You are on page 1of 14

www.nature.

com/scientificreports

OPEN Neural Correlates of Sexual


Orientation in Heterosexual,
Bisexual, and Homosexual Women
Received: 30 August 2017 Adam Safron1, Victoria Klimaj1, David Sylva2, A. M. Rosenthal2, Meng Li3,4,5, Martin Walter3,4,5
Accepted: 11 December 2017 & J. Michael Bailey   1
Published: xx xx xxxx
We used fMRI to investigate neural correlates of responses to erotic pictures and videos in heterosexual
(N = 26), bisexual (N = 26), and homosexual (N = 24) women, ages 25–50. We focused on the ventral
striatum, an area of the brain associated with desire, extending previous findings from the sexual
psychophysiology literature in which homosexual women had greater category specificity (relative to
heterosexual and bisexual women) in their responses to male and female erotic stimuli. We found that
homosexual women’s subjective and neural responses reflected greater bias towards female stimuli,
compared with bisexual and heterosexual women, whose responses did not significantly differ. These
patterns were also suggested by whole brain analyses, with homosexual women showing category-
specific activations of greater extents in visual and auditory processing areas. Bisexual women tended
to show more mixed patterns, with activations more responsive to female stimuli in sensory processing
areas, and activations more responsive to male stimuli in areas associated with social cognition.

Studies using physiological measures have found that women tend to have non-specific patterns of genital
arousal1–3. That is, in contrast to men, women tend to show similar degrees of arousal to erotic stimuli depicting
either sex. For example, heterosexual women have generally shown equivalent arousal to both erotic stimuli
featuring men and erotic stimuli featuring women. This has been repeatedly demonstrated with vaginal
photoplethysmography3,4. This pattern has also been found using less direct measures such as looking time5, pupil
dilation6, and fMRI7. Notably, homosexual women’s arousal patterns are more category-specific than heterosexual
women’s, although less so than men’s8.
The fact that women’s sexual arousal patterns are less category-specific than men’s has been interpreted as a
potential contributor to gender differences in “erotic plasticity”9, which Baumeister has defined as “the extent to
which sex drive is shaped by social, cultural, and situational factors.”
Baumeister offered three lines of evidence when he initially proposed that women may have greater erotic
plasticity compared with men: (1) women show larger effects of social and cultural factors on sexual attitudes,
desire, and behavior; (2) sexual attitude-behavior consistency is lower in women than in men; (3) individual
women exhibit more variation in sexual behavior across time than men. Women’s less specific arousal patterns
may also contribute to their increased “sexual fluidity”10, which Diamond has defined as an individual’s “capacity
for situation-dependent flexibility in sexual responsiveness, which allows individuals to experience changes in
same-sex or other-sex desire across both short-term and long-term time periods”11,12.
One might hypothesize that arousal patterns of bisexual women should be similar to the non-specific arousal
patterns of heterosexual women; however, studies of women’s arousal patterns have mostly neglected to include
bisexual women. Heterosexual women’s arousal does not appear to favor erotic stimuli of either sex, and thus
may be considered to reflect a bisexual pattern. (We do not mean to imply that heterosexual women are con-
fused or in denial about their “real preferences”; rather, the findings in need of explanation are why heterosex-
ual women show non-heterosexual arousal patterns in the laboratory). The implication of women’s non-specific
arousal patterns for their sexual orientations is difficult to interpret. Most women, like most men, behave and

1
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA. 2Department of Psychiatry, Kaiser
Permanente, Oakland, California, USA. 3Department of Psychiatry, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg,
Germany. 4Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany. 5Department of Psychiatry, Eberhard Karls
University, Tubingen, Germany. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to A.S. (email:
asafron@gmail.com)

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 1


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

identify heterosexually13–18. However, men are more likely than women to identify as completely heterosexual or
completely homosexual, and women are more likely than men to identify as bisexual or “mostly heterosexual”19.
If arousal patterns are similar between heterosexual and bisexual women, the question remains what distin-
guishes the two groups. One possibility, supported by some research, is that bisexual women tend to have greater
sexual motivation, which may increase the likelihood of exploring a capacity for attraction to both sexes20,21.
Or, bisexual women may be more aware than heterosexual women of their non-specific arousal22, which could
partially contribute to bisexual sexual motivation. Alternatively, bisexual women may be more likely than heter-
osexual women to interpret their non-specific arousal states in sexual or romantic terms.
It is also possible that bisexual women’s arousal patterns differ from those observed in heterosexual women.
Consistent with this possibility, recent studies suggest that women with bisexual interests tend to be more aroused
by female than by male erotic stimuli23–25. Perhaps for some women with female-biased arousal patterns, this bias
can motivate non-heterosexual feelings, behavior, and identity.
Interpretations of non-specific arousal patterns in women are further complicated by the fact that female
genital arousal exhibits relatively low correlations with subjectively reported sexual arousal, in contrast to the
high correlations observed in men26. Discrepancies between existing genital and subjective measures indicate that
some women may report substantial subjective arousal without substantial genital arousal, and vice versa. It has
also been suggested that non-specific arousal patterns may not indicate affective responses to erotic stimuli, but
may instead reflect a kind of protective preparatory response27.
Neuroimaging assessments may shed light on the neural systems that are involved in responding to a given
paradigm. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is a neuroimaging approach that allows for the indi-
rect assessment of brain activity by tracking ratios of oxygenated and deoxygenated blood a proxy for neural
firing. When used in the context of presenting erotic stimuli, this non-invasive neural measure could provide a
converging line of evidence for interpreting the genital and subjective arousal findings described above. In this
study, we used fMRI to specifically focused on the “reward system” in order to address the question: to what extent
is there an affective significance to findings from the literature on women’s sexual orientation and genital arousal?
The part of the “reward system” that we focused on is the ventral striatum, a dopamine-sensitive area of
the brain that is a reliable measure of reward-related processing–and in particular, wanting and “incentive
motivation”28,29–including with respect to sexual orientation30. Most neuroimaging studies of sexual response
have focused on men31,32, but the ventral striatum has also been found to reliably activate in studies of women’s
responses to erotic stimuli33–35. However, until now, no studies have measured neural responses to erotic stimuli
in bisexual women.
The present investigation primarily focused on two hypotheses: (1) Homosexual women may show greater
category-specificity than non-homosexual women in brain activity, as suggested by the genital arousal litera-
ture; (2) Bisexual women may show larger biases towards female stimuli, compared with heterosexual women.
We tested these hypotheses with respect to subjective and neural responses to erotic pictures and erotic videos.
We used two different kinds of erotic stimuli because of their potentially non-overlapping strengths and weak-
nesses. Erotic pictures may be particularly well-suited for assessing the initial appraisal of sexual stimuli, but
their brevity may not reflect the kinds of experiences that drive sexuality in the real world. Erotic videos may
allow for the measurement of more intense states, but their extended duration may also provide opportunities for
self-regulatory efforts to modify erotic responses.

