You are on page 1of 9

Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Schizophrenia Research

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/schres

Experimentally induced psychosocial stress in schizophrenia spectrum


disorders: A systematic review
Claudia Lange ⁎,1, Lorenz Deutschenbaur 1, Stefan Borgwardt, Undine E. Lang, Marc Walter, Christian G. Huber
Department of Psychiatry, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background: There is evidence that exposure to social stress plays a crucial role in the onset and relapse of schizo-
Received 14 June 2016 phrenia; however, the reaction of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder (SSD) to experimentally in-
Received in revised form 22 September 2016 duced social stress is not yet fully understood.
Accepted 4 October 2016 Method: Original research published between January 1993 and August 2015 was included in this systematic lit-
Available online 10 October 2016
erature research. Social stress paradigms, reporting subjective responses to stress measures, plasma or saliva cor-
tisol, or heart rate (HR) in patients with SSD were included. 1528 articles were screened, 11 papers (390 patients)
Keywords:
Psychosocial stress
were included.
Schizophrenia spectrum disorder Results: Three main findings were attained concerning chronically ill patients: (1) overall similar subjective re-
HPA axis sponses to stress ratings between SDD patients and controls, (2) no group differences in cortisol response to psy-
Heart rate chosocial stress and (3) an increase in HR after the stress exposure was seen in patients and controls. The study
examining first-episode patients found higher subjective responses to stress and lower stress-induced cortisol
levels.
Conclusion: The results indicate that first-onset medication free patients may show differences in subjective re-
sponses to stress measures and cortisol release while chronically ill patients display no differences in subjective
and cortisol response. This may be the correlate of a pathophysiological dysfunction of the hypothalamic-pitui-
tary-adrenal axis prior or at the onset of SSD and a subsequent change in dysregulation during the course of
the illness. Given the paucity of studies investigating psychosocial stress in SSD and the pathophysiological rele-
vance of psychosocial stress for the illness, there is need for further research. (PROSPERO registration number:
CRD42015026525)
© 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction (Mizrahi, 2015): The neural diathesis-stress model of Walker and


Diforio (1997) (Walker and Diforio, 1997) assumes that stress is a trig-
Schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) are major mental illnesses ger factor for transition to psychosis in patients with a pre-existing vul-
that are characterized by positive and negative symptoms, as well as nerability. In line with this hypothesis, a meta-analysis by Beards et al.
cognitive impairment (Dinzeo et al., 2008). The onset and the course found about 3-fold increased odds of adverse life events in the period
of SSD are regulated by an interplay of biological factors, vulnerability, prior to psychosis onset (Beards et al., 2013). Psychosocial stress can
and psychosocial influences (Meltzer et al., 2001; Norman and Malla, also be regarded as a cause triggering recurrent psychotic episodes
1993). (Mizrahi, 2015). Overall, there seem to be a coincidence of adverse life
There is evidence that patients with SSD are exposed to higher rates events and the onset or exacerbation of psychotic symptoms (for review
of psychosocial stress: For example, it has been reported that patients see van Winkel et al., 2008; Dinzeo et al., 2004).
with SSD often live a stressful social life with limited social support In addition, psychosocial stressors such as childhood trauma appear
and critical family environment (Lukoff et al., 1984; van Winkel et al., to play a significant role for the development of a vulnerability for SSD:
2008). A recent meta-analysis found that childhood trauma scores are substan-
In SSD, stress is not only a consequence of the illness (Rusch et al., tially higher in patients with ultra high risk for psychosis compared with
2015), but also an important factor modulating the course of the disease healthy controls (Kraan et al., 2015). Furthermore, the stress of mothers
during pregnancy – for example the death of the father before the
child's birth (Huttunen and Niskanen, 1978), or exposure to war during
⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychiatry, University of Basel, Wilhelm
Klein-Strasse 27, CH-4012 Basel, Switzerland.
pregnancy (Maslina et al., 2008) – can also be considered as a risk factor:
E-mail address: claudia.lange@upkbs.ch (C. Lange). research found that prenatal maternal stress is associated with the de-
1
Equal contribution. velopment of SSD in the children.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.008
0920-9964/© 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12 5

