You are on page 1of 9

Non-Random Fitness Variation in Two Populations of Darwin's Finches

Author(s): Peter R. Grant and B. Rosemary Grant


Source: Proceedings: Biological Sciences, Vol. 267, No. 1439 (Jan. 22, 2000), pp. 131-138
Published by: The Royal Society
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2665857
Accessed: 27/07/2010 11:19

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=rsl.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

The Royal Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Proceedings:
Biological Sciences.

http://www.jstor.org
rr THE ROYAL
*I SOCIETY

Non-random fitness variation in two populations of


Darwin's finches
Peter R. Grant* and B. Rosemary Grant
Department
ofEcologyand Evolutionary
Biology,
Princeton
University, NY 08544-1003, USA
Princeton,
Darwinian fitnessof an individual is measured by the number of recruitsit contributesto the next genera-
tion. We studied variation in fitnessamong members of three cohorts of two species of Darwin's finches
living on the Galapagos island of Daphne Major: the medium ground finch (Geospizafortis)and cactus
finch(Geospizascandens).Individuals of both species live forup to 16 years.Variation in fitnesswas neither
random nor heritable. Non-randomnessarises as a resultof a few individuals living foran exceptionally
long time and breeding many times. For each cohort, the number of recruits per breeder is strongly
predicted by the number of fledglingsper breeder. In turn,the number of fledglingsis stronglypredicted
by longevityof the breeder.These results suggest that the most important determinantof fitnessis the
ability of an individual to survive to breed in many years. Morphological traits affect this ability.
Although morphological traitsare heritabletheydo not change unidirectionallybecause theyare selected
in opposite directions,and in differentcombinations, under fluctuatingenvironmentalconditions. Non-
random fitnessvariation in fluctuatingpopulations implies much smaller geneticallyeffectivesizes than
breeding population sizes.
Keywords: Galapagos; Darwin's finches;longevity;fledglings;recruits;heritability

islands where populations are discretely defined by


1. INTRODUCTION
geography,and immigration and emigration are low or
Fitnesses of members of a population vary enormously, effectivelynil (Hochachka et al. 1989; Grant & Grant
for reasons that have remained elusive (Clutton-Brock 1992, 1996). In this paper we report the resultsof a 24-
1988). Long-term studies of bird populations have quan- year study of fitnessvariation in two species of Darwin's
titatively demonstrated the variation in nature most finches,Geospizafortis (medium ground finch)and Geospiza
extensively.A consensus of findingsfrom 23 studies was scandens(cactus finch),on the small (0.34 kmi2)Galapagos
summarized ten years ago by Newton (1989) as follows: island of Daphne Major (Grant & Grant 1996). The
(i) a large fraction of fledglings die before they can nearest island is 8 km distant.The goals of the studywere
breed, (ii) not all the individuals which survive to to use different cohortsof individuallymarked birds to (i)
attempt breeding subsequently produce offspring, test for non-randomnessin the frequencydistributionof
(iii) successful individuals vary greatly in productivity, recruitsamong parents, (ii) attempta statisticalexplana-
(iv) breeding life span is the most importantdeterminant tion of the variation in termsof life-historyand morpho-
of fledgling production, (v) number of fledglingsis an logical variables of the parents, (iii) test the hypothesis
important determinantof number of recruits,and so is that recruitmentsuccess is partlydeterminedby variation
breeding life span but to a lesser extent,and (vi) much of in propertiesof the offspringthemselves,and (iv) test the
the variation in fitness,as indexed by lifetimeproduction hypothesisthat variation in fitnessof the parents, and in
of offspring,is apparentlydue to chance. This last conclu- the major contributorsto their fitnessof survival and
sion followed from a general failure to find correlations reproduction,is heritable.
between fitnessand other propertiesof the breeders such
as body size and social factors,and failure to find heri-
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS
table variation in fitness,ratherthan fromexplicit testing
and failure to reject a null hypothesisthat variation in (a) Study methods
fitnessamong parents can be ascribed to chance (but see In the years1976-1978mostnestswere foundand nestlings
McCleery & Perrins 1988; Dhondt 1989; Fitzpatrick & werebanded. In theyears1979-1991and 1998 an attemptwas
Woolfenden1989). made to findeveryneston theislandthroughout each breeding
Assessing the roles of chance and deterministicfactors season and to band all the nestlings(Grant & Grant 1992;
has been hampered by incompleteinformationon recruit- Grantetal. 2000). From1992 to 1997 we spentapproximately
ment, because many offspring disperse beyond the thefirsthalfofeach breedingseasonon theisland.Therewas no
borders of study areas (Lambrechts et al. 1999). As a breedingin 1996 and 1999.Thus no morethanhalfofthenest-
result,all but one of the studies surveyedby Newton had lingswere banded and halfof the numberof fledglings deter-
to use local recruitmentor the surrogate of offspring minedin sixofthelastnineyears.
(fledgling) production as a measure of fitness. This Most birdswere banded as eight-daynestlings,otherswere
problem has not been rectifiedby more recent work (e.g. capturedin mist-nets, beginningin 1973.Capturedbirdswere
Perrinset al. 1991; Lebreton & North 1993; Lambrechts et measured(see ? 2(b)) and givena unique combination of three
al. 1999). It can be eliminated on small and well-isolated colouredplasticand one numberedmetal band priorto their
release. Parentagewas determinedby observationof banded
*Authorforcorrespondence (prgrant(princeton.edu). adults attendingnests.MicrosatelliteDNA analysesshow no

