You are on page 1of 9

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2013, 54, 328–336 DOI: 10.1111/sjop.

12055

Personality and Social Psychology


Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity: the role of sex
and sexual orientation
PIETERNEL DIJKSTRA,1 DICK P. H. BARELDS2 and HINKE A. K. GROOTHOF3
1
Private Practice, Schildwolde, The Netherlands
2
University of Groningen, Department of Psychology, The Netherlands
3
Open University, Department of Psychology, The Netherlands

Dijkstra, P., Barelds, D. P. H. & Groothof, H. A. K. (2013). Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity: the role of sex and sexual orientation.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 54, 328–336.

The goal of the present study was to examine the emotional content and intensity of jealousy in response to different types of infidelity (both online
and offline unfaithful partner behaviors) among Dutch heterosexuals (n = 191) and homosexuals (n = 121). Based on previous research (Dijkstra,
Barelds & Groothof, 2010), participants were presented with ten jealousy-evoking situations following which the intensity of two different emotional
aspects of jealousy was assessed (betrayal/anger and threat). Results showed that scenarios describing a partner having sex with someone else or falling
in love with someone else primarily evoked betrayal/anger-related jealousy, whereas scenarios describing an emotional connection between a partner
and someone else primarily evoked threat-related jealousy. In addition, women experienced more jealousy than men in response to scenarios in which a
partner engaged in potentially extra-dyadic online (but not offline) behaviors. Finally, compared to same-sex heterosexuals, homosexuals, both male and
female, responded with less intense jealousy to scenarios describing a partner having sex with someone else. Implications for the treatment of (internet)
infidelity are discussed.
Key words: Jealousy, offline, online, infidelity, sexual orientation.
Pieternel Dijkstra, Private Practice, Schildwolde, The Netherlands. Email: pieterneldijkstra@ziggo.nl

INTRODUCTION the advantages of infidelity at the same time (Mileham, 2007).


Indeed, sex is reported to be one of the most frequently searched
Feelings of jealousy are experienced in response to a threat to, topics on the Internet (e.g., Cooper, Delmonico & Burg, 2000;
or the actual loss of, a valued, mostly sexual, relationship with Freeman-Longo & Blanchard, 1998; for a recent overview see,
another person, due to an actual or imagined rival for one’s part- for example, Seattle Organic SEO, 2012), and married individu-
ner’s attention (e.g., Bringle & Buunk, 1985; DeSteno & als may use the Internet to meet strangers, flirt, and engage in
Salovey, 1996; Dijkstra & Buunk, 1998). In contrast to envy (an sexualized conversations or sexual behaviors, with or without
emotion that, in lay language, is often used interchangeably with using a webcam (e.g., Young, Griffin-Shelley, Cooper, O’Mara
jealousy), jealousy is evoked in the context of a ‘triangle’ of & Buchanan, 2000). There are even chat rooms specifically
people, that is, one-self, one’s partner and one’s rival (e.g., geared to married people who wish to engage in sexual conver-
Dijkstra, Barelds & Groothof, 2010; Pam & Pearson, 1996). sations and activities with someone else (e.g., Yahoo’s Married
Consistent with this triangle, three broad lines can be detected in And Flirting; MSN’s Married But Flirting chat rooms). Those
jealousy research, that is, studies that have related jealousy to who participate in online unfaithful behaviors often rationalize
characteristics of the person, studies that have related jealousy to online sexual acts as being OK, because there is no real physical
characteristics of the rival, and studies that have related jealousy contact with someone else. However, Whitty (2003) found that
to the type of infidelity the partner engages in. With regard to most people believe that cybersex (describing the sexual act
the self in the triangle, it has, for instance, consistently been while typically masturbating), hot chatting (a type of erotic talk
found that individuals high in neuroticism experience more that moves beyond light-hearted flirting), and viewing pornogra-
intense feelings of jealousy than individuals low in neuroticism phy on the Internet are all acts of betrayal and adultery (see also
(e.g., Dijkstra & Barelds, 2008; Melamed, 1991). Regarding Schneider, 2003). In addition to sexual acts, also emotional dis-
rival characteristics, it has, for instance, been found that attrac- closure with someone else through the Internet, for example by
tive rivals evoke more jealousy than unattractive ones, especially means of an email or chats, is often seen as an act of infidelity.
among women (e.g., Buunk & Dijkstra, 2001; Dijkstra & Dijkstra et al. (2010), for instance, found that both a partner’s
Buunk, 1998, 2002). The present study focuses on the third line sexual and emotional involvement with someone else by means
of jealousy research, that is, the relation between jealousy and of the Internet evoked considerable amounts of jealousy.
the type of infidelity the partner engages in. Usually, a partner’s extra-dyadic involvement and the jealousy
An important reason for the focus of our study is that, mainly that is caused by it has devastating consequences for the quality
due to the extensive use of the Internet, the number of unfaithful of the primary relationship (Cooper et al., 2000). In the
behaviors individuals can engage in has grown explosively the Netherlands, where the present research took place, extra-dyadic
last two decades. Usage of the Internet has made it very easy for affairs are mentioned by four out of ten people as the primary
people to enjoy both the stability of a committed pair-bond and reason for divorce (De Graaf, 2006). Specifically concerning

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


Scand J Psychol 54 (2013) Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity 329

