You are on page 1of 24

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Int. J. Production Economics


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpe

Critical success factors for human resource outcomes in Kaizen


events: An empirical study
Jennifer A. Farris a,, Eileen M. Van Aken b, Toni L. Doolen c, June Worley c
a
Department of Industrial Engineering, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409, USA
b
Grado Department of Industrial & Systems Engineering, Virginia Polytechnic Institute & State University, Blacksburg, VA 24061, USA
c
School of Mechanical, Industrial, & Manufacturing Engineering, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o abstract

Article history: Kaizen events are an increasingly common organizational improvement mechanism
Received 9 January 2008 aimed at work area transformation and employee development. While many anecdotal
Accepted 29 August 2008 design prescriptions exist, there is little empirical evidence of which input and process
Available online 17 September 2008
factors are most strongly related to Kaizen event outcomes in practice. This paper uses
Keywords: results from a field study of 51 events in six manufacturing organizations to identify the
Lean production set of input and process factors that most strongly relate to the development of
Teams employee attitudinal outcomes and problem-solving capabilities in Kaizen events.
Productivity improvement These results are used to develop guidelines for organizations and identify directions for
Quality management
future work.
Manufacturing companies
& 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 1 week or shorter) (Farris et al., 2008b). In addition to


potential, direct improvements in the target work area,
The study of improvement programs has long been a Kaizen events are purported to serve as a ‘‘just-in-
focus of the operations management (OM) and industrial time’’ training mechanism for participating employees
engineering community (e.g., Chan et al., 2005; Dar-El, (Drickhamer, 2004a), helping these employees develop
1997; Guimaraes, 1997; Gunasekaran et al., 1994; Hales new problem-solving capabilities and increased motiva-
and Chakravorty, 2006; Herron and Braiden, 2006; tion to participate in future improvement activities.
Launonen and Kess, 2002; McIntosh et al., 2001; Van However, despite their popularity and potential benefits,
Landeghem, 2000; Vits and Gelders, 2002). Recently, lean Kaizen events have not been widely studied to date
manufacturing (Womack et al., 1990) has become a—if not (Bateman, 2005; Melnyk et al., 1998). Many guidelines for
the—dominant improvement paradigm, leading to a Kaizen event design exist, primarily in the practitioner
variety of studies examining this topic (e.g., Matusi, literature; however, these guidelines do not appear to
2007; Panizzolo, 1998; Simons and Taylor, 2007; have been tested through empirical research. Until the
Warnecke and Huser, 1995). determinants of Kaizen event outcomes are well under-
Within lean manufacturing, one increasingly utilized stood, organizations will not be able to systematically
mechanism is the Kaizen event, a focused and structured manage Kaizen events to consistently achieve positive
continuous improvement project, using a dedicated cross- results. This paper presents findings from a field study of
functional team to address a targeted work area, to 51 Kaizen events in six manufacturing organizations,
achieve specific goals in an accelerated timeframe (usually where multiple regression was used to test the relation-
ships between Kaizen event input and process factors and
employee attitudinal and problem-solving capability out-
 Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 806 742 3543; fax: +1 806 742 3411. comes. Findings are used to develop design guidelines for
E-mail address: jennifer.farris@ttu.edu (J.A. Farris). organizations using Kaizen events and to lay a foundation

0925-5273/$ - see front matter & 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijpe.2008.08.051
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 43

for future research. Section 2 reviews the literature related needed to determine which lean system designs produce
to this topic, Section 3 presents the research methodology, the most positive outcomes.
Section 4 presents results, and Section 5 discusses study
findings, limitations, and directions for future research.
2.2. Kaizen event research literature

2. Literature review Kaizen events appear to have originated with Toyota,


who purportedly used them to train their suppliers in the
1970s (Sheridan, 1997). However, they did not become
2.1. Employee development within lean manufacturing
popular in the US until the 1990s (Schonberger, 2007) and
do not appear in the literature until that time. Key
Samson and Whybark (1998) issued a general charge to
publications from the current research literature on
the OM and industrial engineering community to put
Kaizen events are summarized in Table 1. In addition to
more focus on ‘‘softer’’ human resource issues, and recent
the limited number of studies, there is no clear agreement
research suggests that many are answering this call
on which factors determine either initial outcomes or
(e.g., Jun et al., 2006; Kathuria and Partovi, 1999;
results sustainability. The methodologies used in the
Korhonen and Pirttila, 2003; Polychronakis and Syntetos,
studies also present certain limitations. Three studies,
2007; Tranfield et al., 2000). In lean manufacturing
Bateman and David (2002), and Bateman and Rich (2003),
research, human resource practices, such as employee
and Miller (2004), do not focus on the relationship
participation in continuous improvement programs,
between input and process factors and event-level out-
cross-functional teams, employee training, and job rota-
comes, but instead on Kaizen event program-level effects.
tion systems, are acknowledged to form core components
Half of the remaining studies (Farris et al., 2008b;
of a lean manufacturing program, at least in theory
Montabon, 2005; Patil, 2003) are based upon the analysis
(e.g., Dankbaar, 1997; deTreville and Antonakis, 2006;
of a single event, while Bradley and Willett (2004) based
Niepce and Molleman, 1996; Panizzolo, 1998; Shah and
their conclusions on interviews with participants from 12
Ward, 2003, 2007; Warnecke and Huser, 1995).
events in a single company, Doolen et al. (2008) studied
Several studies have reported a relationship between
two events in a single company, and Melnyk et al. (1998)
the degree of implementation of human resource prac-
do not link their conclusions to the study of any specific
tices and lean manufacturing success (Huber and Brown,
events. Only Bateman (2005) studied multiple events
1991; Matusi, 2007; Olorunniwo and Udo, 2002; Sawhney
within multiple organizations. All of the studies except
and Chason, 2005; Schonberger, 2007). Other work has
Doolen et al. (2008) and Farris et al. (2008b) focus on the
addressed the relationship between lean implementation
relationship between event characteristics and technical
and employee satisfaction. Findings have been mixed,
performance outcomes, without empirically measuring
with several authors suggesting a negative relationship
human resource outcomes. Finally, most of the studies
between lean implementation and employee satisfaction
rely heavily on qualitative data and do not include
(e.g., Bailey and Rose, 1988; Delbridge, 1998; Delbridge
investigation of the quantitative relationships between
et al., 1992; Fucini and Fucini, 1990; Klein, 1989; Parker
outcomes, input factors and process factors. Doolen et al.
and Slaughter, 1988; Sewell and Wilkinson, 1992), while
(2008); Farris et al. (2008b) and Patil (2003) quantified
others argue that the relationship is positive (e.g., Adler
certain outcomes, input factors, and process factors in
1993a, b; deTreville and Antonakis, 2006; Womack et al.,
their studies but, due to their small sample sizes, only
1990), null (Huber and Hyer, 1985), or ambivalent (Jackson
very limited conclusions about quantitative relationships
and Mullarkey, 2000; Shafer et al., 1995). In addition, the
can be drawn. Only, Bateman (2005) empirically investi-
majority of this work has been theoretical or anecdotal,
gated the quantitative relationships between work area
rather than empirical. (Notable exceptions include Huber
and organizational characteristics and the sustainability
and Hyer, 1985; Jackson and Mullarkey, 2000; Shafer
of event outcomes across a larger number of events. Thus,
et al., 1995.) Further, potential reciprocal effects between
there is clearly a need for additional empirical research on
specific lean implementation activities and employee
Kaizen events.
outcomes do not appear to have been systematically
investigated. Positive attitudinal outcomes from specific
lean implementation activities, such as Kaizen events, 2.3. Kaizen events in the context of team effectiveness
could increase employee commitment to the lean program research
as a whole, ultimately improving the program’s success
and sustainability (Adam et al., 1997; Co et al., 1998; A Kaizen event team represents a specific type of
Keating et al., 1999). Similarly, participation in Kaizen team—a short-duration (generally, 1 week or shorter),
events or other problem-solving activities could help to dedicated project team—which does not appear to have
develop operations employees’ problem-solving capabil- received much attention in empirical research, although
ities, which is crucial to the success of lean systems teams in general have been widely studied (e.g., Campion
(Biazzo and Panizzolo, 2000; Brown and Mitchell, 1991; et al, 1993; Gladstein, 1984; Hackman, 1987; Hyatt and
Dankbaar, 1997; Huber and Hyer, 1985; Safayeni and Ruddy, 1997; Katzenbach and Smith, 1993; Kolodny and
Purdy, 1991). Thus, the relationships between lean Kiggundu, 1980; Pinto et al., 1993; Sundstrom et al., 1990;
production implementation activities and employee out- Vinokur-Kaplan,1995). Furthermore, as Pagell and LePine
comes are not fully understood, and additional research is (2002) note, there are relatively few empirical studies of
ARTICLE IN PRESS
44 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Table 1
Previous research on Kaizen events

Source Study type Sectors Event outcomes Factors related to outcomes


measured

Melnyk et al. (1998) Empirical, Various None Short/finite duration, narrow focus, low capital
observation investment, team-based nature, action orientation, use
of specific and verifiable metrics, repetition
Bateman and David (2002) Empirical, Automotive Proposes a framework None
observation for measuring
sustainability of
technical results
Bateman and Rich (2003) Empirical, field Automotive Sustainability of Resource availability, recognition of need for change,
study of 21 technical results at culture that supports change, consistency of focus on
organizations organization level improvement, Kaizen program champion quality, team
leader quality, management support, employee
turnover, communication quality, measurement system
alignment, ability to financially justify events
Patil (2003) Empirical, case Aerospace Sustainability of Employee involvement, job security, training, employee
study in single technical results needs surveys, standard operating procedures (SOPs),
organization follow-up reviews, time for completion of action items,
strategic alignment, knowledge sharing
Bradley and Willett (2004) Empirical, case Mechanical None Problem complexity, team tool knowledge, team
study in single products process knowledge, team leader experience, facilitator
organization experience, effective internal processes, goal clarity,
organizational stability, team functional heterogeneity,
event scope, lack of manipulation, lack of competition,
rewards for participation, flexibility in approach, action
item list, historical performance data
Miller (2004) Empirical, field Various None Expectancy beliefs, task value beliefs, achievement
study in 4 behaviors
organizations
Bateman (2005) Empirical, field Automotive Sustainability of 5S, performance measurement, employee problem-
study of 40 events technical results solving systems, work area manager support, employee
in 21 empowerment, full-time Kaizen event coordinator,
organizations training in new work methods, work area management
involvement in Kaizen events, top management support
for PI, strategic alignment
Montabon (2005) Empirical, case Durable goods None Team functional heterogeneity
study in single
organization
Doolen et al. (2008) Empirical, case Electronics Initial levels and Goal difficulty, management support
study in single sustainability of goal
organization achievement,
perceived success, and
human resource
impacts
Farris et al. (2008b) Empirical, case Large Initial goal Goal clarity, team functional representation, team
study in single transportation achievement, autonomy
organization equipment perceived success,
human resource
impacts

teams in an operations management (OM) context and and the team chemistry model by Nicolini (2002). The
even fewer involving operations personnel. model contains five basic factor groups:
Likely due to the diversity of team types and contexts,
there is a lack of consensus in the team literature on the
exact set of variables that determines team effectiveness  Kaizen event design antecedents, which are the group
(Cohen and Bailey, 1997; Salas et al., 2005). However, design and task design input factors describing the
there is some consistency in the types of factors identified internal characteristics of a given event.
(see Table 2). Many studies utilize the input-process–out-  Organizational and work area antecedents, which are the
put framework (Guzzo and Shea, 1992; McGrath, 1964), in input factors describing the context of the event, e.g.,
which process variables are assumed to at least partially the support and constraints supplied by other organi-
mediate the relationships between inputs and outcomes. zational entities.
The research model for this study (Fig. 1) is based on the  Kaizen event process factors, which describe group
input-process–output framework and, particularly, the activities and intermediate psychosocial traits arising
team effectiveness model by Cohen and Bailey (1997) from team member interactions.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 45

Table 2
Previous studies of team effectiveness

Source Type of study Factors related to outcomes

McGrath (1964) Theoretical Group composition; Group structure; Task and environment; Group processes
Kolodny and Kiggundu (1980) Empirical Leadership and supervision; Organizational arrangements; Task conditions; Group characteristics;
Technical skills; Group interactions
Gladstein (1984) Empirical Group composition; Group structure; Resource availability; Organization structure; Group processes;
Task design
Hackman (1987) Theoretical Group design; Organizational context; Group synergy; Group processes (effort, knowledge, task
strategies); Resource availability
Pinto and Slevin (1987) Empirical Project mission; Top management support ; Project schedule/plan; Client consultation; Personnel;
Technical tasks; Client acceptance; Monitoring and feedback; Communication; Trouble-shooting
Sundstrom et al. (1990) Theoretical Organizational context; Boundaries; Team development
Susman and Dean (1992) Theoretical Integrative mechanisms; Group processes; Codification/computerization; Project risk; Goal difficulty
Ancona and Caldwell (1992) Empirical Frequency of external communication Ambassador activities; Task coordination; Scouting; Guarding;
Internal processes; Cohesion
Campion et al. (1993) Empirical Job design; Group interdependence; Group composition; Organizational context; Group processes
Katzenbach and Smith (1993) Empirical Group skills; Group accountability; Group commitment
Pinto et al. (1993) Empirical Superordinate goals; Physical proximity; Accessibility; Project team rules and procedures;
Organizational rules and procedures; Cross-functional cooperation
Vinokur-Kaplan (1995) Empirical Group structure and composition; Task clarity; Organizational context; Training availability; Physical
environment; Effort; Knowledge application; Task strategies
Cohen et al. (1996) Empirical Group task design; Encouraging supervisory behaviors; Group characteristics (composition, beliefs,
processes); Employee involvement context
Belassi and Tukel (1996) Empirical Factors related to the project; Factors related to the project manager; Factors related to project team
members; Factors related to the organization; System response variables (process variables)
Cohen and Bailey (1997) Theoretical Task design; Group composition; Organizational context External group processes; Internal group
processes; Group psychosocial traits; Environmental factors
Hyatt and Ruddy (1997) Empirical Process focus; Work group support; Goal orientation; Work group confidence; Customer orientation;
Internal processes
Janz (1999) Empirical Organizational climate; Autonomy; Cooperative learning; Team development
Bailey (2000) Empirical Task dependence; Training; Autonomy; Supportiveness; Rewards; Performance Feedback;
Communication; Conflict; Cohesion
Stewart and Barrick (2000) Empirical Interdependence; Team self-leadership; Task type; Intrateam processes
Nicolini (2002) Theoretical Project level antecedents; Business environment and organizational antecedents; Project chemistry
(group processes and psychosocial traits)
Pagell and LePine (2002) Empirical Team composition; Task and team structure; Technology; Trust; Novel problems to solve;
Operational interdependence
Lemieux-Charles et al. (2002) Empirical Organizational context; Quality improvement practices; Team norms; Process strategies; Decision-
making process
Linderman et al. (2006) Empirical Goal difficulty; Adherence to task strategy (Six Sigma)

