sex pher-
due to incomplete coadaptation among genes (Møller and omones and human sexual selection. Specifically, the fact that
Swaddle, 1997; Polak, 2003). Variation in developmental EPCs and associated desire for nonpartner men and positive
instability should often reflect heritable differences, leading evaluation of sexually dimorphic substances in human sweat
to selection for mate preferences based on traits that honestly peak near maximal menstrual-cycle fertility suggests that
advertise developmental instability (e.g., scents). The herita- selection has led to a preference in women for their
bility of developmental instability is unknown, despite the offspring’s sire and that the preference may include an
many studies that have been performed, due to low power olfactory component.
(Fuller and Houle, 2003; see also Møller and Thornhill, Based on this notion, Gangestad and Thornhill (1998b)
1997a,b; Van Dongen, 2000). Based on these studies, Gang- proposed that olfactory stimuli pertaining to men’s pheno-
estad and Thornhill (2003) estimated that the heritability of typic and genetic quality, measured by degree of body FA,
developmental instability is, on average, 0.2–0.3. positively affects men’s scent attractiveness to women,
In humans, FA may be involved in viability-based good- particularly during the fertile phase of the cycle when
genes sexual selection. In both sexes, low FA appears to be expressed preference for offspring’s sire is critical. Three
associated with increased genetic, physical, and mental health, published studies now support this hypothesis (Gangestad
including cognitive skill and IQ (Furlow et al., 1997; Thornhill and Thornhill, 1998b; Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thorn-
and Møller, 1997; Yeo et al., 2000). Compared to asymmetric hill and Gangestad, 1999b). Specifically, all studies show that
men, symmetric men seem to be more muscular, vigorous, the scent of symmetric men is attractive to women who are
and socially dominant (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997b), cycling normally (i.e., not using hormone-based contra-
have lower basal metabolic rate (Manning et al., 1997), and ceptives) and are in the fertile phases of their menstrual
may be larger in body size (Manning, 1995; Gangestad and cycles. Women using hormonal contraceptives and normally
Thornhill, 1997b) than asymmetric men. Low FA in men ovulating women in infertile phases of their cycles appear to
predicts components of mating success such as a relatively show no preference for the body scent of either symmetrical
high number of sexual partners, quicker sexual access to a new or asymmetrical men. This research did not find that men rate
romantic partner (Baker, 1997; Gangestad and Simpson, the scent of symmetrical women more attractive than the
2000; Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997b; Gangestad et al., 2001; scent of asymmetric women. If women’s mid-cycle scent
Thornhill and Gangestad, 1994), facial attractiveness (Baker, preference for low FA is caused by a preference for MHC
1997; Gangestad et al., 1994; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1994; dissimilarity, then men who, on average, are dissimilar to
but see also Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997a; Simpson et al., many others (for whatever reason) should have low FA.
1999; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b), and number of The research on the scent of men’s symmetry also gave
extrapair copulation (EPC) partners and number of times results suggesting that facial attractiveness (as judged from
670 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 5

photos) correlates positively with body scent attractiveness to Provera, or Norplant). We asked women whether they used
the opposite sex in both sexes and that ovulating women’s a contraceptive of this sort and did not include the responses
preference for the scent associated with men’s facial at- of those who did.
tractiveness is maximal when their fertility is highest across Wedekind and colleagues (Wedekind and Füri, 1997;
the menstrual cycle (Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b; also see Wedekind et al., 1995) found a different pattern of MHC
Rikowski and Grammer, 1999). One theory of human physical scent preferences for women using contraceptive pills. These
attractiveness is that it reflects, in part, genetic differences in women prefer the scent of individuals with similar MHC
viability and, from the standpoint of sexual selection, is genotypes. The pill is thought to generate a physiological
preferred partly for this reason (Gangestad, 1993; Thornhill state like that in pregnancy. Pill-users preference for MHC-
and Gangestad, 1993). Facial attractiveness, in part, reflects similar scents is not a mate preference, but instead may
judging facial hormone markers, which appear to be estrogen be a preference for the smell of genetic relatives who may
facilitated in women and androgen facilitated in men assist nepotistically.
(Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999a). The male faces that The mechanisms of infertility suppression caused by the
women in the fertile phase of their cycle most prefer are three types of hormone-based contraception used by some
more masculine than the faces they most prefer when in women in our study apparently differ (e.g., prevention of

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


infertile phases ( Johnston et al., 2001; Penton-Voak and ovulation or implantation), making it difficult to predict
Perrett, 2001; Penton-Voak et al., 1999). These results raise how women using the various hormone-based contraceptives
the possibility that scent attractiveness may be related to the should behave in scent-based mate choice. Furthermore,
chemistry of sex hormones. sample sizes of each of the three hormone-based contracep-
Overall, evidence indicates that pheromones are important tion users are too small for any meaningful estimates of
in the human sexual selection system. Pheromones appear to effects, if any.
be associated with MHC, men’s FA, sexually differentiated
odor substances, and traits of facial attractiveness. The specific
Measurement of FA
chemicals that signal MHC, FA, or hormone status may or may
not be shared. (For instance, the scent of low FA may or may Participants reported in unisex groups of up to four for an
not involve the chemistry of sex hormones; scents associated initial session. After reading and signing an informed consent
with rare or otherwise dissimilar MHC genotypes may or may form, each participant was given a brief questionnaire on
not be those associated with the scent of low FA; etc.) Whether demographic and other information (e.g., age, height,
involving the same chemistry or not, the pheromone systems weight, sexual orientation, socioeconomic status of family of
may or may not be interrelated in functional terms (e.g., they origin, lifetime number of sexual partners). Participants were
may or may not all be associated with parasite-mediated scheduled for a later date, at which time they provided a blood
sexually selected signals). To further examine their roles, sample (see below). For each participant, the right and left
including the various hypotheses for MHC-based mate choice, sides of the following 10 characters were measured using
and to explore their interrelationships, if any, we conducted a digital caliper, sensitive to 0.01 mm: ear length, ear width,
a study of men’s and women’s attraction to the body scents of elbow width, wrist width, ankle width, foot breadth, and
members of the opposite sex. The MHC of all participants was lengths of all fingers excluding the thumb. In previous
genotyped at the A, B, and DRb loci (as in Wedekind et al., samples (n . 700), these characters have been shown to
1995; Wedekind and Füri, 1997). In addition, we measured exhibit very little directional asymmetry and slight leptokur-
the body symmetry of all participants and had facial photo- tosis, as expected of FA (Furlow et al., 1997; Gangestad and
graphs of them rated for attractiveness. We recruited for the Thornhill, 1999). To assess and increase reliability, we
study women who did not use contraceptive hormones. The measured each character twice. In addition, a facial photo-
hypotheses for the MHC-dissimilarity preference all predict graph was taken with a 35-mm camera (50 mm lens), placed
that the preference should be seen at fertile cycle times. approximately 2.5 ft. from the participant’s face (using 400
Singh and Bronstad (2001) reported that men prefer the ASA color film).
scent of women who are in the fertile phase of their cycle, After measurements were taken and the questionnaire was
suggesting yet another pheromone-based system of prefer- completed, each participant was given a clean, unworn, white,
ences in humans. We examined Singh and Bronstad’s finding cotton Hanes-brand t-shirt and provided with explicit wearing
in the current study. instructions. Each was told that he/she should wear the t-shirt
2 particular nights (identical for each sex) while sleeping.
Each was also instructed to wash his/her bed sheets with
MATERIALS AND METHODS
unscented laundry detergent (provided by us) before those
With the exception of obtaining a blood sample for MHC two nights and, during the 2-day period, refrain from (1)
genetic analysis from each research participant and paying using scented soaps, deodorant, or fragrance such as
each participant $20, methods were identical to those in the perfume, cologne, or aftershave, and instead use only
study of the scent of symmetry by Thornhill and Gangestad unscented soap (which we provided); (2) eating garlic, onion,
(1999b). Research participants were 97 men and 100 women. green chile, pepperoni, pungent spices, herbs, strong cheeses,
Ages ranged from 18 to 54 for men (mean 6 SD ¼ 23.1 6 6.1) cabbage, celery, asparagus, yogurt, and lamb; (3) drinking
and 17 to 44 for women (21.2 6 5.5). Self-reported ethnicities alcohol or using recreational drugs; (4) smoking tobacco; (5)
of men were 62% Caucasian, 29% Hispanic, 2% African engaging in sex with another person; or (6) sleeping with
American, 5% Asian, 1% Native American, 1% other; of another person. Each participant was further instructed to
women, 48% Caucasian, 37% Hispanic, 2% African American, place the t-shirt in a plastic bag (provided and identified with
1% Asian, 6% Native American, 5% other. Most participants an arbitrary code number) during the day, when not worn,
received experimental course credit in an introductory and return the shirt, in the bag, the morning following the
psychology course in return for their participation in the second night at 0900 h. Almost all of male and female
research (see Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998b). Some participants returned their shirts on time, allowing their use
received grade points in a biology course. Recruitment in the scent rating phase of the study (see below). When
postings specifically asked for women who did not use participants dropped off their shirts, each filled out a brief
hormone-based contraception (i.e., birth control pills, Depo- questionnaire about guidelines, if any, they had violated
Thornhill et al.  Human scent attractiveness 671

