You are on page 1of 93

CULTURAL INFLUENCES ON LEADERSHIP AND ORGANIZATIONS:

PROJECT GLOBE

Robert J. House, Paul J. Hanges, S. Antonio Ruiz-Quintanilla, Peter W. Dorfman, Mansour


Javidan, Marcus Dickson, and About 170 GLOBE Country Co-Investigators to be listed by name
and institution

Running Head: Project GLOBE

Robert J. House
The Wharton School of Management
University of Pennsylvania

Paul J. Hanges
Department of Psychology
University of Maryland

S. Antonio Ruiz-Quintanilla
New York School of Industrial Labor Relations
Cornell University

Peter W. Dorfman
Department of Management
New Mexico State University

Mansour Javidan
Faculty of Management
Policy and Environmental Area
University of Calgary

Marcus Dickson
Department of Psychology
Wayne State University

Vipin Gupta
Faculty of Business Administration
Fordham University

Add list of 170 authors and their institutions here.

* The authors are indebted to Markus Hauser, University of Zurich, for his thoughtful comments
and suggestions relevant to this monograph.
2
ABSTRACT

GLOBE is both a research program and a social entity. The GLOBE social entity is a

network of 170 social scientists and management scholars from 61 cultures throughout the world,

working in a coordinated long-term effort to examine the interrelationships between societal

culture, organizational culture and practices, and organizational leadership. The meta-goal of the

Global Leadership and Organizational Effectiveness (GLOBE) Research Program is to develop

an empirically based theory to describe, understand, and predict the impact of cultural variables

on leadership and organizational processes and the effectiveness of these processes.

This monograph presents a description of the GLOBE research program and some initial

empirical findings resulting from GLOBE research. A central question in this part of the research

concerns the extent to which specific leadership attributes and behaviors are universally endorsed

as contributing to effective leadership and the extent to which the endorsement of leader

attributes and behaviors is culturally contingent.

We identified six global leadership dimensions of culturally endorsed implicit theories of

leadership (CLTs). Preliminary evidence indicates that these dimensions are significantly

correlated with isomorphic dimensions of societal and organizational culture. These findings are

consistent with the hypothesis that selected cultural differences strongly influence important

ways in which people think about leaders and norms concerning the status, influence, and

privileges granted to leaders.

The hypothesis that charismatic/value-based leadership would be universally endorsed is

strongly supported. Team-oriented leadership is strongly correlated with charismatic/value-based

leadership, and also universally endorsed. Humane and participative leadership dimensions are

nearly universally endorsed. The endorsement of the remaining global leadership dimensions --

self-protective and autonomous leadership vary by culture.


3
We identified 21 specific leader attributes and behaviors that are universally viewed as

contributing to leadership effectiveness. Eleven of the specific leader characteristics composing

the global charismatic/value-based leadership dimension were among these 21 attributes. Eight

specific leader characteristics were universally viewed as impediments to leader effectiveness.

We also identified 35 specific leader characteristics that are viewed as contributors in some

cultures and impediments in other cultures. We present these, as well as other findings, in more

detail in this monograph.

A particular strength of the GLOBE research design is the combination of quantitative

and qualitative data. Elimination of common method and common source variance is also a

strength of the design strategy. Future directions, research strategies, and anticipated

contributions are presented in anticipation of continued GLOBE efforts.


4
INTRODUCTION

To what extent is leadership culturally contingent? The Global Leadership and

Organizational Behavior Effectiveness Research Program (GLOBE), as well as a substantial

amount of other empirical research (House, Wright, & Aditya, 1997), has demonstrated that what

is expected of leaders, what leaders may and may not do, and the status and influence bestowed

on leaders vary considerably as a result of the cultural forces in the countries or regions in which

the leaders function. For instance, Americans, Arabs, Asians, English, Eastern Europeans,

French, Germans, Latin Americans, and Russians tend to glorify the concept of leadership and

consider it reasonable to discuss leadership in the context of both the political and the

organizational arenas. People of the Netherlands and Scandinavia often have distinctly different

views of leadership. Consider the following statements taken from interviews with managers

from various countries:

Americans appreciate two kinds of leaders. They seek empowerment from leaders who

grant autonomy and delegate authority to subordinates. They also respect the bold, forceful,

confident, and risk-taking leader, as personified by John Wayne.

The Dutch place emphasis on egalitarianism and are skeptical about the value of

leadership. Terms like leader and manager carry a stigma. If a father is employed as a manager,

Dutch children will not admit it to their schoolmates.

Arabs worship their leaders--as long as they are in power!

Iranians seek power and strength in their leaders.

Malaysians expect their leaders to behave in a manner that is humble, modest, and

dignified.
5
The French appreciate two kinds of leaders. De Gaulle and Mitterand are examples. De

Gaulle is an example of a strong charismatic leader. Mitterand is an example of a consensus

builder, coalition former, and effective negotiator.

Does extant empirical research literature confirm the expectations that are implied in the

above statements? Because we are just beginning to understand how the role of culture

influences leadership and organizational processes, numerous research questions remain

unanswered. What characteristics of a society make it more or less susceptible to leadership

influence? To what extent do cultural forces influence the expectations that individuals have with

respect to the role of leaders and their behavior? To what extent will leadership styles vary in

accordance with culturally specific values and expectations? To what extent does culture

moderate relationships between organizational processes, organizational form, and

organizational effectiveness? What principles and/or laws of leadership and organizational

processes transcend cultures?

We do not have comprehensive answers to these questions, but progress has been made

in a number of areas (see House, Wright, & Aditya [1997] for an extensive review of relevant

leadership literature). This essay describes a programmatic effort undertaken to explore the

fascinating and complex effects of culture on leadership and organizational processes

THE NEED FOR CROSS-CULTURAL LEADERSHIP THEORY AND RESEARCH

Given the increased globalization of industrial organizations and increased

interdependencies among nations, the need for better understanding of cultural influences on

leadership and organizational practices has never been greater. Situations that leaders and would-

be leaders must face are highly complex, constantly changing, and difficult to interpret. More

than ever before, managers of international firms face fierce and rapidly changing international
6
competition. The trend toward the global economic village is clear, and the 21st century may

very well become known as the century of the “global world” (McFarland, Senen, & Childress,

1993). Since effective organizational leadership is critical to the success of international

operations, this globalization of industrial organizations presents numerous organizational and

leadership challenges. For instance, the cultural diversity of employees found in worldwide

multinational organizations presents a substantial challenge with respect to the design of

multinational organizations and their leadership. What practical knowledge and advice does the

management literature provide to assist leaders in adapting to cultural constraints? Unfortunately,

though the need for such information clearly exists, little if any help is available at this time

(House, Wright, & Aditya, 1997; House & Aditya, 1997). Cross-cultural research and the

development of cross-cultural theory are needed to fill this knowledge gap.

From a scientific and theoretical perspective, compelling reasons exist for considering the

role of societal and organizational culture in influencing leadership and organizational processes.

Because the goal of science is to develop universally valid theories, laws, and principles, there is

a need for leadership and organizational theories that transcend cultures. There are inherent

limitations in transferring theories across cultures. What works in one culture may not work in

another culture. As Triandis (1993) suggests, leadership researchers will be able to “fine-tune”

theories by investigating cultural variations as parameters of those theories. In addition, a focus

on cross-cultural issues can help researchers uncover new relationships by forcing investigators

to include a much broader range of variables often not considered in contemporary theories, such

as the importance of religion, language, ethnic background, history, or political systems

(Dorfman, 1996). Thus, cross-cultural research may also help to develop new theories of

leadership and organizational processes and effectiveness, as well as to fine-tune existing


7
theories by incorporating cultural variables as antecedents and moderators within existing

theoretical frameworks.

While the research literature on cross-cultural leadership has blossomed in the last fifteen

years (House, Wright, & Aditya, 1997), it is often atheoretical, fraught with methodological

problems, and fragmented across a wide variety of publication outlets (Dorfman, 1996). More

important, far more questions than answers exist regarding the culturally contingent aspects of

leadership. Project GLOBE is intended to contribute theoretical developments and empirical

findings to fill this knowledge deficiency.

THE GLOBE RESEARCH PROGRAM

The idea of a global research program concerned with leadership and organization

practices (form and processes) was conceived in the summer of 1991. In the spring of 1993 a

grant proposal was written that followed a substantial literature review and development of a

pool of 753 questionnaire items. GLOBE was funded in October 1993, and the recruiting of
1
GLOBE Country Co-Investigators (referred to hereafter as CCIs) began. In this section, we

present an overview of Project GLOBE.

GLOBE is a multi-phase, multi-method project in which investigators spanning the world

are examining the interrelationships between societal culture, organizational culture, and

organizational leadership. One hundred seventy social scientists and management scholars from

61 cultures representing all major regions of the world are engaged in this long-term

programmatic series of cross-cultural leadership studies. Table 1 lists the countries in which
2
cultures are being studied as part of the GLOBE research.

TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE


8
GLOBE was conceived and initially designed by the first author of this monograph as

the Principal Investigator. He was later joined by Michael Agar, Marcus Dickson, Paul Hanges,

and S. Antonio Ruiz-Quintanilla as Co-Principal Investigators. Because cross-cultural research

requires knowledge of all the cultures being studied, we have developed a network of

approximately 170 CCIs who are social scientists or management scholars from around the

world . The CCIs, together with the Principal Investigators and Research Associates, constitute

the members of the GLOBE community.

The CCIs are responsible for leadership of the project in a specific culture in which they

have expertise. Their activities include collecting quantitative and qualitative data, ensuring the

accuracy of questionnaire translations, writing country-specific descriptions of their cultures in

which they interpret the results of the quantitative data analyses in their own cultural context, and

contributing insights from their unique cultural perspectives to the ongoing GLOBE research. In

most cases, CCIs are natives of the cultures from which they are collecting data, and in most

cases, they reside in that culture. Some of the CCIs are persons with extensive experience in

more than one culture. Most cultures have a research team of between two and five CCIs

working on the project. The activities of the project as a whole are coordinated by the GLOBE
3
Coordinating Team (GCT). The GCT is also responsible for designing quantitative measures

and qualitative methods, performing cross-cultural statistical analyses, and coordinating efforts

to present results of the project to the scholarly community. To date, CCIs have made over 80

presentations at professional meetings and over 30 papers and book chapters have been written.

An initial goal of the GLOBE Project was to develop societal and organizational

measures of culture and leader attributes that are appropriate to use across all cultures. We have

accomplished this in the first phase of the research project. Items were analyzed by conventional
9
psychometric procedures (e.g., item analysis, factor analysis, generalizability analysis) to

establish nine dimensions of societal culture and nine isomorphic dimensions of organizational

culture. In addition, as part of the first phase of the project, we were able to identify six

underlying dimensions of global leadership patterns that are viewed by managers as contributors

or impediments to outstanding leadership. The psychometric properties of these scales exceed

conventional standards (Hanges, House, Dickson, Ruiz-Quintanilla, Dorfman & 103 co-authors,

1997, under review).

One of the major questions addressed by GLOBE research concerns the dimensions by

which societal and organizational cultures can be measured. We identified nine dimensions of

cultures that differentiate societies and organizations. That is, with respect to these dimensions,

there is high within-culture and within-organization agreement and high between-culture and

between-organization differentiation.

A second major question addressed by GLOBE concerns the extent to which specific

leader attributes and behaviors are universally endorsed as contributing to effective leadership,

and the extent to which attributes and behaviors are linked to cultural characteristics. We found

that cultures can be differentiated on the basis of the leader behaviors and attributes that their

members endorse. We also found high within-culture agreement with respect to leader attributes

and behaviors that are viewed as contributors or impediments to effective leadership. These

leader behaviors and attributes constitute Culturally endorsed implicit Leadership Theories

(CLTs).

We empirically identified six global leader behavior dimensions of CLTs. Two of these

dimensions are universally viewed as contributors to effective leadership, one is nearly

universally endorsed as a contributor, and one is nearly universally perceived as an impediment


10
to outstanding leadership. The endorsement of the remaining two dimensions varies by

culture. In addition, we identified 21 specific leader attributes or behaviors that are universally

viewed as contributors to leadership effectiveness and 8 that are universally viewed as

impediments to leader effectiveness. Further, 35 specific leader attributes or behaviors are

viewed as contributors in some cultures and impediments in other cultures. We present these, as

well as other findings, in more detail below.

Project GLOBE also addresses questions relevant to how societal cultural forces

influence organizational form and effectiveness. We describe the research questions, hypotheses,

and research design relevant to both leadership and organizational aspects of GLOBE in some

detail further on.

Project GLOBE employs both quantitative and qualitative methods to provide richly

descriptive, yet scientifically valid, accounts of cultural influences on leadership and

organizational processes. Quantitative aspects include measurement of societal culture,

organizational culture, and leadership attributes and behaviors. Contemporaneous with the

quantitative analysis, qualitative culture-specific research is being conducted in the same

cultures. Qualitative culture-specific interpretations of local behaviors, norms, and practices are

being developed through content analysis of data derived from interviews, focus groups, and

published media.

The planned GLOBE research program consists of four phases. GLOBE Phase 1 was

devoted to the development of research instruments. Phase 2 is devoted to assessment of nine

dimensions of societal and organizational cultures and tests of hypotheses relevant to the

relationships among these cultural dimensions and cultural-level implicit theories of leadership.

Phase 2 also concerns relationships between organizational contingencies (size, technology,


11
environment, and strategy), organizational form and processes, and organizational

effectiveness. Phase 2 data collection has been completed. A projected third phase of the

research project will investigate the impact and effectiveness of specific leader behaviors and

styles on subordinates’ attitudes and job performance and on leader effectiveness. Phase 3 will

also be directed toward the identification of emic (culture-specific) aspects of leadership and

organizational practices, as well as the longitudinal effects of leadership and organizational

practices and form on organizational effectiveness. A projected fourth phase will employ field

and laboratory experiments to confirm, establish causality, and extend previous findings.

GLOBE Objectives

The meta-goal of GLOBE is to develop an empirically based theory to describe,

understand, and predict the impact of specific cultural variables on leadership and organizational

processes and the effectiveness of these processes. Specific objectives include answering the

following fundamental questions:

1. Are there leader behaviors, attributes, and organizational practices that are universally

accepted and effective across cultures?

2. Are there leader behaviors, attributes, and organizational practices that are accepted

and effective in only some cultures?

3. How do attributes of societal and organizational cultures affect the kinds of leader

behaviors and organizational practices that are accepted and effective?

4. What is the effect of violating cultural norms relevant to leadership and organizational

practices?

5. What is the relative standing of each of the cultures studied on each of the nine core

dimensions of culture?
12
6. Can the universal and culture-specific aspects of leader behaviors, attributes, and

organizational practices be explained in terms of an underlying theory that accounts for

systematic differences across cultures?

Construct Definitions of Leadership and Culture

Leadership has been a topic of study for social scientists for much of the 20th century

(Yukl, 1994), yet there is no consensually agreed-upon definition of leadership (Bass, 1990). A

seemingly endless variety of definitions have been developed, but almost all have at their core

the concept of influence--leaders influence others to help accomplish group or organizational

objectives. The variety of definitions is appropriate, as the degree of specificity of the definition

of leadership should be driven by the purposes of the research. Smith and Bond (1993)

specifically note: “If we wish to make statements about universal or etic aspects of social

behavior, they need to be phrased in highly abstract ways. Conversely, if we wish to highlight

the meaning of these generalizations in specific or emic ways, then we need to refer to more

precisely specified events or behaviors” (p. 58). The GLOBE goals are both etic (investigating

aspects of leadership and organizational practices that are comparable across cultures) and emic

(examining and describing culture-specific differences in leadership and organizational practices

and their effectiveness). We recognize and expect that the evaluative and semantic interpretation

of the term leadership, and the ways in which leadership and organizational processes are

enacted, are likely to vary across cultures, but we also expect that some aspects of leadership will

be universally endorsed.

