You are on page 1of 11

bs_bs_banner

Japanese Psychological Research doi: 10.1111/jpr.12055


2014, Volume 56, No. 4, 309–319

The effects of brand popularity as an advertising cue on


perceived quality in the context of internet shopping
JI-HERN KIM Sejong University

DONGWON MIN* Dankook University

Abstract: Brand popularity as a descriptive norm has been used as an advertising


cue by internet malls. It is based on the assumption that consumers prefer brands
with popularity claims because they perceive the popularity claim results from
superior quality. However, little research has been done on how popularity cues
affect perceived quality in internet shopping contexts. Particularly, the interaction
effect between brand popularity and price on the quality perception has never been
investigated. This research presents the results of two experiments showing that
brand popularity and price have an interaction effect on the estimation of sales, which
in turn influences perceived quality. Only when consumers believe that the sales
volume is high would the perceived quality increase. This research also analyzes the
manner in which brand popularity reduces consumers’ perceived risk in purchase
decision-making.
Key words: brand popularity, perceived quality, risk perception, price, internet
shopping.

Social norms that affect consumer behaviors based on the premise that consumers prefer
are classified into injunctive and descriptive brands with popularity claims because they per-
norms (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). The ceive that proclaimed popularity results from
former (called “ought norm”) refers to what superior quality (Dean, 1999). According to
most people approve or disapprove of, while observational learning theory, consumers tend
the latter (called “is norm”) refers to what most to adopt the brands most chosen by their peers
people do. Most consumer research has focused because they believe the brand has high quality
on investigating the effect of injunctive norms and generality to fulfill most consumers’ needs
on consumer behaviors (e.g., theory of rea- (Cai, Chen, & Fang, 2009).
soned action), and thus less is known about Despite the widespread interest in brand
the role of descriptive norms in consumers’ popularity, only a few studies have investigated
decision-making process (Magnini, Karande, its effect on perceived quality, and the results
Singal, & Kim, 2013). Despite the lack of aca- are also mixed (Hellofs & Jacobson, 1999),
demic research, descriptive norms have often possibly owing to the fact that past research has
been used by practitioners to influence con- used market share as an indicator of brand
sumer behaviors. popularity in offline shopping contexts. The
In particular, it is a common practice for brand popularity that internet malls use as an
internet malls to use brand popularity as an advertising cue has a distinctive aspect. Internet
advertising cue (e.g., “No. 1 in sales”). This is malls usually select popular brands based not

*Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to: Dongwon Min, Dankook University, 126 Jukjeon-
dong, Su-ji, Yong-in, Gyeonggi-do, Seoul 448-701, Korea. (E-mail: dwmin@dankook.ac.kr)

© 2014 Japanese Psychological Association. Published by Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd.
310 J-H. Kim and D. Min