In addition, patients with schizophrenia report an increased feeling experimental protocols to induce psychosocial stress. Potentially rele-
of uncontrollability in these stressful events (Horan et al., 2005). Uncon- vant articles were identified, retrieved, and assessed for possible inclu-
trollability is one of the main factors in eliciting a physiological stress re- sion in this review by author CL. The reference lists of all retrieved
sponse (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). In summary, current literature articles were additionally screened for further applicable original papers
suggests that psychosocial stress constitutes an important factor in the (CL). Review articles were not considered. The screening of the articles
clinical course of SSD, which can facilitate transition to psychosis, trigger was performed in a three-step procedure: The first step determined
the onset of a psychotic episode, and can aggravate psychotic symptoms whether an article might be appropriate for this review based on the
(Mizrahi, 2015). article's title and abstract. In the second step, full-text articles were ex-
Psychosocial stress reactions have been intensively investigated in amined to decide whether they should be included based on the in- and
experimental settings over the past decades. Inducing stress in an ex- exclusion criteria (see below). Lastly, the selected studies were screened
perimental setting is a good way to imitate real life stressors (Allen et for potential sample overlap.
al., 2014). These tests serve to examine the stress response more direct-
ly, leading to subjective responses to stress, constituting a potent 3.1. In- and exclusion criteria
challenge of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and eliciting
vegetative stress reactions (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer, 1999). The Psychosocial stress tasks had to include an uncontrollable (e.g. time
biomarkers mostly investigated in stress research are cortisol and pressure, unsolvable task) and/or an evaluating (e.g. speech in front of a
heart rate (HR). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA axis) is panel) characteristic. Tasks investigating chronic stress exposure (e.g.
commonly studied as the physiological system that acts in response to caregiving) and naturalistic observations (e.g. class examinations)
stressors of everyday life. It regulates the release of the stress hormone were not included. To be included, studies had to examine at least one
cortisol (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer, 1999). Psychosocial stress of the following outcome parameters: subjective responses to stress,
commonly leads to an increased HR and to the activation of the HPA HPA axis activity in response to psychosocial stress, or heart rate. Corti-
axis, inducing the release of significantly more cortisol compared to a sol levels had to be analyzed with a standard biological assay; only re-
resting situation. ports on plasma cortisol or free/saliva cortisol were used. Studies
HPA axis abnormalities can be involved in the development of examining HPA axis response to biological/pharmacological challenges
psychiatric illnesses and are often seen in psychiatric patients. Panic dis- (e.g. DEX/CRH test) or only reporting adrenocorticotropic hormones
order (Petrowski et al., 2012; Schreiber et al., 1996), substance use dis- (ACTH) as measures of HPA axis response were excluded.
order (Walter et al., 2008; Walter et al., 2011), and depression (Claes, Patients had to have a DSM-IV or ICD-10 diagnosis of a schizophrenia
2009; Holsboer et al., 1992; Horstmann and Binder, 2011) are associat- spectrum disorder (i.e., paranoid, disorganized, catatonic, undifferenti-
ed with a dysregulated HPA axis function. As detailed above, neuroen- ated and residual schizophrenia; schizophreniform disorder; brief
docrinological studies have shown that a dysfunction of the HPA axis psychotic disorder; delusional disorder; shared psychotic disorder;
might also play a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia schizoaffective disorder; excluding drug induced psychotic disorder).
(Mizrahi, 2015; Walker and Diforio, 1997; Altamura et al., 1999; Guest Furthermore, studies investigating only individuals under the age of
et al., 2011). To date, there is a narrative review examining the role of 18 were not included. We included only studies written in English.
stress in general and the HPA axis for the course of schizophrenia Out of 1528 screened papers, 13 papers matched inclusion criteria
(Walker et al., 2008) and covering the literature until 2007, a recent sys- based on the article's title and abstract (cf. Fig. 1). Of these, 2 papers
tematic review of the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal were excluded after evaluation of the full-text articles: one because pa-
axis only pertaining to first-episode psychosis (Borges et al., 2013), tients had not been diagnosed with schizophrenia spectrum disorder
and a meta-analysis comparing HPA axis activation in schizophrenia (Smith and Lenzenweger, 2012), and one because the study did not
and depression (Ciufolini et al., 2014) including only a limited subset use a psychosocial stress task (Dinzeo et al., 2004). Overall, 11 papers
of three studies on SSD. However, there is no current systematic review were included in the qualitative review.
focused on experimentally induced psychosocial stress as an important To assess the methodological quality of the current review, we used
parameter for the development and course of SSD, examining the sub- the PRISMA 2009 checklist (Moher et al., 2009). While seven items of
jective responses to stress and vegetative stress response in addition the checklist pertained to meta-analyses and were not applicable, 17
to HPA axis functioning, and covering the broad diagnostic range of out of the remaining 20 items were fulfilled (items 1–11, 17, 18, and
schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. 24–27), indicating an overall good methodological quality. The study pro-
tocol was registered in the International Prospective Register of System-
2. Aim/Objective atic Reviews (PROSPERO, n.d; registration number: CRD42015026525)
prior to the completion of data extraction.
This systematic review aims to give an overview on all studies pub-
lished from January 1993 to August 2015 using psychosocial stress tasks 4. Results
to examine the emotional response, as well as the two most frequently
employed biomarkers of acute stress response, namely cortisol and The main results including level of significance can be found in Table 1.
heart rate, in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
We included 11 studies examining 420 patients with SSD. Sample over-
3. Method lap was found between Brenner et al., 2009 and Brenner et al., 2011
(Brenner et al., 2009; Brenner et al., 2011), both reporting findings from
For this review, we performed a computer-based systematic litera- the same 30 patients and 29 controls. Overall, 7 studies investigated subjec-
ture search using PubMed and Web of Knowledge. A combination of tive responses to stress, 8 studies investigated cortisol and 7 studies inves-
keywords, one part pertaining to experimental induction of psychoso- tigated heart rate. Only one study examined first-onset medication naïve
cial stress (“cortisol”, “stress”, or “HPA axis”) and one pertaining to the SSDpatients(vanVenrooijetal.,2012),all otherstudiesincludedchronical-
group of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder (“schizophre- ly ill SSD patients.
nia”, “schizoaffective”, “psychotic” or “psychosis”) was employed
using explosion of search terms. Our literature search included original 4.1. Subjective responses to psychosocial stress
work published from January 1993 until August 2015. The start date
was chosen as the development of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the subjective re-
in 1993 preceded a substantial increase of research using standardized sponse to psychosocial stress in first episode, medication naïve patients
6 C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12

Fig. 1. Flowchart of the studies considered and included (PRISMA Flow Diagram).

with SSD. The public speaking task study by van Venrooij (2012) presented findings suggesting a blunted cortisol response (p b 0.01)
showed that Spielberger anxiety scores were significantly higher in (Jansen et al., 1998; Jansen et al., 2000). Brenner et al. (2009) applied
the patient group (p = 0.002). Additionally, patients experienced less a modified version of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) where the
control during the task (measured via 10-point visual analog scales topic of the patient's speech was changed and no recording equipment
(VAS), p = 0.035). Following the task, patients felt more nervous (4 was used. The authors only found a trend for a blunted cortisol response
points vs. 2 points in the state trait anxiety inventory (STAI)) (van in patients with SSD compared to controls (measured via area under the
Venrooij et al., 2012). Studies focusing on the subjective emotional re- curve (AUC), p = 0.062). In a later publication reevaluating these results
sponse in medicated, chronically ill SSD outpatients found that the pa- the authors concluded that there were no significant group differences
tients and the healthy control group both experienced a similar regarding baseline cortisol (p = 0.90) as well as AUCi (controls: 5.0
elevation in negative mood (Nugent et al., 2014) and a similar decrease (±7.4), patients: 2.3 (±8.9); p = 0.21) (Brenner et al., 2011). This is
in positive mood (based on BPRS scores, p N 0.05) (Horan and in line with Lincoln et al. (2015) and Steen et al. (2011), who also did
Blanchard, 2003). While Brenner et al. (2009) found similar subjective not find significant group differences (Lincoln et al., 2015; Steen et al.,
responses to stress ratings between the groups (measures via VAS; 6.5 2011). It should be mentioned that the Steen and Lincoln studies were
in controls and 6.6 in schizophrenia patients) following the psychosocial not classical TSST studies. Yet, the factors of uncontrollability and/or so-
stress exposure, Lincoln et al. (2015) found significantly more stressed cial evaluation were still given and the experimental settings were
patients than controls (measured via 10 cm long VAS scale, p ≤ 0.01), therefore considered as psychosocial stress test in this review. Only
but no group × time effect (p = 0.18). Similar results have been found one more recent study has found endocrinological differences between
by Jansen el al. (higher STAI scores for patients: p = 0.014 resp. chronically ill SSD patients and controls (p = 0.01) with lower levels for
p b 0.05 but no time × group interaction p = 0.255) (Jansen et al., the patients (Rubio et al., 2015), yet this study was also no classical TSST
1998; Jansen et al., 2000). but rather a socially evaluated cold-pressor test study, lacking the one-
on-one interaction with the participants.
4.2. HPA axis response
4.3. Heart rate
So far, only one study has examined the endocrinological response
to psychosocial stress in first-episode, medication naïve patients with In general, while some studies have found no differences between
SSD. The cortisol level of these patients was significantly lower (p = patients and healthy controls for heart rate (31, 39), there is broad evi-
0.042) compared to healthy controls, possibly revealing an already im- dence for a significantly increased HR in SSD patients (Dinzeo et al.,
paired HPA axis in first-episode patients (van Venrooij et al., 2012). 2008; van Venrooij et al., 2012; Jansen et al., 1998; Rubio et al., 2015)
Experimental studies examining psychosocial stress in patients with over all phases of illness irrespective of time of testing. Considering
chronic SSD revealed differing endocrinological results: Early studies the autonomic nervous system stress reaction it has been shown that
from one group using psychosocial stressors in patients with SSD first episode, medication naïve patients with SSD (p = 0.004) (33) as
C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12 7

