You are on page 1of 10

Rehabilitation Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association

2011, Vol. 56, No. 3, 161170 0090-5550/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024509

Mechanisms of Health Behavior Change in Persons With Chronic Illness


or Disability: The Health Action Process Approach (HAPA)
Ralf Schwarzer Sonia Lippke
Freie Universitat Berlin Jacobs University Bremen

Aleksandra Luszczynska
University of Colorado and Warsaw School of Social Sciences & Humanities

Objective: The present article presents an overview of theoretical constructs and mechanisms of health
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior change that have been found useful in research on people with chronic illness and disability. A
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self-regulation framework (Health Action Process Approach) serves as a backdrop, making a distinction
between goal setting and goal pursuit. Risk perception, outcome expectancies, and task self-efficacy are seen
as predisposing factors in the goal-setting (motivational) phase, whereas planning, action control, and
maintenance/recovery self-efficacy are regarded as being influential in the subsequent goal-pursuit (volitional)
phase. The first phase leads to forming an intention, and the second to actual behavior change. Such a mediator
model serves to explain social cognitive processes in health behavior change. By adding a second layer, a
moderator model is provided in which three stages are distinguished to segment the audience for tailored
interventions. Identifying persons as preintenders, intenders, or actors offers an opportunity to match theory-
based treatments to specific target groups. Numerous research and assessment examples, especially within the
physical activity domain, serve to illustrate the application of the model to rehabilitation settings and health
promotion for people with chronic illness or disability. Conclusions/Implications: The theoretical develop-
ments and research evidence for the self-regulation framework explain the cognitive mechanisms of behavior
change and adherence to treatment in the rehabilitation setting.

Keywords: rehabilitation, motivation, volition, self-efficacy, intention, risk perception

This article describes a health promotion model for persons with Health Behavior Change in Rehabilitation
chronic illness or disability. The Health Action Process Approach
(HAPA) has been found useful to describe, explain, and predict For people with chronic illness or disability, behavior change
changes in health behaviors in a variety of settings, in particular in can be an effective strategy to prevent further morbidity and
rehabilitation settings. Interventions need to be theory-guided to mortality. When people are enrolled in medical rehabilitation,
allow for meaningful interpretations of empirical findings and to there is a good chance to help them learn how to improve their
draw valid conclusions. Recommendations for successful practice health behaviors and maintain them after discharge. For example,
need a theory base. It is therefore important for rehabilitation persons with Coronary Artery Disease (CAD) received the follow-
psychologists to understand different theories and judge their ing recommendations regarding their health behaviors to improve
potential for diverse research questions. In the following section, their prognosis: (1) No smoking; (2) moderate to intensive phys-
we will provide a brief introduction on lifestyle change in reha- ical activity, (3) moderate or no alcohol consumption; (4) healthy
bilitation. Then we will describe various theoretical advances in body weight control; (5) limited intake of saturated fats and trans
health behavior change. fatty acids; (6) regular fish consumption; (7) sufficient amounts of
fruit and vegetables; (8) Sufficient fiber intake, for example from
grains, legumes, or nuts; (9) reduced salt intake (Iestra et al.,
2005). In a systematic review, Iestra et al. (2005) found for
all-cause mortality, that positive changes in all behaviors de-
This article was published Online First July 18, 2011. creased the mortality risk. For example, physical activity in CAD
Ralf Schwarzer, Department of Psychology, Freie Universitat Berlin, patients reduced the mortality risk by 25%. The authors also
Berlin, Germany; Sonia Lippke, Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and compared their findings with the general population and found
Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Bremen, Germany; comparable effects for four health behaviors.
Aleksandra Luszczynska, Trauma, Health, & Hazards Center, University The main challenge is to increase the patients motivation to
of Colorado and Warsaw School of Social Sciences & Humanities, War-
adhere to health behavior changes. Whether they adhere to an
saw, Poland.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ralf
intervention program has a substantial effect on health outcomes.
Schwarzer, PhD, Department of Psychology, Freie Universitat Berlin, However, some programs fail to show evidence that they increase
Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: ralf.schwarzer@ the level of adherence (Cutrona et al., 2010; Dean, Walters, &
fu-berlin.de Hall, 2010; McLean, Burton, Bradley, & Littlewood, 2010). What

161
162 SCHWARZER, LIPPKE, AND LUSZCZYNSKA

makes it more difficult to improve adherence are comorbidities 2 before proceeding to Stage 3 and finally adopting the criterion
(e.g., depression, anxiety) and increased pain levels during exer- behavior. Finally, (3) individuals in the same stage are more
cise, as well as health care-related factors, such as characteristics similar than those in different stages; that is, they face the same
of the health care providers and organizations. Moreover, patients barriers, but these barriers are different from those in other stages.
often face lower preexisting levels of physical activity, helpless- The main purpose of applying stage models lies in the identifica-
ness, poor social support, and more perceived barriers to exercise tion of relatively homogeneous target groups for interventions and
than the general population (McLean et al., 2010). Consequently, the design of stage-matched treatments. The most popular stage
more effective interventions to increase adherence are needed. theory of health behavior change is the Transtheoretical Model
Theory- and evidence-based interventions that are tailored to psy- (TTM; J. O. Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983) that proposes five
chological constructs are more effective (Noar, Benac, & Harris, stages of change. It has also been applied to rehabilitation settings
2007). (e.g., Gorczynski, Faulkner, Greening, & Cohn, 2010).
Health self-regulation is mandatory, but it is very demanding for Both continuum models and stage models have their advantages
someone with chronic illness and disability. Research on health and disadvantages. Continuum models have been found useful for
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

self-regulation examines how change takes place, and why some explanation and prediction, whereas stage models are often pre-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