during the 2-day period when shirts were worn during sleep. taken did not covary with scent attractiveness, and, hence, no
They were told that they would lose no experimental course adjustment was made for number of showers.)
credit or grade points for violating any of our instructions, Of the 89 women who reported to smell shirts, 21 used the
and that it was important that they be honest. The pill or another hormone-based contraceptive (Depoprovera,
questionnaire also asked whether the participant had been Norplant), 1 was postmenopausal, and 1 did not appropriately
ill, how often he or she had bathed, and whether scented soap fill in the rating sheet. Also, one woman reported an
or shampoo were used during the two-day period. Because exclusively homosexual orientation; because this study exam-
we had told participants that, should they happen to use ined attractiveness of scent in a heterosexual situation, we
deodorant during the day, they should shower before putting excluded her from analyses. Of the 77 men who reported
on the shirt, we asked whether they had showered before to smell shirts, none reported an exclusively homosexual
wearing the shirt. orientation. In total, our primary analyses of shirt raters in-
cluded 65 women who did not use hormone-based contra-
ception and 77 men.
Scent attractiveness ratings
At 1000 h of the morning that participants of one sex
Facial attractiveness ratings

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


returned shirts, the opposite-sex participants began reporting
in groups of up to five. Following informed consent, each We recruited 14 women and 15 men unfamiliar with the
woman or man was placed in a separate room for rating the t- research to rate participants’ facial attractiveness based on
shirts. Shirts had been separated into groups of approximately facial photographs. Ratings were made on a 1 (least attractive)
10 and each group placed in a box. In addition to the shirts to 10 (most attractive) scale. Raters were instructed not to rate
worn by men and women, one unworn shirt was included in pictured individuals whom they recognized. All opposite-sex
the sample. Boxes were circulated through the sample of ratings were averaged to yield an index of facial attractiveness.
raters present during a session. Though no attempt was made Internal consistency of these indices was high: for male
to fully randomize the order in which raters smelled shirts, it participants, a ¼ .90; for female participants, a ¼ .86. (We
is likely that no two raters smelled them in precisely the same used opposite-sex ratings only, though mean ratings made
order. For each shirt, raters were asked to open the plastic bag by the two sexes were highly correlated: for male participants,
and, without touching the shirt, smell it and rate the scent on r ¼ .88; for female participants, r ¼ .88; both p , .00001.)
three dimensions: (1) pleasantness, on a scale of 1 ¼ very un-
pleasant to 10 ¼ very pleasant; (2) sexiness, on a scale of 1 ¼
HLA typing
very unsexy to 10 ¼ very sexy; and (3) intensity, where 1 ¼ not
at all intense to 10 ¼ very intense. They were instructed to roll Approximately 10 ml of whole blood from each subject was
the top of the bag shut before putting it back in its box and collected by a phlebotomist into Vacutainer tubes containing
moving onto the next shirt. All researchers presenting shirts EDTA to prevent clotting. Genomic DNA was prepared for
to participants for smelling were unaware of the symmetry typing by the BioTest SSP System (BioTest Diagnostics
scores of the participants who had worn them and of the MHC Corporation, Denvile, NJ) following manufacturer’s recom-
genotypes of t-shirt raters and wearers. mended protocols. DNA was typed by polymerase chain
Women raters were also given a brief questionnaire to fill reaction (PCR) for the HLA-A, B, and DRb loci following the
out, which assessed (1) whether the woman currently used manufacturer’s recommended protocols. Briefly, this tech-
a contraceptive pill or other hormone-based contraceptive; nique uses allele-specific primers to amplify PCR products. We
(2) the first day of the woman’s last menstrual period (women determined the presence or absence of a PCR product in each
were provided a calendar to assist with this task); and (3) the reaction by resolving the products on 1.5% agarose.
typical length (in days) of the woman’s menstrual cycle. These
data allowed our calculation of the women’s probability of
Data treatment: symmetry
conception during shirt wearing and rating (see below).
In total, rating sessions lasted about 1 h and were con- Repeatabilities (rIC) for the 10 measures of signed asymme-
ducted from 1000 until 1600 h the day of and from 0800 to tries of individual traits ranged from .79 to .93 for men (all p ,
1500 h the day after the collection of the t-shirts. .00001, mean rIC ¼ .86) and for women from .76 to .93 (all p ,
Despite our instructions, the smell of fragrance (from .00001; mean rIC ¼ .84). For unsigned asymmetries, rIC for
perfume, soap or lotion) or smoke was evident on some shirts. men ranged from .63 to .87 (all p , .00001; mean rIC ¼ .73)
Scent raters were asked to indicate if they smelled a non- and for women from .60 to .87 (all p , .00001; mean rIC ¼ .74).
human odor on any of the shirts. Guided by these comments, Previous analysis on a large sample (n . 700) demonstrated
four researchers systematically smelled shirts and confirmed that the traits studied exhibit at most small directional
ones that had such odors. Of women’s shirts, 14 had smell asymmetry (see Furlow et al., 1997). As was true in that
of fragrance and 4 had smell of smoke. Of men’s shirts, 7 sample, only foot width in this study showed significant
smelled of fragrance and 3 smelled of smoke. In addition, we but small directional asymmetry (with Bonferroni correction
asked men and women whether they had broken any rules. An for number of traits; t192 ¼ 4.22, p , .001; left . right; the
additional 3 men said that they wore fragrance without mean  0 ¼ .3 SDs). No signed asymmetry exhibited sig-
showering, 4 claimed to smoke, and 3 said that they slept with nificant platykurtosis, indicative of antisymmetry. Mean g2
someone; corresponding numbers for women were 4, 8, and was 0.78 across the traits, and hence distributions were
2. Finally, we asked whether participants had been sick over mildly leptokurtic, as expected of FA (see Gangestad and
the preceding 2 days. An additional 9 men and 8 women Thornhill, 1999).
reported sickness. The scent of men who had been sick To guard against the effects of large asymmetries due to
tended to be less attractive than other men’s scent (r ¼ .16, injury, FA of traits that participants reported as injured by
p , .10); sick women did not smell less attractive (r ¼ .12, ns). break or sprain were excluded in these analyses if they were
As a conservative measure, all these participants’ shirts were greater than the mean (4.6% for men, 1.1% for women). For
eliminated from analysis. A total of 56 men’s shirts and 48 measurement purposes, the mean FA for the trait in the whole
women’s shirts were used for analysis. (Unlike in a previous sample of participants was substituted in these instances (see
study [Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b], number of showers Thornhill and Gangestad, 1994).
672 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 5