Method
Participants.  Participants were 26 heterosexual women, 26 bisexual women, and 24 homosexual women,
recruited from a variety of publicly-posted and online advertisements seeking (paid) volunteers for a neuroim-
aging study of sexual orientation and arousal. Bisexual women were required to have had at least two previous
sexual partners and one romantic partner (of three months or greater duration) of each sex. Homosexual and
heterosexual participants all met these criteria with respect to their respective preferred sexes.
After responding to advertisements, participants were screened for inclusion using online questionnaires.
Participants provided information about sexual orientation, sexual interests, and personality, in addition to
answering screening questions relevant to medical eligibility for fMRI research. Participants were required to be
right handed, non-claustrophobic, free from ferromagnetic implants, and not currently taking psychiatric medi-
cations. Participants were informed of the risks and nature of the study and agreed to participate in all portions of
the research. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Northwestern University and carried
out in accordance with its guidelines. Informed consent was obtained from each participant for every portion of
the study in which they participated.
Participants’ sexual orientation was assessed using self-reported identities (i.e. “Homosexual”/“Gay”,
“Bisexual”/“Bi”, “Heterosexual”/“Straight”), as well as with a modified Kinsey score, which asked participants
about their sexual fantasies throughout adulthood as well as in the past year. The scale ranged from 0 to 6, with 0
corresponding to an exclusively heterosexual orientation and 6 corresponding to an exclusively homosexual ori-
entation. Responses to questions about adulthood and about the past year were averaged to create a Kinsey score
for each participant. The average Kinsey score was 0.8 for heterosexual women (SD = 0.7, range = 0–2), 2.63 for
bisexual women (SD = 0.7, range = 2–4.5), and 5.2 for homosexual women (SD = 0.68, range = 4–6).
Participants’ ages ranged from 21 to 46 years old. Mean ages were 29.7 for heterosexual women (SD = 5.86,
range = 25–46), 30.27 for bisexual women (SD = 6.41, range = 21–48), and 29 for homosexual women (SD = 3.12,
range = 25–38). The sample of 76 participants was racially and ethnically diverse, with 23 non-Caucasian partic-
ipants including two Latina participants, ten African-American participants, four Asian-American participants,
and seven participants who identified otherwise or who identified as multiethnic/multiracial. Groups did not sig-
nificantly differ either with respect to age (F(2,73) = 0.348, p = 0.708) or ethnicity (c2(2, N = 76) = 2.94, p = 0.23).
We also confirmed that ethnicity did not significantly impact responses to the erotic stimuli.

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 2


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Stimuli and Procedure.  Subjects experienced two experimental paradigms in the scanner: first erotic pic-
tures were shown (over a period of ~21 minutes), and then erotic videos were shown (over a period of ~19 min-
utes) after a brief rest period. Picture stimuli were shown before video stimuli for all participants in an attempt
to promote stimulus engagement. That is, it was assumed that potentially less intense stimuli might be better
presented earlier in the experimental session while attentional resources are highest. Further, there was concern
that first showing more intense stimuli would reduce engagement with subsequent stimuli. As such, pictures and
videos stimuli were not counterbalanced with respect to each other.
Participants watched stimuli while laying down with a combination of earplugs (to minimize scanner noise)
and over-ear headphones (for video sound and communication with experimenters). Images were displayed via
projector onto a wall, which was made viewable to participants via an angled mirror placed above the eyes.

Erotic pictures paradigm.  The present study employed a subset of the picture stimuli used in Safron et al.36
and Sylva et al.7. Pictures depicted a nude man, a nude woman, or a same-sex couple (i.e., either two men or two
women) engaged in explicit sexual contact. Erotic stimuli featuring both individual nudes and same-sex pairs
engaging in explicit sexual interaction is common in research on sexual arousal and sexual orientation2,4,37, which
is not the case when stimuli featuring male-female couples is presented. However, erotic stimuli featuring explicit
sexual activity in same-sex couples tends to be substantially more arousing compared with pictures of single
nudes4. Such stimuli are similar to pictures of nude individuals, in the sense that only men or women, but not
both, are depicted in a given picture. Thus, sexual arousal induced by them is relatively unambiguous in terms of
the gender to which participants are responding.
In each of two 10.5-minute runs (ordering counterbalanced), participants viewed 40 erotic pictures featuring
male models and 40 erotic pictures featuring female models. Each picture was shown for 3.5 seconds, followed by
a variable-duration fixation cross presented for either 1.5, 6.5, or 11.5 seconds. Variable-duration baselines were
utilized for superior deconvolution of the BOLD signal in a rapid event-related design for fMRI (in which evoked
signals are never allowed to return to baseline levels). During the presentation of each picture, participants used
buttons held in their right hands to rate that image on a scale of −2 to +2 (respectively: “strongly disliked,”
“disliked,” “liked,” “strongly liked”), with no option of 0 for neutral ratings. Neutral options for ratings were not
provided for the sake of consistency with previous research using the same stimuli. Note: Subjective ratings of
pictures were lost for some participants due to a button-box equipment error.

Erotic videos paradigm.  Following picture assessment, participants were shown six video clips depicting indi-
vidual masturbating men and six video clips depicting individual masturbating women. Depicted individuals
appeared sexually aroused but did not reach orgasm. To estimate baseline responses, six natural landscape videos
were shown.
In each of two 9.25-minute runs (ordering counterbalanced), videos were presented for 15 seconds each, fol-
lowed by a 15-second distraction task requiring participants to indicate via button-press when a number in a
series decreased by an interval other than seven. This task was intended to facilitate a return to emotional and
physiological baseline. 15-second stimulus presentations were chosen as a desirable stimulation period in an
fMRI block design, which can potentially be more sensitive than event-related designs38.
After leaving the scanner, participants viewed the videos once more and provided ratings of each clip. Videos
were rated using a 5-point scale for degree of sexual appeal, ranging from “not at all” (0) to “very much” (4), with
a midpoint of “somewhat” (2).’

fMRI signal extraction methods.  Image acquisition.  A Siemens Trio 3 T magnet and 12-channel RF
head coil were used to collect T2*-weighted gradient-recalled EPI images from the whole brain (32 3-mm slices
with a 0.99-mm interslice gap; TR = 2500 ms; TE = 20 ms; flip angle = 80°; FOV = 200 × 220 mm, 120 × 128
acquisition matrix). Slices were taken along the plane connecting the anterior and posterior commissures, with a
1.72 mm × 1.72 mm × 3.99 mm resolution, with more refined axial dimensions intended to produce less distor-
tion and signal dropout in sub-cortical areas, although possibly at the expense of signal-to-noise ratio. During
each picture run, 250 whole-brain volumes were collected, and during each video run, 220 whole-brain volumes
were collected, with the first four volumes discarded to account for initial magnetization effects. For anatomical
localization, a structural MRI scan consisting of T1-weighted images was conducted after the testing runs (160
1-mm axial slices; TR = 2.1 ms; TE = 4.38 ms; flip angle = 15°; FOV = 220 mm; 256 × 192 matrix).

