You are on page 1of 10

Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner


driver behavior, perceived motivation and user experience
Zachary Fitz-Walter a, b, *, Daniel Johnson a, Peta Wyeth a, Dian Tjondronegoro b,
Bridie Scott-Parker c, d
a
Games Research and Interaction Design Lab, Queensland University of Technology, 2 George St, Queensland 4001, Australia
b
Mobile Innovation Lab, Queensland University of Technology, 2 George St, Queensland 4001, Australia
c
University of the Sunshine Coast Accident Research, University of the Sunshine Coast, Sippy Downs Drive, Sippy Downs, Queensland 4556, Australia
d
The Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety, Queensland University of Technology, 2 George St, Queensland 4001, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Driving can be dangerous, especially for young and inexperienced drivers. To help address the issue of
Received 28 July 2014 inexperience, a gamified logbook smartphone application was designed and developed for learner
Received in revised form drivers in Queensland, Australia. The application aims to make it easy for learner drivers to record their
1 August 2016
mandatory practice sessions while the added gamification aims to encourage learners to undertake a
Accepted 31 August 2016
wider range of practice. Previous research reported on a lab-based study of a gamified version and a non-
Available online xxx
gamified version of this application. This paper presents an updated design of the application and in-
vestigates the effect of the application when tested in the field. Results are provided from a within-
Keywords:
Gamification
groups field study undertaken with 25 learner drivers over a four-week period, during which the ef-
Gameful fect of the gamification on behavior change, perceived motivation and user experience was studied.
Game design Although results suggest that the gamified logbook was perceived as more enjoyable and motivating
User experience than the non-gamified version, no significant change in behavior was found. This encourages discussion
Learner driver on the effectiveness of gamification to encourage behavior change and the feasibility of using gamifi-
Motivation cation in this particular context.
Behavior change © 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Smartphone

1. Introduction totaled 18.8% of the state's fatalities (DTMR, 2013), despite only
comprising 12.9% of the licensed population (DTMR, 2014a). In
Younger drivers are at a greater risk of death and injury from order to address this issue in Australia, state governments have
road crashes when compared to older, more experienced drivers. employed a range of strategies. In Queensland, Australia, learner
Road crashes are the leading cause of death for persons aged 16e25 drivers are required to undertake 100 h of supervised driving
years (e.g., OECD & ECMT, 2006; World Health Organization, 2014) practice. Although undertaking 100 h of mandatory practice can be
and the second most common cause of disability for male and fe- seen as encouraging a greater amount of driving experience, it
male adolescents alike (World Health Organization, 2014). In doesn't necessarily encourage a greater variety of practice (Scott-
Australia in 2013, young drivers aged 17e25 years contributed Parker, Bates, Watson, King, & Hyde, 2011). All drivers should be
21.3% of the fatally-injured drivers (BITRE, 2013). In the Australian able to handle themselves in a variety of different road conditions,
state of Queensland in 2014, 25.1% of all road user fatalities, and therefore, it is important that learner drivers undertake a wide
34.7% of all road user hospitalized casualties, arose from a crash range of practice while learning to drive with a supervisor.
involving a driver aged 17e24 years. These drivers themselves This paper presents the design of a gamified logbook application
that has been updated to address usability and playability issues
found in a previous lab-based study (Fitz-Walter, Wyeth,
Tjondronegoro, & Scott-Parker, 2013). A within-groups study was
* Corresponding author. S-Block, QUT Gardens Point, 2 George St, Queensland
undertaken in the field using the update design, with 25 learner
4001, Australia.
E-mail addresses: z.fitz-walter@qut.edu.au (Z. Fitz-Walter), dm.johnson@qut. drivers recruited as participants. The effect of the updated gamifi-
edu.au (D. Johnson), peta.wyeth@qut.edu.au (P. Wyeth), dian@qut.edu.au cation design on behavior change, motivation and user experience
(D. Tjondronegoro), bscottpa@usc.edu.au (B. Scott-Parker).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
0747-5632/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
2 Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