well as patients with chronic SSD displayed a significantly higher heart situation, therefore leading to a dysfunctional cortisol response (van
rate after exposure to psychosocial stress, indicating a physiological Venrooij et al., 2012). One possible explanation for the HPA axis
stress reaction (all p's b 0.05) (Brenner et al., 2009; Jansen et al., 2000; hypoactivity in psychosocial stress tasks in first-onset patients with
Rubio et al., 2015). Yet, some findings suggest the absence of group SSD could be that this particular patient group seems to experience
differences regarding the ANS stress reaction in SSD patients more anxiety and less control (van Venrooij et al., 2012). Heightened
and controls (Brenner et al., 2009; van Venrooij et al., 2012; Jansen ongoing feelings of fear and anxiety could result in elevated cortisol
et al., 1998; Jansen et al., 2000; Rubio et al., 2015). Only one study levels, initiating a negative feedback inhibition of cortisol at the pituitary
by Rubio et al. (2015) also reporting a blunted cortisol response gland and the hypothalamus and leading to reduced HPA axis activity
(see above) found significantly higher HR in SSD patients than in (for review: Shirayama et al., 2002; Wolf, 2003). This could explain a re-
controls in response to stress (p = 0.01), presumably due to the use duced reactivity of the HPA axis to psychosocial stress stimulation. In
of a different stress paradigm with the absence of a 1:1 interaction addition, this assumption is supported by data of individuals with
(Rubio et al., 2015). ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis. Pruessner et al. (2011) found that
individuals with UHR for psychosis are more stressed compared to
5. Discussion first-episode psychosis patients (Pruessner et al., 2011). Additionally,
when confronted with the TSST, UHR patients release significantly less
We conducted a systematic review on original research covering ex- cortisol than the healthy controls (Pruessner et al., 2013), a pattern
perimentally induced psychosocial stress in schizophrenia spectrum we have already seen in first-episode medication naïve schizophrenia
disorders, examining publications from 1993 to August 2015. Although patients (van Venrooij et al., 2012). Taken together, these data suggest
the stress response in patients with SSD is an increasingly active that individuals with high risk for psychosis as well as first-episode pa-
research area, only 11 studies have investigated the subjective psycho- tients may show a dysregulated HPA axis activity in response to stress
social stress response, cortisol levels, or heart rate in response to exper- compared to healthy controls. However, these assumptions are based
imentally induced social stress since 1993. on a limited number of observations, and future research is needed in
The existing reviews and meta-analyses pertaining to our topic suf- this area.
fer from several limitations, employing no systematic literature search Endocrinological stress response data in chronically ill patients, on
(Walker et al., 2008), not covering the current literature (Walker et al., the other hand, mostly fail to show group differences between the SSD
2008), only pertaining to patients with schizophrenia (Walker et al., patients and healthy controls (Brenner et al., 2011; Lincoln et al.,
2008; Ciufolini et al., 2014), only covering patients with first-episode 2015; Steen et al., 2011). Two studies, however, found a blunted cortisol
psychosis (Borges et al., 2013), employing narrow in- and exclusion response (Jansen et al., 1998; Jansen et al., 2000). It is noticeable that
criteria (Ciufolini et al., 2014), and only reporting HPA axis related re- these findings come from only one group and emerge in the earlier pub-
sults (Walker et al., 2008; Borges et al., 2013; Ciufolini et al., 2014). No lications (published until 2009), while most current studies (published
review was focused on experimentally induced social stress. The pres- between 2011 and 2015) found no statistical differences between the
ent systematic review addresses these issues: literature on first-episode groups. This could be due to improved methodology and increased sam-
as well as chronically ill patients with SSD and published until August ple size in the studies conducted from 2011 onwards. The publications
2015 was examined. In addition to HPA axis functioning, subjective re- with non-significant results included a higher number of participants,
sponses to stress response and findings on heart rate were described. increasing statistical power. Alternatively, this could be an effect of pub-
Furthermore, the methodological quality of our systematic review was lication bias, assuming that earlier studies would have had a higher
confirmed by fulfilling 17 out of 20 applicable criteria of the PRISMA chance to get published if they could show a blunted HPA axis response,
checklist, and the study was registered in the PROSPERO database. whereas newer studies would have a better chance to get published
even with non-significant findings, as the absence of group differences
5.1. Main findings became more accepted in the literature. Although one recent study re-
ported a blunted cortisol response in SSD (Rubio et al., 2015), this
The primary focus of this review was to examine acute subjective, should not be interpreted as conflicting finding, as it is presumably the
endocrinological and ANS response to experimentally induced psycho- result of using a psychosocial stress paradigm lacking 1:1 interaction
social stress in patients with SSD. and differing from the experimental settings used in the other included
Considering the subjective response to psychosocial stress, this re- studies (Rubio et al., 2015). This is in line with differing findings
view shows that first-episode, medication naïve patients with SSD expe- concerning heart rate in the same study (see below).
rience more anxiety and less control during the task than healthy Another biomarker that is frequently investigated in stress research
controls. At the end of the task, patients also felt more nervous indicat- is heart rate (HR). The studies included in this review found a signifi-
ing that they experienced stress (van Venrooij et al., 2012). Reviewing cantly increased HR in SSD patients compared to healthy controls
the subjective emotional response in medicated, chronically ill schizo- (Dinzeo et al., 2008; van Venrooij et al., 2012; Jansen et al., 1998;
phrenia-spectrum patients we found homogeneous results: most stud- Jansen et al., 2000; Rubio et al., 2015). Furthermore, both patients and
ies report no differences between patients with schizophrenia and controls showed a significant increase in HR after exposure to psychoso-
healthy controls (Brenner et al., 2009; Nugent et al., 2014; Horan et cial stress (Brenner et al., 2009; Jansen et al., 2000; Rubio et al., 2015). In
al., 2005). One study found that patients were significantly more all but one study there was no evidence for group differences regarding
stressed than controls in general, but not in respect to the psychosocial stress reaction of the autonomous nervous system in SSD patients and
stress task (Lincoln et al., 2015). It is noticeable that first episode, med- controls (Brenner et al., 2009; van Venrooij et al., 2012; Jansen et al.,
ication free patients seem to be more disposed to a subjective responses 1998; Jansen et al., 2000; Lincoln et al., 2015), and the study reporting
to stress reaction compared to chronically ill patients. Yet, only one significant differences used a different experimental setting which can
study has investigated first-episode patients thus far, and further re- be assumed to be responsible for the different findings (Rubio et al.,
search has to show if these results can be replicated. 2015). In summary, while patients tend to have higher HR in general
A similar pattern of results is evident in the HPA axis data: Endocri- and respond to psychosocial stress with a HR increase, the autonomous
nological studies regarding psychosocial stress found a lower cortisol nervous system (ANS) response to social stress in first-episode patients
response in medication-free first episode patients (van Venrooij et al., and chronically patients with SSD seems to be similar to controls. This
2012). Taking into consideration that this particular patient group dis- suggests that psychosocial stress tasks for SSD patients are suitable to
plays hypercortisolism in their cortisol day profile, it is plausible that trigger at least an autonomic response, regardless of the chronicity of
the HPA axis is not able to additionally adapt to the new, stressful the illness.
8 C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12