In August 1994 the first GLOBE research conference was held at the University of

Calgary in Canada. Fifty-four researchers from 38 countries gathered to develop a collective

understanding of the project and to initiate its implementation. In this meeting considerable time
13
was spent generating a working definition of leadership that reflected the diverse viewpoints

held by GLOBE researchers. A consensus with respect to a universal definition of organizational

leadership emerged: the ability of an individual to influence, motivate, and enable others to

contribute toward the effectiveness and success of the organizations of which they are members.

Note that this is a definition of organizational leadership, not leadership in general. Simonton

(1994:411), speaking of leadership in general, defines a leader as a “group member whose

influence on group attitudes, performance, or decision making greatly exceeds that of the

average member of the group.” The GLOBE project concerns the phenomenon of organizational

leadership, not leadership in general.

As with leadership, there is no consensually agreed upon definition among social

scientists for the term culture. Generally speaking, culture is used by social scientists to refer to

a set of parameters of collectives that differentiate the collectives from each other in meaningful

ways. The focus is on the “sharedness” of cultural indicators among members of the collective.

The specific criteria used to differentiate cultures usually depend on the preferences of the

investigator and the issues under investigation, and tend to reflect the discipline of the

investigator. For the GLOBE research program, we theoretically define culture as shared

motives, values, beliefs, identities, and interpretations or meanings of significant events that

result from common experiences of members of collectives and are transmitted across age

generations. Note that these are psychological attributes and that this definition can be applied at

both the societal and the organizational levels of analysis.

GLOBE Operational Definition of Culture

The most parsimonious operationalizations of societal culture consist of commonly

experienced language, ideological belief systems (including religion and political belief
14
systems), ethnic heritage, and history. Parallel to this, the most parsimonious

operationalizations of organizational culture consist of commonly used nomenclature within an

organization, shared organizational values, and organizational history. For purposes of GLOBE

research, therefore, culture is operationally defined by the use of measures reflecting two kinds

of cultural manifestations: (a) the commonality (agreement) among members of collectives with

respect to the psychological attributes specified above; and (b) the commonality of observed and

reported practices of entities such as families, schools, work organizations, economic and legal

systems, and political institutions.

The common cultural attributes we have chosen to measure are indicators of shared

modal values of collectives. These values are expressed in response to questionnaire items in the

form of judgments of What Should Be. Emphasis on values grows out of an anthropological

tradition of culture assessment (Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961). Another measure of culture,

modal practices, is measured by indicators assessing What Is, or What Are, common behaviors,

institutional practices, proscriptions and prescriptions. This approach to the assessment of culture

grows out of a psychological/behavioral tradition, in which it is assumed that shared values are

enacted in behaviors, policies, and practices. This assumption will be tested as part of Project

GLOBE.
15
The GLOBE Conceptual Model

The theoretical base that guides the GLOBE research program is an integration of implicit

leadership theory (Lord & Maher, 1991), value/belief theory of culture (Hofstede, 1980), implicit

motivation theory (McClelland, 1985), and structural contingency theory of organizational form

and effectiveness (Donaldson, 1993; Hickson, Hinings, McMillan, & Schwitter, 1974). The

relevant and essential features of each of these theories are briefly described in the following

paragraphs. The integrated theory is then described. For a more detailed description of the

integrated theory, see House, Wright, & Aditya (1997).

Implicit Leadership Theory

According to this theory individuals have implicit theories (beliefs, convictions, and

assumptions) about the attributes and behaviors that distinguish leaders from others, effective

leaders from ineffective ones, and moral leaders from evil ones. Implicit leadership theories

influence the values that individuals place on selected leader behaviors and attributes, and their

motives relevant to acceptance and enactment of leader behavior. The following propositions

express the major assertions of implicit leadership theory.

1. Leadership qualities are attributed to individuals, and those persons are accepted as

leaders, on the basis of the degree of fit, or congruence, between the leader behaviors they enact

and the implicit leadership theory held by the attributers.

2. Implicit leadership theories constrain, moderate, and guide the exercise of leadership,

the acceptance of leaders, the perception of leaders as influential, acceptable, and effective, and

the degree to which leaders are granted status and privileges. There is substantial experimental

evidence in support of this theory (Lord & Maher, 1991; Hanges, Braverman, & Rentsch, 1991;

Hanges, Lord, Day, Sipe, Smith, & Brown, 1997; Sipe & Hanges, 1997).
16
Value/Belief Theory

Hofstede (1980) and Triandis (1995) assert that the values and beliefs held by members

of cultures influence the degree to which the behaviors of individuals, groups, and institutions

within cultures are enacted, and the degree to which they are viewed as legitimate, acceptable,

and effective. Hofstede’s version of value/belief theory includes four dimensions of cultural

values and beliefs: Individualism versus Collectivism, Masculinity versus Femininity, Tolerance

versus Intolerance of Uncertainty, and Power Distance (Stratification) versus Power

Equalization. We have substituted two cultural dimensions labeled Gender Egalitarianism and

Assertiveness for Hofstede’s Masculinity dimension. As explained below we also measured

collectivism with two, rather than one, scale. Finally, we have added three additional

dimensions: Humanistic, Performance, and Future Orientation. Collectively, the nine dimensions

reflect not only the dimensions of Hofstede’s theory but also David McClelland’s theories of

national economic development (McClelland, 1961) and human motivation (McClelland, 1985).

The humanism, power distance, and performance orientation of cultures, when measured with

operant (behavioral) indicators, are conceptually analogous to the affiliative, power, and

achievement motives in McClelland’s implicit motivation theory. We believe that the nine core

GLOBE dimensions reflect important aspects of the human condition.

Implicit Motivation Theory

Implicit motivation theory is the theory of non-conscious motives originally advanced by

McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, and Lowell (1953). In its most general form the theory asserts that

the essential nature of human motivation can be understood in terms of three implicit (non-

conscious) motives: achievement, affiliation, and power (social influence). In contrast to

behavioral intentions and conscious values, which are predictive of discrete task behaviors for
17
short periods of time under constant situational forces (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1970), implicit

motives are predictive of (a) motive arousal in the presence of selected stimuli, (b) spontaneous

behavior in the absence of motive-arousal stimuli, and (c) long-term (as long as twenty years)

individual global behavior patterns, such as social relationship patterns, citizenship behavior,

child-rearing practices, and leadership styles. While McClelland’s theory is an individual theory

of non-conscious motivation, the GLOBE theory is a theory of motivation resulting from cultural

forces.Structural Contingency TheoryThe central proposition of this theory is that the there

is a set of demands that are imposed on organizations that must be met if organizations are to

survive and be effective. These demands are referred to as organizational contingencies. It is

asserted that these contingencies influence organizational form and practice and that congruence

between the demands of the contingencies and organizational form and practice is associated

with organizational effectiveness. While this is a popular theoretical perspective, its empirical

verification is mostly limited to small sample studies of organizations in industrialized countries

(Child, 1981). Hickson, Hinings, McMillan & Schwitter (1974) have asserted that the

propositions of structural contingency theory are universal and culture-free. This assertion rests

on the assumption that organizational contingencies impose demands on organizations that are so

strong that it is imperative for all organizations to respond in essentially the same way to them in

order to perform effectively and survive in competitive environments. We refer to this assertion

as the task environment imperative. Child (1981) has presented a serious challenge to this

assertion.

The Integrated Theory

A diagram of the integrated theory is presented in Figure 1.

INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE


18
The Central Theoretical Proposition.

The central theoretical proposition of the integrated theory is that the attributes and

entities that distinguish a given culture from other cultures are predictive of the practices of

organizations and leader attributes and behaviors that are most frequently enacted, acceptable,

and effective in that culture. The integrated theory consists of the following propositions, which

are also shown in the system diagram in Figure 1:

1. Societal cultural values and practices affect what leaders do. Substantial empirical

evidence supports this assertion (House, Wright, & Aditya, 1997). First, founders of

organizations--the organizations’ original leaders--are immersed in their own societal culture,

and they are most likely to enact the global leader behavior patterns that are favored in that

culture. Founders influence the behavior of subordinate leaders and subsequent leaders by use of

selective management selection criteria, role modeling, and socialization. Further, the dominant

cultural norms endorsed by societal cultures induce global leader behavior patterns and

organizational practices that are differentially expected and viewed as legitimate among cultures.

Thus, the attributes and behaviors of leaders are, in part, a reflection of the organizational

practices, which in turn are a reflection of societal cultures (e.g., Kopelman, Brief, & Guzzo,

1990).

2. Leadership affects organizational form, culture, and practices. Founders of

organizations establish the initial culture of their organizations (e.g., Schein, 1992; Schneider,

1987; Schneider, Goldstein, & Smith, 1995), and founders and subsequent leaders continue to

influence the organizational culture (e.g., Bass, 1985; Miller & Droge, 1986; Schein, 1992; Yukl,

1994; Thompson & Luthans, 1990).


19
3. Societal cultural values and practices also affect organizational culture and practices.

Societal culture has a direct influence on organizational culture, as the shared meaning that

results from the dominant cultural values, beliefs, assumptions, and implicit motives endorsed by

culture, results in common implicit leadership theories and implicit organization theories held by

members of the culture (e.g., Lord and Maher, 1991; House, Wright, and Aditya, 1997).

4. Organizational culture and practices also affect what leaders do. Over time, founders

and subsequent leaders in organizations respond to the organizational culture and alter their

behaviors and leader styles (e.g., Schein, 1992; Trice and Beyer, 1984; Lombardo, 1983).

5, 6. Societal culture and organizational form, culture and practices both influence the

process by which people come to share implicit theories of leadership. Over time, CLTs are

developed in each culture in response to both societal and organizational culture and practices

(e.g., Lord & Maher, 1991). CLTs thus differentiate cultures.

7. Strategic organizational contingencies affect organizational form, culture and practices

and leader behaviors. Organizational contingencies (size, technology, environment) impose

requirements that organizations must meet in order to perform effectively, compete, and survive.

Organizational practices are largely directed toward meeting the requirements imposed on

organizations by organizational contingencies (Burns & Stalker, 1961; (Donaldson, 1993;

Lawrence & Lorsch, 1967; Tushman, Newman, & Nadler, 1988).

8. Strategic organizational contingencies affect leader attributes and behavior. Leaders

are selected and adjust their behaviors to meet the requirements of organizational contingencies.

9. Relationships between strategic organizational contingencies and organizational form,

culture and practices will be moderated by cultural forces. For example, in low uncertainty

avoidance cultures we expect that forces toward formalization will be weaker, and therefore the
20
relationship between such forces and organizational formalization practices will be lower. In

low power distance cultures we expect that forces toward centralization of decision making will

be weaker and therefore the relationship between such forces and decentralization and delegation

practices will be lower. We specify such moderating effects in detail below when we discuss

Phase 2 and 3 hypotheses.

10. Leader acceptance is a function of the interaction between CLTs and leader attributes

and behaviors. Accordingly, leader attributes and behaviors that are congruent with CLTs will be

more accepted than leader attributes and behaviors that are not congruent with CLTs.

11. Leader effectiveness is a function of the interaction between leader attributes and

behaviors and organizational contingencies. Leaders who effectively address organizational

contingencies will be more effective than leaders who do not.

12. Leader acceptance influences leader effectiveness. Leaders who are not accepted will

find it more difficult to influence subordinates than those who are accepted. Thus, leader

acceptance facilitates leader effectiveness.

13. Leader effectiveness influences leader acceptance. Leaders who are effective will, in

the long run, come to be accepted by all or most subordinates. Subordinates will either be

dismissed or voluntarily leave the organization led by leaders they do not accept.

In summary, the attributes and practices that distinguish cultures from each other, as well

as strategic organizational contingencies, are predictive of the leader attributes and behaviors,

and organizational practices, that are most frequently perceived as acceptable are most frequently

enacted, and are most effective.

Cultural Change. Thus far, the theory does not accommodate or account for cultural

change. For example, exposure to international media, cross-border commerce, international


21
political and economic competition, or other forms of cross-cultural interaction may introduce

new competitive forces and new common experiences, which may result in changes in any of the

culture or leadership variables described above.

Further, when strategic organizational contingencies change as a result of new

technological developments or changes in the economic or political environment, new leader

behaviors and/or organizational practices that violate cultural norms may be required. These new

behaviors and practices, when enacted, will constitute new common experiences, which can in

turn result in changes in psychological commonalties and consequently changes in any of the

cultural variables described above.

While cultural change can be stimulated by external events and forces, the process of

change is hypothesized to be governed by the set of relationships described in the above

propositions. There will almost certainly be resistance to new leadership and organizational

practices when such practices violate existing collectively shared norms and expectations for

leaders (Gagliardi, 1986). Recent work by Hanges and his colleagues on person perception

confirms this hypothesis by demonstrating that people resist new leaders when the new leader

initially behaves in a manner inconsistent with perceivers’ expectations or stereotypes (Hanges,

Braverman & Rentsch, 1991; Hanges, Lord, Day, Sipe, Smith & Brown, 1997; Sipe & Hanges,

1997). These laboratory studies have shown that the resistance to acceptance of a new leader is

so strong that the functional relationship between leadership perceptions and other variables is

nonlinear and discontinuous in nature. Various individual difference variables (e.g., personality,

stereotypical attitudes), as well as situational factors (e.g., mental workload, job-context), have

been found to increase or diminish this resistance to accept new leaders (Hanges et al., 1997;

Sipe & Hanges, 1997). This research suggests that newly introduced practices will often be
22
modified to accommodate existing norms in an emic manner, and there may be substantial

lags in the rate at which changes in the variables of the theory take place.

Substantial additional theoretical development is required to reach a better description of

the forces of resistance, the likely resulting conflicts within cultural entities, the time lags and the

feedback processes that will occur among the relationships, and the functional form of the

relationships depicted in Figure 1. Nevertheless, we advance the theory presented here as a

framework to guide investigation of the major relationships and variables relevant to cultural

influences on leadership and organizational practices and effectiveness. The theory is depicted in

Figure 1 in the form of a systems model. The complexity of the model, however, mitigates

against its being tested in its entirety. Rather, individual linkages or subsets of linkages can be

tested, and the validity of the model can be inferred from such tests. For a more detailed, fine-

grained elaboration of the relationships depicted in Figure 1, see House, Wright, and Aditya

(1997).

TWO FUNDAMENTAL CROSS-CULTURAL ISSUES

Two central aspects of cultures are frequently discussed in the cross-cultural literature:

etic aspects and emic aspects. For Project GLOBE, we employ multiple methodologies to assess

etic and emic issues.

Etic phenomena are common to all cultures, or at least to all cultures studied to date. A

phenomenon is etic if all cultures can be assessed in terms of a common metric with respect to

the phenomena. Thus cultures can be compared in terms of etic phenomena. In contrast to etic

phenomena, emic phenomena are culture specific phenomena that occur in only a subset of

cultures.
23
Etic Issues: Cross-cultural Comparisons and Relationships

Project GLOBE employs a number of different methods to make etic comparisons

among the cultures included in the GLOBE database. The primary method will be latent

constructs that measure the nine core GLOBE dimensions. These constructs are developed from

questionnaire responses obtained from middle managers and unobtrusive measures of the

dimensions. The industries studied are food processing, financial services, and

telecommunications services. Our sampling strategy for the collection of questionnaire data

controls for nation, industry, occupation broadly defined (managers), and organizational level

broadly defined (middle management). The samples for Phase II, in which hypotheses are

tested, are described later on.