Proc.R. Soc.Lond.B (2000) 267,131-138 131 ?) 2000 The Royal Society


Received13 September1999 Accepted 20 October1999
infitness
132 P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant Variation

evidence of misidentifiedmaternity,but a low level of misidenti- Table 1. Numbersoffledglingsbanded,and thenumbersof


fied paternity (ca. 8% in G. scandens (Petren et al. 1999), and bred,in 11 setsofcohorts
thosethateventually
approximatelydouble that in G.fortis(L. E Keller, unpublished (In the remainingyears there was no breeding,or little
data)). Survival was determined every year by extensive and breedingyieldingno recruitment.)
repeated surveys over the whole island, and by observing and
identifyingterritorialmales and their mates. Recruitmentto the G.fortis G.scandens
breeding population, as recognized by the laying of at least one
egg by the individual or its mate, is likely to have been deter- year banded bred(0) banded bred(0)
mined accurately. A very small number of recruits may have
been missed in the years 1992-1997 if they bred only in the 1976 376 1 (<0.1) 93 0(0)
second half of a single breeding season. 1978 222 89 (40.0) 164 43 (26.2)
Longevities of all members of the 1978-1987 cohorts were 1979 82 13 (15.8) 110 15 (13.6)
determined with the following exceptions; one 1983 G.fortis 1980 37 0 (0) 134 2 (1.5)
female, two 1987 G.fortismales and one 1987 G. scandensmale 1981 276 50 (18.2) 231 29 (12.6)
were alive in 1999. Maximum longevitywas 16 years for G.fortis 1983 1018 310 (30.5) 785 154 (19.6)
1984 427 45 (10.5) 208 4 (1.9)
(both sexes) and G. scandensmales, and 11 years for G. scandens
1986 8 2 (25.0) 11 0 (0)
females. Effectsof incomplete breeding data on analyses of the
1987 1043 183 (17.6) 164 27 (16.5)
cohorts of 1978 and their offspringare likely to be minor. We 1990 33 6 (18.2) 4 0 (0)
may have missed one nest of each of the last three surviving 1991 782 245 (31.3) 138 32 (23.2)
members of the 1978 cohort of G.fortisin 1992, and one nest of
the last survivor in 1993. Breeding of the offspringof the 1978
cohort after 1991 was underestimated.However, the number of
recruitshatched in these years was small, and a minor fraction hatchingdate (days1... n foreach memberof a cohort),age at
of the total recruits produced during the lifetimes of the firstbreedingin years,beak shape (length-depth)and struc-
offspringof the 1978 cohorts: fiveout of 382 (1.3%) G.fortisand turalbodysize as indexedby thefirstcomponentfromprincipal
three out of 43 (7.0% ) G. scandens. Underestimation of the componentsanalysesof morphologicalvariation.Correlation
breeding of the 1981 and 1983 cohorts and their offspringwas matriceswere used in separateprincipalcomponentsanalyses
greater. foreach sex ofeach species.The measuredtraitsweremass (g),
winglength,tarsuslength,beak length,beak depthand beak
(b) Analyses and measurements width (all in millimetres), as describedin Grant & Grant
Fitness is measured by the number of offspringper parent (1994).Predictedlifetimenumberoffledglings was estimatedby
that survive to breed, i.e. they become recruitsto the breeding all thesevariablestogetherwithlongevity,thenlifetime number
population (generation 2) and hence become potential contribu- of recruitswas predictedby all theseand lifetimenumberof
tors to generation 3. This ignores the temporal pattern of fledglings.
recruitment, which influences fitness for short-lived species Logisticregressionwas used to testforassociationsbetween
undergoing systematic changes in breeding density (Stearns recruitmentsuccess and offspringmorphology (principal
1992). componentscores) and theirdates of hatching;offspring that
Fitness is governed by the survival and reproduction of became recruitswere scoredas 1 and thosethatdid not were
parents and the survival to breeding of their offspring.If same- scoredas 0. Fortheseanalyseshatchingdateswerecombinedfor
sex members of a breeding population have equal abilities to all years afterstandardizingby dividingdeviationsfromthe
survive, reproduce and produce recruits, and they and their annual mean by the standarddeviationforthatyear. Signifi-
offspringare subjected to random hazards, the expected varia- was testedbyX2-tests.
cance ofcoefficients
tion in fitnessof the breeders is a Poisson distribution.This null hypotheses
Heritability weretestedbysimpleregressions with
hypothesiswas tested using the X2-testwith data on fitness(life- data fromthe 1978 cohortmembersand theiroffspring that
time number of recruits),survival (longevity) and reproduction survivedto breed.The traitswerelongevity, and mean numbers
(lifetime number of fledglings) from the largest and most offledglingsand recruits.Membersofthe1978cohortofG.fortis
complete cohorts: 1978, 1981 and 1983. Negative binomial (table 1) produceda total of 888 fledglings in theirlifetime
distributionswere also fittedto the data and tested by x2. The (1979-1993),of which244 (27.5%) bred (1981-1998).Members
negative binomial is equivalent to a particular mixture of ofthe G.scandenscohortproduced469 fledglings (1979-1991),of
Poisson distributionswith differentmeans (Cavalli-Sforza & which89 (19.0%) bred (1981-1998).Analysesofheritablevaria-
Bodmer 1971),and is appropriate with our data because entryof tionare necessarilyrestrictedto thoseindividualsthatsurvived
recruitsinto the breeding population is usually staggered. Coef- tobreed.All statistical
testsweretwo-tailed.
ficientswere estimated by maximum likelihood with the Nlin
procedure in SAS (SAS Institute,Inc.).
3. RESULTS
For the same three cohorts multiple regression analysis was
used to predict fitness,survival and reproduction in terms of a (a) Variation in fitness
few morphological and life-historyvariables suspected of being Breeding,and the survivalof offspringto breed (recruit-
important for fitness. All independent variables were trans- ment), vary greatly among years (table 1) as a result of
formed to natural logs (logex) prior to analysis. Logex + 0.5 was annual fluctuations in rainfall and food supply in this
used for age at firstbreeding for the 1983 cohort because some seasonally arid environment (Grant & Grant 1996; Grant
birds bred (and some of those died) in their year of hatching, et al. 2000). Entry into the breeding population occurred
and logex+ 1 was used for number of fledglingsfor all cohorts gradually over fiveyears formembersof the 1978 cohorts,
and longevity of the 1983 cohort. Longevity was predicted by abruptlyaftertwo years for1981 cohorts and afteras little

