You are on page 1of 5

CLINICAL GASTROENTEROLOGY AND HEPATOLOGY 2008;6:1135–1139

Patients With Minimal Hepatic Encephalopathy Have Poor Insight Into


Their Driving Skills
JASMOHAN S. BAJAJ,* KIA SAEIAN,‡ MUHAMMAD HAFEEZULLAH,‡ RAYMOND G. HOFFMANN,§ and
THOMAS A. HAMMEKE㛳
*Division of Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition, Virginia Commonwealth University and McGuire VA Medical Center, Richmond, Virginia; ‡Division of
Gastroenterology and Hepatology, §Department of Biostatistics, 㛳Department of Neurology, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin

ties in patients with MHE can stem from deficiencies in several


See Mardini H et al and Bajaj JS et al in the components, including impaired reaction times, psychomotor
December 2008 issue of Gastroenterology. slowing, and navigation skill disturbances.9,10 A recent study
showed a higher risk of traffic violations and motor vehicle
accidents in patients with MHE.11 Insight into or self-awareness
See CME exam on page 1065. of driving impairment is essential for patients to seek interven-
tion. Because patients with MHE have no specific symptoms, it
is important that factors that could prevent adverse driving
Background & Aims: Minimal hepatic encephalopathy outcomes such as traffic accidents be studied by the physician
(MHE) is associated with impaired driving skills. It is not and the validity of the patient’s self-report of driving adequacy
clear whether patients have insight into this. The Driving be examined in detail.1
Behavior Survey (DBS) is a validated self- or observer- The Driving Behavior Survey (DBS) is a 26-item scale that
administered questionnaire. DBS consists of a total score has been validated by Barkley et al12 in children and adults with
(maximum, 104) and an attention-related driving skills sec- attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). This question-
tion (maximum, 40). DBS was used to compare self-assess- naire was chosen because both MHE and ADHD patients strug-
ment with observer-assessment of driving skills in cirrhotic gle with attention problems and also show driving impairment.
patients tested for MHE. Methods: Forty-seven nonalco- It has been studied in MHE and previously was called the
holic cirrhotic patients and 40 controls underwent psycho- Driving Behavior Questionnaire.12 Each question is rated on a
metric tests, DBS, and driving simulation with navigation/ scale of 1 to 4 (maximum score, 104). Items on the DBS inquire
driving tasks. An adult familiar with the subject’s driving about both driving skills that can be readily affected by impair-
completed the DBS independently. Simulator perfor- ment in attention, vigilance, and safety judgments (10 items
mances, total DBS scores, and driving skill scores were collectively referred to here as driving skills; eg, “reacts quickly
compared between/within groups (MHEⴙ, MHEⴚ, and and properly to brake lights when activated on vehicles ahead”)
controls) with respect to self-assessment and observer as well as items that relate to long-standing driving habits (16
assessment. Results: Thirty-six patients were MHEⴙ and 11 items) (eg, “refers to maps before driving through a new area or
were MHEⴚ. MHEⴙ had a significantly higher simulator city”). In addition to the total scores, this research investigated
crash (MHEⴙ, 3; MHEⴚ, 1.2; controls, 1.7; P ⴝ .001) and the self-assessment and observer-assessment of driving skills.
illegal turn rate (MHEⴙ, 1.2; MHEⴚ, 0.3; controls, 0.1; P ⴝ Higher scores indicate better driving.
.0001). Despite this worse performance, MHEⴙ patients The hypothesis of the study was that patients with MHE
rated themselves similar to MHEⴚ patients and control have impaired insight into their driving behavior. The aims
groups on total (P ⴝ .28) and driving skills scores (P ⴝ .19). were first to determine whether patients with MHE, a group
Observer assessment in MHEⴙ was significantly lower for already known to make more errors in driving, accurately char-
total (P ⴝ .0001) and driving skills (P ⴝ .0001) compared acterize their driving behavior when compared with measures of
with observer assessment for MHEⴚ patients and control their driving skills and ratings of their driving from observers,
groups. MHEⴙ patients were rated significantly lower on and, second, to determine whether impaired insight in MHE is
driving skills (34 vs 37; P ⴝ .02) and trended lower in the identified best by items on the DBS that are thought to be most
total score (P ⴝ .08) by observers compared with self-ratings. affected by an encephalopathic state (driving skills assessment).
In contrast, MHEⴚ and control groups rated themselves sim-
ilar to their observers on driving skills and total DBS scores.
Methods
Consecutive nonalcoholic patients with cirrhosis who
Conclusions: MHE patients have poor insight into their
were car drivers and were tested for MHE were included.13
driving skills. A part of the MHE patient’s clinical interview
Cirrhotic patients on psychoactive drugs (including psychiatric
should be to increase awareness of this driving impairment.
and seizure medications, ADHD therapy, and interferon), those

