You are on page 1of 12

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Human brain EEG indices of emotions: Delineating responses to affective


vocalizations by measuring frontal theta event-related synchronization
Marni Y.V. Bekkedal a,∗ , John Rossi III b , Jaak Panksepp c
a
Two Steps Forward, LLC, 3199 Bookham Dr., Sun Prairie, WI 53590, United States
b
Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Florida Gulf Coast University, 10501 FGCU Blvd South, Fort Myers, FL 33965-6565, United States
c
Department of Veterinary and Comparative Anatomy, College of Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-6520, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: At present there is no direct brain measure of basic emotional dynamics from the human brain. EEG pro-
Received 4 September 2010 vides non-invasive approaches for monitoring brain electrical activity to emotional stimuli. Event-related
Received in revised form 23 April 2011 desynchronization/synchronization (ERD/ERS) analysis, based on power shifts in specific frequency
Accepted 3 May 2011
bands, has some potential as a method for differentiating responses to basic emotions as measured during
brief presentations of affective stimuli. Although there appears to be fairly consistent theta ERS in frontal
Keywords:
regions of the brain during the earliest phases of processing affective auditory stimuli, the patterns do
Affective sounds
not readily distinguish between specific emotions. To date it has not been possible to consistently differ-
Emotion
EEG
entiate brain responses to emotion-specific affective states or stimuli, and some evidence to suggests the
Alpha blocking theta ERS more likely measures general arousal processes rather than yielding veridical indices of spe-
Theta cific emotional states. Perhaps cortical EEG patterns will never be able to be used to distinguish discrete
Gender differences emotional states from the surface of the brain. The implications and limitations of such approaches for
Auditory perception understanding human emotions are discussed.
Event-related cortical desynchronizations © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
and synchronizations

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1960
2. Electrophysiological correlates of brain affective processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1960
3. Classifications of emotions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1960
4. Affective communication versus induction of emotion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1961
5. Measuring affective communication in the brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1961
5.1. Spectral power change . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1961
5.2. Event related potentials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1962
5.3. The ERD/ERS algorithm . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1962
6. Summary of EEG studies and emotions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1963
7. ERD methodology and specific emotion evocation methods: from the production, selection and description of stimuli and
experimental procedures to the results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.1. Production of initial set of stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.2. Selection of stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.3. Description of stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.4. Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.5. Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.6. Data analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1965
7.7. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1966
7.7.1. Changes in emotional states . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1966
7.7.2. ERD/ERS analysis: power spectral analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1966

∗ Corresponding author.
E-mail address: 2steps@charter.net (M.Y.V. Bekkedal).

0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.001
1960 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

7.7.3. ERD/ERS analysis—theta (3.5–7/5 Hz) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1966


7.7.4. ERD/ERS analysis—alpha (8–12 Hz) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1967
7.7.5. ERD/ERS analysis—beta (13–30 Hz) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1967
7.8. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1967
8. Theoretical implications for future work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1967
9. Implications for the eventual understanding of affective consciousness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1968
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1969
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1969

1. Introduction logical functions do not provide compelling evidence for specific


time-dependent subcortical state-control functions that underlie
An understanding of the underlying brain dynamics that gener- emotional feelings, which may be reflected in cortical dynamics
ate emotional states, especially those that mediate cortical affective to serve various emotional information-processing and regulatory
information- and state-processing remains a chaotic area of neuro- capacities.
science. If one were able to identify emotional feelings and related Since it is likely that most psychiatric disorders are charac-
cognitive processing of associated stimuli from direct cortical neu- terized by imbalances of brain emotional, and other affective
ral recordings, it would have broad implications including the and affect-regulating systems, it would be good to have direct
potential to be applied for improving our approaches for diagnos- brain measures of emotional processing. That has not yet been
ing and treating neuropsychiatric disorders with symptoms that achieved. However, by indirectly monitoring the small changes in
include dysfunctional processing of emotional information, such energy ripples on the surface of the vast unseen reservoir of brain
as autism, depression, mania, and schizophrenia. The aspiration energy utilization (called “dark-energy” by Raichle (2010a,b)), for
to perfect such “neurometric” measures has a long history (e.g., instance via fMRI and PET measures, some consistent changes
John et al., 1988), albeit without any explicit consideration of the in neuroanatomically specific arousal patterns for different emo-
underlying emotional issues. tions have been documented. Although some earlier meta-analyses
If we could obtain knowledge about the specificity of EEG yielded ambiguous evidence for localization of specific emotions
response patterns in the brain related to primary-process emo- (Murphy et al., 2003; Phan et al., 2002), new and more sophisticated
tions, such information may serve as a standard for more accurately meta-analyses, based on 83 studies fulfilling stricter inclusion cri-
identifying atypical brain activities. The emergence of the field of teria, along with voxel by voxel analyses, have documented that the
affective neuroscience has opened opportunities for applying sci- brain does have clear anatomical signatures for at least five basic
entific rigor and objectivity to the study of the neural substrates affective feelings, namely happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and dis-
of specific human emotions (Panksepp, 1998), but so far only at gust (Vytal and Hamann, 2010). Likewise, clarity has been achieved
a subcortical level, which is difficult to extend to human studies about the secondary-processing of affects, namely emotional mem-
except at homologous neurochemical levels (Panksepp, 1986, 1998, ory encoding, in various overlapping brain regions (Murty et al.,
2004). The present paper summarizes the state of the field from an 2010). Important as that is, such measures of emotionality have
affective neuroscience point of view, rather than an information- not yet been translated into reliable, psychiatrically useful diagnos-
processing cognitive perspective, and summarizes some of our tic measures, although changes in mid-line resting-state measures
human EEG work that sought to illuminate such issues. appear to reflect rumination, have been useful in distinguishing
depression (see Northoff et al., in this issue). In contrast, we still
have no reliable direct measure of specific neuronal network activ-
2. Electrophysiological correlates of brain affective
ities that can distinguish the various basic emotions that arise from
processes
the brain.
Basic emotional feelings reflect evolutionarily ancient neurody-
namics that have yet to be reliably measured from either human 3. Classifications of emotions
or animal brains. In human research, emotional states are esti-
mated largely from subjective self-reports, and in animals from A significant amount of research into brain correlates of emotion
ethologically distinct emotional behavior patterns that arise from does not aim to distinguish specific emotions from one another,
neural circuits with distinct affective qualities (Panksepp, 1998). but considers them as a generic, more inclusive type of mental
Part of the problem is that emotional feelings probably reflect function. The most popular classification is the obvious positive
large-scale brain network dynamics or emotional action systems versus negative affective groupings. Indeed, in early research it was
(Panksepp and Bivin, 2011), for which we simply do not yet have reported that participants demonstrate a preference to linguisti-
good tools that can directly monitor the relevant subcortical cir- cally describe emotions as positive or negative rather than label
cuit activities (Panksepp, 2000). We do have various impressive them with specific names (Osgood et al., 1957). This classification
indirect measures, especially fMRI (functional Magnetic Reso- has been further promoted through the use of a well-validated set
nance Imaging) and PET (Positron Emission Tomography) that can of visual stimuli from the International Affective Pictures System
estimate energy consumption changes in the brain, albeit those (Bradley and Lang, 2007). While there are pictures that represent a
statistically extracted indirect measures only reflect a very small number of different emotions, much of the research that uses the
fraction of ongoing brain activity, with most of it remaining as if pictures for generating responses in EEG has focused on positive vs.
it were background “dark energy” that does not serve immediate negative. While the categories of the valence dimension have been
information-processing capacities, but rather the intrinsic needs of further delineated based on arousal, there has not been routine use
the brain (Raichle, 2010a,b). It is thought that major problems with of the IAPS for differentiating patterns of brain electrical activity
brain functions, as evidenced by various neurological and psychi- for individual emotions.
atric disorders may arise from imbalances of this “dark energy” of Another popular approach reflects a broader theory that there
the brain (Zhang and Raichle, 2010). In any event, such measures are two opposing behavioral control systems in the brain: the
that can identify brain loci that participate in various psycho- Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) and the Behavioral Activation
M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970 1961

