You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

ISSN: 1380-3395 (Print) 1744-411X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

Speed of processing and strategic control of


attention after traumatic brain injury

Alicia Rhian Dymowski, Jacqueline Anne Owens, Jennie Louise Ponsford &
Catherine Willmott

To cite this article: Alicia Rhian Dymowski, Jacqueline Anne Owens, Jennie Louise Ponsford &
Catherine Willmott (2015) Speed of processing and strategic control of attention after traumatic
brain injury, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37:10, 1024-1035, DOI:
10.1080/13803395.2015.1074663

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1074663

Published online: 01 Sep 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 488

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 10 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ncen20

Download by: [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] Date: 30 October 2017, At: 04:43
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2015
Vol. 37, No. 10, 10241035, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1074663

Speed of processing and strategic control of attention


after traumatic brain injury
Alicia Rhian Dymowski1,2, Jacqueline Anne Owens1,2, Jennie Louise Ponsford1,2,3,
and Catherine Willmott1,2
1
School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia
2
Monash-Epworth Rehabilitation Research Centre, Epworth HealthCare, Richmond, VIC, Australia
3
Centre of Excellence in Traumatic Brain Injury Rehabilitation, National Trauma Research Institute,
Alfred Hospital, Prahran, VIC, Australia
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

(Received 19 May 2015; accepted 15 July 2015)

Introduction: Slowed information processing speed has consistently been documented after traumatic brain injury (TBI).
Debate continues as to whether deficits in strategic control are proportionate to, or remain after controlling for, reduced
speed of processing. The study aim was to investigate the association of speed of processing and strategic control of
attention with working memory, selective attention, response inhibition, and mental flexibility task performance after
TBI using traditional and novel clinical measures. Method: Twenty-five individuals with complicated mild to severe TBI
(post-traumatic amnesia duration, M = 39.52 days, SD = 38.34; worst Glasgow Coma Scale score, M = 7.33, SD = 4.35;
time post-injury, M = 392.64 days, SD = 537.19) and 25 matched healthy controls completed assessment of attentional
and executive functioning. Measures included the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), the computerized Selective
Attention Task (SAT), the Ruff 2&7 Selective Attention Test (2&7), the visual n-back, Digit Span, Hayling Test, and
Trail Making Test (TMT). Results: t tests revealed that individuals with TBI demonstrated reduced processing speed on
the SDMT, n-back, SAT, 2&7, Hayling Test, and TMT-A (p .002 for all). Digit Span performance did not differ
between groups. Mixed-model ANOVAs revealed that individuals with TBI demonstrated a disproportionate increase
in reaction time with complexity, which was accounted for by speed on the SAT but remained on the Hayling Inhibition
Test after controlling for speed in ANCOVAs. Mann-Whitney U tests revealed that individuals with TBI also made
more errors on the Hayling Test, missed responses on the n-back and were unable to benefit from the automatic
condition of the 2&7. Conclusions: While slowed speed of information processing was pervasive across tasks after TBI,
residual difficulties in response inhibition remained after controlling for slowness, which suggests impaired strategic
control. These findings support targeted intervention for slowed speed of thinking and inhibition following TBI.

Keywords: Traumatic brain injury; Attention; Information processing speed; Strategic control; Working memory.

Attentional deficits are common and disabling distractions, concentrating for extended periods,
after traumatic brain injury (TBI; Olver, and paying attention to more than one thing at a
Ponsford, & Curran, 1996) and can interfere with time are commonly reported by individuals with
everyday roles (Lewis & Horn, 2013). Slowed pro- TBI and their family members (Olver et al., 1996;
cessing speed and difficulty with ignoring Ponsford & Kinsella, 1991).

