You are on page 1of 15

The Journal of Positive Psychology

Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice

ISSN: 1743-9760 (Print) 1743-9779 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20

Transforming or restraining rumination: The


impact of compassionate reappraisal versus
emotion suppression on empathy, forgiveness,
and affective psychophysiology

Charlotte vanOyen Witvliet, Alicia J. Hofelich Mohr, Nova G. Hinman & Ross
W. Knoll

To cite this article: Charlotte vanOyen Witvliet, Alicia J. Hofelich Mohr, Nova G. Hinman & Ross
W. Knoll (2015) Transforming or restraining rumination: The impact of compassionate reappraisal
versus emotion suppression on empathy, forgiveness, and affective psychophysiology, The Journal
of Positive Psychology, 10:3, 248-261, DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2014.941381

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.941381

Published online: 29 Jul 2014. Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 640 View related articles

View Crossmark data Citing articles: 7 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpos20
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2015
Vol. 10, No. 3, 248–261, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.941381

Transforming or restraining rumination: The impact of compassionate reappraisal versus


emotion suppression on empathy, forgiveness, and affective psychophysiology
Charlotte vanOyen Witvliet*, Alicia J. Hofelich Mohr, Nova G. Hinman and Ross W. Knoll
Psychology Department, Hope College, Holland, MI 49422-9000, USA
(Received 13 June 2013; accepted 23 June 2014)

We tested the effects of practicing compassionate reappraisal vs. emotional suppression as direct coping responses to
victims’ ruminations about a past interpersonal offense. Participants (32 females, 32 males) were randomly assigned to
learn one coping strategy which immediately followed three of six offense rumination trials (counterbalanced). For both
strategy types, coping (vs. offense ruminating) reduced ratings of negative emotion, decreased the use of negative
emotion language, and reduced tension at the brow muscle (corrugator EMG). Only compassionate reappraisal coping
(vs. offense rumination) immediately prompted greater empathy and emotional forgiveness toward the offender. Empathy
ratings for the first coping trial mediated the relationship between strategy type and empathy ratings for the final
rumination trial. Compassionate reappraisal strategy participants increased their empathy toward the offender while
ruminating at the end of the study. Compassionate reappraisal participants (vs. emotional suppression) described coping
(vs. rumination) with more positive language, and also had calmer cardiac pre-ejection period responses.
Keywords: emotion regulation; empathy; forgiveness; reappraisal; suppression; Stroop; PEP

Theoretical and empirical work in positive psychology (Witvliet et al., 2011). This work also responds to theo-
has recently addressed how people respond to real-world rizing that repeated practice may strengthen the effects
interpersonal hurts, such as a betrayal or dishonesty, by of strategies to regulate responses to interpersonal offen
exploring connections to the emotion regulation literature ses (see Sandage & Worthington, 2010; Worthington &
on suppression, reappraisal, and forgiveness (Witvliet, Sotoohi, 2009).
DeYoung, Hofelich, & DeYoung, 2011; Witvliet & The current experiment tests the effects of practicing
McCullough, 2007). Our goal in the current experiment a single coping strategy in response to offense rumina-
is to test effects of learning and practicing compassionate tion, whereas prior research on both compassion and
reappraisal for the offender vs. suppressing one’s experi- suppression in the forgiveness literature tested both
ence and expression of negative emotions. Compassion- strategies within individuals (Witvliet et al., 2011). The
ate reappraisal considers the individual responsible for current study is designed to mimic what people do out-
an interpersonal hurt as a human being who behaved side the lab, when only a single strategy may be known
unjustly, and genuinely wishes for his or her positive or available to them, or when an intervention is used in
transformation in response. Conversely, emotional sup- cognitive behavioral therapy. The between subjects
pression entails the effort to not experience or express methodology is important for testing compassionate
negative emotions while remembering the offender and reappraisal (unaffected by suppression) and emotional
offense. For example, an individual who experienced suppression (unchanged by compassion). This design
relationship betrayal might compassionately reappraise allows us to see ways in which the strategies are similar
by thinking about that individual as a human whose act in contrast to rumination about the offender and the
of betrayal shows that person’s need to undergo a change offense. It also allows us to evaluate differences between
that will equip him or her to show loyalty. Or, an indi- strategies that occur immediately and which emerge over
vidual coping through emotional suppression may strive time.
to stifle the experience and expression of feelings of
anger, sadness, or other negative emotions that arise
when thinking about the person and the betrayal. The Offense rumination, compassionate reappraisal, and
current study responds to recent work calling for the emotional suppression
evaluation of compassionate reappraisal and emotion Rumination about a past interpersonal offense has been
suppression as coping responses that immediately follow found to increase negative self-reports and facial
periods of ruminating about one’s offense and offender expressions, heighten skin conductance levels and blood

*Corresponding author. Email: witvliet@hope.edu

© 2014 Taylor & Francis


The Journal of Positive Psychology 249

pressure, and accelerate heart rate (Witvliet, Ludwig, & Empathy and forgiveness
VanderLaan, 2001). Additionally, McCullough, Orsulak, In the forgiveness literature, empathy holds a central
Brandon, and Akers (2007) found that salivary cortisol place. Empathy is a common thread from early empirical
levels were higher when participants self-reported having intervention studies (e.g. Hebl & Enright, 1993) to more
ruminated more than usual about a real-life offense. recent intervention research (e.g. Sandage & Worthington,
Rumination about an anger-provoking laboratory event 2010). Empathy for the offender plays an essential role in
was also found to generate higher sympathetic nervous promoting victim forgiveness as a moral response (see
system activity and greater cognitive perseveration than Enright, 2001; Enright & Fitzgibbons, 2000; Hargrave,
reappraisal (Ray, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2008). 2001), particularly promoting emotional forgiveness
Compassionate reappraisal has begun to be examined beyond the decision to forgive (Worthington, 2009). In a
experimentally. The first study compared compassionate landmark meta-analysis, state empathy emerged as a
reappraisal to another positive reappraisal strategy strong predictor of forgiveness toward an offender (Fehr,
focused on benefits (e.g. lessons learned, insight gained, Gelfand, & Nag, 2010).
or strengths shown), testing both in contrast to ruminat- Building empathy, or compassion, for a transgressor is
ing about an interpersonal offense (Witvliet et al., 2010). pivotal for cultivating forgiveness, but it also poses a criti-
While both positive reappraisals increased positive emo- cal obstacle. Forgiveness is a moral response that needs to
tion and decreased negative emotion in comparison to address both justice and compassion. In forgiving, people
offense rumination, compassionate reappraisal prompted need to tell the truth about the offender and the injustice
the greatest use of forgiveness and social language in while transforming unforgiving and retributive responses
typed narratives, while calming heart rate. so they are constructive, prosocial, and can genuinely pro-
The second experimental study directly compared mote flourishing (Witvliet, 2012). Concerns for victim
compassionate reappraisal with emotional suppression safety (Gordon, Burton, & Porter, 2004; McNulty, 2010),
and offense rumination (Witvliet et al., 2011), during self-differentiation (Sandage & Jankowski, 2010),
which participants engaged in each response for two- self-concept clarity, and self-respect (Luchies, Finkel,
minute trials. Only compassionate reappraisal increased McNulty, & Kumashiro, 2010) are important for setting
positive emotions, forgiveness, social language, and the boundary conditions of a just forgiveness that is mor-
facial EMG associated with smiling when compared to ally grounded, promotes responsible healing, and does not
rumination about the offense. Compassionate reappraisal excuse, minimize, or tolerate injustice (Worthington,
also quelled negative emotion ratings and negative word 2009). Empathy and compassion are responses that are
use in typed narratives, similar to the effects of emo- typically directed toward others who are victims deserving
tional suppression. Indeed, for those engaging in emo- of help when one has the resources to assist, according to
tional suppression, focusing on the offender and offense the appraisal model of Goetz, Keltner, and Simon-Thomas
while not experiencing or expressing negative emotion (2010). By contrast, when the victim is oneself, responses
effectively reduced negative emotion expressions in are more likely to include sadness, anger, and shame
ratings, writing, and corrugator (brow) EMG, as well as (Goetz et al., 2010). Empirically, rumination about being
heart rate (Witvliet et al., 2011). Consistent with Dunn, the victim of an interpersonal offense activated sadness
Billotti, Murphy, and Dalgleish’s (2009) adaptive and anger that was significantly decreased by empathic
suppression hypothesis, emotional suppression did what perspective-taking (Witvliet et al., 2001) and compassion
it was supposed to do; it simply failed to induce forgive- for the human being (the transgressor) whose offense gave
ness and positive emotion. evidence of his or her need to experience positive trans-
To summarize, no existing research has tested sympa- formation (Witvliet et al., 2011).
thetic nervous system and cognitive effects of rumination Because compassion is more understandably directed
vs. compassionate reappraisal and suppression in an toward others who are victims (Goetz et al., 2010), com-
offense context. Further, no studies have tested how the passion for an offender likely takes practice (Sandage &
effects of ruminations change over time based on the Worthington, 2010). By contrast, people may more often
type of coping strategy used. Finally, research has not implement emotional suppression in everyday coping
empirically examined the use of a single one of these (e.g. stifling one’s sadness or anger about an offense in
coping strategies in response to a transgression, which order to comport oneself professionally in a meeting or
may often be the approach used in a real-world or inter- while teaching). If so, in prior research, suppression may
vention environment. Given the emerging literature on have accrued its benefits in a single-trial measure of it
emotion regulation in relation to interpersonal transgres- (even though compassionate reappraisal still had more
sions and forgiveness, our aim was to implement an forgiveness, positive, and prosocial benefits; Witvliet
experimental design that would have implications for an et al., 2011). The goal of the current study was to test
intervention environment. the effects of multiple trials of emotion suppression vs.
250 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

