You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [New York University]

On: 19 May 2015, At: 04:22


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Angelaki: Journal of the Theoretical


Humanities
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cang20

WE ARE NOT SO STUPID ANIMALS


NEITHER
a
Vinciane Despret
a
Philosophie et Lettres, Universit de Lige, 4000 Lige, Belgium
E-mail:
Published online: 18 May 2015.

Click for updates

To cite this article: Vinciane Despret (2015) WE ARE NOT SO STUPID ANIMALS NEITHER, Angelaki:
Journal of the Theoretical Humanities, 20:2, 153-161, DOI: 10.1080/0969725X.2015.1039855

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0969725X.2015.1039855

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
Content) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015
ANGELAKI
journal of the theoretical humanities
volume 20 number 2 june 2015

translators foreword
Vinciane Despret is a psychologist and philoso-
pher at the University of Liege, but, as she
herself might insist, let us not go too fast
(The Body We Care For 128). Always cir-
cling back, swift to question and complicate,
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

Despret can be counted on to meticulously sift


through whatever is deemed true, neutral, or
objective, particularly in the eld of ethology.
Products of habit and power are not safe in
her hands. Her terrain is how we create, co-
create, and re-create our lives, identities, and vinciane despret
bodies, as seen through the wide viewnder of
our tales and texts about nature, culture,
animals, and science. In this, she seems to
translated by hollis taylor
don a third hat, that of a literary critic and
here she credits the profound inuence of nov- WE ARE NOT SO
elist Michel Tournier (on whom she wrote her
thesis).1 STUPID ANIMALS
Despret is fascinated by how we report on
relationships on the different narratives
NEITHER
that are told and might be told about the see-
mingly same event. This burden infuses all of and French intellectual thus extend well
her work: to judge among all the versions beyond her formal citation index. For instance,
not which is the most true (that is indetermin- Saturday and Sunday mornings, we can tune in
able) but, in line with James pragmatism, to her radio show on Bon week-end, which is
which is the most interesting, the most fecund aired on Radio Television Belge Francophone.4
the one that adds to the world, opens more Her ve-minute segment is entitled We are not
deliberations, and makes us think and so stupid Animals neither, which is
imagine.2 She often refuses to take sides, appended with: One nds plenty of evidence
claiming of her disparate sources: They need of it and sometimes even contradictory
one another.3 evidence.5
No respecter of disciplinary limits, species The Saturday programme nds Despret pre-
borders, nodes of prestige, impact factor, pre- senting half-page vignettes sent to her by lis-
existing truths and facts, or Western sciences teners true stories involving a pet or perhaps
exclusive authority, she instead seeks out unan- an animal someone randomly encountered.
ticipated communicatory modes and dialogic She reads these to us as she might a bedtime
mixes highlow, inout, natureculture. story, vocalizing animal sounds as required
Desprets reach and popularity as a Belgian and presenting them without comment. They

ISSN 0969-725X print/ISSN 1469-2899 online/15/020153-9 2015 Taylor & Francis


http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0969725X.2015.1039855

153
we are not so stupid

are memorable readings of memorable without the need to anaesthetize or restrain


accounts. them. Our goal has been to determine how
On Sundays, she shifts to a more scientic dogs brains work, and even more important,
perspective, although without leaving behind he writes, what they think of us humans.
her penchant for narrativity and wonder. Focus- Now, he concludes, after training and scanning
ing both on ethologists who study animal be- a dozen dogs, he has arrived at an inescapable
haviour in natural environments and conclusion: dogs are people. However, it is the
cognitive psychologists who question animals manner in which dogs have become people
in laboratories, Despret mulls over their discov- that interests me most. Youll see.
eries and probes their methodologies. Do Because dogs cant speak, says Gregory
animals have a face or better still, do Berns, scientists have relied on behavioural
animals give us a face? Could mice be having observations to infer what dogs are thinking.
fun during experiments? Who is the star of It is a tricky business. You cant ask a dog
Darwins selection theory? These and many why he does something. And you certainly
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