M inimal hepatic encephalopathy (MHE) is a significant


neurocognitive complication of cirrhosis.1,2 It is present
in up to 80% of patients tested, it is associated with a poor
Abbreviations used in this paper: ADHD, attention deficit hyperac-
tivity disorder; DBS, Driving Behavior Survey; MHE, minimal hepatic
encephalopathy.
quality of life, and it is associated with increased progression to © 2008 by the AGA Institute
overt hepatic encephalopathy.3–5 Importantly, MHE also is as- 1542-3565/08/$34.00
sociated with driving behavior impairment.6 – 8 Driving difficul- doi:10.1016/j.cgh.2008.05.025
1136 BAJAJ ET AL CLINICAL GASTROENTEROLOGY AND HEPATOLOGY Vol. 6, No. 10

who had consumed alcohol within the past 6 months, and of the DBS) across MHE⫹, MHE⫺, and control groups were
those on treatment for overt hepatic encephalopathy were ex- calculated and compared using multiple comparisons adjusted
cluded. Alcohol use history was corroborated by relatives and in t tests on the differences after a one-way analysis of variance.
the medical chart using serum alcohol levels at several points Results are expressed as mean ⫾ SD unless otherwise specified.
during the medical evaluation. Patients underwent a detailed
physical examination to exclude overt hepatic encephalopathy.
After this they underwent a psychometric battery consisting of Results
the number connection test-A, the number connection test-B, Seventy-four cirrhotic patients and 74 controls were
block design test, and digit symbol test components of the approached; 6 patients with cirrhosis refused to participate. A
Wechsler’s adult intelligence scale-III.6,14 MHE was diagnosed if total of 68 cirrhotic patients and 74 controls underwent psy-
any 2 of these tests were impaired 2 SDs beyond age- and chometric testing, self-assessment of DBS, and driving simula-
education-matched community controls.6 The inhibitory con- tion. The observers, whose information was provided by the
trol test, a test of response inhibition, with 90% sensitivity and subjects, could be contacted in only 47 cirrhotic patients and 40
specificity for the diagnosis of MHE, also was administered to controls, who then were included in the final study. Of the 21
all subjects during the same sitting.15 A group of age- and cirrhotic patients whose observers could not be contacted, 14
education-matched healthy community controls, who were re- were MHE⫹ and 7 were MHE⫺.
cruited through community advertisements, also underwent There were 29 cirrhotic patients with hepatitis C, 5 had both
the same testing. Written informed consent was obtained from hepatitis C and alcoholic liver disease (without alcohol intake
each subject. All subjects were paid for their participation. within 6 months documented through personal interviews and
The DBS then was administered to all subjects and they also through chart review), 5 with autoimmune hepatitis, 3 with
were asked to provide the contact information for an adult primary sclerosing cholangitis, 1 with primary biliary cirrhosis,
familiar with their driving habits. This adult, who was blinded and 1 with cryptogenic cirrhosis. Based on the psychometric
to the patient’s MHE status, then was contacted in the subject’s tests, 36 cirrhotic patients had MHE and the remaining 11 were
absence. After confirming that the observer was familiar with MHE⫺.
the subject’s driving, the DBS was administered to them by There was no significant difference in Child status, serum
telephone interview to judge the research subject’s driving habits. creatinine level, bilirubin level, and international normalized
All participants (cirrhotic patients and controls) underwent ratio between groups (Table 1). Two MHE⫹ and 1 MHE⫺