on the market share but on their own sales. involves a virtual network (e.g., a network
Accordingly, the sales volume of a brand with a of Chevrolet owners) rather than a physical
popularity cue would differ among internet network (e.g., a telephone network; Katz &
malls. This indicates that the effect of brand Shapiro, 1986). Consumers are likely to believe
popularity on perceived quality needs to be that a greater number of purchasers would indi-
reexamined in internet shopping contexts. Fur- cate a higher quality product, as the manufac-
thermore, internet malls tend to use both brand turer would be forced to improve the product
popularity and price as advertising cues. Past by reflecting the feedback from many custom-
research, however, has considered only the ers (Liebowitz & Margolis, 1994).
main effect of brand popularity on perceived Dean (1999) used “most popular” as an
quality. Accordingly, the interaction effect advertising cue instead of the market share and
between brand popularity and price on per- examined its effect on perceived quality in
ceived quality needs to be examined. offline shopping contexts. Unexpectedly, the
This research has two objectives, in order to popularity effect was found to be insignificant.
shed new light on the effect of brand popularity Based on cue utilization theory, the author
as a descriptive norm on brand evaluation in explained that the result may have arisen from
internet shopping contexts. First, it aims to inappropriate experimental conditions. Specifi-
examine the main effect of brand popularity on cally, participants may have had too much
perceived quality in internet shopping contexts. knowledge and involvement with TV sets as
Second, it seeks to analyze the manner in which stimuli products. Thus, the participants may
brand popularity affects perceived quality with have evaluated them based on intrinsic cues
price. rather than popularity claims as extrinsic cues
(Petty et al., 1983).
In this research, the construct “brand popu-
larity” is defined as the extent to which a brand
Conceptual background
is popular in a product category offered by a
Brand popularity specific internet mall. The brand popularity is
Brand popularity can be defined as the extent proclaimed based on sales of a specific shop-
to which a brand is widely purchased by the ping mall. Accordingly, despite the mixed
general public (Kim & Chung, 1997). It has results of past research, brand popularity is
often been operationally defined as the market expected to positively affect perceived quality
share of a product (Hellofs & Jacobson, 1999). in internet shopping contexts. First, unlike
Accordingly, past research has focused primar- market-share claims made in the total market,
ily on analyzing the effect of market share as popularity claims in a specific mall would not
an indicator of brand popularity on perceived induce the congestion problem. Additionally,
quality; however, the direction of the effect is luxurious brands whose exclusivity is consid-
mixed (Dean, 1999). ered important are less likely to be distributed
Hellofs and Jacobson (1999) examined the through internet malls. Moreover, consumers
relationship between market share and per- feel a greater risk when making a purchase
ceived quality for 85 brands across 28 product decision in internet malls than in offline malls
categories. They concluded that market share (Tucker & Zhang, 2011); hence, they need more
tends to negatively affect perceived quality due information to reduce the risk and thus use
to the loss of exclusivity and a congestion both extrinsic and intrinsic cues (Richardson,
problem. However, the authors also mentioned Dick, & Jain, 1994).
that market share can positively affect per- Taken together, popularity cues of low-
ceived quality when the exclusivity of a product involvement products1 in internet shopping
is not important. Furthermore, a high market
share leads consumers to expect a positive indi- 1
A low-involvement product refers to a product that
rect network externality. An indirect network entails a low risk in the decision-making process

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


Effects of brand popularity on perceived quality 311

contexts can be used effectively to signal a high whelmingly high; this inference is based on the
level of quality. This leads to the following augmentation principle. Therefore, brand popu-
hypothesis: larity and price would have an interaction effect
on perceived quality. This leads to the following
H1. Brand popularity as an advertising cue hypothesis:
in the context of internet shopping has a
positive effect on the perceived quality of H2. The effect that brand popularity has on
low-involvement product. perceived quality of low-involvement product
is greater when the price is high than when the
Brand popularity and price price is low.
The interaction effect between brand popular-
ity and price on perceived quality can be
explained based on two principles in Kelly’s Study 1
attribution theory: discounting and augmenta-
tion principles (Kelly, 1972). The two principles Pretest
address how individuals assess the importance Participants should not have too much knowl-
of a particular cause in explaining others’ edge and involvement with the stimuli products
behaviors when there are multiple potential because they might not use popularity cues in
causes. The discounting principle states that the inferring the quality (Dean, 1999). Accordingly,
importance of a given cause in explaining a a multivitamin was selected as a candidate
certain result is discounted if other plausible product and was compared with a TV set in
causes exist. In contrast, the augmentation prin- terms of product knowledge and involvement
ciple holds that the importance of a given cause by 29 Korean students (18 men). Product
is emphasized if the result occurs, despite the knowledge and involvement were measured
presence of other inhibitory factors. with three items (α = .86) adapted from a study
Both low price and high quality can be poten- by Park, Mothersbaugh, and Feick (1994) and
tial causes of brand popularity. If a product is five items (α = .91) adapted from a study by
not a prestige good, price is likely to negatively Mittal (1995). All the measures in Studies 1
affect brand choice because high prices place and 2 were scored on a 7-point scale and are
consumers at a greater financial risk (Shimp & presented in Table 1. The scores on product
Bearden, 1982). In contrast, product quality knowledge and involvement were 2.7 and 3.8,
positively affects brand choice (Aaker, 1996). respectively, which are significantly lower than
As such, when consumers are exposed to a those of a TV set (M = 3.7, p < .001; M = 4.7,
popularity claim with a low price, they discount p < .05).
the importance of good quality in inferring the To control for prior experiences, fictitious
reason for the brand’s popularity. Besides the names of a brand and internet malls2 had to be
high quality, a low price can be a potential selected (Keller & Aaker, 1992). Eight multivi-
means of explaining brand popularity; this logic tamin brand names, 10 complex mall names
is based on the discounting principle. for Study 1, and 10 category killer names for
On the contrary, when consumers are exposed Study 2 were assessed to determine which
to a popularity claim with high price, they would was the most adequate: “VitaPlex” (M = 4.64),
place greater weight on high quality to explain
the reason for the brand popularity. Consumers 2
Category killers refer to specialty stores, providing
may perceive that a brand is popular despite its consumers with a wide assortment of choices
high price because the product quality is over- within a specific product category. Complex malls
refer to department stores, dealing with a variety
of product categories and a narrower assortment of
(Mittal, 1995). Consumers perceive that a low- choices within those categories. The necessity
involvement product has relatively less personal rel- of classification is described in the Discussion of
evance and importance on their life. Study 1.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