 Social system outcomes, which describe the human Hackman, 1987; Sundstrom et al., 1990; Susman and Dean,
resource impacts of the event, e.g., problem-solving 1992), the model further hypothesizes that input factors
capability development and affective outcomes. can impact outcomes both indirectly and indirectly via
 Technical system outcomes, which describe the impact group processes. Thus three types of relationships are of
of the event on the technical performance of the target interest, which can be framed as three broad research
work area, e.g., goal achievement and perceived hypotheses:
success.
Hypothesis H1. Input variables (Kaizen event design
antecedents and organizational and work area antece-
Environmental factors related to the external market dents) have a direct relationship to social system out-
context of the organization were not included in the comes.
research, as they were not expected to vary substantially
Hypothesis H2. Kaizen event process variables have a
at the event level, i.e., within organizations, during the
direct relationship to social system outcomes.
short time horizon of the research. However, data
describing each organization’s external environment were Hypothesis H3. Kaizen event process variables mediate
collected though an organization-level Kaizen program the effect of input variables on social system outcomes.
interview, and each organization was monitored for
sudden changes in its environment. Further, due to scope To identify the specific variables to be studied in this
constraints, this paper focuses on social system outcomes research, published accounts of Kaizen events were reviewed
only. However, technical system outcomes are included in in addition to the studies of team effectiveness. While there
Fig. 1 to make the full structure of the research model has been little empirical research on Kaizen events to date
clear. (see Table 1), there have been numerous anecdotal publica-
In accordance with many of the previous input- tions, primarily in non-refereed journals, conference pro-
process–outcome models (e.g., Cohen and Bailey, 1997; ceedings, industry magazines and newsletters, professional
ARTICLE IN PRESS
46 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Input Factors Process Factors Outcomes

Kaizen Event Design Antecedents


- Goal Clarity Social System Outcomes
- Goal Difficulty
- Team Autonomy - Understanding of CI
- Team Kaizen Experience Kaizen Event Process Factors - Skills
- Team Leader Experience - Attitude
- Team Functional Heterogeneity - Action Orientation
- Affective Commitment to Change
- Intemal Processes
Organizational and Work Area - Tool Quality Technical System
Antecedents - Tool Appropriateness Outcomes
- Management Support - Goal Achievement
- Event Planning Process - Impact on Area
- Work Area Routineness - Overall Perceived Success

Fig. 1. Initial research model.

society magazines and newsletters, websites, and news- quality circles, failed in the US (Lawler and Mohrman,
papers. These publications typically describe the phenom- 1985, 1987). Thus, many Kaizen event practitioners
enon, design suggestions, and example results. To date, suggest that the higher autonomy in Kaizen events makes
the authors have reviewed over 80, primarily anecdotal, these projects more successful than older improvement
sources on Kaizen events published between 1993 and structures. The empirical team literature generally sup-
2007. The next sections discuss how findings from the ports a positive relationship between team autonomy and
team effectiveness literature and published Kaizen event team outcomes (Cohen and Ledford, 1994; Wall et al.,
accounts were used to identify the specific variables 1986), although a few studies have failed to find a
studied in this research (Fig. 1). relationship (e.g., Janz, 1999).
Most Kaizen event accounts suggest that goal clarity
2.3.1. Kaizen event design antecedents (i.e., specific, well-defined goals) is another key task
The design of a given Kaizen event can be described in design characteristic promoting event effectiveness (e.g.,
terms of the design of the target task and the design of Adams et al., 1997; Bradley and Willett, 2004; Rusiniak,
the Kaizen event team. Task design factors include both 1996). Generally, the accounts have further suggested the
task structure characteristics, e.g., team autonomy, and use of measurable targets or benchmarks (e.g. Bradley and
task content characteristics, e.g., scope and complexity Willett, 2004; Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Heard, 1997;
(Campion et al., 1993), while group design factors describe Martin, 2004; Melnyk et al., 1998; Treece, 1993; Vasilash,
the characteristics of team members and the team leader 1993). However, a few Kaizen event accounts have
(Cohen and Bailey, 1997). In the context of Kaizen events, suggested using loosely defined goals, including allowing
three task design factors appear to be of particular the team to develop the goals and even identify the target
interest: team autonomy, goal clarity, and goal difficulty. work area during the event (e.g., Kumar and Harms, 2004;
Similarly, three group design factors also appear to be of Wittenberg, 1994). The empirical team literature generally
particular interest: team functional heterogeneity, team supports a positive relationship between goal clarity and
kaizen experience, and team leader experience. team outcomes (e.g., Doolen et al., 2003a; Koch, 1979;
High team autonomy, typically including actual Pritchard et al., 1988; Van Aken and Kleiner, 1997),
authority to implement changes during the event, is although a few studies have failed to find a relationship
frequently cited as one of the key task design character- (e.g., Gladstein, 1984). The goal-setting literature, which
istics contributing to Kaizen event effectiveness (e.g., has typically focused on the individual level rather than
Adams et al., 1997; Bicheno, 2001; Bradley and Willett, the group level (Linderman et al., 2006), also lends some
2004; Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Kumar and Harms, support for the importance of clear, specific goals,
2004). Conversely, lack of implementation authority although it has generally reported that the relationship
(autonomy) has been suggested as one of the reasons between goal specificity and performance is moderated by
that some continuous improvement mechanisms, such as goal difficulty (Locke and Latham, 2002). The project
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 47

management literature has more consistently identified 1984), but typically experience lower levels of enjoyment
goal clarity as a key factor affecting team success (e.g., in working together (Ancona and Caldwell, 1992; Amason
Pinto and Slevin, 1987). and Schweiger, 1994; Baron, 1990), suggesting a mixed
Kaizen event accounts acknowledge goal difficulty as effect of functional heterogeneity in terms of team
an important task design characteristics, but present outcomes.
conflicting design recommendations. Many suggest a Team and team leader task experience are other group
positive relationship between goal difficulty and Kaizen design characteristics, which may contribute to event
event outcomes (e.g. Bicheno, 2001; Cuscela, 1998; Kumar effectiveness (e.g., Bradley and Willett, 2004). A key
and Harms, 2004; LeBlanc, 1999; Minton, 1998; Tanner proposition in Bradley and Willett (2004) is that teams
and Roncarti, 1994; Treece, 1993). However, others suggest require at least one experienced individual, who may or
that the problems targeted by Kaizen events must not be may not be the team leader. Similarly, based on their
too complex and must be suitable for well-known and study of business process reengineering (BPR) teams,
simple lean tools (e.g. Harvey, 2004; Sheridan, 1997), and Launonen and Kess (2002) suggest that it is the respon-
some accounts contain both, potentially conflicting, sibility of the team leader to supply, either directly or
suggestions (e.g. Bradley and Willett, 2004; Gregory, indirectly, any needed problem-solving skills not present
2003; Rusiniak, 1996). Literature on goal setting has among other team members. In addition, the role of the
typically reported a positive relationship between goal team leader is consistently emphasized in the project
difficulty and task performance (Locke and Latham, 1990; management literature (e.g., Belassi and Tukel, 1996;
Locke et al., 1981; O’Leary-Kelly et al., 1994). However, Nicolini, 2002: Pinto and Slevin, 1987), as well as the
some authors have suggested that this relationship might team literature (e.g., Salas et al., 2005).
not hold at high levels of goal difficulty (Erez and Zidon, Two other Kaizen event design factors ultimately not
1984; Locke and Latham, 1984, 1990). In addition, goal- included in the research model are worthy of brief
setting research has generally centered on the relationship mention. The relatively short duration of Kaizen events
between researcher-assessed levels of goal difficulty is a task characteristics commonly mentioned as one
and task performance in controlled experiments where of the defining characteristics of Kaizen events, possibly
extensive training on the task was provided prior to contributing to success as compared with longer-duration
experimental manipulation. It is not clear that this improvement mechanisms (e.g. Adams et al., 1997;
relationship holds for other measures of goal difficulty Bicheno, 2001; Bradley and Willett, 2004; Heard, 1997;
and other contexts. For instance, Martin et al. (1999) Melnyk et al., 1998). For instance, the organizational
reported a negative relationship between respondent- change literature has emphasized the need for short term,
perceived goal difficulty and task performance when goals visible improvements to generate longer-term support for
also had high researcher-assessed goal difficulty. Earley improvement programs (e.g., Keating et al., 1999; Kotter,
et al. (1989) found that, when training on the specific 1995). Event duration was ultimately not included in the
tactics and strategies needed to achieve the goals was research due to expected limited variation across events.
omitted, subjects with high researcher-assessed levels of The final sample supported this assumption, as events
goal difficulty had lower task performance than subjects ranged from 2 to 7 days, with 86% of the events lasting
without assigned goals. Similarly, Linderman et al. (2006) between 3 and 5 days. Post-hoc analyses were used
found that, while goal difficulty had a positive relation- to determine whether event duration was significantly
ship with team performance for Six Sigma teams who related to Kaizen event outcomes, after accounting for the
closely adhered to the Six Sigma methodology, goal effects of other variables, and no significant relationships
difficulty was negatively related to performance in teams were found. In addition, many published accounts make
that did not. some recommendation regarding team size, a group
Cross-functional heterogeneity is frequently cited as design characteristic. The range of recommended team
one of the group design characteristics determining event sizes is relatively small, from three to five members
effectiveness (Bradley and Willett, 2004; Melynk et al., (Rusiniak, 1996) to 14 to 15 members (Laraia 1998; Larson,
1998; Montabon, 2005). The Kaizen event literature 1998), and reasons for specific recommendations are
generally recommends that the team consist of a cross- generally unclear. Team size was ultimately not included
spectrum of the organization and, particularly, the target in the research model due to an expected limited range of
process (operators, managers, support personnel, etc.). variation. The final sample supported this assumption,
A variety of more specific suggestions regarding team with team sizes ranging from three to 22, and only eight
composition also exist, such as the inclusion of partici- teams having more than 10 members. Post-hoc analyses
pants unfamiliar with the target process or work area (e.g., were conducted to determine whether team size had a
Bradley and Willett, 2004; Cuscela, 1998; David, 2000; significant effect, after controlling for the other variables
Foreman and Vargas, 1999; LeBlanc, 1999; Martin, 2004; studied, and no significant relationships were found.
McNichols et al., 1999; Minton, 1998; Melnyk et al., 1998),
and the inclusion of process suppliers or customers (e.g., 2.3.2. Organizational and work area antecedents
Adams et al., 1997; Heard, 1997; Larson, 1998; McNichols Organizational design factors (or organizational con-
et al., 1999; Melnyk et al., 1998; Tanner and Roncarti, text factors) describe the organizational environment in
1994). Previous team research has found that cross- which the team operates. Typically, this includes struc-
functional teams may make better decisions (Jackson, tures such as reward systems, training programs, resource
1992; Jehn et al., 1999; Lovelace et al., 2001; McGrath, provision, and supervisory systems (Cohen and Bailey,
ARTICLE IN PRESS
48 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

1997). While clearly identified in theoretical models as the group and outside personnel, while group psychoso-
potential determinants of team effectiveness, organiza- cial characteristics are shared knowledge, beliefs and
tional context factors have received less attention in attitudes, such as team mental models, group norms and
previous research than other, more locally controllable team affect (Cohen and Bailey, 1997), which arise from
variables (e.g., task design, group design) (Doolen et al., team processes. Kaizen event accounts identify several
2003a; Pagell and LePine, 2002). However, some previous process-related variables, which appear important to the
research has identified positive relationships between study of event outcomes: action orientation, tool appro-
organizational context and team outcomes (e.g., Campion priateness, tool quality, internal processes and affective
et al. 1993; Doolen et al., 2003a; Hyatt and Ruddy, 1997; commitment to change.
Pinto and Slevin, 1987). Published accounts of Kaizen Team action orientation, or the extent to which the
events frequently discuss several characteristics related to team focuses on implementation rather than analysis, is
organizational context, including: frequently cited as one of the key determinants of event
success (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bicheno, 2001; Foreman
 rewards provided for Kaizen event teams after the and Vargas, 1999; Larson, 1998a; Martin, 2004; Melnyk
event (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Foreman and Vargas, et al., 1998; Sheridan, 1997; Smith, 2003; Tanner and
1999; Larson, 1998; Martin, 2004; Melnyk et al., 1998; Roncarti, 1994; Taylor and Ramsey, 1993; Treece, 1993;
Tanner and Roncarti, 1994; Taylor and Ramsey, 1993); Vasilash, 1993). Published Kaizen event accounts suggest
 resources provided for the Kaizen event team during that events that emphasize ‘‘hands-on,’’ implementation-
the event (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bicheno, 2001; focused activities, such as moving equipment during the
Bradley and Willett, 2004; Kumar and Harms, 2004; event to implement a future state, will be more effective
Martin, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Sheridan, 1997; than events which focus primarily on analysis or planning.
Tanner and Roncarti, 1994; Taylor and Ramsey, 1993); However, it appears that action orientation has not yet
 time and other resources allocated for planning the been studied in the team or project literature.
event (e.g., Sheridan, 1997; Bradley and Willett, 2004; Two other process variables that appear important to
Heard, 1997; Gregory, 2003; Foreman and Vargas, the study of Kaizen event teams are the appropriateness
1999); of team tool selection and the quality of tool use (e.g.,
 training provided to the team (e.g., Foreman and Vargas, Bradley and Willett, 2004; Kumar and Harms, 2004;
1999; Heard, 1997; Larson, 1998; McNichols et al., 1999; Minton, 1998; Tanner and Roncarti, 1994; Vasilash, 1993).
Melnyk et al., 1998; Minton, 1998; Tanner and Roncarti, Few studies have explicitly included the quality of team
1994; Taylor and Ramsey, 1993; Treece, 1993). tool use, although a few have studied the relationship
between improvement process adherence and outcomes,
This research studied the resources provided during the generally reporting a positive relationship (Douglas and
event (management support), as well as the total hours of Judge, 2001; Ghosh and Sobek, 2007; Handfield et al.,
support provided prior to the event (event planning process). 1999; Linderman et al., 2006).
Team-level reward mechanisms were not included, as they The quality of team coordination processes, or the
do not appear likely to vary much across teams within the internal process dynamics of the team, is also frequently
same organization. Training was not directly studied due to mentioned as a contributor to event success (e.g., Bradley
the fact that many events occurring within the same and Willett, 2004; Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Tanner
organization appear to receive similar training, although and Roncarti, 1994; Vasilash, 1993; Wheatley, 1998).
training may at times be customized to the type of event, Team research has typically suggested that internal team
and some organizations do omit training or conduct it coordination is related to attitudinal outcomes and to
before the formal start of the event (Bicheno, 2001; Gregory, team perceptions of team effectiveness (e.g., Bailey, 2000;
2003; McNichols et al., 1999). However, data on organiza- Jehn, 1995; Pinto et al., 1993; Vinokur-Kaplan, 1995), but
tional-level reward mechanisms and on training processes not to objective performance (e.g., Bailey, 2000; Jehn,
were collected during this research, thus enabling potential 1995; Vinokur-Kaplan, 1995). (One notable exception is
future post-hoc analyses involving these variables. Hyatt and Ruddy, 1997.)
In addition to support from the larger organization, Finally, one group psychosocial trait explicitly men-
Kaizen event accounts also discuss the importance of tioned is team member buy-in for the goals of the event
context factors in the target work area, including the (Bradley and Willett, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Melnyk
stability or routineness of the target work area (e.g., et al., 1998; Taylor and Ramsey, 1993; Wheatley, 1998).
Bradley and Willett, 2004; LeBlanc, 1999) and work area Indeed, the organizational change literature suggests that
buy-in to the changes identified (e.g., Bicheno, 2001; affective commitment to change from participants is vital to
Gregory, 2003; Sheridan, 1997). This research studied the transformation effort success (Keating et al., 1999). Team
effect of work area routineness. Commitment from work member commitment has also been emphasized in
area employees not on the event team was not studied, as studies focused specifically on the effectiveness of lean
it appears to relate more to sustainability than to initial manufacturing tools (e.g., Chan et al., 2005).
results (Bateman 2005; Patil, 2003).
2.3.4. Social system outcomes
2.3.3. Kaizen event process factors The team literature has studied a variety of outcomes,
Group process factors describe the interactions be- both technical and social (e.g., Cohen and Bailey, 1997).
tween members of the group, and between members of Published Kaizen event accounts also consistently mention
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 49

