You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/19867384

The Relation of Empathy to Aggressive and Externalizing/Antisocial


Behavior

Article  in  Psychological Bulletin · June 1988


DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.324 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

720 8,654

2 authors:

Paul A. Miller Nancy Eisenberg


Arizona State University Arizona State University
19 PUBLICATIONS   3,731 CITATIONS    514 PUBLICATIONS   38,578 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Stress, Coping, and Adaptation View project

Nancy Eisenberg View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nancy Eisenberg on 05 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1988, Vol. 103, No. 3,324-344 0033-2909/88/J00.75

The Relation of Empathy to Aggressive


and Externalizing/Antisocial Behavior

Paul A. Miller and Nancy Eisenberg


Arizona State University

A number of researchers have proposed that empathy, sympathy, or both, defined primarily in affec-
tive terms, may inhibit aggressive and antisocial behaviors (N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, 1982; S.
Feshbach, 1970; Parke & Slaby, 1983). Apart from brief reviews, however, no systematic review
of the research concerning the relation of empathy/sympathy to aggression and other antisocial,
externalizing behaviors has been conducted. In this review, we organized the relation of empathy/
sympathy to relevant negative behaviors principally by mode of assessing empathy (i.e., picture/
story, questionnaire methods, facial/gestural reactions, and experimental inductions) and analyzed
empirical findings with meta-analytic techniques. Empathic/sympathetic responding was negatively
related to aggression and antisocial, externalizing behaviors for questionnaire methods and nega-
tively but nonsignificantly related for other indexes of empathy. Child abuse also was associated with
low levels of empathy/sympathy, as was the receipt of such abuse. Relations between the empathy
indexes and aggression/externalizing behaviors were generally the same for male and female subjects,
especially after controlling for sample size. We discuss conceptual issues related to the pattern of
findings, as well as directions for future research.

Over the years, philosophers have debated the mechanisms recent research, such affective processes appear to be positively
underlying or mediating prosocial and moral behavior. Kant associated with moral development (see Eisenberg & Miller,
(1788/1949), for example, proposed that moral principles and 1987; Eisenberg & Strayer, 1987).
behavior engendered by such principles are based on rational It also has been suggested that sympathetic and empathic re-
processes and are influenced little by emotion. On the other actions play an important function in the reduction or inhibi-
hand, Hume (1777/1966) argued that affective responses such tion of aggressive or antisocial actions toward others (N. D.
as sympathy and benevolence are primary motives underlying Feshbach, 1978, 1987; N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, 1982; S.
individuals' moral actions. More recently, some theorists have Feshbach, 1970; Parke & Slaby, 1983). That is, individuals who
sought to integrate emotional and cognitive influences in their vicariously experience the negative reactions of others that oc-
analyses of moral action (e.g., Blum, 1980; Hoffman, 1987). cur because of their own aggressive behavior may be less in-
The diversity of philosophical positions is reflected in the the- clined to continue their aggression or to aggress in future inter-
oretical and empirical approaches that psychologists have taken actions. Moreover, if sympathy and empathy inhibit negative
in their investigations of moral development. Until recently, the
social behaviors that have hurtful effects for others, including
bulk of the research literature concerned the role of rational
aggressive behavior, lower levels of individuals' empathic capac-
moral principles and social-cognitive skills such as perspective
ity or its expression may be associated with delays, arrests, or
taking in moral reasoning and behavior (e.g., Blasi, 1980; Kohl-
other dysfunctions of sociomoral development. These may in-
berg, 1981; Rest, 1983; Underwood & Moore, 1982). There is
clude greater incidence of antisocial behaviors and other forms
now, however, a considerable body of research in which the role
of externalizing psychopathology. Thus, one might expect indi-
of empathy or sympathy, denned primarily in affective terms,
viduals from normal as well as clinical populations who express
has been examined in relation to moral and positive social be-
aggressive, delinquent, and antisocial behavior to exhibit less
havior (e.g., Batson & Coke, 1981; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987;
empathic responsiveness toward others than do other people.
see Eisenberg & Strayer, 1987). Especially when one considers
Indeed, a deficiency in the capacity to respond to others em-
pathically may be a contributing factor to dysfunctions in social
interaction that attain clinical levels (Gibbs, 1987).
This work was funded in part by National Institute of Mental Health The primary objectives of this review were to examine two
National Research Service Award 1F32MH09263-01 to Paul A. Miller related issues: (a) whether individuals who are relatively aggres-
and National Science Foundation Award BNS-8509223 and National sive in their interactions with others also tend to be lower in
Institute of Child Health and Development Career Development Award
empathic/sympathetic responsiveness or both and (b) whether
1 K04HD00717 to Nancy Eisenberg.
individuals who exhibit antisocial and other forms of negative
We would like to express our gratitude to William Stock for advice
behavior that have negative consequences for others (but are not
regarding meta-analytic procedures.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paul solely or clearly aggression per se) differ from other people in
A. Miller or Nancy Eisenberg, Department of Psychology, Arizona State empathic responsiveness. Before reviewing the research, how-
University, Tempe, Arizona 85287. ever, we consider definitional issues regarding empathy and ag-

324
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 325

gression among clinical and nonelinical populations, as well as 1983), defining aggression has proven to be a controversial is-
theoretical issues regarding the relation of empathy to aggres- sue. In early research, the focus was on the injurious or hostile
sion and externalizing negative behaviors. intent of the aggressive act wherein the goal was harmful or de-
structive consequences to the person or object (e.g., Berkowitz,
Definitional Issues 1962; Dollard, Dobb, Miller, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939). Many
researchers currently draw distinctions among aggressive be-
In the psychological literature, empathy has been denned as haviors on the basis of intent, for example, distinctions among
both a cognitive and affective response. Moreover, the terms instrumental aggression used to claim an object or to gain sta-
sympathy and empathy have often been used interchangeably. tus in a group (e.g., Campbell, Muncer, & Bibel, 1985; S. Fesh-
For example, both sympathy (e.g., Cooley, 1902/1956; Mead, bach, 1970; Parke & Slaby, 1983), retaliatory aggression in re-
1934) and empathy (e.g., Hogan, 1969) have been defined as sponse to provocation, and aggressive behaviors used simply to
the ability to identify others' emotional states or to assume the succeed in game playing (see Attili, 1985). Other researchers
emotional role of the other, that is, to achieve a cognitive under- have included other social-cognitive criteria, in which the char-
standing of the feelings of the other person. Others, however, acteristics of the aggressor, social context, nature of the recipi-
have denned empathy (N. D. Feshbach, 1978; Hoffman, 1984; ent's response to the act, and observer perceptions determine
Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972; Stotland, 1969) and sympathy whether a behavior is aggressive (e.g., Bandura, 1973; Dodge,
(Batson, in press; McDougall, 1908/1950; Smith, 1759/1948) 1980; Perry, Perry, & Rasmussen, 1986). In reviewing the litera-
mostly in affective terms, that is, as a vicarious emotional re- ture, however, it often was not possible to determine how re-
sponse to a person's affective state or situation that is similar to searchers defined and operationalized aggressive behavior.
or at least congruent with that state. In the latter sense, sympa- Moreover, the types of aggression included in empirical studies
thetic responses are not necessarily denned as matching the have varied considerably (e.g., from verbal remarks to physical
emotions of the other person; they are merely consistent with violence). Thus, to provide a comprehensive assessment of the
the other's state. For example, a person may report feelings of relation of aggression to empathy, we included in this review a
concern regarding someone else's distress. range of behaviors defined as aggressive. Moreover, because
For the purposes of this review, empathy is denned as an emo- other negative behaviors—such as cheating, lying, and steal-
tional response evoked by the affective state or situation of the ing—frequently involve aggressive encounters and possible in-
other person. This emotion may be either identical or similar to jury to others, we also examined the relations of such behaviors
the state of the other and involves at least a minimal degree to empathy and sympathy.
of self-other differentiation. Thus, empathy includes responses There also exist a number of systems for classifying psycho-
such as emotional matching and, in general, the vicarious expe- pathological behavior, including antisocial behavior. An espe-
riencing of emotions consistent with those of others. Sympathy, cially promising approach has grown out of efforts to develop
on the other hand, is defined as an emotional response, elicited behaviorally based classification systems for psychopathology
by the emotional state or situation of the other person, that is (see Achenbach, 1978; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1979; Quay
not identical to the other's emotion and involves feelings of con- & Parsons, 1971; Spivak, Swift, & Prewitt, 1971). With these
cern or sorrow for the other person. Frequently, especially for systems, forms of psychopathology among children and adoles-
individuals mature enough to differentiate between their own cents are differentiated as a function of expressed, observable
and others' internal states, empathy can be expected to lead to behavior. In factor analytic research of relevant behaviors, in-
sympathy or to co-occur. In the literature, however, it often is vestigators have repeatedly identified two "broadband" clinical
not possible to ascertain whether authors are referring to empa- groups (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1984). These groups have
thy, sympathy, or some combination. Therefore, for the sake of been labeled "undercontrolled versus overcontrolled" or "inter-
simplicity, in such cases we use the term empathy to refer to nalizing versus externalizing" (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1979,
both sympathy and empathy. 1984).
Most theorists believe that empathy, albeit characterized by Of relevance to this discussion is the category of externalizing
its affective component, involves cognition as well as affect. For behavior. The negative behaviors of externalizing individuals
example, some have suggested that the ability to differentiate tend to be expressed outwardly and are likely to directly affect
among and identify others' affective states and the ability to take other people and society at large. Externalizing behaviors range
the cognitive and affective perspective of others (i.e., role take) from threatening, attacking, and fighting with others to general
are prerequisites for empathizing with someone (Batson, in disobedience and serious conduct disorders (see Achenbach &
press; N. D. Feshbach, 1978). Others have argued that less ad- Edelbrock, 1979), behaviors that seemingly reflect less aware-
vanced cognitive processes, such as classical conditioning, may ness of or concern for the affective consequences of one's behav-
be all that are necessary for some modes of empathic respond- ior for others.
ing (Hoffman, 1984). Most likely, cognitive processes affecting Because of the general recognition of this differentiation in
empathic expression operate at levels consistent with normal clinical research (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1984) and the role
cognitive-developmental processes, thus becoming more so- of aggression and other negative social behaviors in externaliz-
phisticated with age and more evident among older than youn- ing conditions, we included externalizing behavior in our review
ger people (Hoffman, 1984). of the relevant research. Although no equivalent behaviorally
A number of definitional issues are also related to the con- based classification system exists for adults' behaviors, the Di-
cepts of aggression and negative, externalizing behavior. As re- agnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders of the
cent reviewers have indicated (Attili, 1985; Parke & Slaby, American Psychiatric Association (1980) contains diagnostic
326 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