extra-dyadic Internet activities, it has been reported that, in Dijkstra, 2004), a type of jealousy that has been called ‘fait
recent years, one-third of divorce litigation in the US is due to accompli’ jealousy (Parrot, 1991). The present study extended
online affairs and/or cybersex (cf., Mileham, 2007). In the mean- Dijkstra et al.’s (2010) study by not only examining the intensity
time, marital infidelity and the feelings of jealousy that result of the jealousy response in response to partner behaviors, but
from it are relatively difficult to treat. Therapists often feel also the emotional content of these responses. To date, little is
unprepared for this kind of work (Whisman, Dixon & Johnson, known about the emotional content of jealousy, especially in
1997), and are still struggling with the issue of Internet infidelity response to infidelity that takes place through the Internet. This
and the dilemmas that arise from it (Hertlein, 2011). Moreover, knowledge, however, might be highly relevant: it may help ther-
treatment models for Internet infidelity and its aftermath are still apists identify the most prominent emotional consequences of
in their developing stages (Hertlein & Piercy, 2006). One of the (suspected) Internet infidelity and help focus treatment on those
challenges for researchers, for instance, is the appropriate classi- emotional issues that matter the most. Our first research question
fication of problematic behavior concerning, among other things, (RQ1) therefore is: What emotions (i.e., threat or betrayal/anger)
Internet infidelity (Jones & Hertlein, 2012). Nonetheless, characterize the jealousy response most strongly when individu-
research on Internet infidelity and the jealousy it may cause, is als are confronted with important types of infidelity-related part-
relatively scarce. In addition, recent studies have taken a rela- ner behaviors (both offline and online)?
tively narrow approach, focusing mainly on the question whether
the sex differences in jealousy that have been found offline (i.e.,
the finding that men’s jealousy focuses more on a partner’s sex- Sex and sexual orientation
ual infidelity and women’s jealousy on a partner’s emotional Our study also aimed to examine the extent to which an individ-
infidelity), can be generalized to online infidelity (e.g., Groothof, ual’s sex and sexual orientation affect the emotional content and
Dijkstra & Barelds, 2009; Guadagno & Sagarin, 2010). An intensity of the jealousy individuals experience in response to
exception is a recent study by Dijkstra et al. (2010) that studied important types of infidelity-related partner behaviors (both off-
jealousy in response to a much wider array of potentially line and online). Previous studies have shown these variables to
unfaithful situations. Based on a literature review these authors be related to the experience and/or expression of jealousy (e.g.,
identified 42 infidelity-related partner behaviors, both offline and Carpenter, 2012; Sagarin, Martin, Coutinho et al., 2012). With
online, and examined the extent to which these 42 partner regard to an individual’s sex, for instance, studies have shown
behaviors evoked jealousy. Their study showed, for instance, that men more often than women chose sexual infidelity over
that a partner’s explicit infidelity, such as a partner kissing or emotional infidelity as the most upsetting event (e.g., Bailey,
having sex with someone else, evoked most jealousy, closely Gaulin, Agyei & Gladue, 1994; Buss, Larsen, Westen &
followed by a partner’s emotional or romantic involvement with Semmelroth, 1992; Dijkstra, Groothof, Poel, Laverman, Schrier
someone else by means of the Internet (for instance, when a & Buunk, 2001; Sagarin et al., 2012; but also see Carpenter,
partner was sending romantic mails or chats to someone else). 2012). Another example is that women have been found to report
This latter type of jealousy evoked more intense feelings of jeal- stronger feelings of jealousy than men in response to physically
ousy than a partner watching pornography or behaving suspi- attractive rivals, whereas men report stronger feelings of jealousy
ciously in real life, for instance by spending a lot of time with a than women in response to socially dominant rivals (e.g., Dijkstra
cross-sex friend or by intimately dancing with someone else. & Buunk, 1998). The relation between jealousy and an individ-
The present study builds on and extends this study. ual’s sex has mostly been explained from both an evolutionary or
biological point of view and a sociocultural one (but see also, for
example, Levy & Kelly, 2010). According to the first perspective,
The emotional content of jealousy for instance, men are more focused on sexual infidelity than
In their study, Dijkstra et al. (2010) assessed jealousy by means women, because men may suffer from paternity uncertainty (e.g.,
of the question ‘How jealous would you feel if ….’ followed by Buss, 2000). According to the sociocultural view, men and
42 potentially jealousy-evoking partner behaviors. However, women’s jealousy responses are shaped through the internaliza-
according to Sharpsteen (1991; see also Buunk & Dijkstra, tion of societal norms that dictate men and women how to
2004; Parrot & Smith, 1993) jealousy is a compound emotion of behave. For instance, because of the norm that a man’s honor is
which the exact content is a blend of the basic emotions of crucial, for instance, in many honor cultures it is accepted that
anger, sadness and fear. Which of these emotions becomes the men respond with aggression and sometimes abuse when they are
most salient at any given moment is determined by the aspects jealous (e.g., Vandello & Cohen, 2003). Our second research
of the situation individuals focus upon. More specifically, Buunk question (RQ2) therefore is: Do men and women differ in the
and Dijkstra (2004) found that, when sexual contact between intensity and content (i.e., threat and/or betrayal/anger) of their
one’s partner and someone else has not yet occurred, in particu- jealousy responses when confronted with important types of infi-
lar feelings of anxiety and insecurity concerning a mate’s extra- delity-related partner behaviors (both offline and online)?
dyadic sexual involvement color the jealousy experience, a type In addition to an individual’s sex, several studies found an
of jealousy that centers around feelings of threat (Buunk & individual’s sexual orientation to be related to jealousy. For
Dijkstra, 2004) and has been called ‘suspicious’ jealousy instance, it has been found that lesbian women, more than gay
(Parrott, 1991; DeSteno & Salovey, 1994). In contrast, when men, chose a mate’s sexual infidelity as the most upsetting
extradyadic sex has already occurred, the jealousy experience is event, whereas gay men more than lesbian women chose a
usually strongly colored by feelings of betrayal/anger (Buunk & mate’s emotional infidelity as the most upsetting event (Dijkstra