was studied. The results of the study suggest that the gamified (Fitz-Walter et al., 2013). The results of this study suggested that
logbook was more enjoyable and more motivating than a non- there was potential for a gamified logbook to be more motivating
gamified version. However, no significant effect on behavior than a non-gamified logbook. However, this study was a
change was measured. laboratory-based study using drivers who had recently graduated
from learner to provisional (intermediate/restricted) licensure.
2. Previous research Further research in the field is needed with learner drivers in order
to properly evaluate the effect of a gamified logbook application.
2.1. Learning to drive
2.3. Evaluating the effect of gamification
As of July 2007, all learner drivers in the state of Queensland,
Australia under the age of 25, must undertake a minimum of 100 h A popular construct investigated by empirical gamification
of supervised driving experience (including 10 h of driving at night) studies has been behavior change (e.g., Chiu et al., 2009; De Oliveira
before they can apply for a Provisional (intermediate) license et al., 2010; Fujiki et al., 2008; Landers & Callan, 2011; Li, Grossman,
(DTMR, 2014b). In Queensland, practice hours are logged manually & Fitzmaurice, 2012). Behavior change has often been measured in
in a large (approx. 16 cm  22.5 cm) logbook. The information previous studies by recording a participant's time and frequency
required to be logged for each driving session includes the date, the spent on a gamified activity. This usage data is generally obtained
time at the start and end of the session, the driving duration (mi- automatically by the gamified system using various sensors. For
nutes), the car's odometer at the start and end of the session (ki- example, Chiu et al. (2009) created a system that recorded daily
lometers), the license number of the supervising driver, the State in intake of water, and De Oliveria et al. (2010) created a system that
which the supervising driver is licensed, the car number plate, and automatically recorded when medication was taken. Gamified us-
if the person is a registered driving instructor. Once complete, the age data was often compared with usage data from either a pre-test
logbook is submitted to the state licensing authority, the Depart- or control group. A number of these studies in various domains
ment of Transport and Main Roads, and audited for accuracy prior have reported that the addition of game elements led to an increase
to permitting the young novice driver to undertake their practical in desired behavior during the study (e.g., Cafazzo et al., 2012; Chiu
driving assessment. In a previous study (Fitz-Walter et al., 2013) it et al., 2009; De Oliveira et al., 2010).
was proposed that a transition to a smartphone logbook application However, behavior change is only one part of a user's experi-
would make this process easier and may be more appropriate for ence. Most software applications also have usability goals, such as
the technologically-oriented youth of today. A smartphone appli- efficiency, learnability, good utility, and ease of use. Other hedo-
cation could also streamline the logging process by using sensors nistic user experience goals exist as well, such as entertainment,
on the smartphone (e.g., location, time and date) and could backup fun, and emotional fulfilment (Rogers, Sharp, & Preece, 2011). Some
data to a server in case the smartphone is broken, lost, or stolen. empirical gamification studies have explored the effect of game
Given that a breadth of experience is an important part of driver elements on usability (e.g., ease of use). Interestingly, some studies
safety, it is imperative that learner drivers undertake a wide range have reported potential problems; for example, adding game ele-
of practice while learning to drive. Although novice drivers repre- ments to non-game contexts has led to usability issues (Guin, Baker,
sent those facing the greatest risk of death and injury on the road, Mechling, & Ruylea, 2012), confused users (Montola, Nummenmaa,
the learning-to-drive stage itself is one the safest periods of any Lucero, Boberg, & Korhonen, 2009), and cheating (Singer &
driving career. This is because learner drivers are required to be Schneider, 2012). This suggests that in addition to the effect of
supervised by an experienced driver throughout this period. In the gamification on behavior change, it is worthwhile exploring the
local context of Queensland, Australia, recent research suggests that effect of gamification on usability and user experience constructs as
learner drivers should aim to spread the driving practice over the well. Therefore, exploring the use of gamification in this study also
learning period, be encouraged to continue to practice beyond the allows for further contributions to be made to this area of research.
mandated one hundred hours, and drive in a variety of different
circumstances that become progressively more challenging in na- 3. Gamified learner logbook design
ture (Scott-Parker et al., 2011). In order to motivate such driving
practice, we employed the use of gamification. This section briefly describes the first version of the gamified
logbook evaluated in a previous study and then describes the
2.2. An overview of gamification updated design of the logbook used for the field study.

Gamification is a design strategy where game design elements 3.1. First version
are used in non-game contexts to encourage behavior change. A
game design element in this sense could include anything from A learner logbook smartphone application was developed. This
game aesthetics, such as graphics and progress bars, to complete application allowed learner drivers to record their driving practice
games with goals, rules, levels, quests, and achievements using a smartphone device and view a summary of all previous
(Deterding, Dixon, Kahled, & Nacke, 2011). Results from previous practice sessions undertaken. The application made it easy to re-
studies have generally indicated that adding game elements can cord practice by automatically capturing some of the information
lead to positive behavior change (e.g., Cafazzo, Casselman, required (e.g., start time and location, end time and location,
Hamming, Katzman, & Palmert, 2012; Chiu et al., 2009; De weather, time of day) using smartphone sensors. A gamification
Oliveira, Cherubini, & Oliver, 2010; Farzan et al., 2008; Flatla, experience was subsequently designed for the logbook application
Gutwin, Nacke, Bateman, & Mandryk, 2011; Froehlich et al., 2009; by using a gamification design framework proposed in earlier
Fujiki et al., 2008; Gustafsson, Katzeff, & Bång, 2009; Landers & research (Fitz-Walter et al., 2013). The activity of undertaking a
Callan, 2011; Liu, Alexandrova, & Nakajima, 2011; Thom, Millen, & virtual road trip around Australia formed the basis for the gamifi-
DiMicco, 2012). Gamification could also be explored further as a cation experience. A road trip is something often associated with
way to encourage learner drivers to undertake diverse practice. the primary target demographic, where it can be a coming of age
This theory is supported by a previous lab-based study that activity that young people may often undertake when they attain
compared a gamified and non-gamified learner logbook application their license (Tourism Australia, 2014). Using a road trip theme also