Image pre-processing.  Image pre-processing and analysis was performed using SPM 12b (Wellcome Trust Centre
for Neuroimaging, London, UK), and implemented in Matlab v 8.1.604 (The MathWorks Inc., MA, USA).
Functional (EPI) volumes were first corrected for slice timing. Each participant’s volumes were then regis-
tered to the mean slice, after which the registered volumes were resliced, used to create a mean resliced image,
and then co-registered to the structural (T1) image. All EPI images, including the mean resliced image, as well
as the structural (T1) scans were then spatially normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and
re-sampled to 3 × 3 × 3 mm (27 mm3) resolution. Normalized functional images were then smoothed to an 8 mm
full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

Signal to noise ratio and head coverage exclusions.  To exclude participants with poor signal due to either head
motion or scanner conditions, average signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) over time was calculated for each subject (after
preprocessing, using a mask that included only voxels with appreciable EPI signal). The SNR ratio for each voxel
(mean divided by standard deviation) was averaged across all voxels in the brain39. Participants whose picture
data SNR was less than one standard deviation below the mean were excluded from picture analyses. Similarly,

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 3


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 1.  Mask used as the ventral striatum (VS) ROI, drawn using an average brain in the WFU PickAtlas
toolbox for SPM 8. MNI coordinates displayed: x = 0, y = 17, z = −8.

participants whose video data SNR was less than one standard deviation below the mean were excluded from
video analyses.
Based on these criteria, fourteen participants (five heterosexual, five bisexual, and four homosexual) were
excluded from fMRI and subjective picture analyses, and sixteen participants (six heterosexual, six bisexual, and
four homosexual) were excluded from fMRI and subjective video analyses. After exclusions were performed for
SNR, we included a total of twenty-one heterosexual women, twenty-one bisexual women, and twenty homosex-
ual women in fMRI picture analyses. Video analyses after SNR exclusion included eighteen heterosexual women,
eighteen bisexual women, and twenty homosexual women. To check the validity of our SNR criterion, head
motion plots were visually inspected for all participants (Parrish, et al.39). Excluded participants had highly var-
iable head positions as compared to included participants. An additional validity-check was performed using
evoked responses to erotic pictures minus a fixation-cross baseline. Excluded participants had substantially
reduced activity in visual cortices as compared to included participants.
An additional thirty-two participants (twelve heterosexual, twelve bisexual, and eight homosexual) were
excluded from subjective picture rating analyses due to insufficient subjective data resulting from a button-box
equipment error. Five participants (three bisexual and two homosexual) were excluded from subjective video
analyses for the same reason. Thus, after exclusions were performed for insufficient subjective data, we included
a total of nine heterosexual women, nine bisexual women, and twelve homosexual women in subjective picture
analyses, and twenty heterosexual women, seventeen bisexual women, and eighteen homosexual women in sub-
jective video analyses.
For whole-brain analyses, mean functional scans were individually examined to identify participants with
substantial cutoffs in head coverage. As a result, one heterosexual female who had substantial frontal lobe cutoff
was excluded from whole-brain analyses in addition to those participants excluded for SNR.

First-level analyses.  For both the video and picture assessments, a standard general linear model (GLM)40 was
used in identifying hemodynamic changes for each participant, and a high-pass filter (cutoff 128 s) was used to
remove low-frequency temporal noise.
Estimated average activity was calculated for each participant’s separate responses to male pictures, female
pictures, male videos, and female videos (contrasted with fixation cross for pictures and neutral nature scenes for
videos). These estimates were used for region of interest analyses. For whole-brain analyses, estimated average
activity was also calculated for each participant’s response to male compared with female pictures and videos. For
both the picture and video assessments, each participant’s responses to each stimulus contrast of interest were
concatenated within stimulus type, using data from both the 1st and 2nd runs.
Ventral striatum region of interest analyses. An a priori region of interest (ROI) analysis was performed on
the ventral striatum—centered on the nucleus accumbens—as this was the area most likely to indicate desire.
The ventral striatum and hypothalamus are the only two areas that have been shown to be specifically associated
with sexual (as opposed to general) arousal41,42. We focused on the ventral striatum because it likely has higher
validity for reflecting sexual incentive value compared with the hypothalamus, which contains a variety of nuclei
with heterogeneous functions (including sexual arousal) that would be difficult to disambiguate with the limited
spatial resolution of 3 T fMRI.
The ventral striatum ROI mask used in the present study was drawn on an MNI template brain using the WFU
PickAtlas toolbox for SPM 843. It was anatomically defined as a dilated intersection of the ventral anterior caudate
and putamen. The resulting ventral striatum ROI is shown in Fig. 1.
Estimates of average ventral striatum activity for each participant were extracted using the MarsBar toolbox
for SPM844. Extracted ventral striatum ROI data were analyzed using JMP Pro v11 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Planned contrasts and within-group tests.  We constructed separate dependent variables for each com-
bination of stimulus type (i.e. picture or video) and response type (i.e., subjective or ventral striatum activation) by
subtracting response to female stimuli from response to male stimuli. That is, we constructed dependent variables
for 1) subjective response to pictures, 2) subjective response to videos, 3) ventral striatum activation to pictures,
and 4) ventral striatum responses to videos, each of which reflected responses to male stimuli minus responses to
female stimuli. We refer to this as the Male-Female contrast.
Because there were three groups (i.e., heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual women), two orthogo-
nal between-groups contrasts were constructed to examine what we believe to be the most interesting pair of

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 4


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Variable: Male – Female


β t(df) p
PICTURES
Subjective Ratings
Homosexual vs. non-homosexual −0.489 −2.60 (28) 0.015*
Bisexual vs. heterosexual 0.094 0.4 (28) 0.692
Ventral Striatum Activation
Homosexual vs. non-homosexual −0.462 −3.32 (59) 0.002**
Bisexual vs. heterosexual −0.194 −1.22 (59) 0.226
VIDEOS
Subjective Ratings
Homosexual vs. non-homosexual −0.820 −5.80 (55) <0.001***
Bisexual vs. heterosexual −0.373 −2.29 (55) 0.026*
Ventral Striatum Activation
Homosexual vs. non-homosexual −0.199 −1.38 (57) 0.172
Bisexual vs. heterosexual −0.114 −0.688 (57) 0.494

Table 1.  Planned contrasts comparing women of different orientation groups. *Significant p-value < 0.05. 
**Signifcant p-value < 0.01. ***Significant p-value < 0.001.

independent questions based on previous literature45. The first question was whether homosexual women differed
from the other two groups in their Male-Female contrasts. The second question was whether bisexual women dif-
fered from heterosexual women in their Male-Female contrasts. The use of orthogonal planned contrasts allowed
us to test these hypotheses with maximum statistical power while simultaneously minimizing the number of
overall comparisons.
Within-group t-tests were also performed separately in each group in order to characterize relative responding
to male and female stimuli.

Whole-brain analyses.  Finally, we examined overall patterns of differential activation in response to male
compared with female erotic stimuli across the entire brain. If bisexual and heterosexual women have less specific
arousal patterns, then they are likely to exhibit less extensive differential activity between male and female stimuli
compared with the activity patterns expected for homosexual women.
Tests of average group responses to stimulus conditions were performed using one-sample contrasts. Each
group (heterosexual women, bisexual women, and homosexual women) was tested individually for clusters of
greater activity for male stimuli compared with female stimuli, and female stimuli compared with male stimuli,
using a corrected statistical threshold (p < 0.05 FWE).
For these analyses, cluster reports were generated in SPM. Peak activations and cluster extents (extent thresh-
old k = 5) were visually examined as overlays on slice and render maps. Neuroanatomical descriptions were deter-
mined based on agreement between two trained investigators, and checked against designations from the WFU
Atlas (Maldjian et al., 2003).

Data availability statement.  The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available
from the corresponding author on request.

Results
Between-group planned contrasts.  As previously described, planned comparisons for the ventral stria-
tum ROI were conducted via multiple regression using two orthogonal between-groups contrasts: one compar-
ing homosexual women with heterosexual and bisexual women, and one comparing heterosexual with bisexual
women. Separate analyses were conducted for each of the Male-Female contrasts (i.e., responses to female stimuli
subtracted from responses to male stimuli). Results are presented in Table 1.

