You are on page 1of 8

Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Social Science & Medicine


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/socscimed

Maternal breastfeeding and children's cognitive development*


Kanghyock Koh*
Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology (UNIST), Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Do children with lower test scores benefit more from breastfeeding than those with higher scores? In
Received 28 November 2016 this paper, I examine the distributional effects of maternal breastfeeding on the cognitive test scores of
Received in revised form 11,544 children who were born in 2000 and 2001 in the United Kingdom using a semiparametric quantile
30 May 2017
regression model. I find evidence that maternal breastfeeding has larger positive impacts on children
Accepted 11 June 2017
with lower test scores. Effects for children below the 20th percentile are about 2e2.5 times greater than
Available online 12 June 2017
those for children above the 80th percentile. I also find that these distributional effects are larger when
the duration of breastfeeding is extended. One policy implication is that a public policy aims at pro-
Keywords:
United Kingdom
moting breastfeeding might narrow a disparity in children's cognition.
Distributional effects © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Maternal breastfeeding
Children's development
Semiparametric quantile regression

1. Introduction et al., 1981). Note that the compositional superiority of breast


milk may be reduced because DHA and AA are now added to for-
Economic theories of human capital have emphasized the mula. However, Fitzsimons and Vera-Hernandez (2012) reported
importance of parental investment in children (Becker, 1981; that DHA and AA were not included in formula in the UK (the
Cunha and Heckman, 2007). A large number of empirical studies context of this paper) until most of the infants who are considered
show that human capital developments in early childhood (e.g., in the empirical analysis were born. Deoni et al. (2013) provided
cognitive ability) play a significant role in human capital de- MRI evidence that breastfed infants exhibit better development in
velopments later in life, as measured by educational attainment, specific brain areas associated with language and visual reception
employment, wages, etc. (Heckman, 2006; Cunha and Heckman, abilities compared to those who are fed formula or a mixture of
2007; Cunha et al., 2010; Almond and Currie, 2011a,b). breast milk and formula. Second, skin-to-skin contact between
Maternal breastfeeding has been emphasized as an influential mother and child stimulates maternal hormonal responses such as
factor in early childhood development (e.g., Kramer et al., 2001, the production of prolactin and oxytocin, which may indirectly
2008) based on biological mechanisms through which maternal improve cognitive development (Del Bono and Rabe, 2012).
breastfeeding aids children's development. First, the composition In spite of this clear biological mechanism, empirical evidence
of breast milk is superior to that of formula. Breast milk contains for the effects of breastfeeding on children's cognitive development
long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, such as docosahexaenoic has been conflicting. A comprehensive review by the Agency for
acid (DHA) and arachidonic acid (AA), which form the major Healthcare Research and Quality (Ip et al., 2007) summarized 400
structures of neuronal membranes and play critical roles in nervous articles (out of 9000 abstracts) and found that breastfeeding has
system functioning by positively stimulating development of the few or small effects on children's cognitive ability. Many studies are
human brain (Fernstrom, 1999). Infants require sufficient amounts based on observational data, and it is difficult to infer causality due
of these acids during the first few months after birth (Clandinin to many potential confounding variables, such as duration of
breastfeeding, children's demographics and birth outcomes (such
as birth weight and gestational age), parental health-related
*
I thank Anna Aizer, Kenneth Chay, Hyojin Han, Hyunjoo Yang, and the editor behavior, socioeconomic status (SES), intelligence, and family
and four anonymous referees for valuable comments. Any remaining errors are characteristics (Anderson et al., 1999).
mine. This work was supported by the New Faculty Research Fund(1.160080) of
UNIST(Ulsan National Institute of Science & Technology).
Researchers use various empirical strategies to address the
* 50 UNIST-gil, Ulju-gun, Ulsan 44919, Republic of Korea. endogeneity issue. First, they estimate the probability of breast-
E-mail address: kanghyock.koh@unist.ac.kr. feeding using detailed information regarding the characteristics of

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.06.012
0277-9536/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
102 K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108