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


infitness P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant 133
Variation

0.3 -(a) Table 2. Standardizedvariances(variance-mean)of fitness


(recruitsper breeder)and two contributors
tofitness,
fledglings
per breederand breederlongevzity
0.2-
G.fortis G.scandens
n0 -
0.1
males females males females

1978cohort
numbers 48 41 20 23
0.3 - (b) recruits 3.46*** 3.54***b 1.21 2.45*a
fledglings 37.89***b 12.17***c 5. O***c 8.26***C
.~0.2-
longevity 1.23**c 1.63**a 1.10a 0.93
1981 cohort
numbers 32 19 12 8
0.1 recruits 3.05*** 3.05*** 3.90*** 3.67
fledglings 11.58*** 5.68*** 6.72*** 5.25***
longevity 2.60***c 1.78*c 2.82***C 0.54a
0Cv

1983 cohort
0.3- (c) numbers 113 194 52 103
recruits 2.38*** 2.59 2.49*** 3.09** *a
fledglings 8.44***c 9.46***C 5 43***a 7 34***c
0.2- longevity 3.01 ***C 3.24** c 4.01 ***c 1.83***c

0.1 1 *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, statisticallysignificantdepar-


turesfromPoisson expectationby X2-tests.
ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp< 0.001, departuresfromnegativebinomial
0- expectationby X2-tests.
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10111213141516
lifespansin years (b) Predictions offitness from properties of the
Figure1. Annualvariationin thedistribution oflifespansof parents
males,afterthebreedingofthefirstmemberofa
G.fortis There are two major results from multiple linear
cohort.(a) 1978,n= 48; (b) 1981,n= 32; (c) 1983,n= 1 2. regressionanalyses (table 3). First,the strongestpredictor
of fitnessis reproductiveoutput; the more fledglingsan
as three monthsat rapidly rising densityfor 1983 cohorts individual produces in its lifetimethe larger is its contri-
(maximum four years). Recruitment was proportionally bution of recruits to the next generation of breeders.
higher in G.fortis,the more abundant species, than in Second, the strongestpredictorof reproductiveoutput is
G. scandens.It neverexceeded 40% foreitherspecies. longevity; the longer an individual lives the more fledg-
Frequencydistributionsof longevity(figure1),fledglings lings it produces.
and recruitsamong parents is non-random in most cases. Longevity is less well predicted than recruitmentand
Variances are greater than expected (table 2); some fledgling production. Separate analyses of the shorter-
parents have exceptionally high recruitmentsuccess. For lived and the longer-livedmembersof a cohort show that
members of the 1981 and 1983 cohorts the variance is variation in longevity among the shorter-livedgroup is
proportionallygreaterwhen allowance is made forincom- sometimes predictable, whereas variation among the
plete recruit data after 1991 by doubling the known longer-lived groups is not (all F< 1.7, p >0.2; most
number of recruits they produced during this time. p>0.6). For these analyses the data were split into two
However, this adjustmenthas littleeffecton the statistics, halves either side of the median age for the cohort.
and is not included in the table. With few exceptions,the Despite the reduced power of these tests as a result of
Poisson distributiondoes not provide a good fitto any of smaller sample sizes (8-55), and the reduced range of
the data. The negative binomial does not fit the distri- variation (zero to two years), longevitywas predicted in
bution of longevities or fledglingsproduced by the 1978 the short-livedG.fortis1983 males by age at firstbreeding
and 1983 cohorts,even though entryinto their respective (b'= 0.499, t32= 2.797, p = 0.0091), and in the short-lived
breeding populations was more staggered than it was for (zero to two years) G. scandens1983 females also by age at
the 1981cohorts.The negativebinomial best fitsthe distri- first breeding (b' = 0.409, t43= 2.219, p = 0.0322). Thus
butionsof recruitsamong breeders,but not forall cohorts. deterministicfactors may come into play prominently
Non-randomness increases through the life span of early in life, while for those who survive beyond the
cohorts (figure 2). This temporal pattern arises as a cohort's median age it may be more a matter of chance
consequence of relatively few individuals living long who survivesthereafterfora shorttime and who survives
enough (figure 1) to take advantage of very favourable fora long time.
conditionsforbreeding. Standardized variances increased In the analyses of complete data (table 3) all of the
in 1987 and 1991, both productive El Ninio years ( >four independent variables made some contribution to
broods), but, with one exception (C.fortismales), did not predicting longevity, fledglings or recruits of some
increase in 1984, a relativelyunproductive year (one or cohorts. A featureof these predictionsis inconsistencyin
two broods) (figure2). sign and among years. For example, early hatching is

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


134 P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant Variation
infitness

the regressionanalyses. The indirectpathway fromlong-


S-(a) evity to recruits via fledglingproduction is consistently
4 stronger than the direct pathway. Other pathways are
inconsistentbetween species and sexes.
3 0... .0.. .0
(c) Properties of the offspringand recruitment
2 -l success
Offspring that became recruits differed occasionally
0.1- .. --...... from those that did not in small and inconsistentways.
For offspringof 1978 cohorts G. scandensrecruits were
0- I I I I I I larger (males) and hatched earlier (females) than non-
a0 -
recruitsof the same sex (logistic regressions: male body
(b) size = 0.855 ? 0.443 (s.e.), p = 0.0533, r2= 0.114, n = 50;
female hatch date coefficient = - 1.634 ? 0.778, p = 0.0357,
4 - r2=0.134, n = 38). For offspringof 1981 cohorts G.fortis
> -U
female recruitswere larger and hatched later than non-
3 ~~0. recruits (body size = 1.024 ? 0.451, p = 0.0233; hatch date
coefficient= 1.421?0.737, p = 0.0537, r2= 0.137,n = 61). For
offspringof 1983 cohorts G.fortismale recruits were
4-E
smaller than non-recruits (body size = - 0.332 ? 0.162,
p = 0.0409, r2= 0.022, n = 225). None of the other coeffi-
(A(c
02
cientswas significantly different fromzero (all p > 0.1).