individuals change, whereas others do not. It identifies relevant ferred to guide interventions. For health promotion, the continuum
causal factors and their interplay in the change process. This leads models are often too general because all variables involved in such
to theories that may be generalized to several health behaviors and a model need to be addressed in interventions, without considering
to several types of illness. For example, a theory could present the special needs of particular subgroups of participants. However,
particular mechanisms that are valid for motivating orthopedic it is possible to integrate both approaches when researchers use a
patients to adopt and maintain physical exercise. continuum model as a theoretical template and, when it comes to
This article presents an overview of theoretical constructs and interventions, subdivide the audience into stage groups to allow for
frameworks that have been found useful in the context of behavior stage-matched treatments.
change, in particular for individuals with chronic illness and dis-
ability. We will focus on empirical evidence in the context of Mechanisms of Change: Mediation and Prediction
rehabilitation settings, confining the discussion to physical activity
as one of the main health behaviors, although the principles se- The HAPA (Schwarzer, 2008) has two layers: a continuum layer
lected also apply to other health behaviors. and a stage layer. We now describe this model in more detail. The
traditional continuum models have been criticized mainly because
Health Behavior Change: The Need for Theory of the so-called intention-behavior gap (referring to the frequent
failure of intention to predict behavior). A model that explicitly
To explain, predict, and effectively improve the self-regulation includes postintentional factors to overcome this gap is the HAPA.
of individuals and to optimize treatment, theories of health behav- The model suggests a distinction between (1) preintentional mo-
ior change are needed (Dunn & Elliot, 2008). In general, such tivation processes that lead to a behavioral intention, and (2)
theories are divided into continuum models and stage models. In postintentional volition processes that lead to the actual health
continuum models, people are positioned along a range that re- behavior. Within the two phases, different patterns of social
flects the likelihood of action. Influential predictor variables are cognitive predictors may emerge (see Figure 1). In the initial
identified and combined in one prediction equation. The goal of an motivation phase (1), a person develops an intention to act. In this
intervention is to move the person along this route toward action. phase, risk perception is seen as a distal antecedent (e.g., I am at
Health promotion, then, focuses on increasing all model-inherent risk for cardiovascular disease). Risk perception in itself is insuf-
variables in all persons, without matching treatments to particular ficient to enable a person to form an intention. Rather, it sets the
audiences. The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1991) is
such a continuum model that is applied to rehabilitation settings
(Dixon, Johnston, Rowley, & Pollard, 2008; Eng & Martin Ginis,
2007; Galea & Bray, 2006; Latimer, Martin Ginis, & Arbour,
2006; Yardley & Donovan-Hall, 2007). Other continuum models
are the Social-Cognitive Theory (SCT; Bandura, 1986, 2004) and
the Protection Motivation Theory (PMT; Rogers, 1975).
To overcome the practical limitations of continuum models, the
change process has been subdivided by a number of qualitative
stages. According to stage theories, health behavior change con-
sists of an ordered set of categories (or stages) into which people
can be classified. These categories reflect cognitive or behavioral
characteristics, such as the intention to perform a behavior. Fol-
lowing Weinstein, Rothman, and Sutton (1998), the common de-
fining properties of stage models are: (1) Individuals can be
classified into different stages by a valid assessment procedure; (2)
The stages are ordered, that is, Stage 3 is closer to the criterion
behavior than Stages 1 and 2, and Stage 1 is farthest from the
criterion behavior. A person in Stage 1 has to move first to Stage Figure 1. The HAPA (Schwarzer, 2008).
SPECIAL SECTION: HEALTH BEHAVIOR CHANGE 163

stage for a contemplation process and further elaboration of The term stage in this context was chosen to allude to the
thoughts about consequences and competencies. Similarly, posi- stage theories, but not in the strict definition that includes irrevers-
tive outcome expectancies (e.g., If I exercise five times per week, ibility and invariance. The terms phase or mindset may be
I will reduce my cardiovascular risk) are chiefly seen as being equally suitable for this distinction. The basic idea is that individ-
important in the motivation phase, when a person balances the pros uals pass through different mindsets on their way to behavior
and cons of certain behavioral outcomes. Further, one needs to change. Thus, interventions may be most efficient when tailored to
believe in ones capability to perform a desired action (perceived these particular mindsets. For example, preintenders are supposed
self-efficacy; e.g., I am capable of adhering to my exercise sched- to benefit from confrontation with outcome expectancies and some
ule in spite of the temptation to watch TV). Perceived self- level of risk communication. They need to learn that the new
efficacy operates in concert with positive outcome expectancies, behavior (e.g., becoming physically active) has positive outcomes
both of which contribute substantially to forming an intention. (e.g., well-being, weight loss, fun) as opposed to the negative
Both beliefs are needed for forming intentions to adopt difficult outcomes that accompany the current (sedentary) behavior (such
behaviors, such as regular physical exercise. After forming an as developing an illness or being unattractive).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

intention, the volitional phase (2) is entered. In contrast, intenders should not benefit much from health
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

When a person is inclined to adopt a particular health behavior, messages in the form of outcome expectancies because, after
the good intention has to be transformed into detailed instruc- setting a goal, they have already moved beyond this mindset.
tions on how to perform the desired action. Once an action has Rather, they should benefit from planning to translate their inten-
been initiated, it has to be maintained. This is not achieved through tions into action. Finally, actors should be prepared for particular
a single act of will, but involves self-regulatory skills and strate- high-risk situations in which lapses are imminent. Interventions
gies. Thus, the postintentional phase should be further broken should help them if they desire to change something in their
down into more proximal factors, such as planning, action control, routine (e.g., adopting or altering a behavior).
social support, and recovery self-efficacy. Most other social cog-
nition models do not explicitly address postintentional factors.
Social support is one factor reflecting the barriers and resources A Set of Principles
part of the HAPA model: Support represents a resource, and the
lack of it can be a barrier to adopt or maintain health behaviors. The HAPA is designed as an open architecture that is based on
Instrumental, emotional, and informational social support can en- principles rather than on specific testable assumptions. It was
able the adoption and continuation of behaviors. This was found in developed in 1988 (Schwarzer, 1992) as an attempt to integrate the
studies with chronically ill people, for instance diabetics (Plot- model of action phases (Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987) with
nikoff, Lippke, Courneya, Birkett, & Sigal, 2008). It has also been social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986). It has five major princi-
found in a sample of multimorbid older adults that exercising ples that make it distinct from other models and that help to apply
together with a partner has a beneficial effect on adherence (Gel- the HAPA to research and interventions.
lert, Ziegelmann, Warner, & Schwarzer, in press). Principle 1: Motivation and volition. The first principle
Including planning and self-efficacy as volitional mediators suggests that one should divide the health behavior change process
renders the HAPA into an implicit stage model because it implies into two phases. There is a switch of mindsets when people move
the existence of at least a motivational and a volitional phase. The from deliberation to action. First is the motivation phase in which
purpose of such a model is twofold: It allows a better prediction of people develop their intentions. Afterward, they enter the volition
behavior, and it reflects the assumed causal mechanism of behavior phase.
change. Research that is based on this continuum layer of the Principle 2: Two volitional phases. In the volition phase,
model, therefore, uses path-analytic methods (e.g., Lippke, Ziegel- there are two groups of people: those who have not yet translated
mann, & Schwarzer, 2005; Luszczynska & Schwarzer, 2003; their intentions into action, and those who have. There are inactive
Renner et al., 2008; Schwarzer et al., 2007; Ziegelmann, Luszc- as well as active persons in this phase. In other words, in the
zynska, Lippke, & Schwarzer, 2007). volitional phase one finds intenders as well as actors who are
characterized by different psychological states. Thus, in addition to
health behavior change as a continuous process, one can also
Designing Stage-Matched Interventions: create three categories of people with different mindsets, depend-
Preintenders, Intenders, Actors ing on their current location within the course of health behavior
change: preintenders, intenders, and actors.
When it comes to the design of interventions, one can consider Principle 3: Postintentional planning. Intenders who are in
turning the implicit stage model into an explicit one. This is done the volitional preactional stage are motivated to change, but do not
by identifying individuals who are located either in the motiva- act because they might lack the right skills to translate their
tional stage or in the volitional stage. Then, each group receives a intention into action. Planning is a key strategy at this point. It
specific treatment that is tailored to this group. Moreover, it is serves as an operative mediator between intentions and behavior.
useful and theoretically meaningful to subdivide the volitional Principle 4: Two kinds of mental simulation. Planning can
group further into those persons who perform and those who only be divided into action planning and coping planning. Action plan-
intend to perform. In the postintentional-preactional stage, indi- ning pertains to the when, where, and how of intended action.
viduals are labeled intenders, and in the actional stage actors. Coping planning includes the anticipation of barriers and the
Thus, a suitable subdivision within the health behavior change design of alternative actions that help to attain ones goal despite
process yields three groups: preintenders, intenders, and actors. the impediments.
164 SCHWARZER, LIPPKE, AND LUSZCZYNSKA