System (BAS) (Bijttebier et al., 2009). Focus on these systems pro- these types of studies have been either visual (Aftanas et al., 2001;
vides a dichotomy for defining emotions based on whether they Aftanas et al., 2002; Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi and Mazza,
are aversive and lead to avoidance behaviors, or are appetitive and 2009; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2009; Balconi et al., 2009a,b; Knyazev
result in approach behaviors. At first this may appear to simply et al., 2008; Knyazev et al., 2009, 2010) or auditory (Paulmann
be a more refined classification of positive versus negative affect. and Kotz, 2008; Spreckelmeyer et al. (2009). We suspect that the
However, this perspective considers more than just the emotional use of visual stimuli adds an element of irrelevant stimulus com-
experience but also the corresponding behaviors emitted during plexity that might be minimized with the use of auditory stimuli
those experiences, as well as overall personality style (Corr, 2004). such as short human, primate-like vocalizations of various distinct
While there is some utility in this classification scheme, recent emotions.
evidence challenges the clear association of the BIS with negative While it is recognized that detection and interpretation of affect
emotions and the BAS with positive emotions. The issue centers in the human voice is a complex issue (Banse and Scherer, 1996;
on the emotions of fear and anger, both of which have tradition- Zinken et al., 2008), the use of stimuli with short duration and free
ally been classified as negative. While fear seemingly fits with the of linguistic content can minimize the non-affective components
categorization of negative emotions associated with activation of of stimuli (i.e., noise) and provide a more pure representation of
the BIS and resulting defensive behaviors, anger can also involve emotion. Nonlinguistic components of vocal information play a
appetitive arousal and approach behaviors associated with BAS significant role in communicating affective information (Scherer,
activation (Balconi and Mazza, 2009; Carver and Harmon-Jones, 1994). Research shows humans can accurately identify emotional
2009). Indeed, EEG response patterns support the later character- meaning from prosodic features, and this differentiation is for spe-
ization. Balconi and Pozzoli (2009) report changes in activation of cific emotions, not simply positive versus negative valences, and
the frontal lobes during pictures of anger that more closely match not based solely on levels of arousal (Banse and Scherer, 1996).
what one might expect from activation of the BAS. In the same Thus, such stimuli might be optimal for use in attempts to assess dif-
report, responses for fear were consistent with those activating the ferential brain response patterns during the perception of emotion.
BIS. Thus, it would seem feasible that although the two emotions
resemble each other on both arousal and valence dimensions, their 5. Measuring affective communication in the brain
activation of underlying behavioral response circuits could result
in differential patterns of activity at the cortical level. If one aims to monitor brain activity during emotional states and
Although there are some studies (e.g., Balconi et al., 2009a,b) make comparisons across different states, it is reasonable to employ
where the research design is conducive to the differentiation of measurement tools with relatively low temporal resolution, since
individual emotions, there is far more research that utilizes the emotions are sustained responses of the brain. In other words, it
broader positive/negative categorizations. As such, most studies is not imperative that immediate responses be measured, or that
include the examination of only three stimulus types: one of assessments involve small (fractions of a second) increments of
negative affect, one of positive affect, and an emotionally neu- time. To assess the brain’s immediate responses to affective stimuli,
tral stimulus. Given the paucity of research that makes direct a measurement tool that actually measures neural activity in real
comparisons within a single affective valence category, the ques- time is desirable, compared to indirect measures as with fMRI and
tion remains whether or not individual emotions, especially those PET. For routine human research, such instruments are limited to
revealed by localized subcortical stimulations (Panksepp, 1998) the electroencephalogram (EEG) and the magnetoencephalogram
can be delineated from one another based on neurophysiological (MEG).
response patterns. The disadvantage of the EEG is its poor spatial resolution at the
cortical level, and the difficulty of extrapolating from the corti-
cal activity to what is happening among subcortical processes that
4. Affective communication versus induction of emotion more directly generate affective states. MEG addresses these issues
to some extent, however, it has been used in only a small number
Approaches used to study the delineation of affective responses of studies related to the perception of emotion. In two studies using
can differ depending on whether the goal is to measure the brain repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to differentiate brain
during a specific emotional state or its response to a stimulus responses related to prosody, brain responses to two negative emo-
communicating a specific emotion. Measuring an emotional state tions (fear and sadness) did not significantly differ from one another
requires the induction of the emotion, and presumably is com- (Hoekert et al., 2008, 2010). Similar results from a third study (van
plicated by the latency of onset for the emotion as well as the Rijn et al., 2005) have been reported, however, response patterns
duration of the state. While it will not be the focus here, it bears for sadness and fear differed from those for an emotion of the same
noting that EEG changes related to state induction have tradition- valence (anger) as well as for responses to happiness
ally been assessed in the alpha frequency band (Davidson, 1992). In contrast to the small number of studies using MEG, the EEG
Changes in alpha power have been interpreted as indicating differ- has been used much more extensively. Electroencephalography is a
ential arousal patterns related to the frontal lobes. A summary of relatively non-invasive approach for measuring brain responses to
the work suggests frontal hemispheric lateralization that is based discrete stimuli. Although a comparatively old technique for study-
in the positive versus negative dichotomy, but not in the differ- ing brain activity, advances in electronics and computer technology
entiation of specific emotional states. These patterns have been have allowed increasingly innovative approaches to analyzing
most reliably defined as left frontal activation associated with a patterns of electrical activity measured in an EEG. Approaches
positive emotional state and right frontal activation with negative for evaluating changes in such electrical brain activity fall into
emotional arousal (Davidson, 1992). three main categories: (i) overall spectral power changes, (ii)
In contrast to measuring changes in emotional feelings, mea- event-related potentials (ERP), and iii) event-related desynchro-
surement of responses to stimuli that communicate emotions is nizations/synchronizations (ERD/ERS).
simplified by the short latency for response onset, as well as a short
duration of attention to the stimulus. Our interest here is in the 5.1. Spectral power change
communication of affect and evaluating the instantaneous neuro-
logical changes that occur during the instinctual interpretation of One traditional approach has been to evaluate the overall power
stimuli that convey distinct emotions to the receiver. Stimuli for within a given frequency band under different affect conditions.
1962 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

As reviewed in Davidson (1992), alpha is commonly evaluated 5.3. The ERD/ERS algorithm
for changes related to the induction of different emotions. Over-
all changes in alpha power, and lateralization effects related to The event-related desynchronization/event-related synchro-
these changes, as already noted, suggest a relative right frontal nization algorithm combines the strengths of the spectral power
activation is associated with negative emotions, while a relatively analysis with those of the ERP analysis. Specifically, the ERD/ERS
greater left frontal activation is associated with positive emotions. analysis allows for the evaluation of power changes within speci-
The effects are limited to categorizing affects as either positive fied frequency bands with temporal resolution that rivals the ERP.
or negative. In addition to the findings related to alpha, changes As such, it is appropriate for measuring the instinctual reactions to
in the slower theta band have also been found. As reported in affective communications as they occur. There are also reasons to
Jausovec et al. (2001), subjects were asked to process emotional think it holds more promise than traditional ERP measures as they
content in films while their brain activity was monitored. It was appear to more closely align along a high versus low arousal contin-
found that changes in theta occurring 2–3 s into the clip differenti- uum and thus may be less able to differentiate between emotions
ated between subjects with low versus high scores on assessments and better for indicating more general levels of arousal.
of emotional intelligence. Likewise, Andersen et al. (2009), found ERD/ERS algorithms measure rapid changes of power within
the theta band to be especially responsive to anxious ruminative defined frequency band ranges in order to assess responses that
thinking. occur within milliseconds of a stimulus presentation. Increased
Overall, there are two general problems with simply using tra- power within a frequency band following the presentation of a
ditional spectral power change to measure immediate responses stimulus is defined as an ERS. ERD refers to a decrease of power in
to affective stimuli. While these studies show differences between the specified band following a stimulus. These analyses were first
negative and positive emotional valences, patterns that distinguish introduced by Pfurtscheller and Aranibar (1977) to quantitatively
between emotions of the same valence have not been consistently evaluate changes in the alpha frequency at various cerebral loca-
reported. Also, while spectral power changes may be an effective tions under different conditions of mental engagement. Their utility
approach for categorizing brain activity during different emotional has since been broadly expanded to include studying concepts
states, it may not be optimal for measuring affective commu- such as verbal comprehension (Pfurtscheller and Klimesch, 1992),
nication responses. Perhaps analytical techniques with greater mental planning (Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989) and memory
temporal resolution are needed. processing (Klimesch et al., 1994).
Additionally, ERD/ERS measures have been assessed, with some
success, for utility in studying emotional responses to affective
5.2. Event related potentials communication. Some of the studies have used visual stimuli
(Aftanas et al., 2001, 2002; Balconi et al., 2009a,b; Balconi and
The event related potential is an EEG measure with very high Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2009) while others have opted
temporal resolution that allows for the measure of immediate for auditory stimuli including music (Panksepp and Bekkedal, 1997;
responses to short stimuli. Traditionally, ERPs are measured as Sammler et al., 2007). Although much of the historic ERD/ERS
latencies and amplitudes of positive and negative potentials at research, which was generally unrelated to the study of emotions,
specified millisecond intervals following a stimulus. focused on the assessment of changes within frequency ranges cor-
Indeed, this approach has been used to evaluate emotional pro- responding to the alpha band, in the range of 8–12 Hz, more recent
cessing in the brain, including responses to prosodic features in evaluations related to emotion have focused on the slower theta
human vocalizations. Paulmann and Kotz (2008) showed results band (approximately 3–8 Hz) (Aftanas et al., 2001, 2002; Balconi
that suggest the ERP, while able to differentiate affect-related et al., 2009a,b; Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi and Pozzoli,
processing from non-affect-related processing, the ERP compo- 2009). Its utility for this purpose is supported by observations that
nents did not differ based on emotional valence. In contrast, theta changes distinguish between emotional versus neutral stim-
Spreckelmeyer et al. (2009) showed ERP components did differ- uli (Balconi and Pozzoli, 2009; Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Knyazev
entiate between short vocalizations representing happy and sad et al., 2008, 2009), as well as differentiate emotional stimuli based
emotions. However, these were the only two emotions tested, and on their level of reported arousal (Aftanas et al., 2002; Balconi and
since they differ on the valence dimension they do not supple- Pozzoli, 2009; Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi et al., 2009a).
ment our ability to identify distinct patterns for more discrete Furthermore, patterns of increased theta power (theta ERS) have
emotions. also been shown to differ based on emotional valence as por-
Several other studies using ERP measures to assess affective trayed in visual stimuli (Aftanas et al., 2001; Balconi et al., 2009a,b;
responses suggest that neural changes within the theta band may Knyazev et al., 2009, 2010). Results show some of these effects can
be especially informative. Balconi and Pozzoli (2009) present evi- be modulated by individual traits such as gender (Knyazev et al.,
dence that the increased theta in response to affective pictures 2010), alexithymia (Aftanas et al., 2003) and anxious personality
correlates directly with ERP changes, particularly the N2 compo- (Aftanas et al., 2003; Knyazev et al., 2008). Furthermore, similar
nent. Furthermore, Kamarajan et al. (2008), reviews evidence to theta ERSs are noted in other attempts to identify EEG patterns
suggest that some of the ERP changes are a result of underlying for emotion including responses to music (Panksepp and Bekkedal,
theta increases related to information processing. The changes in 1997; Sammler et al., 2007) and to films specifically selected for
the ERP components and theta increases were a result of partici- their emotional content (Jausovec et al., 2001).
pant responses during gains versus losses in a gambling situation. The intent of our work in this area was to use the ERD/ERS
While it is clear that theta power does change, there are conflicting analysis as a method for detecting the instinctual brain reactions
results as to whether gains or losses are most closely associated that occur during affective communication. It can be used to assess
with increased theta (see Kamarajan et al., 2008 vs. Cohen et al., responses in both implicit or explicit perception paradigms. In
2007). implicit situations, the receiver is more passive and simply experi-
In any event, as with other studies, the ERP data do not show ences the stimuli representing the differing emotions. In an explicit
convincing evidence for patterns of brain activity that readily dif- condition, the receiver is asked to identify the emotion portrayed
ferentiate between specific emotions. However, it is noteworthy in the stimulus. Knyazev et al. (2009, 2010) report timing differ-
that some of the ERP components may have a direct correlation ences in theta ERS during such implicit versus explicit affective
with theta activity. processing. Results showed that the earliest theta synchroniza-
M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970 1963