5- (d) Heritability offitness, reproduction and longevity


(c) Consequences of fitnessvariation depend on whetherit
4- is heritableor not (Charlesworth1987). If it is heritablethe
lifetimenumber of recruits (generation 2) produced by
O 3 - I I I I I I
1978 males or females (parental generation 1) should
predictthe mean lifetimenumberoftheirrecruits(genera-
2 -. tion 3). Regression analyses were performed to predict
7 ....
0........ either (i) all generation 3 recruits, or (ii) just those
0 -0.
produced by the largest single cohorts (1981 or 1983) of
generation2. Sample sizes of 1978 cohort parents were 36
G.fortismales and 26 females (14 pairs), and 15 G. scandens
79 81 83 84 87 91 males and 17 females. None of the regressions of mid-
years offspringvalues on single parent or mid-parent values is
0 G. fortismale - a- G. scandens male significant(all p >0.15) fromeither type of analysis. The
O. G. fortisfemale --D-- G. scandens female hypothesisof heritablevariation in fitnesshas no support.
Variation in lifetimeproductionoffledglingsand longevity
Figure2. Changesin thestandardizedvariances(variance- are also not detectablyheritableby the same methods and
mean) ofrecruitsper parentoverthelifetimes
ofthreesetsof withthe same samples (allp > 0.24).
cohorts.(a) 1978, (b) 1981 and (c) 1983.
(e) Predictability offitness between generations
associated with long life in G.fortis and G. scandens, Absence of detectable heritable variation does not
whereas late hatching is associated with high recruit mean the absence of predictability of fitness between
production by female G.fortisand high fledglingproduc- parents and offspring.The number of recruitsproduced
tion by female G. scandens.Data for male and female by members of the 1978 cohorts does predict the total (as
parental groups are not entirelyindependent since some opposed to mean) number of recruitsthat theycontribute
recruitshad both male and female parents fromthe same to the next generation (generation 3); for Gfortis males
cohort. As a result the regressionsfor male and female b = 1.559 ? 0.263, t34= 5.925, p = 0.0001, and females
parents might be expected to be similar, with those for b = 1.708? 0.417, t24= 4.091, p = 0.0004, and for
males being weaker as a result of some misassigned G. scandens females b = 0.806 ? 0.260, t15= 3.106,
paternityof offspring,especially in G.fortis.Regressions p = 0.0072, but for males b = 0.057 ? 0.575, t13= 0.100,
do tend to be similar, but female regressions are not p = 0.920. In general more recruitsbeget more recruits.
consistentlystrongerthan male regressions,nor are the Similarly the total (as opposed to mean) number of
coefficientsconsistentlylarger (table 3). generation 3 fledglingsis predicted from the number of
Results for the 1978 cohorts are illustrated with path fledglings (generation 2) produced by their G.fortis
diagrams (figure 3). The diagrams are based on the parents (generation 1) (males b =1.511 ?0.401, t34= 3.766,
methods of path analysis of causal pathways, hypothes- p = 0.0006; females b = 1.292 ? 0.535, 124= 2.400,
ized a priori, between longevity,fledglings and fitness p = 0.0249), although not by their G.scandens parents
(generalizations (iv) and (v) in ?1). They are not (both p > 0.7).
restrictedto those pathways,and forcomprehensiveillus- Breeding success (fledglingsproduced) of a parent in
trationthe diagrams include other pathways identifiedby generation1 predictsthe numberofrecruitsin generation3

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


Variation
infitness P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant 135

Table 3. Standardizedpartial regression fromregression


coefficients analysespredicting
longevity,
fedglingsor recruits
producedby
membersofthreesetsofcohorts
(1, 2 and 3 are generations1, 2 and 3. Values in parenthesesindicatecoefficients
afterfledglingsor longevityhave been deleted
because ofhighintercorrelations. All but threeindividualsofthe 1981 cohortsbredforthefirsttimein 1983,therefore age at first
breedingwas not includedin theseanalyses.Body size is PC 1. Beak shape is length-depth.Two samplesizes are shown,thefirst
forlongevityand fledglings, and thesecondforrecruits.Resultsforthe 1981 and 1983 cohortsare scarcelyaffectedwhenrecruit
numbersare adjustedby doublingtheknownnumberofrecruitsproducedafter1991 (see ? 3(a)). indicateslack ofsignificance
(p > 0.1). The samplesize oftheG. scandens 1981 cohortoffemales(eight)is too smallforanalysis.)

G.fortis G.scandens

males females males females

1978cohort
1: longevity
hatchdate -0.423**
beak shape 0.402**
2: fledglings
longevity 0.693**** 0.868 0.444*
age at 1stbreeding -0.763*
bodysize -0.212*
beak shape -0.614***
3: recruits
fledglings (0.452*) (0.62 **
hatchdate 0.386*
samples 41, 36 36,26 18, 13 19,14
1981cohort
1: longevity
2: fledglings
longevity 0.744**** 0.691**** 0.818**
beak shape -0.486*
3: recruits
fledglings 0.758**** 1.043**** (0.773****)
longevity (0.547***) (0.483*) (0.682***)
samples 29,25 14,13 15,13
1983cohort
1: longevity
hatchdate -0.343*
age at 1stbreeding 0.339* 0.430***
bodysize 0.402**
beak shape 0.296*
2: fledglings
longevity 0.836**** 0.818**** 0.706**** 0.941****
age at 1stbreeding -0.424* -0.218*
3: recruits
fledglings (0.627****) 0.651 0.717*** 0.847****
longevity (0.577****) (0.603****) (0.420*) (0.536****)
bodysize 0.160*
samples 76,64 108,97 37,25 55, 39

*p< 0.05,**p< 0.01,***p< 0.005and ****p


< 0.001.

