You are on page 1of 10

Acute Threat to the Social Self: Shame, Social Self-esteem, and

Cortisol Activity
TARA L. GRUENEWALD, PHD, MARGARET E. KEMENY, PHD, NAJIB AZIZ, MD, AND JOHN L. FAHEY, MD
Objective: Our Social Self Preservation Theory asserts that situations which threaten the “social self” (ie, one’s social value or
standing) elicit increased feelings of low social worth (eg, shame), decrements in social self-esteem, and increases in cortisol, a
hormone released by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. To test our theoretical premise, cognitive, emotional, and physio-
logical responses to the performance of laboratory stressor tasks were compared in participants who performed these tasks in the
presence or absence of social-self threat. Methods: Pre- and poststressor emotion, self-esteem, heart rate, blood pressure, and
salivary cortisol were compared in 81 participants randomly assigned to complete speech and mental arithmetic stress tasks with
social evaluation present (n ⫽ 41) or absent (n ⫽ 40). Results: As hypothesized, participants in the social evaluation condition
exhibited greater increases in shame and greater decrements in social self-esteem. Other psychological states (eg, anxiety,
performance self-esteem) did not show differential changes as a function of the social context. Salivary cortisol increased in social
evaluation condition participants but did not increase in participants who performed the same tasks in the absence of social
evaluation. Cortisol increases were greater in participants who experienced greater increases in shame and greater decreases in
social self-esteem under social-self threat. Conclusion: Threat to the social self is an important elicitor of shame experience,
decreases in social self-esteem and cortisol increases under demanding performance conditions. Cortisol changes may be
specifically tied to the experience of emotions and cognitions reflecting low self-worth in this context. Key words: stress, cortisol,
shame, emotion, self-esteem.

DBP ⫽ diastolic blood pressure; SBP ⫽ systolic blood pressure; tinent to a primary human goal: that of achieving and main-
HR ⫽ heart rate; SOC-EVAL ⫽ social evaluation condition; NON- taining a positive “social self.”
EVAL ⫽ non-evaluation condition; ABS ⫽ Affect Balance Scale; Humans are argued to be social animals that possess fun-
SSGS ⫽ State Shame and Guilt Scale damental motivations to belong and be accepted by others
(2– 4), and the status of one’s social self is argued to be an
INTRODUCTION
important determinant of the distribution of social and phys-

W e propose that threats to the social self, or situations


which threaten to demean one’s social image or stand-
ing, engender a specific set of psychological and physiological
ical resources (5,6). Thus, concerns with achieving or main-
taining a positive social self may be an important human
motive that is supported by specific psychological and phys-
reactions. Our position, which we have labeled the Social Self iological systems that provide signaling (ie, “the social self is
Preservation Theory (1), is that these events elicit increases in under threat”) and mobilizing (ie, initiation of psychobiolog-
feelings of low social worth (eg, shame, humiliation), decre- ical activity to help deal with the threat) functions.
ments in social self-esteem, and increases in cortisol, a hor-
mone of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) system.
Psychological Responses to Social-Self Threats
We define threats to the social self as situations that contain
the potential to devalue one’s social self by calling into Numerous social psychological, evolutionary, and emotion
question abilities, competencies, or traits on which a positive theories converge to suggest that situations which threaten the
social image is based (eg, evaluative situations, such as giving social self should elicit the experience of a specific class of
a presentation at work or school), or situations characterized emotions, those that can broadly be referred to as feelings of
by potential or explicit rejection. We assert that such situations low social worth: shame, humiliation, embarrassment, etc
are provocative because they contain social information per- (7–11). A number of theorists have asserted that shame is the
prototypical emotion experienced in response to devaluations
of the social self (6 – 8,12–14). Phenomenological studies also
From the University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA (T.L.G., support this hypothesis, as people have reported that previous
N.A., J.L.F.); and the University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, experience of shame was characterized by a sense of social
CA (M.E.K.).
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Tara L. Gruenewald, De- isolation, feeling small and inferior, and a pressure to hide
partment of Medicine/Geriatrics, UCLA David Geffen School of Medicine, from others (15,16). To date, however, there is little experi-
10945 LeConte Ave, Suite 2339, Los Angeles, CA, 90095-1687. E-mail: mental research on this topic; most of the previous research
tlgruen@ucla.edu or tgruenewald@mednet.ucla.edu
Received for publication December 23, 2003; revision received June 12, employed imagined scenarios thought to elicit shame emo-
2004. tions or recall of past situations in which these emotions were
This research was supported by a National Research Service Award experienced (15–18).
(MH12703) from the National Institute of Mental Health, an American
Psychological Association Dissertation Research Award, a Society for the Acute threats to the social self are also likely to affect one’s
Psychological Study of Social Issues Grants-In-Aid Award, and a UCLA thoughts and opinions of oneself. This may be especially true
Graduate Division Dissertation Research Award supporting the first author. for the social component of state self-esteem: thoughts regard-
Tara L. Gruenewald is currently in the Department of Medicine/Geriatrics,
Najib Aziz is in the Department of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine, and ing the positive or negative state of the social self (eg, feeling
John L. Fahey is in the Department of Microbiology and Immunology, at the inferior, feeling as if one has made a poor social impression).
UCLA David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Leary and associates (19,20) have demonstrated that imagined
Angeles. Margaret E. Kemeny is in the Health Psychology Program at the
University of California, San Francisco. or actual social rejection in the laboratory leads to lowered
DOI: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143639.61693.ef overall state self-esteem and more negative ratings on a num-

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004) 915


0033-3174/04/6606-0915
Copyright © 2004 by the American Psychosomatic Society
T. L. GRUENEWALD et al.