We calculated an aggregated FA score in two different ways. scored 1. If both individuals were homozygous for A1,
First, each trait’s absolute asymmetry was divided by the mean similarity was scored 2. Similarity scores could thus range
trait size ([right þ left]/2) for that participant, and the FA of from 0 to 6. Actual similarity for pairs averaged 1.25 alleles
all 10 characters was summed to yield an overall index (SD ¼ 0.37). MHC heterozygosity was the number of loci at
(relative FA). Second, each trait’s absolute asymmetry was which the participant was heterozygous. Mean heterozygosity
standardized (divided) by the sample mean of trait size and was 2.69 (SD ¼ 0.54).
summed to yield an overall index (absolute FA). These two Because our multiethnic sample may have unique charac-
measures correlated .97 with each other and yielded nearly teristics, MHC allele frequencies were calculated within the
identical results. Thus, we report results for just one measure sample. Total number of allele observations within the sample
(the first) below. The intraclass correlation across the was 352, 348, and 348 for the A, B, and DRb loci, at which
two measurements for the summed index was .82 for men we detected 20, 28, and 14 different alleles, respectively. Rela-
(F96,97 ¼ 10.10, p , .000001) and .84 for women (F99,100 tive frequencies of all alleles are given in Figure 1. We com-
¼ 11.13, p , .000001). The mean composite FA was .174 (SD puted the commonness of a participant’s alleles as the
¼ .052), close to the mean observed in previous studies proportion of alleles within the sample of the type possessed
(Gangestad and Thornhill, 1999). by the participant, averaged across the participant’s six alleles.
Commonness scores averaged 0.113 (SD ¼ 0.032, range ¼

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


0.04–0.21).
Data treatment: scent attractiveness
For women, we estimated fertility (probability of conception
RESULTS
following sex) on the basis of their day of the cycle and
conception values reported in the medical literature (Baker For all scent effects that have been found in prior research on
and Bellis, 1995; Jöchle, 1973). We did so in two ways, one humans (preference for the scent of MHC dissimilarity,
based on a forward method, the other on a backward method women’s preference for the scent of symmetrical men when
(see Baker and Bellis, 1995). For the forward method, we fertile, preference for the scents of facially attractive individ-
simply took the day of the cycle that women were at on the uals, men’s preference for the scent of fertile women), we
day of smelling based on their reported first day of last used directed tests (Rice and Gaines, 1994), which use a p
menstruation and used the actuarial table to estimate value of .04 for a predicted relationship and .01 for
probability of conception. For the backward method, we took a relationship opposite of prediction (rather than .025 for
into account women’s reported cycle length (mean ¼ 27.9 each, as with a two-tailed test). All directed tests are labeled
days, SD ¼ 4.0; average of two reports, which correlated .82, as such. In all other instances, we used two-tailed tests.
p , .0001). Women who have longer cycles, on average, ovulate Although unsigned (i.e., absolute) FA tends to be non-
later in the cycle than women who have shorter cycles normally distributed (Swaddle et al., 1994), Monte Carlo
(Baker and Bellis, 1995). We assumed that the typical day of analyses reveal that significance tests on parametric correla-
ovulation was about 15 days before the end of their typical tions involving FA are robust (Gangestad and Thornhill,
cycle (e.g., day 14 in a 29-day cycle) and that the average 1998b), and hence we performed standard parametric anal-
day that actuarial tables are based on is day 14. Women’s yses on this variable.
probability of conception was then based on how far they
had come toward or after their assumed day of ovulation
Descriptive statistics
and the actuarial tabled values for women in general. The
two methods yielded values that correlated .62 (p , .0001), On average, men’s scents were rated 4.54 (SD ¼ 0.95) on the
and results based on each were highly similar. As each 1–10 attractiveness scale. Women’s shirts received a mean
method may have some validity that the other lacks, we rating of 4.71 (SD ¼ 0.63). There was no significant sex
averaged the two sets of values for our main analyses. (Full difference in mean scent attractiveness (t102 ¼ 1.08). Men’s
results are available from the authors.) The minimum value shirts tended to receive more variable ratings than did
was .01, the maximum .39 (approximately day 12 of the women’s shirts, though this difference was only marginally
cycle). Fertility steeply rises from near-zero to near .2 shortly significant (F1,102 ¼ 2.94, p ¼ .089; Levene’s test for equality of
after the average end of menses (days 5–6) and sharply falls variances). On average, men rated the unworn t-shirt’s scent
from ..2 to ,.1 immediately after mean ovulation (day 14). attractiveness 3.82 and women rated it 4.02. On average,
Undoubtedly, our method of estimating conception risk is women’s and men’s scents were rated by the other sex as
likely to be in error for some individual women (though, more attractive than the blank shirt’s scent (t76 ¼ 4.97 and
across all women, it should correlate highly with actual t64 ¼ 3.38, respectively, p , .002).
risk). As it is unlikely that error introduced by our method
spuriously generates significant relationships, the associa-
Preferences for MHC dissimilarity
tions we report probably underestimate rather than over-
estimate true relationships with conception risk. Analyses that treat the t-shirt wearer as the unit of analysis
The mean correlation between individual participants’ (and examine whether raters who possess dissimilar MHC
‘‘pleasantness’’ and ‘‘sexiness’’ ratings was .78 for women genotypes rate their scents more favorably) control for factors
raters and .82 for men. Hence, these ratings were averaged for that contribute to overall differences in scent attractiveness
each sex into a total attractiveness index for reported analyses. and, hence, should offer the greatest power to detect
Analyses on specific ratings yielded highly similar results. preferences for MHC dissimilarity (Wedekind and Füri,
1997). Using the procedures of Wedekind and Füri (1997),
we computed for each t-shirt wearer a correlation between
Data treatment: MHC genetics
individuals’ ratings of scent attractiveness and the number of
MHC similarity for individual male–female pairs was com- MHC alleles shared between the t-shirt wearer and the rater.
puted as the total number of alleles shared across the three We then asked whether these correlations differed signifi-
loci. Hence, if both individuals were A1, A3 at the A locus, cantly from zero in the predicted negative direction.
similarity for that locus was scored 2. If one individual was We found no evidence that men’s scents were preferred
homozygous for A1 and the other was A1, A3, similarity was by women with dissimilar MHC genotypes (mean r ¼ .025,
Thornhill et al.  Human scent attractiveness 673