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10 3

provides a clear goal for the user e to make it all the way around non-gamified logbook. However, participants did no prefer one
Australia e as well as provide smaller sub-goals, such as visiting version over the other. The results also suggested that the proposed
each state in Australia, and also visiting particular towns and gamification design added a level of complexity that created
famous tourist attractions within these states. This gamification confusion for some participants. Also, some participants noted that
design added an extrinsic fantasy layer on top of the process of they could potentially cheat by adding practice manually to receive
learning to drive in an attempt to encourage more regular, diverse additional coins.
and extended driving practice.
An iterative design process was used where a number of pro- 3.2. Second version
totypes were created and used to gather feedback from a wide-
range of different people, including game designers and re- Using the results of this first study, an updated version of the
searchers, learner drivers, driving supervisors, and experts in the gamification design is proposed. The overall design has been
field of learner driver research. Rather than being simply reward- simplified while still retaining the same key functionality of the
based alone (e.g., drive for 10 h and receive points or a badge) the original application; a logbook, a record practice function, and a
road trip design aimed to provide an experience where interesting settings view. The updated logbook provides an overview of the
choices could be made by the player (e.g., which route should the total amount of practice recorded using the application, as well as
player take? which car should the player choose?). A gamification details of each individual practice session (see Fig. 2 e left). Further
design document was created (similar to a game design document) practice details are provided when a learner taps the total number
that outlined the story, mechanics, goals, rules, challenges, actions, of minutes logged (see Fig. 2 e right).
feedback, player strategies, and aesthetics of the design. Using the Learners can press the record button at any time to begin
gamification design document, a digital prototype was created (see recording a new practice session. After entering the current
Fig. 1) that could run on iOS devices (iPhone, iPod Touch and iPad). odometer reading of the car (see Fig. 3 e left) the application starts
In this prototype, when learner drivers recorded real-life practice to automatically record practice (see Fig. 3 e middle). Once learners
they received virtual coins which could be used to buy fuel and finish practicing, tapping the stop button allows them to finalize
items to help them progress along their road trip. Bonus coins were the session by entering the vehicle details, odometer reading, and
received when players undertook recording different driving skills supervisor (see Fig. 3 e right). All other information is calculated
(e.g., reverse parallel parking), drove in different contexts (e.g., automatically using smartphone sensors and the Internet.
unsealed road), or practiced regularly (e.g., twice a week), in order The updated gamification design still encourages learners to
to encourage these desired behaviors. undertake a virtual road trip around Australia. However, rather
A combined usability and playability study was undertaken in than have the learner collect coins to buy fuel to proceed further
order to evaluate the gamified logbook. The design of the gamifi- along their road trip, the design has been simplified. The learner's
cation and outcome of this study are detailed in the previous virtual car now drives 1 km for every kilometer the learner drives in
publication (Fitz-Walter et al., 2013). To briefly summarize, 12 real life. The road trip overview page shows how close learners are
recent learner drivers were recruited to participate in a combined to reaching the next city on the road trip (see Fig. 4). This view also
usability and playability study testing both a gamified and non- provides links to a summary of the road trip, a list of friends and
gamified version of the logbook application. The purpose of this their progress, achievements unlocked, and coins available to
study was to identify potential usability and playability issues, as spend.
well as compare the experience of the two different versions of the Coins are still awarded when learners undertook diverse and
logbook application. The findings of the study suggested that the regular practice. These bonus coins can be used to upgrade to a
gamified logbook was more enjoyable and motivating than the better car. New cars provide various bonuses that help learners

Fig. 1. Screenshots from the first road trip logbook prototype for iOS.

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
4 Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

Fig. 2. Updated logbook design.

Fig. 3. Recording a trip.

complete the road trip (see Fig. 5 e left). Additionally, learners can have the application are progressing on their own virtual road trip.
choose their next destination city for the journey in order to pro- This aims to encourage friendly competition and create shared
vide a greater sense of autonomy, as well as allow for some varying experiences (see Fig. 6 e left). Learners can also unlock driving
strategy. Learners can often choose between taking a quick route achievements along the way for completing various milestones
that is more difficult to travel without an upgraded vehicle, or a (e.g., undertaking 200 h of practice) which provides extra goals for
slower route that is easier to travel with any vehicle (see Fig. 5 e those interested (see Fig. 6 e right).
right). After practice is recorded, a virtual road trip screen is displayed
A friends list allows learners to see how their friends who also (see Fig. 7 e left). This shows the learner's car travelling to the next

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10 5

Fig. 4. Road trip overview before and after adding practice.

Fig. 5. Car and map views.

destination on the road trip. This aims to provide a greater sense of information is provided to the learner that instructs them on how
progression, along with a breakdown of bonus coins they received they could receive coins on the next trip. This aims to provide useful
to give strategic feedback. If they did not receive any coins, further feedback to the learner. If the learner unlocks an achievement after

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
6 Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

Fig. 6. Friends and achievements view.

