You are on page 1of 10

Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research Report

Magical food and health beliefs: a portrait of believers


and functions of the beliefs
Kia Aarnio*, Marjaana Lindeman
Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, PO Box 9, FIN-00014, Finland
Received 20 August 2003; revised 21 January 2004; accepted 8 March 2004

Abstract
The aims of the present two studies were to delineate a portrait of people who are attracted to magical beliefs about food and health and to
study the self-reported functions for the beliefs. Participants were Finnish men and women ranging in age from 15 to 66 years (N ¼ 3261 in
study 1 and N ¼ 189 in study 2), and they filled in either an Internet-based or a paper questionnaire. The believers were more often women
than men, vegetarians than omnivores, and they relied more on alternative medicine, thought in a more intuitive way, and demonstrated more
eating-disordered thinking than the nonbelievers. Additionally, the believers had experienced slightly more negative life events than the
nonbelievers in study 1 but contrary to our hypothesis, they did not differ in their desire for control. The believers reported value-expressive
function as the most important one served by their beliefs, followed by the control and utilitarian functions. The emotional, intuitive nature of
food beliefs and their connection to values and identity are discussed.
q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Magical beliefs; Food; Health; Functions of beliefs; Intuitive thinking

Regardless of the enormous and growing amount of Rozin & Nemeroff, 1990). The assumed contagion may be
scientific knowledge on nutrition and health many people positive or negative. An example of positive contagion is a
are drawn to unfounded nutritional beliefs and practises. belief that food prepared by a loved one is better for health
This is demonstrated in the numerous lay theories on proper than that made by a stranger. Another illustration of positive
eating, effective dieting, or best ways of maintaining health contagion is the belief of raw food eaters and fruitarians that
or recovering from illness. Examples include blood type sprouts should be eaten immediately after picking them up
diet, paleo diet, anti-ageing food, detoxification diets, yin so as to be able to consume the ‘life energy’ in them (Kandel
and yang food, herbal products, miracle pills, cellulite & Pelto, 1980). More often, however, contagion is
removers, calorie-blockers, massage belts, and body associated with its negative effects, namely contamination
cleansing programmes, to name a few. In the present two and pollution. For example, the Hua people from the
studies we suggest that part of the popular unfounded food highlands of New Guinea believe that a newly married
and health beliefs are actually based on magical thinking, woman is so polluted that no initiated person in her
and analyze the characteristics of the believers and the husband’s community may eat anything she has prepared or
psychological functions the beliefs may serve. served (Meigs, 1984). Modern versions of negative
Beliefs that are defined magical follow either of the two contamination are beliefs that food cooked with electrical
laws of magical thinking, contagion or similarity. The heat lowers vitality and fragments thinking (Kushi, 1989),
magical law of contagion holds that things that have once and that even a brief contact with any animal product (e.g. a
been in contact with each other continue to act upon each piece of meat) renders a whole vegetarian dish inedible
other at a distance after the physical contact has been broken (Lindeman & Stark, 1999; Rozin, Markwith, & Stoess,
off, and that things that have been united in imagery 1997).
have a physical impact on each other (Frazer, 1922/1963; The magical idea of contamination is usually linked to a
need for some purificatory actions (Rozin & Nemeroff,
* Corresponding author. 1990). For instance, rituals of purification, such as vomiting
E-mail address: kia.aarnio@helsinki.fi (K. Aarnio). and eye washing have been performed in some cultures to
0195-6663/$ - see front matter q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.appet.2004.03.002
66 K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