the importance of both technical and social system 1998; Minton, 1998; Smith, 2003; Tanner and Roncarti,
outcomes, but have rarely attempted to measure social 1994; Watson, 2002).
system outcomes. In fact, the development of participat- Many published accounts of Kaizen events further
ing employee attitudes, knowledge and skills is often a suggest that the visibility of changes made during the
formal objective of Kaizen events (Sheridan, 1997; Melnyk events, along with the direct employee participation,
et al., 1998; Laraia et al., 1999), although these effects create employee enthusiasm for and buy-in to the lean
have not been validated empirically. In alignment with manufacturing program (Bradley and Willett, 2004;
the knowledge, skills, and attitude framework from Butterworth, 2001; Heard, 1997; Melnyk et al., 1998;
the industrial and organizational psychology literature Watson, 2002; Tanner and Roncarti, 1994). Thus, published
(Muchinsky, 2000), as well as published Kaizen event accounts suggest that Kaizen events can be intrinsically
accounts, this research aimed to study two outcomes motivating, impacting employee attitudes by creating the
related to employee problem-solving capability develop- desire for participation in future improvement activities
ment, understanding of continuous improvement and skills, (Bicheno, 2001; David, 2000; Hasek, 2000; Heard, 1997;
and one outcome measuring employee attitude toward Kumar and Harms, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Rusiniak,
Kaizen events. 1996; Tanner and Roncarti, 1994; Wittenberg, 1994).
Published Kaizen event accounts suggest that Kaizen
events can serve as a ‘‘just-in-time’’ training mechanism
(Drickhamer, 2004a), impacting employee knowledge and 3. Methodology
skills related to continuous improvement. Team members
typically receive training on the improvement tools 3.1. Sample selection
needed to address their goals as a formal part of the
event and then are empowered to immediately apply This research used a multi-site cross-sectional field
those tools, with a trained facilitator available to provide study of six manufacturing organizations (Table 3).
coaching and to guide the team through the solution Organizations were non-randomly selected, due to the
process (Bicheno, 2001; Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Heard, need for access to data from multiple events, as well as
1997; Melnyk et al., 1998; Minton, 1998; Perry, 1995). certain organization-level data, requiring top manage-
Thus, it is suggested that participating employees develop ment buy-in and longer-term commitment to the study.
new knowledge, skills, and abilities that they may apply to However, several selection criteria (Table 4) were applied
subsequent problem-solving tasks, as well as to the to increase the reliability and validity of study results (Yin,
current Kaizen event (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bradley 1994; Eisenhardt, 1989). Within each organization, Kaizen
and Willett, 2004; David, 2000; Drickhamer, 2004b; events were randomly selected during a 9 month study
Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Jusko, 2004; Laraia et al, period (October 2005–June 2006). Three organizations
1999; Martin, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Melnyk et al., agreed to provide data for all events conducted during the

Table 3
Characteristics of study organizations

Org. A Org. B Org. C Org. D Org. E Org. F


Org. description Secondary Electronic motor Secondary Manufacturer Specialty Steel component
wood products manufacturer wood products of large equipment manufacturer
manufacturer manufacturer transportation manufacturer
equipment

SIC code 2434 3621 2434 3731 3843 3443


Public/private Public Public Public Public Private Private
Year founded 1946 1985 1946 1939 1964 1913
No. employees 560 700 500 18,000 950 3500
First Kaizen event 1998 2000 1992 1998 2000 1995
Event rate during research 2–3 per month 1 per month 2 per month 5–6 per month 6–8 per month 1 per month
Percent of org. experiencing 100 90 Data not 85 100 20
events available
Major processes targeted Operations Operations, sales and Operations Engineering All areas of Manufacturing, order
marketing, customer and related organization entry, accounts
service and technical activities receivable,
support, product distribution, vendors,
design, production engineering product
planning and development
inventory control,
process design
Percent of events in Almost 100% 75% manufacturing Almost 100% 70% non- Data not 80–85%
manufacturing areas manufacturing manufacturing manufacturing available manufacturing
No. events sampled (retained) 15(15) 8(8) 11 (7) 4(4) 12(11) 6(6)
Percent of events sampled 100 56 100 13 33 24
during study period
Target sampling percentage 100 100 100 25–33 50 60
ARTICLE IN PRESS
50 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Table 4 the organization does not appear to bias the sample due to
Selection criteria for study participants the fact that the withdrawing organization was from the
same industry as two of the other participating organiza-
Criterion Description Purpose
tions (secondary wood products) and sample size was
Organization All participating To provide baseline sufficient without including events from this organization.
type organizations similarities in focus, Data were initially collected on 56 events. However,
manufacture products of fundamental processes five events were ultimately excluded due to incomplete
some type. However, and metrics used to
organizations in measure performance
data, leaving the final sample size of 51. Thus, only a
different industries were relatively small percentage of data was ultimately ex-
recruited to increase the cluded due to partial response (8.9% of original sample),
generalizability of leaving a sufficient sample size for regression modeling.
results
Comparisons of the excluded events with the included
Kaizen event All participating To minimize learning
experience organizations had been curve effects from events on their known characteristics (e.g., duration, size,
conducting Kaizen organizations just types of goals, type of target area) did not reveal any
events for at least 1 year starting to implement systematic differences, even within Organization C, which
prior to the start of the Kaizen events and thus contributed four out of five excluded events, suggesting a
study to increase the
reliability, i.e.,
lack of bias.
representative nature, of
the internal sample
Systematic All participating To indicate senior 3.2. Instruments and measures
use of Kaizen organizations use management
events Kaizen events as part of commitment to the
a formal organizational Kaizen event program, Five data collection instruments were used in the
improvement strategy, and systematic and research. All instruments were developed in accordance
rather than as a ‘‘single strategic planning of with commonly accepted principles for questionnaire and
use’’ change mechanism Kaizen events interview script design (e.g., Dillman, 2000), based on
Kaizen event All participating To enable an adequate
frequency organizations conduct sample size of Kaizen
previously existing instruments where possible, and
Kaizen events relatively events to be drawn from revised based on pilot research, which included feedback
frequently, i.e., at least each organization on usability and initial analysis of Cronbach’s a for survey
one event per month on within a reasonable scales. Table 5 summarizes the administration sequence
average length study period
and content of the data collection instruments.
For each organization, the mid-level manager respon-
sible for coordinating the Kaizen event program (Kaizen
study period, enabling a census approach; the other three event program coordinator) served as the contact person
organizations requested a lower data collection frequency, responsible for working with the authors to coordinate
requiring a systematic sampling procedure (Scheaffer, data collection. For each event studied, data were directly
et al., 1996). For most organizations, the actual sampling collected by the facilitator of the event. The use of internal
frequency was somewhat lower than the target sampling organizational personnel increased the naturalism of the
frequency due to non-response (see Table 3). Comparison data collected and enabled the collection of data from
of known characteristics (event type, facilitator, etc.) of multiple concurrent events within multiple organizations.
responding events versus non-responding events did not A standard data collection protocol was used to ensure
clearly indicate any differences. consistency across facilitators and organizations. In addi-
Organizations were initially identified through prior tion, standard training was provided to facilitators in how
contact in university–industry partnerships, conferences to collect the data, and data collection instruments were
and word of mouth. The selection criteria were then designed to be as stand-alone as possible, such that the
applied to determine which organizations should be facilitator’s interaction with the team during data collec-
invited to participate. As incentive to participate, organi- tion was minimal. All questionnaires, i.e., kickoff ques-
zations were provided with a description of the study tionnaire, team activities log, report out questionnaire and
objectives and the benefits to the organization. Each event information sheet, included a cover page with
organization was provided with a short feedback report instructions, description of the study motivation, and
for each event, as well as a summary of findings within contact information for the researchers and their institu-
and across participating organizations (provided after tional review boards.
study conclusion). Seven organizations initially agreed to The kickoff questionnaire and report out question-
participate. However, one organization withdrew after naires were distributed to the team by the event
providing data for only one Kaizen event which was not facilitator, who also read a short standard script with
included in the analysis due to insufficient within- brief instructions, which were repeated in the question-
organization sample size. The seventh organization with- naire cover letter, and then left the room for a pre-
drew due to shifting internal priorities, as well as the specified period of time. An envelope was available to
turnover of a key individual, a general manager, who had collect the surveys of the team members who wished to
strongly supported the research. The authors did not participate. The sealed envelope was then collected by
receive any negative feedback on the data collection the facilitator and returned to the researchers via mail.
methods as a reason for withdrawal. The withdrawal of The team activities log included an instruction page and
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 51

Table 5
Summary of data collection instruments

Instrument Frequency Timing Respondents Method Length Measures

Kaizen Once per Commencement of Kaizen event Semi-structured 38 items Organizational characteristics,
program organization study participation coordinator telephone or in- approach to conducting events,
interview person interview perceived benefits from Kaizen
event programa
Kickoff Once per End of kickoff All Kaizen Group 19 close- Goal clarity, goal difficulty,
questionnaire event meeting at beginning event team administered ended items affective commitment to change,
of event members questionnaire team kaizen experience
Team Once per Ongoing during event, One Kaizen Self-administered One page High-level description of event
activities log event begins immediately event team questionnaire template activities broken down by days
following kickoff members and by half hour incrementsb
meeting
Report out Once per End of report out All Kaizen Group 37 closed- Attitude, understanding of CI,
questionnaire event meeting at end of event team administered ended items, skillsc, team autonomy,
event members questionnaire 2 open-ended management support, action
items orientation, internal processes
Event Once per Initial request sent Kaizen event Self-administered 15 items, Team functional heterogeneity,
information event immediately facilitator questionnaire or primarily team leader experience, event
questionnaire following notice of telephone factual and planning process, work area
end of event interview open-ended routineness, tool
appropriateness, tool quality

a
These interviews were not used to draw inferences about individual Kaizen events, but to collect data on organizational characteristics (see Table 3)
and to better understand the organizational context of the events studied.
b
Data from the team activities log were not directly used in the analyses presented in this paper, but were used to help the research team better
understand the context and activities of each event.
c
Note that the understanding of CI and skills scales were ultimately combined in the kaizen capabilities scale.

two examples of completed logs. At the end of the event, a nature but difficult to measure through other means or to
team member volunteer returned the completed activities normalize across events. Finally, some variables, i.e., team
log to the facilitator in a sealed envelope. functional heterogeneity, team kaizen experience, team
The team facilitator was asked to complete the event leadership experience, event planning process, were mea-
information questionnaire electronically and return it to sured through factual (objective) data. The potential for
the research team within 1 week of receiving it. Reminder bias due to the use of perceptual measures was addressed
emails were sent if the event information questionnaire by using multiple types of measures (perceptual and non-
was not received within 2 weeks of the initial request. In perceptual), multiple data sources (facilitator and team
cases where the event information questionnaire was still members), multiple respondents per team, and previously
not received 4 weeks after the reminders, a follow-up via tested instruments, as well by cross-validating data,
phone was made to collect the data using an alternative conducting tests of reliability and validity, and analyzing
method (phone interview). As the data collected via the the consistency of responses for individuals within the
event information questionnaire are largely factual, this same team.
mixed method approach appears warranted and unlikely
to create undue bias in the data.
In addition to the data collected through the five 3.3. Construct validity
instruments, a copy of the actual report out presentation
created by each team was collected. The report out Exploratory factor analysis was used to evaluate the
presentations were not directly used in the analyses construct validity of the multi-item scales. All factor
reported in this paper; however, they were reviewed by analyses were conducted using principal components
the researchers to verify the accuracy of data collected for extraction with oblique (direct quartimin) rotation, to
certain variables, e.g., team functional heterogeneity, team allow correlation between factors hypothesized to be
goals, to provide more detailed descriptions of events for related (Fabrigar et al., 1999, Finch, 2006). The established
post-hoc analyses, and to create feedback reports for heuristic of extracting all factors with eigenvalues greater
participating organizations. than 1.0 (Field, 2005; Johnson, 1998) was used to
Most variables were operationalized as multi-item determine the number of factors in each analysis. In the
scales, which, where possible, were based on previously factor analyses, individual items were considered to have
existing instruments. Several of the variables measured loaded onto a given factor when the primary loading was
through perceptual scales, e.g., affective commitment to 0.500 or greater and all cross-loadings were less than
change, are inherently perceptual in nature and cannot be 0.300 (Kline, 1994).
accurately measured through other means. Other vari- The initial data set contained 347 kickoff question-
ables measured through perceptual scales, e.g., manage- naires and 305 report out questionnaires. Prior to
ment support and action orientation, are more objective in performing further analyses, individual-level data were
ARTICLE IN PRESS
52 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

screened for evidence of systematic response bias and for Table 8


missing data at the item level. This screening resulted in Pattern matrix for factor analysis of report out questionnaire outcome
scalesa
the removal of a small proportion of individual-level data
(o1% for the kickoff questionnaire and 8% for the report Component
out questionnaire). Three factor analyses were performed:
kickoff questionnaire measures, report out questionnaire 1 2 3
input and process measures, and report out questionnaire
UCI1 0.829 0.000 0.031
outcome measures. Tables 6–8 present the results of the SK3 0.804 0.207 0.028
factor analyses. For each item label in the tables, the SK1 0.780 0.032 0.094
letters refer to the scale name, while the number refers to UCI4 0.767 0.107 0.012
the item ID. For instance, GC1 refers to the first item in the AT1 0.759 0.078 0.014
UCI3 0.755 0.067 0.010
original goal clarity scale (see the Appendix A for a full list
UCI2 0.713 0.040 0.172
of items by scale). In cases where all the items in a given AT3 0.707 0.164 0.032
scale failed to load onto a single factor, or items from IMA4 0.571 0.299 0.055
different scales loaded onto a single factor, content IMA2 0.118 0.835 0.082
IMA3 0.031 0.808 0.218
IMA1 0.018 0.741 0.272
SK2 0.445 0.489 0.161
Table 6 AT4 0.004 0.001 0.848
Pattern matrix for factor analysis of kickoff questionnaire scalesa AT2 0.205 0.009 0.712
SK4 0.105 0.173 0.607
Component
a
Bold text indicates that the item loads onto the given component.
1 2 3