Table 1
Summary of studies investigating experimentally induced psychosocial stress in schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

First author, Year Sample characteristics Phase of illness Tasks, outcome measure Main results, outcome, and comments

Brenner et al., 2009 N = 30 patients • Chronically ill patients • Modified TSST • No group difference in subjective stress
rating
• Cortisol baseline in patients similar to
controls (p-value not reported)
− Gender: 24 m/6 f • Subjective stress (self-rated 10-point
• Lower increase in cortisol in patients in
− Age: 30.5 ± 7.3 scale)
stress reaction (t4 – t1) (p = 0.062)
• Cortisol
• No group difference in cortisol in stress
N = 29 controls • HR
reaction (t5-t1) (p = 0.48)
• No group differences in peak of stress
− Gender: 21 m/8 f reaction (p = 0.290)
Age: 29.3 ± 8.1 • Only trend for lower overall cortisol
(AUC) in patients (p = 0.062)
• Significant HR increase at stress measure
for both groups (p-value not reported)
• No HR differences between groups
(p-value not reported)

• Controls also displayed positive


symptoms
Brenner et al., 2011 N = 30 patients • Chronically ill patients • Modified TSST • No group differences regarding baseline
cortisol (p = 0.90) and auci (p = 0.21)

− Gender: 24 m/6 f
− Age: 30.5 ± 7.3 • Cortisol

N = 29 controls

− Gender: 21 m/8 f
Age: 29.3 ± 8.1
Dinzeo et al., 2008 N = 58 patients • No information regarding • Stress/challenge paradigm: unsolvable • Patients: higher heart rate than controls
chronicity reported anagram task in general (p b 0.001)
• HR
− Gender: 48 m/10 f
− Age: 41.4 ± 8.7
• Most patients had forensic history
N = 21 controls

− Gender: 14 m/7 f
Age: 39.4 ± 10.9
Horan and N = 36 patients • Chronically ill patients • Role play task • Similar elevation in negative mood
Blanchard, 2003 • Subjective stress (36-item self-report (p N 0.05) and decrease in positive
questionnaire; COPE) mood (p N 0.05) in both groups
− Only male participants
− Age: 39.0 ± 12.2

N = 15 controls

− Only male participants


Age: 36.1 ± 7.9
Jansen et al., 1998 N = 10 patients • In- and outpatients, no in- • Public speaking task • Higher STAI scores in patients for stress
formation regarding chro- task (p = 0.014)
nicity reported • Higher von Zerssen mood scores equal-
− Only male participants
ling more negative mood in patients
− Age: 27.1 ± 7.0
(p b 0.05) except after stress task
• Subjective stress (STAI & Von Zerssen
(p b 0.10), but no significant differences
N = 10 controls mood scale)
between scores prior to and after task
• Cortisol
for patients and controls
• HR
− Only male participants • Significant test × time × group effect
Age: 26.9 ± 5.8 regarding cortisol response (p = 0.033)
with a smaller cortisol response to psy-
chosocial stress in patients
• Patients had higher heart rate irrespec-
tive of time (p = 0.025)
• No significant test × time × group effect
in HR (p = 0.967)
Jansen et al., 2000 N = 18 patients • Outpatients, no informa- • Physical (bike) and psychosocial • STAI scores higher in patients (p b 0.05)
tion regarding chronicity (speech) task but no test × group effect (p = 0.53)
reported nor time × group effect (p = 0.255)
− Gender: 11 m/7 f
• Lower cortisol response to psychosocial
− Age: 27.7 ± 4.3
(p b 0.01) but not physical task (p =
0.87) in patients
N = 21 controls
• Overall significant increase in HR in both
groups (p-value not reported)
C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12 9

Table 1 (continued)

First author, Year Sample characteristics Phase of illness Tasks, outcome measure Main results, outcome, and comments

− Gender: 13 m/8 f • Subjective stress (STAI) • No group × test interaction (p-value not
− Age: 27.0 ± 5.4 • Cortisol reported) but patients had overall
• HR higher heart rate (p b 0.001)
Lincoln et al., 2015 N = 35 patients • Acute and remitted • Noise and social stress task • Patients felt significantly more stressed
patients (p ≤ 0.01) but no group × time effect
(p = 0.18)
− Gender: 20 m/15 f
• No group × condition differences re-
− Age: 40.5 ± 12.5 • Subjective stress (VAS)
garding cortisol (p = 0.68)
• Cortisol
• No group × time interaction in cortisol
N = 28 controls • HR
(p = 0.29)
• No group × time interaction in heart
− Gender: 17 m/11 f rate (p = 0.32)
Age: 34.9 ± 14.4 • No group differences regarding heart
rate (p = 0.15)
Nugent et al., 2014 N = 65 patients • Outpatients, no informa- • Psychological distress challenge • Patients more likely to quit task early
tion regarding chronicity • Subjective stress (PANAS) (p = 0.005)
reported • No group differences in negative affect
− Gender: 44 m/11 f
in response to the task
− Age: 36.8 ± 12.1

N = 43 controls

− Gender: 22 m/21 f
Age: 38.5 ± 12.8
Rubio et al., 2015 N = 58 patients • Chronically ill patients • Socially evaluated cold-pressor test • Increase of cortisol in both groups
(p ≤ 0.05)
• Patients had lower cortisol levels than
− Only male participants
controls (p = 0.01) in the stress condi-
− Age: 36.3 ± 6.4 • Cortisol
tion
• HR
• Increase of heart rate in both groups
N = 28 controls
• Patients had higher heart rates than
controls as response to stress (p =
− Only male participants 0.01)
Age: 36.6 ± 6.3
Steen et al., 2011 N = 70 patients • No information regarding • Mental challenge task • Generally no group difference in cortisol
chronicity reported • Cortisol reaction (p = 0.06)
• Blunted cortisol response in male pa-
− Gender: 40 m/30 f
tients compared to male controls (p =
− Age male: 25.5 ± 11.0
0.037)
− Age female: 29.5 ±
15.0

N = 98 controls
• Patient group included also bipolar dis-
order patients
− Gender: 52 m/46 f
• Testing in morning
− Age male: 32.0 ± 15.0
• Only one baseline and one post-test cor-
Age female: 31.5 ± 14.0
tisol sample
van Venrooij et al., N = 10 patients • First-episode, medication • Public speaking task • Spielberger anxiety scores higher in pa-
2012 naïve patients tients during the task (p = 0.002)
• No VAS differences before and during
− Only male participants
task (p-value not reported)
− Age: 23 (19–29) • Subjective stress (PSS, STAI, VAS)
• Patients more nervous after task (p =
• Cortisol
0.001)
N = 15 controls • HR
• Patients experienced less control during
the task (p = 0.035)
− Only male participants • Patients: significantly lower cortisol re-
Age: 22 (20–25) sponse to stress task (p = 0.042)
• Patients: significantly higher heart rate
at baseline (p = 0.002) and irrespective
of time (p = 0.004)
• No group × time interaction concerning
HR (p = 0.245)

Age is given in years (mean ± standard deviation, or median and range). AUC = Area under the curve, AUCi = Area under the curve in respect to increase, COPE = measured coping style,
f = female, HR = Heart Rate, m = male, PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale, STAI = Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory, TSST = Trier Social
Stress Test, VAS = Visual Analog Scale.