Homosexual versus non-homosexual women.  Subjective ratings. Compared with non-homosexual women,
homosexual women had significantly more negative (i.e., gynephilic) Male-Female contrasts for both pictures
(p = 0.015) and videos (p < 0.001). That is, homosexual women showed a greater preference for pictures and
videos of females relative to males, compared with both bisexual and heterosexual women.
Ventral striatum activation patterns. Homosexual women had significantly more female-biased ventral stria-
tum responses compared to non-homosexual women for pictures (p = 0.002), but not videos.

Bisexual versus heterosexual women.  We compared heterosexual and bisexual women’s subjective and ventral
striatum responses to erotic pictures and videos, and observed only one significant difference: for video stimuli,
bisexual women had significantly more female-preferring subjective responses than did heterosexual women
(p = 0.026).

Within-group tests comparing responses to male and female erotic stimuli.  Figure 2 (show-
ing the distribution of Male-Female contrasts for the three groups) shows that heterosexual women exhibited

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 5


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 2.  Within-group male – female (male minus female) stimuli difference scores for subjective ratings and
ventral striatum (VS) responses, by sexual orientation. Difference scores are defined as a participant’s average
response to stimuli depicting males minus average response to stimuli depicting females. Points represent
individual participants. Horizontal bars indicate group means and 95% confidence intervals of the means.
Horizontal lines at 0 indicate no difference between ratings to erotic stimuli depicting each sex. (a) Difference
scores for subjective ratings of picture stimuli. (b) Difference scores for VS activation evoked by picture stimuli.
(c) Difference scores for subjective ratings of video stimuli. (d) Difference scores for VS activation evoked by
video stimuli. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

a non-significant trend (p = 0.079) towards favoring female erotic pictures compared with male erotic pictures,
and had no differentiation between stimulus sex for other tests. Bisexual women also did not subjectively differ-
entiate among stimulus types based on sex, although they did exhibit (non-significant) marginal female-favoring
ventral striatum scores for picture (p = 0.063) and video (p = 0.054) stimuli. Homosexual women, in contrast to
heterosexual and bisexual women, showed clear favoring of female stimuli as assessed by subjective liking of
pictures (p < 0.001), appeal ratings of videos (p < 0.001), as well as in ventral striatum responses to pictures
(p = 0.003) and non-significantly for ventral striatum responses to videos (p = 0.073). Note that these results are
presented descriptively. Inferences about differences among the three groups depend on the tests presented in
Table 1.

Whole brain tests comparing responses to male and female erotic stimuli.  Note: Activation pat-
terns are described in greater detail in the discussion, with interpretations of possible functional significances.

Picture stimuli.  Comparing activation to female versus male erotic pictures, heterosexual women exhibited rel-
atively greater activity for female pictures in occipital (i.e., visual) and occipitotemporal cortices, with no brain
areas showing significantly greater activation for male pictures (Fig. 3; Table 2). Bisexual women also showed
greater activity in visual cortices for female relative to male pictures, but they showed greater activity for male
pictures in other areas including supramarginal and angular gyri, as well as the posterior cingulate. Homosexual
women exhibited significant activations for female compared with male pictures in visual cortex, parietal lobes,
and parahippocampal cortex, but with no brain areas showing significantly greater activation for male pictures.

Video stimuli.  When viewing female compared with male erotic videos (Fig. 4; Table 3), all groups showed activ-
ity in bilateral superior temporal cortices, likely indicating an auditory confound in which more extensive and
substantial vocalizations were present in female erotic videos46. However, this effect appeared to vary by sexual
orientation, with homosexual women showing the most extensive and robust evoked activity (peak T = 14.22)
compared with heterosexual (peak T = 11.71) and bisexual women (peak T = 8.83). In the opposite direction of
greater responses to male compared with female erotic videos, heterosexual and bisexual (but not homosexual)

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 6


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 3.  Differential brain activations towards male and female pictures in heterosexual, bisexual, and
homosexual women. Whole brain activations are shown for the male picture minus female picture contrasts
(with brain activation evoked by viewing neutral stimuli subtracted from activations toward the erotic pictures).
Height threshold is set at p < 0.05 FWE with a cluster threshold of k = 5. Axial slice 31, sagittal slice 50, and
coronal slice 38 are shown for all groups.

women exhibited activations in occipital cortices. While all groups had greater activity towards male videos
in (anterior) superior parietal cortices, these activations appeared to be more extensive and robust in bisexual
women (peak T = 11.55) compared with heterosexual (peak T = 7.64) and homosexual women (peak T = 7.99).

Discussion
In this fMRI study of female sexual orientation—the first to include bisexual women—we extended several key
findings from the sexual psychophysiology literature1,2,47,48. Using the ventral striatum as a neural measure of
incentive motivation, we demonstrated that homosexual women have greater gender bias in their responses to
male and female erotic stimuli.

Main findings: subjective and ventral striatum responses to male and female erotic stimuli. 
Direct comparisons of bisexual and heterosexual women revealed no significant differences, with the exception
of bisexual women having more gynephilic subjective responses to erotic videos. However, bisexual and heter-
osexual women did not differ with respect to their ventral striatum responses toward these stimuli. When con-
trasted to bisexual and heterosexual women, homosexual women showed distinctly greater bias toward female
stimuli in both their subjective responses to videos and pictures, and also in their ventral striatum responses to
pictures. In sum, our planned contrast findings are consistent with the genital arousal literature in which more
category-specific responses were observed in homosexual women2,4.
Another set of tests, comparing male vs. female stimuli within each group, revealed that neither bisexual
nor heterosexual women were significantly biased toward stimuli depicting males or stimuli depicting females.
This was true both in ventral striatum response and in subjective arousal, for both picture and video stimuli.
Homosexual women, however, were uniquely gynephilic (i.e., female-preferring), with significantly greater
responses to female stimuli for subjective responses to pictures, subjective responses to videos, and ventral stria-
tum responses to pictures. This gynephilic bias in homosexual women was consistent with our direct comparisons
and previous literature.
Our findings are only partially consistent with observations from the genital arousal literature in which homo-
sexual and bisexual women both had gynephilic responses, but where heterosexual women had non-specific
responses23–25. We found significant biases in ventral striatum responses toward female stimuli among homo-
sexual women, but with more indifferent patterns among heterosexual and bisexual women. However, with one
exception—bisexual women showing more gynephilic subjective responses to erotic videos than did heterosexual
women, in a direct comparison—heterosexual and bisexual women’s patterns of results did not differ significantly.