forG.fortismales (b= 0.321 ?0.103, t34= 3.132,p = 0.0036), chance in producing variation in fitness.The main result
possiblyforfemales(b= 0.256 ? 0.152,t24= 1.688,p = 0.1044), here is that variation in fitnessis neitherdetectablyheri-
males (b=- 0.028 ? 0.120,t13= 0.234,
but not forG.scandens table nor entirely random. The variation has environ-
p = 0.8186) or females (b=-0.045 ? 0.104, t15= 0.431, mental causes and, despite the lack of heritability,
p = 0.6725). evolutionary(genetic) consequences.
As appears to be the case in other bird species (Grant
& Grant 1992; Barrowclough & Rockwell 1993), non-
4. DISCUSSION
randomness arises throughsome individuals being able to
Results of this study are consistentwith the firstfive live long enough to reproduce many times. Long life
generalizations given in ?1. They strengthenthem by (maximum 16 years, both species) is translatedinto large
being based on two long-lived, tropical, passerine bird production of fledglings (maximum 46, from a G.fortis
species whose recruitmenthas been determined comple- male), which is translated into high recruitment
tely or almost completely.The resultsthrow new light on (maximum 16, from a G.fortisfemale; see also figure 3).
the sixth generalization concerning the enigmatic role of Variation in recruitmentdeparts stronglyfrom random

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


136 P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant Variation
infitness

(a) (b)

bodysize 0.160
0.067A 0.6
fledglings 0 460*** fledglings
0.65 1***
/0.836 recruits 0.818**** recruits
beak 0296* longevity
/ 0.195 longevity 03
shape

(c) (d)

bodysize
0.402 fledglings0717 fledglings 0847
hatchdate 0.089 /0.706**** .

recruits
hatchdate
-0.3
* A9 ****
.941 ru
recruits
/0.117
0.425 longevity,~ 0.397 0on 005t
0.339 0.430 longevit
age at 1st S 24age ast S0.21
breeding breeding

Figure3. Path diagramsto representthecausal relationships betweenmeasuredvariablesand fitness(recruits)forthe 1983


cohorts.Whereage at firstbreedingsignificantly predictslongevitythepathwaysfromhatchingdate to bothofthemhave been
added. Pathwaysforunmeasuredvariablesand correlations betweencausal factorshave been omitted.Path coefficients are the
standardizedpartialregression coefficients
recalculatedafterdeletionofnon-significant morphologicaland life-history variables;
theydifferto a minorextentfromthecoefficients in table 3. Significance
ofthecoefficients is indicatedby ****p <0.001,
***p< 0.005, **p< 0.01, *p< 0.05. (a) G.fortis
males; (b) G.fortis
females;(c) G. scandensmales; (d) G. scandensfemales.

expectations according to a Poisson model, but conforms densityearly in the season when food supply is generally
to negative binomial expectations in most cases. This can at its highest level confers an advantage, environmental
be interpretedas reflectingvariation in the reproductive in origin, upon both species in some years. They may
abilities and circumstancesof breeders, and variation in gain good condition thereby,in terms of maturation of
the abilities and circumstances experienced by their tissues and storage of energy, and if circumstances are
offspringprior to reaching breeding age. Both determi- favourablethey may be able to breed at an early age as a
nistic and stochastic factors contribute to a shift,from result (1983).
fledglingproduction to recruitment,in the distributionof The importance of morphological traits is shown by
fitnessamong parents over their lifetimestowards that statistical associations between long life and large body
expected from a negative binomial, in other words size in G. scandens(1983, males) and beak shape in G.fortis
towards the expectation froma mixtureof Poisson distri- (1978 and 1983, males). All cohorts,but especially the 1983
butionswith differentmeans. ones, experienced a decline and change in composition in
The key question is: What governs longevity of the their food supply in 1984-1986. Mortality was heavy,
breeders? Combining the present results with long-term particularly among G. scandenswhose cactus supply was
ecological studies on the same island (Grant & Grant depleted. Large size was an advantage to male G. scandens
1996; Grant et al. 2000), we suggest the answer is that at this time, as their success is dependent upon gaining a
both life-historyand morphological traitsinfluencelong- territorywhich they occupy and defend year round, and
evity in ways that vary depending upon fluctuationsin large size confers an advantage in territorialdisputes.
environmental conditions. The particular factors and G.fortismales, in contrast, are territorial only in the
directionsof selectiveinfluencechange because the envir- breeding season. G.fortisin general, and notjust the 1983
onment fluctuatesfromdroughtsand low food supply to cohort, were subject to directional selection in favour of
an abundance of rain and enhanced food supply at size-independent long and shallow beaks when the food
approximately four-year intervals, or once per finch supplychanged in compositionand declined in abundance
generationon average (Grant & Grant 1992). These influ- (Gibbs & Grant 1987; Grant & Grant 1995,1996).
ences are manifested against a background of largely In addition to the above example, the importance of
random environmental effects, and while biologically environmentalfluctuationsis shown by a general lack of
important they generally stand out rather weakly in consistencyamong cohorts of the same species and sex
statisticalanalyses. groups in the effectsof morphological and life-history
Among life-historytraits the importance of hatching traits on fitness, longevity and the production of
date is shown by the results of multiple regression fledglings.There are two interrelated reasons for this
analyses. Long life was associated with early hatching in inconsistency.
G.fortis(1978, males) and G. scandens(1983, females).This First, environmental conditions differ between pre-
suggests that entering a population at relatively low and post-recruitmentphases of the life cycle. The fatesof