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


330 P. Dijkstra et al. Scand J Psychol 54 (2013)

et al., 2001). In addition, especially homosexual men have been Table 1. Composition of the Subsamples
found to respond with relatively low jealousy to a partner’s (sus-
Men Women
pected) infidelity (e.g., Bailey et al., 1994; Barelds & Dijkstra,
Heterosexual Homosexual Heterosexual Homosexual
2006; Bringle, 1995; Hawkins, 1990; Sheets & Wolfe, 2001).
This finding has been explained by the fact that gay men often N 67 49 124 72
have relatively promiscuous relationships and permissive atti-
tudes towards extra-dyadic sex (Buunk & Van Driel, 1989; Age: 28.1 (11.1) 29.3 (12.7) 28.2 (12.2) 32.4 (15.3)
M (SD)
Dijkstra et al., 2001; Peplau & Spalding, 2000). Although this
Educational level
has been found for offline infidelity-related partner behaviors, -low 3% 10% 2% 4%
such as having oral or anal sex, knowledge is still lacking on -middle 13% 20% 15% 21%
homosexuals’ jealousy responses to the infidelity-related behav- -high 84% 69% 84% 75%
iors that a partner engages in through the Internet. Our third
research question (RQ3) therefore is: Do homosexuals differ
from (same-sex) heterosexuals in the intensity and content (i.e., participants who did not fully finish the questionnaire (all questions had
threat and/or betrayal/anger) of their jealousy responses when to be answered in order to proceed to the next screen in the study) were
removed from the final sample, and are not shown in Table 1. The final
confronted with important types of infidelity-related partner
sample consists of 312 participants: 191 heterosexual and 121 homosex-
behaviors (both offline and online)? ual participants.
Educational level was divided into low (roughly equivalent to prepara-
tory secondary vocational education), middle (roughly equivalent to
The present study senior general education), and high (college/university level) levels of
education, mainly for comparison purposes between the subsamples dis-
The present study examines multidimensional jealousy responses
tinguished in Table 1. In general, all subsamples had, on average, a high
to ten short scenarios in which the partner engages in different level of education, probably as a result of the collection process (in
infidelity-related behaviors. These ten scenarios were derived which mainly highly educated individuals were involved). Also, all subs-
from Dijkstra et al. (2010; see Method section for details). Sum- amples were relatively young, with a mean age around 30 years. An
marizing, the present study examined the following research ANOVA showed that the subsamples did not differ significantly with
regard to age [F(3, 311) = 1.80, p = ns]. Moreover, there was no signifi-
questions:
cant difference between the groups with regard to educational level
(1) RQ1: What emotions (i.e., threat or betrayal/anger) character- [Kruskal-Wallis Χ2(3) = 6.77, p = ns].
ize the jealousy response most strongly when individuals are
confronted with important types of infidelity-related partner
Materials
behaviors (both offline and online)?
(2) RQ2: Do men and women differ in the intensity and content Jealousy evoking scenarios. In the present study, ten scenarios were
used, based on the Dijkstra et al. (2010) study on jealousy evoking part-
(i.e., threat and/or betrayal/anger) of their jealousy responses
ner behaviors. In that study, 42 jealousy evoking partner behaviors
when confronted with important types of infidelity-related (including both online and offline behaviors) were identified based on a
partner behaviors (both offline and online)? review of the literature. Simultaneous components analyses conducted on
(3) RQ3: Do homosexuals differ from (same-sex) heterosexuals the two samples in that study resulted in a four-factor solution underly-
in the intensity and content (i.e., threat and/or betrayal/anger) ing these 42 jealousy evoking partner behaviors, that were labeled ‘Sus-
picious Behavior’ (14 behaviors; e.g., ‘During a conversation your
of their jealousy responses when confronted with important
partner touches someone of the opposite sex’), ‘Unfaithful Behavior’ (15
types of infidelity-related partner behaviors (both offline and behaviors; e.g., ‘Your partner has sex with someone else’), ‘Pornogra-
online)? phy’ (5 behaviors; e.g., ‘Your partner watches pornographic pictures or
movies on the Internet’), and ‘Technological Investment’ (4 behaviors;
e.g., ‘Your partner shares his/her feelings and secrets with someone of
METHOD the opposite sex by chat or email’; Dijkstra et al., 2010).
To decrease the work load for the participants, we decided to use a
selection of the most relevant partner behaviors for the purpose of the
Participants and procedure
present study. From the 42 jealousy evoking partner behaviors mentioned
Participants were recruited in various ways. The heterosexual participants above we first selected the two partner behaviors that most clearly
were recruited by the researchers and five psychology students conduct- reflected sexual and emotional offline infidelity, that is ‘Your partner has
ing research for their bachelor thesis. These students were asked to sex with someone else,’ and ‘Your partner falls in love with someone
search for participants in their network, and to make sure that the sample else.’ These two partner behaviors are both part of the ‘Unfaithful
would be heterogeneous with regard to sex, educational level, and age. Behavior’ factor and are routinely used in jealousy studies, particularly
The questionnaire was distributed online. Although some homosexual those examining (evolutionary) differences in the jealousy response of
participants were also recruited in this process, the bulk of the homosex- men and women (e.g., Groothof et al., 2009; for recent meta-analyses
ual participants were recruited by means of postings on social network see Carpenter, 2012; Sagarin et al., 2012) and/or heterosexuals and
sites geared at homosexuals, and mailings to members of homosexual homosexuals (e.g., Harris, 2002). Two matching online unfaithful behav-
groups or communities. Also, some homosexual participants distributed iors were next selected from the list of 42 partner behaviors, that is
the link to the questionnaire to other homosexual individuals. The final ‘Your partner falls in love with someone with whom he/she communi-
result of this collection process is shown in Table 1. Participants who cates through the Internet,’ and ‘Your partner has sex with someone by
had indicated that they were bisexual were removed from the final sam- using the webcam’ (the original partner behaviors are both also part of
ples (ten female participants and one male participant; these participants the ‘Unfaithful Behavior’ factor). The latter of these was adapted from
could be classified as both heterosexual and homosexual, and the num- ‘Your partner has cybersex with someone.’ The reason for this adaptation
bers are too small to use bisexuals as a separate group). In addition, is that the term cybersex might cause some confusion: not everybody

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


Scand J Psychol 54 (2013) Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity 331