other questions intrigue her and, through her cant ask him how he feels. Besides, speaking
skill as a presenter, captivate us. She excels in of emotions in animals frightens many scien-
delivering quick, rapid summaries of compli- tists, and most have preferred to put this ques-
cated issues, which she describes as an exer- tion aside as scientically unanswerable.
cise in style that I like.6 Until now, says Gregory Berns.
The compilation below of recent Sunday pro- By looking directly at their brains and
grammes nds her sleuthing theory construc- bypassing the constraints of behaviourism,
tion, legal status, taxonomic classication, MRIs can tell us about dogs internal states.
research conditions, and capacities vis--vis However, magnetic resonance image-taking pro-
dogs, horses, humans, cassowary, dinosaurs, cedures are cumbersome: they make noise, its a
sheep, and tortoises. Does this culminate in bit frightening, and you must remain absolutely
radio listeners who are progressing in their still. Animals had to be anaesthetized so they
understanding of animals (including could get through it. But you cant study the
humans)? Maybe yes, a little, she responded, feelings of an anaesthetized animal. And so,
but its hard to say things are changing. We from the beginning, these scientists treated
cannot know whether we are with change or dogs as people.
in change All that can be said is that we First, they did as is done when people are
accompany transformations.7 asked to participate in an experiment: they are
asked to sign a consent form (sometimes called
an Informed Consent Form). They thus
chronicle of sunday, december 22, adopted a consent form typically used for chil-
dren, not because dogs are children but
2013 because, as with children, the form must be

L ast October, the Sunday Times headlined


an article: Dogs Are People, Too.8 By
the way, I thank my student at ULB, Thibault
signed by the person responsible in this
case, the dogs owner. The form stipulates that
participation is entirely voluntary and that a
De Meyer, who regularly sends me references dog has the right to leave the experiment when-
to fascinating articles, sometimes accompanied ever their owner wishes. Then, scientists used
with a comment that pinpoints exactly where only positive training methods. No sedation.
it is good to think. No restraints. If the dogs didnt want to be in
Lets go back to our dogs, who supposedly are the MRI scanner, they could leave. Same as
people like any other. The article was written by any human volunteer.
American neuroscientist Gregory Berns. He My dog Callie, says Gregory Berns again,
reports how for two years he and his colleagues was the rst. Rescued from a shelter, Callie
trained dogs to agree to go in an MRI scanner was a skinny black terrier mix, what is called a

154
despret

feist in the southern Appalachians, from where by adding to them those of our non-human
she came. I looked feist up in the dictionary; companions!
it has no translation, but feisty means spirited, We will return next week.
so I think we can work out the character of
this little dog. And in fact, true to her roots,
this little dog preferred hunting squirrels and chronicle of sunday, december 29,
rabbits in the backyard to curling up in my
lap. She had a natural inquisitiveness, which
2013
probably landed her in the shelter in the rst We saw last week that scientists have succeeded
place, but also made training a breeze. in teaching dogs to surrender voluntarily to an
With the help of a dog trainer, Mark Spivak, MRI scan so that the electrical activity of their
we started teaching Callie to go into an MRI brain may be measured, and these scientists
simulator that I built in my living room. She have begun to contemplate reading what they
learned to go in, place her head on a pillow, think with the assistance of magnetic reson-
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

and stay still for periods of up to thirty ance imaging.