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


infitness P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant 137
Variation

males and females of the 1983 cohort of G. scandensillus- are still present when the offspringbreed (Merila 1996;
trate the point. Those that became recruitsearly in their Potti 1999) may contribute to the trans-generation
life, in their year of hatching or in the following one, continuity.
produced a relatively large number of offspringbut,
possibly as a consequence, lived for a short time. The We thankthe Charles Darwin ResearchStation (GalApagos)
environmentdeteriorated in 1984-1986. Despite living and theGalApagosNationalParksServiceforlogisticalsupport,
long the survivors which had not bred previously the NationalScience Foundation(USA) and NationalScience
produced relatively few offspring. Second, different and Engineering ResearchCouncil (Canada) forfinancialsup-
port,manyfieldassistants,and L. F. Keller forestimationof
cohorts experienced differentconditions at one or both forthenegativebinomialdistributions.
coefficients
phases: one cohort'spre-recruitment phase is its predeces-
sor's post-recruitmentphase. For example the 1984-1986
period of strongmortalityoccurred afterall recruitment REFERENCES
of members of the 1978 and 1981 cohorts had taken place
Barrowclough,G. F. & Rockwell, R. F. 1993 Variance of lifetime
but beforerecruitmentof the 1983 cohortswas complete.
reproductivesuccess: estimation based on demographic data.
Thus components of fitness at differentlife-history
Am.Nat.141,281-295.
stages of the same and differentcohortsmay covary,posi- Both, C., Visser, M. E. & Verboven, N. 1999 Density-dependent
tively or negatively (Price & Grant 1984; Price & recruitment rates in great tits: the importance of being
Schluter 1991; Schluter & Smith 1986), or be entirely heavier. Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 465-469.
independent, depending on the specific details of inter- Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Bodmer,W. F. 1971 The geneticsofhuman
annual variation in environmentalconditions. populations.San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman & Co.
These conclusions are not restrictedto Darwin's finches Charlesworth,B. 1987 The heritabilityof fitness.In Sexual selec-
and their particular tropical environment.Passerine bird tion: testingthealternatives(ed. J. Bradbury & M. Andersson),
populations in the temperate zone experience significant pp. 21-40. New York:Wiley.
Clutton-Brock,T. H. (ed.) 1988 Reproductive success.Studiesof indi-
annual variation in environmental conditions, even
vidual variationin contrasting breedingsystems.University of
though less extreme than in the Galapagos archipelago.
Chicago Press.
Temperaturesand food supply vary in both non-breeding Collias, N. E. & Collias, E. C. 1996 Social organization of a red
seasons and at the beginning of breeding. This causes junglefowl, Gallus gallus, population related to evolution
marked variation in survival and local recruitment, theory.Anim.Behav.51, 1337-1354.
mediated by variation in food supply, the timing of Dhondt, A. A. 1989 Blue tit. In Lifetime reproductionin birds(ed. I.
breeding and local density (Hochachka et al. 1989; Van Newton), pp. 15-33. London: Academic Press.
Noordwijk et al. 1995; Visser et al. 1998; Both et al. 1999; Fitzpatrick,J. W. & Woolfenden, G. E. 1989 Florida scrub jay.
Visser & Verboven 1999). Such details illustrate the In Lifetimereproduction in birds (ed. I. Newton), pp. 201-218.