Fig. 7. Post-practice road trip summary and achievement unlock views.

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10 7

practicing then this is displayed using an alert (see Fig. 7 e right). 0.94). A number of additional questions were administered after
the gamified version was used to gather feedback on the experience
4. Field study of the gamification (e.g., “I found the game elements in this appli-
cation enjoyable”). Short, semi-structured interviews were also
4.1. Study overview undertaken to gather data to augment usage and questionnaire
findings, and to probe participants further about their experiences.
A field study was undertaken in order to evaluate the effec-
tiveness of the updated gamification design. The aim of the eval- 4.3. Study method
uation was to investigate the effect the updated gamification design
had on the user experience, perceived motivation, and behavior of Participants were recruited via a Facebook event page created
the learner. The evaluation also aimed to identify any potential is- for the study and by snowball-sampling techniques. Participants
sues with the gamification design. Three hypotheses were formed: had to meet two criteria for recruitment: (1) the participant had to
be currently learning to drive in Queensland, Australia, and (2) they
1. We predict that participants will have a more positive user had to own and use an iOS mobile device (iPhone, iPod touch or
experience when using the gamified version compared o using iPad) on which to use the application. Participants received a $50
the non-gamified version. gift voucher to thank them for their participation in the field study.
2. We predict that participants will feel more motivated when A 30-min introduction session took place with the participant
using the gamified version compared to using the non-gamified either in person or over the phone before the field study. Partici-
version pants completed an online questionnaire to provide demographic
3. We predict that the gamified version will have a positive effect information and were interviewed regarding their current practice
on behavior when compared to the non-gamified version. habits and experience using the physical logbook. Participants
received one of the two interventions to install, along with an
In addition to these hypotheses, two research questions were introduction to the assigned application. Participants were asked to
also investigated: use the application whenever they undertook driving practice over
the following two weeks. At the end of the two-week period an
1. Does the addition of game elements negatively affect the ease of online questionnaire was administered and a short, semi-
use of the gamified application compared to the non-gamified structured interview took place. Participants were then instructed
application? to delete the application and were given a link to download the
2. Does the addition of game elements lead to any unintended other version of the application. They were asked to use the alter-
consequences compared to the non-gamified application? native version for two weeks. The same procedure was followed for
the second intervention with the addition of a number of further
The field study ran over four weeks during the months of July questions which asked participants to compare their experience of
and August 2013. Twenty-five participants undertook the study both versions of the application (e.g., “Of the two applications, which
(male ¼ 11, 44%; female ¼ 14, 56%). Their ages ranged from 16 to 28 was more motivating to use? Why/Why not?”).
years old, with an average age of 19.44 years (SD ¼ 3.43). All par-
ticipants held a Queensland Learners license during the period of 5. Results
the study. On average, the participants had held their licenses for 22
months (SD ¼ 19.331). During the study all participants tried both a To test the hypotheses, and to explore the first research ques-
gamified version and a non-gamified version of the application for tion, a series of Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were conducted
two weeks each, distributed in a random, counterbalanced order. comparing the non-gamified and gamified versions of the appli-
Driving practice was automatically captured by the device, and a cation. To control the familywise experimental error rate we
questionnaire and interview were administered at the end of each applied a Bonferroni adjustment resulting in a required significance
two-week period to measure the effect of each application. Data level of 0.0055 for the nine Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (Field,
from four participants was unable to be captured due to technical 2013). To explore research the second research question the inter-
limitations of their devices, therefore only 21 sets of data were used view results were coded and common themes identified.
for behavior change comparisons.
5.1. User experience results
4.2. Study measures
The gamified version of the application elicited a statistically
Driving behavior data was automatically sent to the researcher significant median increase in enjoyment (z ¼ 4.037, p < 0.0001,
each time a participant completed a practice session. In addition to gamified median ¼ 6, non-gamified median ¼ 5) with 21 partici-
the amount of practice undertaken, the skills the participant pants reporting a positive difference in enjoyment in response to
attempted in each practice session were also captured. The ques- the gamified version, 3 participants reporting no difference, and 1
tionnaire administered to the participant after each two-week participant reporting a negative difference. There were no signifi-
period adopted validated multi-item measurement scales from cant median increases in reported concentration, perceived control,
Koufaris (2002) for measuring concentration, enjoyment, perceived challenge, perceived ease of use, and perceived usefulness when
control, challenge, perceived ease of use, and perceived usefulness. using the gamified version compared to the non-gamified version.
These scales used 7-point, Likert-type responses. Additionally, in
order to measure participants' feelings of engagement and moti- 5.2. Motivation results
vation a two-item scale was developed (“While using the logbook
application I found it engaging”, “While using the logbook application I The gamified version of the application elicited a statistically
found it motivating”). To assess the reliability of the additional scales significant median increase in reported motivation (z ¼ 2.804,
developed for the questionnaire, Cronbach's alpha was calculated p ¼ 0.005, gamified median ¼ 6.5, non-gamified median ¼ 5.5)
for both the gamified and non-gamified responses and all scales with 16 participants reporting a positive difference in motivation in
were found to have acceptable reliability (alpha between 0.7 and response to the gamified version, 6 participants reporting no