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 7


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

R/L Region BA MNI voxels peak T


Heterosexual Women
    Female > Male Pictures
      L lingual gyrus, inferolateral occipital 18, 19 (−33 −76 −10) 21 8.01
      R fusiform gyrus 37 (30 −67 −7) 26 7.81
      R inferolateral occipital, fusiform gyrus 19, 37 (42 −70 −16) 7.78
      R fusiform gyrus 37 (30 −58 −10) 7.59
    Male > Female Pictures: no differential activations
Bisexual Women
    Female > Male Pictures
      R inferior occipital cortex, middle occipital gyrus, lingual gyrus 18, 19 (36 −70 −10) 86 10.68
      R middle occipital gyrus 19 (48 −64 −10) 7.96
      R fusiform gyrus 37 (33 −55 −13) 7.15
      L inferolateral occipital, middle occipital gyrus 19, 18 (−39 −85 −7) 138 9.07
      L middle occipital gyrus 18, 19 (−30 −88 5) 8.47
      L inferior occipital gyrus, primary visual cortex, lingual gyrus 18, 17 (−24 −94 −4) 8.06
      R occipital cortex, middle occipital gyrus 19 (30 −79 17) 43 9.03
      R occipital cortex, middle occipital gyrus 19 (30 −82 26) 7.38
      L inferior occipital cortex, lingual gyrus 18 (−24 −85 −13) 5 8.22
      R primary visual cortex, cuneus 17 (15 −100 −4) 12 8.09
      R middle occipital gyrus 18 (42 −82 −1) 27 8.05
      R middle occipital gyrus 18 (27 −85 2) 7.62
      R middle occipital gyrus 18 (33 −91 8) 6.86
    Male > Female Pictures
      L angular gyrus, supramarginal gyrus 39, 40 (−51 −64 41) 21 9.37
      R/L posterior cingulate 23 (0 −22 35) 10 8.23
      R angular gyrus 39 (48 −61 35) 27 7.94
       R supramarginal gyrus 40 (51 −61 44) 7.75
      R retrosplenial cingulate 30 (15 −52 29) 6 7.68
      R supramarginal gyrus 40 (57 −49 35) 5 7.38
      L supramarginal gyrus 40 (−36 −61 44) 5 7.08
Homosexual Women
    Female > Male Pictures
      L middle occipital gyrus 19 (−27 −82 14) 139 10.01
      L middle occipital gyrus 18, 19 (−33 −88 −1) 8.99
      L middle occipital gyrus, primary visual cortex 18, 17 (−18 −97 2) 8.57
      R middle occipital gyrus 19 (33 −79 14) 89 9.83
      R inferior lateral occipital cortex 19 (42 −85 2) 8.19
      R middle occipital gyrus 19 (33 −70 8) 7.78
      R fusiform gyrus, posterior paraphippocampal gyrus 37 (33 −52 −10) 36 9.56
      R fusiform gyrus, middle occipital gyrus 18 (33 −70 −13) 8.03
      R primary visual cortex 17 (18 −94 −1) 11 8.56
      L fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, posterior parahippocampal gyrus 37 (−33 −61 −7) 26 8.45
       L fusiform gyrus 37 (−39 −61 −13) 7.62
      R precuneus, occipitoparietal 7 (24 −73 41) 6 7.18
    Male > Female Pictures: no differential activations

Table 2.  Differential whole-brain activations in response to male vs. female pictures.

Our a priori tests in the ventral striatum allowed us to explore whether women of different sexual orientations
also exhibited different degrees of incentive motivation toward male and female erotic stimuli. But fMRI also
provides the ability to look at activation patterns across the entire brain, potentially allowing for a more detailed
characterization of the neural systems involved. Below we review activation patterns for each group in viewing
male compared with female erotic stimuli, along with some reverse inferences as to their functional significance.

Whole brain responses to erotic pictures.  For heterosexual women viewing erotic pictures, activity
was greater for female relative to male stimuli bilaterally in lateral occipital cortices, likely indicative of visual
attention49,50, as well as in right-lateralized fusiform cortex, potentially suggesting face or body processing51,52.
In no brain areas did heterosexual women have significantly greater activation for male relative to female erotic
pictures. Rather, they seemed to have a somewhat gynephilic pattern of visual attention, consistent with results

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 8


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 4.  Differential brain activations between male and female videos in heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual
women. Whole brain activations are shown for the male video minus female video contrasts (with brain activation
evoked by viewing neutral stimuli subtracted from activation toward the erotic videos). Height threshold is set at
p < 0.05 FWE with a cluster threshold of k = 5. Axial slice 37, sagittal slice 61, and coronal slice 38 are shown for all
groups.

from eye-tracking and looking-time studies in which heterosexual women attended to erotic characteristics of
female pictures53,54.
Bisexual women showed more activity in response to female (relative to male) erotic pictures throughout
the visual system, including fusiform cortex, which (as described above) is often associated with perception of
faces and bodies55. Patterns were similar to those observed in heterosexual women (and presumably with similar
functional significances), but with larger spatial extents of activation. Although bisexual and heterosexual women
were not directly contrasted, this more extensive visual activation could be taken as support for somewhat greater
gynephilic interest on the part of bisexual women, consistent with ventral striatum activation patterns.
For bisexual women viewing erotic pictures, activity was greater for male relative to female stimuli in posterior
midcingulate and right retrosplenial cingulate cortices, potentially suggesting greater perceptual salience and
emotional memory for male erotic stimuli56–59. Additional male-biased activations were identified bilaterally in
supramarginal and angular gyri, indicating processes relating to mental imagery, or possibly mentalizing60–64.
Thus, in contrast to heterosexual participants, bisexual women showed greater activity towards male (relative
to female) erotic pictures in affect-related brain areas. In this way, it seems that it would be overly simplistic to
say that bisexual women are similar to heterosexual women, but with the addition of gynephilic interest. Rather,
bisexual women seem to have greater responses to both male and female erotic stimuli, depending on the brain area
being considered. Patterns of greater overall responsiveness are consistent with suggestions that bisexual women
may be distinguished by having overall greater degrees of sexual motivation relative to heterosexual women20,21.
It is also notable that bisexual women uniquely showed greater activations to male stimuli in areas of the brain
implicated in higher-order cognition, including mentalizing. Speculatively, these activations could be related to
more complex processing of sexual motivation in bisexual women65. To the degree that these activation patterns
in bisexual women actually specifically reflect social cognition, the question remains open as to why this may be
more likely to be observed in bisexual but not heterosexual or homosexual women.
For homosexual women viewing erotic pictures, greater activations for female (relative to male) stimuli
extended throughout the visual system, with additional clusters in occipitotemporal cortices. Clusters in the right
inferior precuneus may have indicated mental imagery66,67, and clusters in posterior parahippocampal cortex may
have indicated either memory encoding or retrieval68,69. For homosexual women, no brain areas had significantly
greater activation for male relative to female erotic pictures. Thus, homosexual women were the only group that
exhibited an overall pattern of differential brain activity (between male and female sexual stimuli) greater only for
pictures depicting their preferred gender.

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 9


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

R/L Region BA MNI voxels peak T


Heterosexual Women
    Female > Male Videos
      L superior temporal gyrus, primary and secondary auditory cortex 22, 41, 42 (−57 −25 8) 124 11.71
      L superior temporal gyrus, primary and secondary auditory cortex 22, 41, 42 (−42 −31 11) 10.15
      L superior temporal gyrus 22 (−66 −28 8) 8.99
      R superior temporal gyrus, primary and secondary auditory cortex 22, 41, 42 (54 −16 −1) 48 10.42
    Male > Female Videos
      L primary visual cortex, lingual gyrus 17, 18 (−9 −82 2) 30 10.14
      R inferolateral postcentral gyrus 3 (60 −16 29) 5 7.94
      R superior parietal lobule 5 (33 −49 62) 5 7.64
Bisexual Women
    Female > Male Videos
      L superior temporal, primary and secondary auditory cortex 22, 41, 42 (−54 −10 5) 22 8.83
      R superior temporal gyrus 22 (63 −25 2) 8 7.26
    Male > Female Videos
      R superior parietal lobule, extending into supramarginal gyrus 7, 5, 40 (36 −46 59) 211 11.55
      R postcentral gyrus 3, 1, 2 (36 −37 53) 11.27
      R postcentral sulcus 1, 2 (33 −31 41) 9.51
      L postcentral gyrus, postcentral sulcus 3, 1, 2 (−36 −37 56) 25 8.45
      L superior parietal lobule 5, 7 (−33 −49 59) 7.55
      R supplementary motor area 6 (30 −10 59) 12 8.14
       L supplementary motor area 6 (−24 −13 62) 5 7.73
      L cuneus 17 (−9 −82 2) 9 7.6
Homosexual Women
    Female > Male Videos
      R superior temporal gyrus 22 (51 −10 2) 578 14.22
      R primary and secondary auditory cortex, superior temporal gyrus 41 42, 22 (54 −19 5) 13.91
      R superior temporal gyrus 22 (60 −1 −4) 13.26
      L superior temporal gyrus 22 (−63 −19 2) 247 12.7
      L primary and secondary auditory cortex 41, 42 (−45 −25 8) 11.97
      L superior temporal gyrus 22 (−66 −28 5) 10.99
     Male > Female Videos
      R postcentral gyrus 3 (33 −34 47) 7 7.99
      R postcentral sulcus 2 (42 −28 41) 5 7.76

Table 3.  Differential whole-brain activations in response to male vs. female videos.