ACC6 0.774 0.128 0.022


ACC1 0.770 0.019 0.061
ACC5 0.756 0.079 0.055
analysis was used to verify the loading patterns and
ACC4 0.722 0.019 0.154 propose interpretations for the resulting factors. Con-
ACC3 0.715 0.086 0.077 firmatory factor analysis was also used to verify the final
ACC2 0.711 0.081 0.130 scales (Farris, 2008a).
GDF2 0.060 0.862 0.059
As shown in Table 6, in the kickoff questionnaire factor
GDF1 0.043 0.843 0.026
GDF3 0.003 0.747 0.074 analysis, three underlying factors were extracted, and
GDF4 0.122 0.723 0.025 these factors exactly matched the content of the three
GC3 0.013 0.046 0.881 multi-item scales as originally designed. Thus, the original
GC1 0.024 0.030 0.875 constructions of all three scales (goal clarity, goal difficulty,
GC4 0.098 0.026 0.808
GC2 0.001 0.007 0.754
and affective commitment to change) were used in further
analyses.
a
Bold text indicates that the item loads onto the given component. As Table 7 shows, the factor analysis of the report out
questionnaire input and process factors measures gener-
ally supported the construction of the scales but
Table 7 suggested some revisions to the original constructions.
Pattern matrix for factor analysis of report out questionnaire input and In the factor analysis, all five of the internal processes items
process scalesa
(IP1–IP5) loaded onto a single factor (component one),
Component supporting the initial construction of this scale. Mean-
while, three of the five management support items (MS3,
1 2 3 4 MS2, and MS5) loaded onto a single factor (component
two), three of the four team autonomy items (TA1, TA2, and
IP3 0.882 0.096 0.152 0.055
IP2 0.872 0.051 0.041 0.004
TA3) loaded onto a single factor (component four), and
IP4 0.850 0.094 0.015 0.109 two of the four action orientation items (AO1, AO2) loaded
IP5 0.617 0.075 0.102 0.085 onto a single factor (component two), suggesting the need
IP1 0.591 0.158 0.060 0.201 to remove the other items from these scales (see the
AO4 0.423 0.066 0.119 0.028
Appendix A).
AO2 0.090 0.861 0.062 0.036
AO1 0.223 0.733 0.169 0.059 As Table 8 shows, the loading pattern produced by the
MS3 0.025 0.046 0.891 0.029 factor analysis of report out questionnaire outcome
MS2 0.021 0.033 0.839 0.003 measures was the most complex in the study, but also
MS5 0.115 0.148 0.655 0.121 generally supported the scale construction. The report out
MS1 0.160 0.009 0.396 0.350
questionnaire originally contained three scales designed
TA3 0.037 0.054 0.014 0.830
TA2 0.132 0.139 0.013 0.794 to measure the human resource outcomes of Kaizen
AO3 0.001 0.301 0.235 0.763 events:
TA1 0.054 0.052 0.276 0.508
MS4 0.153 0.181 0.280 0.481
 understanding of continuous improvement (UCI), designed
TA4 0.025 0.003 0.266 0.479
to measure the increase in team members’ knowledge of
a
Bold text indicates that the item loads onto the given component. the philosophy of continuous improvement;
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 53

 skills (SK), designed to measure the increase in team AT1, AT3, and IMA4). Although the emergent factor
members’ problem-solving skills; and consisted of items from different scales, it appeared to
 attitude (AT), designed to measure impact on team contain a consistent focus on employee problem-solving
members’ motivation. capability development. To differentiate it from the
previous knowledge (UCI) and skills (SK) scales, a new
label, kaizen capabilities (KC), was developed. The only
However, the factor analysis suggested only two item that did not load cleanly onto any factor was SK2,
underlying dimensions. All the items related to affective which was therefore removed from further analyses.
response toward Kaizen event participation (AT2, AT4, and Although examination of the items in the final attitude
SK4) loaded onto a single factor (component three). This and kaizen capabilities scales (Table 9) supports the
revised scale measures employee affect for Kaizen event content validity of the revised scales, the scales should
activities and thus the original attitude (AT) name was be tested in future research, and it is possible that further
retained. A second factor (component one) also emerged, revisions may be warranted. The factor analysis also
consisting of items related to growth in employee included a fourth scale from the report out questionnaire,
problem-solving capabilities, including understanding of impact on area (IMA), designed to measure event impact
continuous improvement, problem-solving skills, moti- on the target work area, a technical system outcome. A
vation to examine daily work, and ability to work in third factor (component three), including three out of four
problem-solving teams (UCI1, UCI2, UCI3, UCI4, SK1, SK3, IMA items, also emerged from the factor analysis.

3.4. Reliability
Table 9
Final construction of attitude and kaizen capabilities scales
Following the factor analyses, Cronbach’s a (Cronbach,
Kaizen capabilities (KC) Attitude (AT) 1951) was used to evaluate the reliability of the final,
revised scales. The a values (see Table 10) were evaluated
 Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team  Most of our
against the commonly applied thresholds of 0.70 for
members’ knowledge of what continuous team members
improvement is. (UCI1) liked being established scales (Nunnally, 1978) and 0.60 for newly
 In general, this Kaizen event increased our part of this developed scales (deVellis, 1991). All scales except one
team members’ knowledge of how Kaizen event. had a values greater than 0.70 and most scales (six out of
continuous improvement can be applied. (AT2)
nine) had a values of 0.80 or greater. The final action
(UC2)  Most members
 Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team of our team
orientation scale had an a of 0.64. Although this a value is
members’ knowledge of the need for would like to acceptable for newly developed scales, particularly given
continuous improvement. (UCI3) be part of the small number of items in this scale (two), this value
 In general, this Kaizen event increased our Kaizen events suggests that the action orientation measure could benefit
team members’ knowledge of our role in in the future.
from further refinement in future research.
continuous improvement. (UCI4) (AT4)
 Most of our team members can communicate  In general, our
new ideas about improvements as a result of Kaizen event 3.5. Team-level properties
participation in this Kaizen event. (SK1) team members
 Most of our team members gained new skills are
as a result of participation in this Kaizen comfortable The theoretical unit of analysis in this research was the
event. (SK3) working with team. To support inferences at the team level, all study
 In general, this Kaizen event motivated the others to measures used the team as the referent. However, for the
members of our team to perform better. (AT1) identify
measures of team perceptions, questionnaires were
 Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team improvements
members’ interest in our work. (AT3) in this work administered to all members within a team to increase
 Overall, this Kaizen event helped people in area. (SK4) the reliability of the data collected (Chan, 1998). Thus, it
this area work together to improve was necessary to verify that the actual data collected
performance. (IMA4)
demonstrated evidence of shared, team-level properties.

Table 10
Summary of psychometric properties of multi-item scales

Measure Low. prim. load. Cron. a ICC(1) (p) Avg.rwg Percentage rwg 40.70

Goal clarity (GC) 0.754 0.86 0.060 (0.046) 0.88 94


Goal difficulty (GDF) 0.723 0.81 0.216 (o0.001) 0.83 88
Affective commitment to change (ACC) 0.711 0.86 0.173 (o0.001) 0.94 96
Internal processes (IP) 0.591 0.86 0.326 (o0.001) 0.97 100
Management support (MS) 0.655 0.80 0.125 (0.003) 0.88 90
Team autonomy (TA) 0.508 0.79 0.185 (o0.001) 0.88 98
Action orientation (AO) 0.733 0.64 0.373 (o0.001) 0.76 75
Kaizen capabilities (KC) 0.571 0.93 0.121 (o0.001) 0.98 100
Attitude (AT) 0.607 0.74 0.300 (o0.001) 0.90 96
ARTICLE IN PRESS
54 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Interrater agreement (rwg) (James et al. 1984, 1993) and direct relationships (H1–H2), while mediation analysis
the intraclass correlation coefficient (1), ICC(1), were used (Judd and Kenny, 1981; Baron and Kenny, 1986; Kenny
to determine whether the data collected on individual et al., 1998) was used to test indirect relationships (H3).
team members via the kickoff and report out question- Due to the nested structure of the data, it could not be
naires demonstrated evidence of shared team-level prop- assumed that the data for teams within a given organiza-
erties, and therefore could justifiably be aggregated. tion were uncorrelated (Kenny and Judd, 1986), a funda-
Interrater agreement, rwg, was evaluated using the mental and necessary assumption of multiple regression
commonly applied threshold of 0.70, which is interpreted using ordinary least squares (OLS) estimation (Neter et al.,
as strong within-group agreement (George, 1990; Klein 1996). Correlations between observations can result in
and Kozlowski, 2000; Van Mierlo et al., 2006). For each biased standard error estimates and, therefore, spurious
study variable, both the average rwg and the proportion of statistical test results, although estimates of the regres-
teams with rwg greater than 0.70 were analyzed. As Table 10 sion parameters themselves remain asymptotically un-
indicates, all kickoff and report out survey variables had biased (Hox, 1994; Lawal, 2003).
an average rwg value greater than 0.70, and further had at Therefore, generalized estimating equations (GEE)
least 75% of individual team rwg values greater than 0.70. (Liang and Zeger, 1986), executed in SAS 9.1.3 using PROC
In addition, for seven out of nine teams, 90% or more of GENMOD, were used to account for correlation between
individual team rwg values were greater than 0.70. Thus, teams within the same organization. GEE is a quasi-
an examination of rwg supports the existence of team- likelihood estimation procedure, which can be used with
level properties for all kickoff and report out questionnaire regression models where the response variable comes
variables. In addition, the range of rwg within each team from a distribution within the exponential family (Horton
was examined to determine whether any teams displayed and Lipsitz, 1999). The GEE procedure uses a correlation
low rwg on more than 50% of the perceptual measures, matrix to model the association between clustered
which might suggest that the team be removed from observations in the data set, e.g., repeated measures data,
the research, as aggregation of individual-level data to the individuals within groups, etc., when developing para-
team level might not be appropriate in such a case. meter and standard error estimates, often resulting in
However, no such cases were found. differences in the estimates for parameter standard errors
ICC(1) was calculated using the Bartko (1976) formula- for GEE versus OLS. However, since the parameter
tion, where MSB is the between-team mean square, MSW estimates are asymptotically unbiased for both OLS and
is the within-team mean square and k is the average team GEE, GEE models usually produce similar parameter
size from analysis of variance (ANOVA). As the data set estimates to OLS regression models. GEE has seen recent,
contained two levels of nesting (teams within organiza- growing use in the life and social sciences (Hardin and
tions), a nested ANOVA was used to control for error Hilbe, 2003), e.g., Pickering and Kisangani (2005), Szino-
due to correlation between teams within a given organi- vacz and Davey (2001), Whitford and Yates (2003), where
zation (Jetten et al., 2002). A significant ANOVA with hierarchically nested data are common. For each regres-
team as the main effect indicates that ICC(1) is signi- sion in this research, the GEE model used a Gaussian link
ficant (Bliese, 2000; Klein and Kozlowski, 2000), support- function, assuming a normally distributed dependent
ing the presence of shared team-level properties for variable, and an exchangeable correlation matrix, assum-
the given measure. In addition, the magnitude of ICC(1) ing equal correlation between all observations within a
was evaluated using the commonly applied thresholds of given cluster, i.e., teams within a given organization. Other
0.200, indicating strong team-level properties (Molleman, options for modeling the correlation matrix include
2005), and 0.100, indicating weaker team-level properties autoregressive and similar time-based correlation struc-
(James, 1982; Molleman, 2005; Schneider et al., 1998). As tures, none of which appeared to be strictly applicable to
shown in Table 10, except for one variable, all kickoff and the current data set, especially given differences in timing,
report out questionnaire variables had significant ICC(1) i.e. the interval between events, across organizations.
values that were greater than 0.100. In addition, four out Other approaches for modeling nested data include
of nine variables had ICC(1) values greater than 0.200. For SEM (e.g., Koufteros and Marcoulides, 2006) and hier-
one variable, goal clarity, the ICC(1) value was lower than archical linear modeling, HLM (e.g., Raudenbush and Byrk,
the threshold of 0.100 but still significant. This, combined 2002), both of which require larger sample sizes, e.g.,
with the rwg values, led the authors to conclude that the 50–100 observations per level. Due to the exploratory
inference of team-level properties was justified for goal nature of this research, a relatively large number of
clarity on the balance of the evidence. candidate predictors were tested, and the sample size was
fairly small at the organization level.
MSB  MSW
ICCð1Þ ¼ (1) Sections 4.1 and 4.2 describe the regressions used to
MSB þ ½ðk  1Þ  MSW
identify direct and indirect predictors, respectively. Prior
to building the regression models, the distributions of all
4. Results study variables were examined to determine whether any
variables demonstrated severe departures from normality.
Regression modeling was used to identify which event In the cases of three variables, team leader experience,
input and process factors were most strongly related to team kaizen experience, and event planning process, a log
each of the two human resource outcomes (attitude and transformation was used to reduce the variance and
kaizen capabilities). Multiple regression was used to test absolute distance to outlying observations. The variance
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 55

inflation factor (VIF) was also calculated for all 14 input Table 11
and process variables to determine whether multi- Regression analysis for direct predictors of attitude (AT) and kaizen
capabilities (KC)
collinearity appeared to be problematic in the current
data set. A single VIF greater than 10.00 (Neter et al., 1996) Variable Model 1 (y ¼ AT) Model 2 (y ¼ KC)
or an average VIF greater than 3.00 is generally taken to
_ _
indicate a multi-collinearity problem. In the current b
p
b
p
research, the maximum VIF was 2.91, while the average
VIF was 1.83. Thus, multi-collinearity was deemed not to Intercept 0.467 0.447 0.219 0.687
Team functional heterogeneity 0.547 0.012
be problematic. The VIF values were also calculated for the
Management support 0.250 0.013
variables in each final regression model, and these values Internal processes 0.694 o0.001 0.465 o0.001
were also well below the suggested thresholds. Goal difficulty 0.119 0.032
Team autonomy 0.234 0.014
Team kaizen experience 0.398 o0.001
4.1. Identification of direct predictors of outcomes Team leader experience 0.195 0.020
Work area routineness 0.094 0.002
Affective commitment to change 0.222 0.049
Due to the exploratory nature of this research, no R2 0.605 0.706
established hierarchy of predictor variable importance R2a 0.580 0.658
a
existed. Therefore, an exploratory regression (e.g. Olor- r 0.019 0.071