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
8 Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

difference, and 3 participants reporting a negative difference. 5.4.3. Potential to distract


Participants were asked in the interview if the added game el-
5.3. Behavior change ements distracted them from practicing. The majority of partici-
pants responded that they did not find the game elements
Of the 21 sets of captured data from participants it was found distracting. This included while driving; “the main aspect of the
that the gamified version of the application did not lead to a greater application is pressing start and stop at the end, so looking at the game
time spent practicing; no statistically significant difference was elements was just where I'd gotten, and how fast I could look through
found between the gamified and non-gamified versions (gamified it. It wasn't in the way during the actual driving time or anything like
median ¼ 117 min, non-gamified median ¼ 91 min). Similarly, no that” (P1), “it didn't affect my driving, it was just after the fact that I
statistically significant difference was found in terms of the total wanted to see how far I'd gone and if I earnt any coins” (P2), and “If
number of skills recorded at the revised (bonferroni adjusted) you're driving, you don't pay attention to that. You're just paying full
significance level of 0.0055 (z ¼ 2.19, p ¼ 0.029, gamified attention to what you're doing e which is driving. And you only see
median ¼ 1, non-gamified median ¼ 0). Across both conditions that after you stop driving. So it wouldn't be distracting at all” (P6).
participants reported that a number of factors influenced the Participants generally did not find the game elements distract-
amount of practice undertaken. These factors included the limited ing while using the application; “It takes a little bit more time to pay
availability of a supervisor, the cost of hiring a professional driving attention to it, but it's worth it because you're motivated in the first
instructor, external commitments such as school and work place, if that makes sense” (P10), and “It just worked the same for me.
impacting on their time available to practice, and events outside the Apart from having the extra bits to play around with to see how far I
control of the learner driver that affected their ability to practice, could go on the map” (P11). However, one participant thought the
such as falling ill. application would be simpler without the game elements; “It's like,
it keeps it simpler (without the game). I play games because I'm in the
mood to play games but if I'm adding another game as well as driving
5.4. Gamification experience results
it sometimes gets me distracted while I'm driving” (P3).
The results from the coded short answer responses and inter-
5.4.4. Potential dangers
view data suggest that the game elements were well received by
Participants were asked in the interview if they believed the
participants and that very few gamification design issues arose.
game elements did, or could, lead to dangerous driving behavior.
The majority of participants responded that did not believe the
5.4.1. Goal alignment game elements did: “No. I don't see how there could be dangers.
Participants generally agreed that the game elements aligned Unless you're driving too long, or using the app whilst you're driving, I
well to the goals of learning to drive. Participants reported that the don't see there's anything from using it” (P11), and “No (…) the game
game elements linked to the practice sessions well: “It was just good itself doesn't require you to do anything other than drive” (P13). Some
to see how many kilometers you'd actually done and see how far you mentioned that they were not dangerous because interaction with
could have actually driven if you did those kilometers in real life” (P2). the game elements happened after driving was over: “No I don't
In particular, participants discussed how the road trip provided a think there's any, because you don't have to be on the game while
sense of progress that was missing from the non-gamified appli- driving, so that's not a problem. I think the game's a perfectly safe
cation: “I like how it shows you that (distance travelled around addition” (P22), “when you've turned on the app, once you say start, it
Australia), because a lot of the time if you're just driving around your will tell you don't touch your phone and nothing will happen until you
neighborhood you don't really realize how far it actually is” (P6), “I like turn it off. So it's not invasive to your driving” (P9), and “Not really
how it does that (…) it shows you how far you've travelled in real life” because you do that (interact with the game elements) after the drive,
(P6), and “I liked how you could see it sort of going around the map in theory at least. So I don't think it's really a problem, it's safe to drive
(…) you can see the progress you were doing, rather than just numbers with” (P23). However, one participant did mention that they might
in a logbook” (P10). think about the game while they drive; “I do like games, but at the
same time I do like having my full attention on driving, because it's
5.4.2. Cheating driving and if you're like thinking 'Oh what I'm doing right now could
Two participants admitted to cheating during the study, one get me an achievement'” (P7).
participant cheated to progress further in the gamified version, and
the other cheated in the non-gamified version simply to see if he 5.4.5. Enjoyment
could. A further six participants admitted in the questionnaire that When participants were asked if there was a particular part of
they may consider cheating if they continued to use the gamified the gamified version they preferred, a number of elements were
version beyond the study, but generally only if it wasn't an official mentioned. Some participants mentioned they liked the competi-
logbook that could be submitted for review: “It depends, if the road tive aspect encouraged by some of the game elements: “it definitely
trip part was official? Then no” (P19), “If it was like just a game then motivated me to want to practice more because there was that
maybe but because it's about me actually driving, related to the real competition factor” (P8), “I would just want to achieve them all (the
world, then I probably wouldn't” (P14) and “No! They would probably achievements), like to see what happens when you get it and try and
catch me out” (P2). get it before anyone else. I'm so competitive, I love playing games and
Although the majority of participants said they would not cheat, beating people” (P8), and “for anyone who's competitive like me, that's
some participants did not necessarily trust other users not to cheat. kind of a good thing to have” (P7). Other preferred elements included
But this view seemed to come from the belief that some other achievements, upgrading the car, getting coins, and undertaking
learner drivers currently fake their real practice in the physical the road trip. Although many thought the game elements were
logbook in order to receive their license earlier, not because of game enjoyable, one participant reported that adding game elements was
elements; “I guess people would cheat in real life for hours, but in the an unnecessary addition to an already serious application: “It keeps
end it's a game” (P9), and “I guess some people may cheat with their it simpler (without the game). I play games because I'm in the mood to
logbook hours to get their P (Provisional) license earlier, but I don't play games but if I'm adding another game as well as driving it
really see a point” (P6). sometimes gets me distracted while I'm driving” (P3).