ber of emotions (eg, anxious, upset, happy, calm, ashamed) which the participant was the target of a negative social
and self-relevant adjectives (eg, self-confident, superior, ef- comparison, led to greater cortisol reactivity as compared with
fective, smart). However, their postrejection assessments have paradigms in which social-evaluative threat was absent or
not clearly distinguished shame from other emotional re- minimized. This meta-analytic review provides strong support
sponses or effects on social vs. nonsocial (eg, performance, for the hypothesis that factors that threaten the social self
appearance) dimensions of self-esteem, which are goals of the within laboratory stressor paradigms may be potent elicitors of
present investigation. HPA responses. To date, however, we know of no acute stress
investigation that has compared cortisol reactivity to a stressor
Physiological Responses to Social-Self Threats task that manipulated the presence or absence of social-eval-
One physiological system that we believe may be provoked uative threat. The present investigation provides an opportu-
by threats to the social self is the HPA axis, a hormonal system nity to experimentally validate the conclusions drawn by the
that regulates metabolism and energy production and is meta-analysis and our theoretical premise that the HPA sys-
thought to be centrally involved in regulating physiological tem will be activated by threat to the social self.
responses to stress (21–24). A large body of human and
animal literature indicates increased levels of HPA hormones The Present Investigation
(eg, corticotrophin releasing hormone, adrenocorticotrophic In the present investigation, we tested the hypothesis that a
hormone, and cortisol/corticosterone) under physically and situation that had the potential to threaten the social self would
psychologically threatening conditions. Narrative reviews be more likely to elicit increases in shame, decrements in
suggest that certain characteristics of situations may be more social self-esteem, and increases in cortisol, than a situation in
likely to activate the HPA system, including situations char- which threat to the social self was minimized. To do this, we
acterized by novelty, uncontrollability, unpredictability, and manipulated participants’ performance of “stressful” activities
ego involvement (24 –27). (speech and math tasks) so that half of the individuals per-
Threat to the social self as an important elicitor of the HPA formed these activities in the presence of an unfriendly, eval-
axis has received less attention in the stress literature, but uative audience (“social-evaluative condition,” SOC-EVAL),
social challenges, especially those marked by negative evalu- to create a situation in which participants’ social selves might
ations from others and loss of status, have been identified as be threatened, whereas the other half performed the demand-
important elicitors of HPA stress responses (see 27). Acute ing tasks in private (“nonevaluative condition,” NON-EVAL)
and chronic social rejection, important forms of social-self so that potential threat to the social self was minimized.
threat, have been found to correlate with cortisol levels. Engaging in the tasks in either condition required cognitive
Stroud and associates (28) demonstrated that a social rejection resources (participants had to prepare their speech, solve dif-
experience in the laboratory led to short-term increases in ficult math problems), demanded active performance (oral
salivary cortisol, especially in females. Similar associations delivery of speech, solve math problems under time pressure),
are also found in more naturalistic environments; children and was characterized by uncontrollability (math problems
socially rejected at school have higher basal cortisol levels became increasingly difficult and impossible to solve),1 char-
than their accepted or even neglected peers (29). Negative, acteristics of situations typically thought to be “stressful”
unsupportive social environments are argued to contribute to (25–28). The manipulated factor that differed between the two
alterations in HPA functioning (30); however, threat to the stress conditions was the presence or absence of direct social-
social self as an important component of this association has evaluative threat.
received less attention. Nonhuman animal research also indi- We were interested in whether increases in cortisol would
cates that levels of and changes in HPA hormones are sensi- be more likely to occur in the social-evaluative as compared
tive to social threats, including subordinate social status or the with the nonevaluative stressor; however, we also included
threat of status loss (important nonhuman animal forms of measures of heart rate (HR) and blood pressure to assess
threat to the “social self;” eg, 31–34). changes in arousal that we believed would occur in both stress
Threat to the social self may also play an important role in conditions.
short-term cortisol elevations witnessed in response to acute
stressor experience in the laboratory. Public speaking tasks METHODS
and activities that tax cognitive resources, such as the combi- Participants
nation of speech and mental arithmetic tasks included in the Eighty-one undergraduate college students (34 male, 47 female) partici-
well-known Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; 35), have been pated in the experiment. Participants ranged in age from 17 to 22 years (M ⫽
18.67). The majority of participants identified their ethnicity as Asian-Amer-
shown to elicit transient elevations in cortisol in the labora- ican (44.9%) or Caucasian (32.1%), with smaller numbers of participants
tory. However, the specific characteristics of acute stressor identifying as Black/African American (1.3%), Latino/a, Chicano/a (6.4%),
paradigms that engage the HPA axis are unclear. In a meta- Filipino/a (3.8%) or “Other” (11.5%). Participants for this experiment were
analytic review of 208 laboratory acute stress studies, Dick- drawn from a larger investigation examining relationships between social
erson and Kemeny (36) demonstrated that stressor paradigms
characterized by social-evaluative threat, such as stressful 1
In this context, uncontrollability refers to the fact that behavior will have
situations in which an evaluative audience was present or in little, if any, effect on the outcome of the situation.