children, parents, and families. They then estimate the effects of 2. Method
breastfeeding on cognitive development by comparing children
with similar propensities of maternal breastfeeding (Jiang et al., 2.1. Data
2011; Belfield and Kelly, 2012; Borra et al., 2012; Rothstein, 2012;
Cesur et al., 2017). For example, Jian, Foster and Gibson-Davis To investigate the effects of maternal breastfeeding on children's
(2011) used the Panel Study of Income Dynamics in the U.S. and development, I used data from the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS),
found that the positive associations between breastfeeding initia- a longitudinal study of about 18,500 children who were born in
tion and children's cognitive development measured by the 2000 and 2001 in the United Kingdom. Specifically, I used data from
Woodcock Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery Revised (WJ-R) and four surveys (at ages 9 months, 3 years, 5 years, and 7 years) and
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) were excluded data for multiple births and children who were not living
significantly reduced when observational characteristics were with their biological mothers at the time of the first interview
controlled for. The estimated associations are between one-tenth (n ¼ 293). Since I used secondary observational data, ethics
and one-fifth of a standard deviation, which is small in magni- approval was not required.
tude. Second, some researchers use sibling or family fixed effects to I collected data on self-reported maternal breastfeeding status
control for unobserved family characteristics (Der et al., 2006; Rees from the first MCS survey, which included a question about the
and Sabia, 2009; Colen and Ramey, 2014; Cesur et al., 2017). Recent duration of maternal breastfeeding. Mothers reported the age of
research points out that maternal intelligence or IQ and home babies when breastfeeding stopped, as measured in days, weeks, or
environment explain breastfeeding status better than other SES months, which was used for calculating the duration of breast-
factors such as income or education, but few previous studies have feeding. It was not feasible to obtain data on exclusive breast-
accounted for these covariates (Der et al., 2006). Many studies using feeding from the MCS because survey questions related to infant
the propensity score approach or fixed effects analysis have found feeding were not exhaustive. Since the recommended duration of
that a positive association became smaller or disappeared once a breastfeeding in the UK was 4 months in 2000, I created a binary
large number of confounding factors were controlled for. Re- breastfeeding variable indicating whether children were breastfed
searchers pointed out these results as evidence for the endogeneity for at least 4 months. To study the effects of breastfeeding with
issue in breastfeeding (Belfield and Kelly, 2012; Cesur et al., 2017). different duration cutoffs, I examined the effects of initiation of
Third, economists use exogenous sources of variation in breast- breastfeeding. Given current policy recommendations [e.g., from
feeding status as instrumental variables to study the causal rela- the World Health Organization (WHO)], I also examined the effects
tionship between breastfeeding and children's cognitive of extended breastfeeding (i.e., for at least 6 months).
development (Del Bono and Rabe, 2012; Fitzsimons and Vera- To assess children's cognitive development, I used scores from
Hernandez, 2012). For example, infant feeding support is pro- six British Ability Scale (BAS) tests, which are used to measure the
vided by midwives and nurses in the U.K. Since staff working hours cognitive abilities of children aged 2.5e8 years old (Elliott et al.,
are reduced during the weekend, this support is reduced as well. 1997). Since these tests are individually administered by trained
Mothers are less exposed to support when they give birth on Friday interviewers, the scores provide a more accurate measure than
or Saturday. Fitzsimons and Vera-Hernandez (2012) use this insti- parents' self-reported measures (Fernald et al., 2009). Using the
tutional fact to estimate the effects of breastfeeding on children's age-adjusted scores from the MCS, I calculated average scores
cognitive development among low-income mothers and their within and across ages and created a summary index. I did this to
children. Finally, Kramer et al. (2001, 2008) studied the causal ef- address a concern that one null hypothesis could be rejected simply
fects of the Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial (PROBIT), because I tested it with multiple null hypotheses (Kling et al., 2007).
which randomly provided health care worker assistance for the This also yields a well-behaved continuous distribution of test
initiation and maintenance of breastfeeding, on breastfeeding scores (Fig. 1).
initiation and duration and consequences for children's health and I also collected information on maternal demographics,
cognitive abilities in Belarus. maternal prenatal characteristics, and spouse and family charac-
Previous literature mainly focused on the average effects of teristics as control variables for the propensity score approach:
breastfeeding on children's cognition. Many measures for cogni- mother's age at birth, race, marital status, and education; planned
tion, such as test scores, are continuously distributed. These dis-
tributions could provide a deeper understanding of the effects of
breastfeeding on children's cognition by evaluating the effects at
different quantiles in addition to the average effects. For example,
children at lower quantiles could benefit more from breastfeeding
than average children or children at higher quantiles, even though
the average effects of breastfeeding are small or insignificant based
on the existing literature. However, there is little knowledge
regarding the effects of breastfeeding on the distribution of
cognition. To fill this gap, I investigated the effects of maternal
breastfeeding on the distribution of children's cognitive ability in
this study. More specifically, I estimated the effects of breastfeeding
at different quantiles of cognitive test scores using a semi-
parametric quantile regression model. To address the endogeneity
of breastfeeding, I used the propensity score as the inverse proba-
bility (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983; Hirano et al., 2003). Based on
rich information about children's characteristics, cognitive test
scores, and maternal breastfeeding from the UK Millennium Cohort
Survey, I compared cognitive test scores among children with
similar propensities for maternal breastfeeding based on observed
parental characteristics. Fig. 1. Distribution of children's cognitive development. Data source: The MCS.
K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108 103