seconds. She also learned to wear earmuffs to I highlighted an important nding: that of
protect her sensitive hearing from the frighten- the similar role and function of the central
ing noise the scanner makes. After months of caudate nucleus, which seems to play the same
training we were rewarded with the rst role in humans and dogs. With us, one sees
maps of brain activity: [her] brain response to this centre activated when we are confronted
two hand signals [as well as] which parts of with pleasurable situations: a piece of music
her brain distinguished the scents of familiar that we appreciate, a painting, food, and love.
and unfamiliar dogs and humans. In dogs, we noted that this centre is activated
Once they published their results, Berns when we make a hand signal to indicate food. It
explains, the local dog community learned of is also activated when the dog smells the scent of
our quest to determine what dogs are thinking. a familiar human. In the rst tests, we saw it
Within a year, we had assembled a team of a activated when their master, who was out of
dozen dogs who were all MRI-certied. the room, reappeared. Do these results prove
For now, Berns says, [a]lthough we are just that dogs love us? We cannot conrm that in
beginning to answer basic questions about the this way. However, we can nonetheless say
canine brain, we cannot ignore the striking simi- that many things that activate the caudate
larity between dogs and humans in both the nucleus in humans, which indicate positive
structure and function of a key brain region: emotions, also do so in dogs. This is what is
the caudate nucleus. In humans, the caudate, called in neuroscience a functional homology,
rich in dopamine, plays a key role in the antici- which says something about dogs emotions.
pation of things we enjoy, like food, love and The ability to experience positive emotions
money. Of course, in light of the incredible like love and attachment shows that dogs
complexity of relations among interconnected possess a level of sensitivity similar to that of
brain regions, cognitive or emotional function human children. And, says Gregory Berns, the
cannot be assigned to a single region. author of this research, this should lead us to
However, the caudate nucleus is quite consist- rethink the way we treat dogs. They have long
ent: one sees it noticeably activated each time been considered as property, as things. I will
we are involved in an activity that we enjoy, note in passing that some European states
indicating quite clearly our preferences for have changed their laws and now consider
food, music and even beauty. animals as non-things, which is already
When will we have dogs review our shows and progress.
exhibitions? And why not? I like the idea of But now, says Berns, by using the MRI to
stretching the limits of how we dene beauty push away the limitations of behaviourism, we
and broadening our wealth of diverse opinions can no longer hide from the evidence. Dogs,

155
we are not so stupid

and probably many other animals (especially what we have already discussed in a previous
our closest primate relatives), seem to have chronicle about bees, the fact that science is a
emotions just like us. And this means we must collaborative practice (in the case of bees,
reconsider their treatment as property. One there were children in a primary school who
alternative is a sort of limited personhood for had led an experiment to understand how bees
animals that show neurobiological evidence of locate owers from which to gather nectar)
positive emotions. Many rescue groups already and that this collaboration can extend far
use the label of guardian to describe human beyond the laboratory proper. The work I am
caregivers, binding the human to his ward going to talk about today is again brought to
with an implicit responsibility to care for her. my attention by him, but more importantly it
Failure to act as a good guardian runs the risk is accompanied by a reection that I will read
of having the dog placed elsewhere. at the end: his remarks are fascinating and
Given that the law provides no legal basis for expand our perspective.
this guardianship, it is difcult to intervene Today, we will talk about horses and riders,
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

based on this concept in order to protect an from a recent article by three researchers
animal. For Berns, we must go a step further three ethnographers Anita Maurstad, Dona
and give dogs the rights of personhood. But, Davis, and Sarah Cowles, who interviewed
he says, I suspect that society is many years sixty riders in Norway and the US Midwest. In
away from considering dogs as persons. Dogs the abstract preceding the article, the three
are not people. researchers write that the stories the riders
However, he suggests, even if they have told them described how the horses, like the
nothing to do with dogs, two recent rulings by riders, were changed in the course of their
the United States Supreme Court have relationship. To account for these changes, the
included neuroscientic ndings that open the authors employ the term proposed by American
door to such a possibility. In both cases, the physicist Karen Barad: that of intra-action
court ruled that juvenile offenders could not (323).
be sentenced to life imprisonment without the Intra-acting refers to the fact that in a
possibility of parole. As part of the rulings, relationship each partner is changed in the
the court cited brain-imaging evidence that the encounter, or even comes to exist in a different
human brain was not mature in adolescence. mode than before the meeting: in this perspec-
And so, says Berns, [a]lthough this case has tive, partners those that are and those that
nothing to do with dog sentience, the justices could be do not pre-date the relationship.
opened the door for neuroscience in the court- They become what they are in an encounter.
room. And we can therefore hope that one One becomes a rider in a tangible engagement
day someone will defend a dogs right based with a horse, in a tangible commitment to this
on its brain images! meeting, and one becomes a horse with a rider
during the same encounter. We sense this
from the rst lines of the article: not only do
chronicle of sunday, january 12, the riders talk about their horses as subjects
as beings with their own will and not as
2014 objects with which they would engage in activi-
Two weeks ago, therefore last year, I presented a ties, but, more importantly, they show that
chronicle about dogs and thanked my student at training together constitutes part of a practice
ULB, Thibault De Meyer, who regularly sends where beings become comprehensible to each
me references to fascinating articles, sometimes other (325).
accompanied with a comment that pinpoints Thus, horsehuman communication crosses
exactly where it is good to think. Not only the species divide (326). When a rider tells the
does this mention allow me to publicly express researchers that she must reassure the horse
my gratitude but it enables me to underline about the fact that birds and moose do not eat