categories for adults and children similar to the Achenbach is, aggressive children appear to interpret social cues in ways
(1978) classification system. Thus, when appropriate, we in- that are relatively likely to elicit feelings of personal distress,
cluded samples of adults' externalizing behaviors in our review. threat, or anger, resulting in behavior consistent with their inter-
pretation of the situation rather than the affective state of the
other person. Consistent with this view, Gough (1948) and Hare
Theoretical Issues
(1970) have suggested that a history of antisocial behavior is
As noted above, social and developmental psychologists have the result of a deficiency in perspective taking, which would be
used empathy to refer to the ability or tendency to be vicari- expected to be associated with lower levels of sympathy (S.
ously aroused by the affective state of another person. This vi- Feshbach & N. D. Feshbach, 1986; Hoffman, 1984). Thus, the
carious arousal often is assumed to engender sympathetic con- relation between empathy (and sympathy) and aggressive/anti-
cern for the other, aversive arousal within the observer (labeled social behavior may occur for a variety of reasons, reasons
personal distress; see Batson & Coke, 1981), or both (Batson, linked to both the cognitive and affective components of em-
in press; Hoffman, 1984). According to some theorists (e.g., pathy.
N. D. Feshbach, 1982; Hoffman, 1984), observation of others' A type of aggression of special note is serious aggression to-
expressions of distress or pain often results in the observer's ward family members. Researchers have found that abusive par-
experiencing similar distress by means of vicarious emotional ents, in comparison with nonabusive parents, tend to engage in
responding. When the observers themselves are the instigators more negative, coercive interactions with their children (Bur-
of aggression, they may vicariously experience the negative gess & Conger, 1978; Reid, 1986); respond less appropriately to
arousal induced by their own actions (i.e., they experience per- their children (Fontana & Robison, 1984); express more nega-
sonal distress). Reduction of aggressive behavior in interactions tive affect; and use more punitive rearing strategies (Howes &
with others would therefore be reinforcing for the aggressor be- Feshbach, 1986; Trickett & Kuczynski, 1983). Conversely,
cause it would result in less vicarious negative arousal (N. D. mothers' communication of concern and altruistic administra-
Feshbach, 1978; N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, 1982). Some tions to their children and others (called empathic caregiving)
theorists (e.g., Hoffman, 1984) further suggest that feelings of have been positively related to their use of reasoning (e.g., refer-
(or anticipation of) such distress will inhibit immoral behavior ences to consequences for others or moral principles) and ex-
primarily when the individual feels responsible for the distress pression of emotion in child-caused distress situations (Zahn-
state of the other person. Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & King, 1979). Thus, it is reasonable to
For those who experience sympathy rather than (or in addi- suggest that abusive parents, in comparison with other parents,
tion to) personal distress as a result of empathizing, one would may be less sympathetically or vicariously aroused by their chil-
also expect a reduction in negative behavior because of the de- dren's pain cues and negative emotional reactions (also see
sire to improve the other's condition and to rectify any harm. N. D. Feshbach, 1987).
Sympathy may evolve from a sense of connectedness with oth- Moreover, to the extent that abused children's needs and feel-
ers and a positive valuing of others (Staub, 1986), both of which ings are not recognized or responded to appropriately in inter-
should preclude harming others. Moreover, the role-taking ac- actions with an abusive parent, they may have little experience
tivities that often are part of sympathizing and mature empathy of empathic responding themselves or opportunity to learn to
should result in a reduction of misunderstandings, accompa- identify and experience the affective cues and states of others.
nied by a lessening of conflict and aggression (S. Feshbach & Furthermore, if Staub (1986) is correct that sympathetic re-
N. D. Feshbach, 1986). sponding evolves from positive feelings toward others and feel-
Consistent with the aforementioned theorizing, such situa- ings of interconnectedness with others, abused children should
tional factors as the immediacy and intensity of pain cues have be relatively unlikely to develop high levels of empathic/sympa-
been associated with lower levels of aggression (e.g., Baron, thetic responding. Consistent with this view, according to the
1971; Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972). These cues should evoke limited relevant data, the development of empathy in children
aversive personal distress reactions or sympathetic concern, ei- appears to be enhanced by supportive parenting (Barnett, 1987;
ther of which could inhibit aggression. In some research, how- Zahn-Waxier et al., 1979). It is also possible, however, that the
ever, a victim's expression of pain has been associated with in- abused child, because of exposure to the strong negative emo-
creased aggression, especially under conditions of prior anger tions of the abusive parent, may become acutely sensitive to
arousal or strong provocation (S. Feshbach, Stiles, & Bitter, emotional cues portending punishment or stress (Squires,
1967;Hartmann, 1969; Perry & Perry, 1974). Moreover, and of 1979). In such cases, the abused child may show awareness of
particular relevance to this review, the presence of cues indica- others' negative affect but express inappropriate social re-
tive of pain appears to be associated with higher levels of aggres- sponses to them, for example, defensiveness, rejection, or ag-
sion in people with established histories of aggressiveness and gression as opposed to nurturant, positive social responses. This
delinquency (Perry & Perry, 1974). Thus, aggressive individuals may be because abusing parents do not provide models of ap-
may not respond vicariously to others' emotions or interpret propriate behavioral and emotional responding to others in
others' pain cues in the way that less aggressive individuals do. need and are likely to use child-rearing practices (e.g., physical
Part of the reason for aggressive children's relative indiffer- punishment and unexplained prohibitions) that do not pro-
ence to pain cues may lie in the fact that aggressive children mote empathic or prosocial responsiveness (N. D. Feshbach,
may make interpretations of others' behaviors in social interac- 1987;Zahn-Waxleretal., 1979).
tions that differ from those made by less aggressive children On the basis of the theoretical considerations above, we hy-
(Dodge, 1980; Gouze, Rayais, & Bieber-Schneider, 1983). That pothesized that aggressive behavior, including physical and ver-
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 327

bal aggression, as well as abusive behavior, should be negatively used with adults as indexes of empathic responsiveness (see Marcus,
related to empathy. We expected this relation to hold both for 1987).
dispositional indexes of empathy and for situational measures The definition of empathy also varies to some extent as a function of
the method of assessment. In picture/story methods, empathy typically
of empathy, that is, measures of both the trait of empathy and
is operationalized as the matching of one's emotional response to that
empathy toward the potential recipient of the person's aggres-
of a story character. Self-report questionnaires and induction methods
sion. We hypothesized that dispositional empathy would be as-
have involved the assessment of sympathetic concerns as well as emo-
sociated with low levels of aggression because individuals who tional matching and personal distress reactions in various situations.
tend in general to empathize should be more likely to do so in Similarly, facial/gestural methods may tap a range of affective reactions,
any given situation. Moreover, we reasoned that other actions including individuals' emotional matching, sympathetic responding,
included in the category of externalizing behaviors (e.g., lying, and personal distress reactions.
stealing, conduct disorders, and antisocial behavior), which are There are advantages and disadvantages to each method for assessing
somewhat different from or more inclusive than aggression, empathy (see Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987;
should be negatively related to empathy. We predicted this be- Eisenberg & Strayer, 1987). Self-report indexes of empathy often may
reflect individuals' desire to conform with gender-role stereotypes (Ei-
cause such behaviors have negative consequences for others,
senberg & Lennon, 1983). Picture/story indexes seem to be particularly
consequences that are similar to, if not often more serious than,
vulnerable to demand characteristics and to confounding factors, such
those for aggression in general. Finally, we hypothesized that
as sex of the experimenter (Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983). Facial indexes
victims of abuse would exhibit relatively low levels of empathy are a promising mode of assessing empathy but may be ineffective for
themselves because of their socialization experiences. Prior to people who mask their emotions. In brief, no single measure of empathy
turning to our review, we must describe the data set and compu- is ideal; a multimethod approach to the assessment of empathy is desir-
tational procedures used to conduct our meta-analyses. able but has seldom been used.
The second group of the research findings that were examined in-
cluded externalizing behaviors (e.g., conduct disorders and antisocial or
Methods of Analysis
problematic social behaviors) that were not explicitly or solely aggres-
Description of the Data Set sive in nature. Studies were included in these analyses if they assessed
(a) externalizing behaviors, classified as such by using standard clinical
As far as we are aware, there are only two reviews of the relation of indexes (e.g., Achenbach & Edelbrock's, 1979, system), or (b) problem-
empathy/sympathy to aggression, and these reviews are brief (N. D. atic social behaviors typically associated with externalizing behaviors
Feshbach, 1978, 1987). Consequently, we conducted a comprehensive (e.g., acting out behaviors). We assessed the relation of empathy to exter-
search of all relevant research—including published studies, unpub- nalizing/antisocial behaviors by using actual or estimated measures of
lished manuscripts, and dissertations—using relevant document association, derived in a number of cases by comparing the empathy
sources (e.g., Social Sciences Citation Index, Educational Resources In- level of these groups with that of samples from the normal population.
formation Center, Psychological Abstracts, recently published reviews, In the third grouping of analyses, studies concerning individuals who
and so forth). Whenever necessary, we sent letters requesting relevant were either the victims or perpetrators of abuse were examined sepa-
information from authors with unpublished or in-progress work. The rately because of the distinctive familial context in which these external-
objective of this wide search was to avoid the "file drawer" bias that can izing behaviors occurred and the fact that abuse can include behaviors
occur because studies involving nonsignificant findings are less likely to in addition to overt aggression (e.g., neglect). We examined studies con-
be obtained and included in reviews of research (Rosenthal, 1979). cerning the victims of abuse, all children in this case, independently of
The grouping of studies for the analyses was influenced by several data concerning adult abusers. Generally, the expression of empathy
theoretical and methodological issues in the empathy and aggression among each of these two groups was compared with normal or control
literature. In prior reviews of empathy and gender (Eisenberg & Len- samples.
non, 1983) and empathy and prosocial behavior (Eisenberg & Miller,
1987), the significance of the relation of empathy to the criterion mea-
sures was a function of the method used to assess empathy. Thus, in our Methods of Evaluation and Data Reduction
first grouping of studies, we examined the empathy-aggression relation
according to the mode of measuring empathy. We identified four fre- We analyzed the literature in the following manner: First, we com-
quently used methods of empathy assessment for studies pertaining to puted meta-analyses for each grouping of studies. When relevant, analy-
aggression: (a) picture/story methods, wherein individuals' self-re- ses were conducted to address questions of conceptual import to a group
ported responses to hypothetical stories (e.g., narratives, slide stories, of studies. Finally, in some meta-analyses, we also reviewed the data
short films, or some combination) are scored in terms of the degree to qualitatively.
which their reported affect matches that of a story protagonist; (b) facial In general, the term mela-analysis refers to statistical procedures
affect/gestural reactions to others' emotions or states as depicted in films used to aggregate the results of independent studies relevant to testing
or picture/story stimuli; (c) individuals' self-reports on questionnaires hypothesized relations between variables. A variety of methods for com-
or scales that assess empathic/sympathetic tendencies across events and puting meta-analyses have been used (e.g., Cooper, 1979; Hedges & Ol-
situations; and (d) experimental induction procedures designed to elicit kin, 1986). The steps that we followed in conducting the meta-analyses
empathic responses, principally through manipulations of observa- are outlined in more detail elsewhere (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987; Hedges
tional set or degree of perceived similarity with a purported victim. & Olkin, 1986). Briefly, the procedure first involved converting all sta-
To some extent, the empathy indexes have been used more or less tistics (e.g., F values, I values, and proportions) into indexes of associa-
with particular age groups. For example, picture/story measures have tion (see Ferguson, 1976; Stock, Okun, Haring, Kinney, & Miller, 1979,
been used solely with very young and elementary-school children, for formulas). Next, we transformed the estimated or actual correlations
whereas self-report questionnaires and induction procedures generally into z values by using r-to-z transformation tables available in a stan-
have been used with older children, adolescents, and adults. Measures dard statistics text. Third, we computed a frequency-weighted estimate
of facial affect and gestural responses, initially used with younger chil- of the common correlation (Z.,) with the formula z+ = Sf., [(«< - 3)
dren as an alternative to picture/story methods, are just beginning to be (zi)}/(N — 3X), in which N = the total number of subjects across all
328 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