tion occurs in the implicit testing condition, and a later theta ERS uate students, perhaps too many with marginal motivations to
is present during the explicit identification of emotions in pic- follow instructions with a difficult emotion-induction task based
tures of facial expressions. It was proposed that these reflect an on Manfred Clynes’s “Sentic Cycle” approach). But also there were
unconscious versus conscious processing of the emotion with the marginal test-retest reliability patterns in a single subject (data not
implicit/passive condition being the one of interest for our effort to summarized here), suggesting that neocortical emotional effects of
identify instinctual affective responses. idiographic as opposed to nomothetic. In any event, those programs
Unfortunately, as with other similar efforts using the EEG, this of research were terminated because of the chaotic nature of the
line of research has failed to clearly demonstrate distinct response topographic brain changes observed from subject to subject.
patterns for individual emotions. A commonality between all of Of course it is also possible that attempts to measure affec-
these studies of affect-related ERD/ERS is their universal use of tive impact on the brain as harvested from the cortical surface,
visual stimuli to communicate emotion. More specifically, they as is typically done in topographic EEG studies, are simply too
used pictures from the standardized International Affective Picture far removed from the subcortical emotion generators identified in
System (Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 2001), or animal research (Panksepp, 1998, 2000). While there have been
pictures of facial expressions from the Ekman and Friesen (1976) some encouraging results (Knyazev et al., 2010), to suggest that
set. As already noted, all visual stimuli tend to have information that certain activity pattern in subcortical regions correlate with dis-
is irrelevant to the emotionality of the pictures used. Thus, we pro- tinct activity patterns in the cortex, the evidence is less convincing
pose that specific emotion-related brain activity changes may be with respect to EEG and emotion. However, as noted, there appears
better detected when the stimuli are presented through the audi- to be an emergence of evidence that suggests the changes in theta
tory modality. Short, nonlinguistic vocal sounds of emotion may be band during the earliest periods of emotion perception are the most
less complex than visual portrayals, which require visual scanning relevant. As such, this may indicate that the cortical theta may be
and interpretation of the stimuli, and thus may reduce confound- an extension of theta commonly found in areas of the limbic system
ing variables introduced by the more complex cognitive processing including the amygdala, and most commonly the hippocampus.
necessary in interpretation of emotional significance. Furthermore, Hippocampal (HC) theta has been extensively studied in animal
evidence suggests there is a difference in a person’s accurate classi- models of neurobehavioral changes related to emotion and mem-
fication of an emotion that is dependent on whether the emotion is ory. Indeed, Nishitani (2003) used magnetoencephalography to
communicated in a facial expression versus nonlinguistic vocaliza- demonstrate a hippocampal theta ERS in response to negative
tion (Hawk et al., 2009). Accordingly, after summarizing additional and positive emotion pictures with some valence differentiation
EEG approaches to emotion studies, we will proceed to present related to hemispheric differences. However, a recent, comprehen-
results from a project using human vocalizations, that provided an sive review of HC theta research and correlations to cortical theta,
opportunity to evaluate consistencies in cortical neural response specifically frontal midline theta, suggests the absence of any direct
patterns related to affect independent of confounding sources of relation between activity in this frequency band at the two dif-
variability as are routinely present in the use of visual sensory input ferent brain levels (Mitchell et al., 2008). Others have proposed a
to the brain. relation between cortical and HC theta based on the theory that
theta reflects a gating mechanism for information that is processed
in the limbic system (reviewed in Knyazev et al., 2010). Even if
6. Summary of EEG studies and emotions this were the case, these authors acknowledge this may mean that
theta analysis could be used to differentiate responses to emotional
In summary, EEG recordings, despite some controversial suc- versus neutral stimuli, although it may hold minimal promise for
cesses (see Harmon-Jones, 2007), have not yet produced response distinguishing between emotions.
patterns sufficient to distinguish brain activity for specific emo- Unfortunately substantive data to address the relationship of
tions, and thus has failed to provide a tool that could be used to waking subcortical theta changes to the corresponding theta at the
move forward attempts to apply such techniques to issues of emo- cortical level are not yet available, although it has been proposed
tional processing changes in psychiatric disorders. Of course there that theta in the limbic system directly correlates with theta oscil-
are abundant general EEG studies in psychiatric patients, includ- lations in the frontal cortex, and thus is likely to be involved in some
ing the production of algorithms (e.g., neurometrics) that have regulatory component of affective states and neurobehavioral pro-
been reported to successfully discriminate different diagnostic cat- files (Miller, 1991; Knyazev, 2007). As previously reviewed, there
egories (Prichep and John, 1992). One of the problems in relating seems to be ample evidence to suggest that in emotion studies,
imbalances in brain emotional processing to psychiatric categories increased cortical theta power does not differentiate much more
may be the use of excessively complex stimuli. Indeed, in our EEG than a simple dichotomy of emotional versus psychologically neu-
laboratory at Bowling Green State University, we spent years of tral states, with data suggesting that theta ERS is greatest for stimuli
fairly frustrating research to see if we could obtain EEG “signatures” of negative valence, somewhat less for positively valenced stimuli,
of basic emotions from cranial EEG recordings of human brains (e.g., and least for emotionally neutral ones.
Panksepp and Bekkedal, 1997) that could potentially be translated Still, it is possible that discrete emotions can be distinguished
to pre-clinical animal models, where relevant causal analyses could at the subcortical level, but the cortical correlates of those primal
be done (e.g., see Panksepp, 2000). We largely failed to get good emotional responses are not typically veridically reflected in cor-
discriminative signals, perhaps because we initially used complex tical activity (Panksepp, 2000). At present, considerable obstacles
emotional stimuli (e.g., music and self-induced emotional motor- remain studying brain EEG responses as they relate to discrete
imagery induced emotional changes). This does not mean that there human emotions and affective communications. A major imped-
were not dramatic changes. There were (see Fig. 1 for one subject iment currently is unavailability of tools that allow investigators
who is an expert at inducing individual emotional feelings as well to employ very clean emotional stimuli and to deploy such stimuli
as systematically shifting from one to another; please also note that with concurrent direct monitoring of brain activity changes with
web access to time dependent changes at narrow alpha ranges are sufficiently high temporal and spatial resolutions. Of course, the
provided, to highlight how dramatically cortical changes vary as a EEG remains the tool of choice with regard to real-time recording
function of minor differences in the EEG frequencies analyzed). The of human neural activities, but adequate temporal resolution may
changes, although dramatic, were simply not stable from individual be imperative for measuring brain emotional changes that reflect
to individual (partly no doubt because of the use of undergrad- distinct affect arising from discrete instinctual-emotional response
1964 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