312 J-H. Kim and D. Min

Table 1 Operational measures and scale reliability values in Studies 1 and 2


Construct Operational measures Cronbach’s α
Study 1 Study 2

Perceived • Compared to other multivitamins, the advertised VitaPlex is a .83 .82


quality superior product
• The advertised VitaPlex is the best in its product class
• The advertised VitaPlex will perform better than other multivitamins
Product • How much do you feel you know about multivitamins in general? .88 .90
knowledge • How much do you feel you know about multivitamins compared
to acquaintances?
• How much do you feel you know about multivitamins compared
to experts?
Consumer For me, a multivitamin: .93 .93
involvement • is important 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 is not important
• is of no concern 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 is of concern to me
• means a lot to me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 means nothing to me
• matters to me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 does not matter
• is significant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 is not significant

“ItsMall” (M = 4.68), and “VitaIn” (M = 4.56) ceived quality of the multivitamin. Product
received the highest scores, respectively. knowledge and consumer involvement were
To manipulate the price level, participants measured as in the pretest. Demographics were
were instructed to estimate the price of a requested last.The measures are summarized in
multivitamin in an advertisement that was Table 1.
developed by using the selected brand and the
name of the shopping malls. The estimated Results
prices ranged from approximately US$10–50 As presented in Table 2, the results of an
(median = US$30), which was similar to the ANCOVA controlling for product knowledge
real price range observed in internet malls. and involvement showed neither a main effect
Accordingly, we selected US$10.95 as a low of brand popularity, F(1, 95) = .45, p = .506, nor
price and US$40.95 as a high price. an interaction effect between brand popularity
and price, F(1, 95) = .02, p = .897, on perceived
Methods quality was significant. Therefore, Hypotheses 1
A 2 (popularity: present vs. absent) × 2 (price: and 2 were not supported.
high vs. low) between-subjects design was used.
Ninety-seven participants (50 men) were ran- Discussion
domly assigned to one of four groups. “No. 1 Unexpectedly, Hypotheses 1 and 2 were not
in sales” was used as a popularity claim as it is supported. This might result from the wrong
commonly employed by Korean internet malls. assumption that consumers perceive the sales of
Four different versions of a multivitamin adver- a brand with a popularity cue to be high regard-
tisement were developed, with variations in the less of the price level. If the perceived sales
presence of the popularity cue and the price volume is not high enough to infer high quality,
level (see the Appendix). Kelly’s attribution theory is not adequate to
Participants were instructed to imagine that explain the brand popularity effects.
they were seeing the advertisement while shop- According to the anchoring and adjustment
ping in an internet mall. They were then asked judgment process, consumers might adopt price
to respond to three items (α = .83) adapted as an initial anchor for the estimation of sales
from Dean’s (1999) study to measure the per- and then slightly adjust the estimation based on

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


Effects of brand popularity on perceived quality 313

Table 2 Tests of the effects of brand popularity and price cues on perceived quality in
Studies 1 and 2
Source Study 1 (complex mall) Study 2 (category killer)
F Sig. F p-value

Brand popularity 0.45 .506 0.29 .595


Price 2.79 .098 0.65 .423
Brand popularity × price 0.02 .897 2.72 .102
Knowledge 0.08 .774 0.14 .712
Involvement 0.02 .896 0.02 .876

a popularity claim (Wansink, Kent, & Hoch, hypothesis-supported manner. Consumers