Principle 5: Phase-specific self-efficacy. Perceived self-ef- eter or accelerometer (tracking steps or movements), a pill-coun-
ficacy is required throughout the entire process. However, the ter, or direct observation of behaviors (attendance rates, observing
nature of self-efficacy differs from phase to phase. This is because which products are bought or used for cooking, etc.). Although
there are different challenges as people progress from one phase to objective measures are less likely to be biased, they are more
the next. Goal setting, planning, initiative, action, and maintenance demanding to gather. As all social cognitive variables are mea-
all pose challenges that are not of the same nature. Therefore, one sured subjectively, the scale correspondence is higher when be-
should distinguish between preactional self-efficacy, coping self- havior is also measured subjectively by self-report (Courneya,
efficacy, and recovery self-efficacy. Sometimes the terms task 1994). Most often, data are collected with questionnaires or in
self-efficacy instead of preaction self-efficacy, and maintenance personal interviews. Other subjective measures, such as diary logs,
self-efficacy instead of coping and recovery self-efficacy are pre- might be more valid and reliable in comparison to typical ques-
ferred. tionnaires or interviews. However, data are often missing because
In summary, the HAPA has two layers: a continuum layer and a of noncompliance. A questionnaire regarding physical activity that
phase (or stage) layer. Depending on the research question, one might has been validated with physiological and anthropometric mea-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

choose one or the other. HAPA is designed as a sequence of two sures (i.e., VO2 max and body fat) is the Godin Leisure-Time
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

continuous self-regulatory processes, a goal-setting phase (motiva- Exercise Questionnaire (GLTEQ; Godin & Shephard, 1985), or its
tion) and a goal-pursuit phase (volition). The second phase is subdi- modified and adapted versions (Lippke, Fleig, Pomp, & Schwar-
vided into a preaction phase and an action phase. One can superim- zer, 2010; Plotnikoff et al., 2007).
pose these three phases on the continuum model as a second layer and
regard phase as a moderator.
Intention
This two-layer architecture allows to switch between the contin-
uum model and the stage model, depending on the given research
Intention to perform a behavior should be assessed the same way as
question. The stage layer is useful for designing stage-matched inter-
assessing behavior itself. For physical activity, one could pose the
ventions. For preintenders, one needs risk and resource communica-
following three items: I intend to perform the following activities at
tion, for example by addressing the pros and cons of a critical
least three [2] days per week for 40 [20] min . . . (1) . . . strenuous
behavior. For intenders, planning treatments are helpful to support
(heartbeats rapidly, sweating) physical activities; (2) . . . moderate
those who lack the necessary skills to translate their intentions into
(not exhausting, light perspiration) physical activities; and (3) . . .
behavior. Furthermore, for actors, one needs to stabilize their newly
mild (minimal effort, no perspiration) physical activity. Answers can
adopted health behaviors by relapse prevention strategies.
be entered on a six-point scale ranging from not at all true (1) to
The HAPA allows both the researcher and the practitioner to make
absolutely true (6). Two different scales may be aggregated corre-
a number of choices. Although it was initially inspired by distinguish-
sponding to the activity measurement: (1) Intention to Perform Stren-
ing between a motivational and a volitional stage, and later expanded
uous and Moderate Activities (the correlation of the two items is
to the distinction between preintenders, intenders, and actors, one
typically rather low, as the two items have desirable discriminant
need not necessarily group individuals according to such stages. If the
validity), and (2) Intention to Engage in Strenuous, Moderate, and
purpose is to predict behavior change, one would specify a mediator
Mild Activities.
model that includes postintentional constructs (such as planning and
Alternatively, intention can also be assessed with an ordinal rating
volitional self-efficacy) as proximal predictors of performance
scale in the same way that four domains of behavior are measured: (1)
(Scholz, Nagy, Ghner, Luszczynska, & Kliegel, 2009).
physiotherapeutic exercises (e.g., back training), (2) fitness activities
For stage-tailored interventions, however, usually three stage
(e.g., using an exercise bike), (3) resistance training (training muscle
groups would be established. This does not exclude the possibility
strength, e.g., on machines), (4) physical activity while commuting
of generating more than three stages. For example, for some
(e.g., going by bicycle or walking for longer distances). Intentions
research questions, one might subdivide the preintenders into
should also refer to exhausting activity and physical activity outside of
precontemplators and contemplators, according to the TTM (J. O.
work. Study participants can indicate the intended frequency and
Prochaska & DiClemente, 1993). Or one might opt for a distinc-
duration by ticking one of the following options: not at all (1), less
tion between preintenders, who are either (1) unaware of an issue,
than once per week for 40 [20] min (2), at least once per week for 40
(2) aware but unengaged, or (3) still deciding (Weinstein, Lyon,
[20] min (3), at least three [1] times per week for 40 [20] min (4), and
Sandman, & Cuite, 1998). Thus, HAPA is not a puristic stage
five times per week for 40 [20] min or more (5). Answers can be
model, but a versatile theoretical framework that allows for a
categorized in such a way that a dichotomous variable results, namely
variety of approaches.
whether cardiac [orthopedic] patients perform at least the recom-
mended activity three [2] times per week for 40 [20] min (1) or not
Measurement of Theory-Implied Constructs
(0).
The following section provides general assessment rules, citing
examples from the physical activity domain that can easily be Planning
adapted to other behaviors.
Action Planning can be assessed with items addressing the when,
Behavior where, and how of the activity. The items in the rehabilitation study
by Lippke, Fleig et al. (2010) were worded: For the month after the
Behavior can be assessed either subjectively or objectively: rehabilitation, I have already planned . . . (1) . . . which physical
Objective measures could be based on a device, such as a pedom- activity I will perform (e.g., walking), (2) . . . where I will be
SPECIAL SECTION: HEALTH BEHAVIOR CHANGE 165

physically active (e.g., in the park), (3) . . . on which days of the Risk Perception
week I will be physically active, and (4) . . . for how long I will be
physically active. Coping planning, on the other hand, can be mea- Risk perception can be measured by items such as: How high
sured with the item stem I have made a detailed plan regarding . . . do you rate the likelihood that you will ever get one of the
and the items (1) . . . what to do if something interferes with my following diseases, or that you will relapse to them? (a) cardio-
plans, (2) . . . how to cope with possible setbacks, (3) . . . what to vascular disease (e.g., heart attack, stroke), (b) diseases of the
do in difficult situations in order to act according to my intentions, musculoskeletal system (e.g., osteoarthritis, herniated vertebral
(4) . . . which good opportunities for action to take, and (5) . . . disk)?Any other health risk can be added, especially if relevant to
when I have to pay extra attention to prevent lapses. In a study with the individual sample included in the study (Fleig, Lippke, Pomp,
cardiac patients, the items proved high measurement qualities & Schwarzer, 2011; Schwarzer et al., 2007).
(Sniehotta, Schwarzer, Scholz, & Schuz, 2005).
Outcome Expectancies
Action Control
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Positive outcome expectancies (pros) and negative outcome