compassionate reappraisal in direct response to rumina- the Stroop task to measure cognitive demand after
tion, by measuring participants’ rated, written, embodied, repeated practice of suppression or reappraisal in com-
and cognitive responses. parison to rumination about an interpersonal offense.
In addition to measuring Stroop interference, we
assessed accuracy rates for the different conditions, as
Do compassionate reappraisal and emotional well as RTs for accurate trials. This allowed us to assess
suppression have cognitive costs? subtle differences between conditions (such as general
While both suppression and reappraisal have been shown speed and accuracy effects or tradeoffs) that may exist
to decrease negative emotion associated with an interper- even if no difference in the Stroop effect occurred.
sonal offense (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011), each coping
strategy may come with cognitive costs. Suppression
often is considered a response-focused coping strategy Summary, hypotheses, and analysis plan
(Gross, 2007) because it involves trying to dampen down In summary, the aims of this study were to test the
the experience and expression of emotion once it is effects of learning and practicing either compassionate
noticed. Reappraisal often is conceptualized as an ante- reappraisal or emotional suppression multiple times as
cedent-focused strategy because it involves changing the direct responses to offense rumination. Taking seriously
way one thinks about a situation before an emotional the importance of replication, we used ratings, linguistic
reaction surfaces. Research has shown that suppression, narratives, facial EMG, and heart rate to build on pro-
but not reappraisal, impairs memory (Gross, 2007) and grammatic research (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011). To
leads to depletion of regulatory control compared with extend what is known, we developed a paradigm to test
free expression of emotion (Inzlicht & Gutsell, 2007). the practice of implementing a single coping strategy
However, reappraisal does not always occur as an directly after offense rumination, while adding new car-
antecedent to the experience of negative emotions. In diac and cognitive measures. Participants completed a
real-life experiences, people often employ regulatory total of six trials, with three rumination-only trials inter-
strategies like suppression or reappraisal after noticing spersed with three rumination-then-coping trials (orders
they have had an emotional response they want to counterbalanced). Because cardiac pre-ejection periods
change. Similarly, in the current paradigm, participants (PEP) are measurable within 45 s trials, PEP was
first ruminate about an interpersonal offense before assessed as an indicator of sympathetic nervous system
engaging in a coping strategy. Studies that have mea- activity. This approach provides a new measure that
sured cognitive costs of reappraising in response to an extends what was assessed in previous research using
emotion find that this strategy requires cognitive control 120 s trials, which were long enough to conduct spectral
and depletes self-control resources more than suppression analysis of heart rate variability (HRV) to assess para-
(Sheppes, Catran, & Meiran, 2009; Sheppes & Meiran, sympathetic activity (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011). We
2008). also extended the literature by testing cognitive effects of
Cognitive impairment and depletion can be measured rumination, reappraisal, and suppression with the Stroop
with the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), which requires the (1935). We used the color Stroop, rather than other ver-
use of selective attention and executive control to over- sions of the task (e.g. to measure attentional capture to
ride the automatic response of reading a word in an emotionally salient words), because our goal in this
effort to identify the color of the font in which it is study was to assess whether rumination and coping
printed. Unlike emotional or clinical versions of the deplete executive functions (e.g. resulting in decreased
Stroop task, which assess general slow-downs that occur ability to resolve response conflict).
when identifying the color of salient emotional or clini- The first set of analyses focused on ratings of empa-
cal words (i.e. attentional capture), the original color thy, forgiveness, and subjective emotion for the first
Stroop task requires executive control to resolve conflict trial of rumination vs. coping (with order counterbal-
that occurs when the word meaning and color are anced). We hypothesized that compassion would induce
mapped to the same response. Increased Stroop interfer- empathy and emotional forgiveness immediately, within
ence (a longer reaction time (RT) to name the font color its first 45 s trial (vs. the rumination trial). We hypothe-
when the word itself is the name of a different color, sized that both coping response types would immedi-
than when the word names the same color as the font) is ately subdue negative emotion compared to rumination.
seen after cognitively depleting events (e.g. Vohs, The second set of analyses tested emotional changes
Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005) and depleting social from the first to the third coping trial by strategy type;
interactions (e.g. Salvatore & Shelton, 2007). Studies we hypothesized that both strategies would continue to
have found increased Stroop interference after engaging move emotion from the negative end of the valence
in suppression (Inzlicht & Gutsell, 2007) and reappraisal spectrum to neutral, and that arousal ratings would
(Sheppes & Meiran, 2008). In the present study, we used decrease. The third set of analyses addressed changes in
The Journal of Positive Psychology 251