1998). As the sales of a bestselling brand differ might develop a hypothesis about the sales of a
among internet malls, price may act as an brand based on the price level and then inter-
anchor in the estimation of sales. This indi- pret a popularity cue in a manner that confirms
cates that the discounting principle would still their initial belief. Therefore, consumers might
be adequate to explain the brand popularity perceive that a popularity cue means higher
effects, but not the augmentation principle. Spe- sales when the price is lower than when it
cifically, when exposed to a low price with a is high. Only when consumers believe that
popularity claim, consumers would believe that the sales volume is high would the perceived
the sales of a brand are high. Both low price quality increase. This leads to the following
and high quality can be potential causes of high hypothesis:
sales. According to the discounting principle,
consumers discount the importance of good H3. The effect of brand popularity on per-
quality when inferring the reason for the ceived sales volume is greater when the price
brand’s high sales. is lower than when it is high.
In contrast, when exposed to a high price
with a popularity claim, consumers might have Past research on internet shopping has investi-
doubts about the sales of a brand. Because gated a way of reducing consumers’ perceived
brand popularity is limited to a specific shop- risk for boosting purchases because consumers
ping mall, consumers may perceive that the tend to perceive online shopping as more risky
sales of a brand with a popularity cue may not than offline shopping (Tan, 1999). The signal
be high enough to guarantee high quality if of high quality through extrinsic cues may be
there are some inhibitory factors (e.g., high an effective risk reduction strategy, which
price) on the high sales. Accordingly, consumers directly increases consumers’ purchase inten-
would not place greater weight on high quality tion (Biswas & Biswas, 2004). Accordingly, we
to explain the reason for brand popularity; need to examine how brand popularity as a
this indicates that the augmentation principle signal of good quality affects consumers’ risk
cannot be applicable. perceptions.
In summary, consumers might estimate the As mentioned before, brand popularity
sales of a brand differently depending on would affect perceived sales with price.
the combination of the popularity cue and the Although consumers may estimate a high level
price. That is, an interaction effect of brand of sales, their perceived risk would not decrease
popularity and price on perceived sales volume unless they believe that the sales result from
may exist (Kim, 1995). It can also be explained the high quality of the associated product. That
by hypothesis-consistent testing in the anchor- is, the impact of perceived sales on risk percep-
ing paradigm (Mussweiler & Strack, 1999); tion may be mediated by the perceived quality.
people tend to interpret new information in a Extant research has shown that the sales

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


314 J-H. Kim and D. Min

volume positively affects perceived quality quality of a product than those in a complex
(Aaker, 1996; Jun, Huh, & Hong, 2003), which mall. To test this possibility, we need to retest
leads to reduced risk perception (Tucker & Hypotheses 1 and 2 with the category killer in
Zhang, 2011). This leads to Hypothesis 4. Study 2. If Hypotheses 1 and 2 are again not
All the above discussion is summarized in supported in Study 2, external validity would
Figure 1. increase.

H4. Perceived quality plays a mediating role


in the relationship between perceived sales
volume and consumers’ risk perception.
Study 2
Methods
Meanwhile, the effects of descriptive norms on One hundred and one Korean students (52 men)
consumer behaviors can change as a function of participated in Study 2.The procedures of Study
reference group tied to a normative message 2 were the same as those of Study 1 except for
(Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2008). The using a category killer and measuring the per-
reference group tied to a popularity claim ceived sales volume and the risk. The perceived
might be different depending on the mall type, sales volume was measured with the following
as the characteristics of shoppers may vary question:“How many VitaPlex do you think was
depending on whether the internet mall is a sold?” The risk perception was measured with
category killer or a complex mall. Specifically, three items of physical risk (α = .93), adapted
the shoppers in a category killer could be more from a study by Jacoby and Kaplan (1972). This
knowledgeable about a specific product cat- was because physical risk, out of the five risk
egory than those in a complex mall, because components (i.e., financial, performance, physi-
shoppers in a specialty store have greater cal, psychological, and social risk), was the most
expertise in evaluating category specific prod- highly related to the purchase intention regard-
ucts than those in a complex mall (Dash, ing vitamins (Jacoby & Kaplan, 1972).
Schiffman, & Berenson, 1976).
Moreover, shoppers in a category killer can
get useful information about a specific pro- Results
duct before making a purchase decision, as Hypotheses 1 and 2 were again not supported
more information tends to be shared among (Table 2). As presented in Table 3, however,
shoppers through the board of a brand commu- ANCOVA, by controlling for product knowl-
nity (Bickart & Schindler, 2001). Accordingly, a edge and involvement, indicated that the
popularity cue in a category killer may function interaction effect between brand popularity
as an endorsement by knowledgeable shoppers and price on perceived sales volume was sig-
and may be more effective in signaling the high nificant, F(1, 99) = 4.64, p < .05. Accordingly,