5.2. Experimental induction of psychosocial stress stress research. The original TSST consists of two parts: a public speak-
ing task (a mock job interview) and a mental arithmetic task in front
Subjective, HPA axis, and vegetative response may be influenced by of an evaluative panel with a microphone and a camera (Kirschbaum
the experimental paradigm chosen to exert psychosocial stress. Some of et al., 1993). This test can lead to a 2- to 4-fold increase above baseline
the studies included in the current review employed the Trier Social in salivary cortisol (e.g. Kirschbaum et al., 1993), and the TSST shows a
Stress Test, which is a well-established paradigm in biopsychological response rate of above 70% for healthy participants (Kirschbaum et al.,
10 C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12

1993; Schommer et al., 2003). A meta-analysis concluded that the TSST further factors have a moderating effect on HPA axis response such as
provokes the most robust cortisol responses to psychosocial stress age, gender (Allen et al., 2014), time of day, and time of assessment
(Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). Overall, the TSST causes substantial (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). For example, older men may have a
changes in cortisol levels, mood as well as increases in HR. It is a reliable, higher cortisol release in response to a psychosocial stress test, men in
effective and standardized tool, which enables comparisons between general release up to twice as much cortisol compared to women,
studies. Furthermore, the TSST allows variations with the purpose of women in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle and with intake
adapting to the specific needs of the participants: variations include, of oral contraceptives release significantly less cortisol, regular nicotine
e.g., a control condition (“placebo TSST”: a speaking/cognitive task com- consumption leads to a reduced HPA axis activity in response to stress,
bination without uncontrollability and without being socially evaluative and early morning testing could interfere with the cortisol awakening
(Het et al., 2009)), a version for children (public speaking/cognitive chal- response or with higher absolute cortisol levels in the morning (for re-
lenge combination where children have to finish telling a story as excit- view see Kudielka et al., 2009).
ing as possible followed by a subtracting task with the panel being Investigating stress related HPA axis activity in schizophrenia is a
instructed to provide adequate positive feedback (Buske.Kirschbaum challenge for research, as psychotic symptoms can involve the loss of
et al., 1997)), groups (includes all elements of the original TSST in groups contact with reality, e.g. through false beliefs (delusions), perceptual ex-
of 6 participants (Von Dawans et al., 2011)), and patients with schizo- periences not shared by others (hallucinations), and bizarre behavior
phrenia spectrum disorder (public speaking/cognitive challenge combi- (Mueser and McGurk, 2004). Those symptoms may cause problems in
nation where the participants are instructed to talk about their physical the execution of the TSST, as stress and fear might intensify the psychot-
appearance without the usage of recording equipment (Brenner et al., ic symptoms, and perception problems and a focus on internal experi-
2009)). ences may decrease subjective stress. Cognitive processes and coping
In addition, several other paradigms can be used to experimentally strategies might also play a significant role in the stress induction in
induce social stress. The following stress tests are used in original re- patients with schizophrenia. Research did show that patients with
search included in the current review not employing the original schizophrenia tend to use more passive and avoidant strategies in psy-
TSST: 1) a modified TSST – a combination of a public speaking task chosocial stress situations, which might also help to explain the stress
and a mental arithmetic task without any recording equipment. The vulnerability of those patients (Jansen et al., 2000). Cognition, on the
topic of the public speaking task is to talk about the optical appearance other hand, also plays a crucial role, as a situation can only be considered
(instead of a mock job interview) (Brenner et al., 2009; Brenner et al., stressful if the individual interprets the situation accordingly, and as im-
2011); 2) an unsolvable anagram task – unknown to the participants, paired cognitive functioning may increase the subjective stress experi-
16 out of the 20 presented anagrams are unsolvable, yet, the partici- enced during demanding cognitive tasks.
pants were told that they would receive money if they solve 5 or In order to overcome such issues, some study protocols omitted re-
more under time pressure (Dinzeo et al., 2008); 3) a role play task – es- cording the psychosocial stress tasks, as this may trigger paranoid
pecially designed for psychiatric patients to evaluate their capabilities to symptoms (Brenner et al., 2009). Furthermore, some research groups
solve interpersonal problems through dialogue (Horan and Blanchard, have decided to exchange the job interview and rather let the partici-
2003); 4) a public speaking task – speaking in public while being re- pants talk about their optical appearance, as patients may not be able
corded (Van Venrooij et al., 2012; Jansen et al., 2000; Jansen et al., to relate to a job interview situation (e.g. Brenner et al., 2009; Brenner
1998); 5) a noise and social stress task – speaking while being recorded et al., 2011). It could therefore be speculated, that the modified TSST
under loud noise (Lincoln et al., 2015); 6) a psychological distress chal- may be less stressful for the participants. Yet, the studies included in
lenge – computerized cognitive tasks (Paced Auditory Serial Addition the current review employing modified TSSTs tended to exert a stronger
Task (PASAT) or Mirror-Tracing Persistence Task (MTPT)) with a combi- stress response, indicating that the changes may have no relevant effect
nation of a loud noise when an error was made (Nugent et al., 2014); 7) on the efficacy of the stress task.
a socially evaluated cold-pressor test – placement of a hand and wrist in
ice cold water while being recorded for facial expression (Rubio et al., 5.3. Further methodological considerations
2015); 8) a speeded mental challenge task while being watched by
the experimenter (Steen et al., 2011). Several methodological aspects limit the results reported in the in-
Psychosocial stress tasks trigger a cortisol response; yet, effects differ cluded studies and complicate their interpretation. These issues mainly
based on the task itself. A meta-analysis of 208 laboratory stress studies refer to the methods of obtaining and reporting the psychobiological re-
found that emotion induction and noise exposure did not provoke a sig- sults and to inter-individual differences in response to stress:
nificant cortisol response, although this could have been a result of hav- Most of the studies included in this review conducted a psychosocial
ing included only a small amount of such studies in the analysis. stress task in medicated patients. Only one study reported results from
Cognitive tasks, public speaking/verbal interaction tasks and public medication naïve patients which show that more research in this area is
speaking/cognitive task combination reveal significant cortisol re- needed. The duration and type of antipsychotic medication differed be-
sponses. Furthermore, the public speaking task combined with solving tween the studies, and it is therefore difficult to draw conclusions on the
a cognitive challenge reached the highest effect size. Additionally, so- effect of the medication on subjective responses to stress, cortisol levels,
cial-evaluative threat predicted these outcomes: when participants and heart rate. Research has shown that antipsychotic medication may
fear to be negatively judged or when the performance is uncontrollable have a normalizing effect on the HPA axis activity in patients with
(e.g. through noise or incorrect feedback), a stronger activation of the schizophrenia. Studies investigating basal cortisol and cortisol awaken-
HPA axis was provoked. Participants' cortisol level increased with the ing response found a cortisol level reduction following antipsychotic
number of social evaluative threats. Physical presence of the experi- treatment (Ryan et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2005; Popovic et al., 2007;
menter, rather than recording the task, also increases the cortisol re- Mondelli et al., 2010; Venkatasubramanian et al., 2010). Differences in
lease (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). In summary, not all psychosocial antipsychotic medication might therefore account for some of the
stressor tasks lead to identical induction of HPA axis, subjective and non-significant group differences in HPA axis functioning of chronically
vegetative stress response, and the strongest HPA axis activation ill SSD patients, and for the different findings between (medication-
can be achieved when using a public speaking/cognitive challenge free) first-episode and (medicated) chronically ill SSD patients. Heart
combination. rate and HR variability are also sensitive to antipsychotic medication.
The repetition of psychosocial stress tests in the same participants For example, it has been found that olanzapine increased HR while thi-
may lead to habituation effects, as has been shown for the TSST oridazine decreased HR (Silke et al., 2002). Thus, differences regarding
(Frisch et al., 2015; Allen et al., 2014; Kudielka et al., 2009). Additionally, antipsychotic medication may also have influenced the HR results
C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12 11