unniwo and Udo, 2002) with a variable selection proce- a


This value is the intraclass correlation coefficient, which measures
dure was used to build the models. A backward selection the extent of correlation between model residuals from teams within the
procedure was used, because it is less likely to result in same organization.
the exclusion of important variables than a forward or
stepwise selection procedure (Neter et al., 1996; Field,
2005). At each step in the selection procedure, if the shown, three variables were significant predictors of
p-value for one or more variables was greater than employee attitude toward events:
a ¼ 0.10/k, where k is the number of parameters in the
model (i.e., the number of predictor variables plus one),
 internal processes (b ¼ 0.694, po0.001);
the variable with the largest p-value was removed. This
 management support (b ¼ 0.250, p ¼ 0.013); and
procedure was repeated until all remaining variables were
 team functional heterogeneity (b ¼ 0.547, p ¼ 0.012).
significant at the a ¼ 0.10/k level.
After the initial model was developed, auxiliary search
methods, e.g., automated OLS-based searches using Cp, R2, The attitude model R2 was 0.61 and the adjusted R2
and adjusted R2, were used to test the robustness of was 0.58, indicating that the model explains approxi-
the final solution and to identify additional models for mately 60% of the variance in the outcome. The three-
consideration. A candidate model was considered to be and two-way interactions of the predictors in the final
viable if all variables had p-values less than 0.05 and most model were also tested to determine whether there were
had p-values less than 0.10/k. A conservative approach, i.e., any significant interaction effects; however, none were
using small p-values, was used due to the fact that found.
an overall test for the significance of the regression cannot As shown in Table 11, seven variables were significant
be accurately calculated for quasi-likelihood estimation predictors of kaizen capabilities:
methods such as GEE in standard statistical software
(Hardin and Hilbe, 2003; SAS Institute Inc., 2006).  internal processes (b ¼ 0.465, po0.001);
Following each regression, R2, adjusted R2, and residual  team autonomy (b ¼ 0.234, p ¼ 0.014);
analysis were used to examine model fit. Potential  affective commitment to change (b ¼ 0.222, p ¼ 0.049);
departures from linearity and normality were evaluated  goal difficulty (b ¼ 0.119, p ¼ 0.032);
through the examination of residual plots and partial  work area routineness (b ¼ 0.094, p ¼ 0.002);
regression plots and through Wald–Wolfowitz runs tests,  team kaizen experience (b ¼ 0.398, po0.001); and
all of which have been recommended for use with GEE  team leader experience (b ¼ 0.195, p ¼ 0.020).
models (Chang, 2000; Hardin and Hilbe, 2003). The
Wald–Wolfowitz test is a nonparametric sign test which
analyzes the degree to which regression residuals follows The kaizen capabilities model R2 was 0.71 and the
a random distribution (Field, 2005). The residual plots, adjusted R2 was 0.66, indicating that the model explained
partial regression plots and Wald–Wolfowitz test did approximately 70% of the variance in the outcome. Due to
not indicate any apparent departures from linearity or the larger number of predictors in the final model, the
normality for the final models. In addition, all standar- two-way and higher-order interactions were not tested for
dized residual values for both models were less than significance.
3.0 and all but a few (three for the attitude model
and two for the kaizen capabilities model) were less  Thus, overall, H1 and H2 were partially supported in
than 2.0. that each social system outcome was significantly
Table 11 shows the final regression models for the two predicted by at least one input factor and at least one
outcome variables: attitude and kaizen capabilities. As process factor.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
56 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

4.2. Mediation analysis to identify indirect predictors of Table 12


outcomes Mediation analysis results for attitudea

Step l (y0 ¼ IP, separate regression) a p


Mediation analysis was used to test indirect relation-
ships. A mediation model hypothesizes that a variable (x) Goal clarity 0.571 o0.0001*
indirectly influences a second variable (y) by acting Goal difficulty 0.031 0.7342
though a third variable (z), which is called the mediating Team autonomy 0.408 0.0001*
variable or mediator (Baron and Kenny, 1986). This Team functional heterogeneity 0.088 0.7745
Team kaizen experience 0.117 0.4650
mediating variable is typically a process variable, whereas Team leader experience 0.004 0.9767
the first variable, x, can be an input variable or another Management support 0.336 0.0036*
process variable. Several methods of testing mediation Event planning process 0.212 0.0759
models are currently available (MacKinnon et al., 1995). Work area routineness o0.001 0.9892
Although the approaches differ somewhat in the estima-
tion methods and the statistical tests used, they all involve Step 2 (y0 ¼ AT, separate regression) b p c0 p
jointly or separately testing the regression paths involving
Internal processes 0.725 o0.0001*
x, y, and z. In the current analysis, a modified version of
Goal clarity 0.147 0.2762
Judd and Kenny (1981) and Baron and Kenny’s (1986) Internal processes 0.701 o0.0001*
classic approach, as presented in Kenny et al. (1998) and Team autonomy 0.185 0.0988
Kenny (2006), was used. This approach includes testing Internal processes 0.678 o0.0001*
three regression coefficients in two equations (see steps 1 Management support 0.285 0.0068

and 2 below). Since each regression coefficient is tested


separately, a Bonferroni correction (a ¼ 0.05/3 ¼ 0.0167) Step 3 (y0 ¼ IP, simultaneous regression) a0 p
is needed to account for the increased chance of type-I
Goal clarity 0.445 o0.0001*
error (Kenny, 2006). All significant process variables from
Team autonomy 0.154 0.2176
the regression models for director predictors (see Table 11) Management support 0.096 0.3984
were tested as potential mediators: Calculated effect of goal clarity on attitudeb 0.414

a
 Step 1: The mediating process variable (z) was An asterisk indicates a significant relationship.
b
Based on a  b; a closer estimate is a  bIP in Table 11, or 0.396.
separately regressed on each of the nine input
variables (x) and the resulting coefficient (a) was
tested for significance.
 Step 2: If a significant relationship was demonstrated in kaizen capabilities. Although three input variables were
step one, the outcome variable (y) was regressed on significant in the first step of the mediation analysis,
both the input variable (x) and the mediating process subsequent steps failed to provide clear support for
variable (z), and the resulting regression coefficients mediation effects.
were tested for significance. A significant relation-  Thus, H3 was partially supported with one significant
ship between the mediating process variable (z) and mediation effect for each outcome variable.
the outcome (y) (coefficient b) suggests a mediation
relationship. The relationship between the outcome 5. Conclusions
variable (y) and the input variable (x) (coefficient c0 )
can be either significant (partial mediation) or non- 5.1. Discussion of findings
significant (full mediation).
 Step 3: After the two preceding steps were accomplished
Following the conclusion of the study, findings were
for all nine input variables, the mediating process
reported to the participating organizations to evaluate the
variable (z) was simultaneously regressed on all the
validity of study conclusions and to allow the organiza-
input variables (xi) significant in step one. This step was
tions to benefit from the results. In general, participating
performed to confirm whether each input variable (xi)
organizations found the study results convincing and used
was a significant unique predictor of the mediator (z),
study feedback to evaluate the effectiveness of their
after controlling for the other input variables.
current practices and to identify potential changes. As
mentioned, the study provided partial support for the
Table 12 shows the mediation analysis results for three broad research hypotheses. Specific findings of the
attitude, while Table 13 shows the mediation analysis study are as follows:
results for kaizen capabilities. The mediation analysis
suggests the following:
 Internal processes and goal clarity were the strongest
 Internal processes is a significant mediator of the effect predictors of both kaizen capabilities and attitude.
of goal clarity on both attitude and kaizen capabilities  Management support and team functional heteroge-
(see Tables 12 and 13), accounting for an estimated 57% neity were significant predictors of attitude, but not
of the direct effect of internal processes. kaizen capabilities.
 Affective commitment to change is not a significant  Team autonomy, affective commitment to change,
mediator of the effect of any of the input variables on goal difficulty, work area routineness, team kaizen
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 57

Table 13
Mediation analysis results for Kaizen capabilitiesa

Step l (y0 ¼ z., separate regression) z ¼ IP p z ¼ ACC p


A a

Goal clarity 0.571 o.0001* 0.663 o.0001*


Goal difficulty 0.031 0.7342 0.168 0.0556
Team autonomy 0.408 0.0001* 0.350 0.0013*
Team functional heterogeneity 0.088 0.7745 0.269 0.3913
Team kaizen experience 0.117 0.4650 0.051 0.7641
Team leader experience 0.004 0.9767 0.005 0.9646
Management support 0.336 0.0036* 0.338 0.0043*
Event planning process 0.212 0.0759 0.119 0.3377
Work area routineness o.001 0.9892 0.032 0.4917

Step 2 (y0 ¼ KC, separate regression) b p c0 p b p c0 p

Mediator variable (z) 0.610 o.0001* 0.254 0.1317


Goal clarity 0.227 0.0770 0.357 0.0424
Mediator variable (z) 0.500 o.0001* 0.230 0.0489
Team autonomy 0.382 o.0001* 0.495 o.0001*
Mediator variable (z) 0.614 o.0001* 0.334 0.0139*
Management support 0.250 0.0142* 0.338 0.0059*

Step 3 (y0 ¼ z., simultaneous regression) a0 p b p

Goal clarity 0.445 o.0001* 0.603 o.0001*


Team Autonomy 0.154 0.2176 0.046 0.6858
Management Support 0.096 0.3984 0.066 0.5178
Calculated effect of Goal clarity on Kaizen capabilitiesb 0.348 N/a

a
An asterisk indicates a significant relationship.
b
Based on a  b; a closer estimate is a  bIP in Table 11, or 0.266.

experience and team leader experience were signifi- specific goals (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bradley and Willett,
cant predictors of kaizen capabilities but not attitude. 2004; Rusiniak, 1996), and contradicts the few that have
 Some variables proposed to affect Kaizen event out- suggested using more loosely defined goals (Kumar and
comes, i.e., action orientation, tool quality, tool appro- Harms, 2004; Wittenberg, 1994).
priateness, and event planning process, showed no The finding that management support had a significant
significant relationship to either outcome in this positive relationship to attitude, but not to kaizen
study. capabilities, partially supports the consistent emphasis
on management support in published Kaizen event ac-
counts (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bateman, 2005; Bicheno,
Internal processes was by far the strongest predictor of 2001; Bradley and Willett, 2004; Kumar and Harms, 2004;
both attitude (b ¼ 0.694) and kaizen capabilities Martin, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Patil, 2003;
(b ¼ 0.465), suggesting that maintaining positive group Sheridan, 1997; Tanner and Roncarti, 1994; Taylor and
dynamics may be the most important factor for generat- Ramsey, 1993). In addition, it agrees with previous team
ing employee motivation to participate in future improve- studies which have reported a positive relationship
ment activities and for developing employee problem- between management support and team effectiveness
solving capabilities. This result is aligned with previous (e.g., Campion et al. 1993; Doolen et al., 2003a; Hyatt
empirical team research, which has consistently reported and Ruddy, 1997; Pinto and Slevin, 1987).
a positive relationship between internal processes and The finding that team functional heterogeneity had a
human resource outcomes (e.g., Bailey, 2000; Jehn, 1995; significant negative relationship to attitude, but not to
Pinto et al., 1993; Vinokur-Kaplan, 1995). kaizen capabilities, is aligned with previous team research
Acting indirectly through internal processes, goal clarity suggesting that cross-functional teams may experience
is also one of the strongest predictors of attitude and lower levels of enjoyment in working together (Ancona
kaizen capabilities, accounting for approximately 60% of and Caldwell, 1992; Amason and Schweiger, 1994; Baron,
the direct effect of internal processes. This finding is 1990). However, the mediation analysis showed no
aligned with several previous studies in team effective- significant relationship between team functional hetero-
ness reporting a positive relationship between goal clarity geneity and internal processes, suggesting that more
and effectiveness (e.g., Doolen et al, 2003a; Koch, 1979; diverse teams were still able to develop effective com-
Pritchard et al., 1988; Van Aken and Kleiner, 1997). This munication processes (Azzi, 1993; Earley and Mosakowski,
finding also supports the majority of published Kaizen 2000). This appears to agree with team research indicat-
event accounts, which have suggested using clear and ing that cross-functional teams may experience high
ARTICLE IN PRESS
58 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

quality decision-making processes (e.g., Jackson, 1992; through the team activities log, there was no formal
Jehn et al., 1999; Lovelace et al., 2001; McGrath, 1984). measure of the extent to which teams adhered to the
The finding that team autonomy had a significant formal Kaizen event process. Recent literature has in-
positive relationship to kaizen capabilities, but not to dicated a link between process adherence and team and
attitude, partially supports the assertions in the published organizational outcomes (Douglas and Judge, 2001; Ghosh
Kaizen event accounts that autonomy promotes positive and Sobek, 2007; Handfield et al., 1999; Linderman et al.,
outcomes (e.g., Adams et al., 1997; Bicheno, 2001; Bradley 2006), although some published Kaizen event guidelines
and Willett, 2004; Foreman and Vargas, 1999; Kumar and propose a less rigid adherence to the organization’s Kaizen
Harms, 2004). Some empirical team studies have similarly event format (e.g., Bradley and Willett, 2004), including
reported a positive relationship between autonomy and even omitting training in some cases (e.g., Bicheno, 2001;
outcomes (Cohen and Ledford, 1994; Wall et al., 1986). Gregory, 2003; McNichols et al., 1999). Teams with high
The finding that affective commitment to change had a team leader experience might also be more likely to jump
significant positive relationship to kaizen capabilities, but to solutions similar to those used in previous events that
not to attitude, partially supports the emphasis in the the leader has conducted, thus limiting team creativity
published Kaizen event accounts on creating team buy-in and team member participation in designing solutions. Or,
for the improvements targeted by the event (Bradley and more experienced leaders might tend to adopt a more
Willett, 2004; McNichols et al., 1999; Melnyk et al., 1998; directive leadership style, allowing individual team mem-
Taylor and Ramsey, 1993; Wheatley, 1998). In addition, bers less role in shaping the solution. Such directive
this finding is aligned with the organizational change leadership has been shown to be negatively related to
literature, which suggests a relationship between partici- team effectiveness in at least one study (Durham et al.,
pant commitment and change initiative effectiveness 1997). Finally, it is possible that in teams with less
(Chan et al., 2005; Keating et al., 1999). experienced leaders the Kaizen event facilitator, who is
The finding that goal difficulty had a significant positive typically highly experienced in problem-solving tools and
relationship to kaizen capabilities, but not to attitude, applications, may tend to become more involved in
suggests that, from the standpoint of employee learning, coaching the team through the solution process, providing
the more challenging the goals, the more positive the skill on-the-floor training, etc., thus contributing to greater
development results, which would support the Kaizen learning. Future research on the relationship between
event accounts that advocate using highly challenging team leader experience, team process adherence, team
goals (e.g. Bicheno, 2001; Bradley and Willett, 2004; creativity, leadership style, facilitator involvement, and
Cuscela, 1998; Gregory, 2003; Kumar and Harms, 2004; kaizen capabilities is clearly needed.
LeBlanc, 1999; Minton, 1998; Rusiniak, 1996; Tanner and Finally, some variables hypothesized to affect out-
Roncarti, 1994; Treece, 1993). However, findings related to comes, i.e., action orientation, tool quality, tool appropriate-
technical system outcomes revealed negative relation- ness, and event planning process, showed no significant
ships between goal difficulty and technical success out- relationship to either outcome in this study. This finding
comes (Farris et al., 2008a), suggesting a trade-off does not prove that these variables are unimportant to the
between technical and skill development outcomes. development of attitude and kaizen capabilities, although it
Meanwhile, the finding that work area routineness had does suggest that they might be less important than other
a significant positive relationship to kaizen capabilities, but variables. It should also be noted that these variables may
not to attitude, supports the suggestions in published be related to technical system outcomes or to social
Kaizen event accounts that less complex work areas system outcomes not studied in this research. Although
provide favorable ‘‘learning laboratories’’ for skill devel- admittedly preliminary, the findings of this research can
opment (LeBlanc, 1999; Bradley and Willett, 2004), and be further used to develop initial guidelines for organiza-
aligns with findings in the organizational learning tions (to be tested in future research), including the
literature (Vits and Gelders, 2002). This effect may be following:
due to the fact that more predictable work areas allow
more complete implementation of lean practices.
The finding that team kaizen experience had a sig-  To obtain maximum impact on both employee attitude
nificant negative relationship to kaizen capabilities, but not and problem-solving ability development, these results
to attitude, is consistent with the well-known learning suggest that organizations should seek to maintain a
curve effect (Wright, 1936), which predicts decreasing high level of positive internal team dynamics, through
incremental gains in learning from each successive the use of structured mechanisms (e.g., team ground
attempt at a given application. rules, ice breakers, training, charters) and facilitator
The finding that team leader experience had a signifi- coaching. A key structural mechanism is the develop-
cant negative relationship to kaizen capabilities, but not to ment of explicit goal statements in advance of the event.
attitude, is unexpected and warrants further investigation.  To further develop employee motivation to participate
A manager in one of the participating organizations in events, these results suggest that organizations must
suggested that, based on his experience, teams with more maintain strong and visible management support
seasoned leaders tended to skip or hurry through steps in during events. Meanwhile, organizations should re-
the Kaizen event process, under the paradigm that they cognize that cross-functional heterogeneity may have a
‘‘already know what they are doing.’’ In this study, negative impact on team member affect toward events,
although data were collected on the activities of the team and potentially compensate for this effect through
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 59