samples, n = the number of subjects in a given sample, and K - the empathy would have to be averaged in a single sample (e.g., picture/
number of samples (Hedges & Olkin, 1986). Then, to determine story and self-report on questionnaire measures). There were no cases,
whether the hypothesized relation among the variables (i.e., expressed however, in which a researcher used two different modes of assessing
as the common correlation) differed from 0, we computed the following empathy with the same sample. When multiple empathy measures were
z statistic: z = z+ V(N- 3K), in which N = the total number of subjects used, they were of the same type (e.g., self-report on questionnaires;
across all samples. We then tested the significance of the effect by using Lee, 1983). In this case, we averaged the data by using procedures de-
the standard normal curve ( f o r p = .05 and z — 1 .96) and critical values scribed previously.
for a two-tailed test. We chose two-tailed tests to provide a more conser- Finally, in a number of the studies, specific statistics on the relation
vative test of the empathy-aggression relation because of the large Ns in of empathy to the relevant behaviors were not provided. Sometimes, all
some analyses (and because they have been used in determining the that was reported was that a particular comparison was significant or
significance of analyses in the meta-analytic work of others, such as nonsignificant. At other times, the results of comparisons were not re-
Hedges & Olkin, 1986). We then calculated confidence intervals for this ported at all, leaving one to infer that they were nonsignificant. When
effect for the sample and population estimates of z+ (Hedges & Olkin, the findings were reported as significant, we assumed them to be so at a
1986). probability value of .05 for a two-tailed test. We used the assumption of
We also conducted tests of the homogeneity of the estimated com- a two-tailed test because it is commonly used among researchers and
mon correlations with the following formula: Q = 2 fc[(«;~ 3)z?) - because it was the criterion that we used to assess the significance of the
results of our meta-analyses. When merely nonsignificance was re-
i/ — 3A). The objective of this computation was to ported, we could not estimate a precise index of association because
1 2 ("i ~ 3)z,- ) /2
\;-i / (.1 there was no means for determining the p value. In such cases, we as-
'
sumed that the nonsignificant findings averaged out, across studies, to
test the assumption that the correlations reported in the samples for a a correlation of 0 (see Cooper, 1979).
given meta-analysis came from a single underlying population (see In summary, we organized the analysis of the relation of empathy to
Hedges & Olkin, 1986). If this assumption is not met, it suggests that aggression according to the method used to assess empathy. We orga-
the estimates of the common correlation from the samples may not fit nized the analyses of the relation of empathy to externalizing/antisocial
the model of a single underlying population correlation. Accordingly, behaviors first independently of the mode of assessment of empathy and
the test for the homogeneity of the common correlations is reported then by the two predominant modes of assessing empathy found (i.e.,
prior to testing for the significance of z+. However, we conducted tests picture/story and questionnaire methods). We collapsed studies rele-
of significance of the common correlation even when the estimates of r vant to the relation of empathy and abuse across measures of empathy
did not meet the assumption of homogeneity. In such cases, we deemed for children and adults. The computation of the various meta-analyses
the outcomes of these tests important but viewed them with caution included (a) a test of the homogeneity of the estimates of the common
because of the heterogeneous nature of the samples. correlation, (b) computation of a weighted estimate of the common cor-
We encountered several issues in selecting samples to include in a relation (z+), (c) a test of the significance of the common correlation
given meta-analysis. As described above, we organized data related to using the standard normal curve, (d) computation of confidence inter-
the empathy-aggression relation by mode of assessing empathy (i.e., vals for z + , and (e) computation of estimates of the population values
self-report questionnaires and facial/gestural, picture/story, and induc- for z+ and its confidence intervals (see Hedges & Olkin, 1986).
tion methods). Sometimes, more than one statistic concerning the rela-
tion between empathy and aggression was reported in a single study. To
conform to the requirement that the data for each sample (not study or Review of the Empirical Research
article) within a meta-analysis should represent an independent esti-
mate of the empathy-aggression relation, we reviewed multiple empa- Relation of Empathy to Aggression
thy indexes in any given study separately, according to the mode of as-
sessment (e.g., facial/gestural and picture/story measures). Picture/story assessments of empathy. Picture/story meth-
When indexes obtained with the same method of assessing empathy ods have been the most commonly used procedure for assessing
were available for the same sample (e.g., two self-report questionnaires), children's empathy. The most popular of these has been N. D.
we usually averaged the statistics and used the mean in the meta-analy-
Feshbach & Roe's (1968) Affective Situations Test for Empathy.
sis. For some samples, a composite score for empathy was reported (e.g.,
With procedures of this sort, children typically are presented
a composite of responses to several picture/story vignettes). We used
with narratives (accompanied by visual stimuli) depicting other
this composite score in the analysis if either (a) no other statistic was
children in emotion-eliciting situations (e.g., the story protago-
available or (b) the composite score was deemed the most adequate and
did not mask potentially significant patterns of association (e.g., age or nist loses her dog). After listening to each story, the child is
gender differences). Moreover, if a given measure of association exhib- asked to report how he or she feels. A response is considered
ited considerable variation across subgroups (e.g., sex or age), then we empathic if it is identical or similar to the emotion that the story
treated the subgroups as separate samples. In general, we reported all character is likely to have experienced.
available measures of a given relation in the tables (unless there were In early research of this type (N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach,
many) but averaged these estimates and used mean values when com- 1969; N. D. Feshbach & Roe, 1968), children were required to
puting the meta-analyses. verbally describe their feeling state in response to story events.
Because relatively few studies concerning externalizing behavior were
More recently, because of concerns about confounding chil-
available, we first combined the modes of assessing empathy (e.g., self-
dren's verbal ability with their actual experience of emotion, a
report questionnaires and facial/gestural methods) in the analysis of the
number of researchers have added nonverbal methods for as-
relation between empathy and externalizing/antisocial behavior and
sessing empathy (e.g., lannotti, 1975). With this approach, chil-
abuse. We computed additional analyses, however, for each commonly
used method of assessing empathy (i.e., picture/story and questionnaire dren are asked to point to pictures of facial expressions depict-
methods) for exploratory purposes. ing different emotions to indicate their response to story events.
The procedure for combining different modes of assessing empathy Some researchers have modified the original procedure or
raises the possibility that indexes for two different modes of assessing have scored empathy differently. Specifically, they have coded
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 329

intensity of affective response (N. D. Feshbach, 1980,1982) or The Mehrabian and Epstein (1972) scale of emotional ten-
emotional responses to different aspects of the story situation dency is the most frequently used index of empathy for older
(lannotti, 1975; lannotti & Pierrehumbert, 1985) or have adolescents and adults. Bryant (1982) developed a modified ver-
scored both empathy and sympathy (Staub & Feinberg, 1980). sion of this scale for use with children 6 years of age and older.
Researchers have noted other problems with picture/story as- Both scales tap a variety of individuals' emotional reactions,
sessments of empathy. Principally, these problems have in- including (a) susceptibility to emotional contagion, (b) under-
volved (a) the effect of sex of experimenter on boys' versus girls' standing of the feelings of familiar or unfamiliar people, (c)
responding (children score higher if interviewed by same-sex emotional responsiveness to others' emotions, and (d) sympa-
experimenters; Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983), (b) the problem of thetic feelings toward others. It is likely, however, that some
repeated elicitations of different emotions in a very short period items also assess the tendency to experience personal distress,
of time, and (c) the use of short hypothetical events that may not as well as other types of emotional or social-cognitive reactions
evoke much emotion (Hoffman, 1982). Furthermore, Eisenberg or both.
and Miller (1987) found this mode of assessing empathy to be Indexes of aggression for adults in studies involving question-
unrelated to indexes of prosocial behavior. Thus, there is reason naire indexes of empathy frequently have been measures of re-
to believe that many picture/story measures are less valid than actions in laboratory situations. For example, aggression often
other indexes of empathy. has been operationalized as the frequency or intensity of noise
Having discussed some of the issues that researchers have or shocks supposedly administered to another person during a
faced with this assessment method, we turn to the results of learning task or as ratings of individuals' aggression toward oth-
studies concerning the relation of aggression to picture/story ers in simulated interactions (e.g., Aleksic, 1976; Gaines, Kir-
indexes of empathy. We located 11 relevant studies (including win, & Gentry, 1977). For children, indexes of aggression more
18 samples). (See Table 1.) These samples were homogeneous often have been based on observational measures or ratings by
with regard to their estimate of the common correlation, x2(l 7, parents, teachers, or peers.
JV = 18) = 20.61, /is. The common correlation (z+) was -.06 Questionnaire studies concerning the relation of empathy to
(z = 1.33); thus, the trend was in the predicted direction, but it aggression are presented in Table 2. We located nine studies,
was not significant. The confidence interval at the 95th percen- including 15 samples. The samples were not homogeneous re-
tile for z+ was -.14 to .03. The population estimate for z+ was garding their estimates of the common correlation, x2(14, N =
—.06, and its high and low confidence interval values were —.14 15) = 23.72,p < .05. The common correlation was —.18, which
and .03, respectively. was highly significant (z = 4.90, p < .001); the confidence inter-
The association between picture/story indexes of empathy val for z+ was -.11 to -.25. The estimates of the population
and aggression appeared more consistent with expectations for correlation and associated confidence intervals were identical.
the older age groups. When we omitted preschool samples from Moreover, when we inspected the individual samples, the nega-
the analysis, the estimates of the common correlation for the tive relation appeared to be consistent across the entire range of
samples of the older groups were homogeneous, x2(8, N = 9) = ages of the samples, from middle childhood to adulthood, as
7.26, ns. The estimate of the common correlation was signifi- well as for subgroups varying in level of aggression (e.g., Aleksic,
cant and in the predicted direction (z+ = -. 13; z - 2.52, p < 1976; Hoppe & Singer, 1976).
.02) for nine samples. The sample confidence interval ranged Facial/gestural indexes of empathy. The use of facial/ges-
from -.24 to -.03. The corresponding population estimate for tural indexes is a relatively new methodology for the assessment
z+ also was —.13, and its associated confidence interval matched of empathic responsiveness. Consequently, the number of avail-
the sample values. able studies is quite limited, and all involve younger children,
One possible reason for the low correlation between empathy for whom the technique was originally designed. This method
and picture/story indexes is that picture/story indexes often in- was developed, at least in part, to offset the problem of requiring
clude the assessment of empathy with others' positive as well as children to report their emotions verbally.
negative emotions. Empathy with positive emotions has been We located five studies, involving 10 samples (Table 3). The
positively related to boys' aggression, whereas empathy with samples were homogeneous regarding their estimate of the
dysphoric emotions has been negatively related (N. D. Fesh- common correlation, x2(9, N = 10) = 3.64, ns. The common
bach, 1982). Moreover, adult males' empathizing with positive correlation (z+) was -.06, ns(z = .84, ns), and the confidence
emotions has been associated with cognitive role-taking skills, interval was -.20 to .08. The population estimates for the com-
whereas empathizing with negative emotions has been associ- mon correlation and the corresponding confidence interval
ated with empathic concern (i.e., sympathy; Davis, Hull, were -.06 and -.21 to .08, respectively.
Young, & Warren, 1987). Thus, it is possible that the relation In a previous analysis of the relation between facial affect
of picture/story indexes to aggression would be more consistent measures of empathy and prosocial behavior (Eisenberg &
if researchers would separate findings for empathy according to Miller, 1987), type of empathy stimuli affected the estimate of
positive and negative emotions. the common correlation. (The relation was stronger for films
Self-report on questionnaires. Questionnaire indexes of em- than for slide stories.) We therefore recomputed the analyses
pathy have been used primarily with older children and with with slide stories removed. The estimate of the common corre-
adults. Generally, questionnaires reflect the assumption that the lation, however, remained nonsignificant (for films only, z+ =
trait of empathy is being assessed because individuals report -.07,z = .92).
their emotional reactions to others across a broad set of situa- In several studies, measures of children's positive as well as
tions. negative facial affect were used. Conceptually, however, the rela-
330 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Table 1
Relation of Picture/Story Indexes of Empathy to Aggression

Estimate Relation to
Study Age, sex, and N Measure of empathy Measure of aggression ofr hypothesis

Bazar 4-5 years Affect matching Teachers' ratings of


(1976/1977) children
M,;v=36 Teasing NR" 0
Physical force in disputes NR' 0
Wildness in games NRa 0
F,tf=36 Teasing NR" 0
Physical force in disputes NR" 0
Wildness in games .24" -
N. D. Feshbach G3-G4 Videotape affect- Peers', teachers' and self-
(1980, 1982) matching task ratings of aggression
M,N=50< Affect matching
Euphoric affect Ratings in general ns 0
Dysphoric affect ns 0
Affect Matching X
Affect Intensity
Euphoric affect Teachers' ratings MR' 0
Peers' ratings .28= —
Self-ratings .28' -
Dysphoric affect Ratings in general -.28= +
F,N=501 Affect matching' Ratings in general -.28° +
Affect Matching X Ratings in general -.28' +
Affect Intensity'

N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach 4-5 years PASTE Teachers' ratings of peer-


(1969) M,JV=24 directed verbal and physical .43° -
F,AT=24 aggression .10= —
6-7 years
M, N = 20 -.44= +
F,JV=20 .24= -
Howard 4 years Affect matching Observers' ratings of peer-
(1983) directed aggression
M,JV=17 Physical -.24 +
Nonphysical .00 0
F,N= 18 Physical .22 —
Nonphysical .19 -

Huckabay GlandG3, PASTE Teachers' ratings of overt -.26 +


(1971) MandF, physical and verbal
N=60 aggression

lannotti K and 03, Affect matching Self-report of aggression in


(1975) M,N=60 Emotional matching* hypothetical situations .06 —
Situational matching" -.23 +

lannotti & Pierrehumbert 2 years Affect matching Observers' ratings of peer-


(1985) (retested at 5 directed aggression
years for
empathy),
M and F,N = Emotional matching* -.52 +
44 Situational matching11 .21 -

Marcus, Roke, & Bruner 3-6 years Affect matching Teachers' ratings of peer-
(1976) M,Ar=21 directed physical and verbal .14 -
F,N-n aggression .39 -

Nielsen Latency and Affect matching Teachers' ratings S1 +


(1976) adolescence, M

Staub ( 1 986) and Staub and G3-G4 Observers' ratings of peer-


Feinberg(1980) directed hostile and
instrumental aggression
(verbal and physical)
M,JV=20 Parallel matching affect' us 0
F,N=26 Parallel matching affect ns 0
Sympathetic affect ns 0
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 331

Table 1 (Continued)

Estimate Relation to
Study Age, sex, and N Measure of empathy Measure of aggression ofr hypothesis