purify the body of the contagious effects of wrong food exciting game. In line with these suggestions, some
(Meigs, 1984). Today, people are instructed to cleanse their studies have shown that rituals and other superstitious
bodies by drinking vinegar or massive amounts of water behaviors emerge as a response to frightening situations
(Billings, 1997), and to detoxify their bodies by fasting or and personal crises (Keinan, 1994; Lillqvist & Lindeman,
taking baths with sea salt (Spence, n.d.). 1998; Padgett & Jorgenson, 1982). Although most of the
The second law of magical thinking, the law of magical beliefs and superstitions examined in the above-
similarity, holds that superficial resemblance indicates, or mentioned studies do not follow the magical laws of
causes, deep resemblance. It implies that an effect similarity and contagion, we predicted that MFH beliefs
resembles its cause and that like produces like (Frazer, have similar correlates, that is, that individuals with
1922/1963; Rozin & Nemeroff, 1990). As the British many MFH beliefs have experienced more negative life
statesman William Gladstone once said: “We would all be events (hypothesis 4) and have a higher desire for control
healthier if we chewed each bite of food precisely 32 than the nonbelievers (hypothesis 5). Moreover, because
times—why else did nature endow us with exactly 32 Keinan (2002) has shown that stress increases the
teeth” (cited in Gilovich, 1991). Similarly, the Hua people emergence of magical rituals, and that the frequency of
do not eat any food that is red because this color is resorting to rituals in stress situations is greater among
associated with menstrual blood and the vagina (Meigs, individuals with a high desire for control, we hypoth-
1984). Today, the law of similarity is exemplified by esized that desire for control mediates the relationship
some people’s reluctance to blow their nose on soft toilet between negative life events and magical beliefs
paper from a brand new roll (Rozin, Millman, & (hypothesis 6).
Nemeroff, 1986), and by a belief that a diet that has an Further, we assumed that magical thinking about food
approximate water content of 70% is needed because our and health is related to intuition. This hypothesis is based
bodies are 70% water (Diamond & Diamond, 1985). on the dual-process theories of thinking and related
On formulating our hypotheses about people who have studies, which suggest that people have two information-
magical food and health (MFH) beliefs, we made use of
processing systems, the reasoning system and the
research on attitudes toward food and health and on
intuitive system (Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Epstein &
magical thinking. First, we predicted that women hold
Pacini, 1999; Haidt, 2001; Lieberman, 2000; McClelland,
more MFH beliefs than men (hypothesis 1) based on the
McNaughton, & O’Reilly, 1995; Sloman, 1996; Smith &
knowledge that women, in general, are more inclined to
DeCoster, 2000). These studies have shown that con-
traditional superstitious beliefs (Irwin, 1993; Vyse, 1997),
scious reasoning and the more automatic intuition differ
more concerned about food and health issues (Pliner,
in many respects. The evolutionarily older intuitive
Chaiken, & Flett, 1990; Rozin, 1999; Rozin, Fischler,
system helps people make approach –avoidance distinc-
Imada, Sarubin, & Wrzesniewski, 1999), and more
tions (essential for species survival) whereas the newer
sensitive to contagion (Nemeroff & Rozin, 2000) and to
magical beliefs-related emotion of disgust (Fessler, reasoning system helps people make true – false distinc-
Arguello, Mekdara, & Macias, 2003; Haidt, McCauley, tions (Epstein & Pacini, 1999). Intuitive processing bases
& Rozin, 1994) than men. Second, we expected to find a assessment of information on affects and personal
positive relationship between vegetarianism and MFH experiences whereas reasoning requires justification with
beliefs (hypothesis 2) because previous studies have objective evidence (Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier,
shown that some vegetarians have magical beliefs about 1996; Haidt, 2001).
bodily functions (Lindeman, Keskivaara, & Roschier, Intuitive and magical thinking are similar in many
2000) and about animal products (e.g. ‘meat arouses the respects. They are not based on conscious reasoning and
animal instinct in people’, Beardsworth & Keil, 1992, do not require justification via logic and evidence: They
p. 273). Third, we assumed positive attitude are rather self-evidently valid. Moreover, like intuitive
toward alternative medicine to be positively associated thinking (e.g. Epstein & Pacini, 1999; Smith &
with MFH beliefs because alternative medicine utilizes DeCoster, 2000), magical thinking is based on associ-
concepts that are in line with the magical laws of ations as shown by the examples of the law of contagion
contagion (the notions of impurities and purification) and and the law of similarity (for a positive connection
similarity (similar cures similar), as exemplified by between magical ideation and associative thinking, see
homeopathy (hypothesis 3). Mohr, Graves, Gianotti, Pizzagalli, & Brugger, 2001).
Many researchers have suggested that magical beliefs There is also empirical evidence that intuitive
and practices provide a sense of control over the thinking is typical for individuals who hold traditional
uncontrollable and that they reduce anxiety created by superstitions (Epstein et al., 1996; Wolfradt, Oubaid,
conditions of risk and uncertainty (Frazer, 1922/1963; Straube, Bischoff, & Mischo, 1999). We thus hypoth-
Malinowski, 1948/1984; Nemeroff & Rozin, 2000; Vyse, esized that MFH beliefs appeal especially to people who
1997; Zebb & Moore, 2003; Zusne & Jones, 1989). For process information in a more intuitive way than other
example, some athletes eat a lucky meal before an people (hypothesis 7).
K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74 67

