You are on page 1of 7

Special Focus on Teaching for Musical Creativity

by Andrew T. Landau and Charles J. Limb

The Neuroscience of
Improvisation
Abstract: Current research in the neuroscience of musical creativity reveals prom-
ising implications for the value of learning to improvise. This article outlines the
neuroscientific literature on musical improvisation and relates these findings to the
benefits of musical creativity. We begin by describing the neural substrates of flow
with respect to the mechanisms of improvisation. We then discuss how neuroscien-
tific literature supports the argument that musical improvisation is a powerful means
to express oneself and communicate with others, facilitating self-actualization and
deeper connections with peers. Music educators can use these discoveries to bolster
their advocacy for the use of creativity in music education and support their use of
improvisation in the classroom.
Keywords: brain research, creativity, discoveries, flow, improvisation, improvise,
neuroscience

What can brain


M
usic is a central aspect of the human changes in deoxyhemoglobin/hemoglobin
experience. It is a reliable source of levels known as the blood oxygen level– research tell us
achievement, meaning, and social dependent (BOLD) signal. The BOLD signal
connection1 and has even been implicated shows a stereotypical response to increases about musical
as a causal factor of human cognitive evo- or decreases in the neural activity of a brain
lution.2 While the reasons for such a posi- region, allowing fMRI to reveal patterns of
creativity, especially
tive view are intuitive and self-explanatory activation, deactivation, and functional con- improvisation?
for anyone who has experience with music, nectivity throughout the brain.4 An increas-
researchers since Darwin have attempted to ing number of studies use fMRI to analyze
provide objective evidence that explains the the neural activity of humans when engag-
importance of music to human life.3 Recent ing in creative tasks, including investiga-
advances in noninvasive neuroimaging tech- tions into the neural mechanisms of musical
nologies such as functional magnetic reso- improvisation.5 Creativity requires the pro-
nance imaging (fMRI) have allowed great duction of novel ideas that are germane to
strides in the investigation of music percep- a given context, which makes the tempo-
tion and production. Functional MRI mea- rally structured format of musical impro-
sures brain activity through the detection of visation an ideal behavior to study this

Andrew T. Landau is a PhD candidate in the Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology at Harvard University,
Boston, Massachusetts. He can be contacted at atlandau@g.harvard.edu. Dr. J. Charles Limb is the Francis A. Sooy Professor
at the UCSF Medical Center’s Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, San Francisco, California. He can be
contacted at Charles.Limb@ucsf.edu.

NAfME is glad to offer one hour of professional development recognition to you for reading this article.
Copyright © 2017 National Association
Please follow the link below and complete a short quiz to receive your certificate of completion. for Music Education
http://bit.ly/NeuroscienceOfImprovisation DOI: 10.1177/0027432116687373
http://mej.sagepub.com