Sampling from middle managers permits us to generalize to the subcultures of middle

managers in the countries and the three industries studied. This sampling strategy increases the

internal validity of the study by ensuring that the units of analysis are well defined and internally

homogeneous. We expect the findings to reflect some aspects of the national cultures of the

countries represented. Strictly speaking, we are studying the cultures of middle managers in 61

countries. Thus, we must be cautious when making generalizations about national cultures or

differences among them because of the specific nature of our sample. As we show below,

however, the use of latent constructs each composed of two indicators to measure the nine core

GLOBE As Is dimensions at the societal level, increases the generalizability of our findings

beyond the culture of middle managers alone. And cross-industry analysis will enable us to

assess the relative impact of strategic imperatives imposed by industry constraints as well as

cultural influences.
24
Emic Issues: Country-Specific Information

While the GLOBE quantitative data allow comparisons and contrasts among cultures,

they do not allow for emic, or culture-specific, descriptions of the cultures studied. CCIs will

describe selected emic attributes and entities of the national setting in which the middle

management informants are embedded. The attributes and entities are those that the CCIs judge

to have nontrivial influences on the interpretation and practice of leadership and organizational

practices of the cultures studied.

CCIs have collected unobtrusive measures and conducted participant observations. They

also collected and content-analyzed media and focus group interview transcripts. With these data

CCIs are able to describe and interpret selected emic characteristics of their national cultures.

Such interpretations will give cognizance to gender, ethnic, and religious diversity, generational

differences, and other relevant complexities of the cultures. Thus the complexity and variability

of complex cultures with two or more subcultures will be described. These qualitative

interpretive analyses will be reported as chapters of a number of anthologies resulting from the

GLOBE research program. These chapters will be authored by the local CCI teams.

CONSTRUCT DEFINITIONS

The major constructs investigated in the GLOBE research program are nine attributes of

cultures, which are operationalized as quantitative dimensions: (1) Uncertainty Avoidance, (2)

Power Distance, (3) Collectivism I: Societal Emphasis on Collectivism, (4) Collectivism II:

Family Collectivisitc Practices, (5) Gender Egalitarianism, (6) Assertiveness, (7) Future

Orientation, (8) Performance Orientation, and (9) Humane Orientation. These dimensions were

selected on the basis of a review of the literature relevant to the measurement of culture in

previous large-sample studies and on the basis of existing cross-culture theory.


25
Uncertainty Avoidance is defined as the extent to which members of an organization or

society strive to avoid uncertainty by reliance on social norms, rituals, and bureaucratic practices

to alleviate the unpredictability of future events.

Power Distance is defined as the degree to which members of an organization or society

expect and agree that power should be unequally shared.

Collectivism I reflects the degree to which organizational and societal institutional

practices encourage and reward collective distribution of resources and collective action.

Collectivism II reflects the degree to which individuals express pride, loyalty and

cohesiveness in their organizations or families.4

Gender Egalitarianism is the extent to which an organization or a society minimizes

gender role differences.

Assertiveness is the degree to which individuals in organizations or societiesare assertive,

confrontational, and aggressive in social relationships.

Future Orientation is the degree to which individuals in organizations or societies engage

in future-oriented behaviors such as planning, investing in the future, and delaying gratification.

Performance Orientation refers to the extent to which an organization or society

encourages and rewards group members for performance improvement and excellence. This

dimension is similar to the dimension called Confucian Dynamism by Hofstede and Bond

(1988).

Finally, Humane Orientation is the degree to which individuals in organizations or

societies encourage and reward individuals for being fair, altruistic, friendly, generous, caring,

and kind to others. This dimension is similar to the dimension labeled Kind Heartedness by
26
Hofstede and Bond (1988). These definitions and examples of questionnaire items for each

dimension are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE

The first six culture dimensions had their origins in the dimensions of culture identified

by Hofstede (1980). The first three scales are intended to reflect the same constructs as

Hofstede’s dimensions labeled Uncertainty Avoidance, Power Distance, and Individualism. The

Collectivism I dimension measures societal emphasis on collectivism, with low scores reflecting

individualistic emphasis and high scores reflecting collectivistic emphasis by means of laws,

social programs or institutional practices. The Collectivism II scale measures group (family

and/or organization) collectivism – pride in and loyalty to family and/or organization and

familyand/or organizational cohesiveness. In lieu of Hofstede’s Masculinity dimension, we

developed two dimensions labeled Gender Egalitarianism and Assertiveness. Future Orientation

is derived from Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck’s (1961) Past, Present, Future Orientation dimension,

which focuses on the temporal mode of a society. Performance Orientation was derived from

McClelland’s work on need for achievement. Humane Orientation has its roots in Kluckhohn and

Strodtbeck’s (1961) work on the Human Nature Is Good vs. Human Nature Is Bad dimension, as

well as Putnam’s (1993) work on the Civic Society and McClelland’s (1985) conceptualization

of the affiliative motive.

THE FOUR PHASES OF GLOBE

Four phases of empirical research are planned as part of Project GLOBE. Phase 1 has

been completed and reported in a monograph by Hanges et al. (1997, under review). Phase 2

questionnaire data collection has also been completed. The analysis of scale properties of the
27
questionnaire administered to approximately 17,000 middle managers in Phase 2 is also

completed. Unobtrusive measures for the nine dimensions have also been developed.

Phase 1: Scale Development and Validation

In this section, we describe Phases 1 and 2 in modest detail. Phase 1 of GLOBE concerned the

development and validation of the GLOBE questionnaire scales designed to measure societal and

organizational culture variables as well as CLTs. The GLOBE scales have sound psychometric

properties, and findings indicate justification for the use of the scales as aggregate measures of

cultural phenomena. All 54 GLOBE scales demonstrated significant and nontrivial within-

culture response agreement, between-culture differences, and respectable reliability of response

consistency. Generalizability coefficients, which are joint measures of these psychometric

properties, exceeds .85 for all scales. These coefficients indicate that the scales can be

meaningfully used to measure differences between cultures in terms of societal, organizational,

and leadership phenomena. In this section we provide a brief description of the questionnaire

development process. Detailed descriptions of scale development and validation can be found in

Hanges, et al. (1997, under review).

Item Generation

Item generation for the culture scales was accomplished by the first author of this

monograph with substantial help from Paul Koopman, Henk Thierry, and Celeste Wilderom of

the Netherlands, and Phillip Podsakoff of the United States. The original item pool contained 753

items, of which 382 were leadership items and 371 were societal and organizational culture

items.

In generating leadership items, our focus was on developing a comprehensive list of

leader attributes and behaviors rather than on developing a priori leadership scales. The initial
28
pool of leadership items was based on leader behaviors and attributes described in several

extant leadership theories. The theories are described in House and Aditya (1997). These

leadership items consisted of behavioral and attribute descriptors. Examples of these items are

presented in Table 3. Items were rated on a seven point Likert-type scale that ranged from a low

of “This behavior or characteristic greatly inhibits a person from being an outstanding leader” to

a high of “This behavior or characteristic contributes greatly to a person being an outstanding

leader.”

TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE

Organizational and societal culture items were written for the nine core GLOBE

dimensions, described above, at both the societal and the organizational levels. We also wrote

the items to reflect two culture manifestations: institutional practices reported “As Is” and values

reported in terms of what “Should Be.” The items were written as “quartets” having isomorphic

structures across the two levels of analysis (societal and organizational) and across the two

culture manifestations (As Is and Should Be).

The basic structure of the items comprising quartets is identical, but the frame of

reference is varied according to the particular cultural manifestation and levels of analysis being

assessed. Table 4 contains an example of a quartet of parallel culture items, showing essentially

the same question in four forms: Organization As Is; Organization Should Be; Society As Is; and

Society Should Be. Items were derived from a review of relevant literature and interviews and

focus groups held in several countries, as well as from extant organizational and culture theory.

Psychometric analyses indicated justification for grouping the items into scales relevant to nine

core GLOBE dimensions of societies and organizations.

TABLE 4 ABOUT HERE


29
Item Screening

Societal and organizational culture items were screened for appropriateness by use of

three procedures: Q sorting, item evaluation, and translation/back translation. Leadership items

were screened by item evaluation and conceptual equivalence of the back translation. The Q

sorting procedure consisted of sorting the culture items into theoretical categories represented by

the a priori dimensions of culture described above, first by seven Ph.D. students in the

Department of Psychology at the University of Maryland and subsequently by CCIs representing

38 countries. The sorters were not informed of the theoretical dimensions for which the items

were intended. Items that were sorted by 80% of the sorters into the categories for which they

were theoretically intended were retained for further analysis. There were no dimensions of

societal or organizational culture for which a majority of items failed to meet this criterion. Thus

a sufficient number of items was retained for the measurement of each dimension.

The ability of the sorters to agree on the allocation of items to dimensions indicates that

the sorters were sorting according to common interpretations of both the theoretical dimensions

and the items that they sorted into these dimensions. This level of agreement indicates that the

scales comprising the retained items were interpreted to have the same meaning in all of the

cultures represented by the CCIs. This is an especially important result because it strongly

suggests commonalty of meaning of the questionnaire scales across cultures.

In addition to this sorting task, CCIs provided Item Evaluation Reports, in which they

noted any items containing words or phrases that were ambiguous or could not be adequately

translated in the target country’s native language. CCIs also identified questions that might be

culturally inappropriate. Most of the items that were problematic were dropped from further
30
consideration. In a few cases, we were able to rewrite items to eliminate potential problems

but retain the intent and dimensionality of the original item.

In order to avoid any systematic bias that may be present when respondents complete a

survey that is not in their native language (Brislin, 1986), CCIs were responsible for having the

survey translated from English into their native language. This was done by the CCI, by some

other person fluent in both languages, or by a professional translator. The translation was then

independently translated again, from the native language of the culture back to English. This

back-translation was then sent to the GLOBE Coordinating Team (GCT), where it was compared

to the original English version of the survey to verify the veridicality of the translation. Through

the process of deleting items based on sorting, item evaluation, and translation, the item pool was

reduced to a total of 379 items, which were retained for further evaluation.

Pilot Studies

Two pilot studies were then conducted to assess the psychometric properties of the

resulting a priori culture scales and to empirically develop leadership scales.

Pilot Study 1. The CCIs in 28 countries distributed the survey of retained items to

individuals in their respective countries who had full-time working experience as a white-collar

employee or manager. Because the survey was lengthy, it was divided into two parallel versions,

A and B. Each version contained approximately half of the leadership items and half of the

organizational and societal culture items. A total of 877 individuals completed the first pilot

study survey.

Several different statistical analyses were performed to assess the psychometric

properties of the scales. Specifically, we conducted a series of exploratory factor analyses,

reliability analyses, and aggregability analyses (e.g., rwg analyses, intraclass correlations [ICC-
31
1], one-way analyses of variance) , and generalizability analyses (ICC-2) of the scales. These

analyses were performed at the ecological level of analysis, i.e., on the means of the country item

responses for each scale. We refined our scales on the basis of these analyses while trying to

maintain, whenever possible, the isomorphic quartet structure of the culture scales described

above and illustrated in Table 4. These statistical analyses, when considered together, provide

useful information about the construct validity of the culture scales.

A separate factor analysis of each of the culture scales indicated that they were all

unidimensional. A first-order exploratory factor analysis of the leader attributes items yielded 16

unidimensional factors that describe specific leader attributes and behaviors.

The rwg analyses (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984), demonstrated that the scales can be

aggregated to either the organizational or the societal levels of analysis (average rwg = .73, n =

54). Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC -1) and one-way analyses of variance for each of the

scales indicated statistically significant within-culture agreement and between-culture

differences. The societal culture scales exhibited low to moderate correlations with each other.

Thus, they provide independent and unique information about societal cultures. The leadership

scales substantially differed in their relationship to one another. The absolute correlation among

the first-order leadership scales ranged from a low of .00 (e.g., Status-Conscious with Calmness)

to a high of .86 (Status-Conscious with Procedural). Thus the leadership scales exhibited

acceptable levels of unidimensionality and internal consistency. Overall, 20 percent of the

interrelationships were statistically significant. A second-order factor analysis of the 16

leadership factors yielded 4 unidimensional factors that describe classes of leader behaviors that

represent global leader behavior patterns.


32
It is interesting to note that some of the same culture dimensions were highly inversely

correlated across the two As Is and Should Be scale orientations for particular dimensions of

culture. The findings indicate that there are substantial differences in people’s perceptions of

how things should be as opposed to people’s perceptions of how things are perceived to be. This

raises an interesting and very important question: Are the most meaningful indicators of the

cultures of collectives current practices reflected by As Is scales or values reflected by Should Be

scales? We shall assess the relationship between (a) societal As Is and Should Be scores and (b)

organizational As Is scores. The findings from this analysis will indicate which of the two ways

of measuring cultural variation is most strongly associated with organizational practices as

measured by the organizational As Is scores.

We ended Pilot Study 1 with 16 first-order factorially derived leadership scales that

represent specific leader behaviors, 4 second-order factorially derived leadership dimensions that

represent global leader behavior patterns, nine organizational culture As Is scales, nine

organizational culture Should Be scales. The factor analyses conducted as part of Pilot Study 1

also demonstrated that the shared themes in all of the scales can be meaningfully identified and

labeled.

Pilot Study 2. The purpose of the second pilot study was to replicate the psychometric

analyses of the scales in a different sample to assess sampling robustness. Data for this study

came from 15 countries that did not participate in the previous pilot study. In general, the

psychometric properties of all of the scales were confirmed by replication. We replicated the

ecological analyses conducted in Pilot Study 1 at the individual level of analysis. We used this

level of analysis because there were too few countries in the replication sample to conduct an

ecological-factor analysis. A total of 1,066 individuals completed one of the two versions of the
33
pilot study questionnaires. Using an individual-level analysis to replicate an ecological-level

analysis is a conservative approach. If this analysis is found to correspond to the ecological

analysis, the findings constitute strong evidence for the generality of the factor structure and

evidence of strong etic phenomena (Leung & Bond, 1989). Pilot Study 2 confirmatory factor

analyses yielded acceptable fit for the first- and second-order CLT factor structures and

replicated the unidimensionality of the societal culture scales.

In summary, we developed 16 unidimensional leadership scales and 36 societal scales

that exhibit acceptable levels of internal consistency. The aggregation tests indicated that we are

justified in aggregating these scales to the societal level of analysis. Correlational analysis

indicated that the leadership scales substantially differed in their relationship to one another. The

leadership scales also exhibited acceptable levels of unidimensionality and internal consistency.

We found sufficient agreement within societies and sufficient differences between societies to

aggregate the scales to the society level of analysis. Further, the shared themes in all of the scales

were replicated by the Pilot Study 2 factor analyses.

Phase 2: Measurement and Hypothesis Testing

Phase 2 consists of assessment of scale properties and measurement of (a) the core

societal and organizational As Is and Should Be dimensions, (b) the CLT dimensions, (c) the

organizational contingencies in firms represented by middle mangement respondents, and (d)

respondent demographic variables. Phase 2 also consists of tests of theoretical hypotheses

presented below. Data collection for these aspects of Phase 2 is complete. In addition, Phase 2

involves the development of unobtrusive measurement scales to assess the societal-level cultural

dimensions. These scales are described below.