The authors would like to thank the participants and their families who generously gave their time. The button box for the computer
task was designed and built by Antonio Benci at the Monash Facility for Instrumentation & Technology Development. Thanks to
Christopher Hocking for programming the n-back task.
This work was supported by the Epworth Research Institute [Grant 80940].
The Epworth Research Institute was not involved in study design, analysis and interpretation of data, writing of the report or
decision to submit the article for publication. The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Address correspondence to: Alicia Dymowski, Monash-Epworth Rehabilitation Research Centre, 185187 Hoddle Street,
Richmond, 3121, VIC, Australia (E-mail: Alicia.Dymowski@monash.edu).

2015 Taylor & Francis


PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1025

The definition of attention remains controversial networks (Povlishock, 1992). Acute cerebral eleva-
(Styles, 2006; Whyte, Ponsford, Watanabe, & tions of dopamine and noradrenaline have been
Hart, 2010). Within limited capacity constraints, reported in cerebrospinal fluid following TBI, and
attention serves to facilitate task performance by return of these neurotransmitters to normal concen-
selecting internal and external stimuli for active trations appears to be related to functional recovery
processing (James, 1890; Raz, 2004). Attention is (Markianos, Seretis, Kotsou, Baltas, &
not a unitary process, and numerous attentional Sacharogiannis, 1992; Markianos, Seretis, Kotsou,
network models have been proposed. Whyte et al. & Christopoulos, 1996).
(2010) proposed that component processes of Research has consistently demonstrated that
attention include arousal, selection, strategic (or individuals with TBI experience reduced speed of
executive) control, and processing speed. A two- information processing across the spectrum of
process model of attention distinguishing auto- injury severity (Felmingham, Baguley, & Green,
matic from controlled processing was proposed 2004; Frencham, Fox, & Maybery, 2005; Mathias
by Schneider and Shiffrin (1977). Automatic pro- & Wheaton, 2007; Ponsford & Kinsella, 1992;
cessing occurs in parallel for well-learned tasks not Willmott, Ponsford, Hocking, & Schnberger,
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

requiring effortful control, whereas controlled pro- 2009). Researchers have postulated that difficulties
cessing is serial in nature, involves conscious pro- on complex attentional tasks demonstrated by indi-
cessing, and has a limited capacity and rate. The viduals with TBI are largely accounted for by, and
concepts of automatic and controlled processing proportionate to, slowed processing speed
are incorporated in an information processing (Felmingham et al., 2004; Ponsford & Kinsella,
model that describes contention scheduling and 1992; Spikman, van Zomeren, & Deelman, 1996;
the Supervisory Attentional System (SAS; Veltman, Brouwer, van Zomeren, & van
Norman & Shallice, 1980; Shallice, 1982). Wolffelaar, 1996). Others have argued that deficits
Contention scheduling uses existing automatic sen- in strategic control of attention remain after
sorimotor schemas to coordinate action for well- accounting for reduced processing speed, particu-
learned behaviors and thoughts, whereas the SAS larly on tasks with greater complexity or increased
is activated when tasks are novel or complex or working memory load (Asloun et al., 2008; Azouvi,
when automatic behaviors need to be overridden. Jokic, van der Linden, Marlier, & Bussel, 1996;
The SAS is akin to the central executive in Park, Moscovitch, & Robertson, 1999; Serino
Baddeleys (1986, 2000) model of working mem- et al., 2006). Strategic control of attention has
ory. Strategic control (used in this paper to refer to been assessed with working memory tasks that
the executive processes of attention, incorporating require controlled processing via maintenance and
the concepts of the SAS and central executive) manipulation of limited attentional resources.
directs, manages, and monitors limited attentional Perlstein et al. (2004) parametrically varied working
resources in a goal-directed manner for tasks memory load using a visual n-back task and found
requiring controlled processing, including sus- severity-dependent and load-related working mem-
tained, selective, divided, and shifting attention ory deficits in error rates, but no group differences
and working memory (Whyte et al., 2010). in reaction time (RT), suggesting deficits in strategic
Hence, impairment of strategic control should control rather than speed. Yet evidence suggests
result in deficits in these cognitive domains that both speed and strategic control may be
(McDowell, Whyte, & DEsposito, 1997; Serino impaired after TBI. Willmott et al. (2009) found
et al., 2006). that speed, but not working memory, accounted
The high prevalence of attentional deficits fol- for group differences in RT on a complex Selective
lowing TBI is understandable, given the neuro- Attention Task (SAT); however, participants with
pathology and neurochemistry of TBI and the TBI also made more errors and missed responses,
distributed nature of attentional networks. which suggests impairments in strategic control. In
Anterior regions of the brain, including the prefron- addition, Ros, Periez, and Muoz-Cspedes
tal cortex, are proposed to be involved in strategic (2004) found that group differences were accounted
control of attention (MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, for by reduced speed of thinking on an inhibition
& Carter, 2000) and are particularly vulnerable to task (Stroop Test) but not on a mental flexibility
insult (Bigler, 2001; Levin, Williams, Eisenberg, task (Trail Making Test; TMT), suggesting that
High, & Guinto, 1992). Furthermore, diffuse axo- attentional processes may be differentially influ-
nal injury (DAI) can result in widespread axonal enced by speed of processing and strategic control.
disconnection, resulting in the disruption of ascend- Individuals with TBI have demonstrated reduced
ing catecholaminergic and serotonergic projections, processing speed on the Hayling Test (Spitz et al.,
which are key neuromodulators of attentional 2013), a measure of response inhibition, but it is
1026 DYMOWSKI ET AL.