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


Figure 1
Relative frequencies of MHC alleles at A, B, and DRb loci.

t54 ¼ 1.39, directed). In contrast, we did find women’s scents to MHC alleles with others (i.e., because they have common
be preferred by men with dissimilar MHC genotypes (mean r ¼ alleles) should be particularly preferred by those who are
.033, t47 ¼ 1.74, directed p ¼ .040). The mean correlations dissimilar, whereas targets who, on average, share few MHC
significantly differed from one another (t101 ¼ 2.30, p ¼ .024), alleles with others (i.e., because they have rare alleles) should
revealing a sex difference in the preference for MHC be more preferred by those who are relatively similar (e.g.,
dissimilarity. Specifically, the attractiveness of women’s scents share even a single allele). This hypothesis was not supported.
to men appears to be positively influenced by MHC For the 48 female targets, the association ran in the direction
dissimilarity to a degree greater than the attractiveness of anticipated if there were a preference for the scent of those
men’s scents to women. with moderately similar MHC genotypes, but fell short of
Analyses that treated the scent rater as the unit of analysis significance (r ¼ .19). For the 55 male targets, the associa-
(and examined whether raters prefer the scents of those with tion between their mean MHC sharing with others and the
dissimilar MHC genotypes) were also conducted. We com- correlation between raters’ attraction to their scent and MHC
puted raters’ preference for dissimilar MHC as the slope of sharing with the target ran in the opposite direction (r ¼.38,
their ratings regressed on MHC dissimilarity (b). For neither p , .01). (Hence, the scent of men who had rare MHC alleles
men nor women raters did we find evidence of preference for and therefore shared relatively few alleles with men were
MHC dissimilarity: For men, mean b ¼ .007, t69 ¼ .42, directed more likely to be preferred by women with dissimilar MHC
ns; for women, mean b ¼ .051, t61 ¼ 1.41, directed ns. The genotypes.) These correlations significantly differ from one
mean preferences for the two sexes did not differ (t130 ¼ another (z ¼ 2.91, p , .01).
1.17). Again, these analyses are less powerful than those
treating the t-shirt wearer as the unit of analysis, which may
Preferences for symmetry
account for the difference in results.
Despite no evidence for an overall preference in women for As in previous research (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998b;
the scent of MHC-dissimilar men, women’s preference for Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b), we calculated a preference
MHC dissimilarity could be moderated by their fertility status for the scent of symmetry for each woman, which is the slope
across the menstrual cycle. However, we found no evidence of of the woman’s scent attractiveness ratings regressed on men’s
a role for fertility status. Women’s probability of conception symmetry (with positive values reflecting preference for
across the cycle had a near-zero correlation with their symmetry and negative values preference for asymmetry).
preference for MHC dissimilarity (r ¼ .029, directed). We correlated these preferences with women’s probability of
Possibly, preference for dissimilarity is not a linear function. conception. Consistent with previous studies, women’s pref-
Perhaps, for instance, a moderate degree of dissimilarity is erence for symmetry was predicted by their fertility status (r ¼
preferred, with high levels of similarity and dissimilarity .269, directed p ¼ .019). Regression analyses revealed that, at
disfavored (see Penn, 2002). If that is the case, then targets’ zero probability of conception, women had a significant
mean level of MHC sharing with others should predict, for preference for symmetry (t63 ¼ 2.45, p , .017), a pattern
individual targets, the correlation between the target’s MHC unique to this study. Moreover, this preference for symmetry
sharing with a rater and the rater’s attraction to the target’s increased as a function of their conception risk (see Figure 2).
scent: those targets who, on average, share relatively many Mean scent-attractiveness ratings made by women (n ¼ 22)
674 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 5

Table 1
Correlations between FA, facial attractiveness, and MHC variables

Men Women

Facial Facial
attractiveness FA attractiveness FA

Mean MHC dissimilarity


to others .084 .045 .078 .066
MHC heterozygosity .066 .096 .137 .161
Mean commonness of
MHC alleles .093 .127 .155 .089

All correlations ns. Correlation between FA (fluctuating asymmetry)


and facial attractiveness: .086 for men, .029 for women, ns.

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


Figure 2 the six alleles an individual possesses at the three loci with
Preference of normally ovulating (not using hormone-based contra- mean scent attractiveness. For women, commonness of alleles
ception) women for the scent of symmetry as a function of their significantly predicted the attractiveness of their scents, such
probability of conception, based on values reported in the medical that those with more common alleles tended to have more
literature (Baker and Bellis, 1995; Jöchle, 1973); r ¼ .269, p ¼ .019. attractive scents (r ¼ .309, p ¼ .032). Men’s scent attractive-
ness was not associated with the commonness of their alleles
(r ¼ .025, ns). Sex did not significantly moderate the
who had conception risks of at least 0.15 (day 6–day 14) relationship between commonness of alleles and scent
correlated (.29; directed p ¼ .02) with men’s FA, whereas attractiveness (t96 ¼ .980). Women’s preferences for rarity of
mean ratings made by all other women (n ¼ 27) were more MHC alleles (calculated as the slope of their ratings regressed
loosely correlated (.11; ns) with men’s FA; these correlations on men’s MHC allelic rarity) did not significantly covary with
significantly differed from one another (t53 ¼ 2.57, p ¼ .008). their fertility status (r ¼ .132).
Women’s conception risk did not significantly correlate with
the mean rating they gave all scents (r ¼ .04, ns) or the Preference for the scent of facial attractiveness
rating they gave the unworn shirt’s scent (r ¼ .06, ns). As also
found by Thornhill and Gangestad (1999b), women’s mean As found in previous studies (Rikowski and Grammer, 1999;
scent attractiveness to men did not correlate with women’s FA Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b), women’s facial attractiveness
(r ¼ .063, ns). predicted their scent attractiveness (r ¼ .268, directed p ¼
.041). Unlike in the previous studies, men’s facial attractiveness
did not predict their scent attractiveness (r ¼ .053, directed
Preferences for MHC heterozygosity ns). Nonetheless, sex did not significantly moderate the
We examined preferences for MHC heterozygosity by corre- relationship between facial and scent attractiveness (t100 ¼
lating the number of MHC loci at which individuals were 1.30).
heterozygotes with their mean scent attractiveness to the Previously, Thornhill and Gangestad (1999b) found that
opposite sex. Male heterozygosity was positively correlated women’s probability of conception predicted their preference
with scent attractiveness (r ¼ .275, p ¼ .049). Follow-up for the scent of facially attractive men. In this sample, though
analyses showed that this effect was driven by heterozygosity at the correlation was in the same direction, no significant
the B locus (r ¼ .329, p ¼ .017); no effect was observed for association emerged (r ¼ .096, directed). In this sample,
either the A or DRb locus (r ¼ .025 and .161, respectively, FA did not predict men’s or women’s facial attractiveness
ns). As these correlations do not significantly differ from one (r ¼ .086 and .029, respectively, ns).
another, however, these locus-specific differences may not be
robust and must be interpreted cautiously. Female heterozy- Male preferences for fertile women
gosity did not predict scent attractiveness (r ¼ .004, ns). Sex Women’s estimated probability of conception at the time of
did not interact with heterozygosity to predict scent attrac- wearing the t-shirt was correlated with their mean scent
tiveness (t93 ¼ 1.56, p ¼ .12). attractiveness. As found by Singh and Bronstad (2001), men
We examined whether women’s preferences for heterozy- were more attracted to the scents of women of high fertility
gosity change across the menstrual cycle. These preferences status than those of low fertility status (r ¼ .334, directed
were calculated as the slope of women’s ratings regressed on p ¼ .021).
men’s MHC heterozygosity. They correlated with women’s
probability of conception at a level just short of statistical Associations of FA and facial attractiveness with MHC
significance(r ¼ .239, t63 ¼ 1.96, p ¼. 055), such that women dissimilarity, heterozygosity, and commonness of alleles
outside of the fertile phase tended to have the strongest scent
preferences for heterozygosity. The correlations of women’s Neither FA nor facial attractiveness was significantly predicted
preference for symmetry and preference for heterozygosity by mean MHC dissimilarity to others, MHC heterozygosity, or
with conception risk differed significantly from one another commonness of MHC alleles within either sex (see Table 1).
(t62 ¼ 2.87, p ¼ .006), indicating that these female preferences These findings bolster the conclusion that, to the extent that
at least partly invoke different systems. the scent of MHC dissimilar individuals is favored, the
phenomenon is unrelated to women’s preference for the
scent of symmetrical men when fertile. They also add further
Preferences for rare MHC alleles
evidence that women’s scent preference for men who are
We examined preferences for rare MHC alleles by correlating heterozygous at MHC sites is distinct from their scent
the mean frequency of MHC alleles within this sample across preference for symmetrical men.
Thornhill et al.  Human scent attractiveness 675