Fig. 1. Cortical regional differences in EEG alpha power shifts (within the full 8–13 Hz range) monitored from the cranial surface of one subject (Jaak Panksepp) during the first
4 s periods after successive emotional inductions using Clynes’s Sentic Cycles procedure (see Clynes (1977) as well as Panksepp and Clynes (1998)). Each emotional dynamic
was repeated ∼20 times, using the timing recommendations for the Sentic Cycles exercise. The sequence of emotions, was left to right: (i) neutral expression, (ii) anger, (iii)
sadness, (iv) love/lust, (v) joy and (vi) reverence. Four successive 1-s epochs (top to bottom) are depicted following initiation of the experiential emotional dynamic, assisted
by bilateral mild dynamic pressure between index finger and thumb, in seated meditative position. A successive quarter second analysis (as a pseudo-video) of each 4 s period,
broken down into 2 Hz analysis is available on the web site provide below. It was remarkable how dramatically power shifts in these fractional alpha-frequency ranges differed
from each other, highlighting the complexity that has to be faced when doing such analyzes (see http://www.vetmed.wsu.edu/research vcapp/Panksepp-endowed.aspx).

systems (Panksepp, 2000). Yet, the EEG has relatively poor spatial imagine that may have efficacy in cross-species follow-up research.
resolution at the cortical level for picking up discrete network activ- We aimed to use what we thought would be the most robust emo-
ities, and at best, one can only infer localized subcortical activity tional stimuli we could ethically use, namely the raw emotional
changes, even though that is where most of the affective “action” intonations of the human voice (see Zinken et al., 2008).
may be occurring (Panksepp, 1998). Accordingly, we selected a classical dimensional approach,
Obviously, electrical brain activity measurement at the subcor- where affective space was split into positive and negative sec-
tical level is invasive, and thus not practical for routine human tors, along with high and low arousal quadrants. The intent was
research. It is possible to attempt parallel types of investigations to create a scenario that would allow for the evaluation of differ-
using simultaneous cortical and subcortical recordings in animal ences that could distinguish within and across valences, as well
models, however, the ability to clearly delineate discrete emotional as delineate effects that appeared to be more based in general-
stimuli, affective states and corresponding emotional responses in ized levels of arousal. The stimuli were “raw” (single exhalation)
these models, in time-locked ways, remains a challenge. We do sus- sounds of four primal feelings: (i) anger (negative valence, high
pect that the use of emotional vocalizations, as in the experiment to arousal), (ii) joy (positive valence, high arousal), (iii) sadness (neg-
be described, is the optimal way to proceed, since the same methods ative valance, low arousal) and (iv) pleasure (positive valence, low
could be used in both human and animal research. In anticipation arousal). We created a library of stimuli generated by an emo-
of such possibilities, in the following experiment we attempted to tionally sophisticated set of volunteers, and the validity of their
first apply this strategy to the human brain, to determine if distinct affective content was insured by only selecting stimuli that a sep-
emotion-specific “signatures” of brain changes could be obtained arate group of “receivers” identified correctly with close to 100%
that could then be translated to animal models for finer analyses. accuracy. These stimuli were used in the following topographic EEG
Of course, other brain imaging techniques, such as the fMRI recording experiment to see if we could also identify four distinct
and PET, have much greater spatial resolution and allow for the affective states through a sophisticated analysis of cortical neuro-
visualization of simultaneous activity throughout the entire brain. dynamics using topographic EEG ERD/ERS recordings as the direct
However, their relatively poor temporal resolution makes them measure of ongoing neurodynamics.
less ideal as a tool for measuring the rapid onset, short duration In the following sections we will describe the measurement
responses to affective communications. Additionally, their routine of changes in cortical electrical responses to nonverbal human
use in cross-species affective neuroscience research is not yet read- vocalizations representing joy, pleasure, sadness or anger. When
ily available. combined with information from related measures, such as per-
Before conceding to the possibility that the EEG cannot be used sonality scales that attempt to monitor primary-process emotional
to delineate various discrete primary-process emotional responses, strengths and weaknesses (e.g., Davis et al., 2003), the results may
we proceeded to design the simplest possible experiment we could contribute to the development of a common reference point from
M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970 1965

which to identify typical versus atypical affective response patterns on a scale from 0 to 10. Some reliable differences were found in
of the brain. the intensity ratings for the emotion-related sounds, and a main
effect for emotion (p < .0005) was found where joy and anger were
7. ERD methodology and specific emotion evocation rated as more intense (7.3 ± 0.3 and 8.0 ± 0.2, respectively) than the
methods: from the production, selection and description of pleasure or sad stimuli (6.5 ± 0.3 and 6.6 ± 0.4, respectively).
stimuli and experimental procedures to the results
7.5. Procedures
7.1. Production of initial set of stimuli
The subjects were fitted with a standard recording cap (Electro-
Eight adult male (n = 5) and female (n = 3) performers provided Cap International) with 19 electrodes systematically placed
the initial set of affective vocalizations. During a recording session, according to the 10/20 system (Jasper, 1958) with linked earlobes
each performer was instructed to emit one individual vocalization as the reference. Each electrode was filled with electrically conduc-
representing their interpretation of the emotions of joy, pleasure, tive gel (Electro-gel, Electro-Cap International), and the scalp was
anger, sadness, excitement, love, disgust and fear. Each performer abraded until impedances at each electrode were <10 k. Subjects
provided one sound for each emotion for a total of 64 sounds. Dur- were fitted with a pair of monaural headphones placed over the cap,
ing the recording session, the sounds were digitally recorded into and were seated in a comfortable chair located inside a dimly lit,
a computer (16-bit, 44.1 kHz). They were transferred, in random sound-attenuated audiology testing booth. They were instructed to
order, to digital audio tape (DAT) for presentation to a group of remain seated with eyes closed and relax in order to minimize the
independent raters. effects of muscle movement.
Each session began with 3 min of silence for a baseline recording,
7.2. Selection of stimuli followed by the presentation of five different pure tones repeated
4–5 times each throughout the next 3 min with an interstimulus
Twelve individual raters were given a list of the eight options interval of 10 s. Then one group of emotion sounds was presented
for emotion type and a rating sheet that provided space to identify such that each of the five different vocalizations for that emotion set
the emotion type for each sound, and separate scales for rating the was presented 4–5 times during the next 3 min with an interstimu-
valence and intensity of each sound. Each of the 64 sounds was lus interval of 10 s. This was followed by another 2 min presentation
played with sufficient time between each sound to allow for rating of the pure tones. Following the second pure tone set, data record-
scales to be completed. The goal was a final set of sounds fitting into ing was paused while the subject was asked to verbally label the
four groups: positive high intensity, positive low intensity, negative emotion represented in the majority of the most recent presenta-
high intensity and negative low intensity. Results from the rating tion of vocalizations, and provide an average intensity rating for
exercise identified joy sounds as positive high intensity, pleasure as those sounds. This pattern of presentation was repeated for the
positive low intensity, anger as negative high intensity and sadness remaining three emotion subsets. The order of emotion subsets was
as negative low intensity. For each of these four emotion types, the arbitrarily chosen for each subject such that a set of positive vocal-
five vocalizations with the highest rate of agreement were selected izations was always followed by a subset of negative vocalizations
for a final experimental set of 20 sounds (five sounds from different or vice versa. Following the recording session, subjects were asked
emitters for each of the four emotion types). to describe any changes in their emotions that may have occurred
during testing by identifying the emotion experienced and rating its
7.3. Description of stimuli intensity. These questions were meant to determine if the sounds
evoked a lasting change in the participant’s emotional state.
Stimuli presented to subjects during the EEG recording were
five sounds for each of the four selected emotion types: joy, plea- 7.6. Data analysis
sure, anger and sadness. The average (±SEM) intensity rating (0 = no
intensity, 6 = high intensity) for each of these groups was 4.0 ± 0.2 Muscle artifacts in the EEG records were manually identified and
for joy, 3.5 ± 0.2 for pleasure, 4.9 ± 0.2 for anger and 3.4 ± 0.2 for removed from further analysis. Data were further reduced with a
sadness. Each of the five sounds in an emotion group was differ- 1–50 Hz filter to eliminate all frequencies outside of that range.
ent and thus had slightly different durations. The average duration The data were Fast Fourier transformed, and power spectra were
for each stimulus subset was 690 ± 69 ms for joy, 977 ± 96 ms for calculated for each of the 3 min presentations of different emotion-
pleasure, 865 ± 66 ms for anger and 968 ± 80 ms for sadness. Only related stimuli. Values were obtained for theta (3.5–7.5 Hz), alpha
joy was significantly different from the others and we deem this (8–12 Hz) and beta (13–30 Hz). Second, ERD/ERS analysis was
to be a potentially interesting ethological finding for human emo- employed to determine stimulus-related changes in the power of
tional vocalizations. The emotionally neutral stimuli were a set of the same three frequency ranges. The percent change in each fre-
five pure tones at 500 Hz, 750 Hz, 1000 Hz, 1500 Hz and 2000 Hz, all quency band was calculated as (R − A/R) × 100), where R and A are
with a duration of 1000 ms. the EEG power of the specified frequency in the reference and test
intervals, respectively (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar, 1977). In this
7.4. Participants study, the reference interval was identified as occurring 1 s prior to
the stimulus onset and the test intervals 0–1 s from the onset of the
Eleven adult males and eleven adult females were recruited for stimulus. The averaging sweep was 0.5 s such that the test interval
EEG recording. Data from one male participant were eliminated due was divided into two equal time increments, 0–0.5 s and 0.5–1 s. As
to excessive muscle artifact apparent in the EEG recording. Each nearly all responses had returned to baseline by the 0.5–1 s interval,
person was tested at least 2 h after their last meal and at least 24 h only the first epoch (0–0.5 s) was included for further analysis.
following the last intake of caffeine. For the purpose of a global analysis, data from individual elec-
To reduce the effects of stimulus novelty, all subjects were asked trodes were averaged together such that the scalp was divided into
to listen to and rate each sound in the collection of emotion-related quadrants and three electrodes from each quadrant were averaged.
vocalizations during a habituation/pre-testing session on the day Specifically, scores for the right anterior quadrant included the Fp2,
prior to EEG recording. Specifically, subjects were asked to iden- F4 and F8 electrode sites. Sites Fp1, F3 and F7 were used for the left
tify the emotion represented in each sound and rate its intensity anterior. Electrodes P4, T6 and O2 made up the right posterior and
1966 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