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

expectancies (cons) can be assessed with the stem If I engage in


While planning is a prospective strategy, that is, behavioral physical activity at least three [two] times per week for 40 [20] min
plans are made before the situation is encountered, action control . . . plus the following items. Pros are measured with five items,
is a concurrent self-regulatory strategy, where the ongoing behav- for example, . . . then I feel better afterward, . . . then I meet
ior is continuously evaluated with regard to a behavioral standard. friendly people, or . . . then my elasticity would increase. Cons
Action control can be assessed with a 6-item scale comprising were assessed by five items such as . . . then every time would
three facets of the action control process (Sniehotta et al., 2005): cost me a lot of money, . . . then I would be financially burned,
self-monitoring (Items a and b), awareness of standards (Items c or . . . then I would have to invest a lot (e.g., into the organiza-
and d), and self-regulatory effort (Items e and f): (1) I consistently tion).
monitored myself whether I exercised frequently enough, (2) I
consistently monitored when, where, and how long I exercise, (3) Social Support
I have always been aware of my prescribed training program, (4)
I often had my exercise intention on my mind, (5) I really tried Received social support can be from different sources, such as
hard to exercise regularly, and (6) I took care to train as much as family and friends. In a rehabilitation sample, social support re-
I indented to. garding physical activity was assessed with 10 items: My family/
friends . . . (1) . . . have encouraged me to perform my planned
activities, (2) . . . have reminded me to engage in physical activity,
Self-Efficacy
(3) . . . have helped me to organize my physical activity, (4) . . .
have managed the household for me so that I could engage in
Perceived motivational and volitional self-efficacy can be physical activity, (5) . . . have engaged in physical activity with
composed of items such as the following. Motivational self- me (Jackson, Lippke, & Ziegelmann, 2010).
efficacy refers to the goal-setting phase and can be measured
with the stem I am certain . . . followed by the two items . . .
Stage Algorithms
that I can be physically active on a regular basis, even if I have
to mobilize myself, and . . . that I can be physically active on
The assessment of stages should be measured without using a
a regular basis, even if it is difficult (correlation of the two
specific time frame, in accordance with Sutton (2000). Study
items r .79; Lippke, Fleig et al., 2010). Volitional self-
participants are asked Please think about the month before the
efficacy refers to the goal-pursuit phase. It can be subdivided
rehabilitation started: Did you engage in physical activity at least
into maintenance self-efficacy and recovery self-efficacy. Main- 3 [2] days per week for 40 [20] min or more? Additionally, the
tenance self-efficacy has been measured with the stem I am study participants read the instruction If you have had an accident
capable of continuous physical exercise on a regular basis . . . or some health conditions restraining you from performing your
followed by the two items . . . even if it takes some time until typical activities, please think about the month before that inci-
it becomes routine, and . . . even if I need several tries until dent. Possible answers were yes or no. Another question was:
I am successful (correlation of the two items r .82; Lippke, For the month after the rehabilitation: Do you intend to perform
Fleig et al., 2010). Items on recovery self-efficacy can be physical activities 3 [2] times per week for 40 [20] min or more?
worded I am confident that I can resume a physically active with the answers yes or no. Again, for the stage items, the follow-
lifestyle, even if I have relapsed several times, I am confident ing information was explicitly provided: When thinking about
that I am able to resume my regular exercises after failures to physical activities, please consider: (1) You are performing the
pull myself together, or I am confident that I can resume my activities during your recreational time or while commuting. (2)
physical activity, even when feeling weak after an illness. In You are not achieving them during your working time. (3) You are
three rehabilitation studies, the scales consisting of three to six doing them because you intend to do physical activities. (4) The
items exhibited high reliability and validity in orthopedic and activities have to be at least somewhat exhausting. Participants
cardiac rehabilitation samples (Schwarzer, Luszczynska, are asked to answer with either yes or no. Those individuals who
Ziegelmann, Scholz, & Lippke, 2008). indicate that they were active before the rehabilitation are then
166 SCHWARZER, LIPPKE, AND LUSZCZYNSKA

categorized as Actors. Those who answer that they were not active, experimental causal-chain-studies (Reuter, Ziegelmann, Lippke, &
but intended to perform the recommended goal activity, are called Schwarzer, 2009) provide further evidence. In addition, meta-
Intenders. And those answering that they were not active and did analyses included aspects of the HAPA (e.g., planning: Gollwitzer
not intend to perform the recommended goal activities are assumed & Sheeran, 2006; self-efficacy, outcome expectancies and risk
to be Preintenders. An alternative stage algorithm can be similar to perception: Milne, Sheeran, & Orbell, 2000; behavior change
the one presented in Table 1, which was used in the study by techniques: Michie, Abraham, Whittington, McAteer, & Gupta,
Lippke et al. (in press). 2009; stages, self-efficacy, social support, intention, risk percep-
Persons indicating 1 and 2 on the list above would be catego- tion: Noar et al., 2007). In the following, findings are presented on
rized as Preintenders, those answering 3 as Intenders, and 4 and 5 the five HAPA principles that may have implications for further
as Actors. The algorithm was designed as a rating scale, with research and theory.
(precontemplating) preintenders on the very left and (maintaining) Principle 1: Motivation and volition. The two phases of
actors on the very right, for two reasons: (1) This format is more behavior change were found to be empirically different, for in-
similar to the assessment of other social cognitive variables, such stance in studies by Lippke et al. (2004, 2005) and Lippke,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