driving simulation using a STISIM simulator, which has been patient were on the waiting list for transplant.
studied in patients with MHE and consists of training, naviga- Observers. Most observers were spouses (MHE⫹,
tion, and driving tasks (Systems Technology Inc, Hawthorne, 61%; MHE⫺, 55%; and controls, 55%; P ⫽ .6), and the remain-
CA; technology in the model BR1100, Beta Research, Inc, Los ing observers were close friends (MHE⫹, 17%; MHE⫺, 27%; and
Gatos, CA).13 The training session lasted for 15 minutes and controls, 23%; P ⫽ .2) or first-degree relatives (MHE⫹, 22%;
familiarized the subjects with the driving simulator. The navi- MHE⫺, 18%; and controls, 12%; P ⫽ .2) who were familiar with
gation task consisted of driving on a fixed path while consult- the subject’s driving.
ing a map on the simulator. Illegal turns, defined as turns away Minimal hepatic encephalopathyⴙ group, mini-
from the marked path on the map, and collisions during this mal hepatic encephalopathy– group, and control group
task were the outcome. The driving task consisted of driving on comparison. There was no significant difference in the de-
a path that ranged from hilly terrain to city driving under mographic characteristics and driving history between the
optimum weather conditions. The number of collisions was the groups. On the other hand, MHE⫹ patients had a significantly
end point for this portion of the study.13 worse psychometric performance and higher rate of collisions
The Institutional Review Board at the Medical College of (both in driving and navigation task) and illegal turns on the
Wisconsin approved this protocol. driving simulator compared to controls and MHE⫺ patients
(Table 1).
Statistical Analysis Comparison between the minimal hepatic en-
Diagnosis of MHE was made based on predetermined cephalopathy–negative group and controls. There was no
criteria in our population, which meant any 2 of the following significant difference between controls and the MHE⫺ group
criteria6,15: number connection test-A, greater than 34 seconds; with respect to demographics and psychometric test perfor-
number connection test-B, greater than 99 seconds; block de- mance. There was no significant difference in the number of
sign test, less than 31 raw score; or digit symbol test, less than collisions (P ⫽ .1) and illegal turns (P ⫽ .3) on the simulator
71 raw score. between MHE⫺ patients and controls (Table 1).
Analysis. Cirrhotic patients were divided into those Self-report of driving abilities (Driving Behavior
with MHE (MHE⫹) and without MHE (MHE⫺), and were Survey). There was no significant difference between MHE⫹,
compared with controls with respect to demographics, self- MHE⫺, and control groups in self-assessment of driving abil-
assessment and observer assessment of DBS total and driving ities as reflected by total DBS scores or the driving skills scores
skill components, and driving simulation outcomes. (Table 2).
Student t tests were used to compare continuous variables Observer versus self-ratings of driving abilities.
across groups and paired t tests were used to compare self- Observers rated MHE⫹ patients significantly lower on the driv-
assessment and observer assessment of DBS total and driving ing skills compared with the subjects’ self-evaluation. There was
skills within groups. Differences between self-assessment and a trend towards a lower observer versus self-report for the total
observer assessment (obtained by subtracting the observer score DBS score. In contrast, there was no significant difference
from the self-score of the total DBS and driving skills component between self-assessment and observer assessment on the total
October 2008 POOR INSIGHT IN MHE 1137