Anterior Left Anterior Right

% of Reference Theta
% of Reference Theta
130 130

120 120

Power
Power 110 110

100 100

90 Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad 90 Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad
Emotion Type Emotion Type

% of Reference Theta
% of Reference Theta

130 130
Posterior Left Posterior Right
120 120

Power
Power

110 110

100 100

90 Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad 90 Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad
Emotion Type Emotion Type

Fig. 2. Changes in theta power based on percent of baseline power in response to different stimuli. Results are averaged across electrodes on each of four quadrants of the
scalp. The figure demonstrates the greatest theta synchronization in the anterior hemisphere and in response to sounds of anger and pleasure. Filled bars are for males and
open bars are for females.

P3, T5 and P1 were the left posterior. There were no significant showed less of a theta power increase than males. This effect was
differences between individual electrodes within a quadrant. Data statistically reliable in the right anterior region only.
from Fz, Cz, Pz, T3, T4, C3 and C4 were not included in the final Some differences were found in response to emotional sounds
analyses. within genders (Fig. 3). In males, moderated ERDs occurred during
For each of the three frequency bands, the percent change joy and anger sounds. This change was significantly different form
in power was averaged and subjected to a five factor, repeated the ERS observed during pleasure sounds (ps < .05). Responses to
measures ANOVA: gender (2) × stimulus type (5) × brain quadrant neutral sounds were modest ERSs that did not differ from responses
(4) × electrode (3) × time interval (2). This was done for both the to any of the other sounds. In females, all responses were theta
power spectra and the ERD/ERS analysis, although the ANOVA for ERSs. The ERSs for joy and pleasure were reliable smaller than
the power spectra did not include the time interval variable. those observed during anger sounds (ps < .05). As in males, female
responses to neutral and sad sounds did not reliable differ from the
7.7. Results brain changes during the other emotional sounds.
A 2-way interaction was found for brain regions × time
7.7.1. Changes in emotional states (ps < .0001) where theta power increased in the 0.0–0.5 epoch
Very few subjects reported any lasting change in emotional
state, and those that did reported minimal changes (intensity rat-
115 Males
% of Reference Theta

ings <2). The data suggest there was not a lasting shift in the
subjects’ affective states during any of the different sound types 110
and changes in brain activity were more related to the instant per-
Power

ception of the stimuli. 105

100
7.7.2. ERD/ERS analysis: power spectral analysis
Greater alpha and less beta power in the anterior regions of the 95
Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad
brain as compared to posterior regions (p < .0001) was observed 90
across all stimulus types. No overall regional differences were found Emotion Type
for the theta band. No overall changes in theta, alpha or beta power
were found in relation to the different emotional sounds. 115
Females
% of Reference Theta

110
7.7.3. ERD/ERS analysis—theta (3.5–7/5 Hz)
A complex 3-way interaction of gender × emotion × brain
Power

105
region was found (p < .05). All differences relating to gender and
emotion occurred in the anterior regions of the brain rather than 100
the posterior ones (Fig. 2). 95
The most robust difference between the males and females was Neutral Joy Pleasure Anger Sad
in response to the anger sounds (p < .05) where females demon- 90
strated a greater increase in theta than did males. This difference Emotion Type
was apparent in both left and right anterior regions (ps < .05), but
Fig. 3. Gender differences in changes of theta power based on percent of baseline
was not found in the posterior regions. A gender difference was power. Changes are in response to sounds of different emotion types. The figure
also found for responses to pleasure sounds (p < .05) where females demonstrates gender differences in response to anger and pleasure.
M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970 1967