general importance ascribed to environmentalvariation London: Academic Press.
in determininglow (or zero) heritabilitiesof life-history Frankham, R. 1995 Effectivepopulation size/adult population
size ratios in wildlife: a review.Genet.Res. Camb.66, 95-107.
traits and fitness itself (Charlesworth 1987; Price &
Freeman-Gallant, C. R. 1996 Microgeographic patterns of
Schluter1991).
genetic and morphological variation in savannah sparrows
Non-random fitness variation caused by fluctuating (Passerellasandwichensis).Evolution50, 1631-1637.
environmentalpressureshas two interestingevolutionary Gibbs, H. L. & Grant, P. R. 1987 Oscillating selection on
implications. First, although morphological traits contri- Darwin's finches.Nature327, 511-513.
bute to fitnessand are heritable (Grant & Grant 1999) Grant, P. R. 1999 Ecologyand evolution ofDarwin'sfinches, 2nd edn.
they do not change unidirectionally because they are PrincetonUniversityPress.
selected in opposite directions,and in differentcombina- Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1992 Demography and the geneti-
tions, under fluctuatingenvironmentalconditions (Gibbs cally effectivesizes of two populations of Darwin's finches.
& Grant 1987). This implies that a net directional evolu- Ecology73, 766-784.
Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1994 Phenotypic and genetic
tionarytrendis more likelyto come about as a resultof a
effects of hybridization in Darwin's finches. Evolution48,
directional change in the environment(Grant 1999), such
297-316.
as might be produced by global warming for example, Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1995 Predicting microevolutionary
than as a resultof currentoscillations. responses to directional natural selection on heritable varia-
Second, non-random fitness variation with standar- tion. Evolution49, 241-251.
dized variances substantiallygreater than 1.0 imply that Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1996 Finch communities in a
genetically effectivepopulation sizes are much smaller climatically fluctuatingenvironment. In Long-termstudiesof
than sizes of the breeding populations. This appears to vertebrate communities (ed. M. L. Cody & J. A. Smallwood),
be generally true forplant and animal populations, espe- pp. 343-390. New York: Academic Press.
cially for those whose sizes fluctuate (Frankham 1995). Grant, P. R. & Grant, B. R. 1999 Quantitative genetic variation
Estimates for bird populations may average about 25% in populations of Darwin's finches.In Adaptivegeneticvariation
in thewild (ed. T. A. Mousseau, B. Sinervo & J. A. Endler),
of breeding numbers (Grant & Grant 1992), with
pp. 3-40. NewYork: Oxford UniversityPress.
minima as low as 10% (Rockwell & Barrowclough 1995;
Grant, P. R., Grant, B. R., Keller, L. F. & Petren,K. 2000 Effects
Collias & Collias 1996), or even less (Freeman-Gallant ofEl Nifioeventson Darwin's finches.Ecology81. (In the press.)
1996).We note that the process of narrowing the genetic Hochachka, W. M., Smith, J. N. M. & Arcese, P. 1989 Song
variation in one generation may be continued in the sparrow. In Lifetimereproduction in birds (ed. I. Newton), pp.
followinggenerationby the tendencyforlineages to have 135-152. London: Academic Press.
high success in successive generations. Genetic and non- Lambrechts, M. M., Blondel, J., Caizergues, A., Dias, P. C.,
genetic maternal effectsupon offspringcondition which Pradel, R. & Thomas, D. W. 1999 Will estimates of lifetime