Steinman GlandG3, PASTE Disrupting peers' winning a -.09 +


(1979) M,AT=72 game in an experimental
situation

Note. M = male; F = female; G = grade; K = kindergarten; PASTE = N. D. Feshbach and Roe (1968) Affective Situation Test of Empathy; Affect
matching = empathy was assumed if the participants' reported affect matched that of the hypothetical other; NR = not reported; + = negative
relation between measure of empathy and aggression; - = positive relation; 0 = no relation.
" Assumed to be 0. b The correlation was Kendall's tau. c Estimate of r was computed from the Mann-Whitney U statistic. d The author did
not report the sample size by sex. Therefore, an equal number of subjects was assigned to each sex. • Estimated from the report of
significance. f The author reported negative correlations between empathy and aggression for girls, regardless of type or intensity of empathic
affect. Therefore, we combined euphoric and dysphoric empathic affects in the table. 8 Matching of one's own affect to the facial cues of another if
facial and situational cues are incongruent. " Matching of one's own affect to situational rather than facial cues if facial and situational cues are
incongruent. ' Although the results were significant (S), the sample size could not be obtained for computing the estimate of r. Therefore, we
omitted this study from the meta-analysis. ' No boys reported sympathetic reactions.

tion between sad or negatively toned facial affect and lower lev- relation between empathy and aggression was negative and,
els of aggression is clearer than the relation of positive facial thus, in the predicted direction for four of the five samples.
affect to aggression. Positive affect could reflect an individual's Sex differences in the research concerning aggression. Re-
enjoyment of the anguish of the "victim" (leading to a positive searchers have frequently noted gender differences in the enact-
relation with aggression) or their discomfort or social anxiety ment of aggressive behaviors (Eagly & Steffen, 1986). Moreover,
(which would be expected to result in less aggression). However, Eisenberg and Lennon (1983) observed sex differences as a
recomputation of the analyses relating only negative facial function of the type of empathy measure. Consequently, we ex-
affect to aggression proved nonsignificant (z+ = -.01). amined the relation of empathy to aggression for each sex by
Experimental inductions of empathy. A common strategy for computing analyses within and across the three modes of mea-
studying the relation of empathy to other personality character- suring empathy (i.e., picture/story, facial/gestural, and ques-
istics has been to experimentally induce empathy and then tionnaire methods). (We omitted experimental induction stud-
compare individuals' subsequent behavior (e.g., aggression) ies because empathic responding was inferred rather than as-
with that of individuals in a control group. Such studies gener- sessed directly.)
ally have involved adults, although we did locate one study con- Across the three modes of empathy assessment, there were
ducted with adolescents. In all the relevant studies, empathy 16 studies, including 23 samples for males. The estimate of the
was manipulated with one of two approaches: (a) manipula- association between empathy and aggression was homoge-
2
tions of individuals' beliefs that they were similar to another neous, X (22, N = 23) = 32.58, ns (z+ = -.09, z = 2.58, which
person or (b) manipulations of individuals' observational sets was significant, p < .01). The confidence interval was from -.16
by means of encouraging them either to imagine how another to —.02. Population estimates of z+ and its associated confi-
felt or to objectively observe them. Manipulations that increase dence interval were identical.
perceptions of similarity are believed to enhance empathic re- For females, there were 13 studies, involving 15 samples. The
sponding because people seem to empathize more with people estimates of the association between empathy and aggression
like themselves (see N. D. Feshbach, 1978; Krebs, 1975; Staub, met the assumption of a common underlying population corre-
1986). Similarly, instructions to imagine how another feels are lation, x2(14, N= 15) = 14.95, ns. The estimate of the common
assumed to increase role taking and, consequently, empathic correlation (z+) was -.06 (z = 1.10), which was nonsignificant,
responding, whereas instructions simply to observe another are and the associated confidence interval for z+ was -.15 to .04.
expected to decrease the likelihood of individuals doing so (see The population estimate for z+ and its confidence interval were
Batson, in press; Stotland, 1969). the same as the sample estimates.
We located five studies, involving six samples (Table 4). The When we examined the relation between aggression and em-
samples were homogeneous, x2(5, N = 6) = 4.20, ns. The esti- pathy separately for different indexes of empathy, the relations
mate of the common correlation (z+) was —.08 (z = 1.37, ns), were nonsignificant for both sexes for picture/story and facial/
and the confidence interval was —. 19 to .03. The estimate of the gestural measures. For questionnaire methods, there was a non-
population correlation —.08, and its corresponding confidence significant trend (z+ = — .11, p < .15) for females in the pre-
interval was-. 19 to .03. dicted direction, whereas for males, the relation was significant
The lack of a relation between aggression and experimental (z+ = -.16, z = 3.24, p < .003). In summary, the estimates
manipulations of empathy did not seem to be due to ineffective of z+ were approximately equal for males and females for the
manipulations; in five of the six studies, the inducements of sim- different modes of assessing empathy: However, only the results
ilarity or perspective taking were effective. It is possible, how- for males were significant for questionnaire methods (which
ever, that the manipulations did not always affect individuals' may have been due, at least in part, to the considerably larger
empathic reactions per se, even if they did alter their perspective sample size for males).
taking or feelings of similarity. It should also be noted that the Empathy training and aggression. Another approach to ex-
332 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Table 2
Relation of Questionnaire Indexes of Empathy to Aggression

Measure of Estimate Relation to


Study Age, sex, and N empathy Measure of aggression ofr hypothesis

Aleksic 14- 16 years, M&E Administration of noise in a -.25 +

(1976) incarcerated learning task


delinquents, Intensity X Duration"
M,JV = 80

Bryant Gl Bryant scale Teachers' ratings of peer- -.57 +

(1982) U,N= 14 (1982) directed physical and


verbal aggression
F,JV= 12 .19 —
G4
M,N = 54 -.45 +
F, N = 57 .07 -
G7
M,N = 41 -.01 +
F,N=52 -.14 +
Gaines, Kirwin, & College students, Self-report of Administration of shock in -.46C +
Gentry M,N= 17 empathic a learning task
(1977) motivationb

Hoppe & Singer Patients at M&E Criminal offenses varying in ns 0


(1976) psychiatric aggression
hospital for
criminal
offenders,
M,,V= 115

Hunter(1984/ 14- 18 years, M&E Number of aggressive -.27 +

1985) incarcerated incidents


delinquents,
M,;V=59

Letourneau Abusive and M&E Aggressive responses in role- -.30 +


(1981) nonabusive playing situations
mothers, F,
N=60
Mehrabian & Study 1: College M&E Administration of shock in
Epstein students, M a learning task
(1972) andF
N=44 Immediate condition (can -.29" +
see victim)
N=44 Nonimmediate condition ns 0
Study 2: College M&E Questionnaire measure of -.31 +
students, aggression
M&F,

_
Polk (1976) College students, M&E Administration of noise in a -.03
M,A'=45 learning task

Rein (1974) College students, Modified Administration of shock in


M,N= 67) M&E a learning task
Duration .00 0
Mean intensity -.23 +
Maximum intensity -.14 +
Note. M = male; F = Female; G = grade; M & E = Mehrabian and Epstein's (1972) scale; Bryant scale = Index of Empathy for Children and
Adolescents (Bryant, 1982); + = negative relation between measure of empathy and aggression; - = positive relation; 0 = no relation.
• The Intensity X Duration index was deemed a better overall measure of aggression, although separate intensity and duration measures were also
available. b Obtained in a postexperiment interview. c The authors only reported significant F ratios for univariate analyses in which the mean
aggression scores for both the empathy and fear motivation groups were similar and lower than the means for other experimental groups. Because
no separate comparisons were reported between empathy and fear and other experimental groups, we derived the estimate of r on the basis of a p =
.05 significance level, even though the reported univariate p levels were more significant. d Estimated from the report of significance.
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 333

Table 3
Relation Between Children's Facial/Gestural Indexes Qf Empathy and Am ression

Estimate Relation to
Study Age, Sex, and TV Measure of empathy Measure of aggression ofr hypothesis

Ekman, Liebert, Friesen, 5-6 years Facial reactions" to Aggressive game and play behavior
Harrison, Zlatchin, aggressive films in experimental situations
Malmstrom, &
Baron (1972)
M,/V=15 Pleasant .18 +b
_b
Interest -.22
Happy .40 +b
Pain -.10 -f
Sad -.10 +
F,7V=15 Pleasant ns obb
Interest ns o
Happy ns 0
Pain .08 _-
Sad ns 0
Howard (1983) 4 years Facial reactions during Observers' ratings of peer-directed
administration of aggression
picture/story
measure
M , J V = 17 Physical -.10
Nonphysical -.01
F,JV = 18 Physical .04
Nonphysical .32
Marcus, Roke, & Bruner 3-7 years Facial reactions during Teachers' ratings of physical and
(1985) administration of verbal peer-directed
picture/story aggression
measure
M,N=2\ -.13
F,N= 11 -.12

Solomon (1985) K, G2, G4, G6 Facial reactions'to Teachers' ratings of verbal hostile,
films of children and physical aggression
in negative toward peers
situations
Pleasure 0"
Sadness 0
F,/V=43 Pleasure 0"
Sadness -.37

Steinman(1979) 6-8 years, M Facial reactions to film Intensity by duration of aggression


of two young men toward peer in experimental
fighting game
Pain .36"
Joy .13"
G5,N=25 Pain -.36"
Joy .13d

Note. M = male; F = female; K = kindergarten; G = grade; + = negative relation between empathy (exhibiting the same emotion as the other or
exhibiting sympathy) and aggression; — = positive relation; 0 = no relation.
1
We included the affective reactions that seemed most relevant to the issue at hand. Therefore, arousal (undifferentiated), anger, and surprise were
omitted. b A positive relation between positive affective reactions to aggressive films and aggressive behavior is consistent with the hypothesis of a
negative relation between empathy and aggression. c Because it was difficult to determine whether children's anger responses to the film situations
were in behalf of (i.e., empathic) or against the protagonist, we omitted anger reactions from the analyses. d We computed the estimate of r by
averaging children's facial affect during pain cue and high-action-violence scenes in which actors' facial pain expressions were shown in one condition
and not visible in a second condition.

amining the relation of empathy to aggression is to determine been to enhance cognitive role taking and problem-solving
whether training in empathy could (a) reduce individuals' ag- strategies rather than affective empathy (e.g., Chandler, 1973;
gressive behaviors, (b) improve their social interactions with Goldstein & Michaels, 1985; lannotti, 1978; Spivak & Shure,
others, or (c) both. Consequently, we reviewed literature on em- 1974). Generally, the results of these efforts have been inconsis-
pathy training and aggression. (See also Goldstein & Michaels, tent; some researchers have reported decreases in delinquent
1985, for a review of empathy training methods.) We found, behavior (Chandler, 1973) and a greater capacity to role take in
however, that the focus of many of the training programs has conflict situations (Berlin, 1978), whereas others have found no
334 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Table 4
Relation of Experimental Inductions of Empathy to Aggression

Type of experimental Estimate Relation to


Study Age, Sex, and N manipulation Measure of aggression ofr hypothesis

Aleksic(1976) Study 1: 14-16 Observational Delivery of noxious noise in -.13° +


years, and direct learning experiment
incarcerated experience sets Intensiy by duration"
delinquents, versus no set"
M,AT=80
Study 2: 14- 17 Similarity of feelings Delivery of noxious noise in a
years, to other person's learning experiment
incarcerated Duration ns 0
delinquents, Intensity -.71C +
M,AT=20

Eliasz(1980) 17-19 years, Observe other's Intensity of shock delivered in a .08" _


Polish, F, thoughts and learning task
N=9Q feelings versus no Duration of shock delivered -.04 +
set

Polk (1976) College students,


M
Imagine self as other Delivery of noxious noise in a -.36
versus no set learning experiment
Focus on selfe versus ns
no set

Rein (1974) College students, Similarity of Administration of shock in a


personality learning task
manipulated Duration .01
Maximum intensity -.12
Mean intensity -.10

Note. M = male; F = female; + « negative relation between the experimental manipulation of empathy and measure of aggression; - = positive
relation; 0 - no relation.
" Data were insufficient for comparison of observational set conditions separately. However, all the empathic observation conditions had lower mean
scores than the no-set (control) condition. " The Intensity X Duration index was deemed a better measure of aggression, although intensity and
duration measures also were available. ' We computed the estimate of r from the analysis of variance F value reported for the comparison of the
d
experimental and control groups. The no-set (control) group was used for comparison with the two observational set conditions. ' The focus-
on-self condition was included because a positive relation between empathic inhibition (as self-focus would be expected to elicit) and aggression is
consistent with the hypothesis of a negative relation between empathy and aggression.