reported above, e.g., the increased baseline HR and the increased HR as a interventions, and to enable prediction and monitoring of treatment
response to psychosocial stress. response.
As we see stress related HPA axis differences in UHR, first-episode
and chronic patients, one may also assume differences within the 5.5. Conclusion and suggestion for future research
chronically ill patient group based on clinical characteristics. Out of
the studies investigating psychosocial stress response, only one study Taken together, these results indicate that first-onset patients felt
performed further analyses regarding positive and negative symptoms. higher subjective response to stress during and after a stress task and re-
Jansen et al. (2000) used the PANSS (Positive and Negative Symptom leased less cortisol in response to a psychosocial stress task compared to
Scale), however, did not find an association between symptoms and the healthy controls. Yet, only one study has investigated these issues in
stress related cortisol release. The patients in the study were on medica- first-onset medication free patients. Chronically ill patients, on the other
tion, which may be reason for not finding a relationship between corti- hand, showed no differences in the subjective and cortisol response
sol response to a stressor and symptomatology of the patients (Jansen et compared to healthy controls. First-episode and chronically ill patients
al., 2000). In addition, the influence of positive and negative symptoms showed a similar heart rate response than healthy controls. A possible
on basal cortisol is also not well investigated. While some studies have pathophysiological overdrive of the HPA axis in prodromal, UHR and
found a correlation between negative symptoms and basal cortisol first-episode schizophrenia spectrum patients and a subsequent dysreg-
(Shirayama et al., 2002a; Zhang et al., 2005; Iancu et al., 2007), most ulation during the course of the illness should be further investigated.
studies did not examine this association. Future studies should include and control for factors that have been
Furthermore, the discrepancy in the described results could be due associated with HPA axis abnormalities, such as comorbid disorders and
to dissimilar patient characteristics. Allowing comorbid diagnoses phase of illness. Studies regarding the influence of antipsychotic medi-
(Steen et al., 2011) or including patients in varying phases of their ill- cation of the psychosocial stress response are needed, especially studies
ness (acutely ill vs. remitted patients, in- vs. outpatients) (Jansen et in medication-free and medicated first-episode patients. This would
al., 1998; Lincoln et al., 2015) may contribute to different findings re- allow a more detailed investigation of the effects of medication on the
garding subjective responses to stress levels, HPA axis response and HPA axis reactivity.
HR. In addition, SSD and even its individual diagnostic groups (e.g., Considering the small number of studies examining the effects of ex-
schizophrenia) constitute a spectrum of different disorders with differ- perimentally induced psychosocial stress in schizophrenia spectrum
ent etiology, course of illness and outcome. Differences in composition disorder and the pathophysiological relevance of psychosocial stress
between study samples with respect to these disorders can be assumed for the course of illness, future research in this area is encouraged.
to have an influence on biological correlates of psychosocial stress.
Moreover, the discrepancy in the described results could be due to
Role of funding
heterogeneous cortisol measures. The sampling procedures were not
standardized as one study relies on only one baseline and one post-
All authors received regular salaries through their primary employ-
stress cortisol sample. This, however, does not fairly represent HPA
er. None of the authors have received special funding for activities relat-
axis activity caused by psychosocial stress and the peak of the cortisol
ed to the preparation and publication of this review.
response might have been missed with such a long time delay (Steen
et al., 2011).
Lastly, a publication bias concerning the studies used in this review Contributions
cannot be ruled out, for example concerning the findings for a blunted
cortisol response in chronically ill SSD patients. CL conducted the literature research and wrote the first draft of the
manuscript. CL, LD, MW, and CGH participated in the discussion and in-
5.4. Implications for future research and clinical practice terpretation of the findings. CGH revised the manuscript providing im-
portant intellectual content. LD, MW, SB, and UEL contributed during
Recent consensus papers have dealt with the question on what bio- literature research and during the writing process. All authors contrib-
markers could be useful as diagnostic indicators for schizophrenia spec- uted to and have approved the final manuscript.
trum disorders and to identify endophenotypes. Whereas biological
markers from electroencephalography (EEG), event-related potentials Conflict of interest
All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest related to this work.
(ERP), sleep monitoring, neuroimaging, and neurogenetics were incor-
porated, psychosocial stress response and HPA axis functioning were
Acknowledgement
not taken into consideration (Thibaut et al., 2015; Schmitt et al.,
N/A.
2016). However, these parameters could constitute promising targets
for new biomarkers and endophenotypes, as we already see HPA axis
dysregulation in UHR (Pruessner et al., 2011). Furthermore, testing is References
non-invasive (e.g., when investigating saliva cortisol), easy-to-perform, Allen, A.P., Kennedy, P.J., Cryan, J.F., Dinan, T.G., Clarke, G., 2014. Biological and psycholog-
standardized and low-priced. Yet, higher sample size and better clinical ical markers of stress in humans: focus on the trier social stress test. Neurosci.
characterization are needed in order to enable differentiation between Biobehav. Rev. 38, 94–124.
Altamura, A.C., Boin, F., Maes, M., 1999. HPA axis and cytokines dysregulation in schizo-
subgroups in further studies and to explore the potential of stress re- phrenia: potential implications for the antipsychotic treatment. Eur.
sponse and HPA axis dysfunction as diagnostic markers. Neuropsychopharmacol. 10 (1), 1–4.
The identification of endophenotypes, potentially together with Beards, S., Gayer-Anderson, C., Borges, S., Dewey, M.E., Isher, H.L., Morgan, C., 2013. Life
events and psychosis: a review and meta-analysis. Envior Schizo 39 (4), 740–747.
other neurophysiological, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and ge- Borges, S., Gayer-Anderson, C., Mondelli, V., 2013. A systematic review of the activity
netic markers could help to identify more homogeneous patient sub- of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in first episode psychosis.
groups with different treatment needs, course and outcome and Psychoneuroendocrinology 38 (5), 603–611.
Brenner, K., Liu, A., Laplante, D.P., et al., 2009. Cortisol response to a psychosocial stressor
therefore also improve prognosis. Possible treatment approaches re-
in schizophrenia: blunted, delayed, or normal? Psychoneuroendocrinology 34,
garding patients with specific reactions to psychosocial stress and HPA 859–868.
axis response could for example include trauma specific interventions Brenner, K., St-Hilaire, A., Liu, A., Laplante, D.P., King, S., 2011. Cortisol response and cop-
and improving resources to counteract social stress (e.g., through social ing style predict quality of life in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 128, 23–29.
Buske.Kirschbaum, A., Jobst, S., Wustmans, A., Kirschbaum, C., Rauh, W., Hellhammer, D.,
competence training). Paradigms experimentally inducing psychosocial 1997. Attenuated free cortisol response to psychsocial stress in children with atopic
stress could be used to choose suitable patient subgroup for these dermatitis. Psychosom. Med. 59, 419–426.
12 C. Lange et al. / Schizophrenia Research 182 (2017) 4–12