156
despret

horses, she is in the process of perceiving the the construction of modern national languages
world as a horse perceives it, and things and that language became a prerequisite to
beings no longer have the same meaning. relationships. What if animals were simply
Similarly, many riders explain how it is essen- teaching us to resist nationalism and other
tial to modify their body movements because empires?
they know that certain movements are difcult
for a horse to interpret, and that they must con-
stantly pay attention not only to what each
gesture may mean to the horse but also to chronicle of sunday, january 19,
what a gesture may signify that could prompt
the horse to consider, hey, there, my rider is
2014
talking to me and telling me something: I would suggest that today we do a bit of anthro-
sitting straight, head resting on the body, pology and talk about an amazing animal, the
being balanced (326), ensuring that every cassowary. In fact, listeners who regularly
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

movement will be received by the horse, etc. follow this programme must be aware that I
Every muscle twitch of the rider will be like have become increasingly interested in non-
a loud symphony to the horse (326, citing scientic knowledge, or even the fact that non-
Hearne 108). Humans learn to play softer and scientic aspects are increasingly invoked in
softer symphonies as they progress in develop- science: primary school children publish their
ing their riding talent, learning to make sense scientic experiments; a researcher conducts
via their own body movements, and to attune fascinating research on his own dog; to know
to and understand the feel of a horses body about horses, we question their riders, etc.
(326). And the horse does the same, adjusting This means that the sacrosanct separation
and coordinating what it transmits and what it between scientic knowledge, academic knowl-
perceives. The horse speaks differently with edge, and that of amateur experts is less and
humans than it does with its conspecic compa- less clear-cut and the fact that we invite our lis-
nions. And the riders say their body acquires a teners to tell stories is part of this movement:
different language, which they say is evident what can we learn about animals from those
even in their relationship with other humans, who live with them?
with whom they become more attentive to However, this separation between scientic
body language on account of what they have knowledge and pre-scientic knowledge has
learned with their horse. In other words, these long (since the nineteenth century) been the
riders are, with their horse, constantly trying rule. This separation between objective knowl-
to develop a shared language, a bodily edge (conducted under very specic rules) and
language. They call this a third language, a more empirical knowledge (said to be stained
one that is co-created by horse and human by subjectivity) falls under the rubric of what
(326). sociologist Bruno Latour calls the Great
It is around this third language that Thibault Divide. Latour borrowed that term from
De Meyer made a most interesting remark to anthropologist Jack Goody, who was referring
me, which accompanied the article: to anthropologists persistent (if not congeni-
Implicitly, he writes, one can read here at tal) tendency to divide up human societies,
what point to anthropomorphize a horse is to types of knowledge, and, more importantly,
zoomorphize a rider (as in isopraxism). One systems of thought into two classes, each separ-
nds this feature, in particular, in the concept ate and distinct one from the other. He [Goody]
that the riders themselves mobilize: the third lists as an example some of the classications
language, the invention of a new language drawn since the nineteenth century: primitive/
which is neither the riders nor the horses, civilized, traditional/modern, pre-logical/
but an undened meeting point. The language logical, scientic/pre-scientic (Journet 40).
is the result of a relationship; it is only with This approach resulted in our considering