change between empathy training and control groups after 198 l;Eisenberg& Miller, 1987;Hoffman, 1984). Thus, training
treatment (Bigler-Williams, 1984; lannotti, 1978) or have ob- of the affective components of empathy may promote the reduc-
tained mixed results (Kameya, 1976). tion of negative social behaviors as well as improve individuals'
One possible reason for these inconsistent findings is that ex- prosocial interactions with others.
periences relevant to the enhancement of the affective compo-
nent of empathy are necessary for reducing aggressive or antiso- Relation of Empathy to Psychopathology
cial behavior toward others. One research effort that has em-
and Physical Abuse
phasized affective (as well as cognitive) empathy training has
shown that the participants, in comparison with a control In this section, we examine data for samples in which individ-
group, reduced their aggressive behavior toward others (N. D. uals' behaviors were antisocial or could reasonably be catego-
Feshbach, 1980, 1982; N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, 1982). rized as belonging to an externalizing condition. Moreover, we
Although this effect was also found for a group of children who review data relevant to physical abuse.
received training in social problem-solving strategies, it is con- Externalizing and antisocial negative behavior. We identified
sistent with previous research to expect aggressive behavior to a variety of behaviors as externalizing for the purpose of this
be mediated by social-cognitive factors as well as empathy (see review. These included negative behaviors that were assessed by
Dodge, 1980; Parke & Slaby, 1983; Perry et al., 1986). More- using (a) other-ratings of problem behaviors that were then
over, children who received training in affective empathy also placed into diagnostic categories by using behaviorally based
exhibited more prosocial behavior, which was not observed for classification systems; (b) observations/ratings of behavior by
the cognitive problem-solving control group (N. D. Feshbach peers, teachers, adults, or correctional staff that were catego-
& S. Feshbach, 1982). This, too, is consistent with theory and rized on the basis of relevant problem behaviors but without
research suggesting that affective empathy may mediate proso- using behaviorally based systems; (c) experimental paradigms
cial and other, related social behaviors (see Batson & Coke, in which relevant behaviors were the focus of investigation; and
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 335

(d) self-report scales of psychopathology or related behaviors fidence interval was -.21 to -.08. This group of studies was
(Table 5). To determine whether a particular self-report scale homogeneous in their estimate of the relation, x2(9, N = 10) =
actually measured behaviors of an externalizing nature (e.g., 10.265, ns. Overall, then, questionnaire but not picture/story
H. J. Eysenck & S. B. G. Eysenck, 1976), we compared scale indexes of empathy were negatively related to externalizing and
items with the behaviors described in other behaviorally based to aggressive behaviors.
classification systems (see Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1979). Finally, the pattern of common correlations revealed a con-
One problem encountered when computing analyses with sistently negative relation between empathy and externalizing/
these studies was statistical. In computing our meta-analyses, antisocial behavior for both males and females. There were no
estimates of the common correlation for individual studies were discernible differences between the sexes across mode of assess-
weighted according to the size of the sample. Thus, the results ing empathy, especially when we omitted the two questionable
of especially large studies could strongly affect the estimate of studies.
the correlation when combined with the results of smaller stud- Physical abuse. We located relatively few studies concerning
ies. In the analyses of data related to the association between the level of affective empathy among physically abusive parents
externalizing behavior and empathy, there were two studies in or children who had been the victims of physical abuse. Thus,
which self-report data were obtained from a large number of although we computed meta-analyses with these samples, we
subjects (i.e., S. B. G. Eysenck, 1981; Saklofske & Eysenck, also reviewed them qualitatively.
1983). Because these two studies may have had an overwhelm- The analysis of the relation of empathy to physically abusive
ing effect on the results of the analyses, we computed two sets parental behavior was based on four studies (Table 6). These
of analyses, one including these two large studies and one ex- studies were not homogeneous with regard to their estimates of
cluding them. the degree of association, x2(3, N = 4) = 9.78, p < .025. The
We located 13 studies, including 19 samples. The samples common correlation (z+) was -.09 (z = 1.71), which was mar-
were heterogeneous with regard to a common underlying popu- ginally significant and in the predicted direction (p < . 10). Con-
lation correlation, x2(18, N = 19) = 56.83, p < .001. The esti- fidence intervals for the sample ranged from -.20 to .01. The
mate of the common correlation was highly significant (z+ = population estimate of z+ and its corresponding confidence in-
-.24, i = 11.85, p < .001) and in the predicted direction. The terval were the same as for sample estimates.
confidence interval for the sample estimate ranged from -.20 In reviewing these studies, it was difficult to interpret the
to -.28. The population estimate of the common correlation findings of the Disbrow study (Stotland, Mathews, Sherman,
was —.23, and its corresponding confidence interval ranged Hansson, & Richardson, 1978). In this study, significantly lower
from-.19 to-.27. levels of empathy were found among abusive families in a pre-
When we excluded the two studies (including 4 samples) with liminary analysis, but subsequent results were nonsignificant
the large sample sizes from the analysis, the resulting test for the (but were unreported in the chapter). In addition, Stotland et
homogeneity of these samples was met, x2( 14, N= 15)= 15.66, al. indicated that a composite derived from the empathy scales
ns. The estimate of the common correlation (z+) was —.13, was one of a number of variables that significantly discrimi-
which, although smaller, remained highly significant (z = 4.20, nated abusive from nonabusive families. Because unreported
p < .001), with a confidence interval ranging from -. 19 to -.07. nonsignificant results were assigned a correlation of 0, incorpo-
The population estimate of the common correlation and its con- rating the results of this large sample (N = 175) with unclear
fidence interval were the same as sample values. Thus, the con- findings may have diluted the overall estimate of the effect. Con-
sistent pattern of negative association in both analyses supports sequently, we discarded the Disbrow study and recomputed the
the notion that lower levels of empathic responsiveness are asso- analysis. As before, the assumption concerning the homogene-
ciated with externalizing negative behaviors. ity of the samples was not met, x2(2, JV = 3) = 6.87, p < .05.
We further analyzed the relation of externalizing/antisocial This time z+ was —.18, which was significant (z = 2.41, p < .02),
behaviors to empathy by breaking the empathy indexes into two and the confidence interval ranged from -.33 to -.03. Popula-
groups: those involving picture/story indexes of empathy and tion estimates for z+ and its corresponding confidence interval
those involving questionnaire indexes. (All but one study fell were -.18 and -.32 to -.03, respectively. Thus, though only
into these two groups; see Table 5.) For the three studies (five a few studies are available, there appears to be some basis for
samples) involving picture/story indexes, the common correla- concluding that lower levels of empathic responsiveness may be
tion was -.06 (z = .86, ns). The confidence interval ranged from a characteristic of parents who have histories of physically abu-
.07 to -. 19. Population values were quite similar. These studies sive behavior toward their children. Given the small number of
were homogeneous in their estimate of the common correlation, studies and the omission of the Disbrow study (Stotland et al.,
X2(4, N=5) = 4.36, ns. 1978), however, these results should be considered tentative.
For the 10 studies (and 14 samples) involving questionnaire In contrast, the relation between the experience of physical
indexes of empathy, the common correlation was —.26 (z = abuse and the capacity for empathy was stronger (Table 7). The
12.14, p < .001). The confidence interval for the sample ranged estimate of the degree of association was heterogeneous for the
from -.21 to -.29. The population estimates for z+ and its con- three relevant studies, x2(2, N = 3) = 7.34, p < .05. The estimate
fidence intervals were nearly identical. This group of studies of the common correlation (z+) was -.46 (z = 3.82), which was
was not homogeneous, %2(13, N = 14) = 44.96, p < .001. Fi- significant (p < .001). The confidence interval for z+ ranged
nally, when we omitted data from the two self-report studies from -.22 to -.69. Corresponding population estimates for z+
with large samples sizes (leaving 8 studies with 10 samples), the and its confidence interval were -.43 and -.22 to -.60. In two
common correlation was -. 15 (z = 4.28, p < .001) and the con- studies, researchers found high negative relations between the
336 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Table 5
Relation of Empathy to Externalizing Negative Behaviors

Measure of Estimate Relation to


Study Age, sex, and N Measure of empathy externalizing behavior off hypothesis

Bazar (1976/1977) 4-5 years Picture/story measure Teachers' ratings of


peer-directed
behavior
M,W=36 Affect matching" Under-control scale .10 -
Sympathetic affect .08 -
F,JV=36 Affect matching .09 -
Sympathetic affect -.08 +

Bryant(1984; per- Cohorts at 7 and 10 Bryant scale Mothers' ratings of chil-


sonal com- years, M and F dren's externaliz-
munica- ing behavior"
tion, March
21, 1986)
7-year-olds retested -.03 +
at 10 years
for exter-
nalizing
behaviors,
JV=67
10-year-olds retested -.04 -
at 14 years
for external-
izing behav-
iors, N = 73
S. B. G. Eysenck 13-14 years, British Junior 16 Questionnaire Self-report of
(1981)
M,N= 101 Empathy Scale Psychoticism0 -.37 +
Antisocial behavior -.29 +
F,;v=306 Empathy Scale Psychoticismc -.29 +
Antisocial behavior -.18 +

N. D. Feshbach G3-G4 Videotaped affect- Teachers', peers', and


(1980, 1982) matching task self-ratings of anti-
social behavior
M,7V=50 Affect matching
Euphoric affect Ratings in general MR' 0
Dysphoric affect NR" 0
Affect Matching X
Affect Intensity
Euphoric effect Peers' ratings NR< 0
Self-ratings NR" 0
_
Teachers' ratings .28
Dysphoric affect Ratings in general NRf 0
F.A^SO Affect matching' Ratings in general -,28s -
Affect Matching X Ratings in general -.28 +
Affect Intensity

Hoppe & Singer Adult M&E Criminal offense classi- -.08"'' +


(1976) fication
M psychiatric
offenders,
N= 115; Col-
lege students,
N= 101

Huckabay(1971) 6-8 years, M and F, Picture/story measure Behaviors in an experi-


JV = 60 mental situation
Cheating -.06 +
Lying -.13 +
Lack of resistance to -.16 +
temptation

Hunter (1984/ 14-18 years, incar- M&E Number of serious -.10 +


1985) cerated delin- violent offenses
quents, Af= 59
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 337

Table 5 (Continued)

Measure of exteraaliz- Estimate Relation to


Study Age, sex, and N Measure of empathy ing behavior ofr hypothesis

Hunter (1984/ 1 4- 18 years, incar- M&E Conduct-disorder -.13 +


1985) cerated M de- score'
linquents, N =
59
Psychopathic delin- -.38 +
quency scorek
Self-report of psycho- -.30 +
pathic delin-
quency1
Kurtz & Eisenberg G3, M and F, AT = 86 M&E Behaviors in an experi-
(1983) mental situation
Resistance to devia- -.23 +
tion composite (la-
tency, frequency,
and duration)

Lee (1983) Adolescent M psy- Davis IRI™, M&E Correctional .10'


chopathic de- staff ratings -.18' +
linquents, N = psychopathic-de-
12 linquent
Control group,
JV=18

Marks, Penner, & Study 1: College stu- Self-report of empathic Self-report of sociopa- -.36 +
Stone (1982) dents, M, N = anxiety to modeled thy
40 distress
Study 2: College stu- Classification of sever- -.47 +
dents, M, N = ity of sotiopathy
20
Rushton, College students, M M&E Self-report of level of -.34 +
Chrisjohn, andF,iV= 135" Machiavellianism
&Fekken
(1981)
Stotland FE Scale .09
Saklofske & 7-15 years, Cana- Junior 16 Questionnaire Self-report of psychoti-
Eysenck dian cism"
(1983)
M, N = 542 Empathy Scale -.43 +
F, N = 508 -.20 +

Watson, Grisham, College students, M&E Pathological narcis- -.20 +


Trotter, & 17-38 years, M sism0
Biderman andF,AT= 160
(1984)
Pathological narcis- -.25
sism0