pregnancy; dummy variables for receiving any prenatal care and and interaction terms among them. I estimated the propensity
attending any prenatal classes; timing of initial care; dummy var- scores using the logit regression specification and used their inverse
iables for any complications during pregnancy (including bleeding probability weights to nonparametrically estimate the ATT (Hirano
or threatened miscarriage in early pregnancy; bleeding in later et al., 2003). I used b p ðXÞ/(1  b p ðXÞ) as a weight for the control
pregnancy; pregnancy diagnosed as twins, triplets, or more; group. Since the ATT may not be constant, I also estimated average
persistent vomiting; elevated blood pressure; eclampsia/pre- treatment effects (ATE) using 1/ b p ðXÞ and 1=ð1  b p ðXÞÞ as sampling
eclampsia or toxemia; urinary infection; diabetes; too much fluid weights for treatment and control groups, respectively. For statis-
around the baby; suspected slow growth of baby; and other sus- tical inferences, I calculated robust standard errors to correct for
pected problems) and labor (including breech birth, shoulder first, heteroscedasticity in test scores by breastfeeding status. The pro-
very long (or rapid) labor, fetal distress e heart rate sign, meconium pensity score approach has some advantages over the ordinary least
sign, and other complications); smoking status during pregnancy; squares (OLS) approach with many control variables. First, I can
working status during and after pregnancy; frequency of alcohol alleviate misspecification errors by not assuming any parametric
consumption; dummy variables for countries (England, Wales, relationship between breastfeeding and children's cognitive
Scotland, and Northern Ireland); father's age at birth, education development (Zhao, 2008). Second, I can address the curse of
level, and job status; dummy variables for joint job status of a dimensionality issue associated with OLS when many control var-
couple indicating if both in work, father (mother) in work but iables are included (Dehejia and Wahba, 1999) by summarizing all
mother (father) not in work, both not in work, father (mother) in information from the control variables into a single index (i.e., the
work or on leave but mother (father) not in work, and father propensity score). Third, I can graphically examine the nonpara-
(mother) in work but mother (father) is unknown; number of metric relationship between maternal breastfeeding and children's
household members and smokers in the household; and dummy cognitive development by using the propensity score.
variables for government support and income level. The mothers' Then I investigated the distributional impact of maternal
and fathers' education levels were categorized as NVQ level 1 to 5 breastfeeding using a semiparametric quantile regression model.
and overseas. I defined a dummy variable of low education level if Following Firpo (2007), I estimated the quantile treatment effect for
the NVQ level was 3 or lower to construct interactions with other quantile t using D b t ≡b
q 1;t  b
q 0;t (j ¼ 0; 1Þ; where b q j;t is defined as
covariates. Due to missing values in these control variables, I used PN
argminq i¼1 u b j $rt ðYi  qÞ, the check function rt ð$Þ at a real number
11,540 children in the main analysis.
c is rt ðcÞ ¼ c$ðt  1½c  0Þ; and u b j is the inverse probability of the
estimated propensity score for the ATT, which are
2.2. Empirical strategy
u
b1 ¼ T and u
b0 ¼ 1T where N is the sample size. I esti-
N,b
p ðXÞ N,ð1b
p ðXÞÞ
If T is the treatment indicator, Y1 is the outcome if treated mated the propensity scores using local logit regression. To esti-
(breastfed for more than 4 months), and Y0 is the outcome if un- mate treatment effects at different quantiles, the outcome variable
treated (breastfed for less than 4 months), then the average treat- should be continuous and well behaved without spikes or gaps,
ment effect on the treated (ATT) is EðY1 jT ¼ 1Þ  EðY0 jT ¼ 1Þ. Let which is again confirmed by Fig. 1.
the conditional probability of being treated based on observed
characteristics be the propensity score (Rosenbaum and Rubin,
1983), pðXÞ ¼ PrðTjX ¼ xÞ, where X is a vector of observed pre- 3. Empirical results
treatment characteristics such as maternal characteristics, parental
characteristics, and household environment. I include control var- Table 1 shows summary statistics by breastfeeding status. The
iables mentioned in the previous section. I also include polynomials first three columns show that in general, a statistically significant

Table 1
Baseline characteristics of control variables.

Variables No propensity score adjustment Propensity score adjustment

Breastfeeding Difference Breastfeeding Difference

Y N Y-N Y N Y-N

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Maternal age at birth 30.97 28.31 2.66*** 30.97 30.97 0.00


Low education 0.39 0.67 0.28*** 0.39 0.39 0.00
Low household income 0.38 0.58 0.20*** 0.38 0.38 0.00
Working (after birth) 0.56 0.53 0.03*** 0.56 0.56 0.00
White 0.82 0.88 0.06*** 0.82 0.80 0.02
Married at pregnancy 0.97 0.95 0.02*** 0.97 0.97 0.00
Planned pregnancy 0.69 0.60 0.09*** 0.69 0.69 0.00
Prenatal care (ever) 0.98 0.97 0.01*** 0.98 0.97 0.01
Prenatal class (ever) 0.43 0.34 0.09*** 0.43 0.43 0.00
Smoking during pregnancy 0.10 0.26 0.16*** 0.1 0.09 0.01
Any complications during pregnancy 0.35 0.39 0.04*** 0.35 0.36 0.01
Alcohol consumption 4.82 5.12 0.31*** 4.82 4.82 0.00
Any government support 0.21 0.37 0.16*** 0.21 0.21 0.00
Number of smokers in household 0.05 0.14 0.09*** 0.05 0.05 0.00
Spouse's age at delivery 33.63 31.06 2.57*** 33.63 33.64 0.01
Low education (spouse) 0.41 0.66 0.25*** 0.41 0.41 0.00