unclear whether there is a differential effect of group sufficient understanding of English to complete
with increased executive load on this task. the tasks; (d) sufficient cognitive ability to com-
The aim of the current study was to investigate plete the tasks; (e) dominant hand use; and (f) no
the association between speed of thinking and stra- history of significant neurological or psychiatric
tegic control on traditional and novel attentional disturbance.
tasks requiring controlled processing in people Twenty-five participants with TBI were
with TBI relative to healthy controls. Given the recruited. Median time post-injury was 216 days
distributed nature of attentional networks and cor- (M = 392.64, SD = 537.19, range = 34 to 2497).
respondingly diverse neuropathological and neuro- Considering PTA duration and worst GCS, parti-
chemical disruption after TBI, it was hypothesized cipants had sustained complicated mild (all of
that patients with TBI would demonstrate reduced whom had evidence of intracranial abnormality
speed of information processing. Furthermore, on neuroimaging) to very severe injuries (PTA 1
given the prevalence of lesions in frontal regions day, n = 2; PTA >1 to <7 days, n = 2; PTA = 7 to
responsible for strategic control, it was proposed 28 days, n = 10; PTA >28 days, n = 11). Worst
that on working memory, selective attention, and GCS scores ranged from 3 to 15 (GSC 1315, n =
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

mental flexibility tasks requiring controlled proces- 6; GCS 912, n = 2; GCS 38, n = 16; not recorded,
sing, participants with TBI would demonstrate a n = 1). This study included two individuals with
disproportionate increase in RT, which could not complicated mild TBI, as research has demon-
be accounted for by processing speed (i.e., after strated poorer outcomes following complicated
controlling for the automatic processing condi- compared to uncomplicated mild TBI (Iverson
tion), and would make more errors and missed et al., 2012; Lange, Iverson, & Franzen, 2009). All
responses than controls. A particularly novel participants were out of PTA when assessed. The
aspect of this research was to use the Hayling most common mechanism of injury was motor
Test to investigate the association of speed and vehicle accidents (n = 15), followed by falls (n =
strategic control on response inhibition. It was 4), cycling accidents (n = 3), pedestrians versus
hypothesized that individuals with TBI would motor vehicles (n = 2), and sporting injuries (n = 1).
demonstrate a disproportionate increase in RT, Twenty-five age-, gender- and education-
which could not be accounted for by initiation matched healthy control participants were
speed, and would make more errors on this task recruited from the general community, and exclu-
relative to healthy controls. sion criteria (b) to (f) above applied. There were no
significant differences between individuals with
TBI and controls for background demographics
METHOD
(Table 1).
Participants
Measures
Participants were recruited from the Acquired
Brain Injury Rehabilitation service at Epworth
National Adult Reading Test (NART)
HealthCare, Melbourne, Australia. Eligibility cri-
teria included: (a) history of complicated mild to The NART (Nelson, 1982) comprises 50 words
severe TBI; (b) age between 16 and 65 years; (c) with irregular phonemic pronunciations and was