916 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004)


SOCIAL-SELF THREAT

Figure 1. Timeline of session activities and psychological and physiological measurements.

status and mental health in college dormitories. The results of this larger study (Dinamap Model 8146, Critikon Corporation, Tampa, FL). Measurements
are not reported here. were taken oscillometrically by placing an occluding cuff over the brachial
All participants were screened for and excluded from participation if they artery of the participant’s nondominant arm. Readings were taken every 2
possessed health conditions or engaged in behaviors that might affect the minutes during specific phases of the experimental session (see below) and
biological measures assessed in this study, including a number of health averaged to produce a single value for each phase.
conditions (eg, Cushing’s disease, high blood pressure) and use of prescrip- Salivary cortisol was assessed at four points throughout the experimental
tion (eg, oral contraceptives) and nonprescription (eg, marijuana, regular session (see below) with the Salivette device (Sartstedt, Rommelsdorf, Ger-
tobacco use) drugs. Participants were asked not to engage in strenuous many). This device is essentially a cotton roll which participants were asked
exercise, drink alcohol, or smoke on the day of their appointment,2 and to not to place under their tongue for 3 minutes. The saturated roll was placed into
consume dairy products, caffeine, or eat 1 hour before the session. a sealed, plastic container, refrigerated for no longer than 16 hours, and then
Participants were paid $20 or given psychology course experimental centrifuged to extract saliva. Samples were frozen at ⫺80°C until assayed.
credit, whichever they preferred, for their participation in the study. Cortisol was measured using a high sensitivity salivary cortisol immunoassay
kit (Salimetrics, State College, PA; sensitivity ⬍0.007 ␮g/dl). Values are
Measures reported in ␮g/dl.
Questionnaire Measures
Measures of emotion and self-esteem were administered before and after
the stress tasks (see below). For the prestress administration participants were Design and Procedure
asked to answer each question in response to how they felt during the last few Participants reported individually for a 90-minute laboratory session
minutes, and for the poststress administration how they felt during last ten scheduled between the hours of 2:30 and 7:00 PM to control for diurnal
minutes, including during the speech and math tasks. variations in cortisol. A timeline of major study activities is provided in
The Affects Balance Scale (ABS; 37), a 40-item measure of positive and Figure 1. During the first 20 minutes, participants provided informed consent,
negative state emotion experience (1 ⫽ never to 5 ⫽ always), was used to completed prestress questionnaires, and rested quietly. Baseline HR and BP
measure state emotion. Three additional emotion items (humiliated, self- readings were taken during the last 10 minutes of the rest period, and the first
conscious, embarrassed) were added to more carefully assess shame-related saliva sample was taken immediately after (“baseline” measures).
emotions. Pre- and poststress mean scores for the anxiety (nervous, timid, Participants were then given tape-recorded instructions in which they
tense, anxious, afraid; internal reliability coefficient Cronbach’s ␣pre ⫽ 0.80, were instructed that they would be asked to perform a set of challenging and
␣post ⫽ 0.80) and shame (ashamed, humiliated, self-conscious, and embar- demanding laboratory tasks, which would include having to give a 5-minute
rassed; ␣pre ⫽ 0.86, ␣post ⫽ 0.86) subscales were examined in analyses. The speech and then perform a 5-minute mental arithmetic task on a computer.
shame subscale of the State Shame and Guilt Scale (SSGS; 38) was used as The tasks represent a modified version of the TSST (35). In the speech,
a second measure of pre- and poststress shame experience. Participants participants were asked to explain why they would be good candidates for a
responded to five statements (eg, “I want to sink into the floor and disappear”) job as a member of a focus group in which they would have to work
on a five-point scale (1 ⫽ not feeling this way at all to 5 ⫽ feeling this way effectively with other students to devise solutions to problems typically faced
very strongly; ␣pre ⫽ 0.69, ␣post ⫽ 0.83). by college students. A procedure borrowed from Pruessner et al. (40) was
The social and performance subscales of the State Self-Esteem Scale (39) used for the arithmetic task. During the 5-minute task, subjects were presented
were also administered before and after the performance tasks. Respondents with addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division problems and were
rated their agreement with 14 statements on a five-point scale (1 ⫽ not at all given 5 seconds to select the correct answer from a range of 1-digit choices.
to 5 ⫽ extremely; performance ␣pre ⫽ 0.85, ␣post ⫽ 0.88; social ␣pre ⫽ 0.86, The task became increasingly difficult and eventually reached a point in
␣post ⫽ 0.91). which participants could not solve the problems in the allotted time. An
A task rating questionnaire consisting of three questions assessing feelings annoying noise played during display of the problems, and wrong answers or
of difficulty, threat, and challenge felt during the activities (seven-point scale, failure to answer in the time allotted was followed by a loud buzz.
1 ⫽ extremely poor/not at all true to 7 ⫽ extremely well/very true) was In the SOC-EVAL condition of the study, participants were informed that
administered after the performance tasks. Participants in the social stress they would perform the activities in front of a panel of evaluators who would
condition were also asked to rate how much they liked the panel of evaluators, judge their performance, while participants in the nonevaluative (NON-EVAL)
how much they thought the panelists liked and accepted them, how effective condition were informed that they would perform these activities while alone
they believed they were in convincing panelists that they should get the job in the room.
during the speech portion of the activity, and how well they thought the HR and BP readings were taken while the participants listened to task
panelists would rate their performance overall (see below for protocol de- instructions (“task instruction” measure). The participants then had 10 min-
scription), using a 7-point scale (1 ⫽ not at all/very poor to 7 ⫽ very
utes to mentally prepare their speech. A second saliva sample was taken at the
much/very excellent).
end of this preparation period (“stress anticipation” measure).
The participant was then taken to a second area of the room and asked to
Physiological Measures stand in front of a microphone. In the SOC-EVAL condition, the panel of
Measures of HR, systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pres- evaluators was already seated behind a table facing the microphone. The panel
sure (DBP) were assessed with a Critikon automatic sphygmomanometer of evaluators consisted of two individuals of approximately the same age as
the participant who were trained to respond in a nonaccepting and critical
2
Although habitual smokers were excluded from participation, participants manner (eg, nonsmiling, stony faces, writing comments on their clipboard).
were directed not to smoke on the day of their appointment in the event that The participant was told that the panelists were present to judge his/her
an occasional smoker might smoke on the day of the appointment. All performance and ability to communicate ideas successfully in a social situa-
participants indicated that they did not smoke on the day of their appointment. tion. The experimenter introduced the participant to the panelists, turned on an