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10 9

6. Discussion Although previous research indicates that gamification can posi-


tively affect behavior, it was not the case in terms of behavioral
The results indicate that adding gamification to this context did outcomes of this study. The results of this study suggest that the
not have negatively affect the user experience, rather a significant, process of designing effective gamification is not a straightforward
positive effect was measured on the perceived enjoyment and process and that the addition of game elements to a non-game
motivation of participants. However, there were no significant context may not always result in a change of behavior. A previous
differences in participant practice behavior between the two ver- review investigating empirical studies of gamification (Hamari,
sions. This suggests that designing an effective gamification solu- Koivisto, & Sarsa, 2014), found that of the 24 papers reviewed,
tion is a challenging activity, and that the context and users may most reported positive results for motivation affordances studied.
play a role in the effectiveness of a gamification design. Of these 24, only two reported that all tests were positive while a
larger number (13) reported only part of the tests were positive.
6.1. The gamification did not negatively affect the user experience Using this evidence, Hamari et al. (2014) proposed that the effects
of gamification could be greatly dependent on the context in which
In previous research discussed earlier, the addition of game el- gamification is being implemented, as well as the users that are
ements had the potential to affect the usability of the application. using it. And this could be the case for this logbook study e the
Participants in this study reported that the added gamification had context may not be suitable for gamification, or the users may be
no effect on the ease of use of the application This indicates that less receptive to the design. For example, the context provided a
participants found the gamified version just as easy to use as the number of design limitations such as legal limitations on using
non-gamified version. The usability and playability testing under- mobile devices while driving. Additionally, recording 100 h of su-
taken before this study are likely to have contributed to this result. pervised driving practice is mandatory for most learners to attain a
These results support the importance of using a user-centered, driver's license. Being a mandatory task, the perceived autonomy of
iterative design process when designing a gamified experience. the learner is already likely to be low, and as such, the addition of
game elements might not affect behavior. The goal set by this
6.2. The gamification enhanced application enjoyment and mandatory task is to undertake 100 h, but maybe this goal is mis-
motivation aligned with what the real goal should be e encouraging the
learner to become a better driver. Therefore, the feasibility of
Results suggest that reported enjoyment and motivation effectively using gamification in this particular context needs to be
increased when using the gamified version of the application questioned as there are contextual- and user-related issues that
compared to the non-gamified version. A number of participants may affect the influence of game elements when added to a
liked the link between the game elements and undertaking practice logbook.
in real life. Using the road trip in particular to visualize real life
progress seemed to make the experience more interesting and
motivating for some participants. This suggests that adding game
6.4. The overall gamification experience was positive
elements to this context provided feedback and goals that were not
clear or present when using the non-gamified version and thus,
The results suggest that the game elements were well received
may have led to a more enjoyable and motivating experience.
by participants and that very few issues arose with the gamification
The questionnaire and interview results also suggest the par-
design. One participant did admit to cheating while using the
ticipants enjoyed the game elements. It's also worth noting that
gamified application, and the effect of cheating in gamification
different participants enjoyed different parts of the game, indi-
experiences like this needs to be considered further. Qualitative
cating that designing a gamification experience for different types
results suggest that very few other participants would cheat
of users is important. One participant did describe the gamification
because of the added game elements. Additional participants re-
experience as underwhelming, because the use of the word 'game'
ported that they would be less likely to cheat if this was an official
implied to them that they would be playing a complete gaming
logbook application. One other participant noted that the game
experience comparable to some industry games. This suggests that
elements might lead to distractions while driving. However, this
attention might be needed when it comes labelling similar
was more of a concern rather than the participant reporting that
experiences.
they were personally distracted. Results from other participants
found no issues in terms of the gamification design causing dis-
6.3. There was little influence on behavior change
tractions and dangers while driving.
Although perceived motivation results were encouraging, both
the total amount of practice and total number of driving skills re-
ported were not significantly different when gamification was 6.5. Study limitations
present. Although the gamification was designed to encourage
behavior change, results suggest the addition had no significant A few limitations of this field study include the novelty of the
effect on behavior in this study. The small sample size (as well as smartphone application, the length of study, and the number of
the use of a relatively conservative approach to familywise error participants involved in the study. The novelty factor of both ver-
correction) might preclude definitive conclusions at this time, but sions of the application may have contributed to higher enjoyment
there may be other influencing factors in this particular context. As results. The length of the study meant that each application was
mentioned in the results some participants reported that other used for a minimum of two weeks. Although this was a good length
factors affected the amount of practice they undertook; such as the in terms of gathering application data and feedback, a longer study
supervisor availability, the cost of professional driving instructors, may have provided a clearer picture of the effect of the gamification
external commitments and external events. However, this does not design on long-term behavior change and helped minimise the
necessarily account for the number of different driving skills impact of external events beyond the control of some learners (e.g.,
undertaken. falling ill). Having additional participants would help to provide
Nonetheless, behavior change was not positively influenced. more significant results.