Study 1 The web survey was implemented as a Java Servlet,


running on an Apache web server and a Tomcat application
Method server. The survey was placed in a hidden directory where it
was unavailable to casual browsers. All questionnaire data
Participants and answers were stored in a MySQL relational database.
The participants were 3261 Finnish students. Their mean The backups were encrypted with GPG (industry standard
age was 24 years (SD ¼ 4.67), with a range from 15 to 60 encryption protocol) to ensure confidentiality and all data
years. Seventy-four percent were females. Eighty-five transmissions were further encrypted with SSH (industry
percent of all the participants reported being full-time standard secure connection protocol).
students, 9% as being full-time employed, and the
remaining 6% as being otherwise occupied. Of those Measures
studying, 77% were university students and 23% attended Participants were asked to select their eating status from
a vocational school. Together, they represented a wide the following categories: “I am omnivorous”, “I avoid
variety of disciplines, including social sciences, natural eating red meat”, “I eat only fish and vegetarian food”, “I am
sciences, law, medicine, behavioral sciences, technology, a vegetarian”, and “I am a vegan”. The latter three
agriculture, arts, humanities, business and service. Of the alternatives were categorized as vegetarians, while first
original 3282 participants, 21 were excluded because of two alternatives were categorized as omnivores.
missing data. Magical beliefs were assessed with the Magical Beliefs
About Food and Health Scale (a in this study ¼ 0.84)
Procedure (MFH, Lindeman et al., 2000). The scale includes 17 items
The participants were recruited through six universities on magical thinking, most of which follow the law of
and 10 vocational schools in Finland. Where applicable, we similarity or contagion. The scale consists of items on
sent a recruitment message to a student e-mail list. Where general magical beliefs without any animal content (e.g. ‘If
such lists were lacking, an employee of the educational we don’t somehow clean our bodies, unhealthy toxins
institute posted information on the study on a communi- remain in them’) and items on animal products as
cation board (either real or virtual). A minority of the contaminants of food or personality (e.g. ‘Vegetarian food
participants ðn ¼ 280Þ was informed about the study at the is spoiled if it has been in contact with meat’ and
beginning of a lecture at their school. ‘Consumption of meat dulls thinking’), and also five filler
The 54 e-mail lists that we targeted had a total of items (e.g. ‘A high fiber diet is healthy’). All statements
approximately 16,000 subscribed members, and the edu- were rated on a five-point scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree,
cational institutes that posted messages on their communi- 5 ¼ strongly agree). A total score of magical beliefs was
cation boards had a total of approximately 4000 students. calculated by averaging all the items.
Since no data is available on how many people were reached Attitude toward alternative medicine was measured by
by the recruitment message, the response rate cannot be asking how well the respondents thought that the 12 listed
reliably calculated. treatments heal illnesses and/or promote health. The
The participants were told that the study concerned treatments were chosen out of those officially categorized
beliefs, personality, and cognition. Our names and contact as belonging to alternative medicine (Alternativmedicin-
information were available in the recruitment message sent kommitten, 1989; Eisenberg et al., 1993). They were:
out to the mailing lists. In the message, a hyperlink to the (1) chiropractic treatments, (2) acupuncture, (3) homeop-
Internet questionnaire was included. Confidentiality and athy, (4) nature medicine (e.g. herbs), (5) large doses of
the voluntary nature of participation were stressed, and the vitamins or micronutrients, (6) treatments of magnetic fields
respondents were given between 1 and 3 weeks time to or Kirlian photography, (7) meditation and/or mental
participate in the study. imagery, (8) stone therapy, (9) color therapy, (10) psychic
The respondents were informed that by taking part, they healing, (11) macrobiotics, and (12) living, unheated food.
had a chance of winning a 50e boat trip for two to the city of The participants indicated their belief in the treatments on a
Tallinn. All participants were also promised feedback on six-point scale (0 ¼ I do not know the therapy (scored as 1),
their responses approximately 2 months after their partici- 1 ¼ I do not believe in the therapy at all, 5 ¼ I strongly
pation in the study, which would require them to reveal their believe in the efficacy of this treatment). The scores were
pseudonym, but not their identity. Such feedback, requested averaged into a sum score ða ¼ 0:86Þ:
by and given to about one third of the participants, consisted Negative life events were measured by asking the
of a general description of the phenomena studied, absolute participants to mark whether and how many times they
scale ranges and means, and the participant’s own score on had experienced the following traumatic events: (1) the
each of the scales. Efforts were made to be especially death of a close relative or friend, (2) a serious illness of a
prudent in the choice of words, and to stress that the results close relative or friend, (3) a life-threatening situation, e.g.
were based on how the respondent had described him or an accident or a fire, (4) rape, incest, physical assault or
herself to us. robbery. Further, the participants were asked to mark how
68 K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

Table 1
Correlations between the variables in study 1

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6

MFH beliefs
Gendera 20.17***
Vegetarianismb 0.38*** 20.10***
Alternative medicine 0.44*** 20.14*** 0.10***
Negative life events 0.11*** 0.00 0.02 0.14***
Desire for control 20.03 0.05** 0.05** 0.02 0.12***
Intuitive thinking 0.26*** 20.18*** 0.07*** 0.33*** 0.11*** 0.09***

**p , 0:01; ***p , 0:001:


a
Female gender scored as 1, male gender as 2.
b
Omnivorous diet scored as 1, vegetarian diet as 2.

many, if any, of the following more acute crises situations MFH beliefs, R2 ¼ 0:33; Fð7; 3046Þ ¼ 217:75; p , 0:001
they had recently experienced: (1) menopause or mid-life (Table 2).
crisis (commonly expressed in Finland as a crisis of the 30- It should be noted that while desire for control predicted
or 40-year-olds), (2) divorce, (3) infidelity, (4) children significantly MFH beliefs, these variables did not correlate.
leaving home, (5) notice to quit or unemployment, (6) These results indicated that desire for control was a
economical difficulties, (7) re-qualification or change of suppressor variable, that is, it increased the multiplier R2
profession, (8) some other crisis. The number of experi- only by virtue of its correlations with the other independent
enced traumas and crises were counted together to get the variables (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Thus, desire for
amount of negative life events. control was a variable that enhanced the importance of the
The desirability of control scale (Burger & Cooper, other IVs because it suppressed irrelevant variance in them.
1979) was used to assess the participants’ motivation to
control events in their lives. The scale includes 20 seven-
point items (1 ¼ The statement doesn’t apply to me at all, Study 2
7 ¼ The statement always applies to me), such as “I prefer a
job where I have a lot of control over what I do and when I Study 2 aimed at extending knowledge about both the
do it” and “I enjoy making my own decisions” (a in this determinants of MFH beliefs and the personal meanings of
study ¼ 0.77). the beliefs, and thereby increasing understanding about why
Intuitive thinking was assessed by the experientiality unfounded food and health beliefs are adopted and
scale of the Rational – Experiential Inventory (REI-R, Pacini maintained. First, we expected MFH beliefs to increase
& Epstein, 1999). The scale consists of 20 five-point items along with eating disorder symptoms because individuals
(1 ¼ strongly disagree, 5 ¼ strongly agree), such as “I like with eating disorders have been shown to be highly anxious
to rely on my intuitive impressions” and “I believe in about getting polluted by food and to fear contagious
trusting my hunches”. The reliability of the scale ðaÞ in this contact even with people they like (Nemeroff & Cavanaugh,
study was 0.85. 1999; Schupak-Neuberg & Nemeroff, 1993) (hypothesis 1).
We also set the same hypotheses as in study 1 with regards
Results and discussion gender (hypothesis 2), vegetarianism (hypothesis 3), posi-
tive attitude toward alternative medicine (hypothesis 4),
Table 1 presents correlations between the variables. As negative life events (hypotheses 6), desire for control
illustrated, MFH beliefs were associated with female
gender, vegetarianism, positive attitude toward alternative Table 2
Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting MFH beliefs in study 1
medicine, negative life events, and intuition. Age did not
correlate with MFH beliefs, r ¼ 0:03; p , 0:04: Variable B b t
Next, a standard multiple regression analysis was
conducted between MFH beliefs as the dependent variable Gendera 20.08 20.07 24.27***
and gender, vegetarianism, attitude toward alternative Vegetarinismb 0.53 0.34 22.45***
Alternative medicine 0.02 0.35 22.23***
medicine, negative life events, desire for control, and
Negative events 0.02 0.07 4.77***
intuitive thinking as independent variables. In addition, Desire for control 20.07 20.07 24.61***
because Keinan (2002) has shown that desire for control Desire for control £ Negative events 0.01 0.00 0.19
mediates the relationship between stress and magical rituals, Intuition 0.10 0.11 6.61***
negative life events £ desire for control interaction ***p , 0:001:
term was added into the analyses. Except the interaction a
Female gender scored as 1, male gender as 2.
b
term, all IVs contributed significantly to the prediction of Omnivorous diet scored as 1, vegetarianism as 2.
K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74 69