Table 1. Demographic, Psychometric, and Driving Simulator Performance


MHE⫹ (n ⫽ 36) MHE⫺ (n ⫽ 11) Controls (n ⫽ 40) P value among groups

Age, y 55 ⫾ 5 57 ⫾ 4 53 ⫾ 5 .09
Sex, male/female 21/15 6/5 25/15 .9
Child class, A/B/C 32/4/0 9/2/0 — .6
Serum creatinine level, mg/dL 1.1 ⫾ 0.9 0.9 ⫾ 0.7 — .7
International normalized ratio 1.3 ⫾ 0.9 1.1 ⫾ 0.5 — .5
Serum bilirubin level 1.5 ⫾ 1.2 1.1 ⫾ 0.8 — .3
Driving experience, y 24 ⫾ 5 27 ⫾ 4 28 ⫾ 5 .5
NCT-A, s 32 ⫾ 8 26 ⫾ 5 22 ⫾ 6 .0001
NCT-B, s 107 ⫾ 36 73 ⫾ 32 56 ⫾ 21 .0001
DST, raw score 51 ⫾ 14 80 ⫾ 9 79 ⫾ 15 .0001
BDT, raw score 31 ⫾ 12 38 ⫾ 6 49 ⫾ 18 .0001
ICT lures 10 ⫾ 5 3⫾2 4⫾2 .0001
Simulator driving task collisions 3⫾2 1.2 ⫾ 0.9 1.7 ⫾ 0.7 .0001
Simulator navigation task collisions 0.6 ⫾ 1 0.0 ⫾ 0 0.06 ⫾ 0.3 .009
Simulator illegal turns 1.2 ⫾ 0.8 0.3 ⫾ 0.4 0.1 ⫾ 0.3 .0001

NOTE. No significant difference in age, sex, Child class, serum variables of Model for End-stage Liver Disease (MELD) score, and driving
experience was noted between groups. A significantly impaired performance on the driving simulator and ICT was observed in the MHE⫹ group.
Serum values for controls were not drawn.
NCT-A, number connection test-A; NCT-B, number connection test-B; DST, digit symbol test; BDT, block design test; ICT, inhibitory control test.

DBS or the driving skills in the MHE⫺ or the control groups mance. Also, observers rate MHE⫹ patients as poorer drivers
(Table 2). compared with MHE⫺ patients or controls. These findings
Comparison of observer Driving Behavior Survey indicate that MHE⫹ patients have poor insight into their
assessment between groups. MHE⫹ patients were as- driving deficiencies.
sessed significantly lower on their total DBS and driving skills These results show that this poor insight is evident despite a
component by their raters compared with the ratings in the significantly higher collision and illegal turn rate on the driving
MHE⫺ and control groups (Table 2). simulator. MHE⫹ patients rated themselves equivalent to con-
Comparison of differences in self-scores and ob- trols and MHE⫺ patients on the total DBS score as well as
server scores between groups. There was a significantly driving skill assessment. Observers familiar with their driving
greater difference between self-ratings and observer ratings skills rated MHE⫹ patients significantly lower than the pa-
of the total DBS score in MHE⫹ patients (3.5 ⫾ 1.6 [SE]) tients’ self-rating. Moreover, observer assessment of total DBS
compared with MHE⫺ patients (⫺2.5 ⫾ 2 [SE]) and controls and driving skills component in MHE⫹ patients was signifi-
(⫺2.5 ⫾ 1 [SE]; P ⫽ .03). Similarly, the difference between cantly lower compared with the observer assessment of MHE⫺
self-assessment and observer assessment of the driving skills and control groups. The difference between self-assessment and
component was higher in MHE⫹ patients (2 ⫾ 0.9 [SE]) com- observer assessment also was significantly higher in the MHE⫹
pared with MHE⫺ patients (⫺0.2 ⫾ 0.7 [SE]) and controls group compared with the MHE⫺ and control groups.
(⫺0.3 ⫾ 0.8 [SE]; P ⫽ .04) (Figure 1).
MHE has been associated with difficulty in driving and a
high risk of traffic accidents and violations.9 –11 Proposed rea-
Discussion sons for this driving difficulty include impaired reaction times,
The current study shows that patients with MHE rate decreased visuomotor coordination, and navigation prob-
themselves equivalent to controls and cirrhotic patients with- lems.9,10,13 This multidimensional driving impairment requires
out MHE despite having a significantly worse driving perfor- continued investigation and definition because traffic accidents