might be familiar with this term, whereas the term itself can refer to dif- adjectives may be split into two subscales. The first subscale can be
ferent specific acts of online sexual infidelity. For example, text commu- labeled betrayal-anger, and is composed of the items hurt, sad, rejected,
nication describing sexual acts without actually undressing and/or angry, and betrayed. The second subscale can be labeled threat, and con-
masturbating might also be considered a form of cybersex, as might sex sists of the items anxious, worried, distrustful, suspicious, threatened,
between characters in a virtual environment such as Second Life. Because and jealous.
we wanted to use an online partner behavior corresponding closely to the To facilitate comparisons between the mean scores for betrayal/anger
act of actually having sex with someone else we specified the content as (this subscale is composed of 5 items) and threat (this subscale is com-
described above. posed of 6 items), the summed scores for betrayal-anger were multiplied
Because, in the present study, we wanted to pay specific attention to by 6/5. Cronbach’s alpha across the ten scenarios ranged from 0.80 to
responses to partner behaviors related to the use of modern communica- 0.88 for both betrayal/anger and threat. Correlations between these two
tion media, we also included all four items from the ‘Technological subscales were moderate to large, ranging from r = 0.61 (p < 0.001) for
Investment’ factor described by Dijkstra et al. (2010), that is, ‘Your part- ‘Your partner falls in love with someone else’ to r = 0.82 (p < 0.001)
ner shares his/her feelings and secrets with someone of the opposite sex for ‘Your partner watches pornographic pictures or movies on the Inter-
by chat or email,’ ‘Your partner shares a strong emotional bond with net.’ To further examine the validity of the two scales in the present
someone he/she communicates with through the Internet,’ ‘Your partner study, PCAs were run on the 11 adjectives (for each scenario separately),
sends a romantic email or chat to someone of the opposite sex,’ and extracting two factors, and performing an oblique rotation (Oblimin).
‘Your partner sends SMSes to someone of the opposite sex on a regular The factor structures generally supported the division of the 11 adjectives
basis.’ The latter item refers to using a telephone, rather than a computer. over the two scales (betrayal/anger and threat). Mean correlations across
For comparison purposes we therefore also added the sexual partner scenarios between the a priori factors and corresponding PCA factors
behavior ‘Your partner has phone sex with someone else’ (from the were r = 0.96 for betrayal/anger and r = 0.94 for threat (mean correla-
‘Unfaithful Behavior’ factor). Finally, we decided to add one more tion between PCA factors was r = 0.59). Correlations between the
online behavior listed in the ‘Pornography’ factor, that is, ‘Your partner betrayal/anger scores for the ten scenarios ranged from r = 0.27
watches pornographic pictures or movies on the Internet.’ Although this (p < 0.001; ‘Your partner has sex with someone else’ and ‘Your partner
online partner behavior clearly has a sexual (online) content, it does not sends SMSes to someone of the opposite sex on a regular basis’) to
require the (online) presence of a specific other person, but rather one or r = 0.80 (p < 0.001; ‘Your partner has sex with someone by using the
more anonymous individuals, nor does it require an active interaction webcam’ and ‘Your partner has phone sex with someone else’). Correla-
with that/those person(s). For homosexuals, scenarios 7, 9 and 10 (see tions between the threat scores for the ten scenarios ranged from
Table 2) referred to someone of the same sex, rather than someone of r = 0.37 (p < 0.001; ‘Your partner watches pornographic pictures or
the opposite sex. movies on the Internet’ and ‘Your partner sends SMSes to someone of
the opposite sex on a regular basis’) to r = 0.85 (p < 0.001; ‘Your part-
Jealousy ratings. For each of the ten scenarios described above partici- ner has sex with someone by using the webcam’ and ‘Your partner has
pants were asked how they would feel if they were confronted with the phone sex with someone else’).
situation described in the scenario in real life. For each scenario partici-
pants filled out a multiple-adjective rating scale, that included the adjec-
tives suspicious, betrayed, worried, distrustful, jealous, rejected, hurt,
anxious, angry, threatened, and sad. These 11 adjectives were assessed RESULTS
on 5-point scales (from 1 = ‘not at all’ to 5 = ‘very strongly’), and
were administered directly below each of the ten scenarios. According to Emotional response to the scenarios
Parrott and Smith (1993), these 11 adjectives distinguish the phenome-
non of jealousy from other negative emotional states, such as envy. Pre- We first examined what emotions color the jealousy response,
vious research (Buunk & Dijkstra, 2004) has shown that these 11 that is, feelings of threat or feelings of anger/betrayal, most

Table 2. Mean Betrayal/Anger and Threat Scores for the Ten Scenarios by Participant Sex and Sexual Orientation

Betrayal/Anger Threat

Men Women Men Women

Heterosexual Homosexual Heterosexual Homosexual Heterosexual Homosexual Heterosexual Homosexual

Scenario M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

1 26.8a1 4.3 24.5b1 6.6 27.5c1 3.3 25.5ab1 6.0 23.0a1 4.4 20.8b14 6.3 23.9a1 5.2 22.0ab16 6.3
2 24.1a2 4.4 21.8b2 5.7 25.6c2 4.9 24.1ac2 5.7 20.0a2 4.9 18.6a26 6.0 22.1b2 5.7 20.7ab2 5.9
3 23.6a2 5.5 21.8a2 6.5 25.7b2 4.4 23.5a24 6.1 20.6a24 5.5 19.5a24 6.0 22.5b24 5.3 20.5a2 6.5
4 12.9a3 5.8 11.8a3 5.3 15.9b38 6.3 14.0c3 6.4 12.4a3 5.1 12.6a3 5.4 15.1b3 5.5 13.8ab3 5.6
5 23.0a2 4.9 23.7a1 5.9 26.0b2 4.5 23.7a24 5.8 21.1a24 4.9 21.2a1 5.9 23.2ab4 5.8 21.6a146 6.1
6 22.0a24 5.2 21.4a2 6.0 24.4b4 4.7 22.9ab47 6.0 21.0ab4 4.9 20.3a145 5.7 22.3b2 5.0 20.9ab246 6.4
7 18.6a5 5.7 18.3a4 6.3 20.6b5 5.4 19.3ab5 6.8 18.9a5 5.3 18.9a25 5.5 20.4a5 5.3 19.0a5 6.4
8 16.8a6 6.0 17.0ab5 6.4 19.0b6 6.3 17.1a6 6.3 18.1a5 5.7 18.0a56 5.5 20.3b5 5.5 18.4a5 6.0
9 21.4a4 5.7 21.0a2 5.3 22.5a7 5.3 22.0a7 6.0 21.3a4 4.8 21.1ab1 5.1 22.5b24 4.6 21.9ab6 5.6
10 13.0a37 5.4 12.1a3 4.9 14.8b8 5.8 12.6a3 6.0 16.3a6 5.5 14.7a7 5.3 17.6b6 5.7 15.2a3 6.5

Note: Scenario 1 = partner has sex with someone else, Scenario 2 = partner has sex with someone else using webcam, Scenario 3 = partner has
phone sex, Scenario 4 = partner watches Internet porn, Scenario 5 = partner falls in love with someone else, Scenario 6 = partner falls in love
through the Internet, Scenario 7 = partner shares feelings and secrets online, Scenario 8 = partner shares strong emotional bond online, Scenario 9 =
partner sends romantic emails, Scenario 10 = partners sends SMSes on a regular basis (for details see method section). Means with different subscript
letters differ significantly within rows (computed separately for Betrayal/Anger and Threat), means with different subscript numbers differ significantly
within columns (ps < .05).