and decreased in the 0.5–1.0 epoch. This pattern was the same viewing film clips showed greatest theta increases during seg-
in all brain regions, however, the magnitude of the changes was ments with strong aggression-related content as compared to an
greater in the anterior regions in comparison to the posterior ones emotionally neutral clip or segment portraying sadness (Krause
(p < .005). These differences were not observed during the 0.5–1.0 et al., 2000). Additionally, functional magnetic resonance imaging
epoch. The statistical main effect for time (p < .0001) was due to changes show that women’s brain activation patterns in response
an increase in theta during the 0.0–05 interval (112.3 ± 0.6%) to facial expressions of anger versus fear are more discriminat-
and a subsequent decrease during the 0.5–1.0 interval ing than responses in men. This gender difference is not apparent
(95.2 ± 0.8%). in adolescence suggesting a culturally learned effect where adult
women have learned to attend to indications of anger and a
7.7.4. ERD/ERS analysis—alpha (8–12 Hz) potentially threatening situation, and corresponding emotional
Changes in the alpha band did not differentiate the emotional stimuli cause a characteristic defensive response (McClure et al.,
stimuli. This is important, for alpha-blocking (i.e., ERD) types of 2004).
experiments have been typically successful using cognitive, non- Although theta ERS does correlate with the appraisal of the
emotional stimuli (see especially the cited work by Wolfgang emotion-related sounds, the psychological corollary of theta activ-
Klimesch’s group). Still, some small effects were also observed ity remains unclear. Previous findings related to theta ERS suggest
here. Modest differences were observed between the anterior and its relationship to working memory (see reviews by Klimesch,
posterior regions of the brain, where overall there were greater 1996; Klimesch et al., 1996) and episodic memory (Doppelmayr
reductions in alpha power in the posterior regions than the anterior et al., 1998; Klimesch et al., 1994, 1997a,b). Last, overall theta
ones [brain region × time (p < .05)]. Differences were found during power in the frontal regions of the brain is directly related to
the 0–0.5 epoch where alpha desynchronization was less in the the amplitude of evoked potential response in the same areas
left anterior region than in either the left or right posterior regions (Başar et al., 1998). Greater evoked potentials are traditionally
(ps < .05). During this epoch, the decrease in alpha was less in the interpreted as elevated levels of attention directed towards a
right anterior region than the left (p < .05), but was not different specific stimulus. Such evidence may suggest the theta differences
from the right posterior region (p < .10). There were no regional dif- during emotional sounds, as found here between women and men,
ferences during the second half of the recording interval (0.5–1.0). may reflect differences in allocation of attention. However, it is
not possible from the present data to determine conclusively that
7.7.5. ERD/ERS analysis—beta (13–30 Hz) women paid more attention to the anger sounds than to others or
Similar to changes in the alpha band, response patterns in gave them more attention than males did.
the beta wavelength did not distinguish between the emotional While it is possible women were more psychologically alerted
stimuli. No significant left-right hemispheric differences were by the sounds of anger than the men were, it seems unlikely the
found for either the anterior or posterior regions. A main effect differences occurred because they were more distressed or expe-
for brain regions was observed, reflected by slight decreases in rienced some other more lasting change in their emotional state.
beta power in the left and right anterior regions (99.7 ± 0.6% and Theta ERS was consistently found during the earliest part of the
99.5 ± 0.5%, respectively) accompanied by modest increases of stimulus presentation (first half second), but had returned virtu-
beta power in the left and right posterior regions (101.8 ± 0.5% ally to baseline by the next half second. Presumably a more global
and 100.9 ± 0.4%, respectively). The differences were statistically change related to variations in emotional states would have lasted
reliable only between the left and right anterior regions and left longer than this fraction of a second. Also, there did not appear
posterior region (ps < .01) to be any residual effects from the sounds since there were no
general changes in the overall power spectra across the stimulus
7.8. Discussion types for any of the frequency bands evaluated. Thus, results from
the spectral power analyses suggest a relatively constant baseline
The present experiment evaluated the use of ERD/ERS analysis in state of brain activity, and likely a constant affective state in the
differentiation of brain responses to nonlinguistic vocalizations of test subjects.
joy, pleasure, anger and sadness. This expanded previous research Although the results show consistent theta ERSs, there are only
that has traditionally used visual stimuli including images of emo- modest differences between responses related to different dis-
tional situations (Aftanas et al., 2001, 2002) and pictures of affective crete emotional sounds used, and in some cases changes related
facial expressions (Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006). The results pro- to neutral tones were equal to or greater in magnitude than
vided limited support for our hope that theta ERS patterns would changes during emotional vocalizations. It is possible that such
differentiate responses to distinct emotion-related sounds. The changes reflect the commonly observed see-saw dynamics of cor-
results were consistent with previous research where the most tical and subcortical activities seen in human brain imaging (Liotti
salient effects were observed in theta ERSs in anterior regions of the and Panksepp, 2004). As such, it does not appear that theta ERS
brain (Aftanas et al., 2001, 2002; Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006). How- changes are sufficiently sensitive to delineate distinct brain activ-
ever, in prior studies these changes were not dependent on emotion ity responses related to stimuli representing different emotions.
type but rather were more generically related to emotion versus However, these data, combined with those from research evaluat-
non-emotion related stimuli. Even so, an unanticipated and novel ing visual depictions of emotion, suggest theta ERS may be a more
finding of gender differences was observed that was based on the sensitive marker for the arousing qualities of emotional stimuli and
differentiation of responses related to specific emotions. It may be less differentiating for emotional valence. In conclusion, although
an important consideration for future research to carefully evalu- theta ERS patterns consistently appear in response to emotion-
ate gender-specific responses. It may also be valuable to categorize related stimuli, they are insufficient for clearly defining typical from
stimuli based on specific emotions as opposed to broader catego- atypical response patterns.
rizations of neutral versus emotion-related (Balconi and Lucchiari,
2006), positive versus negative valence (Aftanas et al., 2001) or
differentiation based solely on the rated arousal levels of the emo- 8. Theoretical implications for future work
tional stimulus (Aftanas et al., 2002).
In females, the ‘anger’ stimuli provoked the largest EEG Is it possible to ever get a clean EEG signal from surface record-
response. This is consistent with a previous report where women ings that might highlight the neurodynamics of basic emotional
1968 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

processes in the brain? We suspect that may not be possible,


perhaps largely because the higher reaches of the human brain are
largely programmed by life experiences, and different emotional
neurodynamics in different people are idiosyncratically related to
higher-cognitive brain functions (see Fig. 1 legend). And we say
this with full consideration of the fact that emotional specificity is
evident in subcortical brain regions of all mammals that have been
studied (Panksepp, 1998) and a recent fMRI and PET meta-analysis
of evidence for basic emotions found very robust evidence for
distinct brain metabolism/blood-flow signal changes so that every
one of the following emotions could be distinguished from each
other – happiness, sadness, anger, fear and disgust (Vytal and
Hamann, 2010). Those results are impressive, but practically all
the hot-spots were fairly deep in the brain, in either subcortical
structures, or limbic cortical regions, and practically none were
represented just adjacent to 10/20 cranial recording sites tradi-
tionally used in human EEG studies. Further, it is possible that the
medial frontal cingulate and temporal regions implicated in that
meta-analysis are already high enough in the brain to be mediating
the cognitive thought-related aspects of emotions rather than the
primal feeling states.
However, there are excellent studies that specifically attempted
to monitor basic emotional-affective feelings with PET (e.g.,
Damasio et al., 2000), which found arousals almost exclusively in
subcortical brain regions that have been identified as part of the
primary-process emotional networks in all animal brains that have
been studied with brain stimulation procedures (for summary, see
Fig. 4).
The reasons emotion-specific anatomies are not evident in cor-
tical EEG studies is, as already noted, due to the simple fact that
essentially all cranial sites used in 10/20 recording arrays are far Fig. 4. Relative patterns of cortical and subcortical arousals and inhibitions in
removed from the primary generators of the various basis emo- Damasio et al.’s (2000) imaging of four primal human affects, 189 brain sites exhib-
ited significant changes in blood flow. The top half of the graph indicates the
tions, and hence there is little chance for clear emotional signals to
percentage of sites of those exhibiting arousals that were located in neocortical
be evident at the neocortical surfaces (for more discussion of these areas vs. those that were situated in subcortical brain regions. The bottom of the
points, see Panksepp, 2000). This may preclude the possibility that graph indicate comparative percentages for the brain areas that exhibited reduc-
studies such as this will ever get clear surface EEG indicators of basic tions in blood flow. Clearly, neocortical areas tended to exhibit decreases in arousal
affective processes. However, this should not be taken to mean that for each of the emotions, while those below the neocortex tended to exhibit the pre-
ponderance of increases. Overall, sadness and anger had the biggest effects on the
more refinement of neurometric procedures may not eventually human brain while happiness and fear had smaller overall effects. However, please
be able to provide robust diagnostics of brain-psychiatric prob- note that anger had the largest relative arousal effects on the neocortex, which may
lems that are characterized by poor emotion regulation (e.g., John suggest the largest invigoration of cognitive activities.
et al., 1988). Cortical EEG measure may simply not be adequate to
probe the depths of the subcortical affective mind, except perhaps
in pre-clinical mammalian models where well-situated recording 9. Implications for the eventual understanding of affective
sites can more directly harvest neural field potentials from brain consciousness
networks that generate primary-process emotionality.
Directions for future research may include measuring theta Part of our interest in EEG measures was based on the like-
for similar emotions presented via different modalities. It will be lihood that such approaches might yield the first global insights
important to know if facial expressions of happiness correlate to the wide-scale network dynamics, synchronizing among many
highly with those measured during happy vocalizations. Future brain areas (Singer, 2009), that constitutes affective consciousness
research also needs to focus on including more than a single (Panksepp, 2007). That was not achieved, although a hint of what
emotion within the same valence to ensure observed differences might eventually be obtained was evident (e.g., Fig. 1). Of course
between emotions are not simply related to valence, but rather the neural correlates of consciousness have been studied for some
are specific to that emotion. There is research to suggest that non- time, but what is most critically important is to specify the con-
verbal vocalizations and facial expressions of emotions generate stitutive processes (i.e., in other words, the causal infrastructure)
the greatest accuracy for identifying the represented emotion as of consciousness (Neisser, 2011), especially the most ancient phe-
compared to accurate interpretations of speech embedded prosody nomenal forms which we suspect are experientially affective in
(Hawk et al., 2009). By using modalities and stimulus-types where nature (Panksepp, 1998).
accuracy is not an issue, the neurophysiological correlates could Considering the well-established fact that the foundations of
be the primary focus. However, attempts to utilize EEG mea- consciousness are subcortical (for review, see Watt and Pincus,
sures to monitor specific emotions has barely been developed, 2004) this will require monitoring of global neurodynamics, where
and if successful, such measures could be especially useful for EEG measures are still the only direct measures of neural activ-
addressing the needs of people with neuroaffective disorders. ity that we have, that can be applied in very similar, nonverbal
With modern new computational techniques that yield easy to ways in both humans and animals. We still think such cross species
interpret “heat-maps” of EEG changes (e.g., Gordon, 2011) this studies of emotional vocalizations, along with studies of subcorti-
approach may yet succeed where more traditional procedures cal emotional networks (Panksepp, 1998) are the most promising
have not. cross-species avenue for further investigations.
M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970 1969