157
we are not so stupid

other cultures knowledge in an entirely differ- world run by scientic knowledge, on the
ent way from how we view our own. culture and selections of those who categorize.
Latour takes up the work of the anthropolo- Reality does not impose itself by waving a
gist Bulmer, who notes and wonders why for magic wand, but instead each of these classi-
the Karam the cassowary is not a bird. First of cations is the subject of discussions, debates,
all, this question is based on an unexamined hesitations, and stories. Adrian Desmond, in
conviction, since it supposes that to think that his book on warm-blooded dinosaurs, provides
the cassowary, a large feathered biped that exemplary material for understanding how
does not y, is not a bird would be a deviation classications can be constructed in such a way.
from the normal and natural trajectory that In one of the books chapters Desmond asks
would make the cassowary a bird. And one why, for some palaeontologists, Archaeopteryx
notes that this position is so unambiguous to is a bird and not a dinosaur. Obviously, for
Bulmer that he only inquires about the contin- Desmond Archaeopteryx is not a bird. In fact,
gency of Karam classications when they while Archaeopteryx indeed has feathers, it is
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

differ from ours: those that are the same as quite incapable of ying (and so the cassowary
ours require no explanation. would not have, after all, much reason to be a
Taxa are only subject to investigation when, bird either) all the more so since Archaeop-
and it is Bulmer who stresses this, objective teryx closely resembles a dinosaur and would
biological facts no longer dominate the scene probably be considered one beyond doubt if
(Why is the Cassowary Not a Bird? 6). Since we had not found fossil feathers. However, for
the reasons behind the confusion of the cassow- those palaeontologists who hold to the idea
ary must be unravelled, Bulmer analyses how that dinosaurs are cold-blooded, the presence
the Karam invoke a complicated story of of feathers excludes Archaeopteryx from the
cross-cousins that justies how the cassowary, class of dinosaurs. There are nonetheless propo-
a bird by nature, nds itself in the taxon of nents of the idea that there have been warm-
kobtiy, of which it is the sole representative blooded dinosaurs. And if that is the case,
(5). Happy Englishmen! exclaims Latour. Archaeopteryx can very well be one of those.
No one ever arrived on their shores to investi- It does not y; its feathers do not make it a
gate by what quirk they placed cassowaries in bird. But what purpose do they serve, then? In
the taxon of birds (Comment redistribuer fact, Archaeopteryx solved the problem of
211). warm-blooded dinosaurs, that is to say, the
No cultural explanation is necessary to problem of maintaining a constant body temp-
account for objective facts. But the cassowary erature of how to ensure thermoregulation.
is it a bird? Well, it all depends on how the Either one becomes very large, increasing in
classications are constructed. And if one volume and thus reducing heat loss, or natural
follows closely how such taxa are formulated, selection favours another means, allowing the
one comes to realize that even the category dinosaur to stay small and occupy a distinctive
birds could be problematic. I propose, given ecological niche: it lies in growing feathers on
the complexity of the problem, to take this up the forelimbs. With these feathered forelimbs,
again next week. Archaeopteryx could not only ensure its thermo-
regulation by having a protective layer but also
by waving its limbs; this enabled it to snare
chronicle of sunday, january 26, ying insects, its feathered arms serving as a
net. And in the long run, by dint of beating its
2014 wings, Archaeopteryx (therefore no longer
Last week, we saw on the question of animal Archaeopteryx) became a ying animal. The
classications in the example of the cassowary problem of pre-adaptation is solved: we won-
(a large feathered biped that does not y) that dered how birds got wings if they could not
these classications could depend, even in our help them y when they rst emerged; this