(hypothesis 6), interaction of negative life events and Method


control (hypothesis 7) and intuitive thinking (hypothesis 8).
The second purpose of this study was to examine the Participants
kinds of functions MFH beliefs serve. Although the Participants were 189 individuals, whose ages ranged
importance of functional analyses of magical beliefs has from 18 to 66 years (M ¼ 26:74; SD ¼ 8.88). Of them, 84%
been acknowledged (e.g. Zusne & Jones, 1989), the were women, and 75% were full-time students from over ten
functions have not yet been empirically analyzed. There- fields of study, while the other participants were employed.
fore, to identify the potential functions of magical beliefs,
we utilized research on the functions of attitudes (Green- Procedure
wald, 1989; Herek, 1987; Katz, 1960; Shavitt, 1989, 1990; The participants were recruited from psychology courses
Smith, Bruner, & White, 1956; Snyder & DeBono, 1989). at the University of Helsinki and the Open University of
As Abelson and Prentice (1989) have noted, the concepts of Helsinki, Finland. The majority of the subjects (91%) were
belief and attitude can be used interchangeably in function administered the questionnaires during their lecture time.
research because both of them are typically accompanied by To increase the proportion of vegetarians, students who
cognition, affect, and action tendencies. chose a vegetarian dish in a student restaurant at the
First, MFH beliefs may serve a control function. They University of Helsinki were asked to participate in the study.
may help to organize and structure the world in a Forty students agreed to participate. Of them, 18 returned
meaningful and consistent fashion, imposing control, the questionnaire by mail as agreed. All participants were
order, clarity, and stability on an otherwise potentially told that the study concerned personality and attitudes
chaotic world of eating and health-related issues. Actually, toward food and health, that participation would be
many authors have suggested that the control function might voluntary, and that all information received would be
be the main function of magical thinking and other confidential.
superstitions (Malinowski, 1948/1984; Nemeroff & Rozin,
2000; Tambiah, 1990; Vyse, 1997). The second function Measures
MFH beliefs, desirability of control, and intuitive
that MFH beliefs may serve is the social identity function.
thinking (as in this study ¼ 0.85, 0.80, and 0.85, respect-
People may maintain beliefs and attitudes because they
ively), the number of negative life events experienced, and
allow them to establish their social identities by fostering
eating status were measured in the same way as in study 1.
identification with reference groups (e.g. Vyse, 1997), for
Of all the participants, 79% were omnivores or red-meat
example with proponents of macrobiotics or a group of
avoiders, and thus labeled omnivores. The rest of the
vegetarians (see e.g. Kandel & Pelto, 1980). In turn, beliefs
participants were grouped as vegetarians.
and attitudes serving a value-expressive function convey
Attitude toward alternative medicine was measured as in
concerns important to one’s self-concept. The beliefs thus
study 1 with only minor changes. The respondents were
allow people to express their own true self, their underlying
asked about the following treatments: (1) chiropractic
values, dispositions, and personality. Beliefs may also have treatments, (2) naprapathic treatments, (3) acupuncture,
a self-esteem function in that the belief may associate the (4) homeopathy, (5) natural vitamins or other nature cures,
self with positively regarded objects (e.g. organic foods or (6) large doses of vitamins, (7) treatments of magnetic fields
gourmet foods) and thus bolster self-esteem. Sometimes, or Kirlian photography, (8) primal therapy or Rosen-
beliefs and attitudes provide strategies for coping with therapy, (9) meditation, (10) stone or color therapy, (11)
anxiety generated by intrapsychic conflict or other undesir- psychic healing or shamanism, and (12) life style diets.
able or threatening issues. For example, according to Rozin, Three response alternatives were given to the subjects: 1 ¼ I
Lowery, Imada, and Haidt (1999), food-related disgust have never tried this treatment/the treatment is not familiar
based on magical thinking may help in repressing the idea of to me, 2 ¼ I have received this treatment, 3 ¼ I would like
our animalness, that is, disgust may have a defensive to try this treatment some day. Participants who responded
function. Finally, beliefs with a utilitarian function are with 2 and/or 3 received one score for each item. A summed
thought to help in achieving something positive and score was used in the analyses.
avoiding something negative. This function was clearly Eating disorder symptoms were assessed by the abbre-
illustrated in a TV document in which an anorectic girl said viated version of the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT, Garner,
that believing that animal products contaminate food is Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982) (a in this study ¼ 0.92).
useful for her because the belief allows her to skip many It consists of 26 items about weight, bulimia, and body
meals. image (e.g. ‘Feel extremely guilty after eating’ and ‘Find
To summarize, there are two purposes of this study: the myself preoccupied with food’), which are evaluated on a
first is to portray people who have magical beliefs about six-point scale (1 ¼ never, 6 ¼ always). As the authors
food and health. The second purpose is to examine the order have suggested, points 6, 5, and 4 were turned into points 3,
of perceived importance of six different functions that MFH 2, and 1 (respectively) and points under 4 were counted as
beliefs may serve. 0 points. In the analyses, the mean score was used.
70 K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