34
The questionnaire data collected in GLOBE Phase 2 consist of (a) responses to

approximately 17,000 questionnaires from middle managers of approximately 825 organizations

in 61 countries, relevant to societal and organizational dimensions of culture, (b) unobtrusive

measures of the societal dimensions (Table 5) and responses to four different executive

questionnaires administered to separate top-level executives in the organizations from which the

middle management data were collected. The executive questionnaires, described below, elicited

responses relevant to organizational attributes, organizational contingencies, and performance.

These responses will be used in Phase 2 to test hypotheses relevant to structural contingency

theory of organizational form and effectiveness.

TABLE 5 ABOUT HERE

Questionnaire data collection for Phase 2 is complete. Table 6 provides a summary and

overview of the latent constructs used to test the GLOBE hypotheses and their indicators.

TABLES 6 ABOUT HERE

Based on the pilot studies and on focus groups and interviews conducted by CCIs, which

were ongoing during the pilot studies, several additional CLT items were added to the middle

manager questionnaires. These new items were written to ensure that the 16 leadership scales

were not biased by including only western leadership behaviors. Further, we wrote several items

that described autocratic, narcissistic, manipulative, and punitive behaviors, because it was

suggested in interviews and focus groups that in some societies such behaviors would enhance

leader effectiveness.

Finally, we added several new items to the survey to develop a second measure of

collectivist cultural orientation because the collectivistic scale derived from the pilot studies did

not include items relevant to family collectivism. The new items were adopted from Triandis’
35
work on collectivism (Triandis, 1995) and concerned several descriptors of family practices

usually associated with collectivistic cultures: family pride, loyalty and cohesiveness. We

confirmed the two Collectivism scales by factor analytically deriving two dimensions of

Collectivism: Societal Emphasis on Collective Behavior and Family Collectivism. Thus, there

were nine scales to measure the nine culture as is and nine should be culture dimensions--one

scale for each of the dimensions except Collectivism, which had two scales.

Measurement of Organizational Attributes

The executive questionnaires referred to above were designed to measure several

attributes and processes of the organizations from which middle manager data were collected.

The questionnaire included scales to measure attributes of strategies, perceived organizational

effectiveness and three organizational strategic contingency variables: size in terms of number of

employees; the dominant technology of each organization in terms of the degree to which the

work is repetitive, well understood, and controllable; environments in terms of competitiveness,

hostility, and predictability/uncertainty. Organizational strategy was assessed in terms of strong

versus weak customer orientation, incremental versus comprehensiveness of strategic decision

making, consensus versus individual formulation, formality versus informality, adaptability

versus rigidity and entrepreneurial/risk orientation. The strategic contingency scales were

adapted from questionnaires used in several previous studies in which their construct validity

was established (Lumpkin & Dess, 1996; Khandwalla, 1977). The executives reported the level

of their firms’ performance, relative to major competitors, during the previous five years with

respect to sales and pretax profit. The executives also provided information relevant to the

general market and economic conditions of the firms, frequency of organizational changes,

degree of government regulation of firm activities, and demographic variables relevant to the
36
firm and to themselves. The psychometric properties of these scales and the impact of the

strategic contingency variables on organizational practices and effectiveness remain to be

assessed as part of the Phase 2 research.

Phase 2 Hypotheses

The results of Pilot Studies 1 and 2 set the stage for Phase 2 by providing the necessary

questionnaire scales to test hypotheses. Hypotheses will be tested, concerning (a) relationships

between societal culture dimensions, organizational culture dimensions, and CLTs, (b)

relationships specified by structural contingency theory of organizational form and effectiveness,

and (c) the moderating effects of societal culture dimensions on relationships specified by

structural contingency theory. Tests of the first two GLOBE hypotheses are reported below.

While we do not report tests of the remaining GLOBE hypotheses, we present these hypotheses

here.

Hypotheses Concerning Relationships Between Societal Culture Dimensions,

Organizational Culture Dimensions and CLTs.

Hypothesis 1: The global CLT dimension charismatic/value-based leadership will be

universally endorsed.

The component subscales that constitute the global (second order factor)

charismatic/value-based leadership dimension are visionary, inspirational, self-sacrifice,

integrity, decisive, and performance orientation. We expect charismatic/value-based leader

behavior to be universally endorsed because the visions articulated by, and the integrity enacted

by, value-based leaders stress values that have universal appeal (House, Wright, & Aditya,

1997). Charismatic/value-based leaders articulate and emphasize end-values. Examples of end-

values are dignity, peace, order, beauty, and freedom. End-values are values that are intrinsically
37
motivating, self-sufficient, and need not be linked to other values. Also, end-values are not

exchangeable for other values and have universal appeal (Rokeach, 1973). Thus, the values

stressed by charismatic/value-based leaders are more likely to be universally accepted and

endorsed. Consequently the visions of charismatic/value-based leaders usually stress end-values

that are congruent with the values stressed in the culture (House & Aditya, 1997). Similarly, we

expect leader integrity to be universally endorsed because integrity is an end-value that is also

universally held in all cultures. (Rokeach, 1973)

We recognize that Hypothesis 1 is controversial. Bass (1997) argues that transformational

leadership, a form of charismatic/value-based leadership, is universally acceptable and effective.

In contrast, it may be argued that some cultures may more highly value leaders who can find

pragmatic accommodations with all influential parties. In such cultures, value-based leadership

may be far less important than ability to achieve pragmatic effects, regardless of the means by

which such effects are attained.

Regardless of whether it is supported or not, the test of Hypothesis 1 is of both theoretical

and practical interest. Failure to support this hypothesis would result in identification of the

specific cultures in which value-based leadership is and is not endorsed. Thus, the issue of

universal endorsement of leadership dimensions, of necessity, needs to be answered on the basis

of empirical evidence. The test of Hypothesis 1 is intended to contribute to clarification of that

issue. The discovery of both universally endorsed and culture-specific leadership dimensions is

of major importance to the development of cross-cultural leadership theory and of practical

importance to individuals whose work involves cross-cultural interaction.

Hypothesis 2: There will be positive correlations between societal dimensions and CLT

dimensions.
38
The rationale for this hypothesis is that the dimensions of societal culture will

influence the legitimacy and acceptance of leader behaviors . More specifically, societal culture

will influence the kind of attributes and behaviors that are reported to be expected, acceptable

and effective. Further, dimensions of organizational culture are also expected to influence the

legitimacy and acceptance of leader attributes and behaviors.

Hypothesis 3: There will be positive correlations between organizational culture

dimensions and CLTs.

The rationale for this hypothesis is that shared organizational values and practices will

influence the legitimacy and acceptance of leader attributes and behaviors.

Correlations between these dimensions constitute a test of the cultural influence

proposition (Hofstede, 1980; Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961; Triandis, 1995), which asserts that

societal culture has a pervasive influence on the values, expectations, and behavior of its

members. Therefore societal culture is expected to influence organizational values (Should Be

measures) and practices (As Is measures), as well as expectations for leader behaviors that are

expressed as CLTs in the form of questionnaire item responses.Hypothesis 4: The magnitude of

relationships between organizational cultural dimensions and isomorphic CLT dimensions will

be greater than the magnitude of relationships between societal culture dimensions and

isomorphic CLT dimensions.

The rationale for this hypothesis is that organizational variables are more salient, more

proximate, and more relevant to the tasks and behaviors of managers than societal cultural

variables. Therefore organizational cultural variables will have a stronger influence on CLTs

than societal cultural variables. Correlations between organizational culture dimensions and CLT

dimensions constitute a test of the organizational influence proposition.


39
The cultural influence proposition asserts that societal culture has a pervasive influence

on values, expectations and behavior and will therefore influence CLTs. Correlations between

societal culture dimensions and CLT dimensions constitute a test of the cultural influence

proposition. Thus, comparisons of the regression coefficients of the relationships between

societal culture dimensions and CLTs with the regression coefficients of the

relaitionshipsbetween organizational culture dimensions and CLTs constitute competitive tests of

the cultural influence and the organizational influence propositions.

Hypothesis 5: Relative to organizations in the food-processing industry, organizations in

the financial-services industry will have higher scores on the organizational cultural dimensions

of gender egalitarianism, humanism, and future orientation, and lower scores on power distance
5
practices. The rationale for this hypothesis is that financial institutions need to be employee-

and customer-service-oriented, future-oriented, and flexible in order to compete. Customer

satisfaction depends on the degree to which financial institutions treat customers individually,

design their services to meet customer preferences, and make investments that protect or enhance

the future value of client and organizational assets. Such practices require employees with

relatively high levels of education. Thus, financial institutions need to (a) minimize employee

turnover to retain well-educated employees who would be costly to replace, and (b) maintain

stability with clients. Therefore financial institutions need to adopt a more humane orientation

toward their employees, more future orientation toward their clients and less centralization of

decision-making in comparison with the food processing organizations.

Hypotheses Concerning Relationships Specified by Structural Contingency Theory

and the Moderating Effects of Societal Cultural Dimensions.


40
In addition to the research described above, we will also conduct tests of structural

contingency theory of organizational form and effectiveness (Donaldson, 1993; Hickson et al.,

1974), and tests relevant to the effects of culture on relationships between organizational

contingencies, practices, and effectiveness.

Hypothesis 6 : Organizational contingency variables will be associated with relevant

organizational practices as specified by Structural Contingency Theory (Hickson et al., 1974).

However, these correlations will be moderated by societal dimensions of culture that are

isomorphic with the contingency variables. The rationale for this hypothesis is that organizations

are expected to have a tendency to align their practices with both strategic contingency variables

and the cultural forces of the society in which they function. More specifically

Hypothesis 6a: Organizational size will be positively correlated with organizational

formalization. Formalization is manifested by uncertainty avoidance practices such as

establishment and enforcement of rules, procedures, and regulations.

Hypothesis 6b: Hypothesis 6a will be more strongly supported in high uncertainty

avoidance societies.

Hypothesis 6c: Technological uncertainty will be negatively correlated with

organizational uncertainty avoidance practices at the lower levels of organizations.

Hypothesis 6d: Hypothesis 6c will be more strongly supported in low uncertainty

avoidance societies.

Hypothesis 6e: Environmental uncertainty and hostility will be positively correlated with

organizational power distance and uncertainty avoidance practices. Under conditions of

environmental uncertainty and hostility organizations are predicted to centralize and closely

control decision making (Aldrich, 1979; Staw, Sandelands, & Dutton, 1981). This centralization
41
and control are expected to be manifested in increased organizational power distance and

uncertainty avoidance practices.

Hypothesis 6f : Hypothesis 6e will be more strongly supported in high uncertainty

avoidance and high power distance societies.

Hypothesis 6g: Future-oriented organizational strategies will be positively correlated with

future orientation of organizational cultures and the future-oriented CLT dimension.

Hypothesis 6h: Hypothesis 6g will be more strongly supported in future-oriented

societies.

Hypothesis 6i: Entrepreneurial orientation of organizational strategies will be positively

correlated with the assertiveness and performance orientation dimensions of organizational

cultures and the performance-oriented CLT dimension.

Hypothesis 6j: Hypothesis 6i will be more strongly supported in assertive and

performance-oriented societies.

Hypothesis 6k: Consensually based strategy formulation practices described by high-

level managers in response to questionnaires will be negatively correlated with middle manager

endorsement of participative leadership and with organizational power distance

practices.Hypothesis 6l: Hypothesis 6k will be more strongly supported in low power distance

societies.

Hypothesis 6m: Flexible organic strategy formulation practices will be associated with

organic organizational cultures. Organic organizational cultures are manifested by low

organizational power distance and low uncertainty avoidance practices (Dickson, 1997).

Hypothesis 6n: Hypothesis 5m will be more strongly supported in low power distance

and low uncertainty avoidance societies.


42
Hypothesis 7: The higher the correspondence between structural contingencies,

societal culture dimensions, and organizational culture dimensions specified in Hypotheses 5a -

5n, the higher the economic performance of the organizations studied. The rationale for this

hypothesis is that organizations that practice alignment of strategy, organizational culture and

practices, and societal culture will be most effective.

Samples

National borders may not be an adequate way to demarcate cultural boundaries, since

many countries have large subcultures. It is impossible to obtain representative samples of such

multicultural nations as China, India, or the United States. Nonetheless, the samples drawn from

such countries need to be comparable with respect to the dominant forces that shape cultures,

such as ecological factors, history, language, and religion. The country samples also need to be

relatively homogeneous within cultures. For multicultural countries, whenever possible we

sampled the subculture in which there is the greatest amount of commercial activity. When

possible we also sampled more than one subculture (indigenous and Caucasian subcultures in

South Africa, French and German subcultures in Switzerland, and East and West subcultures in

Germany).

The units of analysis for the GLOBE project consisted of cultural level aggregated

responses of samples of typical middle managers in three industries: food processing, financial

services, and telecommunications services. The food-processing industry is a relatively stable

industry. The telecommunications and financial industries may be stable or unstable, depending

on country and economic conditions. By including these industries, we have obtained a fair

number of dynamic industries and high-technology industries in the overall sample.


43
Cultures in at least three countries in each of the following geographic regions are

represented in the GLOBE sample: Africa, Asia, Europe (Eastern, Central, and Northern), Latin

America, North America, North Africa, Middle East, and the Pacific Rim, as indicated in Table 1.

TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE

Middle managers in these industries were asked to use a seven-point scale to describe

leader attributes and behaviors that they perceive to enhance or impede outstanding leadership.

They were also asked to give their perceptions of the practices and values (in the form of As Is

and Should Be responses, respectively) in the society in which they live, and of the organizations

in which they are employed, using seven-point scales as illustrated in Table 4. Independent

samples of middle managers completed one of two questionnaires. Half of the respondents in

each culture completed the societal culture questionnaire (Sample 1), and the other half

completed organizational culture questionnaire (Sample 2). All respondents completed the

Leadership Attributes Questionnaire. Thus, the societal culture and the organizational culture

questionnaires were completed by independent samples of respondents.

In addition, CCIs collected qualitative information about their societies and organizations

in the industries they studied with respect to etic and emic dimensions of their cultures. Middle

managers participated in interviews and focus groups and completed questionnaires. CCIs have

also recorded archival information and participant observations, and have collected unobtrusive

measures to be used to describe and interpret the cultures studied. CCIs also conducted content

analyses of the dominant general and business media in their cultures. In this capacity, they

provided both etic and emic information concerning the study dimensions described above.

Our design strategy consisted of obtaining responses of middle managers in two of the
6
three target industries in each country studied. This yielded samples from approximately 40
44
countries in each of the target industries. As stated above, data relevant to 54 countries were

available at the time this paper was written.

The sample design also permitted us to relate the within-culture mean dimension

responses to these two questionnaires to the within-culture mean dimension responses to the

Leader Attribute Questionnaire. The means of the leadership item responses of Sample 1 and

Sample 2 within each country were not significantly different. Thus the individual leadership

scale scores for the two samples were averaged to produce means on the leadership scales for all

cultures. As a result of the independent assessment of the organizational and societal variables,

and because the mean CLT responses in each sample in each culture were not different, the

responses are free of common source response bias.


7
Unobtrusive Measures

All instruments are subject to potential unknown biases. One procedure for minimizing

response bias contamination is to use multiple methodologies to measure the same constructs.