Ciufolini, S., Dazzan, P., Kempton, M.J., Pariante, C., Mondelli, V., 2014. HPA axis response Petrowski, K., Wintermann, G.B., Kirschbaum, C., Bornstein, S.R., 2012. Dissociation be-
to social stress is attenuated in schizophrenia but normal in depression: evidence tween ACTH and cortisol response in DEX–CRH test in patients with panic disorder.
from a meta-analysis of existing studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 359–368. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37 (8), 1199–1208.
Claes, S., 2009. Glucocorticoid receptor polymorphisms in major depression. Ann. N. Y. Popovic, V., Doknic, M., Maric, N., et al., 2007. Changes in neuroendocrine and metabolic
Acad. Sci. 1179 (1), 216–228. hormones induced by atypical antipsychotics in normal-weight patients with schizo-
Dickerson, S.S., Kemeny, M.E., 2004. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical phrenia. Neuroendocrinology 85, 249–256.
integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130, 355–391. PROSPERO, n.d. International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviewshttp://www.crd.
Dinzeo, T.J., Cohen, A.S., Nienow, T.M., Docherty, N.M., 2004. Stress and arousability in york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.asp?ID=CRD42015026525 (status: 11/05/2015).
schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 71 (1), 127–135. Pruessner, M., Iyer, S.N., Faridi, K., Joober, R., Malla, A.K., 2011. Stress and protective factors
Dinzeo, T.J., Cohen, A.S., Nienow, T.M., Docherty, N.M., 2008. Arousability in schizophre- in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis, first episode psychosis and healthy con-
nia: relationship to emotional and physiological reactivity and symptom severity. trols. Schizophr. Res. 129, 29–35.
Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 117 (6), 432–439. Pruessner, M., Vracotas, N., Joober, R., Pruessner, J.C., Malla, A.K., 2013. Blunted cortisol
Frisch, J.U., Häusser, J.A., Mojzisch, A., 2015. The trier social stress test as a pardigm to awakening response in men with first episode psychosis: relationship to parental
study how people respond to thereat in social interactions. Front. Psychol. 6 (14), bonding. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 229–240.
1–15. Rubio, G., López-Muñoz, F., Jurado-Barba, R., et al., 2015. Stress induced by the socially
Guest, P.C., Schwarz, E., Krishnamurthy, D., et al., 2011. Altered levels of circulating insulin evaluated cold-pressor test cause equivalent deficiencies of sensory gating in male
and other neuroendocrine hormones associated with the onset of schizophrenia. subjects with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. 228 (3), 281–288.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 36 (7), 1092–1096. Rusch, N., Heekeren, K., Theodoridou, A., et al., 2015. Stigma as a stressor and transition to
Het, S., Rohleder, N., Schoofs, D., Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O.T., 2009. Neuroendocrine and schizophrenia after one year among young people at risk of psychosis. Schizophr. Res.
psychometric evaluation of a placebo version of the “Trier Social Stress Test. Psychi- 166 (1–3), 43–48.
atry 34, 1075–1086. Ryan, M.C., Sharifi, N., Condren, R., Thakore, J.H., 2004. Evidence of basal pituitary-adrenal
Holsboer, F., Spengler, D., Heuser, I., 1992. The role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in overactivity in first episode, drug naïve patients with schizophrenia.
the pathogenesis of Cushing's disease, anorexia nervosa, alcoholism, affective disor- Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (8), 1065–1070.
ders and dementia. Prog. Brain Res. 93, 385–417. Schmitt, A., Rujescu, D., Gawlik, M., Hasan, A., Hashimoto, K., Iceta, S., Jarema, M.,
Horan, W.P., Blanchard, J.J., 2003. Emotional responses to psychosocial stress in schizo- Kambeitz, J., Kasper, S., Keeser, D., Kornhuber, J., Koutsouleris, N., Lanzenberger, R.,
phrenia: the role of individual differences in affective traits and coping. Schizophr. Malchow, B., Saoud, M., Spies, M., Stöber, G., Thibaut, F., Riederer, P., Falkai, P.,
Res. 60, 271–281. WFSBP Task Force on Biological Markers, 2016. Concensus paper of the WFSBP task
Horan, W.P., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K.H., Subotnik, K.L., Hwang, S.S., Mintz, J., 2005. force on biological markers: criteria for biomarkers and endophenotypes of schizo-
Stressful life events in recent-onset schizophrenia: reduced frequencies and altered phrenia part II: cognition, neuroimaging and genetics. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 17,
subjective appraisals. Schizophr. Res. 75, 363–374. 406–428.
Horstmann, S., Binder, E.B., 2011. Glucocorticoids as predictors of treatment response in Schommer, N.C., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., 2003. Dissociation between reactivity
depression. Harv Rev Psychiatry 19 (3), 125–143. of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary
Huttunen, M.O., Niskanen, P., 1978. Prenatal loss of father and psychiatric disorders. Arch. system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom. Med. 65 (3), 450–460.
Gen. Psychiatry 35, 429–431. Schreiber, W., Lauer, C.J., Krumrey, K., Holsboer, F., Krieg, J.C., 1996. Dysregulation
Iancu, I., Tchernihovsky, E., Maayan, R., Poreh, A., Dannon, P., Kotler, M., Weizman, A., of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system in panic disorder.
Strous, R.D., 2007. Circulatory neurosteroid levels in smoking and non-smoking Neuropsychopharmacology 15 (1), 7–15.
chronic schizophrenia patients. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 541–545. Shirayama, Y., Hashimoto, K., Suzuki, Y., Higuchi, T., 2002. Correlation of plasma
Jansen, L.M., Gispen-de Wied, C.C., Gademan, P.J., De Jonge, R.C., van der Linden, J.A., Kahn, neurosteroid levels to the severity of negative symptoms in male patients with
R.S., 1998. Blunted cortisol response to a psychosocial stressor in schizophrenia. schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 58 (1), 69–74.