emotions associated with early vs. late rumination peri- Methods


ods by strategy type. We hypothesized that both strategy Participants
types would be associated with reduced negativity and
Sixty-four introductory psychology students (32 men, 32
arousal over time. Although this has not previously
women) completed the experiment as one way to meet a
been tested, we predicted that participants who learned
research requirement. Participants were an average of
to engage in compassionate reappraisal of the offender
19.24 years old (SD = 3.57). Of the participants, 54 were
(vs. emotional suppression of their responses to the
European-American, 3 were Asian-American, 2 were
offender) would be more likely to increase their
African-American, 1 was multi-ethnic, and 2 had missing
empathic perspective-taking of the offender while rumi-
data.
nating later in the study.
The fourth set of analyses assessed the descriptions
participants wrote about their experiences while ruminat- Design
ing or coping. Participants described their thoughts, feel-
This study used a mixed experimental design, in which
ings, physical responses, and what they wanted to say or
participants were randomly assigned to learn and practice
do to their offender. We predicted that for both strategy
one of two possible coping strategies (between subjects
types, coping would be associated with fewer negative
treatment conditions), each of which was contrasted with
emotion words. However, we predicted that compassion-
offense rumination-only trials using repeated measures.
ate reappraisal coping (vs. suppression coping) would
Specifically, participants were randomly assigned either
prompt larger increases in positive emotion word use
to learn how to compassionately reappraise their offender
compared to offense rumination (Witvliet et al., 2010,
or to suppress their experience and expression of nega-
2011).
tive emotions about the offense and offender. We did not
The fifth major set of analyses examined psycho-
use a control group, such as participants who ruminated
physiological variables and drew on findings from the
for the same number of trials without any coping.
subjective emotion analyses. If rumination emotions were
Unlike other repeated measures research in which
found to change significantly from the beginning to the
every participant ruminated, reappraised, and emotionally
end of the study differently for the two groups, then
suppressed with regard to an interpersonal offense
physiological analyses would assess reactivity for the
(Witvliet et al., 2011), the current participants ruminated
purest rumination trial participants experienced (i.e.
and coped using one strategy: either compassionate reap-
before the introduction of a coping strategy). The design
praisal or emotional suppression. Furthermore, whereas
allowed us to capture pure rumination reactivity by mea-
participants in earlier research were assessed during their
suring physiological data continuously. All physiology
single use of each coping strategy, with relaxation periods
reactivity measures subtract out the particular trial’s base-
between rumination and coping (Witvliet et al., 2011),
line responses, reducing the risk of overall habituation
the current study’s participants had three coping trials that
effects. Based on Witvliet et al. (2011), we predicted that
immediately followed half of the rumination trials. This
coping would decrease corrugator reactivity (i.e. brow
allowed us to assess the effect of practiced coping.
muscle tension) and heart rate vs. rumination, and we
Participants completed six trials. Each trial was com-
tested whether suppression would show a stronger effect
posed of a pretrial baseline followed by a period in
(Witvliet et al., 2011) when repeatedly practiced as the
which participants ruminated about a past real-life
only strategy. We predicted that PEP would also be
offense. On three of the six trials, offense rumination led
slower for coping, testing whether one strategy type was
directly into a 45 s period of coping. Coping was always
associated with a calmer sympathetic nervous system
preceded by offense rumination periods so that these
response.
strategies would be engaged as a response to rumination,
The sixth set of analyses assessed Stroop interference
reflecting how these coping strategies are employed out-
as an executive functioning indicator, while also examin-
side the laboratory.
ing accuracy rates and speed of responses as subtle
Each pretrial baseline served to relax the participant
indicators of cognitive demand. The experiment was
and clear his or her mind before the subsequent rumina-
designed to assess these responses for rumination early
tion or rumination-then-coping induction. The pretrial
in the study – before any significant changes to rumina-
baseline was also important for determining the physio-
tion ratings – in comparison to the learned coping
logical effects of each experimental condition; we calcu-
strategy. Against the backdrop of mixed cognitive effects
lated changes that occurred as participants went from
in a range of paradigms, some evidence found reap-
pretrial baseline to rumination or rumination-then-coping.
praisal to be more cognitively taxing than suppression
(We did not measure self-reports after each pretrial base-
(Sheppes et al., 2009; Sheppes & Meiran, 2008), leading
line period because this would induce movement-related
us to predict similar patterns.
252 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

physiological reactivity that could decrease the accuracy Pretrial relaxation baseline instructions guided partici-
of experimental condition effects). pants to quietly rest in the chair and keep their arms,
Trial orders were counterbalanced across participants. legs, and body still as they sat, relaxed, and thought the
Half of the participants (including equal numbers of men word “one” whenever they naturally breathed out.
and women) were randomly assigned to proceed through Offense rumination instructions guided participants to
six trials in the following order: ruminate, ruminate-cope, think about the person who hurt them and all the ways
ruminate-cope, ruminate, ruminate-cope, and ruminate. that this offense was hurtful to them. They were
The other half were assigned to proceed in this order: instructed to remember what happened, and the thoughts,
ruminate-cope, ruminate, ruminate, ruminate-cope, rumi- feelings, and physical reactions they had. They also
nate, and ruminate-cope. The first two trials in each thought of all the ways that they were affected by the
order included an early rumination trial (first instance of hurtful experience.
rumination) or early coping trial (first instance of rumi- Compassionate reappraisal instructions guided partici-
nate-cope). Coping that occurred in the last two trials pants to think of the offender as a human being whose
comprised the third use of the coping strategy and is offense behavior was bad. Even if the relationship could
thus referred to as practiced coping. not be restored, participants were to try to genuinely wish
In order to accommodate repeated trials within a that this person would undergo a positive transformation
90-min paradigm, we set the duration of pretrial relaxa- or healing experience. Even if it was difficult, participants
tion periods to 60 s of measurement, and we set each were encouraged to focus their thoughts and feelings on
rumination period and each coping period to 45 s of giving a genuine gift of mercy or compassion.
measurement. This duration is too short to measure HRV Emotional suppression instructions were to think
to assess parasympathetic responding (Witvliet et al., about the offender and the offense. At the same time,
2010, 2011). Rather, cardiac PEP was measured to assess participants were to try not to become emotional about
sympathetic nervous system activity. the experience and to try not to show any outward
expression of feelings they might have. As they thought
about the offender and offense, if they noticed any nega-
Procedure tive emotions, they were encouraged to try not to feel or
Procedures adapted Witvliet et al.’s (2010, 2011) para- show them.
digm in order to create multiple trials and to incorporate
additional cardiology and Stroop measures. Participants
individually gave informed consent and were tested. The Self-reports
participant sat in a stationary recliner in front of a flat Empathy, forgiveness, and emotion ratings
screen monitor as electrodes and physiological recording Each participant privately recorded his or her ratings via
devices were applied using standard methods. Each a computer after giving informed consent and then after
physiology variable was monitored from an adjacent completing each rumination or rumination-then-coping
room and tested for clear and reliable signals before trial. SuperLab software was used to present ratings in
beginning a period of baseline relaxation and a practice random orders. Participants used a Biopac RB-730
Stroop task. response-pad with a seven-point scale, rating their emo-
Subsequently, the participant completed a question- tional valence (1 = negative to 7 = very positive) and the
naire, identifying and describing a particular offense in degree to which they experienced arousal, empathy, and
which another person hurt and offended him or her in heartfelt forgiveness toward the offender (1 = not at all to
real life. This interpersonal offense was the basis for all 7 = completely).
subsequent rumination and coping conditions. Upon
study completion, physiological devices were removed,
and participants were debriefed. Analyses of written responses: Linguistic Inquiry and
Word Count
Written responses were measured for the middle trials of
Stimulus materials
rumination and rumination-then-coping conditions
We used published stimulus materials (Witvliet et al., (which were in the opposite order for half of the partici-
2011). A tone signaled participants to open their eyes pants). Participants typed into a laptop their responses to
and read the relevant 30 s computer screen display of four questions about what they were thinking, what they
instructions for the pretrial baseline relaxation, offense were feeling, their physical reactions during imagery,
rumination, compassionate reappraisal, or emotional sup- and what they would do or say to their real-life offender
pression. Participants were instructed to close their eyes (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011).
for all relaxation, rumination, and coping trials.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 253

Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software font). Because red and blue were the only font response
(LIWC2007 Windows v 1.12; Pennebaker, Booth, & options, neutral words would not be facilitated by the
Francis, 2007) counted the number of words in submit- word name and font color matching (as on congruent tri-
ted texts that matched predefined dictionaries. We used als), nor would they cause interference from the word
the standard LIWC2007 English dictionary containing name and font color describing conflicting response
categories for positive emotion and negative emotion. options (as on incongruent trials). Words were presented
for 175 ms, followed by a fixation period for at least
1500 ms between words. Responses were made with the
Physiology1 right (dominant) index and middle finger using a
Throughout all pretrial relaxation baselines, rumination, response pad. A practice Stroop occurred prior to the
and coping trials, we continuously measured physiologi- start of the experiment session so that participants could
cal responses with standard methods (Biopac MP150 and learn the task, and so that we could ensure the groups
Acqknowledge 3.9 software for the Macintosh). We both showed expected Stroop interference and had simi-
measured covert facial muscle activity relevant to emo- lar accuracy rates. The Stroop task was presented four
tion using standard electromyography (EMG) methods at times in the experiment, after both early and late rumina-
the corrugator supercilii (brow) muscle responsive to tion and coping periods (the first two blocks and the last
valence (Witvliet & Vrana, 1995). We used electrocardi- two blocks of the study).
ography (ECG) to obtain R-R (beat-to-beat) interval data Percent accuracy was calculated for each imagery
and calculate heart rate. We also measured impedance period. RT was calculated after removing incorrect trials.
cardiography in order to determine PEP data. PEP is cal- Stroop interference (referred to as the Stroop effect) was
culated through the measurement of cardiac bioimped- assessed for each imagery period by subtracting each
ance, comparing the timing of locations on the derivative participant’s median RT for congruent trials from their
of that curve to their counterparts on a measurement of median RT for incongruent trials. Longer RTs on incon-
ECG. PEP was assessed as a cardiac indicator of sympa- gruent trials reflect the increased difficulty of selectively
thetic nervous system activity, associated with the fight attending and responding to the font color when the
or flight response. word does not match it, because readers have to override
Physiological reactivity data for each condition were the prepotent tendency to read the color named by the
calculated as change from each condition’s pretrial base- word in order to respond correctly in identifying the
line to the subsequent imagery trial (Witvliet et al., color of the font.
2001, 2010, 2011; Witvliet & Vrana, 1995). The physio-
logical data were calculated for the last 45 s of the pretri-
al relaxation baseline, and for every 45 s offense Statistical analysis
rumination or coping strategy imagery trial. Pretrial base- The first three sets of analyses on subjective ratings were
line values were based on the final 45 s of each pretrial conducted using mixed ANOVAs for coping and rumina-
baseline to equalize durations and to ensure a calm tion within participants × Strategy Type (compassionate
pretrial baseline comparison. To determine change from reappraisal vs. suppression coping) between participants.
pretrial baseline, physiological values from this 45 s per- The fourth set of analyses assessed LIWC emotion word
iod were subtracted from the raw physiological values counts using mixed ANOVAs testing the Coping Effect
for the 45 s offense rumination and 45 s coping strategy (rumination vs. coping) within participants × two Coping
trials. This standard procedure enables the reader to see Strategy Groups (compassionate reappraisal vs. suppres-
whether increases or decreases from baseline occurred. sion) between participants. Emotion ratings are reported
The Explore function of SPSS 15.0 was used to identify below and in Table 1 and in Figures 1–3. LIWC results
and remove statistical outliers that may have been caused are reported below and in Figure 4.
by technological difficulties with the apparatus, electrical Ratings results indicated that rumination changed dif-
noise, or movement artifacts. ferently across coping strategy types. Thus, we con-
ducted the fifth set of analyses using physiological
reactivity for the pure rumination trial (before learning a
Stroop coping strategy) and compared this to reactivity for the
The Stroop task we used was selected as a partial repli- learned coping trial. Data were analyzed with mixed
cation of the Stroop used in related research by Gehring, ANOVAs for coping and rumination within partici-
Himle, and Nisenson (2000). It consisted of 72 trials of pants × Strategy Type (compassionate reappraisal vs. sup-
randomly presented words in red or blue ink. Words had pression coping) between participants. The sixth set of
an equal probability of being congruent (“red” appeared analyses for Stroop data similarly used a mixed ANOVA
in red ink), incongruent (“blue” appeared in red ink), or for coping and rumination within participants × Strategy
neutral (“brown” or “green” appeared in a blue or red Type (compassionate reappraisal vs. suppression coping)
254 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

Table 1. Means (SDs) for early and late rumination and coping trials by strategy-type between subjects.

Rumination 1 Coping 1 Rumination 3 Coping 3


Dependent variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Compassionate reappraisal learners
Ratings (1–7 range)
Valence 2.91 (1.40) 3.38 (1.64) 3.84 (1.65) 4.06 (1.52)
Arousal 4.53 (1.63) 4.25 (1.67) 3.31 (1.42) 2.84 (1.55)
Empathy 2.81 (1.47) 3.75 (1.78) 3.88 (1.74) 3.78 (1.66)
Emotional forgiveness 3.44 (1.63) 4.00 (1.72) 4.03 (1.87) 4.13 (1.86)

Suppression learners

Valence 3.13 (1.41) 3.66 (1.36) 3.88 (1.81) 4.19 (1.82)


Arousal 4.22 (1.68) 3.81 (1.71) 3.25 (1.85) 2.97 (1.69)
Empathy 3.00 (1.48) 2.91 (1.59) 2.88 (1.66) 2.94 (1.52)
Emotional forgiveness 3.75 (1.92) 3.47 (1.65) 3.81 (1.99) 3.44 (1.88)
Note: Rumination and coping trial orders were counterbalanced.

Figure 3. The initial compassion trial prompted significantly


Figure 1. In compassion learners, empathy ratings for initial
more emotional forgiveness (vs. rumination), which persisted in
compassionate reappraisal were significantly higher than for ini-
later trials. Rumination and coping trial orders were counterbal-
tial rumination. Rumination empathy levels increased signifi-
anced early and late in the study.
cantly from the initial to the final trial, becoming equivalent to
compassionate reappraisal empathy levels. In suppression learn-
ers, rumination and suppression for initial and final trials all
showed the same level of empathy ratings. Standard error bars
are shown. between participants. We conducted analyses which
ensured that baseline practice trials showed equivalent
Stroop interference effects and accuracy using between
group tests.
The physiological and Stroop means (standard devia-
tions), p values, F values, and partial η2 values are
reported in Table 2.

Results
Ratings of empathy, forgiveness, and emotion
We conducted analyses to answer three questions about
the ratings (Table 1 reports the means and SDs). First,
at the earliest measurement periods, did coping
Figure 2. Empathy ratings after the first coping trial mediate strategies differ compared to rumination? To investigate
the relationship between coping strategy type and empathy this, we used mixed ANOVAs for Condition (early cop-
ratings after the final rumination trial. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01;
***p < 0.001.
ing and rumination) within participants × Strategy Type
The Journal of Positive Psychology 255