Measuring a construct with multiple methodologies permits verification of the measurement of

cultures on the latent construct of interest by triangulation. Latent construct measurement based

on two or more manifest indicators allows one to reduce, if not eliminate, potential response bias

associated with questionnaire responses. The latent constructs used to measure societal level

responses consist of questionnaire responses and unobtrusive measures. As noted above,

examples of unobtrusive measures are shown in Table 5.

The intercorrelations of the unobtrusive measures and the core As Is questionnaire scale

scores for each dimension range from .37 to .73 (all significant, p < .03). These intercorrelations

indicate validity of the GLOBE societal As Is questionnaire measures. They also indicate that the
45
middle manager responses to the societal questionnaire reflect the broader society in which the

managers are embedded and not a more narrowly defined culture of middle managers.

Questionnaire Response Bias

Triandis (1995) has noted that the various cultures have different response patterns when

responding to questionnaires. The presence of these different response patterns can bias cross-

cultural comparisons. Thus several different statistical techniques have been developed to

eliminate the contamination of survey responses. Using latent constructs composed of

questionnaire response scores and unobstrusive measurement scales lessens the effects of

questionnaire response bias on the between-country comparisons.9

Phase 2 CLT Scales

One of the objectives of GLOBE is to determine whether there are dimensions of CLTs

that are universally endorsed and dimensions that are differentially endorsed across cultures.

Recall that CLTs are culturally endorsed profiles of perceived effective or ineffective leader

attributes or behaviors about which members within each culture agree. Profiles of CLT

dimensions reflect what is commonly referred to as “leadership styles” in the leadership

literature.

Shaw (1990) suggests that much of the cross-national literature indicating differences in

managerial beliefs, values, and styles can be interpreted as showing culturally influenced

differences in leader prototypes, which are analogous to CLTs as conceptualized for Project

GLOBE. A study by O’Connell, Lord, and O’Connell (1990) supports the argument that culture

plays a strong role in influencing the content of leader attributes and behaviors perceived as

desirable and effective. Their study specifically examined the similarities and differences

between Japanese and American CLTs. For the Japanese, the traits of being fair, flexible, a good
46
listener, outgoing, and responsible were highly rated in many domains, such as business,

media, and education. For Americans, traits of intelligence, honesty, understanding, verbal skills,

and determination were strongly endorsed as facilitating leader effectiveness in numerous

domains. A study by Gerstner and Day (1994) also provides additional evidence that ratings of

leadership attributes and behaviors vary across cultures. These investigators identified three

dimensions relevant to distinct CLTs as expressed by university students from eight nations.

These dimensions had rank order correlations with Hofstede’s (1980) measures of power

distance, uncertainty avoidance, and individualism of .81, 1.00, and .70, respectively. The

GLOBE research project follows in the tradition of these studies. Following is a brief description

of the development of the final Leader Attribute Questionnaire to identify CLTs.

Using the means of Phase 2 Leader Attribute Questionnaire subscales from 54 countries,

we performed multilevel confirmatory factor analysis to confirm the 16-dimension factor

structure of the leadership scales developed in the two pilot studies. This factor structure was

confirmed. However, Phase 2 research included several additional items not included in the pilot

study questionnaires that reflected the findings from ongoing interview and focus group

research. We conducted a maximum likelihood exploratory factor analysis with a varimax

rotation of these CLT items. This analysis resulted in 5 additional CLT subscales which

exhibited sound psychometric properties. Thus, we have a total of 21 leadership subscales for

Phase 2 analysis. These subscales and sample items are presented in Table 7.

TABLE 7 ABOUT HERE

As with the pilot data, our analysis revealed significant interrelationships among the

factors, hence the need to create a second-order factor structure. A second-order factor analysis

produced four factors: (1) Charismatic/Value-Based Leadership that is Team-Oriented, (2)


47
Autonomous Leadership, (3) Humane Leadership, and (4) Non-Participative Self-Protective

Leadership. Guided by prevailing theory, we divided Factor 1 into Charismatic/Value-Based

Leadership and Team-Oriented Leadership to create two dimensions. We also divided Factor 4

into two dimensions: Self-Protective Leadership and Participative Leadership (the scores of the

nonparticipative subscales were reversed to reflect participative leadership). These divisions of

the empirically derived second-order factors were made to preserve conceptual clarity and to

have dimensions that can be related to prevailing leadership theory and previous empirical

studies.

The 21 subscales are grouped into six higher-order leader behavior/attribute dimensions,

which are presented in Table 8. As stated above, we refer to the higher-order dimensions as

global CLT dimensions because they represent classes of leader behavior rather than specific

leader behaviors. We refer to the 21 first-order factors as CLT subscales. These subscales

measure more-specific leader attributes and behaviors. Composite profiles of the six CLT

dimensions represent what is generally referred to as leadership styles.

TABLE 8 ABOUT HERE

Test of Hypothesis 1

Hypothesis 1 states that charismatic/value-based leadership and integrity attributes will

be universally endorsed as contributors to outstanding leadership. From Table 8, it can be seen

that the global CLT charismatic/value-based leadership dimension had culture scores ranging

from 4.5 to 6.5 on the 7-point response scale, thus indicating positive endorsement by all

cultures.

To test Hypothesis 1 more rigorously, we established the following criteria for Leader

Attribute Questionnaire items to be considered universally endorsed as contributors to


48
outstanding leadership: (a) 95% of country scores had to exceed a mean of 5 on a 7-point scale

for that attribute and (b) the world wide grand mean score for all countries had to exceed 6 for

the attribute. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 9. Three of the positively

endorsed items concern aspects of integrity. Note that most of the other universal positively

endorsed items are components of the first-order Charismatic/Value-Based Leadership and

Team-Oriented dimensions. The portrait of a leader who is universally viewed as effective is

clear: the person should exhibit the integrity and charismatic qualities listed in Table 9 and build

effective teams. Thus, Hypothesis 1 is strongly supported.

TABLE 9 ABOUT HERE

Universal Impediments to Leadership Effectiveness

The criteria for specific attributes, measured at the item level, to be considered

universally viewed as impediments to effective leadership required that (a) an item grand mean

for all countries be less than 3 and (b) 95 percent of country scores on the item be less than 3.

These combined criteria indicate that the attribute was universally perceived as inhibiting

outstanding leadership. Results are presented in Table 10, which shows that many items that

made up the first-order subscales labeled Self-Protective, Malevolent and Face Saver are

universally viewed as ineffective by the international sample of middle managers.

TABLE 10 ABOUT HERE

Culturally Contingent Endorsement of Leader Attributes

Most interesting, from a cross-cultural viewpoint, are the attributes that in some countries

were considered to enhance outstanding leadership and in other countries were considered to

impede outstanding leadership. We present in Table 11 those attributes (items) that yielded

scores above and below the scale midpoint of 4, contingent on country-specific responses. Many
49
of these attributes fell into the leadership subscales entitled Self-Centered and Individualistic.

For instance, while the attribute Individualistic had a grand country mean of 3.11 (slightly

inhibits outstanding leadership), individual country scores ranged from a low of 1.67 (moderately

impedes) to a high of 5.10 (moderately contributes). Similarly, the item Status Conscious ranged

in value from a low of 1.92 (moderately impedes) to a high of 5.77 (moderately contributes).

Even more striking was the Risk Taking item, which is a component of the Charismatic/Value-

Based second-order factor. Risk Taking ranges in value from a 2.14 (moderately impedes) to a

5.96 (moderately contributes).

TABLE 11 ABOUT HERE

These findings raise several important questions. For instance, if some attributes (items)

and some global CLT dimensions are differentially endorsed among nations, as indicated by our

analyses thus far, are they equally compelling and influential? What psychological and

sociological processes link the CLT dimensions to dominant cultural values? Are CLT

dimensions more rigidly set for homogeneous societies, such as Japan, than for culturally diverse

societies, such as the United States?

Test of Hypothesis 2

The “sine qua non” of the GLOBE project concerns the link between culture and

leadership. While all of the analysis addressing this issue has not been completed, we do have

positive preliminary findings. Hypothesis 2 states that there will be significant positive

relationships between isomorphic societal culture dimensions and CLT dimensions.

TABLE 12 ABOUT HERE


50
Table 12 shows our predictions concerning the specific cultural dimensions that should

predict cross-cultural differences in perceived effective leadership styles. We generated these

hypotheses by examining each first order leadership scale and identifying the societal and

organizational cultural dimensions which are isomorphic with each second order leadership

dimension (for example, perceived effectiveness of Charismatic/Value-Based leadership was

expected to be associated with the societal and organizational dimensions entitled Performance,

Future, and Humane Orientation Table 12 shows these hypotheses

We tested Hypothesis 2 by using hierarchical linear modeling, a procedure that allows

one to identify the amount of variance in a dependent variable that is accounted for by

organizations as well as the societies in which the organizations function. The total amount of

variance of CLTs accounted for is thus a joint function of the societal-level variables and the

organizational-level variables in the societies in which the organizations are nested. Table 13

reveals that the endorsement of CLT global leader behavior dimensions are associated only with

respondent value orientation (i.e., Should Be responses), and not with observed practices (i.e., As

Is responses). The statistically significant relationships between societal and organizational

culture dimensions and CLT dimensions presented in Table 13 are as follows:

TABLE 13 ABOUT HERE

1. Approximately 11 percent of the total variance in team-oriented leadership

endorsement is accounted for by organizational collective value orientation and by the humane

and collective value orientations of societies in which these organizations reside. It is important

to realize, however, that some of the variance in team-oriented leadership is likely attributable to

levels of analysis in which we are not presently interested (e.g., industry, individual levels).

When only the portions of variance that are of direct interest were examined, the organizational
51
variable accounted for 37 percent of the variance in endorsement of all team-oriented

leadership that occurred at the organizational level of analysis. The two society variables

accounted for 32 percent of the endorsement of team-oriented leadership variance that occurred

at the society level of analysis.

2. Approximately 27 percent of the total variance in participative leadership endorsement

is accounted for by organizational-level power distance and uncertainty avoidance value

orientation and by four societal-level variables: uncertainty avoidance, power distance, humane

orientation, and assertiveness value orientations. The two organizational-level variables

accounted for approximately 30 percent, and the societal-level variables accounted for

approximately 82 percent of the variance in endorsement of participative leadership that occurred

at their respective levels.

3. Approximately 7 percent of the total variance in endorsement of humane-oriented

leadership is accounted for by organizational-level and societal-level humane value orientation.

The organizational-level variable accounted for approximately 21 percent, and the societal-level

variable accounted for approximately 32 percent, respectively, of the variance in endorsement of

participative leadership that occurred at their respective levels.

4. Approximately 12 percent of the total variance in endorsement of charismatic/value-

based leadership is accounted for by organizational-level and societal-level value placed on

performance orientation. The organizational-level variable accounted for approximately 41

percent, and the societal-level variable accounted for approximately 14 percent, respectively, of

the variance in endorsement of charismatic/value-based leadership that occurred at their

respective levels.
52
5. The variance in endorsement of self-protective and autonomous leadership

accounted for by organizational-level and societal-level variables is negligible and not reported

in Table 13. This finding indicates that the variance in these two types of CLT endorsement is

likely attributable to industry or individual differences, or other unmeasured situational variables.

Hypothesis 2 is supported with respect to endorsement of the above four global leader

dimensions. These findings show that both societal and organizational cultural variables have

nontrivial influences on CLTs and explain in part why there is variance across cultures with

respect to what is expected of leaders and the influence and privileges they are granted.

Country-Level Uses of the Middle Manager Data

A profile shall be constructed for each nation consisting of the societal, organizational,

and global CLT scores. In essence, for each culture CCIs shall construct a quantitative

description of the attributes perceived as facilitating or impeding outstanding leadership, the

culturally endorsed values (Should Be responses), and the common practices (As Is responses) in

the societies and organizations studied. The CCIs can then interpret the results and compare the

data from their culture to the data relevant to all other cultures. Fourteen of these interpretations

have been included as part of the culture-specific chapters of the first of several GLOBE

anthologies. The content of the anthologies is described below. Upon completion of Phase 2, we

shall have profiles of the dominantly endorsed leader behaviors and attributes and of the societal

and organizational dimensions of each culture. We shall also have substantially greater

knowledge concerning cultural and organizational influences on endorsed leader attributes in the

dominant cultures or subcultures of the 61 countries studied. This information shall be based on

both the quantitative and the qualitative findings, and when published, it shall have substantial
53
practical value for leaders who practice management in the cultures studied or deal with

individuals of these cultures.

In-Depth Country Specific Descriptions of Cultures

Country-specific qualitative research by the CCIs has been ongoing from the beginning

of Phase 1 and will continue to the final phase of the project. Many of the CCI teams will write a

qualitative description of the major cultural variables that are relevant to leadership and

organizational practices in their particular culture. The in-depth description of each culture will

incorporate the following topics: (a) an overall description of the culture in terms of its political

and economic system and the major historical forces and leaders that have shaped that system;

(b) a brief description of prevailing organizational practices in the industries and organizations

studied by the CCIs in terms of the constructs that underlie the core study dimension (c) a

description of the emic (culture-specific) manifestations of the core dimensions of the study at

the societal, organizational, and leader levels of analysis; (d) a description of other emic

characteristics of the society, industries studied, and leadership practices within these industries

that have nontrivial implications for the practice of leadership and organizations; (e) the culture-

specific semantic interpretation of the concept of leadership--what it means, the role and status of

leaders in the culture, leadership functions, privileges, responsibilities, and the like; (f)

identification of qualitative unobtrusive indicators of the importance assigned to leadership or

leaders based on CCI participant observation; (g) an interpretation of the quantitative dimensions

relevant to their cultures and in relation to other cultures; (h) an interpretative discussion of the

kinds of leadership behavior required for effective leadership in the industries under

investigation; and (i) prescriptive implications.


54
In essence, the CCIs will write a qualitative analysis of major variables relevant to

leadership and organizational practices in the industries studied. The qualitative description and

interpretation will be based on CCI participant observation, unobtrusive measures, and content

analyses of media, interviews, and focus group discussions. CCIs have been provided with a set

of self-instruction guides to ensure at least a moderate level of uniformity and quality of the

qualitative research.

The completed chapters will be based on the combination of the quantitative survey data

and the qualitative research findings produced by CCIs. An interpretive analysis of all of the

findings will then be possible. It is hoped that this interpretation will lead to the development of a

cross-cultural theory of leadership and organizational practices.

Projected Phase 3: Assessment of the Effectiveness of Culturally Endorsed Leadership Behavior

Phase 3 will consist of longitudinal tests of the following hypotheses Hypothesis 8:

Societal culture dimensions assessed in Phase 2 will predict isomorphic organizational practices

and leader behavior dimensions assessed in Phase 3.

Hypothesis 9: The correlations between organizational cultural dimensions measured in

Phase 2 and isomorphic CLT dimensions measured in Phase 3 will be greater than the

correlations between societal culture dimensions measured in Phase 2 and isomorphic CLT

dimensions measured in Phase 3.

The rationales for Hypotheses 8 and 9 are specified following Hypotheses 2 and 3 above.

Hypothesis 10: The stronger the alignment among strategic organizational contingencies,

societal culture dimensions, and organizational culture dimensions measured in Phase 2, as

specified in Hypotheses 6a-n, the higher the economic performance of the organizations studied.

The rationale for this hypothesis is specified following Hypothesis 7 above.


55
In addition, armed with Phase 2 measures of CLTs and the nine core GLOBE societal

dimensions for 61 cultures, we will be able to test the following hypotheses with regard to

leadership.