TABLE 1
Demographics and injury characteristics

TBI Control
(n = 25) (n = 25) p

Gender (male) 18 18 1.00


Age (years): Mean (SD) 38.44 (16.25) 36.36 (14.83) .64
Education (years): Mean (SD) 13.20 (2.72) 13.38 (2.51) .81
% Employed 84 92 .39
SES: Mean (SD) 4.48 (1.13) 4.33 (1.03) .63
NART FSIQ: Mean (SD) 106.62 (8.25) 107.22 (5.93) .77
GCS (worst score): Mean (SD) [Range] 7.33 (4.35) [315]
PTA (days): Mean (SD) [Range] 39.52 (38.34) [1142]

Note. TBI = traumatic brain injury; SES = socioeconomic status as measured by the Scale of Occupational Prestige (Daniel, 1983);
NART = National Adult Reading Test; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale; PTA = post-traumatic
amnesia.
PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1027

used as a measure of pre-morbid intellectual func- conditions: Simple (SSAT) and Complex (CSAT)
tioning. The number of errors and predicted Full Selective Attention Tasks. Participants responded
Scale IQ (FSIQ) were recorded. based on color for the SSAT (requiring automatic
processing), and color and content for the CSAT
(requiring controlled processing; see Willmott
Digit Span
et al., 2009; Ziino & Ponsford, 2006 for more
Digit Span (DS) from the Wechsler Adult information). RT for correct responses, errors,
Intelligence Scale 4th Edition (WAIS-IV; and missed responses were recorded for each con-
Wechsler, 2008) was used to assess auditory work- dition. The practice session of 20 trials was
ing memory. Raw scores for digit span forwards repeated until an accuracy criterion of at least
(DSF), backwards (DSB), sequencing (DSS), and 70% was reached.
total (DST) were analyzed.
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)
Trail Making Test
The written SDMT is a measure of processing
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