Table 2. DBS Comparison Within and Between Groups


P value of comparison
between self-assessment
and observer assessment
MHE⫹ MHE⫺ Controls P value
(n ⫽ 36) (n ⫽ 11) (n ⫽ 40) among groups MHE⫹ MHE⫺ Controls

Total DBS: self-assessment (maximum, 104) 95 ⫾ 7 96 ⫾ 6 95 ⫾ 7 .28 .08 .4 .5


Total DBS: observer assessment (maximum, 104) 89 ⫾ 9 98 ⫾ 4 97 ⫾ 7 .0001
Driving skills: self-assessment (maximum, 40) 37 ⫾ 3 38 ⫾ 1 37 ⫾ 1 .19 .02 .8 .9
Driving skills: observer assessment (maximum, 40) 34 ⫾ 5 38 ⫾ 2 38 ⫾ 2 .0001

NOTE. A significantly lower observer assessment of driving skills in MHE⫹ group compared with self-assessment was noted. Observer
assessment of total DBS and driving skills was significantly lower in the MHE⫹ group compared with the observer assessment in MHE⫺ and
control groups.
1138 BAJAJ ET AL CLINICAL GASTROENTEROLOGY AND HEPATOLOGY Vol. 6, No. 10

of most ADHD patients is associated with a high risk of traffic


accidents and violations even without the compounding dele-
terious effect of ADHD.23 On the other hand, MHE⫹ patients
are older than ADHD patients. The patients in the current
study population are in the age range of the safest age group of
any population with respect to traffic violations and accidents.
Thus, driving assessment differences are likely to be more subtle
than those expected in ADHD. Parents assessing children for
whom they are seeking treatment are also likely to rate them
differently compared with assessment of adult raters of a pa-
tient (with MHE) who has not sought medical attention for
that condition. Vested interests, such as a spouse protecting
their partners’ ability to drive by falsely rating them higher, also
would be expected to play a key role in that assessment. Because
subjects were free to provide the contact information of an
observer of their choice, it may be possible that these vested
interests would dictate the choice of the observer, that is,
subjects would probably name an observer who would be likely
to give a favorable assessment of their driving. Despite these
Figure 1. Difference between MHE⫹, MHE⫺, and controls’ self-as- complicating factors, observers rated MHE⫹ patients much
sessment and observer assessment of total DBS and driving skills lower with respect to their driving skills compared with their
component showed that there was a significantly higher difference be- self-assessment. There was also a trend towards lower total DBS
tween the self-ratings and observer ratings in MHE⫹ compared with scores in observers compared with self-assessment in the
MHE⫺ patients and controls. MHE⫹ group. In sharp contrast, there was no significant dif-
ference in the driving skills rating or total DBS score between
self and observer assessment in the group without MHE and
are a leading cause of morbidity and mortality in the United controls.
States.16 Insight into a disease state is one of the most significant
Because patients with MHE have no symptoms, evaluation forces that can spur patients to seek health care.25 Absence of
of self-awareness in MHE patients can be challenging in this insight or poor insight can hinder acknowledgment of the
group.1,2,17,18 However, the manifestations of poor driving skills disease process and interfere with potential recognition and
are evident from the higher number of traffic accidents and treatment. The trans-theoretical model of health behavior
violations that MHE⫹ patients experienced in the simulator change also suggests that patients with MHE and driving dif-