Hypothesis 11: CLTs measured in Phase 2 will predict observed leader behaviors in

Phase 3.

Hypothesis 12: The more congruent the individual leader behaviors are with the CLTs,

(a) the more readily leadership attempts by such individuals will be accepted and effective, (b)

the more the individuals will be perceived as legitimate leaders, (c) the more highly motivated

will be their subordinates/followers, (d) the more committed will be their subordinates/followers,

and (e) the higher will be the leaders’ performance and that of their subordinates.

Hypothesis 13: For purposes of introducing substantial organizational change,

charismatic/value-oriented leadership will be the most effective leader behavior.

Hypothesis 14: Changes in organizational contingency variables measured from Phase 2

to Phase 3 will be correlated with changes in relevant organizational practices as specified by

structural contingency theory.

Hypothesis 15: The correlations specified in hypotheses 6a-nwill be moderated by

isomorphic societal dimensions of culture. The rationale is the same as that for Hypothesis 6a-n

above. The tests of this hypothesis will replicate the tests of Hypotheses 6a-n above, using

longitudinal rather than correlational data. For example, the correlation between changes in

organizational size and changes in organizational formalization is hypothesized to be more

strongly supported in high uncertainty avoidance societies. Because of space limitations, these

longitudinal structural contingency hypotheses are not stated here.


56
Phase 3 Method

Based on the findings of Phase 2, a Multi-Culture Leader Behavior Description

Questionnaire (MCLQ) designed to capture respondents’ perceptions of leaders with whom they

are familiar shall be developed. The samples to be investigated will be similar to the samples of

Phase 2. In countries in which it is not a violation of cultural norms, respondents will be asked to

describe the leader behavior of their immediate supervisor.8 Independent measures of the

immediate supervisor’s performance and work units will also be collected.

The MCLQ will be developed to reflect the leader behaviors identified in Phase 2,

described above, and listed in Table 7. For example, since leader integrity is identified as a

specific leadership attribute, the MCLQ will include several items describing behaviors that

reflect leader integrity. Leader integrity items of the MCLQ will take the form “The leader deals

with subordinates honestly . . . , fairly . . . , sincerely . . ., authentically.” The MCLQ will be

validated by the same process that was applied to the Phase 1 questionnaires.

Respondents will also be asked to express their emotional and evaluative responses to the

leaders, their willingness to support the leaders, their willingness to go above and beyond the call

of duty in the interest of the leaders’ vision and direction, their confidence in the leaders, their

commitment to the leaders’ goals, and the like. Measures of leadership and work-unit

effectiveness shall also be collected. As in Phase 2, high-level executives shall be asked to

complete the organizational practices and structural contingency scales developed in Phase 1.

Search for Emic Leader Behaviors and Emic Manifestations of Etic Dimensions of Cultures

In addition to the etic research described above, we are also interested in identifying the

specific behavioral manifestations and mannerisms employed in enacting CLTs. Since

respondents in all countries could describe the degree to which the leader attributes included in
57
Table 7 contribute to or impede leader effectiveness, these dimensions are etic. They represent

universal continua, along which leader behavior or attributes in all countries can be scaled. As

mentioned above, the second-order CLT dimensions describe global etic leader behavior

patterns. However, some important specific behaviors or attitudes by which these global etic

dimensions are enacted will likely vary among cultures. For example, Smith, Misumi, Tayeb,

Paterson, and Bond (1989) found that American managers are more likely to provide directions

to subordinates on a face-to-face basis, while Japanese managers are likely to use written memos.

In the United States, subordinates are usually provided negative feedback directly from their

supervisors in face-to-face interactions. In Japan, such feedback is usually channeled through a

peer of the subordinate. Thus, Smith et al. (1989) concluded that the global etic behavior

dimension referred to as “performance-oriented leadership” is enacted with different specific

emic behaviors in Japan and the United States. These differences in behaviors reflect the U.S.

individualistic norm of “brute honesty” and the Japanese collectivistic norm of “face saving.”

In order to search for cultural specific (emic) leader behaviors and mannerisms, CCIs will

interview middle and high-level managers using the procedure established by Misumi (1985),

which will provide substantial information concerning culture-specific aspects of leadership.

GLOBE researchers will also search for emic organizational practices and forms. We expect to

find emic organizational practices as well as emic organizational forms in several cultures.

Family-founded and -managed firms in Hong Kong, and post-Soviet entrepreneurial firms

managed by the “New Russians” in Russia are examples of such emic organizational practices.

For each culture, items will be written describing emic organizational practices as well as emic

leader behaviors.
58
Summary: Projected Phase 3 Results

In summary, respondents will describe their organizational cultures and their immediate

superiors. Measures of leader and work-unit effectiveness and individual emotional and

cognitive evaluative responses to leader behaviors will also be obtained. These measures will be

used to test Phase 3 hypotheses and to determine the performance effectiveness of the leader

behaviors described by the dimensions of the CLTs identified in Phase 2. Measures of

organizational culture, structural contingencies, and effectiveness of organizations will also be

collected. These measures will be used to conduct longitudinal tests of structural contingency

theory. Measures of emic leader behaviors and organizational practices shall also be obtained to

gain culture-specific knowledge about leadership in each of the cultures studied.. We shall

eliminate the possibility of common source bias by collecting measures of organizational

cultures and leader behavior from different subsamples within each culture studied.

Projected Phase 4: Laboratory and Field Experiments

The research through Phase 3 will allow us to determine (a) those leader behaviors that

are universally perceived as facilitators of or impediments to outstanding leadership, (b) whether

there are any universally practiced leader behaviors and universal organizational practices, (c)

leader behaviors and organizational practices that have positive or negative cognitive, affective,

and performance consequences, and (d) leader behaviors and organizational practices that are

culture-specific, i.e., those that are practiced in only some cultures and have positive or negative

effects in only some cultures, (d) the effects of culture on the frequency, acceptability, and

effectiveness of organizational practices, (e) whether the associations among organizational

contingencies, organizational practices, and organizational effectiveness predicted by structural


59
contingency theory hold longitudinally, and (e) the moderating effects of societal culture on

the associations predicted by structural contingency theory.

Phase 4 of the program will be designed to determine experimentally the effects of the

various leader behaviors by cultures and thus determine causal relationships among leader

behaviors and outcomes. Here we describe the conceptual fundamentals of the projected Phase 4

research. An operational research design will be specified when the results of Phase 3 are

available.

Using the culture-specific endorsed leader profiles as guides, we will conduct controlled

field and laboratory experiments in at least two cultures in each major region of the world. The

leader behaviors to be studied will be those that were found in Phase 3 to be most relevant to

leader effectiveness, either positively or negatively, in the 61 cultures studied. The experiments

will be designed so that we will be able to assess the effects of various leader behaviors on

follower affective responses, behavior, and effectiveness.

Phase 4 Hypotheses

Hypotheses 12 and 13 of Phase 3 concerning the moderating effect of CLTs on leader

behavior-outcome relationships and charismatic/value-based leadership to stimulate

organizational change will be tested experimentally as Hypotheses 16 and 17 to determine the

causal effect of leader behaviors and CLTs on the dependent variables.

Hypothesis 18. Leader behaviors that are consistent with CLTs measured in Phase 2 will

be more accepted and will have more positive cognitive, affective, behavioral, and performance

effects on followers than leader behaviors that are inconsistent with CLTs measured in Phase 2.
60
Hypothesis 19. For purposes of introducing substantial organizational change in

organizational practices, charismatic/value-based leadership will be the most effective leader

behavior.

Phase 4 Method

Using the culture-specific endorsed leader profiles as guides, we will conduct controlled

experiments in at least two cultures in each major region of the world. The cultures to be selected

for the experiments will be those with well-defined and consensually agreed CLT profiles as

indicated by low within-country variance of responses to Phase 2 CLT scales. The experiments

will be designed so that we shall be able to assess the effects of various leader behaviors on

follower affective responses, behavior, and effectiveness.

The laboratory setting in which Hypothesis 18 will be tested will be a realistic simulation

of an organization. The independent variables will be three kinds of confederate leader

behaviors. The dependent variables will be the affective responses, behavior, and performance

effectiveness of individual followers and groups of followers.

In Treatment Condition 1, the effects of the leader behaviors endorsed by each culture

will be assessed. That is, the experimental treatment will consist of leader behaviors endorsed by

the CLT of the country in which the experiment is conducted. The leader behavior will be

enacted by confederate leaders.

In Treatment Condition 2, the effects of those leader behaviors found in Phase 3 to have

the most consistent positive effects on follower cognitions, affect, and performance across

cultures will be assessed. Thus this treatment will consist of universally or near universally

endorsed leader behaviors enacted by confederate leaders.


61
In Treatment Condition 3, confederates will enact leader behaviors that are in conflict

with the CLT profiles of each culture.

The outcome of these experiments will be substantially increased knowledge concerning

the following questions:

1. Are there any universally effective leader behaviors?

2. What is the effect of violating strongly held culturally endorsed preferences for

selected leader behaviors?

3. Are behaviors that are consistent with culture specific preferences more effective than

a select set of other behaviors that have been found in Phase 3 to be the most consistently

positively endorsed leader behaviors across cultures? The answer to this question will tell us

whether the behaviors specified in CLTs are also the behaviors that are more effective and will

indicate whether leaders can make a difference by being different. More specifically, we will be

able to determine whether a select set of behaviors can consistently have more positive effects

than a set of culturally endorsed behaviors, even if the former behaviors are in conflict with

culturally endorsed norms.

Hypothesis 19 will also be tested by using field experimental design. As one possible

example, we may ask university business school students to respond to a proposed change in the

system by which they are graded in their educational program. It will be proposed that the

grading system be changed to grade subjects on the basis of their relative standing and a forced

curve distribution. It will be explained that this system is consistent with competitive schools,

will enhance the reputation of their school, and will enhance the amount of learning achieved by

students.
62
In the first experimental treatment, the confederate leader, acting as a representative of

the school’s curriculum committee or in another relevant official capacity, will introduce and

advocate the proposed change by enacting the leader behaviors most strongly endorsed by the

CLT of the culture.

In the second treatment, the leader will introduce and advocate the proposed change by

linking it to a vision that emphasizes increased international status and competitiveness of the

school and appeals to the patriotism and to the values endorsed in the culture. The leader will

also express high performance standards and strong confidence in the students and appeal to

country loyalty by stressing international competition among business schools. These behaviors

are part of the global dimension titled Charismatic/Value-Based Leadership.

In sum, in Phase 4 we hope to make a substantial contribution to knowledge concerning

the behavioral and performance effects of leader behaviors and organizational practices as well

as their cognitive and affective effects. The results of Phase 4 will be reported in a monograph or

book relevant to leadership and organizational practices.

UNIQUE STRENGTHS OF THE GLOBE RESEARCH DESIGN

Project GLOBE differs from previous cross-cultural research in several ways. The

primary strength of this research is that we have not made assumptions about how best to

measure cultural phenomena. Rather, we use multiple measurement methods in order to

empirically test which methods are most meaningful. This is most evident in the development of

three sets of measures assessing culture: a) those based on shared values of organizational or

society members, b) those based on current organizational and societal practices, and c)

unobstrusive measures In addition, we developed measures of leader attributes that differentiate

cultures in terms of perceived effectiveness, as well as leader attributes that are universally
63
endorsed (or rejected) across cultures. Further, we have collected data relevant to

organizational contingency variables.

We developed new measures and collected original data for our hypotheses and research

questions, rather than collecting data on only some variables and relying on measures developed

at other times in other places from other samples for the other variables. Since the organizational

culture, societal culture, and leadership measures employed in Phase 2 were completed by

different people, we were able to eliminate the frequently encountered problem of common

source bias. By use of multiple indicators of societal culture, we were able to eliminate common

method variance. The psychometric properties of the GLOBE scales and tests of their validity

exceed normal empirical research standards and are described in the previously mentioned paper

(Hanges et al., 1997, under review).

CONTRIBUTIONS

The GLOBE research program is directed toward filling a substantial knowledge gap

concerning the cross-cultural forces relevant to effective leadership and organizational practices.

The research findings will be useful for resolving several important theoretical social science

issues and for a wide variety of practical purposes. In this section, we briefly describe the various

contributions we expect to result from the GLOBE research.

Practical Relevance

The final product of the GLOBE research program, the books and articles in which the

various cultures will be described and interpreted, will include practically useful information

about the cultures studied.

It is expected that the quantitative findings resulting from the GLOBE research program

will provide substantial enlightenment concerning the processes by which culture influences
64
leadership and organizational practices. In the GLOBE anthologies consisting of culture-

specific chapters, cultures will be described in terms of the nine core dimensions as well as their

unique (emic) attributes. A description will be provided concerning universal and culturally

contingent leader attributes and behaviors, the commonly enacted and most favored leader

behavior patterns and organizational practices found in the cultures studied, and the cultural

influences on the effectiveness of leader behaviors and organizational practices. In addition,

leader behaviors that are culturally offensive will be identified and described. As mentioned

previously, fourteen chapters which will comprise the first anthology have been completed.

The qualitative research chapters in the GLOBE anthologies describe the most critical

leader behaviors and organizational practices in each culture studied, the constraints imposed on

leaders by cultural prescriptions and proscriptions, and unique norms of the cultures studied

relevant to leadership and organizational practices. This information will be useful as case

content for leadership training and career development programs and for the design of

management and leadership education programs intended to prepare individuals who will

manage and lead others in cultures other than their home cultures.

The descriptions of cultural prescriptions and proscriptions will be useful for the

adjustment and effective interaction of individuals who work with others from the cultures

studied. More specifically, this information will be useful to expatriates assigned to other than

their native cultures, managers of diverse cultural and ethnic groups both domestic and abroad,

individuals involved in the management of public and private international affairs, and those who

conduct negotiations with commercial and political organizations in other cultures.

Knowledge of the culturally endorsed implicit theories of leadership in each culture, and

most and least effective leader attributes and behaviors, will be useful for selecting, counseling,
65
and training individuals who are to be assigned to, or who work with, members of the cultures

studied. The resulting findings will be useful for informing potential managers of the kinds of

behaviors and organizational practices that are acceptable and effective and unacceptable and

ineffective in the cultures studied.

Information concerning the constraints imposed on leaders by cultural norms will be

useful to decision makers who need to anticipate and respond to the actions of leaders of other

cultures. Knowledge about cultural and organizational norms and practices in the cultures

studied can inform the formulation of meaningful prescriptions for managing in other cultures--

for strategy and policy formulation, organizational improvement interventions, human resource

management practices, and the design of organization structures and incentive and control

systems.

The industries studied are subjected to a wide variety of organizational contingencies.

Many of the findings relevant to the effects of organizational contingencies on organizational

practices and effectiveness in these industries are thus likely to be relevant to other industries and

therefore useful to managers whose industries face similar organizational contingencies.

In sum, the findings of the study will provide a wide variety of information about 61

cultures, representing all major regions of the world, that can help managers and leaders in their

adjustment, strategy and policy formulation, human resource management practices, and

organizational practices.

Beneficial Social and Economic Applications

The research program is expected to have several additional beneficial social and

economic applications. Within regions, countries that share similar regional resources and

backgrounds can make comparisons to determine similarities and differences among themselves
66
and share ways to improve inter-country relationships, economic productivity, and quality of

life for their citizens. The research program is also expected to lead to increased intercultural

communication among educators who normally would not have contact with each other, and thus

it will result in greater intercultural awareness and cooperation among scholars. CCIs have been

extremely active in practicing cross-cultural communications. As stated above, GLOBE related

research has been presented in over 80 conference papers and over 30 working papers, chapters

of books, or journal publications.