Perceived
quality

Brand Perceived Risk


popularity sales volume perception

Price

Figure 1 The research model.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


Effects of brand popularity on perceived quality 315

Table 3 Tests of the effects of brand t = 4.42, p < .001) while other independent vari-
popularity and price cues on perceived ables had no impact on it. Therefore, brand
sales volume in Study 2 popularity and price have an interaction effect
Source F p-value on the perceived sales volume, which in turn
influences the perceived quality.
Brand popularity 17.44 <.001
Price 21.68 <.001 To confirm that the effect of perceived sales
Brand popularity × price 4.64 .034 volume on risk perception was mediated by
Knowledge 1.02 .315 perceived quality, regression analyses were con-
Involvement 0.12 .729 ducted three times (Baron & Kenny, 1986).
First, perceived sales volume had a marginally
significant effect on risk perception (β = −.16,
Table 4 Averages of perceived sales t = −1.66, p < .1). Second, the effect of perceived
volume depending on brand popularity and sales volume on perceived quality was signifi-
price in Study 2 cant (β = .38, t = 4.12, p < .001). Finally, the
Brand popularity Price M SD effect of perceived sales volume on risk percep-
tion after controlling for perceived quality
Absent Low 3.56 0.92
High 3.00 0.00
was no longer significant (β = −.08, t = −0.73,
Total 3.28 0.99 p = .465). Perceived quality had a significant
Present Low 4.96 1.14 effect on risk perception (β = −.23, t = −2.18,
High 3.46 1.24 p < .05). Accordingly, the impact of perceived
Total 4.20 1.40 sales volume on risk perception was fully medi-
Total Low 4.26 1.24 ated by perceived quality (marginally signifi-
High 3.24 1.14
cant: Sobel z = −1.92, p = .055). Accordingly,
Total 3.74 1.29
Hypothesis 4 was supported.
Although it is not hypothesized, in order to
see whether the effect of brand popularity on
Hypothesis 3 was supported.3 The average of
brand evaluation can change depending on the
perceived sales volume depending on the brand
mall type, we asked each participant to answer
popularity and price is summarized in Table 4.
the following question in Studies 1 and 2: “The
Moreover, multiple regression analysis was
shoppers in ItsMall (or VitaIn) have a great
used to see if perceived sales volume positively
deal of knowledge about a multivitamin.” Shop-
affects perceived quality. Perceived quality was
pers in category killers were perceived to be
regressed against the perceived sales volume,
more knowledgeable about a multivitamin
price, product knowledge, and involvement.
than those in complex malls, but the difference
The result showed that perceived sales volume
was not statistically significant (M = 3.82, M =
positively influenced perceived quality (β = .30,
3.48; t = 1.50, p = .135). Moreover, the interac-
tion effect between the mall type and brand
popularity on perceived quality was not signifi-
3
This may be interpreted with another explanation. cant, F(1, 196) = .03, p = .873.
According to the Elaboration Likelihood Model,
extrinsic cues such as a popularity cue tend to
affect consumers’ information processing in low- Discussion
involvement situation than in high involvement situ- The findings of Study 2 demonstrate that brand
ation (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983). Low popularity does not directly affect perceived
price may be related to low involvement. This indi-
cates that participants in the low-price condition quality with price, whether the mall is a
may consider a popularity claim to be more impor- complex mall or a category killer. Brand popu-
tant than those in the high-price condition. However, larity and price have an interaction effect on
a statistical difference was not found in involvement
the perceived sales volume, which in turn influ-
depending on the price level (ML = 3.94, MH = 4.24,
p = .245). We appreciate an anonymous reviewer ences perceived quality. The perceived quality
for this insightful comment. increases only when consumers believe that the