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


332 P. Dijkstra et al. Scand J Psychol 54 (2013)

strongly when individuals are confronted with important types of [F(2, 307) = 4.00, p < 0.05] or watching pornographic pictures
infidelity-related partner behaviors (RQ1). For this purpose, on the Internet [F(2, 307) = 3.04, p < 0.05]. In all these cases,
repeated measures analyses of variance were conducted examin- heterosexual participants, on average, responded with more feel-
ing the differences between the betrayal/anger and threat scores ings of betrayal/anger and threat (see Table 2) than homosexual
in response to the scenarios. Significant differences were found participants. There were no significant sex by sexual orientation
for seven of the ten scenarios. More feelings of betrayal/anger interaction effects.
were reported in response to the scenarios that seemed to The highest scores for betrayal/anger and threat (see Table 2)
describe explicitly sexually unfaithful partner behaviors, that is, are generally reported by heterosexual women, followed by
‘Your partner has sex with someone else’ [F(1, 311) = 218.25, homosexual women and heterosexual men, who report about
p < 0.001], ‘Your partner has sex with someone else using the equal amounts of betrayal/anger and threat. The lowest betrayal/
webcam’ [F(1, 311) = 200.07, p < 0.001], and ‘Your partner anger and threat scores are generally found for homosexual men
has phone sex’ [F(1, 311) = 152.59, p < 0.001]. Also scenarios (see also Figs. 1 and 2). Overall, the scenarios in which the part-
in which the partner falls in love with someone else, both offline ner is sexually unfaithful or falls in love with someone else (be
[F(1, 311) = 75.94, p < .001] and online [F(1, 311) = 53.69, it offline or online) evoke most feelings of jealousy (both
p < 0.001] evoked stronger feelings of betrayal/anger than threat. betrayal/anger and threat). Table 2 also shows that extradyadic
In contrast, more feelings of threat than anger/betrayal were offline behaviors evoke, on average, slightly stronger feelings of
reported in response to two scenarios that seemed to describe jealousy than comparable online behaviors (comparison of sce-
non-sexual acts and that (merely) implied an emotional connec- narios 1 and 2, and scenarios 5 and 6).
tion with someone else, that is, the scenarios in which the part-
ner shares a strong emotional bond with someone else online
[F(1, 311) = 31.61, p < 0.001] and the scenario in which the DISCUSSION
partner sends SMSes to someone else on a regular basis Our study set out to examine more closely the emotional content
[F(1, 311) = 166.73, p < 0.001]. About equal intense feelings of of the jealousy response that is evoked by offline and online
anger/betrayal and jealousy were evoked by the scenarios ‘Your potentially unfaithful partner behaviors, including examining dif-
partner watches Internet porn,’ ‘Your partner shares feelings and ferences between the sexes and individuals with a different sex-
secrets online’ and ‘Your partner sends romantic emails to some- ual orientation. Our first question was what emotions (i.e.,
one else’ (Fs ns; mean scores for betrayal/anger and threat are feelings of threat or feelings of betrayal/anger) characterize the
reported in Table 2, by participant sex and sexual orientation). jealousy response most strongly when individuals are confronted
with important types of infidelity-related online and offline part-
ner behaviors. We found jealousy colored by feelings of
Participant sex and sexual orientation betrayal/anger to be reported most strongly in response to sce-
Next, we examined whether there are differences between men narios that described explicitly sexually unfaithful partner behav-
and women (RQ2) respectively between heterosexuals and homo- iors, either online or offline, and that described a partner falling
sexuals (RQ3) in their response to the ten jealousy evoking part- in love, also either offline or online. These findings are in line
ner behaviors. For this purpose, multivariate analyses of variance with previous studies that show ‘fait accompli jealousy,’ that is,
were conducted, using the betrayal/anger and threat scores for the jealousy characterized by feelings of betrayal/anger, to be espe-
scenarios as dependent variables and participant sex and sexual cially evoked by a partner’s extradyadic sexual behaviors (e.g.,
orientation as independent variables. Significant multivariate main Buunk & Dijkstra, 2004). Because anger is a primary response
effects of participant sex were found for the scenarios in which to transgressions (e.g., Fessler, 2010), this finding suggests that
the partner watches pornographic pictures on the Internet individuals do not only perceive a partner having sex with some-
[F(2, 307) = 6.53, p < 0.01], falls in love with someone else on one else as a serious transgression that evokes ‘fait accompli,’
the Internet [F(2, 307) = 5.00, p < .01], or has sex with someone but also a partner falling in love with someone else. Previous
else using the webcam [F(2, 307) = 5.53, p < 0.01]. A margin- studies, in contrast, mostly assumed that a partner falling in love
ally significant effect of participant sex was found for the partner with someone else is perceived as hurtful because it is likely to
having phone sex [F(2, 307) = 4.34, p < 0.05]. In all these cases lead to a partner’s extradyadic sexual contact, with the ‘real’
women reported significantly more betrayal/anger and threat than transgression being extradyadic sex (Buunk & Dijkstra, 2004).
men (with the exception of the scenario in which the partner falls Because of the relatively strong evocation of anger/betrayal
in love with someone else on the Internet, for which only a sig- related jealousy, our findings imply that falling in love with
nificant difference, p < 0.01, between men and women was someone else is likely to be a serious transgression in itself.
found for betrayal/anger; see also Table 2 and Fig. 1). In contrast, we found feelings of threat to be most characteris-
Significant multivariate main effects of sexual orientation were tic of two technology-related partner behaviors that implied an
found for the three scenarios in which the partner is actively emotional, but not necessarily sexual or romantic, connection
involved in sexual activities with someone else: the partner has with someone else (i.e., sending SMS-messages to someone else
sex with some else offline [F(2, 307) = 7.59, p < 0.01] or online and sharing a strong emotional bond with someone else online).
using the webcam [F(2, 307) = 4.71, p < 0.01], or has phone The reaction to these behaviors parallels what previous studies
sex with someone else [F(2, 307) = 4.66, p < 0.01]. Marginally have called ‘suspicious’ jealousy, that is, a response that is usu-
significant effects of sexual orientation were found for the ally evoked by partner behaviors that imply an emotional con-
partner sending SMSes to someone else on a regular basis nection with someone else but not per se a sexual or romantic