Many of the country co-investigators, being indigenous to their cultures, are influence

and change agents within those cultures, at least with respect to those with whom they have

contact in their roles as university faculty members, social scientists, and consultants. The intra-

country social influence of the CCIs will most likely be substantially enhanced by participating

in the GLOBE research. These CCIs, in turn, will serve as country boundary spanners and will

facilitate importation and transfer of knowledge within their countries. Thirty five scholarly

papers based on GLOBE data have been presented at national or regional conferences thus far.

CCIs in several nations have begun to translate the products of the research (which will

be several books and scholarly articles) into other languages and thus increase the dissemination

of this information to a wider number of countries. The chapters of the books will make reports

on each of the cultures studied available in the public domain.

Ancillary Social Science Contributions

The resulting data can be used for multiple purposes beyond the hypotheses of the study.

For example, the worldwide Phase 2 data can be used to compare countries with their trading

partners or their major competitors with respect to cultural, organizational, or leadership

practices that are relevant to improving trade between them or with respect to practices that
67
facilitate harmonious and productive trade. We have already witnessed over 50 research

projects and papers presented at scholarly conferences in which cultural and managerial practices

have been compared among subsets of the GLOBE participating nations. An entire issue of the

Polish Psychological Bulletin has been devoted to this research (Maczynski, 1997).

Relationships between the variables under study and economic practices and outcomes

can also be subjected to analysis. The societal-level data can be used in econometric or

sociological models and related to firm-level practices such as forms of production systems and

organization, transfer of technology, pricing, risk taking with respect to entry into new markets,

investment with respect to research and development, and foreign investment practices. With the

exception of four studies we were able to locate concerning economic growth, little attention is

given to cultural influences on economic practices and output. The four studies to which we

refer are those authored by Schlomo; Hofstede & Bond; Franke; and House, Wright & Aditya.

The measures of culture can also be related to national levels of saving, distribution of

wealth and social privileges, consumption levels and patterns, issues of economic growth and

development, regulatory practices, and national productivity and efficiency. To date, cultural

influences on such variables have gone largely ignored.

Indices of economic practices, adherence to norms of human rights, safety, and quality of

life, by country, are either available in published form or can be collected by CCIs, other

scholars, or interested government agencies or foundations. The GLOBE Phase 2 worldwide data

can be analyzed in relation to these indices. Thus it will be possible to determine concurrent or

predictive relationships between the GLOBE dimensions and such indices.

The worldwide data can also be analyzed to determine relationships between the

variables under study and many indices of social and physical well-being. For example, the
68
GLOBE societal culture dimensions can be related to such outcomes as mortality rates, life

expectancy rates, hygiene practices, preventive or remedial medical practices, stress levels,

suicide rates, frequency of ethnic and border conflicts, indicators of social unrest, and violations

of human rights. Following are some examples of expected relationships between GLOBE

societal dimensions and socially and practically relevant variables:

Cultural tendencies toward power stratification and assertiveness are likely positively

related to tendencies toward intra-country conflict among labor and management and possibly

even to tendencies of nations to enter into aggressive ethnic border conflicts and military actions.

•Humane orientation is likely inversely related to the frequency and severity of hostile

actions within cultures. Humane orientation is also likely positively related to such practices as

the establishment and enforcement of human rights norms and laws, and inversely related to their

violations.

•Assertiveness is likely positively related to the frequency and severity of hostile actions

within cultures.

•Performance and future orientation are likely positively related to national

competitiveness and economic development.

•Gender equalization is likely inversely related to female abuse and positively related to

female literacy, education, and labor market participation

Following are three examples of findings that show how some of the cultural variables

under study have been shown to relate to, or predict, important behavior. In The Achieving

Society, David McClelland (1961) demonstrated rather convincingly that cultural indicators of

achievement motivation were predictive of subsequent economic development in developing

countries over a twenty five year period. Hofstede and Bond (1988) found that a measure of
69
cultural future orientation and delay of gratification referred to as the Confucian Dynamic was

positively related to the economic growth of the Asian tigers from 1965 to 1985. Kogut and

Singh (1986) have shown that the level of cultural uncertainty avoidance is inversely related to

the level of risk taken by organizations when entering markets in foreign nations. Finally, one of

the unobtrusive measures that correlate with the United Nations’ measures of gender equalization

is the United Nations index of female participation in labor markets.

OUTCOMES

The outcomes of Phases 1 and 2 of the GLOBE project will consist of several books and

a series of articles. The first book will report the comparative quantitative cross-cultural results

and hypothesis tests of GLOBE Phase 2 research. Another book or article will report the research

findings relevant to the tests of structural contingency theory of organizational form and

effectiveness as well as other findings relevant to organizational culture and practices enacted

cross-culturally. The remaining books will be the anthologies described above, consisting of

country-specific descriptions of cultures and interpretations of the Phase 2 quantitative data. Two

or more methodological monographs or articles will illustrate new quantitative methods of cross-

cultural research and also illustrate how recently developed sophisticated cross-level statistical

procedures can be applied in cross-cultural research. One such monograph is currently under

review (Hanges et al., 1997). Measurement papers will present the development and validation of

questionnaires, unobtrusive measurement, and participant observation scales.

Phase 3 will test relationships found in Phase 2 longitudinally and will investigate emic

as well as etic phenomena. Phase 3 results will appear in at least one additional book and several

articles. Phase 4 will also result in at least one book or monograph that will report the results of

the laboratory and field experiments.


70
CONCLUSION AND FUTURE ACTIVITIES

In summary, the GLOBE research is designed to contribute to the development of

empirically based cross-cultural leadership and organizational theory by investigating the roles

of societal and organizational values and institutionalized practices, organizational contingency

variables, and implicit leadership theories as antecedents to cross-cultural variance in leader

behavior, leader influence, leader effectiveness, and organizational practices and performance.

GLOBE research is also designed to contribute to organizational theory and practice by

exploring relationships between societal and organizational cultural variables and organizational

effectiveness and by conducting cross-cultural tests of structural contingency theory. Based on

the preliminary findings reported in this monograph, we are encouraged to believe that the

GLOBE project has the potential of making a noteworthy contribution to the cross-cultural

leadership and organizational literature.


71
NOTES

1. Phases 1 and 2 of the GLOBE research program were funded by the Dwight D.

Eisenhower Leadership Education Program of the Department of Education of the United States.

Funds to support data collection in three African countries were provided by a grant from the

Reginald Jones Center for Strategic Leadership.

2. While there are 61 cultures in the full Project GLOBE sample, the findings [reword?]

reported here are based on data from only 54 countries. The data for the remaining countries were

not yet entered into the computer files at the time this paper was finalized. The remaining cultures

are Albania, China, Denmark, French-speaking Switzerland, Japan, Kazakhstan, and the United

States.

3. Current members of the GCT are Staffan Akerblom, Stockholm School of Economics,

Sweden; Felix Brodbeck, University of Munich, Germany; Jagdeep S. Chhokar, Indian Institute of

Management, Ahmedabad, India; Marcus W. Dickson, Wayne State University; Peter W. Dorfman,

New Mexico State University; Paul J. Hanges, University of Maryland; Robert J. House, University

of Pennsylvania; Mansour Javidan, University of Calgary, Canada; Enrique Ogliastri, University of

Los Andes, Colombia; Antonio Ruiz-Quintanilla, Cornell University; and Marius van Wyk,

University of South Africa.

4. This hypothesis was suggested by Celeste Wilderome, Tilburg University, The

Netherlands. The mean culture scores of the CLT dimensions from the two sub-samples in each

culture were found not to be significantly different. Thus, the CLT scale mean scores were

combined within each culture to yield culture-specific CLT scores.

5. CCIs were asked to collect data from organizations in only two industries per country

because it was believed that CCIs would find collection of data from three industries to be
72
excessively burdensome. In fact, approximately two-thirds of the CCI teams collected data

from all three target industries.

6. Interviews and focus groups revealed that in several of the cultures studied it would be a

violation of cultural norms for subordinates to complete a questionnaire or answer interview

questions that might be construed as evaluative with respect to individuals in positions of authority.
73

Footnotes

1. Phases I and II of the GLOBE research program were funded by the Dwight D. Eisenhower

Leadership Education Program of the Department of Education of the United States. Funds to

support data collection in three African countries were provided by a grant from the Reginald

Jones Center for Strategic Leadership.

2. While there are 61 cultures in the full Project GLOBE sample, the findings reported here are

based on only 54 countries. The data for the remaining countries were not yet entered into the

computer files at the time this monograph was finalized. The remaining cultures are Albania,

China, Denmark, French-speaking Switzerland, Japan, Kazakhstan, and the United States.

3. Current members of the GCT are: Staffan Akerblom, Stockholm School of Economics,

Sweden; Felix Brodbeck, University of Munich, Germany; Jagdeep S. Chhokar, Indian Institute

of Management, Ahmedabad, India; Marcus W. Dickson, Wayne State University, U.S.A; Peter

W. Dorfman, New Mexico State University, U.S.A; Mansour Javidan, University of Calgary,

Canada; Enrique Ogliastri, University of Los Andes, Colombia; Antonio Ruiz-Quintanilla,

Cornell University, U.S.A; Marius van Wyk, University of South Africa, South Africa;

4. Data relevant to organizational practices were collected using the organizational level questions.

Data relevant to families and societal practices were collected using the societal level questions.

5. This hypothesis was suggested by Celeste Wilderom, Tilburg University, The Netherlands.

6. CCIs were asked to collect data from organizations in only two industries per country because it

was believed that CCIs would find collection of data from three industries to be excessively
74

burdensome. In fact, approximately two-thirds of the CCI teams collected data from all

three target industries.

7. Vipin Gupta idenditifed the specific unobtrusive measures based on a literature survey of

information published by the United Nations and the World Bank and other relevant published

information. Gupta also conducted the statistical analyses to develop and validate the

unobtrusive measures.

8. Interviews and focus groups revealed that, in several of the cultures studied, it would be a

violaion of cultural norms for subordinates to complete a questionnaire or answer interview

questions that might be construed as evaluative with respect to individuals in positions of

authority.
75
Table 1. GLOBE-Participating Countries

*Albania England Italy Russia

Argentina Finland *Japan Singapore

Australia *France *Kazakhstan Slovenia

Austria Georgia Kuwait South Africa


(Black sample
Bolivia Germany Malaysia South Africa
(former FRG) (White sample)
Brazil Germany Mexico Spain
(former GDR)
Canada Greece Morocco Sweden
(English speaking)
China Guatemala Netherlands Switzerland
(French speaking)
Colombia Hong Kong New Zealand Switzerland
(German speaking)
Costa Rica Hungary Nicaragua Taiwan

Czech Republic India Nigeria Turkey

Denmark Indonesia Philippines *United States

Ecuador Iran Poland Venezuela

Egypt Ireland Portugal Zambia

El Salvador Israel Qatar Zimbabwe

*The questionnaire data for these countries were not added to computer data files by press time.
76
Table 2. Culture Construct Definitions and Sample Questionnaire Items

Culture Construct Definitions Specific Questionnaire Item


Power distance: The degree to which Followers are (should be) expected to obey
members of a collective expect power to be their leaders without question.
distributed equally.
Uncertainty avoidance: The extent to which Most people lead (should lead) highly
a society, organization, or group relies on structured lives with few unexpected events.
social norms, rules & procedures to alleviate
unpredictability of future events.
Humane orientation: The degree to which a People are generally (should be generally)
collective encourages & rewards individuals very tolerant of mistakes.
for being fair, altruistic, generous, caring &
kind to others. Aging parents generally live (should live) at
home with their children.
Collectivism I: The degree to which Leaders encourage (should encourage) group
organizational and societal institutional loyalty even if individual goals suffer.
practices encourage and reward collective
distribution of resources and collective action
Collectivism II: The degree to which Aging parents generally live (should live) at
individuals express pride, loyalty and home with their children.
cohesiveness in their organizations or
families.
Assertiveness: The degree to which People are (should be) generally dominant.
individuals are assertive, dominant &
demanding in their relationships with others.
Gender egalitarianism: The degree to which Boys are encouraged (should be encouraged)
a collective minimizes gender inequality. more than girls to attain a higher education.
(Scored inversely.)
Future orientation: The extent to which a More people live (should live) for the present
collective encourages future-oriented than for the future. (Scored inversely.)
behaviors such as delaying gratification,
planning & investing in the future.
Performance orientation: The degree to Students are encouraged (should be
which a collective encourages & reward encouraged) to strive for continuously
group members for performance improved performance.
improvement & excellence.
77
Table 3. Sample CLT Questionnaire Items and Response Alternatives

Definition of Leadership Ability to influence, motivate and enable others to contribute to


success of their organization.

Sample CLT Items Sensitive: Aware of slight changes in moods of others.

Motivator: Mobilizes, activates followers.

Evasive: Refrains from making negative comments to maintain


good relationships and save face.

Diplomatic: Skilled at interpersonal relations, tactful.

Self-interested: Pursues own best interests.

Response Alternatives Impedes or facilitates unusually effective leadership


1. Substantially impedes
2. Moderately impedes
3. Slightly impedes
4. Neither impedes nor facilitates
5. Slightly facilitates
6. Moderately facilitates
7. Substantially facilitates
78
Table 4. Example of Parallel Items for the Culture Scales

Organization As Is
The pay and bonus system in this organization is designed to maximize:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Individual Collective
Interests Interests

Organization Should Be
In this organization, the pay and bonus system should be designed to maximize:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8
Individual Collective
Interests Interests

Society As Is
The economic system in this society is designed to maximize:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Individual Collective
Interests Interests

Society Should be
I believe that the economic system in this society should be designed to maximize:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Individual Collective
Interests Interests
79
Table 5. Sample Unobtrusive Measures*

Avoidance of uncertainty High number of information processing equipment


items (e.g. fax machines, cell phones) per 1,000 people,
indicating highemphasis on information availability..

Limited number of of scientists/GNP, indicating


Power distance
suppression of intellectual inquiry.
Societal emphasis on collectivism Early time zone, indicating Eastern and South Eastern
location where societal collectivism is predominant.

Low divorce rates per marriage, indicating pressure for


Family collectivism sustaining intimate relationships.

High proportion of females with earned income,


Gender egalitarianism
indicating low discrimination against females and
pressure for females contribution to work force..
Humane orientation Few retail outlets per capita, indicating low emphasis
economic amenities and high emphasis on relationship
orientation

High proportion of public education expenditure


Future orientation
devoted to higher education, indicating public
investment for future opportunities and future economic
performance.

Performance orientation Low share of government funded R & D, indicating


free market competition and low government intervention.