Note: I report differences in selected characteristics by maternal breastfeeding status. The first three columns show average values and their differences without any
adjustment. The last three columns show average values and their differences after the propensity score adjustment. I used b
p ðXÞ/(1  b
p ðXÞ) as a weight for the control group.
***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.10.
Data source: The MCS.
104 K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108

proportion of children who were breastfed for less than 4 months To show the effects of maternal breastfeeding graphically, I
came from families with lower SES. The last three columns show plotted the averages of children's cognitive test scores against the
average values and their differences after the propensity score estimated propensity scores separately in 20 equally sized cells by
adjustment. I used b p ðXÞ/(1  b
p ðXÞ) as a weight for the control treatment status (Fig. 4). The positive slopes indicate a general
group. Compared to the unadjusted differences, the adjusted dif- positive relationship between maternal breastfeeding and chil-
ferences are smaller and statistically insignificant, implying that dren's cognitive test scores. Among children with similar pro-
treatment and control groups appear to be comparable based on pensity scores, children who were breastfed generally had higher
observable characteristics. Fig. 2 shows the distributions of esti- test scores than their counterparts who were not breastfed. This
mated propensity scores by breastfeeding status, which are virtu- implies that maternal breastfeeding has a positive impact on
ally the same. This also implies that the treatment and control cognitive development among children with similar propensity
groups have similar observable characteristics conditional on the scores.
propensity score. In panel A and B of Table 2, I present the estimated ATT and ATE.
In Fig. 3, I present a plot of the estimated propensity scores Maternal breastfeeding increases cognitive test scores by 1.04 and
against the actual proportion of breastfeeding in 100 equally sized 0.94 points, respectively. Compared to the standard deviation of the
cells. The graph shows that the 100 cells are generally clustered dependent variable, the magnitudes of the impact of maternal
around the 45-degree line, which implies that the estimated pro- breastfeeding on children's cognitive development are around 10%
pensity score predicts the actual proportion of breastfeeding fairly of the standard deviation. The estimated effects of maternal
well. breastfeeding are also robust under other regression specifications.
In columns (1) and (2) of Appendix Table 1, instead of nonpara-
metric estimations with inverse probabilities, I used b p ðXÞ and b
p ðXÞ2
as parametric controls and included an interaction term between
maternal breastfeeding and b p ðXÞ to capture differential relation-
ships between the propensity of maternal breastfeeding and chil-
dren's cognitive test scores, respectively. Finally, in column (3), I
added the control variables used in the propensity score estimation
to the parametric specifications.
In panel C of Table 2, I present the quantile regression coefficient
values for cognitive test scores. The impact of maternal breast-
feeding is around 1.1 to 1.2 points for children below the 20th
percentile of the cognitive test score distribution and around 0.5
points for children above the 80th percentile of the cognitive test
score distribution. The impacts of maternal breastfeeding on chil-
dren's development are greater among children with lower
cognitive test scores.
Table 3 shows estimated effects of breastfeeding with different
cutoffs. I used ever breastfeeding status and extended breastfeed-
ing status (i.e., whether a child was breastfed for at least 6 months).
I used the same regression specification for the propensity score
Fig. 2. Distribution of estimated propensity score by breastfeeding status. Data source: estimation. Columns (1) and (2) show ATTs, ATEs, and quantile
The MCS. regression coefficient values. The estimated ATTs and ATEs on
cognitive test scores do not change. One thing to note is that the

Fig. 3. Estimated propensity score and actual probability of breastfeeding


(4 Months). Data source: The MCS. Note: This figure shows a plot of the average
estimated propensity scores against the actual probability of breastfeeding in 100 Fig. 4. Children's cognitive development as a function of estimated propensity score.
equal-sized cells. The red line is the 45-degree line. (For interpretation of the refer- Data source: The MCS. Note: This figure shows a plot of average cognitive test scores
ences to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this against estimated propensity scores in 20 equal-sized cells by maternal breastfeeding
article.) status.
K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108 105