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)


138 P. R. Grant and B. R. Grant Variation
infitness

recruitmentof breeding offspringon small-scale study plots Price, T. & Schluter, D. 1991 On the low heritabilityof life
help us to quantifyprocesses underlyingadaptation? Oikos86, historytraits.Evolution45, 853-862.
147-151. Rockwell, R. F. & Barrowclough, G. F. 1995 Effectivepopula-
Lebreton,J.-D. & North, P. M. (eds) 1993 Markedindividualsin tion size and lifetime reproductive success. Biol. Conserv.9,
thestudyofbirdpopulations.Basel, Switzerland: Birkhauser. 1225-1233.
McCleery, R. H. & Perrins, C. M. 1988 Lifetime reproductive Schluter, D. & Smith,J. N. M. 1986 Natural selection on beak
success of the great tit, Parus major. In Reproductive success. and body size in the song sparrow.Evolution40, 221-231.
Studiesof individualvariationin contrastingbreedingsystems(ed. Stearns, S. C. 1992 The evolutionoflifehistories.
Oxford University
T. H. Clutton-Brock), pp. 136-153. University of Chicago Press.
Press. Van Noordwijk, A. J., McCleery, R. H. & Perrins, C. M. 1995
Merila, J. 1996 Genetic variation in offspringcondition: an Selection for the timing of great tit breeding in relation to
experiment.Funct.Ecol. 10, 465-474. caterpillar growth and temperature. _. Anim. Ecol. 64, 451-
Newton, I. (ed.) 1989 Lifetimereproduction in birds. London: 458.
Academic Press. Visser, M. E. & Verboven, N. 1999 Long-term fitnesseffectsof
Perrins, C. M., Lebreton, J.-D. & Hirons, G. J. M. 1991 Bird fledglingdate in great tits. Oikos85, 445-450.
population studies. Relevance to conservationand management. Visser, M. E., Van Noordwijk, A. J., Tinbergen, J. M. &
Oxford UniversityPress. Lessells, C. M. 1998 Warmer springs lead to mistimed repro-
Petren, K., Grant, B. R. & Grant, P. R. 1999 Extrapair pater- duction in great tits (Parus major). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265,
nityin the cactus finchGeospizascandens.Auk 116, 252-256. 1867-1870.
Potti,J. 1999 Maternal effectsand the pervasive impact of nest-
ling history on egg size in a passerine bird. Evolution53,
279-285.
Price, T. D. & Grant, P. R. 1984 Life historytraits and natural
selection for small body size in a population of Darwin's As this paper exceeds the maximum length normally permitted,
finches.Evolution38, 483-495. the authors have agreed to contributeto productioncosts.

Proc.R. Soc. Lond. B (2000)

You might also like