as intention and planning, and, by this, measurement correspon- Schwarzer et al. (2010). In an orthopedic sample, the three HAPA
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dence is achieved. (2) The format is less space consuming (Lippke, stages were assessed, and it was investigated whether qualitatively
Ziegelmann, Schwarzer, & Velicer, 2009). The algorithm has been different stages of health behavior change and stage-specific mind-
successfully adapted to other behaviors that are relevant for indi- sets would be evident. Significant differences between motiva-
viduals with chronic illness: In a study by Lippke et al. (in press), tional and volitional individuals were supported by paired com-
the adapted algorithm was successfully used to assess the stages parisons and nonlinear trends (Lippke et al., 2005). In particular,
regarding foot care, nutrition, smoking, alcohol, and healthy drink- preintenders reported significantly less self-efficacy, intention, and
ing in two samples of persons who had diabetes. The measurement planning than intenders. In addition, in longitudinal stage-specific
qualities of a stage algorithm cannot be evaluated in terms of prediction patterns, it was important to consider stages: Preintend-
Cronbachs alpha, but in regard to Sensitivity (agreement between ers with high risk perception were much more likely to develop
classification as being active by the stage algorithm, and perform- higher intentions over time than preintenders with low risk per-
ing the goal activity according to a different measure) and Speci- ception. However, this effect of risk perception was not found in
ficity (agreement between classification as being nonactive, and the intenders (Lippke et al., 2005).
nonperformance of the goal activity). The algorithm presented in In an experimental trial with orthopedic patients, a volitional
Table 1 (Lippke, Ziegelmann et al., 2009) had high sensitivity intervention proved to be matched only to volitional study partic-
(70%) and specificity (80%) in comparison to previous studies ipants. Accordingly, planning was not beneficial for persons in the
(Nigg, 2005; Plotnikoff et al., 2007). The algorithm described motivational stages (Lippke et al., 2004). This was replicated in an
above, specifically designed for rehabilitation patients, has dem- Internet study that also demonstrated the specific mechanisms: The
onstrated very high sensitivity, but comparably lower specificity. planning intervention facilitated behavior change by means of
So far, it remains open whether this depends on the stage algorithm intention and planning, but only in the case of intenders, not in
or the rehabilitation contexts. preintenders (Lippke, Schwarzer et al., 2010). Thus, preintenders
first need to develop an intention. Accomplishing this depends
Empirical Evidence mainly on a motivational intervention that targets goal setting.
Principle 2: Two volitional phases, three stages. The study
How Does the Model Work? Mediating Effects outlined above with orthopedic participants (Lippke et al., 2005)
investigated the third HAPA stage in addition. This action stage
Empirical evidence on HAPA assumptions range from single was found to be significantly different from the other ones. When
case studies in therapy and counseling (Fiore, 2007) to large-scale exploring the effects of a planning intervention on actors, they
investigations in rehabilitation settings (e.g., Schwarzer et al., seemed to benefit, but not as much as those volitional participants
2008). Experimental manipulation (Lippke, Ziegelmann, & who had not been active before (i.e., intenders). Thus, it is imper-
Schwarzer, 2004; Luszczynska, Gregajtys, & Abraham, 2007; ative to distinguish the intention stage from the action stage.
Sniehotta, Scholz, & Schwarzer, 2006; Scholz, Knoll, Sniehotta, & Intenders benefit mainly from planning when adopting a new
Schwarzer, 2006; Ziegelmann, Lippke, & Schwarzer, 2006) and behavior. On the contrary, actors rather need relapse prevention

Table 1
Stage Assessment
Please think about the month before the rehabilitation started. Did you engage in physical activity at least 3 days per week for 40 min or more?
Please choose the statement that describes you best.
No, but I am
No, and I do not considering No, but I seriously Yes, but only for a brief Yes, and for a long
intend to start it intend to start period of time period of time

Note. This stage assessment can be adapted to any other goal behavior.
SPECIAL SECTION: HEALTH BEHAVIOR CHANGE 167

that helps them to recover from setbacks, and, in case of a relapse, are likely to maintain the recommended rehabilitation exercise at
they need strategies for coping with them effectively. follow-up, whereas this type of self-efficacy does not help people
Principle 3: Postintentional processes. As described above, who have relapsed. Those who had a setback in adhering to
planning was mainly beneficial in the postintentional phase, that is, recommended exercise, but harbor strong recovery self-efficacy
for the volitional individuals (Lippke, Schwarzer et al., 2010). As beliefs, were more likely to regain control after a relapse (Luszc-
such, planning serves as an operative mediator between intentions zynska & Sutton, 2006).
and behavior. This was also shown in structural equation analyses
(e.g., Schwarzer et al., 2008, 2007). Empirical tests on whether For Whom Does the Model Work? Moderator Effects
planning mediates the relation between intention and behavior
have confirmed this assumption in the general population (Scholz, The previous discussion about mediation addressed the ques-
Schuz, Ziegelmann, Lippke, & Schwarzer, 2008) as well as in tion: How does it work? To further understand the mechanisms of
rehabilitation (Reuter et al., 2009). That planning mediates be- health behavior change, we also have to ask: For whom does it
tween intention and behavior was also demonstrated in orthopedic work? Thus, we need to identify mediator effects as well as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

outpatients (Lippke et al., 2005). moderator effects. The HAPA, for example, as a parsimonious
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Principle 4: Action planning and coping planning. Com- mediator model, includes stage as a moderator. That indicates that
plex planning interventions promote sustained behavior change a prediction model within one stage group operates in a different
after rehabilitation. Planning can be subdivided into action plan- way than a prediction model within an adjacent stage group. This
ning and coping planning, as has been evaluated in different is similar to the assumption that one set of social cognitive vari-
samples of persons with chronic illness and disability. Sniehotta, ables can move people from Stage a to b, whereas a different set
Schwarzer et al. (2005) examined the factorial structure of the two of variables can move people from Stage b to c.
planning aspects. When addressing action planning and coping Although action planning has been found to mediate the inten-
planning separately in interventions, different effects were found: tion-behavior relation (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006), some studies
Those persons in cardiac rehabilitation became more active when have failed to find such mediation effects (Norman & Conner,
both kinds of planning were addressed in the intervention, as 2005). This suggests that the relationships between intentions,
opposed to a mere action planning intervention. In another re- planning, and behavior might also depend on other factors. For
search sample, at 6 months after cardiac rehabilitation, participants example, the degree to which planning mediates between inten-
involved in a planning intervention consumed 13% less saturated tions and behavior has been found to be higher in older than in
fat than controls (Luszczynska, Scholz, & Sutton, 2007). In ortho- younger people (Renner, Spivak, Kwon, & Schwarzer, 2007;
pedic rehabilitation, a delayed effect of coping planning on behav- Scholz, Sniehotta, Burkert, & Schwarzer, 2007). This represents a
ior demonstrated that coping planning seems to be of higher case of moderated mediation. Perceived self-efficacy is one poten-
importance for long-term maintenance: Whereas behavior adop- tial moderator for the degree to which planning has an effect on
tion 2 weeks after rehabilitation was only correlated with action subsequent behaviors. It is expected to moderate the planning-
planning, coping planning came into play only 4 and 26 weeks behavior relation because people harboring self-doubts might fail
later (Ziegelmann et al., 2006). In another study, action planning to act upon their plans. For persons with a high level of self-
transpired to be the mediator between intention and coping plan- efficacy, planning might be more likely to facilitate goal achieve-
ning, and coping planning mediated between action planning and ment. Self-efficacious people feel more confident about translating
behavior. This was true for different age groups in rehabilitation their plans into actual behavior. In other words, whether planning
participants with orthopedic diagnoses (Ziegelmann & Lippke, interventions (independent variable) actually affect behavior (de-
2007). The discriminant validity between action and coping plan- pendent variable) might depend on the individuals level of self-
ning has been confirmed in an experiment by Wiedemann, Lippke, efficacy (moderator). In a study on physical activity, longitudinal
Reuter, and Schuz (2011). data from an online survey were used to examine similar interre-
Principle 5: Phase-specific self-efficacy. Various studies lationships (Lippke, Wiedemann, Ziegelmann, Reuter, & Schwar-
proved that self-efficacy is required throughout the entire process zer, 2009). Only those persons who had a sufficiently high level of
of behavior adoption (Luszczynska, Gregajtys, & Abraham, 2007) exercise self-efficacy acted upon their plans. Conversely, partici-
and maintenance (e.g., Lippke et al., 2005). There is also evidence pants who were harboring self-doubts failed to act upon their plans
from rehabilitation samples on the changing nature of self-efficacy (see also Gutierrez-Dona, Lippke, Renner, Kwon, & Schwarzer,
as people pass through different stages of change (Schwarzer et al., 2009; Richert et al., 2010).
2008; Ziegelmann & Lippke, 2007). Mediator models work well in some groups, but not in others.
In cardiac rehabilitation, three phase-specific kinds of self- By comparing men and women, younger and older people, and
efficacy were distinguished. They demonstrated discriminant va- those from different cultures, we identify relevant moderators
lidity and were analyzed regarding their predictions of intentions (Renner et al., 2007; Ziegelmann et al., 2006). When a mediator
and behavior. Those individuals in the maintenance phase (actors model has strong interrelations within one category of participants,
who remained actors) were more likely to perform physical activ- but weak associations within a different category, then this is a
ities if they reported more maintenance self-efficacy than all oth- case of moderated mediation. The amount to which the mediator
ers. Study participants resuming their physical exercise after a translates the effect of the independent variable on the dependent
health-related break were more successful if they had higher variable depends on the levels of a moderator variable.
recovery self-efficacy in comparison to those who were active Such moderators can be age, chronic illness, disability, and so
without a break (Scholz, Sniehotta, & Schwarzer, 2005). Individ- forth, but also psychological variables that are closely related to
uals in cardiac rehabilitation who have strong coping self-efficacy the constructs used in health behavior models. Temporal stability
168 SCHWARZER, LIPPKE, AND LUSZCZYNSKA