Additional analyses on the whole sample. Our finding (based on the whole
sample) that ethnicity has no effect on scent attractiveness in
For exploratory purposes, we correlated men’s mean scent
either sex suggests that our results on MHC scent effects are
attractiveness rating with their age, weight, and self-reported
meaningful. However, our ethnic categorization was self-
socioeconomic status of family of origin (on a 5-point scale,
reported using broad categories.
where 5 ¼ upper class, 3 ¼ middle class, 1 ¼ lower class).
These correlations were not significant (r ¼ .04, .21, and .08,
respectively). For women, mean scent attractiveness correlat- The scent of symmetry
ed at a marginally significant level with age (r ¼ .28, p ¼ .058
Compared to the body scent of relatively asymmetric men,
and socioeconomic status, r ¼ .26, p ¼ .077, but not with
relatively symmetrical men’s scent is most attractive to
weight, r ¼ .10, ns). Analyses comparing scent attractiveness
normally ovulating women (women not using hormonal
of men and women across ethnicities revealed no significant
contraception) during their period of high fertility in the
differences (F5,50 ¼ .44 and F4,47 ¼.38).
menstrual cycle. We have found little evidence that women
We conducted additional exploratory analyses. We corre-
show scent preference related to male body symmetry during
lated preference for the scent of symmetry with the judge’s
cycle times of low fertility. In this study only, and not in the
own scent attractiveness, age, FA, physical attractiveness, and

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


studies of Gangestad and Thornhill (1998b) and Thornhill
number of self-reported sex partners. Younger women par-
and Gangestad (1999b), women with zero conception risk
ticularly preferred the scent of symmetrical men (r ¼ .34, p ¼
exhibited a significant preference for the scent of symmetry.
.005), even with women’s conception risk controlled (r ¼ .30,
That women at high conception risk in their menstrual cycles
p ¼ .015). In contrast, older men particularly preferred the
prefer the scent of male symmetry has now been found in four
scent of symmetrical women (r ¼ .24, p ¼ .045). These
separate studies (also see Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998b;
correlations significantly differ(z ¼ 3.42, p , .001). Men
Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thornhill and Gangestad,
whose scent was rated as less attractive also preferred the scent
1999b). Although not examined in this study, prior studies
of symmetrical women (r ¼ .38, p ¼ .009; n ¼ 48 for this
have consistently found that women using hormone-based
analysis). Women’s scent attractiveness did not predict their
contraception do not show the preference for the scent of
preference for the scent of symmetry (r ¼ .14, ns). Again,
men’s symmetry. Moreover, the effect of the scent of symmetry
the correlations significantly differed between the sexes (z ¼
appears to be sex-specific. We found no evidence that men
2.18, p , .03). No other correlations were significant. We
find the scent of symmetrical women more attractive, a result
correlated women’s preference for MHC dissimilarity, MHC
that replicates that of Thornhill and Gangestad (1999b).
heterozygosity, and MHC allele frequency with the same
Apparently, the attractiveness of the symmetry scent to
variables. Only a single correlation was significant (men’s age
women does not arise from hygienic differences between men
negatively predicted their preference for heterozygosity), one
in relation to their symmetry (e.g., number of times they
that could easily be due to chance.
showered; see Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b). Nor does it
Older and more facially attractive men particularly pre-
appear to arise from not following our research guidelines for
ferred the scent of women near ovulation (r ¼ .23 and .25, p ,
diet, alcohol and drug use, or sleeping behavior over the
.05). Men’s preferences tended to covary with one another.
period in question (see also Thornhill and Gangestad,
Men’s preference for the scent of symmetrical women was
1999b). The symmetry of men was unknown to the women
negatively associated with their preference for MHC hetero-
rating scents and to the researchers involved in collecting the
zygotic women (r ¼ .32, p ¼ .004). Their preference for
scent ratings; the only identifying information on the plastic
women near peak fertility also negatively predicted their
bag containing each shirt was an arbitrary participant number.
preference for heterozygotic women as well as their prefer-
Although women exhibit increased olfactory sensitivity during
ence for women with rare MHC alleles (r ¼ .24 and .26,
the fertile aspect of the menstrual cycle (Doty, 1981; Kohl and
p , .04). Finally, men’s preference for the scent of MHC-
Francoeur, 1995; Vroon, 1997), the odor preference for
dissimilar women was negatively associated with their prefer-
symmetry shown by normally ovulating women cannot readily
ence for facially attractive women (r ¼ .36, p ¼ .003).
be explained by an increased general olfactory sensitivity or
(Preference for MHC dissimilarity also predicted preference
response with high fertility. There is no significant relation-
for rare MHC alleles [r ¼ .32, p ¼ .006], an association that is
ship between women’s intensity ratings of the shirts and
not surprising, as women with rare alleles are relatively
male symmetry (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998a; Thornhill
dissimilar to men at MHC.) No female preferences signifi-
and Gangestad, 1999b). In sum, we know of no explanation
cantly correlated with one another.
for our results other than that normally ovulating women
use a chemical(s) in men’s sweat or skin as a basis for
discriminating men who have and have not experienced
DISCUSSION
perturbations that generate FA. Thornhill and Gangestad
This study used t-shirts worn by each sex for 2 night’s sleep (1999b) provided a number of hypotheses about the
and then smelled by the opposite sex to examine the chemistry of the scent of men’s symmetry.
relationships between heterosexual attractiveness of one’s Fertile women’s preference for the scent of symmetry may,
body odor and one’s body fluctuating asymmetry (FA), facial in part, account for the pattern of pair-bonded women
attractiveness, MHC dissimilarity, MHC heterozygosity, and tending to choose symmetric men as EPC partners. Given that
possession of rare MHC alleles. Our results address four symmetric men appear to invest less in their romantic re-
adaptive hypotheses that have been proposed for scent-based, lationships than asymmetric men, these choices are probably
MHC disassortative mate preferences: outbreeding, produc- for genetic benefits rather than for increased male investment
tion of MHC-heterozygous offspring or of offspring with rare (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1997a,b).
MHC alleles, and increased within-brood MHC diversity. Conceivably, the scent of symmetric men is preferred by
Additionally, our results address issues concerning the prior women at mid-cycle because of a preference for adequate
finding that women, when fertile in their menstrual cycle, sperm. Manning et al. (1998; also Baker, 1997) have shown
particularly prefer the scent of symmetrical men. a positive correlation between ejaculate size, sperm quality,
MHC allele frequencies may differ among ethnic groups. and body symmetry in men. This, however, is not necessarily
Our sample was multiethnic, and we based allele frequencies an alternative to good-genes choice.
676 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 5