Whole brain responses to erotic videos.  For heterosexual women viewing erotic videos, activity was
greater for female relative to male stimuli in bilateral superior temporal cortices likely indicating an auditory
confound deriving from more extensive and substantial vocalizations being present in female erotic videos46.
Activity was greater for male relative to female videos in posterior occipital cortex, likely indicating enhanced
visual attention70. Further clusters (greater for male compared with female videos) in the inferolateral postcentral
gyrus and parietal somatosensory association areas may have indicated awareness of bodily sensations, possibly
related to sexual imagery71,72.
For bisexual women viewing erotic videos, activations were greater for female (relative to male) stimuli in
superior temporal cortices, likely indicating the same auditory-related activity present in heterosexual women.
Bisexual women’s brain activity was greater for male (relative to female) erotic videos in occipital cortex, likely
indicating visual attention49,51. Male-biased activations in somatosensory cortices may have indicated processing
of bodily sensations73, and further activations in bilateral superior parietal lobules, premotor and supplementary
motor cortices, and right supramarginal gyrus may have indicated mental imagery or possibly mirroring with the
actors shown in the videos74–78.
Similar to the findings for erotic pictures, bisexual women were unique in the degree to which male videos
produced activations in brain areas associated with more abstract (and possibly complex) processing79,80. Again, the
significance of this pattern remains unclear.
For homosexual women viewing female relative to male erotic videos, activity in superior temporal corti-
ces likely indicated the same auditory-related processing observed in heterosexual and bisexual women, albeit
more robustly and extensively, consistent with enhanced attention to emotionally salient stimulus features. When
viewing male relative to female erotic videos, activations in the right somatosensory cortex may have indicated
processing of bodily sensations73, which may have been either positive or negative in valence. Thus, while emo-
tionally associated brain areas did not exhibit differential activations for videos, homosexual women’s particularly

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 10


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

strong engagement of auditory cortices for female stimuli provided yet further evidence of uniquely gender-biased
responding, relative to heterosexual and bisexual women.

Comparisons with previous findings.  Few studies have investigated the category-specificity of brain
activity in non-heterosexual women. Ponseti et al.81 found that both heterosexual and homosexual women
showed gender-specific patterns of brain activity in multiple areas, including the ventral striatum. Sylva et al.7
also found some evidence for category-specific responding in women, although not in the ventral striatum, and
without specifically testing whether or not heterosexual or homosexual women differed in their responses.
The patterns observed Ponseti et al.81 stand in contrast to the present investigation in which homosexual
women tended to be the only group showing strongly category-specific responses to erotic stimuli. One possible
interpretations for their findings of category-specific responses in all women was the unusual nature of the stimuli
(i.e., close-up images of male and female genitalia, isolated from interpersonal contextual factors)82. As suggested
by Chivers (2017)48, it may be the case that sex and gender cues can produce specific responses in heterosexual
women, but that these are usually trumped by contextual factors in driving arousal responses in women. The
stimuli utilized in the present study contained contextual factors (e.g. body posture, facial expression) that are
more typical of those found in the genital arousal literature.
However, it should be noted that the present study did not find support for greater category-specificity in
homosexual women across all stimulus conditions. Rather, planned contrasts in the ventral striatum only revealed
significant group differences between homosexual and non-homosexual women for erotic pictures. There were no
significant differences in ventral striatum response between homosexual and non-homosexual women for erotic
videos (even though subjective evaluations of those stimuli did significantly differ across the groups).
This pattern of differing results for pictures versus videos may be related to differences in how individu-
als respond to these stimuli, limitations of our video paradigm, or both. While erotic videos may theoretically
allow for the assessment of qualitatively different states of sexual response, it may be the case that incentive
motivation is greatest when stimuli are first presented, but then diminishes with longer stimulus presentations83.
Additionally, erotic pictures may have been more effective at driving ventral striatum responses due to fac-
tors such as unpredictably varied presentation times of preferred stimuli contributing to larger magnitude
reward-prediction errors84,85.

Limitations.  One limitation of nearly all studies of erotic responses in women—including this one—is a fail-
ure to control for hormonal conditions or contraceptive usage. By default, it can generally be assumed that most
women were not measured within the ovulatory window, when responses to erotic stimuli might be greatest35,86.
Additionally, a number of women may have been using hormonal contraceptives. Measuring women’s responses
outside of the fertile phase of their cycles—or while they were using hormonal contraceptives87—may have
yielded a restricted range of arousal responses. However, the specificity of erotic responding has not been shown
to be influenced by menstrual cycle in previous studies of genital arousal48.
Another source of potential limitations may have been the nature of the stimuli used. Though our stimuli
were pilot-tested and rated by individuals of different sexual orientations in order to confirm that they would
appeal to a broad participant sample, it is never possible to ensure that common stimuli will evoke the responses
intended. This may be especially true for something as emotionally salient and individual as sexual arousal. Thus,
it is possible that category specificity patterns could appear to be different if stimuli better reflected participants’
subjective preferences. This is a limitation of many studies of sexual responding, although data gleaned from more
individualized stimulus sets are difficult to interpret.
One more aspect of the stimuli that is difficult to control for is sensory details that are inherently different
between male and female stimuli. Differences in actors’ vocalizations (for videos), actors’ body positions (for both
videos and pictures), and actors’ body motions (in the videos) were present (on average) between male and female
stimuli. These features are difficult to control for and could conceivably lead to differences in both subjective and
neural responding when viewing male vs. female stimuli, especially in more primary sensory areas of the brain
such as visual and auditory cortices. However, such differences may also serve to reinforce the gendered nature of
the stimuli and improve their correspondence with real-world experiences and real-world arousal.

Conclusions
Though the neural data presented here align with previously-observed patterns in women’s genital and subjective
arousal, much remains unknown about the relationship between arousal patterns, orientation, and the develop-
ment of sexual motivation towards particular sexes in women. Our study supports past findings indicating that
women tend not to have strongly category-specific responses to erotic stimuli, with homosexual women showing
somewhat greater specificity than heterosexual and bisexual women. Future research should explore the extent to
which women’s non-specific sexual response contributes to erotic plasticity (i.e., change with context) and sexual
fluidity (i.e., change over time)9,10,88.