Note. M = male; F = female; G = grade; Picture/story measure = self-report of feelings that match emotions depicted in picture/story stimuli;
Bryant scale = Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents (Bryant, 1982); Junior 16 Questionnaire = Junior Impulsiveness Questionnaire-
Empathy Scale (S. B. G. Eysenck, 1981; S. B. G. Eysenck, Easting, & Pearson, 1984); M & E = Mehrabian and Epstein's (1972) scale; Davis IRI =
Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1979); Stotland FE Scale = Fantasy Empathy Scale (Stotland, Mathews, Sherman, Hansson, & Richardson,
1978); NR = not reported; -I- = negative relation between measure of empathy and externalizing negative behavior; - = positive relation; 0 = no
relation.
"Only accurate, congruent affective reactions were included here, although the author also assessed congruent but inaccurate empathic
reactions. " A profile of externalizing behaviors derived from the Child Behavior Checklist (cf. Achenbach, 1978; Achenbach & Edelbrock,
1979). " A subscale of the Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (H. J. Eysenck & S. B. G. Eysenck, 1976). * The author reported results by
sex but only a total sample size. Therefore, the sample size was divided evenly by sex. ' Assumed to be 0. f The author reported negative corre-
lations between empathy and antisocial behavior for girls, regardless of type or intensity of empathic effect. Therefore, we combined euphoric and
dysphoric empathic affects in the table. * We computed the estimate of r from the report of significance. h For this comparison, the author used
the male norming sample reported by Mehrabian and Epstein (1972). 'We determined the sign of the estimate of r and the relation to the hypothesis
by comparing the mean scores of the psychiatric and control groups. ' A profile of behaviors derived from the Behavior Problem Checklist (Quay,
1966; Quay & Peterson, 1975). k The Analysis of Life History Checklist rates individuals' psychopathic delinquent behaviors on the basis of
reported background information (Quay, 1964, 1966). 'The Personal Opinion Inventory is a self-report measure of psychopathic delinquency
(Quay & Parsons, 1971). "° The relation between empathy and externalizing behavior may have been attenuated somewhat because total Davis IRI
scores were reported, rather than the empathic concern scale scores alone. " The actual sample size for this comparison was not reported. We
estimated N conservatively on the basis of the lowest sample size reported for related measures. " Two different scales of psychopathologic narcissism
were used.
338 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Table 6
Relation of Empathy to Physical Abuse: Abusing Versus Nonabusing Groups

Measure of Estimate Relation to


Study Age, sex, and N empathy Type of abuse ofr hypothesis
_
Gynn-Orenstein (1981) Adult, F M&E Court-defined physically -.20"
abusive behavior
Physically
abusive,
N= 17
Control group,
JV = 17

Howes, Feshbach, Gilly, Adult, F P&PEmpathy Clinic-defined physically -.20"* +

&Espinosa(1985) Physically Measure abusive behavior


abusive, Empathic
N=26 Distress
Control group, Scale

Letourneau(1981) Adult, F M&E Physically abusive behavior —.34" +


Physically defined by social service
abusive, agency
N=30
Control group,
JV=30

Stotland, Mathews, Adult, M Stotland FE Scale Physically abusive/neglectful iw° 0


Sherman, andF, versus control families
Hansson, & matched
Richardson (1978), couples and
or Disbrow study matched
single
parents,
JV=179

Note. F = female; M = male; M & E = Mehrabian and Epstein's (1972) scale; P & P Empathy Measure = Parent and Partner Empathy Measure-
Empathic Distress Scale (N. D. Feshbach & Caskey, 1985); Stotland FE Scale = Fantasy Empathy Scale (Stotland, Mathews, Sherman, Hansson, &
Richardson, 1978). + = negative relation between measure of empathy and abusive behavior; - = positive relation; 0 = no relation.
" We determined the signs of the estimate of r and the relation to the hypothesis by comparing the mean scores of the nonabusive and abusive
groups. b We computed the estimate of r from the report of significance, assuming p < .05, two-tailed test. ° This finding was based on analyses
using all five Stotland empathy scales. A latter analysis showed that a composite of items drawn from these scales effectively discriminated abusing
from nonabusing parents.

expression of empathy toward others and the experience of treated children responded at greater than chance levels with
abuse. In a third study, Squires (1979) obtained a slightly posi- prosocial behaviors.
tive association. The latter finding may have been due, at least in The latter result raises the possibility that the abused children
part, to two factors. First, the abused children may have scored were generally higher in aggression than nonabused peers,
higher in empathy because they were a full year older on the which might account for their lower levels of empathic respon-
average than the nonabused children. (Lennon and Eisenberg, siveness. Inspection of the foregoing studies revealed no differ-
1987, showed that scores on picture/story assessments of empa- ences in level of aggression between abused and nonabused
thy generally relate positively with age.) Second, the author's groups in two samples but did reveal differences in the range of
definition of abuse included much less serious forms of inter- negative and affective reactions assessed, not all of which could
personal aggression than in the former studies. Moreover, be considered aggressive (e.g., fear and anger as well as physical/
Howes and Eldredge (1985) obtained results consistent with nonphysical aggression in the third sample). Thus, more clearly
these overall findings in a study of five abused (mean age = 20 operationalized measures of aggression may be needed to fully
months) and nine nonmaltreated children (mean age = 20 address this possibility.
months). These children's responses to peers' distresses were
observed in free and structured play situations. In free play,
Summary and Conclusions
abused children responded to peers' distresses at greater than
chance levels with aggression, whereas nonmaltreated children The results of our review indicate that empathy is negatively
responded with prosocial behavior. In addition, in the struc- related to aggression, externalizing and antisocial behaviors,
tured situations abused children responded (at greater than and enactment and receipt of physical abuse. However, esti-
chance levels) with aggression and failed to respond (at greater mates of the common correlation (z+) were in the low-to-mod-
than chance levels) with prosocial behaviors, whereas nonmal- erate range (-.06 to -.46) for these associations. In addition,
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 339

Table?
Relation of Empathy to Physical Abuse: Abused Versus Nonabused Children

Estimate Relation to
Study Age, sex, and If Measure of empathy Type of abuse ofr hypothesis

Main & George 1 -3 years, M and F Expression of concern to Physical injuries inflicted -.62"'b +
(1985) Physically abused, distressed peer in a by parent/caregiver
Ar=9 nursery setting
Control group,
N=9
Squires (1979) 4-9 years, F Picture/story measure Physical injuries denned .05"'c
Physically abused, by regional medical
AT- 13 center
Control group,
ff-10
Straker& 5- 10 years, M and F Picture/story measure Physical injuries inflicted -.56"" -t-
Jacobson Physically abused, by parent/caregiver
(1981) N= 19
Control group,
N= 19

Note. M = male; F = female; Picture/story measure = self-report of matching affect to picture/story stimuli; + = negative relation between measure
of empathy and abuse; - = positive relation; 0 = no relation.
" We determined the signs of the estimate of r and the relation to the hypothesis by comparing group means of abused and nonabused children. b We
computed the estimate of r on the basis of the proportions of abused and nonabused children's expression of empathic concern for others'
distresses. c The positive relation found here may be due, at least in part, to the fact that age and empathy were significantly related and the fact
that the abused group children were more than a year older on the average than the normally reared children. Moreover, extent of physical abuse
was not as severe as reported in the other samples. d The authors reported N = 19 for control and abused groups but, when comparing groups on
empathy scores, reported a (ft. of 17, which resulted in an extremely high estimate of r (.70). It seemed appropriate to estimate r more conservatively
on the basis of the stated sample size of 38; thus we used a <ft of 36 in the calculations.

the significance of several of these estimates was influenced by Facial/gestural indexes of empathy have also been used with
age, mode of assessing empathy, or method of assessing negative picture/story methods and with films, both of which frequently
behaviors. involve stimuli that are clearly hypothetical. Although assessing
Specifically, we found a significant negative relation between children's facial affect responses to these stimuli may minimize
empathy and aggression when empathy was assessed with ques- the influence of demand characteristics, the presentation of
tionnaires and negative but nonsignificant relations for facial multiple emotional stimuli still requires them to change their
affect, experimental induction, and picture/story methods. Age affective state somewhat rapidly if they are to respond empathi-
differences appeared to influence the results for picture/story cally. Perhaps of most importance, because facial affect mea-
methods; the relation between these indexes and aggression was sures of empathy were developed, in part, to minimize young
significant only when we excluded preschoolers from the analy- children's reliance on verbal report of feelings, this mode of as-
ses. Overall, then, our analyses provide modest but not entirely sessing empathy has been used only with children.
consistent support for the notion that empathic responsiveness In contrast to picture/story and facial/gestural measures of
may be an inhibitor of aggression. Several factors may account empathy, questionnaire methods contain many items and
for the pattern of findings. therefore tap individuals' responses to a wide variety of situa-
First, questionnaire methods of assessing empathy differ tions. Questionnaire methods of assessment also do not require
from picture/story and facial affect methods in several poten- a direct empathic response to others in specific hypothetical sit-
tially important ways. The story stimuli involved in picture/ uations; individuals are merely asked to report their general
story procedures clearly involve hypothetical events. The affec- style of emotional responsiveness to others in personally rele-
tive content of the stimuli (i.e., sadness, happiness, fear, and vant events (e.g., "It makes me sad to see a girl who can't find
anger) changes from story to story, sometimes requiring rapid anyone to play with"). Finally, questionnaire methods have
changes in state if one is to be responsive to the stimuli. Also, been used with an older age group; respondents have been
children are asked to report their feelings to an adult experi- adults, adolescents, and school-age children. Given that the re-
menter, which may make this procedure more susceptible to lation of empathy to other modes of behavior (e.g., prosocial
demand characteristics than other empathy indexes (see Ei- behavior; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987) has been found to be less
senberg & Lennon, 1983;Hoffman, 1982). In addition, picture/ consistent for younger children than for adolescents or adults,
story methods have been used exclusively with younger chil- one might expect a similar pattern of findings for aggression. It
dren, who generally may be less consistent in self-report of em- was not possible, however, to determine on the basis of our anal-
pathic responses. Finally, researchers who have used this mea- ysis whether the inconsistent results are due to differences in
sure of empathy often have not differentiated between empa- methods of assessing empathy for the age groups or represent
thizing with positive and negative emotions. actual age differences in the relation between empathy and ag-
340 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

gression. Thus, the moderating effect of age on the relation be- pretation of others' behavior in emotionally arousing situations
tween empathy and aggression is an important issue for future rather than as a consequence of a deficit in their capacity to
research. respond empathically to others' feelings. To the extent that indi-
The results of the meta-analysis concerning the relation of viduals' cognitions influence their vicarious affective responses
empathy to both externalizing and antisocial behaviors gener- toward others, their (mis)perception of the situation may pre-
ally are consistent with those pertaining to aggression. Empathy clude empathic responding without necessarily implying any
and externalizing/antisocial negative behaviors were signifi- lack of capacity for empathy.
cantly negatively related for questionnaire but not picture/story Although gender differences have been noted either in re-
measures of empathy. This pattern of relations did not appear search on aggression or in some indexes of empathy (see Eagly
to be affected by method of classifying negative behaviors. If one & Steffen, 1986; Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983; Parke & Slaby,
assumes that externalizing behaviors often represent relatively 1983), we observed few differences in the relation of empathy
extreme or problematic forms of antisocial behavior, these re- to aggression. Overall, relations of empathy to aggression and
sults can be viewed as providing additional support for the view externalizing behaviors were of similar magnitude and in the
that empathic responding often may inhibit aggressive and anti- predicted (negative) direction for both males and females. How-
social behavior. ever, the results were sometimes slightly higher and more sig-
It is quite possible that the results of our meta-analyses may nificant for males. Because the common correlations were of
underestimate the degree of association between empathy and similar magnitude, these differences appeared to be due, at least
aggression or antisocial behavior. This is possible for several rea- in part, to the larger sample sizes for males. Generally, these
sons. First, the indexes of empathy often included empathic re- data are consistent with the idea that empathic responsiveness is
actions to both positive and negative affective states or situa- an inhibitor of aggression regardless of the sex of the individual.
tions of others (e.g., "Some songs make me happy" and "Anoth- An important issue that has not been addressed in any of the
er's laughter is not catching for me"). The clearest current relevant research is the role of sympathy versus personal dis-
theorizing concerns the regulatory role of empathy in response
tress reactions in the inhibition of aggression and antisocial be-
to others' negative affective states and corresponding levels of
havior. It is logical to assume that these two different types of
aggression, but it is much less apparent that individuals' empa-
reactions would inhibit aggression for different reasons, one
thy to positive affective reactions would be expected to influ-
egoistic and the other altruistic. Individuals who experience
ence their aggressive acts toward others. Thus, if individuals'
personal distress in reaction to another's distress should inhibit
empathic reactions to others' negative affective states were as-
their aggression as a means of reducing their own aversive inter-
sessed separately from reactions to others' positive affective re-
nal state. In contrast, people who experience sympathy (i.e.,
actions, the relation of empathy to aggression might be ex-
empathic concern) should inhibit their negative behaviors be-
pected to be stronger.
cause of concern for the other's feelings or physical state. More-
We also found considerable variation in the criterion mea-
over, experiencing these two different reactions could be associ-
sures of aggression (e.g., rating scales, shock and aversive noise
ated with different attributions about the victim. For example,
administrations, game disruption, and role-playing responses)
people who feel personal distress may be more likely than those
and externalizing behavior (e.g., conduct disorders, lying/steal-
who experience a preponderance of sympathy to derogate or
ing, and ratings of antisocial behavior). It is questionable
blame the victim in an attempt to distance themselves from the
whether some of these measures of aggression could reasonably
other and reduce their own distress (see Hoffinan, 1984). These
be expected to be influenced by individuals' empathic respon-
attributions concerning the controllability of the other's status
siveness. For example, game disruption was used as an index of
as a victim might then be expected to affect the potential aggres-
aggression in some research, but how it might be mediated by
sor's future prosocial and antisocial responding (Eisenberg,
empathic responsiveness is not clear. Game playing may be in-
1986;Weiner, 1986).
fluenced more by existing normative rules and modeled com-
petitive behavior than by concern for others' feelings or vicari- Another issue that has been inadequately examined is the de-
ous responsiveness to those feelings. In fact, in situations in gree to which the purely cognitive aspects of empathizing (e.g.,
which empathic reactions should conceptually be more likely perspective taking) and the affective component of empathy
to influence aggressive behavior (e.g., during the administration differ in their relation to aggressive and antisocial behavior.
of aversive noise or shock accompanied by the victim's pain Some data are consistent with the argument that the relation
cues), the relation of empathy to aggression was somewhat more between empathy and aggression cannot be solely accounted
consistent with expectations. for by the relation of role-taking abilities to aggression (e.g.,
It is also likely that the inhibition of aggressive and antisocial N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, 1969, 1982; lannotti & Pierre-
reactions often is mediated by factors other than empathic re- humbert, 1985). However, role-taking abilities have been nega-
sponding. If this is so, the correlation between empathy and ag- tively related with aggressive behavior in some research (e.g.,
gression will be lowered by the inclusion of studies in which Letourneau, 1981; see S. Feshbach & N. D. Feshbach, 1986).
factors other than empathy influenced the inhibition of aggres- Unfortunately, to our knowledge, no one has explicitly tested
sion. For example, Dodge (1980) found that aggressive boys whether perspective-taking skills mediate (Baron & Kenny,
were more likely than nonaggressive boys to interpret the other 1986) the effects of empathy on aggressive and antisocial re-
person's behavior as aggressive or hostile, especially when the sponding. An empirical test of this issue could be computed if
other's intent was ambiguous. Thus, individuals inclined to re- researchers would obtain indexes of both empathy constructs
spond aggressively may do so as a result of inappropriate inter- (i.e., affective and cognitive) as well as aggression, preferably in
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 341