www.nafme.org 27
elusive phenomenon. Despite the inher- Flow and the Deactivated Brain during improvisation compared to con-
ent challenges of conducting scientific trol conditions only implies that this area
investigations of something defined by Improvisation is a remarkably complex is less active during improvisation. For
novelty and unpredictability,6 a series activity. Musicians are tasked with spon- example, one study found that despite
of experiments using complementary taneously selecting and executing actions overall reductions in DLPFC activity, it
methodologies has begun to reveal that are both novel and appropriate for showed stronger functional connectivity
unique aspects of the creative process.7 the current musical context throughout with other motor areas during improvisa-
There has been an increase in advo- the course of every improvised perfor- tion than for memorized performance.21
cacy for the use of creativity in music mance. Based on these observations, This implies that cognitive control is not
education programs over the past few one may expect that the brain areas entirely disengaged during improvisation
decades. 8 This advocacy has been involved in higher-order motor plan- but used in a frugal and efficient manner
justified by the results witnessed by ning and cognitive control would show and relatively suspended in comparison
music teachers who use creativity in an increase in activity when improvis- to memorized control conditions.
their classrooms as well as psychology ing. However, the dorsolateral prefron- Reductions in cognitive control may
studies that consistently support wide- tal cortex (DLPFC), a brain region that be a neural correlate of flow, a state
ranging benefits of musical creativity.9 sits just behind the forehead and is cen- that musicians commonly report enter-
While human creativity manifests itself tral to cognitive control,14 shows strong ing during improvisation.22 As described
through myriad disciplines beyond art, deactivations during musical improvisa- by researcher Mihály Csíkszentmihályi,
engaging in musical creativity provides tion (Figure 1). Intriguingly, these deac- “flow is an almost effortless yet highly
a unique array of benefits that appear tivations become more pronounced focused state of consciousness,” which
to be more difficult or impossible to when the experimental design allows he often refers to as optimal experience.23
achieve by participating in other activi- higher levels of musical freedom and Entering a flow state during improvi-
ties. Engaging in musical creativity complexity. 15 For example, in studies sation can have a profoundly positive
through improvisation and composition requiring musicians to produce impro- impact on the experience.24 Decreases
is an effective way to gain skill at an visations that are constrained by mel- in self-monitoring and top-down con-
instrument,10 develop divergent thinking ody or rhythm on a five- or twelve-note trol may result in the disappearance of
ability, 11 enhance communication and keyboard (e.g., improvising any melody self-consciousness and reduced worry
trust between musicians, 12 and facili- over a fixed rhythm), the DLPFC has of failure, a key component of flow,
tate self-actualization.13 Neuroscientific either become deactivated or showed thus creating a more immediate connec-
research approaches this topic from a no change in activity during improvisa- tion with the music.25 According to this
biological perspective that supports and tion.16 Two studies that allowed higher argument, an experimental design that
complements these views. levels of musical freedom and complex- requires active self-monitoring should
How do neuroscientific findings jus- ity by removing melodic and rhythmic demonstrate an increase in DLPFC activ-
tify the beneficial impact of creativity constraints, used a thirty-five-note chro- ity. As expected, one study that incor-
within music education programs? First, matic keyboard, and recruited experi- porated self-monitoring by requiring
we explain how widespread deactiva- enced improvisers found widespread musicians to remember their improvisa-
tions in the prefrontal cortex, an area deactivations of the DLPFC. 17 On the tions and reproduce them at a later time
involved in cognitive control and self- other hand, a highly restrictive study showed an increase in DLPFC activity,
monitoring, may facilitate an individual’s requiring musicians to produce rhythmic likely reducing the possibility of expe-
entry into a “flow state,” the psychologi- patterns with a single percussive sound riencing flow. 26 Flow states are most
cal correlate of getting in the groove. showed an increase in DLPFC activity often entered during activities with clear
Second, we discuss potential roles of relative to the control condition.18 goals and immediate feedback,27 both of
the medial frontal cortex in coordinat- The reduction of cognitive con- which are present in musical improvi-
ing the production of a cohesive musical trol during an extremely complex task sation. Producing music that “works,”
narrative during improvisation. Third, we allows rapid selection and execution of so to speak, can lead to an enhanced
describe how the activation of different novel musical material.19 Without the sense of achievement and confidence
brain areas during solo and interactive interference of monitoring processes in music, and this achievement can be
improvisation support meaningful com- that insert a temporal delay into the realized in real time because aural feed-
munication through music. We conclude execution of actions, 20 musicians are back is provided instantly.28 The cor-
by arguing that neuroscience research able to spontaneously generate musi- relation between musical freedom and
clearly supports the use of creativity cal ideas in a timely fashion. It should DLPFC deactivation also relates to the
in music education programs and that be noted that deactivations of cognitive requirement of a challenge-skill balance
music educators can use these findings to control regions during improvisation do during flow states.29 For example, musi-
bolster their advocacy efforts and enrich not indicate an absence of all cognitive cians report that they only experience
their use of creativity in the classroom. control. A relative reduction of DLPFC flow when playing with other musicians

28 Music Educators Journal March 2017


that are matched in skill level.30 Another
study found a significant negative cor- FIGURE 1
relation between DLPFC activity and
Brain Areas Involved in Improvisation
expertise,31 suggesting that flow states
are easier to enter when a musician is
at ease in their musical environment.32
Given that the experience of flow is
highly rewarding and seems to unlock
creative potential,33 findings that relate
observable neural correlates of flow
to controllable behavioral conditions
should be of significant interest to music
educators. These studies make the case
that it is critical for musicians to feel
confident and comfortable in their musi-
cal environment to experience flow. An
educator can facilitate these conditions
by cultivating trust between musicians,
which may encourage them to release
social inhibitions and perform at the
peak of their abilities. Furthermore,
while designing situations in which stu-
dents must improvise with peers at vary-
ing skill levels may be a useful tool to
increase musical competency, improvi-
sational tasks well suited to the student’s
skill level may engender a more imme- (A) Lateral surface of the left hemisphere. (B) Sagittal slice of the medial wall of the brain. See main text for
a description of the functional roles of each area listed. The colored areas indicate approximate locations.
diately rewarding experience.34