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


Scand J Psychol 54 (2013) Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity 333

attachment. Although previous studies have shown ‘suspicious’ jealousy, in relationships that consist of two women rather than
and ‘fait accompli’ jealousy to be evoked by relevant offline one man and one woman. A possible explanation for the finding
behaviors, the present study shows that these types of jealousy that lesbian women experience less jealousy than heterosexual
are also evoked by relevant partner behaviors that concern the women is that among lesbians jealousy is not related to relation-
use of the Internet and/or a mobile phone. A possible explana- ship quality, whereas among heterosexuals it is (Barelds &
tion is that our brain registers virtual events as ‘real’ ones and Dijkstra, 2006). As such, jealousy may play a different role in
responds accordingly (e.g., Mathiak & Weber, 2006). Thus, from lesbian relationships, and perhaps be less crucial to the survival
a cognitive point of view, individuals may become involved in and quality of the relationship.
online extradyadic behaviors as if they were real-life ones. This
may also be true for the betrayed partner: when the infidelity is
uncovered, the partner may respond as if the online partner Implications
behaviors were real life ones. Support for this explanation has, As noted in the introduction section, one of the challenges of
for instance, been found by studies on violent videogames. In researchers in the domain of Internet infidelity is the appropriate
response to violent video games, the brain responds as if the classification of potentially problematic behaviors (Jones &
threat and violence are real, stimulating the body to produce Hertlein, 2012). The present study takes up this challenge, and
more testosterone and cortisol (e.g., Mazur, Susman & shows what offline and online partner behaviors evoke most jeal-
Edelbrock, 1997). In line with this explanation previous studies ousy and, in addition, what, in response to different partner behav-
(e.g., Groothof et al., 2009; Guadagno & Sagarin, 2010) have iors, the emotional content of the jealousy experience is: betrayal/
shown that men and women tend to respond similarly to a part- anger and/or threat. This knowledge may provide clinicians more
ner’s online and offline sexual and emotional infidelity. insight in the emotions and their triggers that surround the infidel-
However, although offline extradyadic behaviors and similar ity-related situations couples in relationship therapy or counseling
online behaviors may evoke jealousy in similar emotional may cope with. In addition, with regard to extra-dyadic sex,
patterns, this does not necessarily mean that they also evoke the couples often lack explicit rules or agreement about these rules
same intensity of emotions. Indeed, our study showed that off- (Dijkstra, 2011; Hertlein & Stevenson, 2010). An effective inter-
line extradyadic behaviors evoked slightly more jealousy than vention can then be to set (new) rules about the boundaries of the
comparable online behaviors, a finding that is consistent with relationship and/or to negotiate about these rules and boundaries
most findings reported by Guadagno and Sagarin (2010). These (Snyder, Baucom & Coop Gordon, 2007). The partner behaviors
authors explain this difference from an evolutionary psychologi- examined by this study may be used as a preliminary checklist that
cal perspective: in contrast to extradyadic offline behaviors, may help identify those situations and/or partner behaviors about
extradyadic online behaviors cannot lead to pregnancy. Another which couples need to set some kind of rules.
explanation is that, although the brain may register virtual events In addition, depending on the specific potentially unfaithful
as real ones, it may do so less intensely. partner behavior, our findings suggest two avenues to interven-
Our study also showed that women responded with both more tions that may help clients cope with issues surrounding a part-
anger/betrayal related jealousy and threat-related jealousy than ner’s (potential) infidelity. If a partner has engaged in behaviors
men to several of a partner’s extradyadic online, but not offline that our study shows to evoke relatively strong feelings of anger/
behaviors (also see Carpenter, 2012). Our results suggest that betrayal-related jealousy, such as cybersex, the therapist may, for
women are relatively sensitive to a partner’s extradyadic online instance, help the betrayed individual by using interventions
behaviors. A possible explanation is that women generally have aimed at anger management. In contrast, if a partner has engaged
worse computer skills than men (e.g., Pinkard, 2005), and, as a in behaviors that our study shows to evoke relatively strong feel-
consequence, may feel they have less control over their partner’s ings of threat-related jealousy, the therapist’s first aim may be to
activities on the Internet. For instance, in 2006, 62% of Dutch help the individual regain a sense of security. This may be espe-
women reported having no or only little computer skills, com- cially important in the treatment of women whose partner has
pared to 48% of Dutch men (Central Bureau of Statistics, 2006). engaged in online extra-dyadic behaviors. A sense of security
Our study also showed extradyadic sex, whether online or off- may then be partially restored by helping women become more
line, to evoke both more anger/betrayal related jealousy and skilled when it comes to using the computer. Improving their
threat-related jealousy among heterosexuals than among homo- computer skills may help women get a more realistic picture of
sexuals of the same sex. In general, homosexual men were less the possibilities but also restrictions of Internet as a means for
jealous than heterosexual men, probably for the reasons already infidelity and help manage and limit worries and rumination.
mentioned in the introduction section: gay men often have rela- Our study also implies that treatment in the case of (Internet)
tively promiscuous relationships and permissive attitudes towards infidelity, may need to include different interventions in the case
extra-dyadic sex (Buunk & Van Driel, 1989; Dijkstra et al., of homosexual clients. Although homosexuals – both men and
2001; Peplau & Spalding, 2000). The finding that also lesbian women – seem to experience less jealousy in response to a part-
women reported less jealousy than heterosexual women comes ner’s infidelity than heterosexuals, this does not mean that the
as more of a surprise. In general, women report experiencing relationship is less damaged by it. It may be that, in these cou-
emotions more intensely than men as well as displaying them ples, infidelity may create or strengthen the norm that ‘infidelity
more frequently and in a more extreme fashion (e.g., Robinson, is okay,’ and, in so doing, increase the risk that couples fall in
Johnson & Shields, 2001). As a result, one might expect partners love with someone else and end the relationship. In the end this
to experience more frequent and intense emotions, among which may not be what both partners want.

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


334 P. Dijkstra et al. Scand J Psychol 54 (2013)

30
28 (3)
(1)
26 (4)
(2)
24
22
(1) Heterosexual men
20
18 (2) Homosexual men

16 (3) Heterosexual women

14 (4) Homosexual women

12
10

Fig. 1. Mean Betrayal/Anger scores for heterosexual and homosexual men and women for the ten scenarios (Scenario 1 = partner has sex with some-
one else, Scenario 2 = partner has sex with someone else using webcam, Scenario 3 = partner has phone sex, Scenario 4 = partner watches Internet
porn, Scenario 5 = partner falls in love with someone else, Scenario 6 = partner falls in love through the Internet, Scenario 7 = partner shares feel-
ings and secrets online, Scenario 8 = partner shares strong emotional bond online, Scenario 9 = partner sends romantic emails, Scenario 10 = part-
ners sends SMSes on a regular basis).