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050
10 Z. Fitz-Walter et al. / Computers in Human Behavior xxx (2016) 1e10

7. Conclusion 9e15). New York, NY, USA: ACM.


DTMR. (2013). Current Queensland Driver's licences. Retrieved July 24, 2014, from
http://www.tmr.qld.gov.au/safety/transport-and-road-statistics/licensing-
This paper presented an updated design of a gamified learner statistics.aspx.
logbook application and the evaluation of it in a field study. The DTMR. (2014a). Queensland weekly road toll report number 912. Year to date to
results of the study found that the addition of game elements had a sunday 19 july 2015. Retrieved July 23, 2015, from http://www.tmr.qld.gov.au/
Safety/Transport-and-road-statistics/Road-safety-statistics.aspx.
positive effect on the enjoyment and engagement of participants. DTMR. (2014b). Licensing. Retrieved July 24, 2014, from http://www.tmr.qld.gov.au/
Although adding game elements can add a level of complexity to a licensing.aspx.
system, results also found that the addition of game elements in Farzan, R., DiMicco, J. M., Millen, D. R., Dugan, C., Geyer, W., & Brownholtz, E. A.
(2008). Results from deploying a participation incentive mechanism within the
this context did not negatively affect the user experience. Perceived enterprise. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in
motivation was significantly higher when participants used the computing systems (pp. 563e572). New York, NY, USA: ACM.
gamified version. We theorize that the iterative, user-centered Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th ed.). London:
SAGE Publications Ltd.
design approach used contributed to this. The qualitative results Fitz-Walter, Z., Wyeth, P., Tjondronegoro, D., & Scott-Parker, B. (2013). Driven to
suggest that the there were few problems with the design of the drive: Designing gamification for a learner logbook smartphone application. In
gamification and that it was well received by participants. These Proceedings of the first international conference on gameful design, research, and
applications (pp. 42e49). ACM.
positive results alone might suggest that this design is likely to Flatla, D. R., Gutwin, C., Nacke, L. E., Bateman, S., & Mandryk, R. L. (2011). Calibration
encourage behavior change, however, there was no significant games: Making calibration tasks enjoyable by adding motivating game ele-
change in behavior measured in either total recorded practice or ments. In Proceedings of the 24th annual ACM symposium on User interface
software and technology (pp. 403e412). Santa Barbara, California, USA: ACM.
total recorded skills measured. This is an important finding, sug-
Froehlich, J., Dillahunt, T., Klasnja, P., Mankoff, J., Consolvo, S., Harrison, B., et al.
gesting that although the addition of game elements can lead to a (2009). UbiGreen: Investigating a mobile tool for tracking and supporting green
more enjoyable user experience, it does not necessarily mean it will transportation habits. In Proceedings of the 27th international conference on
encourage desired behavior change. This raises important ques- Human factors in computing systems (pp. 1043e1052). New York, NY, USA: ACM.
Fujiki, Y., Kazakos, K., Puri, C., Buddharaju, P., Pavlidis, I., & Levine, J. (2008). NEAT-o-
tions for gamification research, indicating that the addition of game games: Blending physical activity and fun in the daily routine. Computers in
elements to a non-game context does not always result in behavior Entertainment, 6(2), 21:1e21:22.
change. These results also question the feasibility of using gamifi- Guin, T. D.-L., Baker, R., Mechling, J., & Ruylea, E. (2012). Myths and realities of
respondent engagement in online surveys. International Journal of Market
cation in this particular context. The context itself may not suitable Research, 54(5).
for gamification (due to various limitations), or the users may be Gustafsson, A., Katzeff, C., & Bång, M. (2009). Evaluation of a pervasive game for
less receptive to the design (as the task of logging supervised domestic energy engagement among teenagers. Computers in Entertainment,
7(4), 1e19.
practice is already a mandatory task). Designing effective gamifi- Hamari, J., Koivisto, J., & Sarsa, H. (2014). Does gamification work? e A literature
cation can be a challenging task, although an enjoyable design may review of empirical studies on gamification. In Proceedings of the 47th Hawaii
be created, identifying any contextual- and user-related issues and international conference on system science (pp. 3025e3034). USA: IEEE.
Koufaris, M. (2002). Applying the technology acceptance model and flow theory to