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


316 J-H. Kim and D. Min

sales volume is high based on the combination sumer behaviors. Cialdini et al. (1990) argued
of brand popularity and price. Moreover, the that when examining the normative influence
impact of perceived sales volume on risk per- on human behaviors, it is necessary to classify
ception was mediated by perceived quality. This social norms into injunctive and descriptive
indicates how brand popularity as an advertis- norms. The two types of norms seem to be con-
ing cue can reduce the risk in internet shopping fusing, as what most people approve is what
contexts. most people do. However, each norm involves
a different source of motivation (Cialdini et al.,
2006). The injunctive norm concerns rules or
General discussion regulations, according to which behavior is
approved or disapproved. Accordingly, a moti-
Theoretical and practical implications
vational source of injunctive norm is a social
The findings of this research have a number of
sanction. In contrast, descriptive norms concern
important theoretical implications. First, this
the statement regarding typical behaviors.
research is one of the first to examine how the
People use it as a decisional shortcut based on a
“No. 1 in sales” cue functions with price as a
belief that what most people do is a sensible
signal of quality in internet shopping contexts.
thing to do (Cialdini et al., 1990). Accordingly,
The findings of past research reveal that brand
the motivational source of descriptive norms is
popularity in general has a negative or insignifi-
an efficient form of information processing.
cant effect on perceived quality (Hellofs &
Although each norm affects human behav-
Jacobson, 1999). In previous literature, market
iors in a different way, most consumer research
share has often been used as a popularity cue,
has mainly focused on the injunctive norm
and the interaction effect between brand popu-
rather than the descriptive norm. After the
larity and price has not been considered. The
theory of reasoned action was introduced
popularity claims by internet malls have a dis-
by Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), the injunctive
tinctive aspect. A popularity cue claimed in the
norm has been the focus of consumer attitude-
limited context of an internet mall does not
behavior research as it explains the weak cor-
produce consumer concerns regarding the con-
relations between attitude and behavior (Hale,
gestion problem. Moreover, the popularity
Householder, & Greene, 2002, p. 259). Only a
claim usually does not include detailed data on
few studies have focused on the descriptive
sales; thus, the sales volume that consumers
norm in order to help nonprofit organizations
estimate is not fixed. That is, the perceived sales
with public campaigns. The findings of these
volume varies depending on the combination of
studies show that the messages conveying the
the popularity cue and the price level. Accord-
descriptive norm could be effective in persuad-
ingly, perceived quality can change depending
ing people not to engage in undesirable behav-
on whether consumers believe that the sales
iors such as tax cheating, littering, smoking, and
volume is low or high. Only when consumers
drug abuse (Cialdini, 2003; Cialdini &
believe that the sales volume is high would the
Goldstein, 2004; Kallgren, Reno, & Cialdini,
perceived quality increase.4
2000; Schultz, 1999).
Second, it extends the scope of previous
This research examines how the descriptive
studies on the effects of social norms on con-
norm affects brand evaluation in commercial
advertising contexts by internet malls as profit
4
This argument is consistent with the results of an organizations. The findings of this research
additional experiment. It reveals that the “No. 1 in
sales” cue has more influence on perceived quality show that the descriptive norm (i.e., popularity
than the “Hit product” cue (M = 4.84, M = 4.33, claim) could be used as a persuasive message
F(1, 57) = 3.34, p < .1), although the difference is in commercial advertisements as well as in
marginally significant. The participants exposed to
public campaigns. Furthermore, according to
the former cue were found to perceive a multivita-
min was sold more than those exposed to the latter Goldstein et al.’s (2008) argument, this research
cue (M = 5.13, M = 4.59, F(1, 57) = 3.47, p < .1). examines whether a reference group tied to