26

24 (3)
(1)
22 (4)
(2)
20
(1) Heterosexual men
18
(2) Homosexual men
16
(3) Heterosexual women
14 (4) Homosexual women
12

10

Fig. 2. Mean Threat scores for heterosexual and homosexual men and women for the ten scenarios (Scenario 1 = partner has sex with someone else,
Scenario 2 = partner has sex with someone else using webcam, Scenario 3 = partner has phone sex, Scenario 4 = partner watches Internet porn, Sce-
nario 5 = partner falls in love with someone else, Scenario 6 = partner falls in love through the Internet, Scenario 7 = partner shares feelings and
secrets online, Scenario 8 = partner shares strong emotional bond online, Scenario 9 = partner sends romantic emails, Scenario 10 = partners sends
SMSes on a regular basis).

Limitations homosexual participants in general can be quite difficult. There-


The major limitations of the present study concern the represen- fore, although we did succeed in collecting data from subsam-
tativeness of the subsamples, and the materials used. The subs- ples that did not differ with regard to age and educational level,
amples were collected through the network of participating it remains unclear to what extent the present study’s findings
students and the researchers, social network sites and targeted can be generalized to other populations. In addition, we did not
mailings to homosexual participants. This will most likely have ask for participant’s relationship status. It could be that people
resulted in non-representative samples of both heterosexual and who are in a relationship respond differently to the scenarios
homosexual men and women. This is, for instance, reflected in than people who are single. Future studies might further
the high percentage of highly educated participants in all subs- examine this issue.
amples. On the other hand, recruiting a representative sample In the present study, we used subscales for Betrayal/Anger
of homosexuals appears to be an almost infeasible endeavor, and Threat that are based on previous work (e.g., Buunk &
since we know little about the exact composition of the homo- Dijkstra, 2004). There were moderate to strong relations between
sexual population in the Netherlands, and because recruiting these two subscales in the present study, and the validity of

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


Scand J Psychol 54 (2013) Jealousy in response to online and offline infidelity 335

these subscales needs further examination. The ten scenarios that Buunk, B. P. & Van Driel, B. (1989). Variant lifestyles and relation-
we used were displayed in random order. It is possible, however, ships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Carpenter, C. J. (2012). Meta-analyses of sex differences in responses to
that there was still some emotional spill-over between scenarios.
sexual versus emotional infidelity: Men and women are more similar
We did not examine this issue. than different. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36, 25–37.
Central Bureau of Statistics (2006). De digitale economie [The digital
economy]. Voorburg: CBS.
CONCLUSIONS De Graaf, A. (2006). Aantal echtscheidingen neemt weer toe [Number of
As noted before, despite its importance for, among other things, divorces is rising again]. Webmagazine Central Bureau of Statistics.
the treatment of couples in the case of Internet infidelity, the Retrieved 20 April, 2013 from http://www.cbs.nl/nl-NL/menu/themas/
bevolking/publicaties/artikelen/archief/2006/2006-1862-wm.htm.
topic of Internet infidelity remains relatively understudied. The Cooper, A., Delmonico, D. L. & Burg, R. (2000). Cybersex users, abus-
present study contributes to the literature by examining the most ers, and compulsives: New findings and implications. Sexual Addic-
important offline and online partner behaviors that are likely to tions and Compulsivity, 7, 5–29.
elicit jealousy and the emotional content of these jealousy DeSteno, D. A. & Salovey, P. (1994). Jealousy in close relationships:
responses among both homosexuals and heterosexuals. Espe- Multiple perspectives on the green-ey’d monster. In A. L. Weber &
J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Perspectives on close relationships (pp. 217–
cially little is known about jealousy among homosexuals, in par- 242). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
ticular in response to Internet infidelity. Of course our study also DeSteno, D. A. & Salovey, P. (1996). Evolutionary origins of sex differ-
suffers from limitations. For instance, asking participants how ences in jealousy? Questioning the fitness of the model. Psychologi-
they would respond in the hypothetical situation that their part- cal Science, 7, 367–372.
ner would be unfaithful may not generate responses that reliably Dijkstra, P. (2011). Omgaan met ziekelijke jaloezie [Dealing with patho-
logical jealousy]. Houten: BSL.
reflect how individuals would behave if infidelity would actually Dijkstra, P. & Barelds, D. P. H. (2008). Self and partner personality and
occur to them. Nonetheless, these responses may provide an responses to relationship threats. Journal of Research in Personality,
index of how subjects tend to react to a comparable situation in 42, 1500–1511.
‘real’ life (Shettel-Neuber, Bryson & Young, 1978). Other meth- Dijkstra, P., Barelds, D. P. H. & Groothof, H. K. (2010). An inventory
ods carry with them a great deal of risk: attempts to create jeal- and update of jealousy-evoking partner behaviours in modern society.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 17, 329–345.
ousy in existing relationships carry with them a great ethical risk Dijkstra, P. & Buunk, B. P. (1998). Jealousy as a function of rival
that may be difficult to justify, whereas attempts to observe natu- characteristics: An evolutionary perspective. Personality and Social
rally occurring incidents of jealousy suffer from a lack of ade- Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1158–1166.
quate experimental control. Nonetheless, we hope our study Dijkstra, P. & Buunk, B. P. (2002). Sex differences in jealousy-evoking
contributes to this goal and invites future studies to further elab- effects of rival characteristics. European Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 32, 829–852.
orate on the jealousy-evoking effect of partner behaviors, espe- Dijkstra, P., Groothof, H. A. K., Poel, G. A., Laverman, T. T. G., Schrier,
cially those regarding a partner’s involvement with media and M. & Buunk, B. P. (2001). Sex differences in the events that elicit
Internet rivals. jealousy among homosexuals. Personal Relationships, 8, 41–54.
Fessler, D. M. T. (2010). Madmen: An evolutionary perspective on anger
and men’s violent responses to transgression. In M. Potegal,
REFERENCES G. Stemmler & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of
anger: Constituent and concomitant biological, psychological, and
Bailey, J. M., Gaulin, S., Agyei, Y. & Gladue, B. A. (1994). Effects of social processes (pp. 361–381). New York: Springer Science + Busi-
gender and sexual orientation on evolutionarily relevant aspects of ness Media.
human mating psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Freeman-Longo, R. E. & Blanchard, G. T. (1998). Sexual abuse in
Psychology, 66, 1081–1093. America: Epidemic of the 21st century. Brandon, VT: Safer Society
Barelds, D. P. H. & Dijkstra, P. (2006). Reactive, anxious and possessive Press.
forms of jealousy and their relation to relationship quality among Hawkins, R. O. (1990). The relationship between culture, personality,
heterosexuals and homosexuals. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, and sexual jealousy in men in heterosexual and homosexual relation-
183–198. ships. Journal of Homosexuality, 19, 67–84.
Bringle, R. G. (1995). Sexual jealousy in the relationships of homosexual Guadagno, R. E. & Sagarin, B. J. (2010). Sex differences in jealousy:
and heterosexual men: 1980 and 1992. Personal Relationships, 2, An evolutionary perspective on online infidelity. Journal of Applied
313–325. Social Psychology, 40, 2636–2655.
Bringle, R. G. & Buunk, B. P. (1985). Jealousy and social behaviour: A Groothof, H. A. K., Dijkstra, P. & Barelds, D. P. H. (2009). Sex differ-
review of person, relationship and situational determinants. In P. ences in jealousy: The case of internet infidelity. Journal of Social
Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 241– and Personal Relationships, 26, 1119–1129.
264). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Harris, C. R. (2002). Sexual and romantic jealousy in heterosexual and
Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as neces- homosexual adults. Psychological Science, 13, 7–12.
sary as love and sex. New York: Free Press. Hertlein, K. M. (2011). Therapeutic dilemmas in treating Internet infidel-
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D. & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex ity. American Journal of Family Therapy, 39, 162–173.
differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Hertlein, K. M. & Piercy, F. P. (2006). Internet infidelity: A critical
Psychological Science, 3, 251–255. review of the literature. The Family Journal, 14, 366–371.
Buunk, B. P. & Dijkstra, P. (2001). Evidence from a homosexual sample Hertlein, K. M. & Stevenson, A. (2010). he seven “As” contributing to
for a sex-specific rival-oriented mechanism: Jealousy as a function of Internet-related intimacy problems: A literature review. Cyberpsychol-
a rival’s physical attractiveness and dominance. Personal Relation- ogy, 4, 1–7.
ships, 8, 391–406. Jones, K. E. & Hertlein, K. M. (2012). Four key dimensions for distin-
Buunk, B. P. & Dijkstra, P. (2004). Gender differences in rival character- guishing internet infidelity from internet and sex addiction: Concepts
istics that evoke jealousy in response to emotional versus sexual infi- and clinical application. American Journal of Family Therapy, 40,
delity. Personal Relationships, 11, 395–408. 115–125.