Crafting a Cohesive Narrative in novel combinations. The medial or mediate the generative processing
The DLPFC is a part of a larger network frontal cortex (MFC)—a cortical brain required for playing repeated patterns
of brain regions called the prefrontal region encompassing several areas such like arpeggios. 43 Furthermore, MFC
cortex (PFC). The PFC often becomes as the presupplementary motor area activity is almost exclusively present
activated to guide the flow of neural (pSMA), the supplementary motor area when there is conflict between compet-
activity to fit the needs of a current (SMA), and the anterior cingulate cortex ing motor representations 44 and when
task,35 especially when that task is not (ACC)—has been strongly implicated in actions must be internally generated,
yet learned. As behaviors like impro- the selection and sequencing of musical both of which are true during improvi-
visation become more automated, the plans during improvisation (Figure 1). sational contexts.45 The MFC may also
brain regions activated to implement The MFC has become activated dur- be involved in shaping a cohesive nar-
them shift from prefrontal control areas ing every imaging experiment that has rative during improvisation. It is exten-
to lower-order regions that store motor reported neural activity during musical sively connected with auditory areas,
programs in long-term memory such improvisation.39 Different areas of the cognitive regions important for higher-
as the premotor area and the cerebel- MFC are selectively activated during order motor control, and limbic regions
lum.36 These brain areas are important preparation and execution phases of involved in emotional processing.46 This
for transforming a musical plan into a motor tasks.40 It is consistently activated confluence of anatomical connectiv-
motor program that can be executed by for the coordination of complex sequen- ity allows the MFC to integrate current
the primary motor cortex. 37 However, tial behaviors41 and likely plays a role musical context, structural rules related
improvisations cannot be fully auto- in the generation of compound action to harmony and form, and the emotional
mated since musicians must decide what patterns. 42 These findings implicate state of the musician to guide the selec-
to play extemporaneously. Even if musi- the MFC in the sequential planning of tion of an optimal musical idea through
cians improvise with automated motor musical ideas during improvisation. For improvisation.
programs stored in long-term memory,38 example, the MFC may be recruited to Transitioning from DLPFC-mediated
the creative element of improvisation coordinate playing a particular harmony cognitive control to MFC-mediated
comes from linking these ideas together at the precise moment of a key change automated integrative processing brings

www.nafme.org 29
about a qualitative shift in a musician’s represent someone’s unique musical right hemisphere homologues during
experience of improvisation and is nec- voice.52 Furthermore, the rPFC is a criti- spontaneous musical interaction (Figure
essary for a range of benefits associated cal brain region for value-based guid- 1). These areas are strongly involved in
with improvisation. Educators can ena- ance of actions, supporting the idea that the production and comprehension of
ble this shift through focused training of expressing oneself through improvisa- both language and music.61 The IFG is
musical structure and by encouraging tion is intrinsically rewarding.53 important for syntactical processing,62
extensive practice outside the classroom. Although there has only been one and the pSTG is important for audi-
The more prepared a student is for a study that reports rPFC activation during tory working memory,63 suggesting that
musical situation, the more likely it is that improvisation,54 this observation strongly these areas work together during inter-
they will be able to call on their auto- supports psychological and pedagogi- active improvisation to track what the
mated resources and fluidly complete cal observations that improvisation is a other musician is playing and “reply”
an improvisation while telling a musi- rewarding behavior and can facilitate with an appropriate musical response,64
cal story in the process. Learning how expression and communication. 55 The such as when one musician transposes
to produce meaningful improvisations correlation between musical freedom, or expands on a motive just performed
can facilitate a higher understanding deactivations of the DLPFC that imply the by the other to engage in meaningful