and in real life.11,13 Therefore, the importance of specifically ficulties may be in the pre-contemplation stage of change.25
inquiring about the driving history in patients with cirrhosis This stage is characterized by an unwillingness to recognize a
would help create awareness in the patient and hopefully help problem either because the patient is unaware of the problem
the development of insight into this issue. In addition, educa- or believes that they will be unable to change their behavior.25
tion of patients and relatives regarding the driving impairments Because of their lack of personal insight into their driving skills,
in MHE also would be helpful in raising self-awareness about MHE patients are not aware of their driving impairment.
driving practices. Anosognosia or attenuated awareness of sensory and cognitive
Recent literature has highlighted the negative impact of deficits has been noted in several diseases such as Alzheimer’s
cirrhosis on survival after trauma and traffic accidents.19 –21 This disease, schizophrenia, and traumatic brain injury.26 –28 It has
further accentuates the risk that MHE⫹ drivers face while been associated with a worse prognosis and dangerous behav-
driving. Therefore, it follows that an objective evaluation, such iors in Alzheimer’s disease.26,29 One study showed that one third
as a driving history and corroboration from relatives in patients of patients with Alzheimer’s disease who failed a driving test
with cirrhosis, would identify individuals at a particularly high still considered themselves “safe” drivers.30 Anosognosia in
risk of poor driving outcomes. Corroboration from relatives has overt hepatic encephalopathy is evident because relatives are
been used to confirm alcohol use in patients with cirrhosis.22 often the first to notice changes in behavior and bring them to
This should be extended to driving behavior as well because this medical attention.31 In contrast, MHE patients by definition
study shows that adult observers are acutely observant of driv- have no specific symptoms.1 We suggest that, similar to their
ing impairments. lack of awareness of the driving deficits, the lack of awareness of
Similar to MHE⫹ patients, patients with ADHD suffer from attention deficits and psychomotor slowing represents a core
attention deficits.23 ADHD patients also have several driving symptom of MHE patients and underlies their diminished
impairments and difficulties with psychometric function, appreciation of driving deficits.
which is why the DBS, which has been studied in ADHD, was In summary, patients with MHE have poor insight into their
chosen for this study.12,23,24 Parents assessing their children driving skills. Because of this, an objective driving history,
with ADHD using the DBS rated the children’s driving skills corroboration with relatives, and education of patients, rela-
significantly lower than those who did not have ADHD.12 Pa- tives, and caregivers regarding driving practices should be com-
tients with ADHD are decidedly younger than most patients pleted as part of the clinical interview. This would increase the
with cirrhosis and are actively seeking medical attention for patient’s self-awareness of the driving impairment and increase
problems related to inattentiveness. In addition, the age range the implementation of appropriate therapy in MHE patients.
October 2008 POOR INSIGHT IN MHE 1139