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004) 917


T. L. GRUENEWALD et al.

audiotape recorder,3 demonstrated how the experimenter would monitor RESULTS


sound from the other room to ensure they spoke for the entire 5 minutes,4 and Baseline Differences
then asked the participant to wait until she left the room to begin his/her
speech. The experimenter was not present during the speech so that she would SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL condition participants did
not act as an additional source of social evaluation. When an alarm signaled not differ on any baseline psychological or physiological
the end of the speech segment, the experimenter reentered the room and variables, except for anxiety. SOC-EVAL participants had
moved the participant to the computer terminal in order to begin the math higher levels of baseline anxiety (M ⫽ 2.14, SEM ⫽ 0.13)
task. Once the experimenter left the room, the participant then completed the than NON-EVAL condition participants (M ⫽ 1.78, SEM ⫽
math activity on the computer within the view of the panel. HR and BP were
monitored during the speech and math tasks.
0.09), t(79) ⫽ 2.31, p ⫽ .02. As this difference in baseline
In the NON-EVAL stressor condition, the participant was asked to per- anxiety may have affected psychological and physiological
form these same tasks while alone in the room. The participant was asked to responses to the stressor tasks, baseline anxiety was used as a
give his or her speech facing the table that the evaluators sat at in the covariate in repeated-measures ANCOVAs.5
SOC-EVAL condition and to complete the same computerized math task; There was a significant main effect of gender for SBP and
however, no evaluators were present. As in the SOC-EVAL condition, the
significant gender by time interactions for SBP and DBP in
speech was audiotaped and sound was monitored.
After the stressor tasks, participants in both conditions were taken to repeated-measures ANOVAs, with males having higher BP
another area of the room where they were asked to sit and complete poststress levels. However, gender did not interact with stress condition
questionnaire measures. Five minutes after the cessation of the stressor, to affect BP levels. There were no baseline or reactivity
participants were asked to provide another saliva sample (“poststress” mea- gender differences for HR, cortisol, or any of the psycholog-
sure). After the saliva sample, participants were asked to rest quietly for 5 ical variables. Time of day analyses indicated that there was a
minutes. HR and BP were monitored during this resting period, and the last
saliva sample was taken at the end of the rest period (“resting recovery”
marginally significant trend for lower baseline cortisol levels
measures). Finally, participants were fully debriefed and paid or awarded during the last timeslot of the day (5:30 –7:00 PM), as com-
experiment credit. pared with the preceding timeslots (2:30 – 4:00 and 4:00 –5:
30). However, time of day did not predict differential patterns
of cortisol responses across the session or interact with exper-
Statistical Analysis imental condition in analyses.
Possible baseline differences between SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL con-
dition participants on all dependent variables were examined with t tests to Manipulation Checks
ensure success of random assignment. Possible gender differences in levels There was not a significant difference in ratings of task
and stress reactivity of cardiovascular and salivary measures, and possible
difficulty for participants in the social and nonsocial stress
time of day of laboratory appointment effects for salivary cortisol were
assessed with repeated-measures ANOVAs. conditions (SOC-EVAL M ⫽ 5.05, SEM ⫽ 0.21; NON-
Repeated-measures ANOVAs with stress condition (SOC-EVAL vs. EVAL M ⫽ 4.80, SEM ⫽ 0.22, t(79) ⫽ 0.82, p ⫽ .42).
NON-EVAL) as a between-subjects factor and time of measurement of SOC-EVAL condition participants rated the tasks as more
psychological and physiological measures as within-subjects variables were threatening (SOC-EVAL M ⫽ 2.80, SEM ⫽ 0.28, NON-
performed to assess hypotheses of differential psychobiological stress re- EVAL M ⫽ 2.03, SEM ⫽ 0.21, t(79) ⫽ 2.21, p ⫽ .03), and
sponses in SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL stress condition participants. With-
in-subjects psychological variables were pre- and poststress levels of (a)
marginally more challenging (SOC-EVAL M ⫽ 5.66, SEM ⫽
anxiety (ABS) and shame (ABS and SSGS) emotions and (b) performance 0.18; NON-EVAL M ⫽ 5.20, SEM ⫽ 0.18, t(79) ⫽ 1.80, p ⫽
and social self-esteem. Physiological within-subjects variables were (a) av- .08). Mean scores for all five ratings of the panel of evaluators
erage HR, SBP, and DBP for the resting baseline, stress task instruction, task for participants in the social condition were significantly
preparation, speech, mental arithmetic, and resting recovery periods; and (b) lower than the scale midpoint of 4 (all p’s ⬍ .05), indicating
salivary cortisol levels for the resting baseline, stress anticipation, poststress
that participants in the social condition did not like the pan-
and resting recovery time points. Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted degrees of
freedom were used when appropriate.
elists or feel that the panelists would judge them or their
performance favorably.

3
In both conditions, participants’ speeches were audiotaped. NON-EVAL Psychological Responses
condition participants were told that this audiorecording would only be used Between- and within-subjects results from repeated-mea-
to ensure that the speeches given were on the correct topic but would not be
evaluated in any way. SOC-EVAL participants were told it would serve as an sures ANCOVAs for emotion and self-esteem variables are
additional source for evaluation. Audiorecording was used in order to encour- presented in Table 1, and pre- and poststress means for each
age and confirm that participants spoke on the assigned topic. variable in each condition are depicted in Figure 2. There was
4
In both conditions, sound vocalizations (but not actual spoken content)
were monitored to ensure that the participants spoke for the entire speech a significant effect of time for each variable (SSGS shame
period. The participant was warned that if he ceased speaking for a period of effect marginally significant), such that negative emotion
more than 30 seconds, the experimenter would send a sound alert (via a poststress scores were higher and poststress self-esteem scores
two-way radio) from the next room to signal the participant that he needed to
continue speaking. Sound monitoring was instituted to ensure that all partic- were lower than prestress scores. As hypothesized, only the
ipants spoke for the full period in both stress conditions, so that there would shame emotion subscales and the social self-esteem measure
not be an imbalance of activity between participants or stress conditions. In
actuality, the alerting procedure was used for only 2 of the 81 participants, one
5
from each experimental condition (one subject alerted two times, one subject Analyses for psychological and physiological variables not including
alerted three times). The remainder of the 81 participants never ceased baseline anxiety as a covariate produced a similar pattern of results to those
speaking for longer than a 30-second period. reported here.