of musical structure, enhanced musical experience of flow, and activations of the musical dialogue.65 A statistical analysis
awareness, and greater comfort commu- rPFC that support self-expression is most of the music improvised in Donnay et
nicating through music,47 often result- likely not a coincidence. Flow and crea- al. showed that the musicians used simi-
ing in heightened self-confidence and tivity are intimately connected in musical lar notes, rhythms, and melodies when
motivation to play music.48 Conferring improvisation. Studies have shown that improvising, confirming that they were
this ability to students is critical to their the degree of flow experienced when truly interacting with one another.
engagement with music and can serve to composing or improvising music is cor- Musicians commonly report feel-
empower the student’s musicality. related with how creative the result is ing strongly connected to others when
and how much personal significance is improvising together, and this can lead
Expression and Communication attached to the experience.56 Flow is also to a heightened sense of connection
through Improvisation impeded when a musician cannot take and empathy toward the other musi-
ownership of their music or if the musical cians.66 In this regard, it is notable that
We now discuss the results of two setting is too easy for their skill level.57 the musicians in Donnay et al. showed
papers with experimental designs that The best explanation for why the rPFC deactivations of the angular gyrus, an
provide a window into unique aspects has not been activated during other stud- area involved in semantic and con-
of the creative process. 49 Limb and ies is methodological differences58; some ceptual processing.67 Musical commu-
Braun found focal activation of a brain studies may not have provided musicians nication devoid of semantic content
region just above the eyes called the with sufficient freedom to generate com- suggests an immediate, raw connection
rostral prefrontal cortex (rPFC) in pro- plex musical expressions.59 However, between musicians,68 which may explain
fessional jazz musicians when improvis- designing an experiment that reliably why music is such a powerful medium
ing with high levels of musical freedom promotes creative self-expression is very for developing social bonds and feel-
and little constraint (Figure 1). Activ- challenging,60 especially when the musi- ing connected to peers. Furthermore,
ity in the rPFC has been linked to the cians must perform inside an fMRI scan- this finding may relate to the notion of
production of internally generated ner. More research must be done to clarify music as a “universal language” in that
thoughts and actions and the integra- what role the rPFC plays in improvisation. no involvement of semantic vocabulary
tion of higher-order goals with current While Limb and Braun’s study sup- appears to be necessary for musical
task rules.50 For example, a study that ports the psychological benefits of solo interaction to occur.
required subjects to extemporaneously improvisation, the findings of Donnay These studies have clear implica-
produce autobiographical narratives in and colleagues strongly corroborate the tions for the music educator. The neu-
English and sign language found that notion that musicians can have mean- ral signature of improvisation strongly
the rPFC was active in both cases.51 The ingful interactions through improvi- suggests that musicians can effectively
activation of the rPFC during speech sation. The musicians in the study express themselves and communicate
and sign language, two highly differ- improvised four-bar segments back and with their peers through musical creativ-
ent modes of communication, indicate forth with one another, allowing the ity. Emphasizing this aspect of improvi-
that its function is fundamental to the authors to image the brains of musi- sation, rather than the technical skills
core feature of the task, autobiographi- cians when interacting through music. required for it, will demonstrate that
cal expression. These findings suggest The results showed enhanced activation students can personalize their improvi-
that rPFC activity during improvisation of Broca’s area (inferior frontal gyrus; sations, making it a more rewarding
supports the production of personally IFG), Wernicke’s area (posterior supe- and valuable experience. Students aim-
motivated musical expressions that may rior temporal gyrus; pSTG), and their ing to express themselves and connect