of intention, for example, may be a moderator (Conner, 2008). Table 2


Older age and stronger baseline self-efficacy levels may moderate Intervention Matrix for HAPA-Based Stage-Specific Treatments
the effects of self-efficacy interventions among individuals with Stage group
degenerative spine diseases enrolled in a rehabilitation program
Preintender Intender Actor
(Luszczynska, Gregajtys, & Abraham, 2007). Moderated media-
tion is also possible, with psychosocial variables, such as intention Motivational constructs
or planning. For example, the intention behavior link is mediated Self-efficacy (motivational) x
by planning, and this mediator effect can be moderated by level of Risk perception x
Outcome expectancies x
intention (Wiedemann, Schuz, Sniehotta, Scholz, & Schwarzer, Goal setting x
2009). Here we have a special case in which the independent Volitional constructs
variable (intention) of a mediator model serves the function of a Action planning x x
moderator in addition. In other words, only in highly motivated Coping planning x x
persons does the intention operate via planning on the improve- Social support x x
Self-efficacy (maintenance) x
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ment of adherence, whereas in poorly motivated persons no such Self-efficacy (recovery) x


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mediator effect is visible. Action control x


The best way to demonstrate the mechanisms of health behavior
change is the experimental manipulation of those variables that are
supposed to produce behaviors or to move people from one stage
ventions specifically targeting intenders or maintainers are prom-
to another (Michie, Rothman, & Sheeran, 2007; Reuter, Ziegel-
ising (Reuter et al., 2008). Such programs need to be further
mann, Wiedemann, & Lippke, 2008). Various experimental stud-
developed and tested in people with different chronic illnesses and
ies have shown that self-efficacy interventions do make a differ-
disabilities.
ence, which attests to the fact that self-efficacy is indeed an
The key characteristic of theory-based interventions such as one
operative construct that facilitates volitional processes, such as
based on HAPA is to first assess the needs of the recipients, for
effort and persistence (Luszczynska, Tryburcy, & Schwarzer,
example, their stage of change. Then, the stage-specific variables
2007). Self-efficacy beliefs, in turn, are mobilized by family sup-
are to be addressed. In an intervention with cardiac and orthopedic
port during rehabilitation (Luszczynska & Cieslak, 2009).
participants, the following contents were included (see Table 2).
The stage-specific evaluation of effects is imperative: If differ-
Outlook: Better Tailoring of Interventions for Persons ent stage-specific interventions are applied to different stages, it is
With Chronic Illness or Disability essential not only to search for changes in behavior or progression
from inactive to active stages, but also to look for stage transitions
between all stages. Alternatively, increases in the stage-specific
Based on their systematic review, McLean et al. (2010) con-
variables (such as goal setting or motivational self-efficacy in
clude that interventions may be most effective when they (1) have
preintenders) are relevant outcomes as well.
motivational-cognitive behavioral approaches (as theorized by the
People with chronic illness and disability have a higher likeli-
HAPA, improving motivational factors), (2) help patients to man-
hood of exhibiting multiple behavior risk factors, and they are at
age barriers (as theorized by the HAPA, improving volitional
risk for premature death compared to those with only one behav-
factors as coping planning and maintenance self-efficacy), and (3)
ioral risk factor or none. Furthermore, they also account for a
tackle issues related to health care providers and organizations.
disproportionate percentage of health care costs (J. J. Prochaska,
So far, the principles of the HAPA have proven to be convincing
Spring, & Nigg, 2008). Thus, it is important to understand not only
for the general population as well as for persons participating in
single behaviors (such as physical activity or smoking), but also
orthopedic or cardiac rehabilitation. However, we know much less
multiple health behavior mechanisms. Only if more is known
about the main differences between persons with various chronic
about multiple behavior mechanisms can we help people to adopt
illnesses and disabilities or their special needs compared to the
and maintain as many recommended health behaviors as possible
regular population. In studies investigating theories other than the
and, eventually, to stay healthy (Flay & Petraitis, 1994; Morabia &
SCT, TPB, PMT, and TTM, only few differences between various
Costanza, 2010). Furthermore, we need a more elaborated model-
chronic conditions and between individuals with and without di-
ing of multiple health behaviors to design effective treatments for
abetes were found (e.g., Plotnikoff et al., 2008, 2007). However, is
people with chronic illness or disability.
this also true for the HAPA? This warrants further study, as only
more empirical findings on such similarities and differences can
help to tailor interventions effectively. References
Evidence from previous intervention studies shows results for
people with various chronic illnesses and disabilities. Especially Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior
planning interventions and action control programs proved to be and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179 211.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood
successful (Lippke et al., 2004; Luszczynska, 2006; Sniehotta et
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
al., 2006; Sniehotta, Schwarzer et al., 2005; Ziegelmann et al., Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health
2006). These interventions are mainly matched to intenders and Education & Behavior, 31, 143164.
actors adopting a new behavior or a behavior change. However, Conner, M. (2008). Initiation and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied
also preintenders and maintainers need effective interventions. Psychology, 57, 4250.
From noninstitutionalized samples, there is evidence that inter- Courneya, K. S. (1994). Predicting repeated behavior from intention: The
SPECIAL SECTION: HEALTH BEHAVIOR CHANGE 169