918 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004)


SOCIAL-SELF THREAT
TABLE 1. Summary of Repeated-Measures ANCOVAs for Psychological Measures*

Anxiety Shame Shame Social Performance


Source
(ABS) F (ABS) F (SSGS) F SE F SE F

Between participants
Stress condition 7.74** 4.22* 1.15 3.81† 0.99
Anxiety — 24.24*** 10.05** 13.17*** 7.20**
Within participants
Time 94.98*** 13.17*** 2.72† 4.35* 14.51***
Time ⫻ anxiety — 2.39 0.51 0.03 0.21
Time ⫻ stress condition 0.16 8.01** 3.74† 7.44** 0.01

SOC-EVAL condition N ⫽ 41, NON-EVAL condition N ⫽ 40. Between- and within-subjects df: anxiety (1,77), ABS shame (1,73), SSGS shame (1,78), social
and performance self-esteem (1, 78).
*p ⬍ .05, **p ⬍ .01, ***p ⬍ .001, †p ⬍ .10.

showed significant stress condition by time effects (SSGS p ⫽ .002), in the SOC-EVAL as compared with the NON-
shame effect marginally significant).6 Poststress shame scores EVAL stress condition.
were higher (ABS shame Mdifference ⫽ 0.59, p ⫽ .01; SSGS To assess whether participants in the SOC-EVAL condition
shame Mdifference ⫽ 0.28, p ⫽ .12), and social self-esteem who showed high or low changes in emotion and self-esteem
scores lower (mean difference ⫽ ⫺0.56, p ⫽ .01), for SOC- variables exhibited differential increases in pre- to poststress
EVAL stress condition participants as compared with NON- cortisol levels, median splits were performed on change scores
EVAL stress condition participants. for each emotion and self-esteem variable, and the mean
change in cortisol for low and high change groups was exam-
Physiological Responses ined (residualized changed scores were computed by regress-
Results of repeated-measures ANCOVAs for HR, SBP, and ing baseline value onto poststress value). Standardized pre- to
DBP are displayed in Table 2, and mean values for each poststress cortisol change for SOC-EVAL condition partici-
measurement time are depicted in Figure 3. There was a pants showing above vs. below the median levels of self-
significant effect of time for all three cardiovascular measures, esteem decreases and emotion increases are displayed in Fig-
with significantly higher than baseline values at the task ure 5. Participants showing above the median levels of social
instruction, task preparation, and speech and math task mea- self-esteem decrease and shame increase had higher levels of
surement points (all p’s ⬍ .01). Recovery BP values did not pre- to post-stress cortisol increase (social self-esteem, t(38) ⫽
significantly differ from baseline values, while HR recovery ⫺2.02, p ⫽ .05; shame (ABS), t(36) ⫽ 2.05, p ⫽ .05; shame
values were lower than baseline values (p ⬍ .001). There were (SSGS), nonsignificant, t(38) ⫽ 1.55, p ⫽ .13). There were no
significant stress condition by time interactions for all three significant differences in mean levels of cortisol change in
variables. Compared with NON-EVAL stress condition par- high and low change groups for performance self-esteem,
ticipants, subjects in the SOC-EVAL stress condition had t(38) ⫽ ⫺0.11, p ⫽ .91, and anxiety, t(38) ⫽ ⫺0.63, p ⫽ .53.
marginally higher HR and SBP values at the speech measure-
ment time point but marginally lower DBP values at baseline DISCUSSION
and marginally lower SBP and DBP values at the task instruc- The present investigation tested the theoretical premise of
tion time points (all p’s between .05 and .10). our social-self preservation model that situations that threaten
Results of repeated-measures ANCOVAs for salivary cor- the social self elicit increases in shame, decrements in social
tisol are displayed in Table 2, and baseline, stress anticipation, self-esteem, and increases in cortisol by comparing psychobi-
poststress, and resting values for the SOC-EVAL and NON- ological responses to performance stressor tasks under the
EVAL stress condition participants are presented in Figure 4. presence or absence of direct social-evaluative threat. Perfor-
There was a significant between-subjects effect of stress con- mance of the tasks in both stress conditions appeared to be
dition, which was qualified by a significant stress condition by “stressful” as indexed by ratings of the tasks as difficult and
time interaction. Baseline and stress anticipation cortisol lev- increases in anxiety and decreases in performance self-esteem.
els did not differ between SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL stress However, those in the social-evaluative condition showed
condition participants. As hypothesized, salivary cortisol lev- higher poststress shame and lower social state self-esteem
els were significantly higher at the poststress (Mdifference ⫽ than those who performed the tasks without direct evaluative
0.12, p ⫽ .002) and recovery time points (Mdifference ⫽ 0.09, threat. Cortisol levels did not increase in response to perfor-
mance of stress tasks in the nonevaluative condition, but as
6
Results for other subscales of the Affect Balance Scale are not discussed hypothesized a large poststress increase occurred in response
here. However, there were significant effects of time for depressed emotion, to performance of these activities under social-evaluative
affection, and contentment (poststress negative emotion higher and positive
emotions lower), but not for anger, joy, or vigor. There were no significant threat. Although we found significant time by stress condition
stress condition by time interactions for any of these other emotion subscales. effects for the cardiovascular system indicators, changes in