30 Music Educators Journal March 2017


with peers through music will be able provided humans with an unprece- as a Teaching Strategy,” Music
to tap into higher-order brain regions dented ability to gain insight into the Educators Journal 92, no. 3 (2006):
during improvisation rather than limiting mechanisms of the creative mind, and 40–45; Lee Higgins and Roger Mantie,
“Improvisation as Ability, Culture, and
themselves to the areas seen for rudi- more work will only serve to expand
Experience,” Music Educators Journal
mentary pattern generation.69 Educators our understanding of why music is such 100, no. 2 (2013): 38–44.
may be able to unlock higher expressive an integral aspect of humanity.
9. Croom, “Music.”
capacities in their students by explicitly
10. Kratus, “Growing with Improvisation”;
designing situations allowing them to
perform improvisations without worry NOTES Riveire, “Using Improvisation”; Higgins
and Mantie, “Improvisation.”
about grades or critique. While demon- 1. Adam M. Croom, “Music, Neuroscience,
11. Elizabeth Menard, “Creative Thinking
strating musical competency and learn- and the Psychology of Well-Being:
in Music Developing a Model for
ing is a critical component of any music A Précis,” Frontiers in Psychology 2,
Meaningful Learning in Middle School
course, giving students an opportunity no. 393 (2012), accessed February
General Music,” Music Educators Journal
to directly reap enjoyment from musical 9, 2017, http://journal.frontiersin.org/
100, no. 2 (2013): 61–67.
article/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00393/full.
creativity in a fully open-ended envi- 12. Emma Hart and Zelda Di Blasi,
ronment will motivate them to become 2. Ian Cross, “Music and Cognitive
Evolution,” in Handbook of Evolutionary “Combined Flow in Musical Jam
the best improvisers they can be and Sessions: A Pilot Qualitative Study,”
Psychology, ed. Louise Barrett and Robin
encourage them to continue playing Dunbar (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Psychology of Music 43, no. 2 (2013):
long after they finish school. Press, 2007), 649–67. 275–91; Augusto Monk, “Symbolic
Interactionism in Music Education:
3. Charles Darwin, The Descent of Man,
Eight Strategies for Collaborative
Benefits of Improvisation and Selection in Relation to Sex
Improvisation,” Music Educators Journal
(London: J. Murray, 1888).
99, no. 3 (2013): 76–81.
Musical creativity is a fundamental and 4. David J. Heeger and David Ress, “What
13. Raymond MacDonald, Charles Byrne,
indispensable component of any educa- Does fMRI Tell Us about Neuronal
Activity?” Nature Reviews Neuroscience and Lana Carlton, “Creativity and Flow
tion in music. Although the neuroscien-
3, no. 2 (2002): 142–51. in Musical Composition: An Empirical
tific literature on musical improvisation Investigation,” Psychology of Music 34,
continues to evolve, early results corrob- 5. Maddalena Boccia, Laura Piccardi,
no. 3 (2006): 292–306; Higgins and
orate and complement the observations Liana Palermo, Raffaella Nori, and
Mantie, “Improvisation,” 38–44.
Massimiliano Palmiero, “Where Do
of music educators on the vast benefits of 14. Earl K. Miller and Jonathan D. Cohen,
Bright Ideas Occur in Our Brain? Meta-
improvisation. Neuroscientific research Analytic Evidence from Neuroimaging “An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal
such as the literature described in this Studies of Domain-Specific Creativity,” Cortex Function,” Annual Review
article can guide a music educator’s Frontiers in Psychology 6 (2015), of Neuroscience 24, no. 1 (2001):
use of improvisation in the classroom. accessed February 9, 2017, https:// 167–202; Jan Gläscher, Ralph Adolphs,
Knowledge of the neural underpinnings www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/ Hanna Damasio, Antoine Bechara,
PMC4531218/; Roger E. Beaty, Mathias David Rudrauf, Matthew Calamia, Lynn
of spontaneous musical creativity can
Benedek, Paul J. Silvia, and Daniel L. K. Paul, and Daniel Tranel, “Lesion
underscore the motivation for cultivating Mapping of Cognitive Control and Value-
Schacter, “Creative Cognition and Brain
an open, encouraging environment that Network Dynamics,” Trends in Cognitive Based Decision Making in the Prefrontal
facilitates optimal musical experiences Sciences 20, no. 2 (2016): 87–95. Cortex,” Proceedings of the National
through improvisation. Furthermore, a 6. Malinda McPherson and Charles J. Limb,
Academy of Sciences 109, no. 36
neurobiological basis of musical creativ- (2012): 14681–86.
“Difficulties in the Neuroscience of
ity can provide scientific grounding for Creativity: Jazz Improvisation and 15. Charles J. Limb and Allen R. Braun,
advocates of this important behavior. the Scientific Method,” Annals of the “Neural Substrates of Spontaneous
With educational funding experienc- New York Academy of Sciences 1303, Musical Performance: An fMRI Study
no. 1 (2013): 80–83; Arne Dietrich and of Jazz Improvisation,” PLoSOne 3, no.
ing heavy cuts that have a significant
Riam Kanso, “A Review of EEG, ERP, 2 (2008): e1679; Aaron L. Berkowitz
impact on non-STEM fields like music, and Daniel Ansari, “Generation of
and Neuroimaging Studies of Creativity
strong quantitative backing for the ben- Novel Motor Sequences: The Neural
and Insight,” Psychological Bulletin
efits of musical creativity is becoming 136, no. 5 (2010): 822–48. Correlates of Musical Improvisation,”
more and more important. While cur- Neuroimage 41, no. 2 (2008): 535–43;
7. Roger E. Beaty, “The Neuroscience of
rent neuroscientific research has already Örjan de Manzano and Fredrik Ullén,
Musical Improvisation,” Neuroscience
begun to justify intuitive notions about “Goal-Independent Mechanisms for
& Biobehavioral Reviews 51 (2015): Free Response Generation: Creative and
why music is so powerful, it is critical 108–17. Pseudo-Random Performance Share
that more work be done to understand 8. John Kratus, “Growing with Neural Substrates,” NeuroImage 59, no.
how the use of creativity in music edu- Improvisation.” Music Educators 1 (2012): 772–80; Ana Luísa Pinho,
cation programs can affect students. Journal 78, no. 4 (1991): 36–40; Örjan de Manzano, Peter Fransson,
Recent advances in neuroscience have Janine Riveire, “Using Improvisation Helene Eriksson, and Fredrik Ullén,