(compassionate reappraisal vs. suppression coping) learners, the final rumination vs. final coping trial
between participants. After only 45 s of compassionate prompted statistically equivalent empathy levels,
reappraisal, participants showed significant Condition × Fs = 0.01, p = 0.92, partial η2 = 0.00.]
Strategy Type interactions for both empathy, F(1, 62) = As shown in Figure 2, an auxiliary mediation analy-
8.33, p = 0.005, partial η2 = 0.12, and emotional forgive- sis was conducted using boot strapping analysis with
ness, F(1, 62) = 4.58, p = 0.036, partial η2 = . 07. Only PROCESS (Hayes, 2012) to examine the indirect effect
compassionate reappraisal learners showed significantly of strategy type on final rumination trial empathy ratings
higher empathy and emotional forgiveness while coping via the initial coping trial empathy ratings. This model
vs. ruminating on the first trial, empathy Condition was conducted with 5000 bootstraps and yielded a boot-
F(1, 31) = 14.56, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.32; emotional strap estimate of the indirect effect through the mediator
forgiveness Condition F(1, 31) = 4.77, p = 0.037, partial of 0.12. The 95% confidence interval did not include 0
η2 = 0.13. The early empathy effect is evident in [0.01, 0.28]. Further, the significant direct effect between
Figure 1, whereas emotional forgiveness is depicted in coping strategy used and empathy at the final rumination
Figure 3. A main effect showed that compared to rumi- trial was no longer statistically significant with inclusion
nation, early coping shifted valence ratings from nega- of the mediator (initial coping trial levels of empathy) in
tive to neutral emotion, Condition F(1, 62) = 5.22, the analysis. Thus, this analysis suggests that empathy
p = 0.026, partial η2 = 0.08. Arousal tests were not signif- ratings for the first coping trial mediated the relationship
icant, Fs(1, 62) ≤ 2.30, ps ≥ 0.14, partial η2s ≤ 0.04. between strategy type and empathy ratings for the final
Second, did ratings for the coping trials change from rumination trial.
the earliest to the latest measurement period? We used
mixed ANOVAs to investigate Coping Practice (early vs.
late coping) within participants × Strategy Type (compas- Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC2007)
sionate reappraisal vs. suppression coping) between To assess emotion through written responses, partici-
participants. We found a main effect of Strategy Type, in pants typed descriptions of what they were thinking,
which compassionate reappraisal (vs. suppression) feeling emotionally and physically, and what they
learners gave higher empathy ratings, F(1, 62) = 5.41, wanted to say or do to their offender, both after
p = 0.023, partial η2 = 0.08. The significant effects on offense rumination and after coping midstudy. The
empathy and emotional forgiveness reported for early responses participants typed in this study were an aver-
trial coping reported above remained constant, with no age of 36 words long, substantially fewer words than
change over time, Coping Practice and Coping Practice × in other studies that requested responses to longer
Strategy Type Fs(1, 62) ≤ 0.19, ps ≥ 0.67, partial η2s ≤ imagery periods (51.4 words in Witvliet et al., 2011;
0.003. However, both strategies showed Coping Practice 61.8 words in Witvliet et al., 2010).
main effects, shifting emotional valence ratings from As shown in Figure 4, written responses showed
negative to neutral, F(1, 62) = 6.92, p = 0.011, partial that participants used fewer negative emotion words
η2 = 0.10; and reducing arousal ratings F(1, 62) = 21.29, after coping than ruminating, Coping F(1, 61) = 15.81,
p = 0.000, partial η2 = 0.26. p ≤ 0.001, partial η2 = 0.21. Positive emotion word counts,
Third, how did ratings for the rumination trials however, showed a Coping × Strategy Type interaction,
change from the earliest to the latest measurement
period? To answer this, we used mixed ANOVAs with
Repetition (early offense rumination, late offense rumina-
tion) within participants × Strategy Type (compassionate
reappraisal vs. suppression coping) between participants.
Main effects of repetition showed that late rumination
was associated with emotional change, which included
lower arousal, F(1, 62) = 25.19, p ≤ 0.001, partial
η2 = 0.29, and valence that became less negative,
F(1, 62) = 15.20, p ≤ 0.001, partial η2 = 0.20.
Figure 1 shows the significant Rumination
Repetition × Strategy Type interaction, F(1, 62) = 12.56,
p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.17 for empathy ratings. Specifi-
cally, participants who learned the compassionate reap-
praisal strategy (vs. the emotion suppression strategy)
Figure 4. Rumination was described with significantly more
reported increased empathy for their offender during late negative words than both coping responses. Only compassion
vs. early offense rumination, F(1, 31) = 20.04, p ≤ 0.001, was described with significantly more positive words than
partial η2 = 0.39. [Indeed for compassionate reappraisal rumination. Standard error bars are shown.
256 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

F (1, 61) = 8.07, p = .006, partial η2 = 0.12. Only compas- reactivity for both corrugator EMG and heart rate. How-
sionate reappraisal coping increased positive emotion lan- ever, cardiac PEP data showed a significant Coping ×
guage use, F(1, 31) = 21.77, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.41. Strategy Type interaction. Specifically, compassionate
[Suppression coping did not differ from rumination in the reappraisal (vs. rumination) tended to show less reactiv-
number of positive emotion words, F(1, 31) = 2.05, ity on this indicator of sympathetic nervous system
p = 0.16, partial η2 = 0.06.] innervation of the heart, F(1, 21) = 3.32, p = 0.08, partial
η2 = 0.14, whereas suppression did not.

Physiology
Analyses addressed the research question of how learn- Stroop3
ing a coping strategy influenced physiological reactivity Before reporting the Stroop effect results, we first report
compared to offense rumination. Because ruminations participants’ accuracy rates. The analyses for Stroop data
changed over time, showing unique compassion effects, revealed that overall accuracy in the Stroop task was
we compared physiological reactivity from pretrial base- high, with a mean of 96.6% across all blocks. As
line for pure rumination trials to reactivity for learned expected from previous demonstrations of the Stroop
coping trials.2 We did not hypothesize that the two ran- effect (e.g. MacLeod, 1991; Stroop, 1935), accuracy was
domly assigned groups would differ when conditions lower for incongruent trials (95.12%) compared to con-
were collapsed, and no significant effects occurred for gruent (97.44%) and neutral trials (97.17%), F(1, 120) =
any of physiological measures (all Fs ≤ 2.39, ps ≥ 0.13, 17.53, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.23. After removing the
and partial η2 ≤.05). error trials, we calculated and compared the magnitude
Table 2 shows a general Coping Effect, such that of Stroop interference (i.e. the Stroop effect) across
coping trials were associated with significantly less participants and blocks.

Table 2. Physiology and Stroop for the 2 coping effect (offense rumination, learned coping) repeated measures ×2 strategy type
(suppression, reappraisal) between subjects design: Means, F values, and partial η2s for offense rumination and learned compassionate
reappraisal, and for offense rumination and suppression.

Compassionate reappraisal learners Suppression learners


Rumination Coping Rumination Coping
Dependent variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Facial EMG (microVolts change from pretrial baseline)
Corrugator 1.01 (2.16) −0.001 (2.32) 0.76 (1.08) 0.21 (1.33)
Cardiovascular (change from pretrial baseline)
Heart rate (bpm) 0.86 (2.65) 0.33 (4.46) 1.63 (2.69) −0.41 (2.27)
Pre-ejection period (s) −0.002 (0.01) 0.01 (0.03) 0.003 (0.01) −0.005 (0.03)
Stroop
Reaction time (ms) 403.84 (88.95) 421.11 (105.07) 444.73 (119.01) 432.06 (99.53)
Trial type
Congruent 392.31 (71.26) 412.31 (97.82) 432.31 (99.44) 418.55 (86.93)
Incongruent 409.17 (102.02) 436.64 (115.60) 468.19 (151.54) 441.36 (116.89)
Neutral 410.05 (92.79) 414.38 (102.95) 433.70 (98.49) 436.27 (94.02)
Stroop effect 16.86 (46.49) 24.33 (55.84) 35.88 (70.45) 22.81 (60.93)