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


Effects of brand popularity on perceived quality 317

brand popularity affects consumer behaviors. perceived quality. Therefore, a solution may be
The shoppers in a category killer are assumed to that a popularity claim includes a hard number
be more knowledgeable about a specific product when it is presented with a high price. Thus, the
category than those in a complex mall. However, possibility of underestimating the popularity is
this assumption was found to be wrong; that is, expected to reduce.
the interaction effect between brand popularity
and the mall type on perceived quality was not Limitations and future research
significant. This indicates that Goldstein et al.’s This research has five major shortcomings that
(2008) argument needs to be approached with need to be addressed in future research. First,
more caution. the price was manipulated at only two levels:
Next, this research investigates how observa- high and low. The medium-priced brand with a
tional learning affects brand evaluation in inter- popularity cue was not considered. Perceived
net shopping contexts. Observational learning sales may be higher when the price is moderate
has to be distinguished from social learning than when it is low because an extremely low
(Cai et al., 2009); the former involves learn- price can signal that the product quality is too
ing from observing other consumers’ actual poor for usage. Future research should consider
purchase behavior, regardless of time, space, the interaction effects between brand popu-
and social connection among individuals, while larity and different levels of price on quality
the latter concerns learning from sharing opin- perceptions.
ions directly and personally with other consum- Second, this research considers only price as
ers. Accordingly, time and temporal proximity an extrinsic cue affecting perceived quality with
among consumers are more important in social brand popularity. There could be other extrinsic
learning than in observation learning. Although cues that consumers may use in internet shop-
extant research has focused on examining ping contexts. For example, store reputation
how social learning (e.g., word of mouth) (e.g., awards from neutral sources) may poten-
affects internet shoppers’ behaviors, observa- tially influence perceived quality and risk per-
tion learning (e.g., sales information) has not ception (Biswas & Biswas, 2004).
been often addressed (Chen, Wang, & Xie, Third, manipulation checks on brand popu-
2011). This research reveals how observational larity and price were not conducted in Studies 1
learning from a popularity cue affects perceived and 2. Although the advertisement was devel-
quality and risk. oped based on the results of the pretest, partici-
This research could also provide practitio- pants in the main tests might have responded
ners with important managerial insights. It pro- differently to the stimuli.
vides evidence for the recent trend of using Fourth, this research did not measure and
brand popularity as an advertisement cue in include the construct “purchase intention” in
internet malls. It is confirmed that a popularity the research model despite its importance in
cue can be utilized as an extrinsic cue to signal internet shopping contexts. The perceived
the quality of a product. However, internet quality may not lead to purchase intention
malls should be cautious when using brand directly. To address this issue, this research con-
popularity as an advertisement cue. According sidered “risk perception” as a dependent vari-
to the findings of this research, consumers are able as it directly affects purchase intention in
not likely to perceive that a brand is truly internet shopping contexts (Biswas & Biswas,
popular when a popularity cue is paired with a 2004). However, in order to make our conclu-
product that has a high price. In this case, sion more convincing, the purchase intention
brand popularity cannot function well as an needs to be included as a dependent variable in
indicator of the product quality. Conversely, the research model.
when a popularity cue is paired with a product Finally, we should be cautious to generalize
that has a low price, the perceived sales volume the findings of this research because only a
increases, which in turn positively affects the limited number of product categories and