other variables, e.g., internal processes, management ment events, often using the ‘‘standard work’’ metho-
support. dology. A smaller number of events were of other
 To further increase team problem-solving capability types: total productive maintenance (TPM) (14%),
development, these results suggest that teams should single minute exchange of die (SMED) (8%), 5S (6%)
be allowed a high degree of autonomy and that the and value stream mapping (VSM) (2%). (Note, these
organization should use mechanisms designed to percentages total 101% due to rounding.) In addition,
increase participant buy-in for the event (e.g., clearly most events included implementation of the solution
describing the reasons for the event, demonstrating during the event (75%), targeted manufacturing pro-
how the event will positively impact both individual cesses or work areas (84%), and were highly successful
and organizational interests). Organizations should with 100% goal achievement (69%). Thus, it is possible
also recognize that the use of difficult goals could that findings might not generalize to events of
increase team learning, although other findings sug- markedly different types.
gests that goal difficulty may compromise technical  This research only included event-level variables. It is
system results (Farris et al., 2008a). possible that some organizational-level effects also
 Finally, these results suggest that organizations should contribute to event-level outcomes. While GEE ac-
recognize that more complex work area may contri- counts for correlation in regression residuals between
bute to a lower degree of team learning, and poten- teams within the same organization, it also assumes
tially compensate for this effect through other uniformity of regression slopes across organizations.
mechanisms, e.g., autonomy, goal clarity, etc. Organi- Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) has the capability
zations should also recognize that greater team and to model differences in effects of both organization and
team leader experience do not necessarily result in event-level variables across organizations. However, as
greater team learning, and should consider compensat- noted, a larger data set are needed to construct models
ing for this effect through other mechanisms or of any size (Raudenbush and Byrk, 2002).
through using less experienced personnel.  Finally, this research only investigated the determi-
nants of initial event outcomes, while sustainability
of both human resource and technical outcomes is
5.2. Limitations and future research of great interest, and many organizations appear to
struggle to sustain initial gains from Kaizen events
To the authors’ knowledge, this study was the first to (e.g., Bateman and David, 2002; Bateman 2005; Laraia
empirically investigate the quantitative relationships et al, 1999).
between input and process variables and initial human
resource outcomes in Kaizen events, using multiple events
from multiple organizations. Additional research is In summary, this research has systematically investi-
needed to confirm and extend these findings. Other study gated the empirical relationships between input and
limitations include: process variables and human resource outcomes using a
sample of 51 events from six organizations. In closing, the
authors reiterate the call from Melnyk et al. (1998)
 Although data were collected from 51 Kaizen events regarding the need for additional research on this topic.
and over 300 participants, the sample represented only
If the proliferation of books, websites, conference pro-
six organizations. Furthermore, the selection criteria
ceedings and non-refereed articles is any indication,
deliberately resulted in the selection of manufacturing
organizational interest in Kaizen events only appears to
organizations that were relatively experienced in the
have increased since the late 1990s, and little scholarly
strategic use of Kaizen events. Thus, it is possible that
research has been conducted to date. It is the responsi-
findings might not generalize to markedly different
bility of the operations management and industrial
organizational contexts. However, the six organizations
engineering research community to help organizations
spanned a variety of industries and approximately 16%
better understand this phenomenon and how it contri-
of the events were in non-manufacturing work areas.
butes to longer-term organizational success.
 Events were selected using a random sampling process
designed to capture the mixture of events occurring
naturally within the participating organizations; the Appendix A
researchers did not attempt to control the percentages
of specific event types sampled. Most of the events in See Table A1 for summary of study variables and
this research (71%) were general processes improve- measures.
60
Table A1
Summary of study variables and measures

Scale Instrument Itemsa Sources Meas. scale Value

Goal clarity (GC) Kickoff Our team has clearly defined goals. (GC1) Van Aken and Kleiner 6-point Likert Team average for
questionnaire The performance targets our team must achieve to fulfill our goals are clear. (GC2) (1997) and Wilson et type scale
Our goals clearly define what is expected of our team. (GC3) al. (1998)
Our entire team understands our goals. (GC4)
Goal difficulty Kickoff Our team’s improvement goals are difficult. (GDF1) Ivancevich and 6-point Likert Team average for
(GDF) questionnaire Meeting our team’s improvement goals will be tough. (GDF2) McMahon (1977) and type scale
It will take a lot of skill to achieve our team’s improvement goals. (GDF3) Hart et al. (1989)
It will be hard to improve this work area enough to achieve team’s goals. (GDF4)
Affective Kickoff In general, members of our team believe in the value of this Kaizen event. (ACC1) Herscovitch and 6-point Likert Team average for
commitment to questionnaire Most of our team members think that this Kaizen event is a good strategy for this work area. (ACC2) Meyer (2002) type scale
change (ACC) In general, members of our team think that it is a mistake to hold this Kaizen event. (REVERSE CODED IN

J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65


FACTOR ANALYSIS) (ACC3)
Most of our team members that this Kaizen event will serve an important purpose. (ACC4)
Most of our team members think that things will be better with this Kaizen event. (ACC5)
In general, members of our team believe that this Kaizen event is needed. (ACC6)

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Internal processes Report out Our team communicated openly. (IP1) Hyatt and Ruddy 6-point Likert Team average for
(IP) questionnaire Our team valued each member’s unique contributions. (IP2) (1997) type scale
Our team respected each others’ opinions. (IP3)
Our team respected each others’ feelings. (IP4)
Our team valued the diversity in our team members. (IP5)
Management Report out Our team had enough contact with management to get our work done. (MS1)* Doolen et al. (2003a) 6-point Likert Team average for
support (MS) questionnaire Our team had enough materials and supplies to get our work done. (MS2) type scale
Our team had enough equipment to get our work done. (MS3)
Our team had enough help from our facilitator to get our work done. (MS4)*
Our team had enough help from others in our organization to get our work done. (MS5)
Team autonomy Report out Our team had a lot of freedom in determining what changes to make to this work area. (TA1) Kirkman and Rosen 6-point Likert Team average for
(TA) questionnaire Our team had a lot of freedom in determining how to improve this work area. (TA2) (1999), Groesbeck type scale
Our team was free to make changes to the work area as soon as we thought of them. (TA3) (2001), and Hayes
Our team had a lot of freedom in determining how we spent our time during the event. (TA4)* (1994)
Action orientation Report out Our team spent as much time as possible in the work area. (AO1) Original to this 6-point Likert Team average for
(AO) questionnaire Our team spent very little time in our meeting room. (AO2) researchc type scale
Our team tried out changes to the work area right after we thought of them. (AO3)*
Our team spent a lot of time discussing ideas before trying them out in the work area. (REVERSE CODED IN
FACTOR ANALYSIS) (AO4)*
Understanding of Report out Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team members’ knowledge of what continuous improvement is. Original to this 6-point Likert N/ab
CI (UCI) questionnaire (UCI1)* researchd type
In general, this Kaizen event increased our team members’ knowledge of how continuous improvement
can be applied. (UCI2)*
Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team members’ knowledge of the need for continuous
improvement. (UCI3)*
In general, this Kaizen event increased our team members’ knowledge of our role in continuous
improvement. (UCI4)*
Skills (SK) Report out Most of our team members can communicate new ideas about improvements as a result of participation Original to this 6-point Likert N/a
questionnaire in this Kaizen event. (SK1)* researchc type
Most of our Kaizen event team members are able to measure the impact of changes made to this work
area. (SK2)*
Most of our team members gained new skills as a result of participation in this Kaizen event. (SK3)*
In general, our Kaizen event team members are comfortable working with others to identify
improvements in this work area. (SK4)*
Attitude (AT) Report out In general, this Kaizen event motivated the members of our team to perform better. (AT1)* Original to this 6-point Likert Team average for
questionnaire Most of our team members liked being part of this Kaizen event. (AT2) researchd type scale
Overall, this Kaizen event increased our team members’ interest in our work. (AT3)*
Most members of our team would like to be part of Kaizen events in the future. (AT4)
Team kaizen Kickoff Not including this event, how many Kaizen events total have you participated in? N/a Continuous Team average
experience questionnaire plus one
(TOTKE)
Team functional Event Please fill-in the number of team members in each job category. (choices: operator, technician, engineer, Shannon (1948) and Continuous H index value
heterogeneity information manager, supervisor, other) Teachman (1980)
(FUNHET) questionnaire
Calculated as follows, where pi is the proportion of team members from each functional category
P
H¼ pi ðlogð1=pi ÞÞ
i
Team leader Event Including this event, how many Kaizen events total has the team leader led or co-led in the past three N/a Continuous Unadjusted
experience information years? response value
(LDREXP) questionnaire
Work area Event The work the target work area does is routine. (WAC1) Perrow (1967), 6-point Likert Average for four

J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65


routineness information The target work area produces the same product (SKU) most of the time. (WAC2) Duncan (1972), type items
(WAC) questionnaire A given product (SKU) requires the same processing steps each time it is produced. (WAC3) Miltenberg (1995),
Most of the products (SKUs) produced in the work area follow a very similar production process. (WAC4) Withey et al. (1983),
and Gibson and

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Vermeulen (2003)
Tool Event (Respondents first listed the problem-solving tools used by the team). For each tool, please rate the team’s N/a 6-point Likert Average rating for
appropriateness information use of the tool on appropriateness of using this tool to address the team’s goals type all tools listed
(TAPP) questionnaire
Tool quality Event (Uses the same tool list above.) For each tool, please rate the quality of the team’s use of this tool. N/a 6-point Likert Average rating for
(TQUAL) information type all tools listed
questionnaire
Event planning Event How many hours total did you and others spend planning and doing other pre-work for this event? N/a Continuous Unadjusted
process (HRSPLN) information response value
questionnaire

a
An asterisk indicates that the item was removed from its original scale following the factor analysis (see Section 3.3).
b
The understanding of CI and skills variables were ultimately replaced by the kaizen capabilities variable (see Section 3.3), the value of which is the team average for the scale.
c
The items in this scale were newly developed, i.e., original to this research stream; however, the concept of team action-orientation was informed by the concepts of personal initiative (Frese et al., 1997)
and individual-level action orientation (Kuhl, 1992).
d
The items in this scale were newly developed, i.e., original to this research stream; however, pilot study items and statistics were reported in Doolen et al., 2003b and Doolen et al., 2008.