Coping effect. Coping effect × strategy type


(rumination vs. learned coping) (suppression vs. reappraisal)
(df) F Partial η2 (df) F Partial η2
EMG (microVolts change from relevant pretrial baseline)
Corrugator (1, 56) 5.65* .09 (1, 56) 0.50 .01
Cardiovascular (change from relevant pretrial baseline)
Heart rate (1, 60) 5.34* .08 (1, 60) 1.85 .03
Pre-ejection period (1, 47) 0.55 .01 (1, 47) 4.38* .09
Stroop
Reaction time (1, 59) 0.03 .001 (1, 59) 3.04+ .05
Interaction with trial type (2, 118) 0.16 .003 (2, 118) 5.39** .08
Stroop effect (1, 59) 0.14 .002 (1, 59) 1.36 .02
Note: Also see Footnotes 2 and 3.
*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; +p ≤ 0.08.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 257

As shown in Table 2, the expected RT Stroop Our focus on empathy follows a recent meta-analysis in
effect was found across coping strategies and blocks, t which a strong predictor of granting forgiveness was a
(121) = 4.71, p < 0.001. However, the magnitude of the state of empathy for the offender (Fehr et al., 2010). The
Stroop effect after early rumination did not differ by current study design allows us to assess empathy not
condition or coping strategy. This means that offense only when using compassionate reappraisal and emotion
rumination and coping across strategy types were asso- suppression, but also during ruminative memories of the
ciated with typical Stroop effect results. offender, early and late in the study. Previous research
Given that no Stroop effect differences occurred on interpersonal offense rumination, emotional suppres-
between conditions and groups, we examined other mea- sion, and compassionate reappraisal called for experi-
sures of subtle cognitive demands, namely accuracy and mentation. Specifically, it was important to test
RT. Compassionate reappraisal was associated with lower hypotheses that changes associated with learning both
accuracy on the Stroop (M = 94.8%) than practiced sup- coping strategies would be found in participants who
pression (M = 97.1%), t(181) = 3.27, p = 0.001. Further- repeatedly coped (Sandage & Worthington, 2010;
more, across trial types with accurate responses, RTs Worthington & Sotoohi, 2009) using only one strategy
showed a Coping × Strategy Type interaction. Specifi- in direct response to rumination rather than as a stand-
cally, compassionate reappraisal and suppression pro- alone condition (see Witvliet et al., 2011). Because our
duced changes from offense rumination in opposite goal was to compare randomly assigned compassionate
directions, leading to an interaction (see Table 2). While reappraisal to emotion suppression patterns, we did not
practiced suppression (vs. early rumination) led to faster compare these strategy types to a control condition (e.g.
responses on both congruent and incongruent trials (but a group of participants who ruminated for all six trials
not neutral trials), practiced compassionate reappraisal without using any coping strategy). Thus, we only draw
(vs. early rumination) led to slower responses on these conclusions about the strategies in comparison to each
trials (see RT means in Table 2). Together these results other as a between subjects factor interacting with
suggest that the slower RTs for compassionate reap- repeated measures conditions such as coping vs. rumina-
praisal indicated increased difficulty with the task, rather tion or repetitions of coping or ruminating.
than the adoption of a more cautious response strategy
(i.e. they did not slow down to be more accurate). Over-
all, results suggest that compassionate reappraisal was Strategy similarities for compassionate reappraisal and
associated with subtle cognitive demands rather than emotional suppression
executive control impairment. Half of the participants practiced compassionate reap-
praisal of the offender as a human being whose offense
demonstrated his or her need to experience positive or
Discussion healing transformation. The other half practiced emo-
The purpose of this experiment was to test whether tional suppression by thinking of their offense and offen-
practicing compassionate reappraisal for an offender or der, but trying not to feel or show negative emotions.
suppressing one’s negative emotions about an offender When compared with ruminating about one’s offender
had similar or different effects on empathy for the offen- and offense, both strategies immediately down-regulated
der, forgiveness of the offender, and emotion-regulation negative emotion ratings (see the first trials, Table 1).
variables. Our aim was to discover which ratings effects Linguistic analyses (depicted in Figure 4) show that across
would emerge quickly and which would develop with strategy type, participants similarly reduced their use of
repeated practice of the strategies. negative emotion words when describing their experiences
Overall, participants using either of these randomly of coping vs. ruminating in the middle of the study. By the
assigned strategies showed similar results related to end of the study, both coping strategy types not only
reductions in negative emotion and arousal. However, reduced the negative valence of emotion, but also decreased
significant differences between the two coping strategies arousal ratings. These ratings and linguistic effects are
emerged for empathy, forgiveness, and positive emotion. consistent with results found by Witvliet et al. (2011).
With coping strategies placed immediately after rumina- Physiologically, both coping strategies significantly
tions, this design showed that learning compassionate reduced corrugator EMG (i.e. brow muscle tension) and
reappraisal of an offender can change empathy for the heart rate in comparison to offense rumination trials (see
offender when later ruminating about the hurtful offense. Table 2). These patterns are consistent with basic
The two coping strategies also produced different emotion research using an imagery paradigm, which
responses for a cardiac response and for measures of found more reactive corrugator EMG for conditions
subtle cognitive demands. characterized by negative (vs. positive) emotional
The current work responds to theoretical develop- valence, and greater heart rate reactivity for conditions
ments in forgiveness and emotion regulation research. with high (vs. low) arousal (Witvliet & Vrana, 1995).
258 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

Finally, Stroop interference effects occurred for all photograph can produce lasting emotional and neural
conditions, and the two strategies (vs. rumination) did effects that persist in later encounters with the same
not differ statistically (see Table 2). This suggests that stimulus (MacNamara, Ochsner, & Hajcak, 2011). Such
the coping conditions were not cognitively depleting results have implications for clinical therapies, suggest-
(e.g. Vohs et al., 2005), in contrast to prior research- ing that imaginal exposure with reappraisal can be com-
associating suppression (Inzlicht & Gutsell, 2007) and bined in ways that transform one’s empathic response to
reappraisal (Sheppes & Meiran, 2008) with increased subsequent ruminations about an offender. It will be
Stroop interference. important to replicate the findings from the current study
with other populations to determine whether samples
varying in spiritual, religious, socioeconomic, academic,
Differences between compassionate reappraisal and and age-related characteristics can also experience imme-
emotion suppression diate compassionate reappraisal increases in empathy for
Interactions between the strategy type participants used an offender, which mediate empathy responses to later
(compassionate reappraisal vs. suppression coping) and ruminations about the offense.
their repeated measures conditions contribute to what is As the first study to measure cardiac sympathetic ner-
known about how quickly empathy and forgiveness vous system control for emotion regulation in relation to
effects emerge, differences in positive emotion, and forgiveness, we found that only compassionate reap-
changes to the cardiac response and subtle cognitive praisal tended to slow down cardiac PEP compared to
demands. Only compassionate reappraisal learners imme- offense rumination. This is consistent with prior research
diately showed significantly higher levels of empathy finding increased sympathetic activations during rumina-
and forgiveness while coping rather than ruminating tion, but not during reappraisal (Ray et al., 2008). Other
(Figures 1 and 3). Furthermore, these changes were studies have shown that compassionate reappraisal had
maintained throughout the study (Table 1). These results equivalent levels of parasympathetic activity compared
demonstrate that transformations of empathy and forgive- to relaxation (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011). However, the
ness emerged despite briefer compassionate reappraisal present study indicates that compassionate reappraisal
trials than prior work (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011). may buffer against the potentially harmful sympathetic
Mid-study, participants described what they had been nervous system activations of the heart during offense
thinking, feeling, how their body had responded, and rumination.
what they would say or do to the offender. Linguistic We found that the Stroop effect was similar across
analyses showed significantly more positive emotion conditions and groups, indicating that this measure of
words for compassionate reappraisal coping versus. executive control was not differentially affected by the
offense rumination. As can be seen in Figure 4, only ways in which participants thought about their offenders.
compassionate reappraisal coping prompted a significant Rather, compassionate reappraisal showed two subtle
increase in positive emotion words – as many positive cognitive demand effects. First, practiced compassionate
emotion words as rumination had negative emotion reappraisal was associated with lower accuracy rates than
words. practiced suppression. Second, on accurate trials, prac-
By the end of the study, rumination was also trans- ticed compassionate reappraisal (vs. rumination) had
formed in compassion learners (Figure 1 and Table 1). longer RTs, whereas emotional suppression showed the
Compared with their early rumination ratings, compas- opposite pattern. Thus, compassionate reappraisal
sionate reappraisal learners changed the way they rumi- incurred subtle cognitive costs that were not evident for
nated, reporting that they had more empathy for the emotional suppression.
offender while ruminating at the end of the study. Trans-
formation of rumination was so potent that the final
rumination and compassion trials had statistically equiva- Conclusions
lent levels of empathy. The auxiliary mediational analy- Forgiveness theory emphasizes the importance of
sis (Hayes, 2012) demonstrated that participants’ responding to injustice in a moral way that maintains the
empathy ratings after the first experience of coping medi- humanity of the wrong-doer without ignoring or
ated the relationship between coping strategy type and minimizing the wrong-doing (e.g. Luchies et al., 2010;
their empathy ratings when ruminating at the end of the McNulty, 2010; Worthington, 2009). Thus, forgiveness
study (Figure 2). This is the first study to show that involves a combination of truth-telling and transforma-
when interspersed with episodes of compassionate reap- tion, which is important for compassion-rooted and
praisal, rumination can take on some of the benefits responsible forgiveness that can contribute to the
offered by compassionate reappraisal. flourishing of the forgiver, the offender, and community
This finding is consistent with recent research (Witvliet, 2012; Worthington, 2009). A victim who
demonstrating that initial instructions to reappraise a forgives ideally will see the veracity of the offender’s
The Journal of Positive Psychology 259