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004) 919


T. L. GRUENEWALD et al.

Figure 2. Mean (⫾ SE) pre- and post-stressor scores on emotion and self-esteem measures in SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL stressor conditions.

these parameters did not seem to be as sensitive to the social groups found the tasks to be “stressful” in accordance with
characteristics of stressor experience. HR and BP showed standards typically used in acute stress research (eg, per-
large increases in both stressor conditions, and HR and SBP ceived task difficulty, increased anxiety). However, perfor-
were only marginally greater in the social evaluation condition mance of these demanding tasks in the presence of an
at the speech measurement point. evaluative social audience led to greater responses in the
specific set of psychological states that we believe are
Implications for Stress Research indicative of a threat to the social self: increases in shame
The pattern of responses in the emotion, self-esteem and emotions and decreases in the social component of self-
task rating measures indicates that both stressor condition esteem. Greater cortisol increases were observed in partic-

920 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004)


SOCIAL-SELF THREAT

TABLE 2. Summary of Repeated-Measures ANCOVAs for HR, SBP, DBP, and Cortisol*

HR SBP DBP Cortisol


Source
F F F F

Between participants
Stress condition 1.36 0.00 0.49 5.44*
Anxiety 0.03 0.04 1.12 3.04†
Within participants
Time 14.44*** 15.36*** 15.78*** 0.46
Time ⫻ anxiety 1.40 1.72 0.92 2.74†
Time ⫻ stress condition 3.41* 6.91*** 3.17* 12.57***

SOC-EVAL condition N ⫽ 41, NON-EVAL condition N ⫽ 40. HR, SBP, and DBP between-subjects df: (1,73); cortisol between subjects df (1,75); F-values
with Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted within-subjects df reported above: HR df (2.66,194.04), SBP df (3.98,290.23), DBP df (3.62,264.43), cortisol df (1.33,99.79).
*p ⬍ .05, **p ⬍ .01, ***p ⬍ .001, †p ⬍ .10.

ipants who experienced greater increases in shame and and loss of control (24 –27). Our goal in formulating the Social
greater decreases in social self-esteem, but cortisol in- Self Preservation Theory is to encourage attention to threat to
creases did not significantly differ as a function of the the social self as an additional important elicitor of this sys-
magnitude of increases in anxiety or decreases in perfor- tem, as maintaining the integrity of the social self may also be
mance self-esteem. The lack of an association between important for survival and well-being. Chronic exposure to
increases in cortisol and anxiety conflicts with other inves- social self threats, such as that which occurs in those with a
tigations that have found associations between stress-in- stigmatizing condition or those consistently subjected to or
duced increases in these parameters (41,42); however, these particularly sensitive to rejection, might provide a consistent
associations are not always found in acute stress research trigger to this system, resulting in increased risk for diseases
(43,44), nor are such associations always examined. The linked to this system (see 22,46).
finding of greater concordance between large stress-in-
duced increases in cortisol and shame complements find-
ings of a previous investigation which documented greater Implications for Emotion Research
cortisol increases in response to task failure in children who
Negative emotional and cognitive responses are hypothe-
experienced more postfailure shame, embarrassment, and
sized to be important pathways between stressor experience
negative self-evaluation (45). Thus, threats to the social self
and adverse physiological and health states. However, the
and the emotions and cognitions that derive from such
majority of research in this area has focused on the physio-
threats may be important elicitors of this critical physio-
logical correlates and health consequences of negative mood
logical system. More research will be needed to thoroughly
(a composite of a number of negative moods) or depressed
elucidate whether other cognitive and emotional processes
mood. Less attention has been given to specific emotions in
not examined in the present investigation may underlie
this research (with the exception of anger and cardiovascular
greater cortisol increases witnessed under social-evaluative
threat, including psychological factors thought to be linked activity). The present findings support the utility of examining
to HPA activity, such as perceived control, novelty, unpre- specific emotional and cognitive responses to different char-
dictability, and ego involvement, and to more clearly de- acteristics of stressful situations; such efforts may help to
lineate links between cortisol reactivity and changes in clarify our understanding of associations between stressor
shame and social self-esteem. experience and health outcomes (47).
In this investigation, we employed a version of the TSST Many theories of the etiology of shame emotions and
developed by Kirschbaum and his colleagues (35,40), a par- social self-esteem hypothesize that these states are partic-
adigm that has been employed in many acute stress investi- ularly likely to occur in social situations in which the self
gations and is known to reliably elicit cortisol increases. Our is devalued (6,8,14); however, little empirical work in
results indicate that threat to the social self inherent in this which aspects of the social environment are manipulated to
paradigm may be an important “active ingredient” in the test this hypothesis exists. The present investigation pro-
elicitation of a cortisol increase. This investigation also ex- vides experimental support for the differential experience
perimentally confirms the results of Dickerson and Kemeny’s of shame and social self-esteem as a function of the social
meta-analysis (36), which demonstrated that social-evaluative context of stressful situations. Future research further de-
threat within stressor paradigms was associated with cortisol lineating changes in specific feelings of low social worth
reactivity. A long history of theoretical and empirical research (eg, shame, embarrassment, humiliation) and other catego-
has documented the types of threats likely to activate the HPA ries of negative emotion, as well as concomitant changes in
axis, such as threats to physical integrity, fear-provoking different physiological parameters, to different types of
events, and events characterized by unpredictability, novelty, social and nonsocial stressors is needed.