* There is no effect of GNP, per capita income, population size of country, or year of
independence on the correlations between the unobtrusive measures and their isomorphic
questionnaire based measures, for the total sample of 54 cultures. However, selected subsamples
such as more or less welthy countries indicated differences in correlations suggesting boundary
conditions for some of the unobtrusive measures. These boundary conditions are being further
investigated.
80
Table 6. Latent Constructs and Manifest and Qualitative Indicators

LATENT MANIFEST QUALITATIVE


CONSTRUCTS INDICATORS INDICATORS
Societal cultural norms Questionnaires Interviews
Nine core dimensions (Phase Unobtrusive measures Media analysis
2) Quantified participant Focus groups
observations CCI participant observation
Organizational practices: nine Questionnaires by middle
core dimensions managers
(Phase 2)
Organizational contingencies: Questionnaires by top
technology, environment, executives
size, strategy (Phase 2)
Societal culturally endorsed Middle manager Interviews & media analysis
implicit leadership theories questionnaire ratings Focus groups
(CLTs) Media indicators CCI participant observation
Leader behavior (Phase 3) Questionnaires Interviews
Media indicators Media analysis
Leader acceptance (Phase 3) Questionnaires CCI participant observation
Ratings based on media Interviews media
indicators
Leader effectiveness Lab/field experiments
(Phase 4)
81
Table 7. Leadership Prototype Scales: First Order Factors and Leader Attribute Items

Administratively Competent willful micro-manager


orderly decisive non-egalitarian
administratively skilled logical individually-oriented
organized intuitive
good administrator Diplomatic Performance oriented
Autocratic diplomatic improvement-oriented
autocratic worldly excellence-oriented
dictatorial win/win problem-solver performance-oriented
bossy effective bargainer Procedural
elitist Face Saver ritualistic
Autonomous indirect formal
individualistic avoids negatives habitual
independent evasive procedural
autonomous Humane orientation Self-centered
unique generous self-centered
Charismatic I: visionary compassionate non-participative
foresight Integrity loner
prepared honest asocial
anticipatory sincere Status consciousness
plans ahead just status-conscious
Charismatic II: inspirational trustworthy class conscious
enthusiastic Malevolent Team 1: Collaborative Team
positive hostile Orientation
morale booster dishonest group-oriented
motive arouser vindictive collaborative
Charismatic III: self- irritable loyal
sacrificial Modesty consultative
risk taker modest Team II: Team Integrator
self-sacrificial self-effacing communicative
convincing patient team-builder
Conflict Inducer informed
normative integrator
secretive
intra-group competitor
Non-participative
Decisive non-delegator
82
Table 8. Global Culturally Endorsed Implicit Leadership (CLT) Dimensions.

1. Charismatic/Value Based, 4.5 - 6.5 2. Team Oriented, 4.8 - 6.2


*Charismatic 1: Visionary *Team 1: Collaborative Team Orientation
*Charismatic 2: Inspirational *Team 2: Team Integrator
*Charismatic 3: Self-sacrifice *Diplomatic
*Integrity *Malevolent (reverse scored)
*Decisive *Administratively competent
*Performance oriented

3. Self-Protective, 2.6 – 4.6 4. Participative, 4.5 - 6.1


*Self-centered *Autocratic (reverse scored)
*Status conscious *Non-participative (reverse scored)
*Conflict inducer Delegator*
*Face saver
*Procedural

5. Humane, 3.8 - 5.6 6. Autonomous, 2.3 - 4.7


*Modesty *Individualistic
*Humane orientation *Independent
*Autonomous
*Unique

Note: The numbered, italicized topics are Global CLT Dimensions. They are comprised of leadership CLT subscales. The only
exception is Topic #6 (Autonomous) which is comprised of questionnaire items, not subscales, and the item delegator (**) which is
included in the participative dimension since it had a .81 correlation with the sum of the two subscales: autocratic and non-
participative (scores revised).. Numbers represent worldwide mean values on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (substantially
impedes) to 7 (substantially facilitates) effective leadership.
83

Table 9. Universal Positive Leader Attributes


Questionnaire Items Corresponding Leadership Scale
(First Order Factors)
Trustworthy Integrity
Just Integrity
Honest Integrity
Foresight Charisma 1: visionary
Plans ahead Charisma 1: visionary
Encouraging Charisma 2: inspirational
Positive Charisma 2: inspirational
Dynamic Charisma 2: inspirational
Motive arouser Charisma 2: inspirational
Confidence builder Charisma 2: inspirational
Motivational Charisma 2: inspirational
Dependable Malevolent (reverse score)
Intelligent Malevolent
Decisive Decisiveness
Effective bargainer Diplomatic
Win-win problem solver Diplomatic
Administratively skilled Administratively competent
Communicative Team 2: team integrator
Informed Team 2: team integrator
Coordinator Team 2: team integrator
Team builder Team 2: team integrator
Excellence oriented Performance oriented
84

Table 10. Universal Negative Leader Attributes

Questionnaire Attributes Corresponding Leadership Scale


(First Order Factors)
Loner Self-protective
Asocial Self-protective
Noncooperative Malevolent
Irritable Malevolent
Nonexplicit Face saver
Egocentric *
Ruthless *
Dictatorial Autocratic

*These items did not load on any factor.


Table 11. Culturally Contingent CLT Items

Able to anticipate (3.84 - 6.51) Intuitive (3.72 - 6.47)


Ambitious (2.85 - 6.73) Logical (3.89 - 6.58)
Autonomous (1.63 - 5.17) Micro-manager (1.60 - 5.00)
Cautious (2.17 - 5.78) Orderly (3.81 - 6.34)
Class conscious (2.53 - 6.09) Procedural (3.03 - 6.10)
Compassionate (2.69 - 5.56) Provocateur (1.38 - 6.00)
Cunning (1.26 - 6.38) Risk taker (2.14 - 5.96)
Domineering (1.60 - 5.14) Ruler (1.66 - 5.20)
Elitist (1.61 - 5.00) Self-effacing (1.85 - 5.23)
Enthusiastic (3.72 - 6.44) Self-sacrificial (3.00 - 5.96)
Evasive (1.52 - 5.67) Sensitive (1.96 - 6.35)
Formal (2.12 - 5.43) Sincere (3.99 - 6.55)
Habitual (1.93 - 5.38) Status-conscious (1.92 - 5.77)
Independent (1.67 - 5.32) Subdued (1.32 - 6.18)
Indirect (2.16 - 4.86) Unique (3.47 - 6.06)
Individualistic (1.67 - 5.10) Willful (3.06 - 6.48)
Intra-group competitor (3.00 - 6.49) Worldly (3.48 - 6.18)
Intra-group conflict avoider (1.84 -
5.69)

Note: Numbers represent worldwide minimum and maximum values on a seven-point scale
ranging from 1 (substantially impedes) to 7 (substantially facilitates) effective leadership.
86

Table 12

A priori hypotheses predicting effective leadership style from societal and organizational culture.
Second Order Leadership Factor Predicted Culture Dimension

Charismatic/Value Based 1. Performance Orientation


2. Future Orienation
3. Humane Orientation

Team Oriented 1. Collectivism I:


2. Collectivism II
3. Humane Orientation
4. Assertiveness
5. Uncertainty Avoidance

Participative 1. Assertiveness
2. Power Distance
3. Humane Orientation
4. Power Distance

Humane Orientation 1. Humane Orientation


2. Gender Egalitarianism

Autonomous 1. Collectivism I
2. Collectivism II

Self-Protective 1. Humane Orientation


2. Power Distance
3. Uncertainty Avoidance

86
87

Table 13

Results for Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analyses Predicting Leadership Dimensions from
Organizational and Societal Culture

Dependent Variable: Team Oriented Leadership

Coefficient
Constant 5.89**

Organizational Level
Collectivism Should Be .28**

Societal Level
Humane Orientation Should Be -.13*
Collectivism Should Be .40**

Organizational Variance Explained: 37.4%


Societal Variance Explained: 32.7%

Total Variance Explained: 10.8%

Dependent Variable: Participative Leadership

Coefficient
Constant 4.79**

Organizational Level
Power Distance Should Be -.16**
Uncertainty Avoidance Should Be -.13**

Societal Level
Uncertainty Avoidance Should Be -.46**
Power Distance Should Be -.35**
Humane Orientation Should Be .25*
Assertiveness Should Be .12*

Organizational Variance Explained: 29.7%


Societal Variance Explained: 82.2%

Total Variance Explained: 26.9%

87
88

Table 13 (continued)
Dependent Variable: Humane Orientation

Coefficient
Constant 4.83**

Organizational Level
Humane Orientation Should Be .37**
Societal Level
Humane Orientation Should Be .41**

Organizational Variance Explained: 20.7%


Societal Variance Explained: 31.6%

Total Variance Explained: 7.0%

Dependent Variable: Charisma

Coefficient
Constant 5.88**

Organizational Level
Performance Orientation Should Be .22**

Societal Level
Performance Orientation Should Be .35**

Organizational Variance Explained: 41.6%


Societal Variance Explained: 14.2%

Total Variance Explained: 11.9%

p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .0001

Note:

The unit of analysis these regressions is the organization. The number of organizations is 391.
The degrees of freedom are 390. All organizations existed in societies from which three or
more organizations provided data.

Organizational variance explained is calculated by dividing the total variance accounted by the
organizational predictors by the total amount of variance occurring at the organizational level
of analysis. Society variance explained is calculated by dividing the total variance accounted
by the society predictors by the total amount of variance occurring at the society level of
analysis.

88
89

REFERENCES

Ajzen, L., & Fishbein, M. (1970). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Aldrich, H. E. (1979). Organizations and environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.

Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill’s handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and

managerial applications (3rd ed.). New York: Free Press.

Bass, B. M. (1997). Does the transactional-transformational leadership paradigm transcend

organizational and national boundaries? American Psychologist, 52(2), 130-139.

Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In W. J. Lohner &

J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. (pp.

137-164).

Burns, T., & Stalker, G. M. (1961). The management of innovation. London: Tavistock

Publications, Tavistock Centre.

Child, J. (1981). Culture, contingency, and capitalism in the cross-national study of

organization. In L. L. Cummings (Ed.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 303-356).

Dickson, M. (1997). Universality and variation in organizationally cognitive prototypes of

effective leadership. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, University of

Maryland.

Donaldson, L. (1993). Anti-management theories of organization: A critique of paradigm

proliferation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dorfman, P. W. (1996). International and cross-cultural leadership research. In B. J. Punnett &

O. Shenkar (Eds.), Handbook for international management research (pp. 267-349). Oxford, UK:

Blackwell,

89
90

Gagliardi, P. (1986). The creation and change of organizational cultures: A conceptual

framework. Organization Studies, 7(2), 117-134.

Gerstner, C. R., & Day, D. V. (1994). Cross-cultural comparison of leadership prototypes.

Leadership Quarterly, 5(2), 121-134.

Hanges, P. J., Braverman, E. P., & Rentsch, J. R. (1991). Changes in raters’ impressions of

subordinates: A catastrophe model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 878-888.

Hanges, P. J., Lord, R. G., Day, D. V., Sipe, W. P., Smith, W. C., & Brown, D. J. (1997).

Leadership and gender bias: Dynamic measures and nonlinear modeling. In R. G. Lord (Chair),

Dynamic systems, leadership perceptions, and gender effects. Symposium presented at the Twelfth

Annual Conference of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology.

Hickson, D. J., Hinings, C. R., McMillan, J., & Schwitter. (1974). The culture-free context of

organization structure: A tri-national comparison. Sociology 8, 59-80.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values.

London: Sage.

Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1988). The Confucius connection. From cultural roots to

economic growth. Organizational Dynamics, 16, 4-21.

House, R. J., & Aditya, R. N. (1997). The social scientific study of leadership: Quo vadis?

Journal of Management, 23(3), 409-473.

House, R. J., Hanges, P., Ruiz-Quintanilla, S. A., & Dickson, M. W. (1997). The development

and validation of scales to measure societal and organizational culture. Under review.

House, R. J., Wright, N. S., & Aditya, R. N. (1997). Cross-cultural research on organizational

leadership: A critical analysis and a proposed theory. In P. C. Earley & M. Erez (Eds.), New

Perspectives in International Industrial Organizational Psychology (pp. 535-625). San Francisco: New

Lexington,

90
91

James, L. R., Demaree, [?] R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group interrater

reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69(1), 85-98.

Khandwalla, P. N. (1977). The sign of organizations. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Kluckhohn, F. R., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Variations in value orientations. New York:

HarperCollins.

Kopelman, R. E., Brief, A. P., & Guzzo, R. A. (1990). The role of climate and culture in

productivity. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 282-318). San Francisco:

Jossey-Bass.

Lawrence, P. R., & Lorsch, J. W. (1967). Organization and environment. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (1989). On the empirical identification of dimensions for cross-

cultural comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 133-151.

Lombardo, M. M. (1983). I felt it as soon as I walked in. Issues and Observations, 3(4), 7-8.

Lord, R., & Maher, K. J. (1991). Leadership and information processing: Linking perceptions

and performance. Boston: Unwin-Everyman.

Lumpkin, G. T., & Dess, G. G. (1996). Clarifying the entrepreneurial orientation construct and

linking it to performance, Academy of Management Review 21(1), 135-172.

Maczynski, J. (1997). GLOBE: The Global Leadership and Organizational Behavior

Effectiveness research program. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 28(3), 215-254.

Marin, G., Triandis, H. C., Betancourt, H., & Kashima, Y. (1983). Ethnic affirmation versus

social desirability: Explaining discrepancies in bilinguals’ responses to a questionnaire. Journal of

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 173-186.

McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.

91
92

McClelland, D. C. & Atkinson, J. W. (1948). The projective expression of needs, I: The effect

of different intensities of the hunder drive on perception. Journal of Psychology. 25, 205-222.

McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (Eds.). (1953). The

achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

McFarland, L. J., Senen, S., & Childress, J. R. (1993). Twenty-first-century leadership. New

York: Leadership Press.

Miller, D., & Droge, C. (1986). Psychological and traditional determinants of structure.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 31(4), 539-560.

Misumi, J. (1985). The behavioral science of leadership: An interdisciplinary Japanese research

program. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

O’Connell, M. S., Lord, R. G., & O’Connell, M. K. (1990, August). Differences in Japanese

and American leadership prototypes: Implications for cross-cultural training. Paper presented at the

meeting of the Academy of Management, San Francisco.

Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making democracy work. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.

Schein, E. H. (1992). Organizational culture and leadership: A dynamic view (2nd ed.). San

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437-454.

Schneider, B., Goldstein, H. W., & Smith, D. B. (1995). The ASA Framework: An update.

Personnel Psychology, 48, 747-783.

Shaw, J. B. (1990). A cognitive categorization model for the study of intercultural

management. Academy of Management Review, 15(4), 626-645.

Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press.

92
93

Sipe, W. P., & Hanges, P. J. (1997). Reframing the glass ceiling: A catastrophe model of

changes in the perception of women as leaders. In R. G. Lord (Chair), Dynamic systems, leadership

perceptions, and gender effects. Symposium presented at the Twelfth Annual Conference of the

Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology.

Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (1993). Social psychology across cultures: Analysis and

perspectives. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

Smith, P. B., Misumi, J., Tayeb, M. H., Paterson, M., & Bond, M. H. (1989). On the generality

of leadership style across cultures. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 30, 526-537.

Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. (1981). Threat-rigidity effects in organizational

behavior: A multilevel analysis, Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4), 501-524.

Thompson, K. R., & Luthans, F. (1990). Organizational culture: A behavioral perspective. In B.

Schneider (Ed.), Organizational Climate and Culture (pp. 319-344). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Triandis, H. C. (1993). The contingency model in cross-cultural perspective. In M. M. Chemers

& R. Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and directions (pp. 167-188). San

Diego: Academic Press.

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. (1984). The cultures of work organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

Prentice-Hall.

Tushman, M. L., Newman, W. H., & Nadler, D. A. (1988). Executive leadership and

organizational evolution: Managing incremental and discontinuous change. In R. H. Kilman & T. J.

Covin (Eds.), Corporate transformation: Revitalizing organizations for a competitive world (pp. 102-

130). San Francisco, Jossey-Bass.

Yukl, G. A. (1994). Leadership in organizations (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

93

You might also like