Because of the bipolar affective structure of raw emotional emotional states in animals both on the input side, as with emo-
feelings, namely various negative and positive affects, raw emo- tional vocalizations, and on the output side using appropriate
tional feelings may be an ideal way to make scientific progress subcortical stimulations. This type of strategy may be able to reveal
on the neural nature of consciousness, and hence the fundamen- the causal/constitutive primary-process infrastructure of human
tal sources of mind in brain evolution. We already know that and other mammalian minds, which may progressively lead to
wherever in the brain one applies highly localized electrical and similar probing of the more diverse, higher-order evolutionarily
chemical stimulation and obtains coherent emotional behavior pat- emergent levels of cognitive mental processes that take hold mostly
terns, animals will treat these within-brain, artificially induced through experience-dependent cortical maturation, and perhaps
arousals as ‘rewards’ and ‘punishments’ in various learning tasks partly through higher neural evolutionary functions that currently
(Panksepp, 1998; Panksepp and Bivin, 2011). Likewise, humans remain largely unknown. If the dynamic signatures of emotion-
consistently report experienced affective changes to such artificial ally felt states of mind could be empirically measured, we expect
brain arousals (early work summarized in Panksepp, 1985, and see that it would lead to a better understanding of our deep affective
Coenen et al., in this issue). These highly replicable effects can cur- nature, which may be fundamental for understanding psychiatric
rently be deemed gold standards for the existence of affective qualia disorders.
in animal and human brains.
Such models provide distinct methodological advantages
for empirically decoding the neural nature of phenomenal- Acknowledgements
affective consciousness, namely the raw emotional experiences
(i.e., affective qualia). A Powerpoint pseudo-video exam- Funding for this research was provided to the Department of
ple of our attempts to do this in humans using cortical Psychology, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH
EEG measures are available at the following web-site 43403, USA from the Office of Naval Research (F3360197MT040) to
(http://www.vetmed.wsu.edu/research vcapp/Panksepp- JR. During the construction of this manuscript, J.P. was supported
endowed.aspx). A progressive epistemology for understanding the by funding by the Hope for Depression Research Foundation.
primal nature of the affective mind might be as follows:
References
(i) Identification of brain sites across several species of mammals
that evoke such hedonic effects (using various types of direct Aftanas, L.I., Varlamov, A.A., Pavlov, S.V., Makhnev, V.P., Reva, N.V., 2001. Affective
brain stimulation including modern optogenetic approaches). picture processing: event-related synchronization within individually defined
human theta band is modulated by valence dimension. Neurosci. Lett. 303,
(ii) Identification of the major additional brain regions that are
115–118.
most strongly impacted by such brain arousals (e.g., via cFos Aftanas, L.I., Varlamov, A.A., Pavlov, S.V., Makhnev, V.P., Reva, N.V., 2002. Time-
histochemistry, etc.). dependent cortical asymmetries induced by emotional arousal: EEG analysis
(iii) Harvesting the fluctuating neurodynamic correlates from these of event-related synchronization and desynchronization in individually defined
frequency bands. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 67–82.
same primary-process brain regions (as with quantitative high- Aftanas, L.I., Varlamov, A.A., Reva, N.V., Pavlov, S.V., 2003. Disruption of early event-
density cortical EEG in humans during experimentally induced related theta synchronization of human EEG in alexithymics viewing affective
affective states, supplemented by relevant subcortical site pictures. Neurosci. Lett. 340 (1), 57–60.
Andersen, S.B., Moore, R.A., Venables, L., Corr, P.J., 2009. Electrophysiological corre-
recording in animal models) as a function of intensity and lates of anxious rumination. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 71, 156–169.
duration parameters of relevant brain stimulations, while mon- Balconi, M., Lucchiari, C., 2006. EEG correlates (event-related desynchronization) of
itoring affective and behavioral changes. This would allow emotional face elaboration: a temporal analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 392, 118–123.
Balconi, M., Bramgilla, E., Falgo, L., 2009a. Appetitive vs. defensive responses to emo-
estimation of the global neurodynamics that instantiate affec- tional cures. Autonomic measures and brain oscillation modulation. Brain Res.
tive mind in brain activities. 1296, 72–84.
(iv) Identification of key chemistries that modify—i.e., attenuate Balconi, M., Bramgilla, E., Falgo, L., 2009b. BIS/BAS, cortical oscillations and coherence
in response to emotional cues. Brain Res. Bull. 80, 151–157.
and intensify—the above effects in animals and humans, and
Balconi, M., Mazza, G., 2009. Brain oscillations and BIS/BAS (behavioral inhi-
monitor the above shifts in brain dynamics. bition/activation system) effects on processing masked emotional cues.
(v) And finally, provide additional testable predictions, genetic ERS/ERD and coherence measures of alpha band. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 74,
158–165.
and otherwise, from the above findings (especially with
Balconi, M., Pozzoli, U., 2009. Arousal effect on emotional face comprehension fre-
respect to ethically/therapeutically acceptable neural inter- quency band changes in different time intervals. Physiol. Behav. 97, 455–462.
vention strategies for various mental problems), for instance Banse, R., Scherer, K.R., 1996. Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J. Pers.
anxiety, depressive and manic states, to help validate the pro- Soc. Psychol. 70, 614–636.
Başar, E., Rahn, E., Demiralp, T., Schürmann, M., 1998. Spontaneous EEG theta activity
ductiveness of the preclinical evidence. controls frontal visual evoked potential amplitudes. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 108 (2), 101–109.
In sum, this program of research – namely, a technological Bijttebier, P., Beck, I., Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., 2009. Gray’s Reinforcement Sen-
sitivity Theory as a framework for research on personality–psychopathology
extension of a standard cross-species affective neuroscience strat- associations. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 421–430.
egy – can be pursued in other animals because of the existing Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., 2007. The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in
robust behavioral indices of the bipolarity of affective neural the study of emotion and attention. In: Coan, J.A., Allen, J.J.B. (Eds.), Handbook
of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. Cambridge University Press, New York,
circuits in many other mammals (i.e., the reward and punish- pp. 29–46.
ment effects of specific network activations), which correspond Carver, C.S., Harmon-Jones, E., 2009. Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence
well with more modest work that has already been conducted and implications. Psychol. Bull. 135, 183–204.
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 2001. The International Affective
in humans. Abundant empirical indications currently converge
Picture System: Digitized Photographs. The Center for Research in Psychophys-
on the conclusion that subcortical neural networks suffice for iology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
the generation of primary-process affective processes, making Cohen, M.X., Elger, C.E., Ranganath, C., 2007. Reward expectation modulates
feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. Neuroimage 35, 968–978.
animal-models potentially highly efficacious for deciphering the
Corr, P.J., 2004. Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neurosci. Biobe-
underlying cross-species neural principles in ways that can be hav. Rev. 28, 317–332.
translated to human research, with predictions that can con- Damasio, A.R., Grabowski, T.J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L.L.B., Parvizi, J.,
firm/support or disconfirm/negate specific aspects as well as Hichwa, R.D., 2000. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of
self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049–1056.
the overall theoretical supra-structure. In such work, it will be Davidson, R.J., 1992. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain
especially important that one can provoke deep primary-process Cogn. 20 (1), 125–151.
1970 M.Y.V. Bekkedal et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1959–1970