Table 2 Table 3
Distributional effects of maternal breastfeeding on children's cognitive test scores. Distributional effects of different durations of maternal breastfeeding on children's
cognitive test scores.
Treatment status Breastfeeding  4 months
Treatment status Ever Breastfeeding Breastfeeding  6 months
A. ATT
1.04*** (1) (2)
(0.22)
A. ATT
B. ATE 1.26*** 0.96***
0.94*** (0.31) (0.24)
(0.27)
B. ATE
C. Quantiles 1.16*** 0.83**
0.1 1.08* (0.27) (0.33)
(0.62)
C. Quantiles
0.2 1.23***
0.1 1.33** 1.42*
(0.46)
(0.55) (0.82)
0.3 1.17***
0.2 1.5*** 1.58***
(0.38)
(0.50) (0.56)
0.4 1.23***
0.3 1.17*** 1.50***
(0.33)
(0.38) (0.43)
0.5 1.17***
0.4 1.17*** 1.00***
(0.29)
(0.37) (0.39)
0.6 1.00***
0.5 1.13*** 1.13***
(0.28)
(0.33) (0.36)
0.7 0.83***
0.6 1.33*** 1.10***
(0.26)
(0.31) (0.33)
0.8 0.50*
0.7 1.00*** 0.70**
(0.27)
(0.36) (0.29)
0.9 0.50*
0.8 1.00*** 0.27
(0.30)
(0.38) (0.29)
Mean (SD) of outcome 61.52 (10.05)
0.9 0.93** 0.33
Note: For treatment variable, I use the dummy variable if a child was breastfed for at (0.44) (0.35)
least 4 months. The cognitive test score measure is the average score of six British Mean (SD) of outcome 61.52 (10.05)
Ability Scales (BAS) for naming vocabulary, pattern construction, picture similarity,
Note: In each column, I estimate the effects of breastfeeding using different cutoffs.
and word reading. I used different sampling weights for ATTs and ATE:
For treatment variable, I use the dummy variable if a child was ever breastfed in
b
p ðXÞ/(1  b
p ðXÞ) as a sampling weight for the control group in panel A and C, and 1/
columns (1), and at least 6 months in column (2). The cognitive test score measure is
b
p ðXÞ and 1=ð1  b p ðXÞÞ as sampling weights for treatment and control groups in
the average score of six British Ability Scales (BAS) for naming vocabulary, pattern
Panel B. Robust standard errors are calculated and presented in the parentheses.
construction, picture similarity, and word reading. I used different sampling weights
***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and *p < 0.10.
for ATTs and ATE: b p ðXÞ/(1  b
p ðXÞ) as a sampling weight for the control group in
Data source: The MCS.
panel A and C, and 1/ bp ðXÞ and 1=ð1  b
p ðXÞÞ as sampling weights for treatment and
control groups in Panel B. Robust standard errors are calculated and presented in the
distributional effects become larger for extended duration of parentheses. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and *p < 0.10.
breastfeeding. The impacts of ever breastfeeding for those below Data source: The MCS.

the 20th percentile are about one and a half times larger than for
those above the 80th percentile. The impacts of extended maternal slightly smaller than my baseline regression results: the effects are
breastfeeding for those below the 20th percentile are about five around 0.9 and 0.8 points, respectively, which again equals
times larger than for those above the 80th percentile. approximately 10% of the standard deviation of the cognitive score
Table 4 shows results of robustness checks. To begin with, I distribution. Panel C of both columns shows that the impacts of
included multiple birth sample because it has different character- maternal breastfeeding on children's development are still greater
istics compared to the baseline sample (Appendix Table A2). Panels among children with lower cognitive test scores: the impact of
A, B, and C of column (1) show ATT, ATE, and quantile regression maternal breastfeeding is around 1.2 (1.0) points for children below
coefficient values. The estimated effects of breastfeeding did not the 20th percentile of the cognitive test score distribution and
change much from the baseline results. Next, the propensity score around 0.3 (0.3) points for children above the 80th percentile of the
approach could not infer the causal effects of breastfeeding because cognitive test score distribution.
of unmeasured confounding (Nickel, 2015). To examine any bias
due to this, I included variables about children and maternal health 4. Discussion
that were not accounted for from the baseline propensity score
estimation. First, I used birth weights and gestational age as mea- Previous literature focused on the average effects of breast-
sures for children's health because children's birth weights and feeding on children's cognition. This study first examined the
birth outcomes are important determinants of children's cognitive distributional impacts of maternal breastfeeding on children's
development (Almond and Currie, 2011b) and may affect maternal development using a semiparametric quantile regression model. I
breastfeeding decisions (Cunha and Heckman, 2007). Children with found that the effects of breastfeeding on children's test scores
better birth outcomes as measured by birth weight and gestational were greater among children with lower test scores. The effects
age are more likely to be breastfed (Appendix Table A3). Second, were around 12e13% of the standard deviation, which is considered
maternal health status is also considered an important factor for small in magnitude according to Cohen's effect size rule (Cohen,
breastfeeding (Jiang et al., 2011). I used self-evaluated health status 1992) and the previous literature (Ip et al., 2007; Jiang et al.,
and a dummy variable of having any chronic disease from the first 2011). However, the effects could become larger at the population
wave of the MCS as measures of maternal health. Since there was level as the disparity in educational achievement becomes larger
no direct information on maternal health status during breast- within a society (Rose, 1985). Given evidence that breastfeeding is
feeding, I used those variables as proxies. Panels A and B of columns associated with a reduction in several health risks among children
(2) and (3) show that the magnitudes of the ATTs and ATEs are and mothers (Ip et al., 2007; Eidelman et al., 2012), the benefits of
106 K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108