Table 3 repeated along with instructions to fill in the Function


Example items and reliabilities of the subscales measuring functions of Scale if the participant agreed at least partly with any of
MFH beliefs
the example statements. The participants evaluated
Function Cronbach’s Example items separately the functions of two items on general
alpha ðaÞ magical beliefs, two items on animal products as food
contaminants and two items on animal products as
Control 0.76 1. Because of these beliefs, my opinions personality contaminants. The results were similar for all
about food and health are clear
three kinds of items. Therefore, the sum scores for each
2. These beliefs have clarified my view
of the world function were calculated by averaging the items across the
Social identity 0.83 1. It is important for me to belong to a three areas of MFH beliefs. Of the participants, 59
group of people who think in this way agreed with the statements presented and filled in the
2. It is important to the people closest to Function Scale.
me that I think this way
Value-expressive 0.92 1. My beliefs reflect my personal values
2. My beliefs are essentially associated Results
with my personal identity
Self-esteem 0.87 1. My beliefs give me a feeling that I am The results showed that MFH beliefs correlated posi-
a good person tively with vegetarianism, positive attitude toward alterna-
2. If I thought otherwise, my self-worth
would be lower
tive medicine, symptoms of eating disorders, and intuition
Defensive 0.90 1. My beliefs help me to cope with (Table 4). Age did not correlate with MFH beliefs, r ¼ 0:11;
emotional issues ns. A standard multiple regression analysis indicated that
2. My beliefs protect me from some the same variables that correlated with MFH beliefs also
anxiety predicted them, although the contribution of intuition was
Utilitarian 0.80 1. My beliefs can be useful to me in
some way
only marginally significant, R2 ¼ 0:34; Fð8; 150Þ ¼ 9:18;
2. I might avoid something negative by p , 0:001 (Table 5).
thinking like this The importance of the various functions of MFH beliefs
was analyzed with one-way within-participants ANOVA,
followed by planned pair-wise comparisons. Because the
Functions of MFH beliefs were assessed with a Function Mauchley’s test of sphericity showed that sphericity could
Scale. It included six five-point items (1 ¼ does not reflect my not be assumed, we used an epsilon-corrected test. The
opinions at all, 5 ¼ reflects my opinions very well) for each of the Greenhouse-Geisser test showed that the functions differed
six functions (i.e. control, social identity, value-expressive, self- significantly from each other, Fð3:846; 223:075Þ ¼ 34:18;
esteem, defensive, and utilitarian function). The scale was p , 0:001; h2 ¼ 0:371: The results of the pairwise tests
developed on the basis of Herek’s (1987) Attitude Functions indicated that the most important function was the value-
Inventory, Shavitt’s (1990) Coding Scheme for Attitude expressive function (M ¼ 2:99; SD ¼ 0.13), which was
Functions, and the literature on attitude functions (e.g. Katz, assessed as more important than the control (M ¼ 2:72;
1960; Smith et al., 1956). Examples of the items and reliabilities SD ¼ 0.10), utilitarian (M ¼ 2:53; SD ¼ 0.09), self-esteem
for the six function subscales are presented in Table 3. The (M ¼ 2:34; SD ¼ 0.12), defensive (M ¼ 2:06; SD ¼ 0.10)
reliabilities were examined by item analysis, but there were no and social identity functions (M ¼ 1:94; SD ¼ 0.09), all
items whose removal would have improved the reliabilities. p , 0:02: The control function was rated as second in
The Function Scale was presented after the MFH Scale, importance, and it differed significantly from all the other
and six example statements of the MFH Scale were functions ðp , 0:02Þ except for the utilitarian function (ns).

Table 4
Correlations between the variables in study 2

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

MFH beliefs
Gendera 20.12
Vegetarianismb 0.32*** 20.13
Alternative medicine 0.39*** 20.02 0.08
Negative life events 0.13 0.04 0.07 0.19*
Desire for control 0.10 0.08 20.03 0.06 0.20**
Intuitive thinking 0.21** 20.19** 0.06 0.26*** 0.18* 0.06
Eating disorder Symptoms 0.24*** 20.11 2 0.04 0.15 0.12 0.06 0.02

*p , 0:05; **p , 0:01; ***p , 0:001.


a
Female gender scored as 1, male gender as 2.
b
Omnivorous diet scored as 1, vegetarian diet as 2.
K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74 71