a situation in which the individual empathized with and then affect in interacting with their children on a task and that these
had an opportunity to aggress against the target person. behaviors were significantly and positively related to parental
In some theoretical conceptions of the relation between em- empathy. Thus, abusive parents' interactions with their chil-
pathy and aggression, situational experiencing of empathy, not dren may be characterized by a lack of empathy—a lack that
dispositional empathy, has been stressed. Cues of the other's has long-term negative consequences for children's socioemo-
distress are viewed as eliciting distress in the observer, which tional development.
inhibits further aggression (e.g., N. D. Feshbach & S. Feshbach, In conclusion, empathic and sympathetic responding appear
1982). In our review, however, the relation between empathy to be linked to a number of negative behaviors that affect indi-
and aggressive behavior was not strong in those studies in which viduals' functioning in a variety of interpersonal realms. The
both empathy toward a given person and aggression toward the processes underlying these relations need further explication,
same individual were assessed (i.e., the experimental induction however. There is a need to attend to differentiations in type of
studies). Rather, the relations were strongest between question- vicarious responding (e.g., sympathetic versus personal dis-
naire indexes of dispositional empathy and aggressive or exter- tress) and to possible differences in the nature of empathy in
nalizing behavior. This general pattern of findings suggests that relation to positive versus negative emotions. In addition, in-
the tendency to experience another's distress may moderate dexes of empathy clearly do not always assess similar con-
(Baron & Kenny, 1986) the relation between affective responsiv- structs. When distinctions of these sorts (as well as among types
ity in a given situation and behavior in that situation. The of aggression) are considered, models of the relation of empathy
differential roles that situational and dispositional empathy to antisocial behavior should be conceptually clearer, and the
may play in inhibiting negative behaviors are intriguing but are results of empirical work on these models should be more con-
virtually unexplored. sistent than they are currently. Nonetheless, we obtained initial
We obtained two important findings concerning the relation relatively clear evidence of a modest negative relation between
of empathy to the receipt or enactment of physical abuse. Abu- empathy and aggressive and antisocial behavior. Thus, it seems
sive parents scored lower on indexes of empathic responsiveness appropriate for practitioners and researchers interested in the
than did mothers from the "normal" population. Moreover, inhibition of individuals' aggressive and antisocial behavior to-
children who were victims of abuse exhibited less empathy than ward others to pay greater attention to the construct of empathy
did nonabused children. Overall, these results are quite consis- in their work.
tent both with researchers' predictions (e.g., N. D. Feshbach,
1987; Squires, 1979) and with previous research on the deleteri-
References
ous effects of abusive rearing practices on children's social in-
teractions and emotional attachments with others (e.g., George Achenbach, T. M. (1978). The Child Behavior Profile: I. Boys aged 6-
& Main, 1979). I I . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 478-488.
Specifically, abusing parents, in comparison with other par- Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbroek, C. S. (1979). The Child Behavior Pro-
file: II. Boys aged 12-16 and girls aged 6-11 and 12-16. Journal of
ents, have been found to be relatively more negative in their
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47,223-233.
relationships with their children, to use more coercive and pu-
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbroek, C. S. (1984). Psychopathology of child-
nitive socialization practices, and to express relatively higher
hood. Annual Review of Psychology, 35,227-256.
levels of negative affect after children's social transgressions Aleksic, P. M. (1976). A study of empathic inhibition of aggression in
(e.g., Burgess & Conger, 1978; Reid, 1986; Trickett & Kuczyn- juvenile delinquents. (Doctoral dissertation, Miami University, Mi-
ski, 1983). In turn, children with histories of parental abuse ami, OH, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 35, 4675B.
exhibit long-term problems in social adjustment, aggressive and American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
antisocial behavior, and difficulties in maintaining relationships manual of menial disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
with others. (See reviews by Lamphear, 1985; Wolfe, 1985.) It Attili, G. (1985). Concomitants and factors influencing children's ag-
gression. Aggressive Behavior. 11,291-301.
is likely that abusive parents' relative inability to respond in a
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood
supportive and empathic manner to their children is related to
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
their children's social and emotional difficulties. That is, abu-
Bamett, M. A. (1987). Empathy and related responses in children. In
sive parents may be less likely than other parents to (a) identify
N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp.
or understand their children's feelings, especially in distress or 146-162). New York: Cambridge Univereity Press.
other situations involving their children's needs; (b) adopt their Baron, R. A. (1971). Magnitude of victim's pain cues and level of prior
children's emotional/cognitive perspective; (c) respond vicari- anger arousal as determinants of adult aggressive behavior. Journal of
ously to their children's affective states (N. D. Feshbach, 1987; Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 236-243.
Howes & Feshbach, 1986); and (d) encourage their children to Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari-
react positively (i.e., emotionally and behaviorally) toward oth- able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strate-
gic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social
ers. Thus, it seems reasonable to find deficiencies in empathic
Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
responsiveness associated with less adequate parenting.
Batson, C. D. (in press). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic?
Consistent with this reasoning, N. D. Feshbach (1987), in a
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology.
study of a nonclinical sample, found that maternal and paternal New York: Academic Press.
empathy were negatively related to children's externalizing and Batson, C. D., & Coke, J. S. (1981). Empathy: A source of altruistic
internalizing behavior and to inconsistencies in parental disci- motivation for helping? In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
pline practices. Moreover, Howes and Feshbach (1986) found Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmen-
that abusive parents showed less investment, involvement, and tal perspectives (pp. 167-211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
342 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Bazar, J. W. (1977). An exploration of the relationship of affect aware- Television and social behavior (Tech. rep., Vol. i, pp. 22-43). Wash-
ness, empathy, and interpersonal strategies to nursery school chil- ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
dren's competence in peer interactions (Doctoral dissertation, Uni- Eliasz, H. (1980). The effect of empathy, reactivity, and anxiety on inter-
versity of California, Berkeley, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts Interna- personal aggression intensity. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 11, 169-
tional, 37, 5691A. 178.
Berkowitz, L. (1962). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. New Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1976). Psychoticism as a dimension
York: McGraw-Hill. of personality. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Berlin, R. J. (1978). Teaching acting-out adolescents prosocial conflict Eysenck, S. B. G. (1981). Impulsive and antisocial behavior in children.
resolution through structured learning training of empathy. Disserta- Current Psychological Research, I, 31-37.
tion Abstracts International, 39, 970B. (University Microfilms No. Eysenck, S. B. G., Easting, G., & Pearson, P. R. (1984). Age norms for
7811634) impulsiveness, venturesomeness, and empathy in children. Personal-
Bigler-Williams, C. A. (1984). The effectiveness of prosocial treatment ity and Individual Differences, 5, 315-321.
with federal engenders (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana State Univer- Ferguson, G. A. (1976). Statistical analyses in psychology and education
sity, Terre Haute, 1984). Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
3089B.
Feshbach, N. D. (1978). Studies of empathic behavior in children. In
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol.
review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1-45. 8, pp. 1-47). New York: Academic Press.
Blum, L. A. (1980). Friendship, altruism and morality. London: Feshbach, N. D. (1980, May). The psychology of empathy and the empa-
Routledge & Kegan Paul. thy of psychology. Presidential address presented at the annual meet-
Bryant, B. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents. ing of the Western Psychological Association, Honolulu, HI.
Child Development, 53,413-425.
Feshbach, N. D. (1982). Sex differences in empathy and social behavior
Bryant, B. (1984, August). Self-criticism of the Bryant (1982) empathy
in children. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial be-
index. In R. Lennon, The construct and assessment of empathy. Sym-
havior (pp. 315-338). New York: Academic Press.
posium conducted at the annual meeting of the American Psychologi-
Feshbach, N. D. (1987). Parental empathy and child adjustment/mal-
cal Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
adjustment. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its de-
Burgess, R. L., & Conger, R. D. (1978). Family interaction in abusive, velopment (pp. 271-291). New York: Cambridge University Press.
neglectful and normal families. Child Development, 49, 1163-1173.
Feshbach, N. D., &Caskey, N. (1985).^ new scalefor measuring parent
Campbell, A., Muncer, S., & Bibel, D. (1985). Taxonomies of aggressive
empathy and partner empathy: Factorial structure, correlates and
behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 11, 217-222.
clinical discrimination. Unpublished manuscript, University of Cali-
Chandler, M. J. (1973). Egocentrism and antisocial behavior The as-
fornia, Los Angeles.
sessment and training of social perspective-taking skills. Developmen-
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1969). The relationship between em-
tal Psychology, 9,326-332.
pathy and aggression in two age groups. Developmental Psychology,
Cooley, C. H. (1956). Two major works: Social organization and human
1,102-107.
nature and the social order. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1982). Empathy training and the regu-
published 1902)
lation of aggression: Potentialities and limitations. Academic Psychol-
Cooper, J. M. (1979). Statistically combining independent studies: A
ogy Bulletin, 4, 399-413.
meta-analysis of sex differences in conformity research. Journal of
Feshbach, N. D., & Roe, K. (1968). Empathy in six- and seven-year-
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 131-146.
olds. Child Development, 39,133-145.
Davis, M. H. (1979). Individual differences in empathy: A multidimen-
Feshbach, S. (1970). Aggression. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's
sional approach (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin,
manual of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 159-259). New York: Wiley.
1979). Dissertation Abstracts International, 40,3480B.
Feshbach, S., & Feshbach, N. D. (1986). Aggression and altruism: A
Davis, M. H., Hull, J. C., Young, R. D., & Warren, G. G. (1987). Emo-
personality perspective. In C. Zahn-Waxier, E. M. Cummings, & R.
tional reactions to dramatic film stimuli: The influence of cognitive
lannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social ori-
and emotional empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 52, 126-133. gins (pp. 189-217). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Dodge, K. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior.
Child Development, 51, 162-170. Feshbach, S., Stiles, W. B., & Bitter, E. (1967). The reinforcing effect of
Dollard, J., Dobb, L., Miller, N., Mowrer, O., & Sears, R. (1939). Frus- witnessing aggression. Journal of Experimental Research in Personal-
tration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ity, 2, 133-139.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A Fontana, V. J., SRobison, E. (1984). Observing child abuse. Behavioral
meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psycholog- Pediatrics, 105, 655-660.
ical Bulletin, 100, 309-330. Gaines, T., Kirwin, P. M., & Gentry, W. D. (1977). The effect of descrip-
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic cognition, emotion, and behavior. Hills- tive anger expression, insult, and no feedback on interpersonal aggres-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. sion, hostility, and empathy motivation. Genetic Psychology Mono-
Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and graphs, 95, 349-367.
related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100-131. George, C, & Main, M. (1979). Social interactions of young abused
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to proso- children: Approach, avoidance and aggression. Child Development,
cial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101,91-119. 50, 306-318.
Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (Eds.). (1987). Empathy and its develop- Gibbs, J. C. (1987). Social processes in the causation and treatment of
ment. New \brk: Cambridge University Press. delinquency: The need to facilitate empathy as well as sociomoral
Ekman, P., Liebert, R. M., Friesen, W. V., Harrison, R., Zlatchin, C, reasoning. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Social interac-
Malmstrom, E. ]., & Baron, R. A. (1972). Facial expressions of emo- tion and sociomoral development (pp. 301 -321). New York: Wiley.
tion while watching televised violence as predictors of subsequent ag- Goldstein, A. P., & Michaels, G. Y. (1985). Empathy: Development,
gression. In G. A. Comstock, E. A. Rubenstein, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), training, and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
RELATION OF EMPATHY TO AGGRESSION 343