Table 4 An economic theory may provide a possible explanation for the


Robustness checks. main findings of this study. Let's assume that children's human
Including Including birth Including both birth capital production function is concave against maternal breast-
multiple births outcomes outcomes and feeding. Low SES children usually have a low chance of being
maternal health breastfed and so achieve low test scores based on the underlying
(1) (2) (3) biological mechanism. Because they are less likely to be breastfed,
A. ATT marginal returns on breastfeeding could be larger than those in
1.06*** 0.90*** 0.91*** higher SES children due to the concavity of the human capital
(0.22) (0.23) (0.23) production function. These results have a policy implication for the
B. ATE promotion of breastfeeding among individuals with lower SES.
0.98*** 0.81*** 0.79*** Significant SES-related disparities in children's academic perfor-
(0.26) (0.26) (0.27) mance are well established in the UK (Banerjee, 2016). Government
C. Quantiles programs such as Sure Start have been introduced to narrow these
0.1 1.08* 1.00 0.83 gaps (Melhuish et al., 2008). Since a high proportion of children
(0.62) (0.65) (0.65) with lower test scores generally come from families with low SES
0.2 1.23*** 1.17*** 1.00**
(0.46) (0.45) (0.46)
and since there are observed SES-related disparities in maternal
0.3 1.17*** 1.17*** 1.00*** breastfeeding incidence and duration, government policies such as
(0.38) (0.39) (0.39) the UNICEF UK Baby Friendly Initiative aims at promoting breast-
0.4 1.23*** 1.17*** 1.16*** feeding may reduce SES-related gaps in children's academic
(0.33) (0.33) (0.34)
development. However, there are at least two reasons why this
0.5 1.17*** 1.00*** 0.83***
(0.29) (0.30) (0.30) policy implication should be interpreted with caution. First, the
0.6 1.00*** 1.00*** 0.92*** policy implication of this study is based on results with strong
(0.28) (0.28) (0.28) statistical assumptions. It is hard to draw causal inferences
0.7 0.83*** 0.73*** 0.73*** regarding the effects of breastfeeding. Second, the results may not
(0.26) (0.26) (0.26)
0.8 0.50* 0.33 0.33
be generalizable. Public agencies such as the American Academy of
(0.27) (0.27) (0.27) Pediatrics and the WHO recommend that mothers exclusively
0.9 0.50* 0.50* 0.50* breastfeed their infants for at least 6 months and ideally continue to
(0.30) (0.30) (0.30) breastfeed beyond 6 months. Under the Affordable Care Act in the
Mean (SD) of 61.52 (10.05)
United States, the initiation and extended duration of maternal
outcome
breastfeeding are promoted. However, this study is based on data
Note: In each column, I estimate the effects of breastfeeding using different speci- from the UK. The results should therefore be cautiously applied to
fications. I include multiple births in column (1), and children's birth weights and
gestational age in column (2), and maternal health status as well as birth weights
other countries owing to significant differences in health care and
and gestational age in column (3). For treatment variable, I use the dummy variable other social policy.
if a child was breastfed for at least 4 months. The cognitive test score measure is the As an avenue for future research, the distributional effects on
average score of six British Ability Scales (BAS) for naming vocabulary, pattern other benefits of breastfeeding, such as the health benefits for
construction, picture similarity, and word reading. I used different sampling weights
children and mothers, could also be considered. Previous literature
for ATTs and ATE: b p ðXÞ/(1  bp ðXÞ) as a sampling weight for the control group in
panel A and C, and 1/ b p ðXÞ and 1=ð1  b p ðXÞÞ as sampling weights for treatment and has mainly focused on the average effects of breastfeeding, but
control groups in Panel B. Robust standard errors are calculated and presented in the there is little evidence of the distributional effects on children's and
parentheses. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and *p < 0.10. maternal health. To draw a more convincing causal inference on the
Data source: The MCS. effects of breastfeeding, it would be useful to exploit a natural
experiment that quasi-randomizes breastfeeding status or
breastfeeding could be larger when considering such health ben- duration.
efits (Cesur et al., 2017).
The method and data suffer from some limitations. First, the
Appendix
propensity score approach is based on the ignorability assumption
that there is no other confounding factor once a large number of
covariates are controlled for (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983). This
assumption is too strong to infer causal effects of breastfeeding due Table A1
to unmeasured or unmeasurable confounding (Nickel, 2015). As an Effects of maternal breastfeeding on children's cognitive test scores.
example of unmeasured confounding, the MCS does not provide
(1) (2) (3)
information on mothers’ intelligence, such as IQ or test scores, or
home environment scores, which are considered important con- Breastfed  4 months 1.022*** 1.032** 1.090***
(0.207) (0.496) (0.214)
founding factors (Der et al., 2006; Belfield and Kelly, 2012; Cesur
et al., 2017). In addition, it is almost impossible from most obser- Parametric control of bp ðXÞ Y Y Y
vational data to obtain information on unmeasurable factors such Breastfed4mon b p ðXÞ Y
Control variables (XÞ Y
as maternal genes. Second, the data do not provide enough infor-
mation to construct an exclusive breastfeeding variable. Since the Data source: The MCS.
treatment group consisted of children who were breastfed at all, Note: Breastfed 4months indicates whether a child was breastfed for at least 4
months. The cognitive test score measure is the average of six British Ability Scales
this may underestimate the actual benefits of exclusive breast- (BAS) for naming vocabulary, pattern construction, picture similarity, and word
feeding (Jiang et al., 2011). Finally, breastfeeding duration was reading. In column (1), I use b p ðXÞ and b p ðXÞ2 as controls instead of using sampling
based on maternal recall. This implies that there may be errors in weights. In column (2), I add Breastfed4mon  b p ðXÞ to the regression specification of
treatment status and could cause additional bias in the estimates. column (1). In column (3), I add b p ðXÞ and bp ðXÞ2 and the control variables that were
used to estimate bp ðXÞ to the regression specification of column (1). Robust standard
Since it is unclear if this type of error is random or systematic
errors are calculated and presented in the parentheses. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and
(Promislow et al., 2005), it is hard to see the exact direction of the *p < 0.10.
bias.
K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108 107