www.nafme.org 31
“Connecting to Create: Expertise in Generation of Musical Structures 39. Steven Brown, Michael J. Martinez,
Musical Improvisation Is Associated during Improvisation in Pianists,” and Lawrence M. Parsons, “Music and
with Increased Functional Connectivity Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Language Side by Side in the Brain: A
between Premotor and Prefrontal 19, no. 5 (2007): 830–42; Dietrich, PET Study of the Generation of Melodies
Areas,” The Journal of Neuroscience “Neurocognitive Mechanisms.” and Sentences,” European Journal of
34, no. 18 (2014): 6156–63; Malinda 27. Csíkszentmihályi, “Flow.” Neuroscience 23, no. 10 (2006):
J. McPherson, Frederick S. Barrett, 2791–803; Örjan de Manzano and Fredrik
28. Riveire, “Using Improvisation.”
Monica Lopez-Gonzalez, Patpong Ullén, “Activation and Connectivity
Jiradejvong, and Charles J. Limb, 29. Fausto Massimini and Massimo Carli, Patterns of the Presupplementary and
“Emotional Intent Modulates the Neural “The Systematic Assessment of Flow in Dorsal Premotor Areas during Free
Substrates of Creativity: An fMRI Study Daily Experience,” in Optimal Experience, Improvisation of Melodies and Rhythms,”
of Emotionally Targeted Improvisation ed. Mihály Csíkszentmihályi Isabella NeuroImage 63, no. 1 (2012): 272–80;
in Jazz Musicians,” Scientific Reports 6 Selaga Csíkszentmihályi (Cambridge, Gabriel F. Donnay, Summer K. Rankin,
(2016): 18460, accessed February 9, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1988): Monica Lopez-Gonzalez, Patpong
2017, http://www.nature.com/articles/ 266–87. Csíkszentmihályi, “Flow.” Jiradejvong, and Charles J. Limb,
srep18460. 30. Hart and Di Blasi, “Combined Flow.” “Neural Substrates of Interactive Musical
16. Berkowitz and Ansari, “Generation”; de Improvisation: An fMRI study of ‘trading
31. Pinho et al., “Connecting to Create.”
Manzano and Ullén, “Goal-independent fours’ in jazz.” PLoSOne 9, no. 2 (2014):
32. Kratus, “Growing with Improvisation”; e88665; Bengtsson, Csíkszentmihályi,
Mechanisms”; Pinho et al., “Connecting
Hart and Di Blasi, “Combined Flow.” and Ullén, “Cortical Regions”; Berkowitz
to Create.”
33. Csíkszentmihályi, “Flow”; MacDonald and Ansari, “Generation”; Limb and
17. Limb and Braun, “Neural Substrates”;
et al., “Creativity and Flow.” Braun, “Neural Substrates”; de Manzano
McPherson et al., “Emotional Intent.”
and Ullén, “Goal-Oriented Mechanisms”;
18. Mirta F. Villarreal, Daniel Cerquetti, 34. Riveire, “Using Improvisation”; Kratus,
Pinho et al., “Connecting to Create”;
Silvina Caruso, Violeta Schwarcz López “Growing with Improvisation.”
McPherson et al., “Emotional Intent.”
Aranguren, Eliana Roldán Gerschcovich, 35. Miller and Cohen, “Integrative Theory.”
Ana Lucía Frega, and Ramón C. 40. Kyoung-Min Lee, Kee-Hyun Chang, and
36. Reza Shadmehr and Henry H. Holcomb, Jae-Kyu Roh, “Subregions within the
Leiguarda, “Neural Correlates of Musical
“Neural Correlates of Motor Memory Supplementary Motor Area Activated
Creativity: Differences between High and
Consolidation,” Science 277, no. 5327 at Different Stages of Movement
Low Creative Subjects,” PloSOne 8, no.
(1997): 821–25; Jason M. Chein Preparation and Execution,” Neuroimage
9 (2013): e75427.