158
despret

issue is resolved: the wings are a response to the scientists were keen to see what was going on
problem of thermoregulation; wings only in their heads, and those who tried came back
became a ying device later. with results conrming that there was nothing
However, if Archaeopteryx is a warm-blooded to expect from them; they were relatively
dinosaur, then it means that the divisions no stupid, primitive, some kind of instinctive
longer hold between mammals and birds but machines without hearts, and, above all
between mammals and dinosaurs, of which more scientically without a hippocampus:
birds would be the living representatives. Isnt in short, cold-blooded animals are rough drafts
this in the end an interesting perspective? Not of what was to come after them in the triumphal
only did dinosaurs not go extinct (they merely march of evolution. We diverged from them 280
underwent a crisis at the end of the Cretaceous million years ago, with good reason. But in
65 million years ago), but, what is more, some recent years, other researchers have decided to
have become very good musicians. So, lets give reptiles a chance.
take good care of them, and if you would, put It was discovered that lizards are capable of
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

small balls of fat and seeds in your gardens so solving complex puzzles, that they adapt to
that dinosaurs remain a while longer among novel situations, and that they can teach us
us. But if you go to your seed merchant, and about the evolution of cognitive abilities, includ-
you order food or shelters and tell them that it ing the fact that some skills are much older than
is for the preservation of dinosaurs, do not be previously thought, or an alternative hypoth-
surprised if he looks at you strangely and asks esis that these emerged on several occasions
if you listen to a radio show called Bon week- during the course of evolution. Im going to
end on Radio Cro-Magnon. focus on the research of an English researcher
in cognitive psychology, Anna Wilkinson, and
her red-footed tortoise, named Moses (Mueller-
chronicle of sunday, february 2, Paul et al.). She asked her tortoise to navigate
in an eight-armed radial maze around a central
2014 area, which formed a kind of cartwheel. At the
We spoke last week about dinosaurs and the end of each arm was a strawberry. The tortoise
change of status that was imposed on certain only got one chance in each arm to retrieve the
animals. Today, we are again going to speak strawberry; the tortoise couldnt return to
about change of status and descendants of dino- where it had previously been. He had to memor-
saurs.9 So, lets talk about reptiles. For listeners ize each of the arms that he had already passed.
who have followed us since the beginning of this In the rst experiment, the walls of each arm
chronicle, one could say that reptiles are under- were lined with a curtain with different cut-out
going a revolution similar to that of sheep when shapes pinned to it.
primatologist Thelma Rowell observed them. One notes that the tortoise relied on these
Once we thought to ask questions that allowed cues (which he memorized) to remember
them to show of what they are capable, sheep where he had already passed. If the curtains
demonstrated much more complex and were removed, he would nd himself in dif-
intelligent behaviours than scientists had pre- culty. Indeed. Except that the tortoise came
viously believed. Note in passing that this revo- up with another strategy: in this case, he under-
lution continues with the work of Michel stood that he had to follow, in order, the arm
Meuret, who had the good idea to query shep- adjacent to the one he had just visited. And he
herds, who show us the extent to which sheep succeeded. The tortoise thus demonstrated
are inventive and socially sophisticated crea- what is called behavioural exibility, reecting
tures. But lets go back to our sheep of today, the fact that he was capable of adapting to new
reptiles. environments and inventing new strategies to
For a long time, reptiles have been, in com- deal with different events. Hence, reptiles are
parative intelligence studies, ranked last. Few capable of innovation.

159
we are not so stupid

Why had this not been seen before? For the 4 On nest pas si btes Les Animaux non plus
simple reason that researchers had hitherto is a feature La Premire on RTBF, the public broad-
used the equipment employed for mammals casting organization of French-speaking Belgium.
and birds, which is not at all suited to reptiles. See <http://www.rtbf.be/lapremiere/emissions_
And furthermore, to shorten the learning bon-week-end?programId=312> (accessed 5 Jan.
2015). This translation fails to take into account
curve, they had used a method also employed
the titles double entendre, since btes can refer
with rodents: bright lights or strident sounds not just to stupidity but also to beasts. Not as
associated with events to mark the memory of beastly as you may think and Far from stupid
experimental subjects. But with reptiles these are just two alternative translations of something
sounds and lights are totally counter-pro- best read in the original French, where we are
ductive; they paralyse them and put them in a left to confront the supposed synonyms animal
kind of cataleptic state. Moreover, they were and stupid.
asked to accomplish impossible tasks: reptiles 5 Ethology is a fabulous story, begins the pro-
never use their paws to manipulate objects, grammes Facebook page, acting as a sort of
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