Table A2
Characteristics of baseline and excluded sample.

Variables Baseline Excluded Excluded - Baseline

(1) (2) (3)

Maternal age at birth 28.29 30.81 2.52***


Low education 0.61 0.56 0.05*
Low household income 0.61 0.56 0.05
Working (after birth) 0.47 0.44 0.03
White 0.84 0.89 0.05**
Married at pregnancy 0.81 0.88 0.07***
Planned pregnancy 0.54 0.63 0.09***
Prenatal care (ever) 0.96 0.97 0.01
Prenatal class (ever) 0.34 0.36 0.02
Smoking during pregnancy 0.25 0.18 0.07***
Any complications during pregnancy 0.38 0.48 0.1***
Alcohol consumption 5.14 4.87 0.27
Any government support 0.43 0.43 0.00
Number of smokers in household 0.14 0.09 0.05**
Spouse's age at delivery 31.88 33.60 1.72***
Low education (spouse) 0.40 0.37 0.03

Data source: The MCS.


Note: I used the same variables that I used in Table 1. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and *p < 0.10.

Table A3
Maternal breastfeeding and children's birth outcomes.

Birth weight (grams) Pr (Birth weight  2500 g) Pr (Birth weight  1500 g) Gestational age (days) Pr (Gestational age  37 weeks)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Breastfed  4 months 77.607*** 0.030*** 0.005** 1.687*** 0.019***


(12.932) (0.006) (0.002) (0.286) (0.005)

Average of control group 3199.442 0.094 0.013 275.617 0.069

Observations 12,788 12,796 12,796 12,703 12,703


R-squared 0.005 0.003 0.001 0.004 0.002

Data source: The MCS.


Note: Breastfed  4 months indicates whether a child was breastfed for at least 4 months. I used b
p ðXÞ=1  b
p ðXÞ as a sampling weight for the control group. Robust standard
errors are calculated and presented in parentheses. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, and *p < 0.10.