Preferences for MHC dissimilarity that men have stronger preference for MHC dissimilarity than
The results from our study of the scent attractiveness of MHC do women. If MHC-disassortative mating functions to obtain
dissimilarity repeated, in part, earlier studies. Our study good genes for offspring, women are expected to exhibit
reveals that men find the scent of MHC dissimilarity attractive, a stronger preference than men. These lines of evidence,
but we detected no clear MHC-dissimilarity preference in then, weaken the three parasite-mediated sexual selection
women. The sex difference in preference for MHC dissimi- hypotheses as explanations for MHC preferences.
larity was statistically significant. Overall, then, there is positive Nonetheless, a good-genes–choice hypothesis could still
evidence of MHC-dissimilarity preference exhibited by men apply if women’s preference for MHC dissimilarity at midcycle
from both of the t-shirt studies that have examined it is only manifested under conditions in which the benefits of
(Wedekind and Füri, 1997; present study). Women have good-genes mate choice can be realized (e.g., when diseases
shown an MHC-dissimilarity preference in two of three t-shirt are prevalent). Such conditionality in the preference may
studies that tested for it (positive in Wedekind et al., 1995; have been selected in the context of EPC due to the
Wedekind and Füri, 1997; negative in present study). The concomitant high potential costs (e.g., divestment of pair-
negative assortative pairing in Hutterite couples reported by bond mate). There is evidence for conditionality in the MHC-
Ober et al. (1997) could be the result of an MHC dissimilarity odor system of mice. Mice infected with mouse hepatitis virus

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


preference in only one sex or both sexes and hence does not produced more MHC-heterozygous offspring than sham-
shine any light on the issue of the consistency of the effect in infected mice, which may result from the expression of the
each sex. Given that our study had a larger sample of women MHC-dissimilarity preference only when a virus threatens
wearing shirts and more raters than either study showing offspring (Rülicke et al., 1998).
a positive effect for women’s scent and detected an MHC- A stronger scent preference in males for MHC dissimilarity,
dissimilarity effect for men, the absence of an effect for as well as the absence of a link between conception risk and
women may be meaningful. However, the two t-shirt studies this preference in women, is also inconsistent with the
finding an effect for women involved an ethnically homoge- inbreeding avoidance hypothesis. Females are expected to
neous sample that may have reduced individual differences in have a higher cost of inbreeding than males as a result of
odor preferences unrelated to MHC; these two studies also more female than male parental investment.
differed from our study in some other ways. One possible explanation for why MHC preferences appear
We did not find any evidence of a relationship between to be stronger in men than women is that they partly function
women’s conception risk across the menstrual cycle and their in men to increase ability to invest discriminatively in their
preference for the scent of MHC dissimilarity. Thus our results own offspring. Both sexes may partly use odors of offspring to
indicate that women at high risk of conceiving do prefer the detect kinship (Gall and Weisfeld, 2001). Potentially, a male
scent of symmetry but not the scent of MHC dissimilarity. could most reliably detect by olfaction his own MHC alleles in
It should be kept in mind, however, that Wedekind and offspring if he shares few alleles with his mate.
colleagues found scent preference for MHC dissimilarity in
women at relatively high conception risk, but none for women
who were infertile due to contraceptive-pill use. Preferences for MHC heterozygosity
Based on our results, we conclude that men’s scent of MHC heterozygosity was not correlated with either facial
symmetry and men’s scent of MHC dissimilarity involve attractiveness or with FA in either sex. Heterozygosity is
different pheromonal systems, at least in part. This conclusion sometimes positively associated with relatively low FA across
is bolstered by the fact that FA and mean MHC dissimilarity to species, and there are data suggesting this association from
others does not covary across individuals (of both sexes). some studies of humans (reviewed in Møller and Swaddle,
Either different chemicals activate one receptor adaptation or 1997; Thornhill and Møller, 1997).
multiple receptors are involved, each with chemical specificity. Women appear to prefer the scent of MHC-heterozygous
The receptor responsible for detecting the scent of symmetry men. Furthermore, a trend in the data suggests that women
would appear to be sexually dimorphic in function or present may prefer the scent of MHC-heterozygous men particularly
only in females. Furthermore, the sensitivity of this detector when outside of the fertile phase of the cycle. Women’s scent
may be affected by menstrual cycle hormones. At least two preference for MHC heterozygosity may be analogous to the
different organs appear to mediate scent perception in female stickleback’s scent preference for males with high MHC
humans: the vomeronasal organ (e.g., Wysocki, 1989) and diversity found by Reusch et al. (2001). We found no evidence
the main olfactory epithelium (Engen, 1982). that men have a scent preference for MHC heterozygosity.
Because women’s attraction to the scent of men’s symmetry If future research confirms a greater female preference for
is only seen in normally ovulating women at high conception the scent of MHC-heterozygous men outside of the fertile
risk, it has been hypothesized that this attraction is an phase, two alternative explanations appear plausible. First,
adaptation that functions to obtain for offspring genes that the function of choosing a heterozygous mate could be to
encode developmental stability and related overall fitness, diversify a brood’s MHC, thereby reducing the chances of a
particularly through EPC (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998b; within-family epidemic caused by a pathogen. This preference
Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b, 2003). The lack of a detect- would be more important for choosing a long-term mate (i.e.,
able association between women’s preference for MHC one with whom a female would have multiple offspring), and
dissimilarity and conception risk suggests that the MHC selection for the preference may have been stronger on
preference does not involve good-genes mate choice because females than on males. The preference may be attenuated
the benefit of choosing good genes—placement of a mate’s midcycle because other preferences, such as preferences for
genes in offspring at conception—cannot be gained in the intrinsic genetic quality, increase in potency at that time.
absence of preference during times of high-conception risk. Second, infertile women’s preference for heterozygous mates
This interpretation also applies to what some investigators may reflect adaptation for securing male material benefits for
would label compatible genes (Penn, 2002; Zeh and Zeh, self and/or offspring. If heterozygosity increases survival or
2001). The interpretation that good-genes (including com- vigor, it might promote male investment capability. Although
patible-genes) mate choice is not the basis of women’s MHC- we found no evidence that MHC heterozygosity is associated
dissimilarity preference is further supported by our finding with developmental instability, it could be associated with
Thornhill et al.  Human scent attractiveness 677

disease resistance (see Penn et al., 2002; Penn and Potts, Men’s preferences for women at high conception risk
1999). Given the substantial MHC diversity that exists, MHC
We also found that men exhibit significant scent preferences
heterozygosity should be only weakly heritable and hence
for women who are at high conception risk in the menstrual
not be the basis of mate choice for producing heterozygous
cycle. Using a within-subject design, Singh and Bronstad
offspring. In either case, as the correlation between women’s
(2001) previously found evidence for this preference. Using
conception risk across the cycle and women’s preference for
a between-subject design similar to the current study, we
the scent of symmetry differed significantly from the cor-
previously did not find this preference (Thornhill and
relation between conception risk and preference for hetero-
Gangestad, 1999b). The within-subject design is statistically
zygosity, the two preference systems may involve distinct
more powerful (as it controls for individual variation in scent
pheromone systems, at least in part.
across women), and we suspect that the variation in this result
is due to the lower power of our between-subject design. The
Preferences for rare alleles chemical basis for this effect is unknown. The scent of
chemicals similar to estrogen evoke hypothalamic responses
We found no evidence that either sex prefers scents associated
in men, but not in women (Savic et al., 2001), which may
with rare MHC alleles. Indeed, men in our study preferred
mediate this effect. Alternatively, the effect could involve