issue of scale correspondence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, and cardiac rehabilitation clinics. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55, 398
580 594. 408. doi:10.1037/a0021563
Cutrona, S. L., Choudhry, N. K., Stedman, M., Servi, A., Liberman, J. N., Lippke, S., Nigg, C. R., & Maddock, J. E. (in press). Multiple behavior
Brennan, T., . . . Shrank, W. H. (2010). Physician effectiveness in inter- change clusters into health-promoting behaviors and health-risk behav-
ventions to improve cardiovascular medication adherence: A systematic iors: Theory-driven analyses in three international samples. Interna-
review. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 25, 1090 1096. doi: tional Journal of Behavioral Medicine.
10.1007/s11606-010-1387-9 Lippke, S., Schwarzer, R., Ziegelmann, J. P., Scholz, U., & Schuz, B.
Dean, A. J., Walters, J., & Hall, A. (2010). A systematic review of (2010). Testing stage-specific effects of a stage-matched intervention: A
interventions to enhance medication adherence in children and adoles- randomized controlled trial targeting physical exercise and its predictors.
cents with chronic illness. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 95, 717 Health Education & Behavior, 37, 533546. doi:10.1177/
723. doi:10.1136/adc.2009.175125 1090198109359386
Dixon, D., Johnston, M., Rowley, D., & Pollard, B. (2008). Using the ICF Lippke, S., Wiedemann, A. U., Ziegelmann, J. P., Reuter, T., & Schwarzer,
and psychological models of behavior to predict mobility limitations. R. (2009). Self-efficacy moderates the mediation of intentions into
Rehabilitation Psychology, 53, 191200. behavior via plans. American Journal of Health Behavior, 33, 521529.
Dunn, D. S., & Elliott, T. R. (2008). The place and promise of theory in Lippke, S., Ziegelmann, J. P., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Initiation and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

rehabilitation psychology research. Rehabilitation Psychology, 53, 254 maintenance of physical exercise: Stage-specific effects of a planning
267. intervention. Research in Sports Medicine, 12, 221240.
Eng, J. J., & Martin Ginis, K. A. (2007). Using the theory of planned Lippke, S., Ziegelmann, J. P., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Stage-specific
behavior to predict leisure time physical activity among people with adoption and maintenance of physical activity: Testing a three-stage
chronic kidney disease. Rehabilitation Psychology, 52, 435 442. model. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 6, 585 603.
Fiore, N. A. (2007). Awaken your strongest self: Break free of stress, inner Lippke, S., Ziegelmann, J. P., Schwarzer, R., & Velicer, W. F. (2009).
conflict, and self-sabotage. New York: McGraw-Hill. Validity of stage assessment in the adoption and maintenance of physical
Flay, B. R., & Petraitis, J. (1994). The Theory of Triadic Influence. activity and fruit and vegetable consumption. Health Psychology, 28,
Advances in Medical Sociology, 4, 19 44. 183193.
Fleig, L., Lippke, S., Pomp, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2011). Exercise maintenance Luszczynska, A. (2006). An implementation intentions intervention, the
after rehabilitation: How experience can make a difference. Psychology of use of planning strategy, and physical activity after myocardial infarc-
Sport & Exercise, 12, 293299. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.01.003 tion. Social Science and Medicine, 62, 900 908.
Galea, M. N., & Bray, S. R. (2006). Predicting walking intentions and Luszczynska, A., & Cieslak, R. (2009). Mediated effects of social support
exercise in individuals with intermittent claudication: An application of for healthy nutrition: Fruit and vegetable intake across 8 months after
the theory of planned behavior. Rehabilitation Psychology, 51, 299 305. myocardial infarction. Behavioral Medicine, 35, 30 38.
Gellert, P., Ziegelmann, J. P., Warner, L., & Schwarzer, R. (in press). Luszczynska, A., Gregajtys, A., & Abraham, C. (2007). Effects of a
Physical activity intervention in older adults: Does a participating part- self-efficacy intervention on initiation of recommended exercises in
ner make a difference? European Journal of Ageing. individuals with spondylosis. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 15,
Godin, G., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise 26 40.
behavior in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sci- Luszczynska, A., Scholz, U., & Sutton, S. (2007). Planning to change diet:
ences, 10, 141146. A controlled trial of an implementation intentions training intervention
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and to reduce saturated fat intake among individuals after myocardial infarc-
goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in tion. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 491 497.
Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69 119. Luszczynska, A., & Schwarzer, R. (2003). Planning and self-efficacy in the
Gorczynski, P., Faulkner, G., Greening, S., & Cohn, T. (2010). Exploring adoption and maintenance of breast self-examination: A longitudinal
the construct validity of the Transtheoretical Model to structure physical study on self-regulatory cognitions. Psychology & Health, 18, 93108.
activity interventions for individuals with serious mental illness. Psy- Luszczynska, A., & Sutton, S. (2006). Physical activity after cardiac
chiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 34, 61 64. rehabilitation: Evidence that different types of self-efficacy are impor-
Gutierrez-Dona, B., Lippke, S., Renner, B., Kwon, S., & Schwarzer, R. tant in maintainers and relapsers. Rehabilitation Psychology, 51, 314
(2009). How self-efficacy and planning predict dietary behaviors in 321.
Costa Rican and South Korean women: A moderated mediation analysis. Luszczynska, A., Tryburcy, M., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Improving fruit
Applied Psychology: Health & Well-Being, 1, 91104. and vegetable consumption: A self-efficacy intervention compared to a
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cog- combined self-efficacy and planning intervention. Health Education
nitive functioning in motivational vs. volitional states of mind. Motiva- Research, 22, 630 638.
tion and Emotion, 11, 101120. McLean, S. M., Burton, M., Bradley, L., & Littlewood, C. (2010). Inter-
Iestra, J. A., Kromhout, D., van der Schouw, Y. T., Grobbee, D. E., ventions for enhancing adherence with physiotherapy: A systematic
Boshuizen, H. C., & van Staveren, W. A. (2005). Effect size estimates review. Manual Therapy, 15, 514 521.
of lifestyle and dietary changes on all- cause mortality in coronary artery Michie, S., Abraham, C., Whittington, C., McAteer, J., & Gupta, S. (2009).
disease patients: A systematic review. Circulation, 112, 924 934. Identifying effective techniques in interventions: A meta-analysis and
Jackson, J., Lippke, S., & Ziegelmann, J. (2010). Who is motivated how? meta-regression. Health Psychology, 28, 690 701.
Stage-specific prediction of physical activity in orthopaedic individuals Michie, S., Rothman, A. J., & Sheeran, P. (2007). Advancing the science
after orthopaedic rehabilitation treatment. Manuscript submitted for of behaviour change. Psychology and Health, 22, 249 253.
publication. Milne, S., Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Prediction and intervention in
Latimer, A. E., Martin Ginis, K. A., & Arbour, K. P. (2006). The efficacy health-related behavior: A meta-analytic review of protection motivation
of an implementation intention intervention for promoting physical theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 106 143.
activity among individuals with spinal cord injury: A randomized con- Morabia, A., & Costanza, M. C. (2010). Multiple health behavior change
trolled trial. Rehabilitation Psychology, 51, 273280. interventions: Tell us what you see. Preventive Medicine, 50, 12.
Lippke, S., Fleig, L., Pomp, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2010). Validity of a stage Nigg, C. R. (2005). There is more to stages of exercise than just exercise.
algorithm for physical activity in participants recruited from orthopedic American College of Sports Medicine, 33, 3235.
170 SCHWARZER, LIPPKE, AND LUSZCZYNSKA