1053e1062.
Del Bono, E., Rabe, B., 2012. Breastfeeding and Child Cognitive Outcomes: Evidence
from a Hospital-based Breastfeeding Support Policy (No. 2012-29). ISER
Working Paper Series, 2012-29. http://hdl.handle.net/10419/91672.
References Deoni, S.C.L., Dean III, D.C., Piryatinksy, I., O'Muircheartaigh, J., Waskiewicz, N.,
Lehman, K., Han, M., Dirks, H., 2013. Breastfeeding and early white matter
Almond, D., Currie, J., 2011a. Human capital development before age five. Handb. development: a cross sectional study. NeuroImage 82, 77e86.
Labor Econ. 4, 1315e1486. Der, G., Batty, D., Deary, I.J., 2006. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children:
Almond, D., Currie, J., 2011b. Killing me softly: the fetal origins hypothesis. J. Econ. prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. Br. Med. J. 333,
Perspect. 25 (3), 153e172. 945e948.
Anderson, J.W., Johnstone, B.M., Remley, D.T., 1999. Breast-feeding and cognitive Eidelman, A.I., Schanler, R.J., Johnston, M., Landers, S., Noble, L., Szucs, K.,
development: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 70 (4), 525e535. Viehmann, L., 2012. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Pediatrics 129
Banerjee, P.A., 2016. A systematic review of factors linked to poor academic per- (3), e827ee841.
formance of disadvantaged students in science and maths in schools. Cogent Elliott, C.D., Smith, P., McCulloch, K., 1997. British Ability Scales (BAS II): Technical
Educ. 3 (1), 1178441. Manual, second ed. NFER-Nelson, London, UK.
Becker, G., 1981. A Treatise on the Family. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Fernald, L.C., Kariger, P., Engle, P., Raikes, A., 2009. Examining Early Child Devel-
Belfield, C.R., Kelly, I.R., 2012. The benefits of breastfeeding across the early years of opment in Low-income Countries. The World Bank, Washington, DC.
childhood. J. Hum. Cap. 6 (3), 251e277. Fernstrom, J.D., 1999. Effects of dietary polyunsaturated fatty acids on neuronal
Borra, C., Iacovou, M., Sevilla, A., 2012. The effect of breastfeeding on children's function. Lipids 34 (2), 161e169.
cognitive and noncognitive development. Labour Econ. 19 (4), 496e515. Firpo, S., 2007. Efficient semiparametric estimation of quantile treatment effects.
Cesur, R., Sabia, J.J., Kelly, I.R., Yang, M., 2017. The effect of breastfeeding on young Econometrica 75 (1), 259e276.
adult wages: new evidence from the add health. Rev. Econ. Househ. 15 (1), Fitzsimons, E., Vera-Hernandez, M., 2012. The Causal Effects of Breastfeeding on
25e51. Children's Development. Institute for Fiscal Studies, London, UK.
Clandinin, M.T., Chappell, J.E., Heim, T., Swyer, P.R., Chance, G.W., 1981. Fatty acid Heckman, J.J., 2006. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvan-
utilization in perinatal de novo synthesis of tissues. Early Hum. Dev. 5 (4), taged children. Science 312 (5782), 1900e1902.
355e366. Hirano, K., Imbens, G.W., Ridder, G., 2003. Efficient estimation of average treatment
Cohen, J., 1992. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112 (1), 155. effects using the estimated propensity score. Econometrica 71 (4), 1161e1189.
Colen, C.G., Ramey, D.M., 2014. Is breast truly best? Estimating the effects of Ip, S., Chung, M., Raman, G., Chew, P., Magula, N., DeVine, D., et al., 2007. Breast-
breastfeeding on long-term child health and wellbeing in the United States feeding and Maternal and Infant Health Outcomes in Developed Countries.
using sibling comparisons. Soc. Sci. Med. 109, 55e65. Evidence Report/technology Assessment No. 153 (Prepared by Tufts-New En-
Cunha, F., Heckman, J.J., 2007. The technology of skill formation. Am. Econ. Rev. 97 gland Medical Center Evidence-based Practice Center, under Contract No. 290-
(2), 31. 02-0022). AHRQ Publication No. 07-E007. Agency for Healthcare Research and
Cunha, F., Heckman, J.J., Schennach, S.M., 2010. Estimating the technology of Quality, Rockville, MD.
cognitive and noncognitive skill formation. Econometrica 78 (3), 883e931. Jiang, M., Foster, E.M., Gibson-Davis, C.M., 2011. Breastfeeding and the child
Dehejia, R.H., Wahba, S., 1999. Causal effects in nonexperimental studies: reevalu- cognitive outcomes: a propensity score matching approach. Maternal Child
ating the evaluation of training programs. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 94 (448), Health J. 15 (8), 1296e1307.
108 K. Koh / Social Science & Medicine 187 (2017) 101e108

Kling, J.R., Liebman, J.B., Katz, L.F., 2007. Experimental analysis of neighborhood Nickel, N.C., 2015. Look before you leap: can we draw causal conclusions from
effects. Econometrica 75 (1), 83e119. breastfeeding research? J. Hum. Lactation 31 (2), 209e212.
Kramer, M.S., Chalmers, B., Hodnett, E.D., Sevkovskaya, Z., Dzikovich, I., Shapiro, S., Promislow, J.H., Gladen, B.C., Sandler, D.P., 2005. Maternal recall of breastfeeding
Collet, J.P., Vanilovich, I., Mezen, I., Ducruet, T., Shishko, G., 2001. Promotion of duration by elderly women. Am. J. Epidemiol. 161 (3), 289e296.
breastfeeding intervention trial (PROBIT): a randomized trial in the Republic of Rees, D.I., Sabia, J.J., 2009. The effect of breast feeding on educational attainment:
Belarus. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 285 (4), 413e420. evidence from sibling data. J. Hum. Cap. 3 (1), 43e72.
Kramer, M.S., Aboud, F., Mironova, E., Vanilovich, I., Platt, R.W., Matush, L., Rose, G., 1985. Sick individuals and sick populations. Int. J. Epidemiol. 14 (1), 32e38.
Igumnov, S., et al., 2008. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new Rosenbaum, P.R., Rubin, D.B., 1983. The central role of the propensity score in
evidence from a large randomized trial. Archives General Psychiatry 65 (5), observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika 70 (1), 41e55.
578e584. Rothstein, D.S., 2012. Breastfeeding and children's early cognitive outcomes. Rev.
Melhuish, E., Belsky, J., Leyland, A.H., Barnes, J., National Evaluation of Sure Start Econ. Statistics 95 (3), 919e931.
Research Team, 2008. Effects of fully-established Sure Start Local Programmes Zhao, Z., 2008. Sensitivity of propensity score methods to the specifications. Econ.
on 3-year-old children and their families living in England: a quasi- Lett. 98 (3), 309e319.
experimental observational study. Lancet 372 (9650), 1641e1647.

You might also like