culpability and also see the offender’s complex humanity; change with practice across even extended longitudinal
a forgiving victim ideally will neither totalize the offen- periods. Longer paradigms such as those used in inter-
der’s identity in terms of the offense nor minimize the vention studies could also be designed to measure other
importance and implications of the injustice. Compas- peripheral nervous system, hormonal (e.g. oxytocin, corti-
sionate reappraisal recognizes that the victim is in a sol), and central nervous system measures (e.g. EEG,
unique position to see the offense as evidence of the fMRI), which this design was not suited to assess, but
offender’s need for learning, growth, change, and/or heal- which would substantially contribute to what is known
ing. about unforgiveness and forgiveness.
Evidence from the current study associated compas- Such explorations have implications for the develop-
sionate reappraisal with immediate increases in empathy ment of clinical interventions for fostering forgiveness
and forgiveness that occurred with even shorter trial dura- and transformative change following real-life transgres-
tions than other research (Witvliet et al., 2010, 2011). sions. In a meta-analysis of group interventions, Wade,
Extending prior research, the current study found that Worthington, and Meyer (2005) found a dose-response
empathy at the initial coping trial mediated the relation- relationship in which time spent empathizing with the
ship between coping strategy type and empathy ratings offender predicted forgiveness. While 4–6 + hour inter-
for the offender during the final offense rumination trial. ventions prompted the strongest forgiveness changes, the
Importantly, by learning to practice compassion in average time spent on empathizing with the offender
response to painful offense ruminations, ruminations in group interventions was slightly over one hour
themselves changed to become more empathic toward the (68.2 min, SD = 54.9). Current findings show that focus-
offender. These findings illuminate an effective way to ing on the humanity of the offender, along with viewing
induce a state of empathy for one’s offender while using the offense as evidence of that person’s need to learn,
a strategy that holds the offender accountable for the grow, and/or change for the better not only activates
wrongdoing. Thus, results from the current study provide empathy immediately during compassion, but also that
an important contribution to the literature recognizing these effects can persist within an intervention session
empathy as a strong predictor of forgiveness (Fehr et al., and can even change ruminations to become more
2010), and the literature emphasizing the importance of empathic. These are important within-session changes
justice in considerations of forgiveness (e.g. Luchies that bode well for long-term forgiveness work. Thus, we
et al., 2010; McNulty, 2010; Worthington, 2009). encourage forgiveness interventions to consider testing
Forgiveness has been theorized to involve self- the effects of teaching compassionate reappraisal and to
control, which may involve the restraint of unforgiving practice the pattern of transitioning directly from offense
emotions and motivations (Worthington & Sotoohi, memories into compassionate reappraisal.
2009). In the current experiment and one other study,
emotional suppression of negative offense-related
emotions de-escalated negative and aroused emotions Acknowledgements
without increasing forgiveness or positive language We gratefully acknowledge support offered through a grant to
responses (Witvliet et al., 2011). Thus, emotional sup- the first and fourth authors from the Fetzer Institute, and
pression appears to have a subduing effect on negative Howard Hughes Medical Institute Research Scholar awards to
emotion consistent with Dunn et al.’s (2009) adaptive the second, third, and fourth authors. This work contributes
suppression hypothesis. to an interdisciplinary project on The Pursuit of Happiness
established by the Center for the Study of Law and Religion
We cautiously raise implications for clinical interven- at Emory University and supported by a grant from the John
tions. Past research suggested that reappraising an inter- Templeton Foundation. The authors thank Nathaniel DeYoung,
personal hurt by either cultivating compassion for the Lindsey Root Luna, John Shaughnessy, Daryl VanTongeren,
offender or finding benefits in the situation had positive and David G. Myers for insightful conversations about the
effects after 120 s periods (Witvliet et al., 2010). project. Correspondence should be directed to the first author.
Currently, Alicia Hofelich Mohr is at the College of the
Research also suggested that compassionate reappraisal Liberal Arts Office of Information Technology, University of
induced positive changes, whereas emotional suppression Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, 55455, USA. Nova Hinman is
provided negative reinforcement through the alleviation at the Department of Psychology, Bowling Green State
of negative subjective and physiological responses University, Bowling Green, OH, 43403-0232, USA. Ross
(Witvliet et al., 2011). The current study suggests that, if Knoll is at the Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois
University, DeKalb, IL, 60115, USA.
replicated in other populations, there may be benefits for
intervention sessions that intersperse imaginal exposures
to offense memories with exposure immediately followed Notes
by compassionate reappraisal. 1. We used Biopac guidelines and the documented methods
Future research may fruitfully address whether the of Witvliet et al. (2011). In addition, impedance cardiology
differences between suppression and reappraisal strategies was measured with the Biopac NICO 100C module and
260 C.V.O. Witvliet et al.