Schizophr. Res. 33 (1–2), 87–94. Silke, B., Campbell, C., King, D.J., 2002. The potential cardiotoxicity of antipsychotic drugs
Jansen, L.M., Gispen-de Wied, C.C., Kahn, R.S., 2000. Selective impairments in the stress re- as assessed by heart rate variability. J. Psychopharmacol. 16 (4), 355–360.
sponse in schizophrenic patients. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 149 (3), 319–325. Smith, N.T., Lenzenweger, M.F., 2012. Increased stress responsivity in Schizotypy leads to
Kirschbaum, C., Hellhammer, D.H., 1999. Noise and stress - salivary cortisol as a non-inva- diminished spatial working memory performance. PD: TRT 4 (4), 324–331.
sive measure of allostatic load. Noise Health 1 (4), 57–66. Steen, N.E., Lorentzen, S., Barrett, E.A., et al., 2011. Sex-specific cortisol levels in bipolar
Kirschbaum, C., Strasburger, C.J., Langkrär, J., 1993. Attenuated cortisol response to psy- disorder and schizophrenia during mental challenge- relationship to clinical charac-
chological stress but not to CRH or ergometry in young habitual smokers. Pharmacol. teristics and medication. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35 (4),
Biochem. Behav. 44, 527–531. 1100–1107.
Kraan, T., Velthorst, E., Smith, F., de Haan, L., van der Gaag, M., 2015. Trauma and recent Thibaut, F., Boutros, N.N., Jarema, M., Oranje, B., Hasan, A., Daskalakis, Z.F., Wichniak, A.,
life events in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis: review and meta-analysis. Schmitt, A., Riederer, P., Falkai, P., WFSBP Task Force on Biological Markers, 2015.
Schizophr. Res. 161 (2-3), 143–149. Concensus paper of the WFSBP task force on biological markers: criteria for bio-
Kudielka, B.M., Hellhammer, D.H., Wüst, S., 2009. Why do we spond so diffenerently? markers and endophenotypes of schizophrenia part I: neurophysiology. World
Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol resonses to challenge. J. Biol. Psychiatry 16, 280–290.
Psychoneuro 34, 2–18. van Venrooij, J.A.E.M., Fluitman, S.B.A.H.A., Lijmer, J.G., et al., 2012. Impaired neuroendo-
Lincoln, T.M., Köther, U., Hartmann, M., Kempkensteffen, J., Moritz, S., 2015. Responses to crine and immune response to acute stress in medication-naive patients with a first
stress in patients with psychotic disorders compared to persons with varying levels episode of psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 38 (2), 272–279.
of vulnerability to psychosis, persons with depression and healthy controls. J Behav van Winkel, R., Stefanis, N.C., Myin-Germeys, I., 2008. Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A
Ther & Exp Psychiat 47, 92–101. review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interac-
Lukoff, D., Snyder, K., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K.H., 1984. Life events, familial stress, and tion. Schizophr. Bull. 34 (6), 1095–1105.
coping in the developmental course of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 10 (2), Venkatasubramanian, G., Chittiprol, S., Neelakantachar, N., Shetty, T., Gangadhar, B.N.,
258–292. 2010. Effect of antipsychotic treatment on insulin-like growth factor-1 and cortisol
Maslina, D., Corcoran, C., Kleinhaus, K.R., Perrin, M.C., Fennig, S., Nahon, D., Friedlander, Y., in schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophr. Res. 119 (1–3), 131–137.
Harlap, S., 2008. Acute maternal stress in pregnancy and schizopghrenia in offspring: Von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., Heinrichs, M., 2011. The trier social stress test for groups
a cohort prospective study. BMC Psychiatry 2008, 1–9. (TSST-G): a new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a
Meltzer, H.Y., Lee, M.A., Jayathilake, K., 2001. The blunted plasma cortisol response to group format. Psychoneuro 36, 514–522.
Apomorphine and its relationship to treatment response in patients with schizophre- Walker, E.F., Diforio, D., 1997. Schizophrenia: a neural diathesis-stress model. Psychol.
nia. Neuropsychopharmacology 24 (3), 278–290. Rev. 104 (4), 667.
Mizrahi, R., 2015. Social stress and psychosis risk: common neurochemical substrates? Walker, E., Mittal, V., Tessner, K., 2008. Stress and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis
[review]. Neuropsychopharmacology. in the developmental course of schizophrenia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4, 189–216.
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D.G., The PRISMA Group, 2009. Preferred Walter, M., Wiesbeck, G.A., Bloch, N., et al., 2008. Psychobiological responses to drug cues
reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. before and after methadone intake in heroin-dependent patients: a pilot study. Eur.
PLoS Med. 6 (6), e1000097. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18 (5), 390–393.
Mondelli, V., Dazzan, P., Hepgul, N., et al., 2010. Abnormal cortisol levels during the day Walter, M., Wiesbeck, G.A., Degen, B., et al., 2011. Heroin reduces startle and cortisol re-
and cortisol awakening response in first-episode psychosis: the role of stress and of sponse in opioid-maintained heroin-dependent patients. Addict. Biol. 16 (1),
antipsychotic treatment. Schizophr. Res. 116, 234–242. 145–151.
Mueser, K.T., McGurk, S.R., 2004. Schizophrenia. Lancet 363, 2063–2072. Wolf, O.T., 2003. HPA axis and memory. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 17 (2),
Norman, R.M., Malla, A.K., 1993. Stressful life events and schizophrenia. I: a review of the 287–299.
research. Br. J. Psychiatry 162, 161–166. Zhang, X.Y., Zhou, D.F., Cao, L.Y., Wu, G.Y., Shen, Y.C., 2005. Cortisol and cytokines in
Nugent, K.L., Chiappelli, J., Rowland, L.M., Daughters, S.B., Hong, L.E., 2014. Distress intol- chronic and treatment-resistant patients with schizophrenia: association with psy-
erance and clinical functioning in persons with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 220, chopathology and response to antipsychotics. Neuropsychopharmacology 30,
31–33. 1532–1538.

You might also like