unlike rodents and other mammals that can be mission statement (https://www.facebook.com/pa
asked to open boxes or unpack things. So, rep- ges/On-nest-pas-si-btes/858670957485128). The
tiles were constantly confronted with impossible most recent programmes are available for
tasks. And nally, being cold-blooded animals, podcast, and the website is replete with compelling
reptiles are very sensitive to temperature; they images of animals.
need warmth. If rats can feel comfortable with 6 E-mail to author, 9 Jan. 2015.
a laboratorys twenty degrees, reptiles need
higher temperatures; they learn considerably 7 Ibid.
less well in lower temperatures. Either research- 8 All quotes from Berns below are from this unpa-
ers imposed their norms on reptiles and ginated article.
obtained poor results, or they had to adapt to
9 And I wish to again thank the tireless and gener-
the norms of their subjects, which was very ous researcher Thibault De Meyer, who sent me
uncomfortable and discouraged many. the article Im going to talk about today and who
With these new studies, one can conclude fully understands its significance.
(along with one of the researchers) that intelli-
gence is probably much better distributed
than we imagined. By taking some of the tests
devised for animals known to bibliography
be intelligent, and adapting Berns, Gregory. Dogs Are People, Too. The
them to other species, we dis- New York Times 5 Oct. 2013. Web. 6 Jan. 2015.
cover that so-called intelligent <http://www.nytimes.com/2013/10/06/opinion/sun
animals are ultimately not that day/dogs-are-people-too.html>.
unique!
Bulmer, Ralph. Why is the Cassowary Not a Bird?
A Problem of Zoological Taxonomy among the
disclosure statement Karam of the New Guinea Highlands. Man 2.1
(1967): 525. Print.
No potential conict of interest was reported by
Desmond, Adrian J. The Hot-Blooded Dinosaurs: A
the author.
Revolution in Palaeontology. New York: Dial/James
Wade, 1976. Print.
notes Despret, Vinciane. The Body We Care For:
Figures of Anthropo-zoo-genesis. Body and
1 E-mail to author, 16 Jan. 2015.
Society 10.23 (2004): 11134. Print.
2 Ibid.
Hearne, Vicki. Adams Task: Calling Animals by
3 Ibid. Name. New York: Skyhorse, 1986. Print.

160
despret

Journet, Nicolas. De loral lcrit: Rencontre


avec Jack Goody. Sciences humaines 83 (1998):
3841. Print.
Latour, Bruno. Comment redistribuer le grand
partage? Revue de Synthse 3.110 (1983): 203
36. Print.
Maurstad, Anita, Dona Davis, and Sarah Cowles.
Co-being and Intra-Action in HorseHuman
Relationships: A Multi-Species Ethnography of
Be(com)ing Human and Be(com)ing Horse.
Social Anthropology 21.3 (2013): 32235. Print.
Mueller-Paul, Julia, Anna Wilkinson, Geoffrey Hall,
and Ludwig Huber. Radial-Arm-Maze Behavior of
the Red-Footed Tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria).
Downloaded by [New York University] at 04:22 19 May 2015

Journal of Comparative Psychology 126.3 (2012):


30517. Print.

Vinciane Despret
Philosophie et Lettres
Universite de Liege
4000 Liege
Belgium
E-mail: v.despret@ulg.ac.be

Hollis Taylor
University of Technology, Sydney
CB10.05.113
PO Box 123
Broadway, NSW 2007
Australia
E-mail: hollis.taylor@uts.edu.au

You might also like