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004) 921


T. L. GRUENEWALD et al.

Figure 3. Mean (⫾ SE) HR, SBP, and DBP values in SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL stressor conditions across the session.

Limitations and Future Directions employed additional and more comprehensive measures of
We included HR and blood pressure measures to assess cardiovascular activity, such as impedance cardiography and
increases in general levels of arousal that we thought would electrocardiographic techniques, we might have been better
result from performance of tasks equally in both stress con- able to detect differences in autonomic response between our
ditions. The finding of marginally higher HR and SBP re- two stress conditions. Research with such techniques has
sponses in evaluative condition participants during the speech indicated differential effects of social evaluation during per-
portion of the stress activities does indicate that these param- formance of learned and unlearned tasks on cardiac and vas-
eters are sensitive to social-evaluative characteristics of stres- cular components of autonomic reactivity (49) and differential
sor performance, a finding consistent with an investigation effects of the introduction of evaluative observation on cardiac
that found greater SBP increases to a public speaking stressor and vascular response adaptation to repeated performance
in a condition in which social evaluation was emphasized stressor exposure (50). Thus, future investigations that include
compared with one in which it was not (48). Perhaps if we had such techniques will be useful in clarifying our understanding

922 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004)


SOCIAL-SELF THREAT

Figure 4. Mean (⫾ SE) salivary cortisol values in SOC-EVAL and NON-EVAL stressor conditions across the session.

Figure 5. Mean (⫾ SE) pre- to poststressor salivary cortisol increases in “above median change” social self-esteem and shame groups (ie, those participants
that showed changes in social self-esteem and shame from pre- to poststressor that were above the median) and “below median change” groups (ie, those
participants that showed changes in these psychological responses that were below the median) in the SOC-EVAL stressor condition.

of the activity of these systems under conditions of threat to social-evaluative condition simply “adds” social-evaluation
the social self. Future research will also be needed to clarify to a performance stressor, yielding “more stress”) rather
the factors that account for the larger cardiovascular responses than in nature (ie, the presence or absence of social-eval-
witnessed during the speech as compared with the math task in uative threat). Although we cannot rule out this alternative
both conditions, which is an outcome that has been demon- explanation for our results, we do feel it is important to
strated in a previous investigation (41). Probable candidates highlight that only certain psychological and physiological
include differences in physical activity (eg, participants stood responses varied significantly across the two stress condi-
during speech task and sat during math task) and psychosocial tions. If our social-evaluative condition was simply more
factors (eg, perhaps speech task more ego involving). The intense, it might be expected that most of the psychological
more pronounced differences in HR and SBP at the speech and physiological stress response measures (eg, anxiety,
time point between the two stress conditions also suggests that performance self-esteem) would have shown greater
speaking tasks may elicit greater concerns regarding others’ changes in this condition. It will be important to address
evaluations of the social self. this issue of difference in intensity versus difference in the
We chose to test our theory in this first investigation by nature of a stressor in future research.
manipulating the presence or absence of social-evaluative Although we believe that performance of speech and math
threat within a commonly used performance stress para- tasks under social-evaluative threat is a provocative situation,
digm, so that our findings could be compared with previous capable of eliciting concerns about the status of one’s social self,
acute stress investigations and so that the role of the social it will be important to examine psychological and physiological
context of stressor experience in the elicitation of specific responses to other forms of threat to the social self, including
emotional, cognitive, and physiological responses would be those outside the laboratory and those that may be common
highlighted. It could be argued, however, that manipulating outside the college environment. Given the potential ubiquity of
the presence or absence of social-evaluative threat while acute and chronic social self threats in our environment, a greater
holding all other activities constant may have produced two understanding of the psychobiological consequences of such
experimental conditions that varied in intensity (ie, the threats is an important aim of future research.