The TMT is a paper-and-pencil task consisting speed requiring the participant to examine a
of two parts: TMT-A and TMT-B (Reitan, 1958). sequence of symbols, then search a key for each
TMT-A requires individuals to connect sequen- symbol and substitute the corresponding number
tially increasing numbers (measuring speed), (Smith, 1991). Number of items correctly com-
whereas TMT-B requires individuals to alternate pleted and errors within 90 s were recorded.
in ascending order between numbers and letters
(measuring speed and mental flexibility).
Completion time and number of errors for each N-back
condition were recorded. The n-back is a visual sequential letter memory
task, which parametrically varies working memory
Hayling Test load by asking participants to decide whether a
target matches the stimuli presented 0, 1, or 2
The Hayling Test consists of two sets of 15 screens earlier, with 0-back requiring automatic
sentences, each with the last word missing processing and higher working memory loads
(Burgess & Shallice, 1997). Individuals must requiring greater controlled processing. (Cohen
replace the missing word with a logical response et al., 1997; Perlstein et al., 2004). The n-back
(Hayling Initiation; measuring response speed) or task was developed on E-Prime and comprised
an illogical response (Hayling Inhibition; measur- single consonants centrally presented for 500 ms.
ing speed and response inhibition). Speed of com- Participants responded with a dominant-hand
pletion and total error raw scores were analyzed. index finger press of one button for targets (p =
.33) and another for non-targets. A missed
Ruff 2 and 7 Selective Attention Test (2&7) response was recorded after 3000 ms. Conditions
were run in blocks of 18 stimuli, with three blocks
The 2&7 is a pencil-and-paper visual search and for each condition. Participants received visual
cancellation task designed to measure selective instructions and practice of each task prior to test
attention (Ruff & Allen, 1995). Participants can- trials. Thereafter, order of the conditions was ran-
celled the digits 2 and 7 among either letters or domized. RT for correct responses, errors, and
numbers, with the former being an automatic, missed responses were recorded for each condition.
single-step retrieval condition and the latter requir-
ing controlled search employing working memory
abilities. Each of the 20 trials has a 15-s time limit. Procedure
Automatic speed (ASRS), controlled speed (CSRS)
and accuracy raw scores were calculated for both This study was approved by Epworth HealthCare
conditions. and Monash University Human Research Ethics
Committees, and all participants provided
informed consent. Worst GCS scores and PTA
Selective Attention Task (SAT)
duration determined by prospective monitoring
This computerized visual response contingency with the Westmead PTA Scale (Shores,
paradigm is designed to assess selective attention Marosszeky, Sandanam, & Batchelor, 1986) were
and the participants ability to inhibit automatic recorded. Neuroimaging results were obtained
responses (Ziino & Ponsford, 2006). It includes two from medical records. Participants completed a
1028 DYMOWSKI ET AL.

demographics questionnaire, followed by the atten- Speed of information processing and


tion tasks. Order of task presentation was rando- strategic control
mized between participants.
Using a Bonferroni adjusted level of .002, bivariate
comparisons revealed that TBI participants com-
Data analysis pleted fewer items on the SDMT and cancelled
fewer targets on both the 2&7 automatic and con-
RTs greater or less than 2 standard deviations trolled conditions than control participants (Table 2).
from the mean for each group were removed (3% The TBI group also demonstrated significantly slo-
for n-back across three conditions and 5% for SAT wed information processing relative to the control
across two conditions), resulting in similar percen- group on TMT-A and on all conditions of the
tage of outliers to previous research (Willmott Hayling Test, SAT, and n-back. There were, how-
et al., 2009). Univariate outliers with z > 3.29 (p ever, no differences in working memory performance
< .001, two-tailed) were identified (1 each in TBI between the TBI and control group on any DS
TMT-A and TMT-B) and assigned a score 1 unit condition.
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