Table 5 the majority of people who use only conventional medicine,


Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting MFH beliefs in study 2 individuals with positive attitude toward alternative medi-
Variable B b t cine avoid preservatives, artificial additives, and unspecified
toxins (Astin, 1998; Gilovich, 1991; Hines, 1988).
Gendera 0.02 0.01 0.15 That MFH beliefs might be related to approach and
Vegetarinismb 0.50 0.36 5.36*** avoidance behavior, rather than making true – false distinc-
Alternative medicine 0.07 0.34 4.87*** tions about food and nutrition, was further supported by the
Negative events 0.00 20.01 20.18
findings that MFH beliefs increased together with intuitive
Desire for control 20.03 20.03 20.46
Desire for control £ Negative events 0.01 0.02 0.23 thinking, which, by definition, is characterized by affective
Intuition 0.13 0.13 1.91 þ information processing and guidance of approach – avoid-
Eating disorder symptoms 0.20 0.17 2.48* ance behavior (Epstein, 1990; Haidt, 2001). Thus, magical
þ p , 0:06; *p , 0:05; ***p , 0:001.
beliefs may not stem only from cognitive mistakes which
a
Female gender scored as 1, male gender as 2. have been shown to be explanations of some magical beliefs
b
Omnivorous diet scored as 1, vegetarianism as 2. (Gilovich, 1991; Hines, 1988; Vyse, 1997), but may also
derive from affective processes and subsequent judgments.
The utilitarian function differed significantly from the other In addition to the positive association between intuitive
functions ðp , 0:012Þ excluding the control and the self- thinking and MFH beliefs, the following results also support
esteem functions (ns). Further, the self-esteem function this notion: the participants agreed that their food and health
differed significantly from all other functions ðp , 0:012Þ beliefs help them to achieve something positive and to avoid
except the utilitarian function (ns). Finally, the social something negative in that they rated the utilitarian function
identity and defensive functions were rated as the least as the second most important function and the expression of
import of all the functions, and their ratings did not differ personal values and ideals as the most important function of
significantly from each other (ns). their beliefs. Given that researchers have typically failed to
The correlations between the functions ranged from 0.32 mention the value-expressive function as a function of
ðp , 0:05Þ to 0.80 ðp , 0:001Þ: The highest correlation was magical beliefs, this result was somewhat unexpected.
between the value-expressive and control function. However, beliefs with a value-expressive function have
distinct properties that link them to approach – avoidance
behavior: they evoke strong emotions in the individual and
General discussion are related to more fundamental values (Abelson &
Prentice, 1989). Values and emotions, in turn, are
According to the results, the average person maintaining constructions by which people evaluate things and make
MFH beliefs was a woman and a vegetarian, thought in a good – bad distinctions; they guide the individual to
more intuitive way, demonstrated more eating-disordered approach issues that create positive feelings and to avoid
thinking, relied more on alternative medicine, and to a issues that cause negative feelings (Gohm & Clore, 2000;
smaller degree, had experienced more negative life events Rohan, 2000; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987).
than the average nonbeliever. The believers reported their A slight connection between MFH beliefs and negative
MFH beliefs to serve a value-expressive function more than life experiences was found in study 1, while no connection
any of the other functions. The second most important was found in study 2. The positive association between
functions were those of control, utilitarianism, and self- MFH beliefs and negative life events like divorce or
esteem, while the social identity and defensive functions unemployment may even seem curious but the hypothesis
were reported less frequently. was based on earlier findings, which have shown that
That vegetarianism, eating-disordered thinking, and traditional superstitions increase along with crises, stress,
positive attitude toward alternative medicine predicted and frightening situations (Keinan, 1994, 2002; Lillqvist &
MFH beliefs was in line with our hypothesis. What unite Lindeman, 1998; Padgett & Jorgenson, 1982). One of the
these three issues are not so much a matter of knowledge explanations of magical thinking is that it provides reasons
about food or health but rather an affective avoidance of for phenomena that are otherwise inexplicable and unfami-
certain substances not avoided by others. Many vegetarians liar (Bronowski, 1978; Vyse, 1997). Besides the relations
avoid animal products not only because of health reasons between negative life events and MFH beliefs, another one
but also because of animal ethics, ecological reasons, and of our results is in line with this explanation: when the
disgust (Amato & Partridge, 1989; Beardsworth & Keil, participants of study 2 were asked about the functions their
1992; Fessler et al., 2003; Rozin et al., 1997). Likewise, beliefs served, they rated the control function as the second
individuals with symptoms of eating disorders avoid many most important function stating that their beliefs had
foods because of their calories and fat content, and also clarified and organized their views of the world, nature,
because of feelings of disgust and fear (Harvey, Troop, people, and health. Thus, the participants’ views on the
Treasure, & Murphy, 2002; Nemeroff & Cavanaugh, 1999; functions of their magical beliefs paralleled many research-
Schupak-Neuberg & Nemeroff, 1993). In turn, unlike ers’ (Malinowski, 1948/1984; Nemeroff & Rozin, 2000;
72 K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