and Walter Schneider, “Neuroimaging 9, no. 1 (1999): 117–23.
19. Evangelia G. Chrysikou, Matthew J. Studies of Practice-Related Change:
Weber, and Sharon L. Thompson-Schill, 41. Hiroshi Shibasaki, Norihiro Sadato, Hugh
fMRI and Meta-Analytic Evidence of
“A Matched Filter Hypothesis for Lyshkow, Yoshiharu Yonekura, Manabu
a Domain-General Control Network for
Cognitive Control,” Neuropsychologia 62 Honda, Takashi Nagamine, Shugo
Learning,” Cognitive Brain Research 25,
(2014): 341–55. Suwazono, Yasuhiro Magata, Akio Ikeda,
no. 3 (2005): 607–23; Nicole M. Hill
20. Clayton E. Curtis and Mark D’Esposito, and Walter Schneider, “Brain Changes Masahito Miyazaki, Hidenao Fukuyama, Renin
“Persistent Activity in the Prefrontal in the Development of Expertise: Asato, and Junji Konishi, “Both Primary
Cortex during Working Memory,” Trends Neuroanatomical and Neurophysiological Motor Cortex and Supplementary Motor
in Cognitive Sciences 7, no. 9 (2003): Evidence about Skill-Based Adaptations,” Area Play an Important Role in Complex
415–23. in The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise Finger Movement,” Brain 116, no. 6
and Expert Performance, ed. K. Anders (1993): 1387–98; Maria Jose Catalan,
21. Pinho et al., “Connecting to Create.”
Ericssson, Neil Charness, Robert Manabu Honda, Robert A. Weeks,
22. Arne Dietrich, “Neurocognitive Leonardo G. Cohen, and Mark Hallett,
R. Hoffman, and Paul J. Feltovich
Mechanisms Underlying the Experience “The Functional Neuroanatomy of
(Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
of Flow,” Consciousness and Cognition Simple and Complex Sequential Finger
Press, 2006): 653–82.
13, no. 4 (2004): 746–61. Movements: A PET Study,” Brain 121,
37. Ricarda I. Schubotz and D. Yves von no. 2 (1998): 253–64; Timo Krings,
23. Mihály Csíkszentmihályi, “Happiness
Cramon, “Predicting Perceptual Events Rudolf Töpper, Henrik Foltys, Stephan
and Creativity,” The Futurist 31, no. 5
Activates Corresponding Motor Schemes Erberich, Roland Sparing, Klaus Willmes,
(1997): S8.
in Lateral Premotor Cortex: An fMRI and Armin Thron, “Cortical Activation
24. Mihály Csíkszentmihályi, Flow: The Study,” Neuroimage 15, no. 4 (2002): Patterns during Complex Motor Tasks in
Psychology of Optimal Performance (New 787–96; Richard E. Passingham, Sara Piano Players and Control Subjects. A
York: Cambridge University Press, 1990). L. Bengtsson, and Hakwan C. Lau, Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
25. Ilan I. Goldberg, Michal Harel, and “Medial Frontal Cortex: From Self- Study,” Neuroscience Letters 278, no. 3
Rafael Malach, “When the Brain Loses Generated Action to Reflection on One’s (2000): 189–93.
Its Self: Prefrontal Inactivation during Own Performance,” Trends in Cognitive
42. Petr Janata and Scott T. Grafton,
Sensorimotor Processing,” Neuron 50, Sciences 14, no. 1 (2010): 16–21.
“Swinging in the Brain: Shared Neural
no. 2 (2006): 329–39. 38. Wendy Hargreaves, “Generating Ideas in Substrates for Behaviors Related
26. Sara L. Bengtsson, Mihály Jazz Improvisation: Where Theory Meets to Sequencing and Music,” Nature
Csíkszentmihályi, and Fredrik Ullén, Practice,” International Journal of Music Neuroscience 6, no. 7 (2003):
“Cortical Regions Involved in the Education 30 no. 4 (2012): 354–67. 682–87.