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations


336 P. Dijkstra et al. Scand J Psychol 54 (2013)

Levy, K. N. & Kelly, K. M. (2010). Sex differences in jealousy: A contri- meta-analytic examination. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33,
bution from attachment theory. Psychological Science, 21, 168–173. 595–614.
Mathiak, K. & Weber, R. (2006). Toward brain correlates of natural Schneider, J. P. (2003). The impact of compulsive cybersex
behaviour: fMRI during violent video games. Human Brain Mapping, behaviours on the family. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 18,
27, 948–956. 329–354.
Mazur, A., Susman, E. J. & Edelbrock, S. (1997). Sex differences in tes- Seattle Organic SEO (2012). Top Google searches in 2012: The most
tosterone response to a video game contest. Evolution and Human Popular keywords study v3. Retrieved from http://seattleorganicseo.
Behavior, 18, 317–326. com/sosblog/top-google-searches-in-2012-the-most-popular-keywords-
Melamed, T. (1991). Individual differences in romantic jealous: The study-version-3/
moderating effect of relationship characteristics. European Journal of Sharpsteen, D. J. (1991). The organization of jealousy knowledge:
Social Psychology, 21, 455–461. Romantic jealousy as a blended emotion. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The
Mileham, B. L. A. (2007). Online infidelity in Internet chat rooms: An psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 31–51). New York: Guilford
ethnographic exploration. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 11–31. Press.
Pam, A. & Pearson, J. (1996). When marriage ends in a love triangle: Sheets, V. L. & Wolfe, M. D. (2001). Sexual jealousy in heterosexuals,
Jealousy, family polarization, effects on children. Journal of Divorce lesbians, and gays. Sex Roles, 44, 255–276.
and Remarriage, 25, 175–199. Shettel-Neuber, J., Bryson, J.B. & Young, L. E. (1978). Physical attrac-
Parrot, W. G. (1991). The emotional experience of envy and jealousy. In tiveness of the ‘other person’ and jealousy. Personality and Social
P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 3–30). Psychology Bulletin, 4, 612–615.
New York: Guilford Press. Snyder, D. K., Baucom, D. H. & Coop Gorden, K. (2007). Getting past
Parrott, W. G. & Smith, R. H. (1993). Distinguishing the experiences of the affair: A program to help you cope, heal, and move on – together
envy and jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, or apart. New York: Guilford Press.
906–920. Vandello, J. A. & Cohen, D. (2003). Male honor and female fidelity:
Peplau, L. & Spalding, L. R. (2000). The close relationships of lesbians, Implicit cultural scripts that perpetuate domestic violence. Journal of
gay men, and bisexuals. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 997–1010.
Close relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 111–123). Thousand Oaks, Whisman, M. A., Dixon, A. E. & Johnson, B. (1997). Therapists’ per-
CA: Sage Publications. spectives of couple problems and treatment issues in couple therapy.
Pinkard, N. (2005). How the perceived masculinity and/or femininity of Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 361–366.
software applications influences students’ software preferences. Whitty, M. (2003). Pushing the wrong buttons: Men’s and women’s
Journal of Educational Computing Research, 32, 57–78. attitudes toward online and offline infidelity. Cyberpsychology &
Robinson, M. D., Johnson, J. T. & Shields, S. A. (2001). The gender Behavior, 6, 569–579.
heuristic and the database: Factors affecting the perception of gender- Young, K. S., Griffin-Shelley, E., Cooper, A., O’Mara, J. & Buchanan,
related differences in the experience and display of emotions. In W. J. (2000). Online infidelity: A new dimension in couple relationships
Parrott (Ed.), Emotions in social psychology: Essential readings (pp. with implications for evaluation and treatment. Sexual Addition and
157–169). New York: Psychology Press. Compulsivity, 7, 59–74.
Sagarin, B. J., Martin, A. L., Coutinho, S. A., Edlund, J. E., Patel, L.,
Skowronski, J. J. & Zengel, B. (2012). Sex differences in jealousy: A Received 4 October 2012, accepted 18 February 2013

© 2013 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations

You might also like