61
ARTICLE IN PRESS
62 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

References Cohen, S.G., Bailey, D.E., 1997. What makes teams work: Group
effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite.
Journal of Management 23 (3), 239–290.
Adam, E.E., Corbett, L.M., Flores, B.E., Harrison, N.J., Lee, T.S., Rho, B.H., Cohen, S.G., Ledford, G.E., 1994. The effectiveness of self-managing
Ribera, J., Samson, D., Westbrook, R., 1997. An international study of teams: A quasi-experiment. Human Relations 47 (1), 13–43.
quality improvement approach and firm performance. International Cohen, S.G., Ledford, G.E., Spreitzer, G.M., 1996. A predictive model of
Journal of Operations and Production Management 17 (9), 842–873. self-managing work team effectiveness. Human Relations 49 (5),
Adams, M., Schroer, B.J., Stewart, S.K., 1997. QuickstepTM process 643–676.
improvement: Time-compression as a management strategy. En- Co, H.C., Patuwo, B.E., Hu, M.Y., 1998. The human factor in advanced
gineering Management Journal 9 (2), 21–32. manufacturing technology adoption, an empirical analysis. Interna-
Adler, P.S., 1993a. The new ‘‘learning bureaucracy’’: New United Motor tional Journal of Operations and Production Management 18 (1),
Manufacturing Inc. In: Staw, B.M., Cummings, L.L. (Eds.), Research 87–106.
in Organizational Behavior, vol. 15. JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, Cronbach, L., 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.
pp. 111–194. Psychiatrika 16, 297–334.
Adler, P.S., 1993b. Time-and-motion regained. Harvard Business Review Cuscela, K.N., 1998. Kaizen blitz attacks work processes at Dana Corp. IIE
71 (1), 97–108. Solutions 30 (4), 29–31.
Amason, A.C., Schweiger, D.M., 1994. Resolving the paradox of conflict, Dankbaar, B., 1997. Lean production: Denial, confirmation or exten-
strategic decision making and organizational performance. Interna- sion of sociotechnical systems design? Human Relations 50 (5),
tional Journal of Conflict Management 5, 239–253. 567–583.
Ancona, D., Caldwell, D.F., 1992. Demography and design: Predictors of Dar-El, E.M., 1997. What we really need is TPQM!. International Journal of
new product team performance. Organization Science 3, 321–341. Production Economics 52, 5–13.
Azzi, A.E., 1993. Implicit and category-based allocations of decision- David, I., 2000. Drilled in Kaizen. Professional Engineering 13 (9), 30–31.
making power in majority–minority relations. Journal of Experi- Delbridge, R., 1998. Life on the Line in Contemporary Manufacturing.
mental Psychology 29, 203–228. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Bailey, D.E., 2000. Modeling work group effectiveness in high-technology Delbridge, R., Turnbull, P., Wilkinson, B., 1992. Pushing back the
manufacturing environments. IIE Transactions 32 (4), 361–368. frontiers: Management control and work intensification under
Bailey, J.E., Rose, R.L., 1988. Maybe Winnebago just wasn’t ready for big JIT/TQM factory regimes. New Technology, Work and Employment
city bosses. Wall Street Journal 17 (17 October), 1. 7 (2), 97–106.
Baron, R.A., 1990. Countering the effects of destructive criticism: The deTreville, S., Antonakis, J., 2006. Could lean production job design be
relative efficacy of four interventions. Journal of Applied Psychology intrinsically motivating? Contextual, configurational, and levels-of-
75, 235–245. analysis issues. Journal of Operations Management 24 (2), 99–123.
Baron, R.M., Kenny, D.A., 1986. The moderator–mediator variable deVellis, R.F., 1991. Scale Development: Theory and Application. Sage
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic Publishing, Newbury Park, CA.
and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Dillman, D., 2000. Mail and Internet Surveys: The Tailored Design
Psychology 51, 1173–1182. Method. Wiley, New York.
Bartko, J.J., 1976. On various intraclass correlation reliability coefficients. Doolen, T.L., Hacker, M.E., Van Aken, E.M., 2003a. The impact of
Psychological Bulletin 83 (5), 762–765. organizational context on work team effectiveness: A study of
Bateman, N., 2005. Sustainability: The elusive element in process production teams. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management 50
improvement. International Journal of Operations and Production (3), 285–296.
Management 25 (3), 261–276. Doolen, T.L., Worley, J., Van Aken, E.M., Farris, J., 2003b. Development of
Bateman, N., David, A., 2002. Process improvement programmes: A an assessment approach for kaizen events. In: 2003 Industrial
model for assessing sustainability. International Journal of Opera- Engineering and Research Conference, Portland, OR.
tions and Production Management 22 (5), 515–526. Doolen, T.L., Van Aken, E.M., Farris, J., Worley, J., Huwe, J., 2008. A field
Bateman, N., Rich, N., 2003. Companies’ perceptions of inhibitors and study of kaizen events and organizational performance. International
enables for process improvement activities. International Journal of Journal of Productivity and Performance Management, in press.
Operations and Production Management 23 (2), 185–199. Douglas, T.J., Judge, W.Q., 2001. Total quality management implementa-
Belassi, W., Tukel, O.I., 1996. A new framework for determining critical tion and competitive advantage: The role of structural control and
success/failure factors in projects. International Journal of Project exploitation. Academy of Management Journal 44 (1), 158–169.
Management 14 (3), 141–151. Drickhamer, D., 2004a. Just-in-time training. Industry Week 253 (7), 69.
Biazzo, S., Panizzolo, R., 2000. The assessment of work organization in Drickhamer, D., 2004b. Braced for the future. Industry Week 253 (10),
lean production: The relevance of the worker’s perspective. Inte- 51–52.
grated Manufacturing Systems 11 (1), 6–15. Duncan, G.M., 1972. Characteristics of organizational environments and
Bicheno, J., 2001. Kaizen and Kaikaku. In: Taylor, D., Brunt, D. (Eds.), perceived environmental uncertainty. Administrative Science Quar-
Manufacturing Operations and Supply Chain Management: The terly 17, 313–327.
LEAN approach. Thomson Learning, London, pp. 175–184. Durham, C.C., Knight, D., Locke, E.A., 1997. Effects of leader role, team-set
Bliese, P.D., 2000. Within-group agreement, non-independence, and goal difficulty, efficacy, and tactics on team effectiveness. Organiza-
reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis. In: Klein, tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes 72 (2), 203–231.
K.J., Kozlowski, S.W.J. (Eds.), Multilevel Theory, Research and Earley, P.C., Mosakowski, E., 2000. Creating hybrid team cultures: An
Methods in Organizations. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 349–381. empirical test of transnational team functioning. Academy of
Bradley, J.R., Willett, J., 2004. Cornell students participate in Lord Management Journal 43 (1), 26–49.
Corporation’s Kaizen projects. Interfaces 34 (6), 451–459. Earley, P.C., Connolley, T., Ekegren, G., 1989. Goals, strategy development
Brown, K.A., Mitchell, T.R., 1991. A comparison of just-in-time and batch and task performance: Some limits on the efficacy of goal setting.
manufacturing: The role of performance obstacles. Academy of Journal of Applied Psychology 74 (1), 24–33.
Management Journal 34 (4), 906–917. Eisenhardt, K.M., 1989. Building theories from case studies. Academy of
Butterworth, C., 2001. From value stream mapping to shop floor Management Review 14, 532–550.
improvement: A case study of Kaikaku. In: Taylor, D., Brunt, D. Erez, M., Zidon, I., 1984. Effect of goal acceptance on the relationship of
(Eds.), Manufacturing Operations and Supply Chain Management: goal difficulty to performance. Journal of Applied Psychology 69,
The LEAN Approach. Thomson Learning, London, pp. 185–193. 69–78.
Campion, M.A., Medsker, G.J., Higgs, A.C., 1993. Relations between work Fabrigar, L.R., Wegener, D.T., MacCallum, R.C., Strahan, E.J., 1999.
group characteristics and effectiveness: Implications for designing Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological
effective work groups. Personnel Psychology 46, 823–850. research. Psychological Methods 4 (3), 272–299.
Chan, D., 1998. Functional relationships among constructs in the same Farris, J., Van Aken, E.M., Doolen, T., Worley, J., 2008a. Identifying the
content domain at different levels of analysis: A typology of determinants of Kaizen event success: An empirical investigation.
composition models. Journal of Applied Psychology 83 (2), 234–246. Working Paper.
Chan, F.T.S., Lau, H.C.W., Ip, R.W.L., Chan, H.K., Kong, S., 2005. Farris, J., Van Aken, E.M., Doolen, T.L., Worley, J., 2008b. Learning from
Implementation of total productive maintenance: A case study. less successful Kaizen events: A case study. Engineering Manage-
International Journal of Production Economics 95, 71–94. ment Journal 20 (3), 10–20.
Chang, Y., 2000. Residual analysis of the generalized linear models for Field, A., 2005. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, second ed. Sage
longitudinal data. Statistics in Medicine 19 (10), 1277–1293. Publications Ltd., London.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 63

Finch, H., 2006. Comparison of the performance of varimax and promax Ivancevich, J.M., McMahon, J.T., 1977. A study of task-goal attributes,
rotations: Factor structure recovery for dichotomous items. Journal higher order need strength, and performance. Academy of Manage-
of Educational Measurement 43 (1), 39–52. ment Journal 20 (4), 552–563.
Foreman, C.R., Vargas, D.H., 1999. Affecting the value chain through supplier Jackson, S., 1992. Team composition in organizations. In: Worchel, S.,
Kaizen. Hospital Materiel Management Quarterly 20 (3), 21–27. Wood, W., Simpson, J. (Eds.), Group Process and Productivity. Sage,
Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., Tag, A., 1997. The concept of London, pp. 1–12.
personal initiative: Operationalization, reliability and validity in two Jackson, P.R., Mullarkey, S., 2000. Lean production teams and health in
German samples. Journal of Occupational and Organizational garment manufacture. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 5,
Psychology 70, 139–161. 231–245.
Fucini, J.J., Fucini, S., 1990. Working for the Japanese: Inside Mazda’s James, L.R., 1982. Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual agreement.
American Plant. The Free Press, New York. Journal of Applied Psychology 69 (2), 219–229.
George, J.M., 1990. Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. Journal of James, L.R., Demaree, R.G., Wolf, G., 1984. Estimating within-group
Applied Psychology 75, 107–116. interrater reliability with and without response bias. Journal of
Ghosh, M., Sobek, D., 2007. Fostering second-order problem-solving in Applied Psychology 69, 3–32.
healthcare. In: 2007 Industrial Engineering and Research Conference, James, L.R., Demaree, R.G., Wolf, G., 1993. rwg: An assessment of within-
Nashville, TN. group interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology 78 (2),
Gladstein, D.L., 1984. Groups in context: A model of task group 306–309.
effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly 29 (4), 499–517. Janz, B.D., 1999. Self-directed teams in IS: Correlates for improved
Gibson, C., Vermeulen, F., 2003. A healthy divide: Subgroups as a systems development work outcomes. Information and Management
stimulus for team learning behavior. Administrative Science Quar- 35, 171–192.
terly 48 (2), 202–239. Jehn, K., 1995. A multimethod examination of the benefits and
Gregory, A., 2003. Haring for change. Works Management 56 (5), 18–21. detriments of intragroup conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly
Groesbeck, R., 2001. An empirical study of group stewardship and 40 (2), 256–282.
learning: Implications for work group effectiveness. Unpublished Jehn, K., Northcraft, G., Neale, M., 1999. Why differences make a
Doctoral Dissertation, Grado Department of Industrial and Systems difference: A field study of diversity, conflict, and performance in
Engineering, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA. workgroups. Administrative Science Quarterly 44 (4), 741–763.
Guimaraes, T., 1997. Empirically testing the antecedents of BPR success. Jetten, J., Duck, J., Terry, D.J., O’Brien, A., 2002. Being attuned to
International Journal of Production Economics 50, 199–210. intergroup differences in mergers: The role of aligned leaders for
Gunasekaran, A., Korukondab, A.R., Virtanerf, I., Yli-Olli, P., 1994. low-status groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 (9),
Improving productivity and quality in manufacturing organizations. 1194–1201.
International Journal of Production Economics 36, 169–183. Johnson, D.E., 1998. Applied Multivariate Methods for Data Analysis.
Guzzo, R.A., Shea, G.P., 1992. Group performance and intergroup relations Brooks/Cole Publishing Company, Pacific Grove, CA.
in organizations. In: Dunnette, M.D., Hough, L.M. (Eds.), Handbook of Judd, C.M., Kenny, D.A., 1981. Process analysis: Estimating mediation in
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, vol. 3. Consulting Psycho- treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review 5, 602–619.
logists Press, Palo Alto, CA, pp. 269–313. Jun, M., Cai, S., Shin, H., 2006. TQM practice in Maquiladora: Antecedents
Hackman, J.R., 1987. The design of work groups. In: Lorsch, J.W. (Ed.), The of employee satisfaction and loyalty. Journal of Operations Manage-
Handbook of Organizational Behavior. Prentice Hall, Englewood ment 24, 791–812.
Cliffs, NJ, pp. 315–342. Jusko, J., 2004. Lonely at the top. Industry Week 253 (10), 58–59.
Hales, D.N., Chakravorty, S.S., 2006. Implementation of Deming’s style of Kathuria, R., Partovi, F.Y., 1999. Work force management practices for
quality management: An action research study in a plastics company. manufacturing flexibility. Journal of Operations Management 18,
International Journal of Production Economics 103, 131–148. 21–39.
Handfield, R., Jayaram, J., Ghosh, S., 1999. An empirical examination of Katzenbach, J.R., Smith, D.K., 1993. The Wisdom of Teams: Creating the
quality tool deployment patterns and their impact on performance. High Performance Organization. Harvard Business School Press,
International Journal of Production Research 37 (6), 1403–1426. Boston.
Hardin, J.W., Hilbe, J.M., 2003. Generalized Estimating Equations. Chap- Keating, E.K., Oliva, R., Repenning, N.P., Rockart, S., Sterman, J.D., 1999.
man and Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, FL. Overcoming the improvement paradox. European Management
Hart, S.H., Moncrief, W.C., Parasuraman, J., 1989. An empirical investiga- Journal 17 (2), 120–134.
tion of salespeople’s performance, effort and selling method during a Kenny, D.A., 2006. Mediation. White paper. Available URL: /http://
sales contest. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 17 (1), davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htmS, November 7, 2006.
29–39. Kenny, D.A., Judd, C.M., 1986. Consequences of violating the indepen-
Harvey, J., 2004. Match the change vehicle and method to the job. Quality dence assumptions in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin 99
Progress 37 (1), 41–48. (3), 422–431.
Hasek, G., 2000. Extraordinary extrusions. Industry Week 249 (17), Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., Bolger, N., 1998. Data analysis in social
79–80. psychology. In: Gilbert, D., Fiske, S.T., Lindzey, G. (Eds.), The Handbook
Hayes, B.E., 1994. How to measure empowerment. Quality Progress 27 of Social Psychology, vol. 1. McGraw-Hill, Boston, pp. 233–265.
(2), 41–46. Kirkman, B.L., Rosen, B., 1999. Beyond self-management: Antecedents
Heard, E., 1997. Rapid-fire Improvement with Short-cycle Kaizen. and consequences of team empowerment. Academy of Management
American Production and Inventory Control Society, Washington, DC. Journal 42 (1), 58–74.
Herron, C., Braiden, P.M., 2006. A methodology for developing sustain- Klein, J.K., 1989. The human costs of manufacturing reform. Harvard
able quantifiable productivity improvement in manufacturing Business Review 67 (2), 60–66.
companies. International Journal of Production Economics 104, Klein, K.J., Kozlowski, S.W.J., 2000. From micro to meso: Critical steps in
143–153. conceptualizing and conducting multilevel research. Organizational
Herscovitch, L., Meyer, J.P., 2002. Commitment to organizational change: Research Methods 3 (3), 211–236.
Extension of a three-component model. Journal of Applied Psychol- Kline, P., 1994. An Easy Guide to Factor Analysis. Routledge, London.
ogy 87 (3), 474–487. Koch, J.L., 1979. Effects of goal specificity and performance feedback to
Horton, N.J., Lipsitz, S.R., 1999. Review of software to fit generalized work groups on peer leadership, performance, and attitudes. Human
estimating equation regression models. The American Statistician 53 Relations 33, 819–840.
(2), 160–169. Kolodny, H.F., Kiggundu, M.N., 1980. Towards the development of a
Hox, J.J., 1994. Hierarchical regression models for interviewer and systems model in woodlands mechanical harvesting. Human Rela-
respondent effects. Sociological Methods and Research 17 (1), tions 33, 623–645.
300–316. Korhonen, K., Pirttila, T., 2003. Cross-functional decision-making in
Huber, V.L., Brown, K.A., 1991. Human resource issues in cellular improving inventory management decision procedures. International
manufacturing: A sociotechnical analysis. Journal of Operations Journal of Production Economics 81–82, 195–203.
Management 10 (1), 138–159. Kotter, J., 1995. Leading change: Why transformation efforts fail. Harvard
Huber, V.L., Hyer, N.L., 1985. The human factor in cellular manufacturing. Business Review 73 (2), 59–67.
Journal of Operations Management 5 (2), 213–228. Koufteros, X., Marcoulides, G.A., 2006. Product development practices
Hyatt, D.E., Ruddy, T.M., 1997. An examination of the relationship and performance: A structural equation modeling-based multi-
between work group characteristics and performance: Once more group analysis. International Journal of Production Economics 103,
into the breach. Personnel Psychology 50 (3), 553–585. 286–307.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
64 J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65