32 Music Educators Journal March 2017


43. Martin Norgaard, “Descriptions of of Spontaneous Thought Processes,” 61. Morton Ann Gernsbacher and
Improvisational Thinking by Artist-Level Cortex 40, no. 4 (2004): 623–30; Dick Michael P. Kaschak, “Neuroimaging
Jazz Musicians,” Journal of Research Passingham and Sakai Katsuyuki, “The Studies of Language Production and
in Music Education 59, no. 2 (2011): Prefrontal Cortex and Working Memory: Comprehension,” Annual Review
109–27; Martin Norgaard, “How Jazz Physiology and Brain Imaging,” Current of Psychology 54 (2003): 91–114;
Musicians Improvise,” Music Perception: Opinion in Neurobiology 14, no. 2 Stefan Koelsch, “Neural Substrates of
An Interdisciplinary Journal 31, no. 3 (2004): 163–68. Processing Syntax and Semantics in
(2014): 271–87. Music,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology
51. Allen R. Braun, Andre Guillemin, Lara
44. Parashkev Nachev, Henrietta Wydell, 15, no. 2 (2005): 207–12; Luciano
Hosey, and Mary Varga, “The Neural
Kevin O’Neill, Masud Husain, and Fadiga, Laila Craighero, and Alessandro
Organization of Discourse,” Brain 124,
Christopher Kennard, “The Role of D’Ausilio, “Broca’s Area in Language,
no. 10 (2001): 2028–44.
the Pre-Supplementary Motor Area in Action, and Music,” Annals of the New
the Control of Action,” Neuroimag 36 52. Strand and Brenner, “Learning to be York Academy of Sciences 1169, no. 1
(2007): T155–63; Tomás Paus, “Primate Creatively Expressive Performers,” Music (2009): 448–58.
Anterior Cingulate Cortex: Where Motor Educators Journal 103, no. 3 (2017): 62. Aniruddh D. Patel, “Language,
Control, Drive and Cognition Interface,” 21–26; Gruenhagen, “Developing Music, Syntax and the Brain,” Nature
Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2, no. 6 Musical Creativity”; Paul W. Burgess, Neuroscience 6, no. 7 (2003):
(2001): 417–24. Iroise Dumontheil, and Sam J. Gilbert, 674–81.
45. Debra A. Gusnard, Erbil Akbudak, “The Gateway Hypothesis of Rostral
Gordon L. Shulman, and Marcus E. 63. Robert J. Zatorre and Severine Samson,
Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10) Function,”
Raichle, “Medial Prefrontal Cortex and “Role of the Right Temporal Neocortex
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11, no.
Self-Referential Mental Activity: Relation in Retention of Pitch in Auditory
7 (2007): 290–98; Menard, “Creative
to a Default Mode of Brain Function,” Short-Term Memory,” Brain 114, no. 6
Thinking.”
Proceedings of the National Academy of (1991): 2403–17.
Sciences 98, no. 7 (2001): 4259–64; 53. Gläscher et al., “Lesion Mapping.”
64. Adam Hampshire, Samuel R. Chamberlain,
Passingham et al., “Medial Frontal 54. Limb and Braun, “Neural Substrates.” Martin M. Monti, John Duncan, and
Cortex.” Adrian M. Owen, “The Role of the Right
55. Higgins and Mantie, “Improvisation.”
46. Helen Barbas, H. Ghashghaei, S. M. Inferior Frontal Gyrus: Inhibition and
Dombrowski, and N. L. Rempel-Clower, 56. MacDonald et al., “Creativity Attentional Control,” Neuroimage 50, no.
“Medial Prefrontal Cortices Are Unified and Flow”; Felicity A. Baker and 3 (2010): 1313–19; Rochelle Caplan and
By Common Connections with Superior Raymond A.R. MacDonald, “Flow, Mirella Dapretto, “Making Sense during
Temporal Cortices and Distinguished by Identity, Achievement, Satisfaction Conversation: An fMRI Study,” Neuroreport
and Ownership during Therapeutic 12, no. 16 (2001): 3625–32.
Input from Memory-Related Areas in the
Rhesus Monkey,” Journal of Comparative Songwriting Experiences with 65. Monk, “Symbolic Interactionism.”
Neurology 410, no. 3 (1999): 343–67; University Students and Retirees,”
66. Ibid.; Hart and Di Blasi, “Combined
Paus, “Primate Anterior Cingulate Musicae Scientiae 17, no. 2 (2013):
Flow.”
Cortex”; Amit Etkin, Tobias Egner, and 131–46.
67. Jeffrey R. Binder, Rutvik H. Desai,
Raffael Kalisch, “Emotional Processing 57. Lori A. Custodero, “Seeking Challenge,
William W. Graves, and Lisa L. Conant,
in Anterior Cingulate and Medial Finding Skill: Flow Experience and
“Where Is the Semantic System? A
Prefrontal Cortex,” Trends in Cognitive Music Education,” Arts Education
Critical Review and Meta-Analysis
Sciences 15, no. 2 (2011): 85–93. Policy Review 103, no. 3 (2002):
of 120 Functional Neuroimaging
47. Kratus, “Growing with Improvisation”; 3–9; Massimini and Carli, “Systematic
Studies,” Cerebral Cortex 19, no.
Hargreaves, “Generating Ideas.” Assessment.”
12 (2009): 2767–96; Mohamed L.
48. Lisa Gruenhagen, “Developing Musical 58. Beaty, “Neuroscience.” Seghier, “The Angular Gyrus Multiple
Creativity,” Music Educators Journal 59. However, one study (McPherson Functions and Multiple Subdivisions,”
103, no. 3 (2017): 40–45; Riveire, et al., “Emotional Intent”) that used a The Neuroscientist 19, no. 1 (2013):
“Using Improvisation”; Croom, “Music.” thirty-five–note chromatic keyboard and 43–61.
49. Limb and Braun, “Neural Substrates”; constrained musicians only by the “emo- 68. Ian Cross, “The Evolutionary Nature of
Donnay et al., “Neural Substrates.” tional target” did not show activation of Musical Meaning,” Musicae Scientiae
the rostral prefrontal cortex (rPFC). 13, no. 2 (2009): 179–200.
50. Kalina Christoff, Justin M. Ream, and
John D. E. Gabrieli, “Neural Basis 60. McPherson and Limb, “Difficulties.” 69. Berkowitz and Ansari, “Generation.”

www.nafme.org 33

You might also like