References
1. Chivers, M. L. & Bailey, J. M. A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biol. Psychol. 70, 115–120 (2005).
2. Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E. & Bailey, J. M. A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychol. Sci. 15, 736–744
(2004).
3. Chivers, M. L. & Timmers, A. D. Effects of gender and relationship context in audio narratives on genital and subjective sexual
response in heterosexual women and men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41, 185–197 (2012).
4. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C. & Blanchard, R. Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus
gender of actors in sexual films. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 1108–1121 (2007).
5. Lippa, R. A. Effects of sex and sexual orientation on self-reported attraction and viewing times to images of men and women: testing
for category specificity. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41, 149–160 (2012).

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 11


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

6. Rieger, G. & Savin-Williams, R. C. The Eyes Have It: Sex and Sexual Orientation Differences in Pupil Dilation Patterns. PLoS ONE
7, e40256 (2012).
7. Sylva, D. et al. Neural correlates of sexual arousal in heterosexual and homosexual women and men. Horm. Behav. 64, 673–684
(2013).
8. Rieger, G., Savin-Williams, R. C., Chivers, M. L. & Bailey, J. M. Sexual arousal and masculinity-femininity of women. J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 111, 265–283 (2016).
9. Baumeister, R. F. Gender differences in erotic plasticity: the female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychol. Bull. 126,
347–374; discussion 385–389 (2000).
10. Safron, A. & Hoffmann, H. What Does Sexual Responsiveness to One’s Nonpreferred Sex Mean? Arch. Sex. Behav. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10508-017-0954-0 (2017).
11. Diamond, L. M. Sexual Fluidity in Male and Females. Curr. Sex. Health Rep. 8, 249–256 (2016).
12. Diamond, L. M., Dickenson, J. A. & Blair, K. L. Stability of Sexual Attractions Across Different Timescales: The Roles of Bisexuality
and Gender. Arch. Sex. Behav. 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0860-x (2016).
13. Chandra, A., Mosher, W. D., Copen, C. & Sionean, C. Sexual behavior, sexual attraction, and sexual identity in the United States: data
from the 2006-2008 National Survey of Family Growth. Natl. Health Stat. Rep. 1–36 (2011).
14. Kinnish, K., Strassberg, D. & Turner, C. Sex Differences in the Flexibility of Sexual Orientation: A Multidimensional Retrospective
Assessment. Arch. Sex. Behav. 34, 173–183 (2005).
15. LeVay, S. Gay, Straight, and the Reason Why: The Science of Sexual Orientation. (Oxford University Press, USA, 2010).
16. McCabe, J., Brewster, K. L. & Tillman, K. H. Patterns and correlates of same-sex sexual activity among U.S. teenagers and young
adults. Perspect. Sex. Reprod. Health 43, 142–150 (2011).
17. Mock, S. E. & Eibach, R. P. Stability and Change in Sexual Orientation Identity Over a 10-Year Period in Adulthood. Arch. Sex.
Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9761-1 (2011).
18. Savin-Williams, R. C., Joyner, K. & Rieger, G. Prevalence and Stability of Self-Reported Sexual Orientation Identity During Young
Adulthood. Arch. Sex. Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9913-y (2012).
19. Bailey, J. M. et al. Sexual Orientation, Controversy, and Science. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest J. Am. Psychol. Soc. 17, 45–101 (2016).
20. Lippa, R. A. Is high sex drive associated with increased sexual attraction to both sexes? It depends on whether you are male or
female. Psychol. Sci. 17, 46–52 (2006).
21. Lippa, R. A. The relation between sex drive and sexual attraction to men and women: a cross-national study of heterosexual,
bisexual, and homosexual men and women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 209–222 (2007).
22. Suschinsky, K. D., Dawson, S. J. & Chivers, M. L. Assessing the Relationship Between Sexual Concordance, Sexual Attractions, and
Sexual Identity in Women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 46, 179–192 (2017).
23. Bouchard, K. N., Timmers, A. D. & Chivers, M. L. Gender-Specificity of Genital Response and Self-Reported Sexual Arousal in
Women Endorsing Facets of Bisexuality. J. Bisexuality 15, 180–203 (2015).
24. Chivers, M. L., Bouchard, K. N. & Timmers, A. D. Straight but Not Narrow; Within-Gender Variation in the Gender-Specificity of
Women’s Sexual Response. PLOS ONE 10, e0142575 (2015).
25. Timmers, A. D., Bouchard, K. N. & Chivers, M. L. Effects of Gender and Sexual Activity Cues on the Sexual Responses of Women
with Multidimensionally Defined Bisexuality. J. Bisexuality 15, 154–179 (2015).
26. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., Lalumière, M. L., Laan, E. & Grimbos, T. Agreement of self-reported and genital measures of sexual
arousal in men and women: a meta-analysis. Arch. Sex. Behav. 39, 5–56 (2010).
27. Suschinsky, K. D. & Lalumière, M. L. Prepared for Anything?: An Investigation of Female Genital Arousal in Response to Rape Cues.
Psychol. Sci. J. Am. Psychol. Soc. APS. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610394660 (2010).
28. Haber, S. N. & Knutson, B. The Reward Circuit: Linking Primate Anatomy and Human Imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26
(2010).
29. Toates, F. An integrative theoretical framework for understanding sexual motivation, arousal, and behavior. J. Sex Res. 46, 168–193
(2009).
30. Safron, A. et al. Neural Correlates of Sexual Orientation in Heterosexual, Bisexual, and Homosexual Men. Sci. Rep. 7, 41314 (2017).
31. Georgiadis, J. & Kringelbach, M. L. The human sexual response cycle: brain imaging evidence linking sex to other pleasures. Prog.
Neurobiol. 98, 49–81 (2012).
32. Stoléru, S., Fonteille, V., Cornélis, C., Joyal, C. & Moulier, V. Functional neuroimaging studies of sexual arousal and orgasm in
healthy men and women: A review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1481–1509 (2012).
33. Arnow, B. A. et al. Women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder compared to normal females: a functional magnetic resonance
imaging study. Neuroscience 158, 484–502 (2009).
34. Hamann, S., Herman, R. A., Nolan, C. L. & Wallen, K. Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nat.
Neurosci. 7, 411–416 (2004).
35. Karama, S. et al. Areas of brain activation in males and females during viewing of erotic film excerpts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 16, 1–13
(2002).
36. Safron, A. et al. Neural correlates of sexual arousal in homosexual and heterosexual men. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 237–248 (2007).
37. Rieger, G., Chivers, M. L. & Bailey, J. M. Sexual arousal patterns of bisexual men. Psychol. Sci. J. Am. Psychol. Soc. APS 16, 579–584
(2005).
38. Maus, B., Breukelen, G. J. P., van, Goebel, R. & Berger, M. P. F. Optimization of Blocked Designs in fMRI Studies. Psychometrika 75,
373–390 (2010).
39. Parrish, T. B., Gitelman, D. R., LaBar, K. S. & Mesulam, M. M. Impact of signal-to-noise on functional MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. Off.
J. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 925–932 (2000).
40. Friston, K. J. et al. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210
(1994).
41. Stark, R. et al. Erotic and disgust-inducing pictures–differences in the hemodynamic responses of the brain. Biol. Psychol. 70, 19–29
(2005).
42. Walter, M. et al. Distinguishing specific sexual and general emotional effects in fMRI-subcortical and cortical arousal during erotic
picture viewing. NeuroImage 40, 1482–1494 (2008).
43. Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A. & Burdette, J. H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-
based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage 19, 1233–1239 (2003).
44. Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R. & Poline, J.-B. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. in (2002).
45. Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H. & Ryan, C. S. Data Analysis: A Model Comparison Approach, Second Edition (Routledge, 2011).
46. Samson, F., Zeffiro, T. A., Toussaint, A. & Belin, P. Stimulus complexity and categorical effects in human auditory cortex: an
activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 1, 241 (2010).
47. Bailey, J. M. What is sexual orientation and do women have one? Neb. Symp. Motiv. Neb. Symp. Motiv. 54, 43–63 (2009).
48. Chivers, M. L. The Specificity of Women’s Sexual Response and Its Relationship with Sexual Orientations: A Review and Ten
Hypotheses. Arch. Sex. Behav. 1–19, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0897-x (2017).
49. Sabatinelli, D., Flaisch, T., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R. & Lang, P. J. Affective picture perception: gender differences in visual
cortex? Neuroreport 15, 1109–1112 (2004).