Kuhl, J., 1992. A theory of self-regulation: Action versus state orientation, Minton, E., 1998. Profile: Luke Faulstick—‘Baron of blitz’ has boundless
self-discrimination, and some applications. Applied Psychology 41 vision of continuous improvement. Industrial Management 40 (1),
(2), 97–129. 14–21.
Kumar, S., Harms, R., 2004. Improving business processes for increased Molleman, E., 2005. The multilevel nature of team-based work research.
operational efficiency: A case study. Journal of Manufacturing Team Performance Management 11 (3–4), 113–124.
Technology Management 15 (7), 662–674. Montabon, F., 2005. Using Kaizen events for back office processes: The
Laraia, A., 1998. Change it NOW. Manufacturing Engineering 121 (4), 152. recruitment of frontline supervisor co-ops. Total Quality Manage-
Laraia, A.C., Moody, P.E., Hall, R.W., 1999. The Kaizen Blitz: Accelerating ment and Business Excellence 16 (10), 1139–1147.
Breakthroughs in Productivity and Performance. The Association for Muchinsky, P.M., 2000. Psychology Applied to Work, sixth ed. Wads-
Manufacturing Excellence, New York. worth/Thomson Learning, Belmont, CA.
Larson, M., 1998. Lantech’s Kaizen diary: Monday through Friday. Quality Neter, J., Kutner, M.H., Nachtsheim, C.J., Wasserman, W., 1996. Applied
37 (6), 40. Linear Statistical Models, fourth ed. McGraw-Hill Companies Inc.,
Launonen, M., Kess, P., 2002. Team roles in business process re- New York.
engineering. International Journal of Production Economics 77, Nicolini, D., 2002. In search of ‘project chemistry’. Construction Manage-
205–218. ment and Economics 20 (2), 167–177.
Lawal, B., 2003. Categorical Data Analysis with SAS and SPSS Applica- Niepce, W., Molleman, E., 1996. A case study: Characteristics of work
tions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., London. organization in lean production and sociotechnical systems. Inter-
Lawler, E.E., Mohrman, S.A., 1985. Quality circles after the fad. Harvard national Journal of Operations and Production Management 16 (2),
Business Review 85 (1), 64–71. 77–90.
Lawler, E.E., Mohrman, S.A., 1987. Quality circles: After the honeymoon. Nunnally, J.D., 1978. Psychometric Theory, second ed. McGraw-Hill, New
Organizational Dynamics 19, 15–26. York.
LeBlanc, G., 1999. Kaizen at Hill-Rom. Center for Quality of Management Olorunniwo, F., Udo, G., 2002. The impact of management and employees
Journal 8 (2), 49–53. on cellular manufacturing implementation. International Journal of
Lemieux-Charles, L., Murray, M., Baker, G.R., Barnsley, J., Tasa, K., Ibrahim, Production Economics 76, 27–38.
S.A., 2002. The effects of quality improvement practices on team O’Leary-Kelly, A.M., Martocchio, W., Frink, D.D., 1994. A review of the
effectiveness: A mediational model. Journal of Organizational influence of group goals on group performance. Academy of
Behavior 23, 533–553. Management Journal 37 (5), 1258–1301.
Liang, K.Y., Zeger, S.L., 1986. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized Pagell, M., LePine, J.A., 2002. Multiple case studies of team effectiveness
linear models. Biometrika 73, 13–22. in manufacturing organizations. Journal of Operations Management
Linderman, M., Schroeder, R.G., Choo, A.S., 2006. Six Sigma: The role of 20, 619–639.
goals in improvement teams. Journal of Operations Management 24, Panizzolo, R., 1998. Applying the lessons learned from 27 lean
779–790. manufacturers. The relevance of relationships management. Inter-
Locke, E.A., Latham, G.P., 1984. Goal Setting: A Motivational Technique national Journal of Production Economics 55, 223–240.
That Works. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Parker, M., Slaughter, J., 1988. Choosing Sides: Unions and the Team
Locke, E.A., Latham, G.P., 1990. A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Concept. South End Press, Boston, MA.
Performance. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Patil, H., 2003. A standard framework for sustaining Kaizen events.
Locke, E.A., Latham, G.P., 2002. Building a practically useful theory of goal Unpublished Master’s Thesis, Department of Industrial and Manu-
setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychol- facturing Engineering, Wichita, KS.
ogist 57 (9), 705–717. Perrow, C., 1967. A framework for the comparative analysis of organiza-
Locke, E.A., Shaw, K.N., Saari, L.M., Latham, G.P., 1981. Goal setting and tions. American Sociological Review 32, 194–208.
task performance: 1969–1980. Psychological Bulletin 90 (1), Perry, L., 1995. Effective facilitators—A key element in successful
125–152. continuous improvement processes. Training for Quality 3 (4), 9–14.
Lovelace, D., Shapiro, D.L., Weingart, L.R., 2001. Maximizing cross- Pickering, J., Kisangani, E.F., 2005. Democracy and diversionary military
functional new product teams’ innovativeness and constraint intervention: Reassessing regime type and the diversionary hypoth-
adherence: A conflict communications perspective. Academy of esis. International Studies Quarterly 49, 23–43.
Management Journal 44 (4), 779–793. Pinto, J.K., Slevin, D.P., 1987. Critical factors in successful project
MacKinnon, D.P., West, G., Dwyer, J.H., 1995. A simulation study of implementation. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management 34
mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research 30 (1), (1), 22–27.
41–62. Pinto, M.B., Pinto, J.K., Prescott, J.E., 1993. Antecedents and consequences
Martin, K., 2004. Kaizen events: Achieving dramatic improvement of project team cross-functional cooperation. Management Science
through focused attention and team-based solutions. Society for 39 (10), 1281–1297.
Health Systems Newsletter August 2004, 6–7. Polychronakis, Y.E., Syntetos, A.A., 2007. ‘Soft’ supplier management
Martin, B.A., Snell, A.F., Callahan, C.M., 1999. An examination of related issues: An empirical investigation. International Journal of
individual differences in the relation of subjective goal difficulty to Production Economics 106, 431–449.
performance in a goal-setting model. Human Performance 12 (2), Pritchard, R.D., Jones, S., Roth, P., Stuebing, I.C., Ekeherg, S., 1988. Effects
115–135. of group feedback, goal setting, and incentives on organizational
Matusi, Y., 2007. An empirical analysis of just-in-time production in productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology 73 (2), 337–358.
Japanese manufacturing companies. International Journal of Produc- Raudenbush, A.S., Byrk, S.W., 2002. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applica-
tion Economics 108, 153–164. tions and Data Analysis Methods, second ed. Sage, Newbury Park, CA.
McGrath, J.E., 1964. Social Psychology: A Brief Introduction. Hold Rusiniak, S., 1996. Maximizing your IE value. IIE Solutions 28 (6), 12–16.
Rinehart and Winston Inc., New York. Safayeni, F., Purdy, L., 1991. A behavioral case study of just-in-time
McGrath, J.E., 1984. Groups: Interaction and Performance. Prentice-Hall, implementation. Journal of Operations Management 10 (2), 213–228.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Samson, D., Whybark, D.C., 1998. Tackling the ever so important ‘soft’
McIntosh, R.I., Culley, S.J., Mileham, A.R., Owen, G.W., 2001. Changeover issues in operations management. Journal of Operations Manage-
improvement: A maintenance perspective. International Journal of ment 17, 3–7.
Production Economics 73, 153–163. SAS Institute Inc., 2006. Generalized Linear Models Theory. SAS/STAT
McNichols, T., Hassinger, R., Bapst, G.W., 1999. Quick and continuous User’s Guide, SAS OnlineDocs 9.1.3. Available URL: /http://support.
improvement through Kaizen blitz. Hospital Materiel Management sas.com/onlinedoc/913/docMainpage.jspS, November 7, 2006.
Quarterly 20 (4), 1–7. Sawhney, R., Chason, S., 2005. Human behavior based exploratory model
Melnyk, S.A., Calantone, R.J., Montabon, F.L., Smith, R.T., 1998. Short-term for successful implementation of lean enterprise in industry.
action in pursuit of long-term improvements: Introducing Kaizen Performance Improvement Quarterly 18 (2), 76–96.
events. Production and Inventory Management Journal 39 (4), Schonberger, R.J., 2007. Japanese production management: An evolu-
69–76. tion—With mixed success. Journal of Operations Management 25,
Miltenberg, J., 1995. Manufacturing Strategy. Productivity Press, Portland, 403–419.
OR. Scheaffer, R.L., Mendenhall, W.III., Ott, R.L., 1996. Elementary Survey
Miller, B.D., 2004. Achievement behaviors within kaizen blitz from an Sampling, fifth ed. Wadsworth Publishing Company, Belmont, CA.
expectancy-value perspective: An empirical study. Unpublished Schneider, B., White, S., Paul, M.C., 1998. Linking service climate and
Doctoral Dissertation, Department of Industrial Engineering, Uni- customer perceptions of service quality: Test of a causal model,.
versity of Houston, Houston, TX. Journal of Applied Psychology 83 (2), 150–163.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J.A. Farris et al. / Int. J. Production Economics 117 (2009) 42–65 65

Sewell, G., Wilkinson, B., 1992. Someone to watch over me: Surveillance, Treece, J.B., 1993. Improving the soul of an old machine. Business Week
discipline and the just in time labour process. Sociology 26 (2), 3342, 134.
271–289. Van Aken, E.M., Kleiner, B.M., 1997. Determinants of effectiveness for
Salas, E., Sims, D.E., Burke, C.S., 2005. Is there a ‘‘big five’’ in teamwork? cross-functional organizational design teams. Quality Management
Small Group Research 36 (5), 555–599. Journal 4 (2), 51–79.
Shafer, S.M., Tepper, B.J., Meredith, J.R., Marshall, R., 1995. Comparing the Van Landeghem, R., 2000. Experiences with a concurrent engineering
effects of cellular and functional manufacturing on employees self-assessment tool. International Journal of Production Economics
perceptions and attitudes. Journal of Operations Management 12, 64 (1–3), 295–309.
63–74. Van Mierlo, H., Vermunt, J.K., Rutte, C.G., 2006. Composing group-level
Shah, R., Ward, P.T., 2003. Lean manufacturing: Context, practice bundles, constructs from individual-level survey data. Working Paper. Available
and performance. Journal of Operations Management 21 (2), URL: /http://www.spitswww.uvt.nl/vermunt/mierlo2006.pdfhttp://
129–149. spitswww.uvt.nl/vermunt/mierlo2006.pdfS, October 4, 2006.
Shah, R., Ward, P.T., 2007. Defining and developing measures of lean Vasilash, G.S., 1993. Walking the talk of Kaizen at Freudenberg-NOK.
production. Journal of Operations Management 25, 785–805. Production 105 (12), 66–71.
Shannon, C., 1948. A mathematical theory of communications. Bell Vinokur-Kaplan, G.S., 1995. Treatment teams that work (and those that
Systems Technical Journal 27, 397–423, 623–656. don’t): An application of Hackman’s group effectiveness model to
Sheridan, J.H., 1997. Kaizen blitz. Industry Week 246 (16), 18–27. interdisciplinary teams in psychiatric hospitals. The Journal of
Simons, D., Taylor, D., 2007. Lean thinking in the UK red meat industry: Applied Behavioral Science 31 (3), 303–327.
A systems and contingency approach. International Journal of Vits, J., Gelders, L., 2002. Performance improvement theory. International
Production Economics 106, 70–81. Journal of Production Economics 77, 285–298.
Smith, B., 2003. Lean and Six Sigma—A one–two punch. Quality Progress Wall, T.D., Kemp, N.J., Jackson, P.R., Clegg, C.W., 1986. Outcomes of
36 (4), 37–41. autonomous workgroups: A long-term field experiment. Academy of
Stewart, G.L., Barrick, M.R., 2000. Team structure and performance: Management Review 29 (2), 280–304.
Assessing the mediating role of intrateam process and the moderat- Warnecke, H.J., Huser, M., 1995. Lean production. International Journal of
ing role of task type. Academy of Management Journal 43 (2), Production Economics 41, 37–43.
135–148. Watson, L., 2002. Striving for continuous improvement with fewer
Sundstrom, E., DeMeuse, K.P., Futrell, D., 1990. Work teams: Applications resources? Try kaizen. Marshall Star, 28 November 2002, vol. 1, 4 pp.
and effectiveness. American Psychologist 45 (2), 120–133. Wheatley, B., 1998. Innovation in ISO registration. CMA 72 (5), 23.
Susman, G.I., Dean, J.W., 1992. Development of a model for predicting Whitford, A.B., Yates, J., 2003. Policy signals and executive governance:
design for manufacturability effectiveness. In: Susman, G.I. (Ed.), Presidential rhetoric in the war on drugs. The Journal of Politics 65
Integrating Design and Manufacturing for Competitive Advantage. (4), 995–1012.
Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 207–227. Wilson, L., Van Aken, E.M., Frazier, D., 1998. Achieving high performance
Szinovacz, M.E., Davey, A., 2001. Retirement effects on parent–adult child work systems through policy deployment: a case application. In:
contacts. The Gerontologist 41 (2), 191–200. Proceedings of the 1998 International Conference on Work Teams.
Tanner, C., Roncarti, R., 1994. Kaizen leads to breakthroughs in Withey, M., Daft, R.L., Cooper, W.H., 1983. Measures of Perrow’s work unit
responsiveness—And the Shingo Prize—At Critikon. National Pro- technology: An empirical assessment and a new scale. Academy of
ductivity Review 13 (4), 517–531. Management Journal 26 (1), 45–63.
Taylor, D.L., Ramsey, R.K., 1993. Empowering employees to ‘just do it’. Wittenberg, G., 1994. Kaizen—The many ways of getting better.
Training and Development 47 (5), 71–76. Assembly Automation 14 (4), 12+ (6pp.).
Teachman, J.D., 1980. Analysis of population diversity. Sociological Womack, J.P., Jones, D.T., Roos, D., 1990. The Machine that Changed the
Methods and Research 8 (3), 341–362. World. Harper Perennial, New York.
Tranfield, D., Smith, S., Foster, M., Wilson, S., Parry, I., 2000. Strategies for Wright, R., 1936. Factors affecting the cost of airplanes. Journal of
managing the teamworking agenda: Developing a methodology for Aeronautical Sciences 3, 122–128.
team-based organization. International Journal of Production Eco- Yin, R.K., 1994. Case Study Research: Design and Methods. Sage
nomics 65, 33–42. Publications, Inc., Thousand Oaks, CA.

You might also like