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


scents associated with common MHC alleles. No association
variation in copulins present in women’s vaginal secretions.
between women’s conception risk and preference for the scent
There is some evidence that men show an olfactory, sexual
of rare MHC alleles was detected. A rare-allele preference is
response to copulins (Grammer and Jutte, 1997).
predicted from the hypothesis that MHC-dependent mating
preferences function to generate offspring that have reduced
autoimmune problems (associated with common MHC We thank P. Andrews, T. Armijo-Prewitt, M. Aronov, C. Fincher, K.
alleles) or that cannot be invaded by parasites as a result of Ganster, K. Schancer, C. Schwenke, S. Singh, N. Terpstra, and J. Vigil
possessing alleles to which parasites are not well adapted. for their assistance in measuring participants, collecting other data,
and data entry. C. Copelin’s extra effort and professional attitude
Gestational drive (Haig, 1997), in which a maternal allele allowed blood samples to be obtained from almost all our research
disfavors offspring during gestation that do not inherit it, participants. The research was supported by a grant from the Sense of
could account for a sex difference in preference for the scent Smell Institute (formerly known as the Olfactory Research Fund). C.
of common alleles. Gestational drive is delayed meiotic drive Fincher, D. Penn, C. Wedekind, R. Ydenberg, and two anonymous
and, like meiotic drive, is advantageous to the alleles involved, reviewers provided useful comments on the manuscript.
but disadvantageous to the remainder of the maternal
genome. Gestational drive may especially be a property of
rare female alleles because any driving effects of common REFERENCES
alleles are likely to be successfully countered by the evolution
of genes that prevent driving. A mate preference in males for Amoore JE, Pelosi P, Forrester LJ, 1975. Specific anosmias to 5 alpha-
females who possess common MHC alleles may function to androst-16-en-3-one and alpha-pentadecalactone: the urinous and
avoid mates with alleles that show gestational drive and musky primary odours. Chem Sens Flavour 2:401–425.
thereby reduce the likelihood of abortion of offspring. Apanius V, Penn D, Slev P, Ruff LR, Potts WK, 1997. The nature of
selection on the major histocompatibility complex. Crit Rev
Females do not face this problem in mate choice. Immunol 17:179–224.
MHC allele rarity did not covary with symmetry in either Baker RR, 1997. Copulation, masturbation and infidelity: state-of-the-
sex. It was previously hypothesized that the scent attractive- art. In: New aspects of human ethology (Schmitt A, Atzwanger K,
ness of men’s symmetry could arise if symmetric men possess Grammer K, Schafer K, eds). New York: Publisher; 163–188.
rare MHC alleles and women prefer rare or dissimilar MHC Baker RR, Bellis MA, 1995. Human sperm competition: copulation,
alleles (Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998b; Thornhill and masturbation and infidelity. London: Chapman and Hall.
Gangestad, 1999b). This study provides no support for this Brooksbank BWL, 1962. Urinary excretion of androst-16-en-3 alpha-ol
hypothesis. in human male axillary sweat. Experientia 30:864–865.
Brooksbank BWL, Haslewood GAD, 1961. The estimation of androst-
16-en3 alpha-ol in human urine. Partial synthesis of androstenol
Preferences for scents associated with facial attractiveness and its beta-glucosiduronic acid. Biochem J 80:488–496.
Brown JL, 1997. A theory of mate choice based on heterozygosity.
Women’s facial attractiveness, but not men’s, significantly Behav Ecol 8:60–65.
correlated with body scent attractiveness. Three studies to Buss DM, 1994. The evolution of desire: strategies of human mating.
date have reported this association for women (also see New York: Basic Books.
Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thornhill and Gangestad, Doty RL, 1981. Olfactory communication in humans. Chem Senses
1999b), indicating that women’s facial attractiveness and scent 6:351–376.
attractiveness convey overlapping information. Engen T, 1982. The perception of odors. New York: Academic Press.
Our study did not replicate the finding from two previous Franzoi SL, Herzog ME, 1987. Judging physical attractiveness: what
body aspects do we use? Pers Soc Psychol Bull 13:19–33.
studies that women with high conception risk prefer the scent Fuller RC, Houle D, 2003. Inheritance of developmental instability.
of facially attractive men (Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; In: Developmental instability: causes and consequences (Polak M,
Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999b). The association between ed). New York: Oxford University Press; 157–183.
men’s facial attractiveness and their masculinity varies across Furlow FB, Armijo–Pruett T, Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1997.
populations and samples (for a review, see Thornhill and Fluctuating asymmetry and psychometric intelligence. Proc R Soc
Gangestad, 1999a), which may partly account for the instability Lond B 264:1–8.
of this finding. Possibly, women prefer the scents of more Gall JA, Weisfeld, GE, 2001. Olfaction-mediated recognition of and
androgenized men midcycle (due to androgen-derived sub- affinity for kin of different degrees of consanguinity. Paper
stances in sweat; e.g., Grammer, 1993), but in some samples presented at 2001 Meeting of Human Behavior and Evolution
Society, London.
these men are not the most attractive in terms of scent. Gangestad SW, 1993. Sexual selection and physical attractiveness:
Facial attractiveness was not significantly correlated with implications for mating dynamics. Hum Nat 4:205–236.
MHC dissimilarity, MHC heterozygosity, or commonness of Gangestad SW, Bennett KL, Thornhill R, 2001. A latent variable
MHC alleles in either sex and hence may be independent of model of developmental instability in relation to men’s sexual
these MHC traits. behavior. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1677–1684.
678 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 5

Gangestad SW, Simpson JA, 2000. The evolution of human mating: Penn DJ, 2002. The scent of genetic compatibility: sexual selection
the role of trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behav Brain Sci 23: and the major histocompatiblity complex. Ethology 801:1–21.
573–644. Penn DJ, Damjanovich K, Potts W, 2002. MHC heterozygosity confers
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1997a. The evolutionary psychology of a selective advantage against multiple-strains infections. Proc Natl
extra-pair sex: the role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evol Hum Behav Acad Sci USA 99:11260–11264.
18:69–88. Penn DJ, Potts WK, 1999. The evolution of mating preferences and
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1997b. Human sexual selection and major histocompatibility genes. Am Nat 153:145–164.
developmental stability. In: Evolutionary social psychology (Simp- Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI, 2001. Male facial attractiveness: perceived
son JA, Kenrick DT, eds). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates; personality and shifting female preferences for male traits across
169–195. the menstrual cycle. Adv Study Behav 30:219–259.
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1998a. The analysis of fluctuating Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI, Castles DL, Kobayashi T, Burt DM, Murray
asymmetry redux: the robustness of parametric studies. Anim LK, Minamisawa R, 1999. Female preference for male faces changes
Behav 55:497–501. cyclically. Nature 399:741–742.
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1998b. Menstrual cycle variation in Polak M, 2003. Developmental instability: causes and consequences.
women’s preference for the scent of symmetrical men. Proc R Soc New York: Oxford University Press.
Lond B 265:927–933. Regan PC, Berscheid E, 1995. Gender differences in beliefs about the
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 1999. Individual differences in de- causes of male and female sexual desire. Pers Relat 2:345–358.