four disposable EL506 bioimpedance electrode strips on review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351–374. doi:10.1037/
the back, with two strips located above the heart on the a0018807
base of the neck, and two strips below the heart on the Gordon, K., Burton, S., & Porter, L. (2004). Predicting the
back. Impedance cardiology data were filtered and ampli- intentions of women in domestic violence shelters to return
fied using Biopac recommended methods for the MP150, to partners: Does forgiveness play a role? Journal of Fam-
NICO 100C, and Acqknowledge software. PEP requires ily Psychology, 18, 331–338. doi:10.1037/0893-
both an ECG channel and a dZ/dt channel (which is the 3200.18.2.331
derivative of the impedance wave). PEP was measured by Gross, J. J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New
comparing the onset of the Q-wave in the ECG to the York, NY: Guilford Press.
onset of the B-point in the dZ/dt wave. Hargrave, T. (2001). Forgiving the devil: Coming to terms with
2. We conducted follow-up analyses of pure rumination reac- damaged relationships. Phoenix, AZ: Zeig, Tucker, and
tivity vs. late rumination reactivity, showing no difference Theisen.
for heart rate, F (1, 57) = 2.38, p > 0.05, partial η2 = 0.04, Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool
and no difference for PEP, F (1, 57) = 0.55, p > 0.05, par- for observed variable mediation, moderation and condi-
tial η2 = 0.01, whereas corrugator reactivity during late tional process modeling. Retrieved from http://www.afhays.
rumination (mean = 0.01, SD = 1.77) was lower than pure com/public/process2012.pdf
rumination (mean = 0.88, SD = 1.67), Repetition F (1, 57) Hebl, J. H., & Enright, R. D. (1993). Forgiveness as a psycho-
= 8.20, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.13. therapeutic goal with elderly females. Psychotherapy: The-
3. Both coping groups showed the expected significant Stroop ory, Research, Practice, Training, 30, 658–667.
effect for the baseline trials, t(29) > 2.91, p < 0.006. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.30.4.658
Accuracy and the magnitude of the Stroop effect did not Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty:
differ between groups for the baseline trials, t(56) < 1.27, Neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Sci-
p > 0.20. Although our main questions concerned the ence, 18, 933–937. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02004.x
cognitive effects of practiced coping vs. the early rumination Luchies, L. B., Finkel, E. J., McNulty, J. K., & Kumashiro, M.
trial, we also assessed differences between early and late (2010). The doormat effect: When forgiving erodes self-
rumination and coping trials. For the early trials, there was respect and self-concept clarity. Journal of Personality and
a larger Stroop effect after initial rumination (mean = 26.83, Social Psychology, 98, 734–749. doi:10.1037/a0017838
SD = 60.53) than after initial coping (mean = 8.23, SD = MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the
47.91), F(1, 59) = 9.50, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.13. This Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulle-
effect and the magnitude of Stroop interference did not dif- tin, 109, 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
fer between rumination and suppression groups, F(3, 57) < MacNamara, A., Ochsner, K. N., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Previously
1.90, p > 0.14, partial η2 < 0.079. There were no significant reappraised: The lasting effect of description type on picture-
differences in the Stroop effect after late rumination and late elicited electrocortical activity. Social Cognitive and Affective
coping, F(1, 59) = 0.24, p = 0.70, partial η2 = 0.002. The Neuroscience, 6, 348–358. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq053
early instances of compassionate reappraisal, as well as sup- McCullough, M. E., Orsulak, P., Brandon, A., & Akers, L.
pression, were associated with reduced Stroop interference (2007). Rumination, fear, and cortisol: An in vivo study of
compared to early rumination. This reduction in interference interpersonal transgressions. Health Psychology, 26,
was not seen for practiced coping, which did not differ from 126–132. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.126
interference after early or late rumination. These findings McNulty, J. K. (2010). Forgiveness increases the likelihood of
suggest repeated efforts at coping may be associated with subsequent partner transgressions in marriage. Journal of
greater cognitive demands than the first instances of these Family Psychology, 24, 787–790. doi:10.1037/a0021678
strategies. Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007).
Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC [Computer soft-
ware]. Austin, TX: LIWC.net.
References Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the
Dunn, B. D., Billotti, D., Murphy, V., & Dalgleish, T. (2009). mind’s eye? Anger rumination and reappraisal. Journal of
The consequences of effortful emotion regulation when Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 133–145.
processing distressing material: A comparison of suppres- doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.133
sion and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, Salvatore, J., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Cognitive costs of
761–773. exposure to racial prejudice. Psychological Science, 18,
Enright, R. (2001). Forgiveness is a choice: A step-by-step pro- 810–815. doi:10.1037/a0014191
cess for resolving anger and restoring hope. Washington, Sandage, S. J., & Jankowski, P. J. (2010). Forgiveness, spiritual
DC: American Psychological Association. instability, mental health symptoms, and well-being: Media-
Enright, R., & Fitzgibbons, R. (2000). Helping clients forgive: tor effects of differentiation of self. Psychology of Religion
An empirical guide for resolving anger and restoring hope. and Spirituality, 2, 168–180. doi:10.1037/a0019124
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Sandage, S. J., & Worthington, E. L., Jr (2010). Comparison of
Fehr, R., Gelfand, M. J., & Nag, M. (2010). The road to forgive- two group interventions to promote forgiveness: Empathy as
ness: A meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and disposi- a mediator of change. Journal of Mental Health Counseling,
tional correlates. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 894–914. 32, 35–57. Retrieved from http://amhca.metapress.com/
Gehring, W. J., Himle, J., & Nisenson, L. G. (2000). Action- Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2008). Divergent cognitive costs
monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. for online forms of reappraisal and distraction. Emotion, 8,
Psychological Science, 11, 1–6. doi:10.1111/1467-9280. 870–874. doi:10.1037/a0013711
00206 Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D. K., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). not distraction) is going to make you sweat: Physiological
Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical evidence for self-control effort. International Journal of
The Journal of Positive Psychology 261

Psychophysiology, 71, 91–96. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008. Witvliet, C. V. O., Knoll, R. W., Hinman, N. G., & DeYoung,
06.006 P. A. (2010). Compassion-focused reappraisal, benefit-
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal focused reappraisal, and rumination after an interpersonal
reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, offense: Emotion regulation implications for subjective
643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651 emotion, linguistic responses, and physiology. The Journal
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Self- of Positive Psychology, 5, 226–242. doi:10.1080/
regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory resource 17439761003790997.
depletion impairs impression management and effortful Witvliet, C. V. O., Ludwig, T., & VanderLaan, K. (2001).
self-presentation depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Granting forgiveness or harboring grudges: Implications for
Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 632–657. emotions, physiology, and health. Psychological Science,
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.632 12, 117–123. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00320
Wade, N. G., Worthington, E. L., Jr, & Meyer, J. E. (2005). Witvliet, C. V. O., & McCullough, M. E. (2007). Forgiveness
But do they work? A meta-analysis of group interventions and health: A review and theoretical exploration of emotion
to promote forgiveness. In E. L. Worthington Jr (Ed.), pathways. In S. Post (Ed.), Altruism and health: Perspec-
Handbook of forgiveness (pp. 423–349). New York, NY: tives from empirical research (pp. 258–276). New York,
Routledge. NY: Oxford University Press.
Witvliet, C. V. O. (2012). Empirical studies of forgiveness as Witvliet, C. V. O., & Vrana, S. R. (1995). Psychophysiological
an altruistic response: Relationships with rumination, sup- responses as indices of affective dimensions. Psychophysi-
pression of negative emotions, and a benefit-focused reap- ology, 32, 436–443. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.
praisal. In M. R. Maamri, N. Verbin, & E. L. Worthington tb02094.x
Jr (Eds.), Mapping forgiveness (pp. 15–32). Oxford: Inter- Worthington, E. L., Jr (2009). A just forgiveness: Responsible
Disciplinary Press. healing without excusing injustice. Downers Grove, IL:
Witvliet, C. V. O., DeYoung, N. J., Hofelich, A. J., & InterVarsity Press.
DeYoung, P. A. (2011). Compassionate reappraisal and Worthington, E. L., Jr, & Sotoohi, G. (2009). Physiological assess-
emotion suppression as alternatives to offense-focused ment of forgiveness, grudges, and revenge: Theories, research
rumination: Implications for forgiveness and psychophysio- methods, and implications. In A. M. Columbus (Ed.),
logical well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, Advances in psychology research (Vol. 64, pp. 135–153).
286–299. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.577091 New York, NY: Nova Science.

You might also like