References hepatic encephalopathy in the United States: an AASLD survey.


1. Ortiz M, Jacas C, Cordoba J. Minimal hepatic encephalopathy: Hepatology 2007;45:833– 834.
diagnosis, clinical significance and recommendations. J Hepatol 18. Vergara-Gomez M, Flavia-Olivella M, Gil-Prades M, et al. [Diag-
nosis and treatment of hepatic encephalopathy in Spain: re-
2005;42(Suppl):S45–S53.
sults of a survey of hepatologists]. Gastroenterol Hepatol
2. Qadri AM, Ogunwale BO, Mullen KD. Can we ignore minimal hepatic
2006;29:1– 6.
encephalopathy any longer? Hepatology 2007;45:547–548.
19. Dangleben DA, Jazaeri O, Wasser T, et al. Impact of cirrhosis on
3. Arguedas MR, DeLawrence TG, McGuire BM. Influence of hepatic
outcomes in trauma. J Am Coll Surg 2006;203:908 –913.
encephalopathy on health-related quality of life in patients with
20. Demetriades D, Constantinou C, Salim A, et al. Liver cirrhosis in
cirrhosis. Dig Dis Sci 2003;48:1622–1626.
patients undergoing laparotomy for trauma: effect on outcomes.
4. Romero-Gomez M, Boza F, Garcia-Valdecasas MS, et al. Subclinical
J Am Coll Surg 2004;199:538 –542.
hepatic encephalopathy predicts the development of overt hepatic
21. Bajaj JS, Ananthakrishnan A, McGinley A, et al. Deleterious effect of
encephalopathy. Am J Gastroenterol 2001;96:2718 –2723.
cirrhosis on outcomes after motor vehicle crashes using the Nation-
5. Stewart CA, Smith GE. Minimal hepatic encephalopathy. Nat Clin
wide Inpatient Sample. Am J Gastroenterol 2007 (in press).
Pract Gastroenterol Hepatol 2007;4:677– 685.
22. Murray KF, Carithers RL Jr. AASLD practice guidelines: evaluation
6. Ferenci P, Lockwood A, Mullen K, et al. Hepatic encephalopathy—
of the patient for liver transplantation. Hepatology 2005;41:
definition, nomenclature, diagnosis, and quantification: final re- 1407–1432.
port of the working party at the 11th World Congresses of Gas- 23. Barkley RA, Cox D. A review of driving risks and impairments
troenterology, Vienna, 1998. Hepatology 2002;35:716 –721. associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the
7. Das A, Dhiman RK, Saraswat VA, et al. Prevalence and natural effects of stimulant medication on driving performance. J Safety
history of subclinical hepatic encephalopathy in cirrhosis. J Gas- Res 2007;38:113–128.
troenterol Hepatol 2001;16:531–535. 24. Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, et al. Hyperactive children as
8. Mullen K, Ferenci P, Bass NM, et al. An algorithm for the man- young adults: driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse
agement of hepatic encephalopathy. Semin Liver Dis 2007;27: driving outcomes. Accid Anal Prev 2007;39:94 –105.
32– 48. 25. Prochaska JORC, Evers K. The transtheoretical model and stages
9. Wein C, Koch H, Popp B, et al. Minimal hepatic encephalopathy of change. Jossey-Bass, Inc., 2002.
impairs fitness to drive. Hepatology 2004;39:739 –745. 26. Ries ML, Jabbar BM, Schmitz TW, et al. Anosognosia in mild
10. Schomerus H, Hamster W, Blunck H, et al. Latent portasystemic cognitive impairment: relationship to activation of cortical midline
encephalopathy. I. Nature of cerebral functional defects and their structures involved in self-appraisal. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
effect on fitness to drive. Dig Dis Sci 1981;26:622– 630. 2007;13:450 – 461.
11. Bajaj JS, Hafeezullah M, Hoffmann RG, et al. Minimal hepatic 27. Fischer S, Trexler LE, Gauggel S. Awareness of activity limitations
encephalopathy: a vehicle for accidents and traffic violations. and prediction of performance in patients with brain injuries and
Am J Gastroenterol 2007;102:1903–1909. orthopedic disorders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:190 –199.
12. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Dupaul GI, et al. Driving in young adults 28. Amador XF, Flaum M, Andreasen NC, et al. Awareness of illness
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: knowledge, perfor- in schizophrenia and schizoaffective and mood disorders. Arch
mance, adverse outcomes, and the role of executive functioning. Gen Psychiatry 1994;51:826 – 836.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:655– 672. 29. Starkstein SE, Jorge R, Mizrahi R, et al. Insight and danger in
13. Bajaj JS, Hafeezullah M, Hoffmann RG, et al. Navigation skill Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurol 2007;14:455– 460.
impairment: Another dimension of the driving difficulties in min- 30. Hunt L, Morris JC, Edwards D, et al. Driving performance in
imal hepatic encephalopathy. Hepatology 2008;47:596 – 604. persons with mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Am
14. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. Psychological Geriatr Soc 1993;41:747–752.
Corp.; 1999. 31. Weissenborn K. Hepatic encephalopathy. Philadelphia: Saun-
15. Bajaj JS, Saeian K, Verber MD, et al. Inhibitory control test is a ders, 2003.
simple method to diagnose minimal hepatic encephalopathy and
predict development of overt hepatic encephalopathy. Am J Gas-
troenterol 2007;102:754 –760. Address requests for reprints to: Jasmohan S. Bajaj, MD, MS, Division of
16. Gerber T, Schomerus H. Hepatic encephalopathy in liver cirrho- Gastroenterology and Hepatology, Medical College of Wisconsin, 9200 W
sis: pathogenesis, diagnosis and management. Drugs 2000;60: Wisconsin Avenue, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53226. e-mail: jasmohan.
1353–1370. bajaj@va.gov.
17. Bajaj JS, Etemadian A, Hafeezullah M, et al. Testing for minimal The authors disclose no financial conflicts of interest.

You might also like