Davis, K.L., Panksepp, J., Normansell, L., 2003. The affective neuroscience personality Murphy, F.C., Nimmo-Smith, I., Lawrence, A.D., 2003. Functional neuroanatomy of
scales: normative data and implications. Neuro-Psychoanalysis 5, 21–29. emotions: a meta-analysis. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 207–233.
Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Schwaiger, J., Auinger, P., Winkler, T., 1998. Theta Murty, V.P., Ritchey, M., Adcock, R.A., Labar, K.S., 2010. Reprint of: fMRI studies
synchronization in the human EEG and episodic retrieval. Neurosci. Lett. 257 of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neu-
(1), 41–44. ropsychologia 48, 3459–3469.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., 1976. Pictures of Facial Affect Consulting. Psychologist Neisser, J., 2011. Neural correlates of consciousness reconsidered. Conscious Cogn.
Press, Palo Alto. April 12 [Epub ahead of print].
Gordon, J.A., 2011. Oscillations and hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony. Curr. Opin. Nishitani, N., 2003. Dynamics of cognitive processing in the human hippocampus
Neurobiol. April 4 [Epub ahead of print]. by neuromagnetic and neurochemical assessments. NeuroImage 20, 561–571.
Harmon-Jones, E., 2007. Asymmetrical frontal cortical activity, affective valence, Osgood, C., Suci, G., Tanenbaum, P., 1957. The measurement of meaning. University
and motivational direction. In: Harmon-Jones, E., Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Social of Illinois, Urbana, IL.
Neuroscience. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 137–156. Panksepp, J., 1985. Mood changes. In: Vinken, P.J., Bruyn, G.W., Klawans, H.L. (Eds.),
Hawk, S.T., van Kleef, G.A., Fischer, A.H., van der Schalk, J., 2009. “Worth a thousand Handbook of Clinical Neurology (Revised Series), vol. 1 (45): Clinical Neuropsy-
words”: absolute and relative decoding of nonlinguistic affective vocalizations. chology. Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, pp. 271–285.
Emotion 9, 293–305. Panksepp, J., 1986. The neurochemistry of behavior. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 37, 77–107.
Hoekert, M., Bais, L., Kahn, R.S., Aleman, A., 2008. Time course of the involvement of Panksepp, J., 1998. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal
the right anterior superior temporal gyrus and the right fronto-parietal opercu- Emotions. Oxford University Press, New York.
lum in emotional prosody perception. PLoS ONE 3 (5), 1–7. Panksepp, J., 2000. The neurodynamics of emotions: an evolutionary-
Hoekert, M., Vingerhoets, G., Aleman, A., 2010. Results of a pilot study on the involve- neurodevelopmental view. In: Lewis, M.D., Granic, I. (Eds.), Emotion,
ment of bilateral inferior frontal gyri in emotional prosody perception: an rTMS Self-Organization, and Development. Cambridge University Press, New York,
study. BMC Neurosci. 11, 93. pp. 236–264.
Jasper, H.H., 1958. The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. Panksepp, J. (Ed.), 2004. Textbook of Biological Psychiatry. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ.
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 10, 371–375. Panksepp, J., 2007. Affective consciousness. In: Velmans, M., Schneider, S. (Eds.), The
Jausovec, N., Jausovec, K., Gerlic, 2001. Differences in event-related and induced EEG Blackwell Companion to Consciousness. Blackwell Publishing, Ltd., Malden, MA,
patterns in the theta and alpha frequency bands related to human emotional pp. 114–129.
intelligence. Neurosci. Lett. 311, 93–96. Panksepp, J., Bekkedal, M.Y.V., 1997. The affective cerebral consequence of music:
John, E.R., Prichep, L.S., Fridman, J., Easton, P., 1988. Neurometrics: computer- happy vs. sad effects on the EEG and clinical implications. Int. J. Arts Med. 5,
assisted differential diagnosis of brain dysfunctions. Science 239, 162–169. 18–27.
Kamarajan, C., Rangaswamy, M., Chorlian, D.B., Manz, N., Tang, T., Pandey, A.K., Panksepp, J., Bivin, L., 2011. Archaeology of Mind: The Neuroevolutionary Origins of
Roopesh, B.N., Stimus, A.T., Porjesz, B., 2008. Theta oscillations during the pro- Human Emotions. Norton, New York.
cessing of monetary loss and gain: a perspective on gender and impulsivity. Paulmann, S., Kotz, S., 2008. An ERP investigation on the temporal dynamics of
Brain Res. 1235, 45–62. emotional prosody and emotional semantics in pseudo- and lexical-sentence
Klimesch, W., 1996. Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. context. Brain Lang. 105, 59–69.
Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 61–100. Pfurtscheller, G., Aranibar, A., 1977. Event-related cortical desynchronization
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Russegger, H., Pachinger, T., 1996. Theta band power detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neu-
in the human scalp EEG and the encoding of new information. Neuroreport 7, rophysiol. 42, 817–826.
1235–1240. Pfurtscheller, G., Berghold, A., 1989. Patterns of cortical activation during planning
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pachinger, T., Ripper, B., 1997a. Brain oscillations and of voluntary movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 72, 250–258.
human memory: EEG correlates in the upper alpha and theta band. Neurosci. Pfurtscheller, G., Klimesch, W., 1992. Functional topography during a visuoverbal
Lett. 238 (1–2), 9–12. judgment task studied with event-related desynchronization mapping. J. Clin.
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schimke, H., Ripper, B., 1997b. Theta synchroniza- Neurophysiol. 9, 120–131.
tion and alpha desynchronization in a memory task. Psychophysiology 34, 169– Phan, K.L., Wager, T., Taylor, S.F., Liberzon, I., 2002. Functional neuroanatomy of emo-
176. tion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage
Klimesch, W., Schimke, H., Schwaiger, J., 1994. Episodic and semantic memory: an 16, 331–348.
analysis in the EEG theta and alpha band. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophys- Prichep, L.S., John, E.R., 1992. QEEG profiles of psychiatric disorders. Brain Topogr.
iol. 91 (6), 428–441. 4, 249–257.
Knyazev, G.G., 2007. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in Raichle, M.E., 2010a. Two views of brain function. Trends Cog. Sci. 14, 180–190.
brain oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 377–395. Raichle, M.E., 2010b. The brain’s dark energy. Sci. Am. 302, 44–49.
Knyazev, G.G., Bocharov, A.V., Levin, E.A., Savostyanov, A.N., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J.Y., Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., Koelsch, S., 2007. Music and emotion: elec-
2008. Anxiety and oscillatory responses to emotional facial expressions. Brain trophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant music.
Res. 1227, 174–188. Psychophysiology 44, 293–304.
Knyazev, G.G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J.Y., Bocharov, A.V., 2009. Event-related delta and Scherer, K.R., 1994. Affect bursts. In: van Goozen, S.H.M., van de Poll, N.E., Sergeant,
theta synchronization during explicit and implicit emotion processing. Neuro- J.A. (Eds.), Emotions: Essays on Emotion Theory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp.
science 164, 1588–1600. 161–193.
Knyazev, G.G., Slobodskoj-Plusnin, J.Y., Bocharov, A.V., 2010. Gender differences in Singer, W., 2009. Consciousness and neuronal synchronization. In: Laureys, S.,
implicit and explicit processing of emotional facial expressions as revealed by Tononi, G. (Eds.), The Neurology of Consciousness. Elsevier Ltd., pp. 43–52.
event-related theta synchronization. Emotion 10 (5), 678–687. Spreckelmeyer, K.N., Kutas, M., Urbach, T., Altenmuller, E., Munte, T.F., 2009. Neural
Krause, C.M., Viemerö, A., Rosenqvist, L., Sillanmäki, T., Åström, T., 2000. Relative processing of vocal emotion and identity. Brain Cogn. 69 (1), 121–126.
electroencephalographic desynchronization and synchronization in humans to van Rijn, S., Aleman, A., van Diessen, E., Berckmoes, C., Vingerhoets, G., Kahn, R.S.,
emotional film content: an analysis of the 4–6, 6–8, 8–10 and 10–12 Hz fre- 2005. What is said or how it is said makes a difference: role of the right fronto-
quency bands. Neurosci. Lett. 286 (1), 9–12. parietal operculum in emotional prosody as revealed by repetitive TMS. Eur. J.
Liotti, M., Panksepp, J., 2004. On the neural nature of human emotions and impli- Neurosci. 21 (11), 3195–3200.
cations for biological psychiatry. In: Panksepp, J. (Ed.), Textbook of Biological Vytal, K., Hamann, S., 2010. Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of
Psychiatry. Wiley, New York, pp. 33–74. basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2864–2885.
McClure, E.B., Monk, C.S., Nelson, E.E., Zarahn, E., Leibenluft, E., Bilder, R.M., Charney, Watt, D.F., Pincus, D.I., 2004. Neural substrates of consciousness: implications for
D.S., Ernst, M., Pine, D.S., 2004. A developmental examination of gender differ- clinical psychiatry. In: Panksepp, J. (Ed.), Textbook of Biological Psychiatry.
ences in brain engagement during evaluation of threat. Biol. Psychiatry 55 (11), Wiley, Hoboken, NJ, pp. 75–110.
1047–1055. Zhang, D., Raichle, M.E., 2010. Disease and the brain’s dark energy. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
Miller, R., 1991. Cortico-Hippocampal Interplay and the Representation of Contexts 6, 15–28.
in the Brain. Springer, Berlin-Heidelberg, New York. Zinken, J., Knoll, M., Panksepp, J., 2008. Universality and diversity in the vocaliza-
Mitchell, D.J., McNaughton, N., Flanagan, D., Kirk, I.J., 2008. Frontal-midline theta tion of emotions. In: Izdebski, K. (Ed.), Emotions in the Human Voice, vol. 1.
from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Prog. Neurobiol. 86, 156–185. Foundations. Plural Publishing Inc, San Diego, CA, pp. 185–202.

You might also like