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004) 923


T. L. GRUENEWALD et al.

We thank Sally Dickerson, Shelley Taylor, Teresa Seeman, and Neil JT, Tassinary LG, Bernston GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology.
Malamuth for their helpful comments on the rationale and design of 2nd ed. Cambridge University Press; 2000.
this research. We are indebted to Julie Bower, Roberta Mancuso, and 25. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychobiological
research: an overview. Neuropsychobiology 1989;22:150 – 69.
Shelley Taylor’s 421 Research Group for their comments and sug- 26. Biondi M, Picardi A. Psychological stress and neuroendocrine function in
gestions on previous versions of this manuscript. We thank Dennis humans: the last two decades of research. Psychother Psychosom 1999;
Montoya for his assistance with the assays for salivary cortisol. 68:144 –50.
27. Mason JW. A review of psychoendocrine research on the pituitary-
REFERENCES adrenal cortical system. Psychosom Med 1968;30:576 – 607.
28. Stroud LR, Salovey P, Epel ES. Sex differences in stress responses: social
1. Dickerson SS, Gruenewald TL, Kemeny ME. When the social self is rejection versus achievement stress. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52:318 –27.
threatened: shame, physiology, and health. J Pers 2004;72:1191–216. 29. Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early
2. Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attach- human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2002;27:199 –220.
ments as a fundamental human motivation. Psych Bull 1995;117:497–529. 30. Seeman TE, McEwen BS. Impact of social environment characteristics
3. James W. The principles of psychology. Chicago: Encyclopaedia on neuroendocrine regulation. Psychosom Med 1996;58:459 –71.
Britannica; 1952. 31. Avitsur R, Stark JL, Sheridan JF. Social stress induces glucocorticoid
4. Bowlby J. A secure base: parent-child attachment and healthy human resistance in subordinate animals. Horm Behav 2001;39:247–57.
development. New York: Basic Books; 1988. 32. Cohen S, Line S, Manuck SB, Rabin BS, Heise ER, Kaplan JR. Chronic
5. Barkow JH. Darwin, sex, and status. Toronto: University of Toronto social stress, social status, and susceptibility to upper respiratory infec-
Press; 1989. tions in nonhuman primates. Psych Med 1997;59:213–21.
6. Gilbert P. Shame, status, and social roles: psychobiology and evolution. 33. Sapolsky RM. Endocrinology alfresco: psychoneuroendocrine studies of
In: Gilbert P, Andrews B, editors. Shame: interpersonal behavior, psy- wild baboons. Recent Prog Horm Res 1993;48:437– 68.
chopathology, and culture. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. 34. Shively CA, Laber-Laird K, Anton RF. Behavior and physiology of
7. Darwin C. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: social stress and depression in female cynomolgus monkeys. Biol Psy-
University of Chicago Press; 1872/1965. chiatry 1997;758:871– 82.
8. Gilbert P. The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, 35. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The “Trier Social Stress
humiliation, guilt and therapy. Br J Med Psychol 1997;70(pt 2):113– 47. Test”: a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a
9. Lewis HB. Shame and guilt in neurosis. Psychoanal Rev 1971;58:419 –38. laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 1993;28:76 – 81.
10. Lewis M. Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride, shame, and 36. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a
guilt. In: Lewis M, Haviland JM, editors. Handbook of emotions. New
theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull
York: Guilford Press; 1993.
2004;130:355–91.
11. Tangney JP, Fischer KW. Self-conscious emotions: the psychology of
37. Derogatis LR. Affects Balance Scale. Riderwood, MD: Clinical Psycho-
shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. New York: Guilford Press; 1995.
metric Research; 1975.
12. Cooley CH. Human nature and the social order. New Brunswick: Trans-
38. Marschall DE, Sanftner JL, Tangney JP. The State Shame and Guilt
action Books; 1902/1983.
Scale. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University; 1994.
13. Fessler DMT. Toward an understanding of the universality of second-
39. Heatherton TF, Polivy J. Development and validation of a scale for
order emotions. In: Hinton AL, editor. Biocultural approaches to the
emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999. measuring state self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol 1991;60:895–910.
14. Scheff TJ. Shame and conformity: the deference-emotion system. Am 40. Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Low self-esteem, induced failure
Sociol Rev 1988;53:395– 406. and the adrenocortical stress response. Pers Individ Dif 1999;27:477–89.
15. Tangney JP, Miller RS, Flicker L, Barlow DH. Are shame, guilt, and 41. Al’Absi M, Bongard S, Buchanan T, Pincomb GA, Licinio J, Lovallo
embarrassment distinct emotions? J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;70:1256 – 69. WR. Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking
16. Wicker FW, Payne GC, Morgan RD. Participants’ descriptions of guilt and mental arithmetic stressors. Psychophysiology 1997;34:266 –75.
and shame. Mot Emotion 1983;7:25–39. 42. Alpers GW, Abelson JL, Wilhelm FH, Roth WT. Salivary cortisol re-
17. Keltner D, Buswell BN. Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, sponse during exposure treatment in driving phobics. Psychosom Med
shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of 2003;65:679 – 87.
emotion. Cogn Emotion 1996;10:155–71. 43. Schedlowski M, Jacobs R, Stratmann G, Richter S, Hadicke A, Tewes U.
18. Tangney JP. Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young adult- Changes of natural killer cells during acute psychological stress. J Clin
hood. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1992;18:199 –206. Immunol 1993;13:119 –26.
19. Leary MR, Tambor ES, Terdal SK, Downs DL. Self-esteem as an 44. Schedlowski M, Wiechert D, Wagner TOF, Tewes U. Acute psycholog-
interpersonal monitor: the sociometer hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol ical stress increases plasma levels of cortisol, prolactin, and TSH. Life Sci
1995;68:518 –30. 1992;50:1201–5.
20. Leary MR, Haupt AL, Strausser KS, Chokel JT. Calibrating the 45. Lewis M, Ramsay D. Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame.
sociometer: the relationship between interpersonal appraisals and state Child Dev 2002;73:1034 – 45.
self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74:1290 –9. 46. McEwen BS, Seeman TE. Protective and damaging effects of mediators
21. Selye H. The physiology and pathology of exposure to stress: a treatise of stress: elaborating and testing the concepts of allostatis and allostatic
based on the concepts of the general-adaptation-syndrome and the dis- load. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1999;896:30 – 47.
eases of adaptation. Montreal: Acta; 1950. 47. Kemeny ME. The psychobiology of stress. Psych Sci 2003;12:124 –9.
22. McEwen BS, Biron CA, Brunson KW, Bulloch K, Chambers WH, 48. Smith TW, Nealey JB, Kircher JC, Limon JP. Social determinants of
Dhabhar FS. The role of adrenocorticoids as modulators of immune cardiovascular reactivity: effects of incentive to exert influence and
function in health and disease: neural, endocrine and immune interac- evaluative threat. Psychophysiology 1997;34:65–73.
tions. Brain Res Rev 1997;23:79 –133. 49. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB, Salomon K. Social facilitation as
23. Sapolsky RM. Neuroendocrinology of the stress-response. In: Becker JB, challenge and threat. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;77:68 –77.
Breedlove SM, Crews D, editors. Behavioral endocrinology. Cambridge, 50. Kelsey RM, Blascovich J, Leitten CL, Schneider TR, Tomaka J, Wiens S.
MA: MIT Press; 1992. Cardiovascular reactivity and adaptation to recurrent psychological
24. Lovallo WR, Thomas TL. Stress hormones in psychophysiological stress: the moderating effects of evaluative observation. Psychophysiol-
research: emotional, behavioral, and cognitive implications. In: Cacioppo ogy 2000;37:748 –56.

924 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:915–924 (2004)

You might also like