Downloaded from beheco.oxfordjournals.org at Santa Monica College Library on October 4, 2010


velopmental precision and fluctuating asymmetry: a model and its Reusch TBH, Haber II MA, Aeschlimann PB, Milinski M, 2001.
implications. J Evol Biol 12:402–416. Female sticklebacks count alleles in a strategy of sexual selection
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, 2003. Fluctuating asymmetry, devel- explaining MHC polymorphism. Nature 414:300–302.
opmental instability, and fitness: toward model-based interpreta- Rice WR, Gaines SD, 1994. ‘‘Heads I win, tails you lose’’: testing
tion. In: Developmental instability: causes and consequences (Polak directional alternative hypotheses in ecological and evolutionary
M, ed). New York: Oxford University Press; 62–80. research. Trends Ecol Evol 9:235–237.
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, Garver C, 2002. Changes in women’s Rikowski A, Grammer K, 1999. Human body odour, symmetry and
sexual interests and their partner’s mate-retention tactics across the attractiveness. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:869–874.
menstrual cycle: evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proc R Rülicke T, Chapuista M, Homberger FR, Macas E, Wedekind C, 1998.
Soc Lond B 269:975–982. MHC-genotype of progeny influenced by parental infection. Proc R
Gangestad SW, Thornhill R, Yeo RA, 1994. Facial attractiveness, Soc Lond B 265:711–716.
developmental stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethol Sociobiol Savic I, Berglund H, Gulyas B, Roland P, 2001. Smelling of odorous
sex hormone-like compounds causes sex differentiated hypothal-
15:73–85.
mic activations in humans. Neuron 31:661–668.
Geary DC, 1998. Male, female: the evolution of human sex differ-
Simpson JA, Gangestad SW, Christensen PN, Leck K, 1999. Fluctuat-
ences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
ing asymmetry, sociosexuality, and intrasexual competitive tactics.
Grammer K, 1993. 5-a-androst-16-en-3a-on: a male pheromone? A
J Pers Soc Psych 76:159–172.
brief report. Ethol Sociobiol 14:201–214.
Singh D, Bronstad PM, 2001. Female body odour is a potential cue to
Grammer K, Jutte A, 1997. The war of odours: importance of
ovulation. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:797–801.
pheromones for human reproduction. Gynaekol-Geburtshilflihe
Swaddle JP, Witter MS, Cuthill IC, 1994. The analysis of fluctuating
Rundsch 37:150–153. asymmetry. Anim Behav 48:986–989.
Haig D, 1997. Maternal–fetal interactions and MHC polymorphism. Symons D, 1979. The evolution of human sexuality. Oxford: Oxford
J Reprod Immunol 35:101–109. University Press.
Hedrick PW, Black FL, 1997. HLA and mate selection: no evidence in Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, 1993. Human facial beauty: averageness,
South Amerindians. Am J Hum Gen 61:505–511. symmetry and parasite resistance. Hum Nat 4:237–269.
Herz RS, Cahill ED, 1997. Differential use of sensory information in Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, 1994. Human fluctuating asymmetry and
sexual behavior as a function of gender. Hum Nat 8:275–286. human sexual behavior. Psychol Sci 5:297–302.
Hughes AL, Hughes MK, 1995. Natural selection on the peptide- Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, 1999a. Facial attractiveness. Trends Cog
binding regions of major histocompatibility complex molecules. Science 3:452–460.
Immunogenetics 42:233–243. Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, 1999b. The scent of symmetry: a human
Ihara Y, Aoki K, Tokumaga K, Takahashi K, Juji T, 2000. HLA and sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evol Hum Behav 20:175–201.
human mate choice: tests on Japanese couples. Anthropol Sci Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, 2003. Do women have evolved adaptation
108:199–214. for extra-pair copulation? In: Evolutionary aesthetics (Voland E,
Jöchle W, 1973. Coitus induced ovulation. Contraception 7:523–564. Grammer K, eds). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer–Verlag; 341–368.
Johnston VS, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K, 2001. Male Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, Comer R, 1995. Human female orgasm
facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone mediated adaptive and mate fluctuating asymmetry. Anim Behav 50:1601–1615.
design. Evol Hum Behav 22:251–267. Thornhill R, Møller AP, 1997. Developmental stability, disease and
Kohl JV, Francoeur RT, 1995. The scent of eros. New York: Continuum. medicine. Biol Rev 72:497–528.
Manning JT, 1995. Fluctuating asymmetry and body weight in men Van Dongen S, 2000. The heritability of fluctuating asymmetry:
and women: implications for sexual selection. Ethol Sociobiol 16: a Bayesian hierarchical model. Ann Zool Fenn 37:15–23.
145–153. Vierling JS, Rock J, 1967. Variations in olfactory sensitivity to
Manning JT, Koukourakis K, Brodie DA, 1997. Fluctuating asymmetry, exaltolide during the menstrual cycle. J Appl Physiol 22:311–315.
metabolic rate and sexual selection in human males. Evol Hum Vroon P, 1997. Smell: the secret seducer [English translation of 1994
Behav 18:15–21. Dutch edition]. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Manning JT, Scutt D, Lewis–Jones DI, 1998. Developmental stability, Wedekind C, Füri S, 1997. Body odor preference in men and women:
ejaculate size and sperm quality in men. Evol Hum Behav 19:273–282. do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygos-
Møller AP, Swaddle JP, 1997. Asymmetry, developmental stability, and ity? Proc R Soc Lond B 264:1471–1479.
evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Wedekind C, Seebeck T, Bettens F, Paepke AJ, 1995. MHC-dependent
Møller AP, Thornhill R, 1997a. Developmental stability is heritable. J mate preferences in humans. Proc R Soc Lond B 260:245–249.
Evol Biol 10:69–76. Wysocki CJ, 1989. Vomeronasal chemoreception: its role in repro-
Møller AP, Thornhill R, 1997b. A meta-analysis of the heritability of ducing fitness and physiology. In: Neural control of reproductive
developmental stability. J Evol Biol 10:1–6. function (Wysocki CJ, ed). New York: Plenum; 545–566.
Ober C, Weitkamp LR, Cox N, Dytch H, Kostyu D, Elias S, 1997. HLA Yeo RA, Hill D, Campbell R, Vigil J, Brooks WM, 2000. Developmental
and mate choice in humans. Am J Hum Gen 61:497–504. instability and working memory ability in children: a magnetic
Pause BM, Sojka B, Krauel K, Fehmwolfsdorf G, Ferstl R, 1996. resonance spectroscopy. Dev Neuropsychol 17:143–159.
Olfactory information-processing during the course of the men- Zeh J, Zeh D, 2001. Reproductive mode and the genetic benefits of
strual cycle. Biol Psychol 44:31–54. polyandry. Anim Behav 61:1051–1063.

You might also like