Gough, H. G. (1948). A sociological theory of psychopathy. American the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
Journal of Sociology, 53, 359-366. ment, Denver, CO.
Gouze, K., Rayais, M., & Bieber-Schneider, R. (1983, August). Cogni- lannotti, R. J. (1978). Effect of role-taking experiences on role taking,
tive correlates of aggression in second-grade children. Paper presented empathy, altruism, and aggression. Developmental Psychology, 14,
at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, 119-124.
Anaheim, CA. lannotti, R. J., & Pierrehumbert, B. (1985, April). The development of
Gynn-Orenstein, J. (1981). The relationship between moral reasoning, empathy in early childhood. Paper presented at the biennial meeting
locus of control, emotional empathy, and parenting profile in physi- of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto, Ontario,
cally abusing mothers (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Canada.
Professional Psychology, Los Angeles, 1981). Dissertation Abstracts Kameya, L. I. (1976). The effect of empathy level and role-taking train-
International, 42, 2056B. ing upon prosocial level. Dissertation Abstracts International, 37,
Hare, R. D. (1970). Psychopathy: Theory and research. New York: 3151B. (University Microfilms No. 76-29,321)
Wiley. Kant, I. (1949). Critique of practical reasoning (L. W. Beck, Trans.).
Hartmann, D. P. (1969). Influence of symbolically modeled instrumen- Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1788)
tal aggression and pain cues on aggressive behavior. Journal of Person- Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral
ality and Social Psychology, 11,280-288. stages and the idea of justice. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Hedges, L. V., &Olkiri, I. (1986). Statistical methods for meta-analyses. Krebs, D. (1975). Empathy and altruism. Journal of Personality and
New York: Academic Press. Social Psychology, 32,1134-1146.
Hoffman, M. L. (1982). The measurement of empathy. In C. E. Izard Kurtz, C. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1983). Role-taking, empathy, and resis-
(Ed.), Measuring emotions in infants and children (pp. 279-296). tance to deviation in children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology,
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 142, 85-95.
Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy. Lamphear, V. S. (1985). The impact of maltreatment on children's psy-
In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, chosocial adjustment: A review of the research. Child Abuse and Ne-
and behavior (pp. 103-131). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- glect, 9, 251-263.
versity Press. Lee, M. (1983). The relationship of empathy, role taking and moral
Hoffman, M. L. (1987). The contribution of empathy to justice and reasoning to dimensions of juvenile delinquency (Doctoral disserta-
moral judgment. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and tion, University of Texas, Austin, 1983). Dissertation Abstracts Inter-
its development (pp. 47-80). New York: Cambridge University Press. national, 37, 1032B.
Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Con- Lennon, R., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Gender and age differences in em-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 307-316. pathy and sympathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy
Hoppe, C. M., & Singer, R. D. (1976). Overcontrolled hostility, empathy and its development (pp. 195-217). New York: Cambridge University
and egocentric balance in violent and nonviolent psychiatric offend- Press.
ers. Psychological Reports, 39,1303-1308. Letourneau, C. (1981, September). Empathy and stress: How they affect
Howard, J. A. (1983). Preschoolers' empathy for specific affects and parental aggression. Social Work, pp. 383-389.
their social interaction (Doctoral dissertation, Kansas State Univer- Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Responses of abused and disadvantaged
sity, Manhattan). Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 3954B. toddlers to distress in agemates: A study in the day care setting. Devel-
Howes, C, & Eldredge, R. (1985). Responses of abused, neglected and opmental Psychology, 21,407-412.
nonmaltreated children to the behaviors of their peers. Journal of Ap- Marcus, R. F. (1987). Somatic indices of empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J.
plied Developmental Psychology, 6, 261-270. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 374-379). New
Howes, C., & Feshbach, N. D. (1986). Compliance and self control in York: Cambridge University Press.
young children from varying family contexts: Relationships with par- Marcus, R. F., Roke, E. J., & Bruner, C. (1985). Verbal and nonverbal
ent empathy, stress and social support. Unpublished manuscript, Uni- empathy and prediction of social behavior of young children. Percep-
versity of California, Graduate School of Education, Los Angeles. tual and Motor Skills, 60, 299-309.
Howes, C., Feshbach, N. D., Gilly, J., & Espinosa, M. (1985, August). Marks, E. L., Penner, L. A., & Stone, A. V. W. (1982). Helping as a
Compliance and self control in young children from varying family function of empathic responses and sociopathy. Journal of Research
contexts: Relationships with parent empathy, stress and social sup- in Personality, 16, 1-20.
port. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psycho- McDougall, W. (1950). An introduction to social psychology (30th ed.).
logical Association, Los Angeles, CA. London: Methuen. (Original work published 1908)
Huckabay, L. M. D. (1971). A developmental study of the relationship Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chi-
of negative moral-social behaviors to empathy, to positive social be- cago Press.
haviors and to cognitive moral judgment (Doctoral dissertation, Uni- Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. A. (1972). A measure of emotional empa-
versity of California, Los Angeles). Dissertation Abstracts Interna- thy. Journal of Personality, 40, 523-543.
tional, 32, 1337A. Nielsen, K. A. (1976). Aggression, empathy, and self esteem in latency
Hume, D. (1966). Enquiries concerning the human understanding and aged and adolescent males living in a residential treatment center for
concerning the principles of morals (2nd ed.). Oxford, England: Cla- emotionally disturbed children. Dissertation Abstracts International,
renden Press. (Original work published 1777) 37, 10A, 6374.
Hunter, E. E. (1985). An examination of the relationships among di- Parke, R. D., & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. In
mensions of psychopathology, prosocial behavior, prosocial moral E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Manual of child psychology. Vol. 4. Social-
reasoning, and empathy within a population of juvenile delinquents ization, personality, and social development (pp. 549-641). New
(Doctoral dissertation, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, 1984). Dis- York: Wiley.
sertation Abstracts International, 45, 3943B. Perry, D. G., & Perry, L. C. (1974). Denial of suffering in the victim as
lannotti, R. J. (1975, April). The effect of role-taking experiences on a stimulus to violence in aggressive boys. Child Development, 45,55-
role-taking, altruism, empathy, and aggression. Paper presented at 62.
344 PAUL A. MILLER AND NANCY EISENBERG

Perry, D. G., Perry, L. C., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social Staub, E. (1986). A conception of the determinants and development of
learning mediators of aggression. Child Development, 57, 700-711. altruism and aggression: Motives, the self, and the environment. In
Polk, W. M. (1976). Perceptual orientation, empathy, and the inhibition C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R. lannotti (Eds.), Altruism
of aggression (Doctoral dissertation, North Carolina State University, and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 135-164). Cam-
Raleigh, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 37,4225B. bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Quay, H. C. (1964). Personality dimensions in delinquent males as in- Staub, E., & Feinberg, H. K. (1980, September). Regularities in peer
ferred from the factor analysis of behavior ratings. Journal of Re- interaction, empathy, and sensitivity to others. Paper presented at the
search in Crime and Delinquency, 1, 33-37. annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Mon-
Quay, H. C. (1966). Personality patterns in preadolescent delinquent treal, Quebec, Canada.
boys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 26,99-110. Steinman, D. R. (1979). Moderators of the effects of television violence
Quay, H. C, & Parsons, L. B. (1971). The differential behavioral classi- on boys' aggressive behavior: Explicitness of consequences and emo-
fication of the juvenile offender (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Bu- tional reactions (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin,
reau of Prisons. 1979). Dissertation Abstracts International, 40, 1343B.
Quay, H. C, & Peterson, D. R. (1975). Manual for the Behavior Problem
Stock, W. A., Okun, M. A., Haring, M. J., Kinney, C., & Miller, W.
Checklist (Rev, ed.). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami. (1979). Codebookfor deriving and coding data points in meta-analy-
Reid, J. B. (1986). Social-interactional patterns in families of abused
sis: The sample case of subjective well-being. Unpublished manu-
and nonabused children. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R.
script, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ.
lannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social ori-
Stotland, E. (1969). Exploratory studies in empathy. In L. Berkowitz
gins (pp. 238-255). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 271-
Rein, B. A. (1974). The effects of empathy, similarity, and attraction on
314). New York: Academic Press.
level of aggression (Doctoral dissertation, University of California,
Stotland, E., Mathews, K. E., Sherman, S. E., Hansson, R. O., & Rich-
Riverside, 1974). Dissertation Abstracts International, 35,1395B.
ardson, B. E. (1978). Empathy, fantasy, and helping. Beverly Hills,
Rest, J. R. (1983). Morality. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Manual of child psy-
CA: Sage.
chology (Vol. 3, pp. 556-629). New York: Wiley.
Straker, G., & Jacobson, R. S. (1981). Aggression, emotional maladjust-
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The "file drawer problem" and tolerance for null
results. Psychological Bulletin, 86,638-641. ment, and empathy in the abused child. Developmental Psychology,
Rushton, J. P., Chrisjohn, R. D., &Fekken, G. C. (1981). The altruistic 17, 762-765.
personality and the self-report altruism scale. Personality and Indi- Trickett, P., & Kuczynski, L. (1983, August). Children's misbehaviors
vidual Differences, Z 1-11. and parental discipline in abusive and nonabusivefamilies. Paper pre-
Saklofske, D. H., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1983). Impulsiveness and ven- sented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Associa-
turesomeness in Canadian children. Psychological Reports, 52,147- tion, Anaheim, CA.
152. Underwood, B., & Moore, B. (1982). Perspective-taking and altruism.
Smith, A. (1948). The theory of moral sentiment. In H. Schneider (Ed.), Psychological Bulletin, 91,143-173.
Adam Smith's moral and political philosophy (pp. 7-280). New York: Watson, P. J., Grisham, S. Q, Trotter, M. V., & Biderman, M. D. (1984).
Hafner. (Original work published 1759) Narcissism and empathy: Validity evidence for the Narcissistic Per-
Solomon, J. (1985, April). The relationship between affective empathy sonality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 301-305.
and prosocial behavior in elementary school children. Paper pre- Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion.
sented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child New York: Springer-Verlag.
Development, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child-abusive parents: An empirical review and
Spivak.G., &Shure, M. B. (1974). Social adjustment in young children: analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 97,462-482.
A cognitive approach to solving real problems. San Francisco: Jossey- Zahn-Waxler, C, Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child rear-
Bass. ing and children's prosocial initiations toward victims of distress,
Spivak, G., Swift, M., & Prewitt, M. A. (1971). Syndromes of disturbed Child Development, 50, 319-330.
classroom behavior: A behavioral diagnostic system for elementary
schools. Journal of Special Education, 5,269-292.
Squires, M. F. (1979). Empathic, nurturant and abusive behavior of nor-
mally and abnormally reared girls (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana Received October 7,1986
University, Bloomington, 1979). Dissertation Abstracts Interna- Revision received September 22, 1987
tional, 40, 937B. Accepted November 9, 1987

View publication stats

You might also like