predicting the effect on behavior change is likely going to be a online consumer behavior. Information Systems Research, 13(2), 205e223.
challenge for any gamification designer. Future work for this Landers, R. N., & Callan, R. C. (2011). Casual Social Games as Serious Games: The
particular study would involve revisiting the design to focus on Psychology of Gamification in Undergraduate Education and Employee
Training. In M. Ma, A. Oikonomou, & L. C. Jain (Eds.), Serious games and
supporting the personal goals of the learner drivers and running a
edutainment applications (pp. 399e423). London: Springer. Retrieved from
longer study with more participants to further solidify the results http://www.springerlink.com.ezp01.library.qut.edu.au/content/
found in this study. m23uk0266722vm02/abstract/.
Li, W., Grossman, T., & Fitzmaurice, G. (2012). GamiCAD: A gamified tutorial system
for first time AutoCAD users. In Uist’12: Proceedings of the 25th annual acm
Acknowledgements symposium on user interface software and technology (pp. 103e112).
Liu, Y., Alexandrova, T., & Nakajima, T. (2011). Gamifying intelligent environments.
This research was carried out as part of the activities of, and In Proceedings of the 2011 international ACM workshop on Ubiquitous meta user
interfaces (pp. 7e12). New York, NY, USA: ACM.
funded by, the Smart Services Cooperative Research Centre (CRC) Montola, M., Nummenmaa, T., Lucero, A., Boberg, M., & Korhonen, H. (2009).
through the Australian Government's CRC Programme (Depart- Applying game achievement systems to enhance user experience in a photo
ment of Innovation, Industry, Science and Research). The authors sharing service. In Proceedings of the 13th international MindTrek Conference:
Everyday life in the Ubiquitous era (pp. 94e97). New York, NY, USA: ACM.
wish to thank the Learner driver experts who provided their time OECD, & ECMT. (2006). Drivers: The Road to Safety. Retrieved from http://www.
for the interviews. internationaltransportforum.org/jtrc/safety/YoungDrivers.html.
Rogers, Y., Sharp, H., & Preece, J. (2011). Interaction Design: Beyond human e Com-
puter interaction. John Wiley & Sons.
References Scott-Parker, B. J., Bates, L., Watson, B. C., King, M. J., & Hyde, M. K. (2011). The
impact of changes to the graduated driver licensing program in Queensland,
BITRE. (2013). Australian road deaths database. Retrieved July 24, 2014, from http:// Australia on the experiences of Learner drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention,
www.bitre.gov.au/statistics/safety/fatal_road_crash_database.aspx. 43(4), 1301e1308.
Cafazzo, J. A., Casselman, M., Hamming, N., Katzman, D. K., & Palmert, M. R. (2012). Singer, L., & Schneider, K. (2012). It was a bit of a race: Gamification of version
Design of an mHealth app for the self-management of adolescent type 1 dia- control. In 2012 2nd international workshop on games and software engineering
betes: A pilot study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 14(3). (GAS) (pp. 5e8).
Chiu, M.-C., Chang, S.-P., Chang, Y.-C., Chu, H.-H., Chen, C. C.-H., Hsiao, F.-H., et al. Thom, J., Millen, D., & DiMicco, J. (2012). Removing gamification from an enterprise
(2009). Playful bottle: A mobile social persuasion system to motivate healthy SNS. In Proceedings of the ACM 2012 conference on computer supported cooper-
water intake. In Proceedings of the 11th international conference on Ubiquitous ative work (pp. 1067e1070). New York, NY, USA: ACM.
computing e Ubicomp ’09 Orlando, Florida, USA (pp. 185e194). Tourism Australia. (2014). Australia for the youth(ful) traveller. Retrieved July 24,
De Oliveira, R., Cherubini, M., & Oliver, N. (2010). MoviPill: Improving medication 2014, from http://www.australia.com/explore/things-to-do/youthful-travellers.
compliance for elders using a mobile persuasive social game. In Proceedings of aspx.
the 12th ACM international conference on Ubiquitous computing (pp. 251e260). World Health Organization. (2014). Health for the world's adolescents. A second
New York, NY, USA: ACM. chance in the second decade. Retrieved July 24, 2014, from http://apps.who.int/
Deterding, S., Dixon, D., Khaled, R., & Nacke, L. (2011). From game design elements adolescent/second-decade/.
to gamefulness: Defining gamification. In Proceedings of the 15th international
academic MindTrek Conference: Envisioning future media environments (pp.

Please cite this article in press as: Fitz-Walter, Z., et al., Driven to drive? Investigating the effect of gamification on learner driver behavior,
perceived motivation and user experience, Computers in Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.050

You might also like