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 12


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

50. Vuilleumier, P. & Driver, J. Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between
human brain regions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 837–855 (2007).
51. Calvo-Merino, B., Urgesi, C., Orgs, G., Aglioti, S. M. & Haggard, P. Extrastriate body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body
stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. Exp. Hirnforsch. Expérimentation Cérébrale 204, 447–456 (2010).
52. Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I. & Kanwisher, N. Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc.
Neurosci. 25, 11055–11059 (2005).
53. Lykins, A. D., Meana, M. & Strauss, G. P. Sex differences in visual attention to erotic and non-erotic stimuli. Arch. Sex. Behav. 37,
219–228 (2008).
54. Rupp, H. A. & Wallen, K. Sex differences in viewing sexual stimuli: an eye-tracking study in men and women. Horm. Behav. 51,
524–533 (2007).
55. Peelen, M. V., Glaser, B., Vuilleumier, P. & Eliez, S. Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus.
Dev. Sci. 12, F16–25 (2009).
56. Apps, M. A. J., Lockwood, P. L. & Balsters, J. H. The role of the midcingulate cortex in monitoring others’decisions. Decis. Neurosci.
7, 251 (2013).
57. Papez, J. W. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 38, 725–743 (1937).
58. Vann, S. D., Aggleton, J. P. & Maguire, E. A. What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 792–802 (2009).
59. Vogt, B. A. Pain and Emotion Interactions in Subregions of the Cingulate Gyrus. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 533–544 (2005).
60. Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Germine, L. T., Knight, R. T. & D’Esposito, M. Neural activity during social signal perception correlates
with self-reported empathy. Brain Res. 1308, 100–113 (2010).
61. Perry, D., Walder, K., Hendler, T. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. The gender you are and the gender you like: sexual preference and
empathic neural responses. Brain Res. 1534, 66–75 (2013).
62. Saxe, R. & Wexler, A. Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43,
1391–1399 (2005).
63. Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R. & Piefke, M. Gender differences in brain networks supporting
empathy. NeuroImage 42, 393–403 (2008).
64. Van Overwalle, F. & Baetens, K. Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis.
NeuroImage 48, 564–584 (2009).
65. Klein, F., Sepekoff, B. & Wolf, T. J. Sexual orientation: a multi-variable dynamic process. J. Homosex. 11, 35–49 (1985).
66. Cavanna, A. E. & Trimble, M. R. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583
(2006).
67. Utevsky, A. V., Smith, D. V. & Huettel, S. A. Precuneus Is a Functional Core of the Default-Mode Network. J. Neurosci. 34, 932–940
(2014).
68. Aminoff, E. M., Kveraga, K. & Bar, M. The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 379–390 (2013).
69. Staresina, B. P., Duncan, K. D. & Davachi, L. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices differentially contribute to later recollection
of object- and scene-related event details. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 31, 8739–8747 (2011).
70. Stănişor, L., Togt, C., van der, Pennartz, C. M. A. & Roelfsema, P. R. A unified selection signal for attention and reward in primary
visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 9136–9141 (2013).
71. Anders, S., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Grodd, W. & Birbaumer, N. Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal: a response-
related fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 23, 200–209 (2004).
72. Grabski, K. et al. Functional MRI assessment of orofacial articulators: neural correlates of lip, jaw, larynx, and tongue movements.
Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2306–2321 (2012).
73. Schmidt, T. T., Ostwald, D. & Blankenburg, F. Imaging tactile imagery: changes in brain connectivity support perceptual grounding
of mental images in primary sensory cortices. NeuroImage 98, 216–224 (2014).
74. Filimon, F., Nelson, J. D., Hagler, D. J. & Sereno, M. I. Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution,
observation, and imagery. NeuroImage 37, 1315–1328 (2007).
75. Guillot, A. et al. Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2157–2172 (2009).
76. Meister, I. G. et al. Playing piano in the mind–an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain
Res. 19, 219–228 (2004).
77. Molnar-Szakacs, I., Kaplan, J., Greenfield, P. M. & Iacoboni, M. Observing complex action sequences: The role of the fronto-parietal
mirror neuron system. NeuroImage 33, 923–935 (2006).
78. Mouras, H. et al. Activation of mirror-neuron system by erotic video clips predicts degree of induced erection: an fMRI study.
NeuroImage 42, 1142–1150 (2008).
79. Fuster, J. M. Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 2047–2072 (2009).
80. Taylor, P., Hobbs, J. N., Burroni, J. & Siegelmann, H. T. The global landscape of cognition: hierarchical aggregation as an
organizational principle of human cortical networks and functions. Sci. Rep. 5, 18112 (2015).
81. Ponseti, J. et al. A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. NeuroImage 33, 825–833 (2006).
82. Spape, J., Timmers, A. D., Yoon, S., Ponseti, J. & Chivers, M. L. Gender-specific genital and subjective sexual arousal to prepotent
sexual features in heterosexual women and men. Biol. Psychol. 102, 1–9 (2014).
83. Diekhof, E. K., Kaps, L., Falkai, P. & Gruber, O. The role of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbitofrontal cortex in the
representation of reward magnitude - An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of passive reward
expectancy and outcome processing. Neuropsychologia 50, 1252–1266 (2012).
84. Schultz, W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1–27 (1998).
85. Ferenczi, E. A. et al. Prefrontal cortical regulation of brainwide circuit dynamics and reward-related behavior. Science 351, aac9698
(2016).
86. Rupp, H. A. & Wallen, K. Sex differences in response to visual sexual stimuli: a review. Arch. Sex. Behav. 37, 206–218 (2008).
87. Boozalis, A., Tutlam, N. T., Chrisman Robbins, C. & Peipert, J. F. Sexual Desire and Hormonal Contraception. Obstet. Gynecol. 127,
563–572 (2016).
88. Diamond, L. M. Female bisexuality from adolescence to adulthood: results from a 10-year longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 44, 5–14
(2008).

Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate Fellowship Research Program award to
Adam Safron as well as by grants from the American Institute of Bisexuality.

Author Contributions
J.M.B., A.S., D.S., and A.R. were responsible for study concept and design. D.S. and A.R. were responsible for data
collection. A.S. and V.K. analyzed the data. M.L. and M.W. provided neuroimaging analysis support. A.S. and
V.K. wrote the manuscript. J.M.B. provided domain expertise and guidance through all stages of research and
manuscript preparation. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript version.

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 13


www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Additional Information
Competing Interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

© The Author(s) 2018

SCIENTIfIC REPOrtS | (2018) 8:673 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-18372-0 14

You might also like