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


318 J-H. Kim and D. Min

internet malls were explored. Particularly as Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, inten-
hypothetical products and malls were used, con- tion, and behavior: An introduction to theory and
cerns about the realism of the study are raised. research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V.
Future research should replicate this research
(2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
with a variety of products and samples, using norms to motivate environmental conservation
real names to increase the external validity. in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472–
482.
Hale, J. L., Householder, B. J., & Greene, K. L. (2002).
References The theory of reasoned action. In J. P. Dillard &
M. Pfau (Eds.), The persuasion handbook: Devel-
Aaker, D. (1996). Building strong brands. New York: opments in theory and practice (pp. 259–286).
The Free Press. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Baron, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator- Hellofs, L. L., & Jacobson, R. (1999). Market share
mediator variable distinction in social psycho- and customers’ perception of quality: When can
logical research: Conceptual, strategic, and firms grow their way to higher versus lower
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality quality. Journal of Marketing, 63, 16–25.
and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Jacoby, J., & Kaplan, L. (1972). The components
Bickart, B., & Schindler, R. (2001). Internet forums as of perceived risk. In M. Venkatesan (Ed.), Pro-
influential sources of consumer information. ceedings of the 3rd Annual Conference of the
Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15, 31–40. Association for Consumer Research (pp. 382–
Biswas, D., & Biswas, A. (2004). The diagnostic 393). Chicago, IL: Association for Consumer
role of signals in the context of perceived risks Research.
in online shopping: Do signals matter more on Jun, S. Y., Huh, J. H., & Hong, S. M. (2003). The effects
the web? Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18, of the types of brand performance information
30–45. on brand equity. Journal of Korean Marketing
Cai, H., Chen, Y., & Fang, H. (2009). Observational Association, 18, 1–19.
learning: Evidence from a randomized natural Kallgren, C. A., Reno, R. R., & Cialdini, R. B. (2000).
field experiment. American Economic Review, A focus theory of normative conduct: When
99, 864–882. norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality
Chen, Y., Wang, Q. I., & Xie, J. (2011). Online social and Social Psychological Bulletin, 26, 1002–1012.
interactions: A natural experiment on word of Katz, M., & Shapiro, C. (1986). Technology adoption
mouth versus observational learning. Journal of in the presence of network externalities. Journal
Marketing Research, 48, 238–254. of Political Economy, 94, 822–841.
Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to Keller, K., & Aaker, D. (1992). The effects of sequen-
protect the environment. Current Directions in tial introduction of brand extensions. Journal of
Psychological Science, 12, 105–109. Marketing Research, 29, 35–50.
Cialdini, R. B., Demaine, L. J., Sagarin, B. J., Barrett, Kelly, H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution
D. W., Rhoads, K., & Winter, P. L. (2006). Man- process. New York: General Learning Press.
aging social norms for persuasive impact. Social Kim, C. K. (1995). Brand popularity and country
Influence, 1, 3–15. image in global competition: Managerial impli-
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influ- cations. Journal of Product and Brand Manage-
ence: Compliance and conformity. Annual ment, 4, 21–33.
Review of Psychology, 55, 591–622. Kim, C. K., & Chung, J. Y. (1997). Brand popularity,
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). country image and market share: An empirical
A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling study. Journal of International Business Studies,
the concept of norms to reduce littering in public 28, 361–386.
places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Liebowitz, S., & Margolis, S. (1994). Network exter-
ogy, 58, 1015–1026. nality: An uncommon tragedy. Journal of Eco-
Dash, J., Schiffman, L., & Berenson, C. (1976). Risk- nomic Perspectives, 8, 133–150.
and personality-related dimensions of store Magnini, V. P., Karande, K., Singal, M., & Kim, D.
choice. Journal of Marketing, 40, 32–39. (2013). The effect of brand popularity statements
Dean, D. (1999). Brand endorsement, popularity, and on consumers’ purchase intentions: The role of
event sponsorship as advertising cues affecting instrumental attitudes toward the act. Interna-
consumer pre-purchase attitudes. Journal of tional Journal of Hospitality Management, 34,
Advertising, 28, 1–12. 160–168.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.


Effects of brand popularity on perceived quality 319

Mittal, B. (1995). Comparative analysis of four scales Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with norma-
of involvement. Psychology and Marketing, 12, tive feedback interventions: A field experiment
663–682. on curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social
Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (1999). Hypothesis- Psychology, 21, 25–38.
consistent testing and semantic priming in the Shimp, T., & Bearden, W. (1982). Warranty and
anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility other extrinsic cue effects on consumers’ risk
model. Journal of Experiment Social Psychology, perceptions. Journal of Consumer Research, 9,
35, 136–164. 38–46.
Park, C., Mothersbaugh, D., & Feick, L. (1994). Tan, S. (1999). Strategies for reducing consumers’ risk
Consumer knowledge assessment. Journal of aversion in Internet shopping. Journal of Con-
Consumer Research, 21, 71–82. sumer Marketing, 16, 163–178.
Petty, R., Cacioppo, J., & Schumann, D. (1983). Tucker, C., & Zhang, J. (2011). How does popularity
Central and peripheral routes to advertising information affect choices? A field experiment.
effectiveness: The moderating role of involve- Management Science, 57, 828–842.
ment. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 135– Wansink, B., Kent, R. J., & Hoch, S. J. (1998). An
145. anchoring and adjustment model of purchase
Richardson, P., Dick, A., & Jain, A. (1994). Extrinsic quality decisions. Journal of Marketing Research,
and intrinsic cue effects on perceptions of 35, 71–81.
store brand quality. Journal of Marketing, 58,
28–36. (Received April 25, 2013; accepted March 8, 2014)

Appendix

Example of advertisements

No.1 in sales

• 29 Multivitamin & Mineral Supplement for 90 Days

• Safe, Gentle and Natural Nutrition


Chewable
• Meet the Recommended Daily Intake of Vitamins
Dietary Supplement
• Take Two Chewable Capsules Daily without Water

Multivitamin
180
Tabs
Artificial Flavors
NO Artificial Color 90
days
Artificial Preservative

Note: Four versions of advertisements were developed according to the combination of brand
popularity and price.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2014.

You might also like