Noar, S. M., Benac, C., & Harris, M. (2007). Does tailoring matter? Scholz, U., Sniehotta, F. F., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Predicting physical
Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interven- exercise in cardiac rehabilitation: The role of phase-specific self-efficacy
tions. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 673 693. beliefs. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 27, 135151.
Norman, P., & Conner, M. (2005). The Theory of Planned Behavior and Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy in the adoption and maintenance of
exercise: Evidence for the mediating and moderating roles of planning health behaviors: Theoretical approaches and a new model. In R.
on intention-behavior relationships. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psy- Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. 217243).
chology, 27, 488 504. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Plotnikoff, R. C., Lippke, S., Courneya, K. S., Birkett, N., & Sigal, R. J. Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: How to predict
(2008). Physical activity and Social-Cognitive Theory: A test in a and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied
population sample of adults with type 1 or type 2 diabetes. Applied Psychology, 57, 129.
Psychology: International Review, 57, 628 643. doi:10.1111/j.1464- Schwarzer, R., Luszczynska, A., Ziegelmann, J. P., Scholz, U., & Lippke, S.
0597.2008.00344.x (2008). Social-cognitive predictors of physical exercise adherence: Three
Plotnikoff, R. C., Lippke, S., Reinbold-Matthews, M., Courneya, K. S., longitudinal studies in rehabilitation. Health Psychology, 27( Suppl.), S54
Karunamuni, N., Sigal, R. J., & Birkett, N. (2007). Assessing the validity S63.
of a stage measure on physical activity in a population-based sample of Schwarzer, R., Schuz, B., Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., Luszczynska, A.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

individuals with type 1 or type 2 diabetes. Measurement in Physical & Scholz, U. (2007). Adoption and maintenance of four health behav-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Education and Exercise Science, 11, 7391. iors: Theory-guided longitudinal studies on dental flossing, seat belt use,
Prochaska, J. J., Spring, B., & Nigg, C. R. (2008). Multiple health behav- dietary behavior, and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,
iour change research: An introduction and overview. Preventive Medi- 33, 156 166. doi:10.1007/BF02879897
cine, 46, 181188. Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Action plans and
Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1983). Stages and processes of coping plans for physical exercise: A longitudinal intervention study in
self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal cardiac rehabilitation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 2337.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 390 395. Sniehotta, F. F., Schwarzer, R., Scholz, U., & Schuz, B. (2005). Action
Renner, B., Kwon, S., Yang, B-H., Paik, K-C., Kim, S. H., Roh, S., Song, planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: Theory and
J., & Schwarzer, R. (2008). Social-cognitive predictors of dietary be- assessment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 565576.
haviors in South Korean men and women. International Journal of Sutton, S. (2000). Interpreting cross-sectional data on stages of change.
Behavioral Medicine, 15, 4 13. Psychology & Health, 15, 163171.
Renner, B., Spivak, Y., Kwon, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Does age make Weinstein, N. D., Lyon, J. E., Sandman, P. M., & Cuite, C. L. (1998).
a difference? Predicting physical activity of South Koreans. Psychology Experimental evidence for stages of health behavior change: The pre-
and Aging, 22, 482 493. caution adoption process model applied to home radon testing. Health
Reuter, T., Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2009). Long- Psychology, 17, 445 453.
term relations between intentions, planning and exercise: A 3-year Weinstein, N. D., Rothman, A. J., & Sutton, S. R. (1998). Stage theories of
longitudinal study after orthopedic rehabilitation. Rehabilitation Psy- health behavior: Conceptual and methodological issues. Health Psychol-
chology, 54, 363371. doi:10.1037/a0017830 ogy, 17, 290 299.
Reuter, T., Ziegelmann, J. P., Wiedemann, A. U., & Lippke, S. (2008). Wiedemann, A. U., Lippke, S., Reuter, T., & Schuz, B. (2011). The more
Dietary planning as a mediator of the intention-behavior relation: An the better? The number of plans predicts health behaviour. Applied
experimental-causal-chain design. Applied Psychology: An International Psychology: Health & Well-Being, 3, 87106. doi:10.1111/j.1758-
Review Special Issue: Health and Well-Being, 57, 194 297. 0854.2010.01042.x
Richert, J., Reuter, T., Wiedemann, A. U., Lippke, S., Ziegelmann, J., & Wiedemann, A. U., Schuz, B., Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer,
Schwarzer, R. (2010). Differential effects of planning and self-efficacy R. (2009). Disentangling the relation between intentions, planning, and
on fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite, 54, 611 614. doi:10.1016/ behaviour: A moderated mediation analysis. Psychology & Health, 24,
j.appet.2010.03.006 6779.
Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and Yardley, L., & Donovan-Hall, M. (2007). Predicting adherence to exercise-
attitude change. Journal of Psychology, 91, 93114. based therapy in rehabilitation. Rehabilitation Psychology, 52, 56 64.
Scholz, U., Knoll, N., Sniehotta, F. F., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Physical Ziegelmann, J. P., & Lippke, S. (2007). Planning and strategy use in health
activity and depressive symptoms in cardiac rehabilitation: Long-term behavior change: A life span view. International Journal of Behavioral
effects of a self-management intervention. Social Science & Medicine, Medicine, 14, 30 39.
62, 3109 3120. Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Adoption and
Scholz, U., Nagy, G., Ghner, W., Luszczynska, A., & Kliegel, M. (2009). maintenance of physical activity: Planning interventions in young, mid-
Changes in self-regulatory cognitions as predictors of changes in smok- dle-aged, and older adults. Psychology & Health, 21, 145163.
ing and nutrition behaviour. Psychology & Health, 24, 545561. Ziegelmann, J. P., Luszczynska, A., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2007).
Scholz, U., Schuz, B., Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R. Are goal intentions or implementation intentions better predictors of
(2008). Beyond behavioural intentions: Planning mediates between in- health behavior? A longitudinal study in orthopedic rehabilitation. Re-
tentions and physical activity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, habilitation Psychology, 52, 97102.
479 494.
Scholz, U., Sniehotta, F. F., Burkert, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2007). Increas- Received September 10, 2010
ing physical exercise levels: Age-specific benefits of planning. Journal Revision received January 27, 2011
of Aging and Health, 19, 851 866. Accepted May 28, 2011

You might also like