Tambiah, 1990; Vyse, 1997) assumptions that magical suggest that affective information on food and health may be
beliefs are experienced as clarifying and interpreting especially effective for women, vegetarians, people with a
different things, therefore making the world appear positive attitude toward alternative medicine, and people
organized, predictable and controllable. Accordingly, a with eating disorder symptoms, that is, to people who are
person whose life is stressful and who wants some order into most prone to MFH beliefs.
the chaos may turn to simple instructions marketed as the
‘truth’, such as instructions on eating a whole watermelon a
day to keep the kidneys clean (a suggestion by Billings, Acknowledgements
1997).
Had we measured the food- and health-related worries This research was supported by grants from the Finnish
instead of general kinds of negative life experiences, we Cultural Foundation (no. 00020204) and the Academy of
might have found a stronger connection between negative Finland (no. 200828).
events and MFH beliefs. This seems possible given that two
of the characteristics connected to increased nutritional
concerns and health worries, namely, female gender and
References
eating disorder symptoms (Rozin et al., 1999) were also
characteristic of the believers of MFH beliefs in our two
Abelson, R. P., & Prentice, D. A. (1989). Beliefs as possessions: a
studies. However, our results did not replicate the finding functional perspective. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G.
that the relationship between MFH beliefs and stressful Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure and function (pp. 361– 381). New
situations is mediated by desire for control (Keinan, 2002). Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Before concluding, a major limitation of our research Alternativmedicinkommitten (1989). Alternativa terapier i Sverige—en
kartläggning. Statens offentliga utredningar. [Alternative medical
should be mentioned. The results concerning the functions
treatments in Sweden-a survey. The state’s official position]. Socialde-
of the beliefs are tentative firstly because the function results partmentet, 62.
were based on the respondents’ self-reports, and thus only Amato, P. R., & Partridge, S. A. (1989). The new vegetarians. Promoting
conscious functions were tapped. This might explain why health and protecting life. New York: Plenum Press.
the participants rated the defensive function among the least Astin, J. (1998). Why patients use alternative medicine: results of a national
study. Journal of American Medical Association, 279, 1548– 1553.
important functions of their beliefs. Second, the small
Beardsworth, A., & Keil, T. (1992). The vegetarian option: varieties,
number of participants who responded to the function scale conversions, motives, and careers. The Sociological Review, 40,
restricts the results of the functions of the beliefs. Thus, it is 253 –293.
important to supplement these findings by research with a Billings, T. E. (1997). Trouble-shooting: avoiding or overcoming problems
larger sample of participants, and to analyze the overlapping in raw and living food diets, Retrieved August 15, 2003 from: http://
functions more specifically. www.living-foods.com/articles/troubleshooting.html.
Bronowski, J. (1978). Magic, science, and civilization. New York:
The results suggest that unfounded food and health Columbia University Press.
beliefs are adopted and hold in the realm of emotions, Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control.
intuition, approach – avoidance behavior, values, and iden- Motivation and Emotion, 3, 381 –393.
tity. As such, the results extend the findings about food Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social
choice which have shown that what is eaten and what psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Diamond, H., & Diamond, M. (1985). Fit for life. New York: Warner.
avoided also indicate the eater’s values and identity Eisenberg, D. M., Kessler, R. C., Foster, C., Norlock, F. E., Calkins, D. R.,
(Fischler, 1988; Furst, Connors, Bisogni, Sobal, & Winter & Delbanco, T. L. (1993). Unconventional medicine in the United
Falk, 1996; Lindeman & Sirelius, 2001; Lindeman & Stark, States—prevalence, costs, and patterns of use. The New England
1999, 2000). Journal of Medicine, 328, 246–252.
Even though food beliefs and food choice thus seem to be Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.),
Handbook of personality (pp. 165–192). New York: Guilford Press.
emotionally laden, much of the nutritional and health
Epstein, S., & Pacini, R. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process
maintenance information provided to people has tradition- theories from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In S.
ally been based on scientific arguments, which include Chaiken, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology
rational, abstract and objective concepts, such as nutrients (pp. 462 –482). New York: Guilford Press.
and probabilities. While not suggesting an abandonment of Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual
differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical –rational thinking
factual information, our results point at a need for a more
styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405.
affective approach in educating about nutritional issues. Fessler, D. M. T., Arguello, A. P., Mekdara, J. M., & Macias, R. (2003).
This view is supported by the results that traditional Disgust sensitivity and meat consumption: a test of an emotivist account
nutritional knowledge and food choice behavior are only of moral vegetarianism. Appetite, 41, 31–41.
moderately connected to each other (Wardle, 1993). We Fischler, C. (1988). Food, self, and identity. Social Science Information, 27,
275 –292.
suggest that changing or preventing unfounded beliefs about
Frazer, J. G. (1963). The golden bough: A study in magic and religion. New
food, dieting, and health would not be best achieved through York: Maximillan.
mere scientific arguments, but by taking into account the Furst, T., Connors, M., Bisogni, C. A., Sobal, J., & Winter Falk, L. (1996).
emotional, value-related side of these issues. We further Food choice: a conceptual model of the process. Appetite, 26, 247 –266.
K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74 73

Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The Mohr, C., Graves, R. E., Gianotti, L. R. R., Pizzagalli, D., & Brugger, P.
eating attitudes test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. (2001). Loose but normal: a semantic association study. Journal of
Psychological Medicine, 12, 871– 878. Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 475–483.
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human Nemeroff, C., & Cavanaugh, C. J. (1999). The ethics of emaciation: moral
reasons in everyday life. New York: Free Press. connotations of the body, self, and diet. In M. Siegal, & C. Petersen
Gohm, C. L., & Clore, G. L. (2000). Individual differences in emotional (Eds.), Children’s understanding of biology and health. Cambridge
experience: mapping available scales to processes. Personality and studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 183–206). New York:
Social Psychology, 26, 679–697. Cambridge University Press.
Greenwald, A. G. (1989). Why attitudes are important. Defining attitude Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (2000). The makings of the magical mind: the
and attitude theory 20 years later. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & nature and function of sympathetic magical thinking. In K. S.
A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure and function (pp. 429–440). Rosengren, & C. N. Johnson (Eds.), Imagining the impossible. Magical,
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. scientific, and religious thinking in children (pp. 1–34). New York:
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist Cambridge University Press.
approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814 –834. Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs and the ratio-
in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 972– 987.
701–713. Padgett, V. R., & Jorgenson, D. O. (1982). Superstition and economic
Harvey, T., Troop, N. A., Treasure, J. L., & Murphy, T. (2002). Fear, threat: Germany, 1918– 1940. Personality and Social Psychology
disgust, and abnormal eating attitudes: a preliminary study. Inter- Bulletin, 8, 736 –741.
national Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 213–218. Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Gender differences in concern
Herek, G. M. (1987). Can functions be measured? A new perspective on the with body weight and physical appearance over the life span.
functional approach to attitudes. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 263– 273.
285–303. Rohan, M. J. (2000). A rose by any name? The values construct.
Hines, T. (1988). Pseudoscience and the paranormal. New York: Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 255–277.
Prometheus. Rozin, P. (1999). Food is fundamental, fun, frightening, and far-reaching.
Irwin, H. J. (1993). Belief in the paranormal: a review of the empirical Social Research, 66, 9–30.
literature. The Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research,
Rozin, P., Fischler, C., Imada, S., Sarubin, A., & Wrzesniewski, A. (1999).
87, 1– 39.
Attitudes to food and the role of food in life in the USA, Japan, Flemish
Kandel, R. F., & Pelto, G. H. (1980). The health food movement. Social
Belgium and France: possible implications for the diet-health debate.
revitalization or alternative health maintenance system? In N. W.
Appetite, 33, 163–180.
Jerome, R. F. Kandel, & G. H. Pelto (Eds.), Nutritional anthropology:
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad
Contemporary approaches to diet and culture (pp. 327–363). New
hypothesis: a mapping between three moral emotions (contempt,
York: Redgrave.
anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy,
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public
divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163– 204.
574– 586.
Keinan, G. (1994). Effects of stress and tolerance of ambiguity on magical
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Moralization and becoming a
thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 48 –55.
vegetarian: the transformation of preferences into values and the
Keinan, G. (2002). The effects of stress and desire for control on
recruitment of disgust. Psychological Science, 8, 67– 73.
superstitious behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of
102–108.
Kushi, M. (1989). The book of macrobiotics. The universal way of health sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality
and happiness. New York: Japan Publications. and Social Psychology, 50, 703–712.
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (1990). The laws of sympathetic magic. A
approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137. psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In J. W. Stiegler,
Lillqvist, O., & Lindeman, M. (1998). Belief in astrology as a strategy for R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology. Essays on
self-verification and coping with negative life-events. European comparative human development (pp. 205 –232). Cambridge, England:
Psychologist, 3, 202–208. Cambridge University Press.
Lindeman, M., Keskivaara, P., & Roschier, M. (2000). Assessment of Schupak-Neuberg, E., & Nemeroff, C. (1993). Disturbances in identity and
magical beliefs about food and health. Journal of Health Psychology, 5, self-regulation in bulimia nervosa: implications for a metaphorical
199–213. perspective of body as self. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
Lindeman, M., & Sirelius, M. (2001). Food choice ideologies: the modern 13, 335–347.
manifestations of normative and humanist views of the world. Appetite, Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a psychological structure of
37, 175 –184. human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
Lindeman, M., & Stark, K. (1999). Pleasure, pursuit of health or negotiation 550– 562.
of identity? Personality correlates of food choice motives among young Shavitt, S. (1989). Operationalizing functional theories of attitudes. In A. R.
and middle-aged women. Appetite, 33, 141 –161. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure
Lindeman, M., & Stark, K. (2000). Loss of pleasure, ideological food and function (pp. 311 – 337). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
choice reasons and eating pathology. Appetite, 35, 263–268. Associates.
Malinowski, B. (1984). Magic, science, and religion. Garden City, NY: Shavitt, S. (1990). The role of attitude objects in attitude functions. Journal
Doubleday. of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 124–148.
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning.
are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.
neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and
models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457. cognitive psychology: conceptual integration and links to underlying
Meigs, A. S. (1984). Food, sex, and pollution. A new Guinea religion. New memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4,
Jersey: Rutgers University Press. 108– 131.
74 K. Aarnio, M. Lindeman / Appetite 43 (2004) 65–74

Smith, M. B., Bruner, J. S., & White, R. W. (1956). Opinions and Wardle, J. (1993). Food choices and health evaluation. Psychology and
personality. New York: Wiley. Health, 8, 65 –75.
Snyder, M., & DeBono, K. G. (1989). Understanding the functions of Wolfradt, U., Oubaid, V., Straube, E. R., Bischoff, N., & Mischo, J. (1999).
attitudes: lessons from personality and social behavior. In A. R. Thinking styles, schizotypal traits and anomalous experiences.
Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 821 –830.
and function (pp. 339–359). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Vyse, S. A. (1997). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition.
Spence, T. H., (n.d.). Detoxification. Retrieved August 15, 2003 from: New York: Oxford University Press.
http://chetday.com/spencedetox.html Zebb, B. J., & Moore, M. C. (2003). Superstitiousness and perceived
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. anxiety control as predictors of psychological distress. Anxiety
Needham Hights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Disorders, 17, 115 –130.
Tambiah, S. J. (1990). Magic, science, religion, and the scope of Zusne, L., & Jones, W. H. (1989). Anomalistic psychology—A study of
rationality. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. magical thinking. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

You might also like