larger than the next most extreme score Mann-Whitney U Tests revealed no significant
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). t tests were used to differences between TBI and control groups on num-
identify differences between groups for normally ber of errors on SDMT, TMT-A, or TMT-B, or
distributed test variables. Error data for the accuracy on 2&7 automatic and controlled condi-
SDMT, TMT, Hayling Test, 2&7 accuracy data, tions (p > .05). In contrast, TBI participants made
and errors and missed responses for the SAT and significantly more errors on Hayling Inhibition than
n-back did not meet parametric assumptions and did control participants (M = 15.84, SD = 18.52; M
were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U Tests. = 4.24, SD = 6.65, respectively), U = 158, z = 3.01,
Bonferroni adjustments were made for multiple p = .002. There were no significant differences
comparisons. Two-way mixed model analyses of between groups on SSAT or CSAT number of errors
variance (ANOVAs) were conducted on 2&7 and or misses (p > .05). On the n-back, there were no
SAT (selective attention), n-back (working mem- significant group differences for number of errors on
ory), TMT (mental flexibility), and Hayling (inhi- any condition (p > .05), but TBI participants made
bition) data to test the hypothesis that participants significantly more missed responses than controls on
with TBI would demonstrate a disproportionately the 0-back (M = 0.64, SD = 2.20; M = 0.00, SD =
greater increase in RT than controls at higher load 0.00, respectively), U = 238, z = 2.58, p = .02, 1-
levels requiring controlled processing. Between- back (M = 1.36, SD = 2.56; M = 0.08, SD = 0.40,
subjects factor was group (control, TBI), and the respectively), U = 176, z = 3.45, p = .001, and 2-
within-subject factor was condition/load. On tasks back conditions (M = 3.16, SD = 7.26; M = 0.04, SD
demonstrating a significant interaction effect, one- = 0.20, respectively), U = 133, z = 4.22, p < .001.
way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were con- The 2&7, SAT, n-back, TMT and Hayling Test
ducted to determine whether disproportionate were analyzed with mixed model ANOVAs to inves-
group differences in RT remained after controlling tigate the influence of load on attentional and execu-
for slowed processing speed (i.e. the automatic tive functioning performance between groups.
processing condition of each task). Finding a significant Group Condition interaction
in the hypothesized direction would suggest that the
performance of individuals with TBI is disproportion-
RESULTS ally poorer than that of controls under conditions of
increased working memory or executive load
A small number of individuals with TBI were pre- (Kinsella, 2008; Perlstein et al., 2004; Willmott et al.,
scribed anticonvulsants (20%) or narcotic analge- 2009).
sics (12%). Separate multivariate analyses of On the n-back task (Figure 1), the TBI group was
variance (MANOVA) were conducted to assess significantly slower than the control group, F(1, 48) =
whether medication influenced performance on 26.36, p < .001, 2p = .35. There was a significant
cognitive assessments. Dependent variables were linear effect across loads, F(2, 47) = 45.02, p < .001,
cognitive measures that demonstrated differences 2p = .66, with step-wise slower performance with
between the TBI and control groups. There was increased working memory load. However, an inter-
no significant effect of group on cognition for action effect was not observed, F(2, 47) = 0.74, p = .48.
anticonvulsants, F(11, 13) = 1.42, p = .27, Wilks On the SAT (Figure 2), participants with TBI
Lambda = .45, or for narcotics, F(11, 13) = 0.37, performed significantly more slowly than controls,
p = .95, Wilks Lambda = .76. F(1, 48) = 26.95, p < .001, 2p = .36, and
PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1029

TABLE 2
Means, standard deviations, significance and effects sizes for TBI and control groups

TBI Control

Task Measure Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p Effect sizea

Digit Span DSF RS 10.92 (2.47) 10.68 (2.64) .74


DSB RS 9.04 (2.70) 8.16 (2.29) .22
DSS RS 9.00 (2.25) 8.32 (2.23) .29
DST RS 28.96 (6.10) 27.44 (5.33) .36
SDMT Number correct 41.96 (10.74) 55.36 (9.95) <.001 1.29
TMT TMT-A time (s) 40.56 (16.68) 26.32 (9.33) .001 1.05
TMT-B time (s) 85.36 (39.47) 63.76 (21.71) .02
Hayling Initiation Time (s) 16.36 (14.67) 5.84 (5.01) .002 0.96
Inhibition Time (s) 46.20 (45.46) 9.80 (9.58) .001 1.11
2&7 ASRS 106.00 (33.56) 153.48 (25.25) <.001 1.60
CSRS 98.56 (27.08) 133.96 (19.21) <.001 1.51
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

SAT SSAT RT (ms) 919.69 (224.26) 662.66 (99.47) <.001 1.48


CSAT RT (ms) 1663.58 (396.53) 1254.34 (199.29) <.001 1.30
N-back 0-back RT (ms) 842.00 (208.21) 598.07 (108.17) <.001 1.47
1-back RT (ms) 896.02 (187.19) 685.73 (137.11) <.001 1.28
2-back RT (ms) 1023.91 (213.87) 786.92 (161.15) <.001 1.25

Note. DSF = Digit Span Forward; RS = Raw Score; DSB = Digit Span Backward; DSS = Digit Span Sequencing; DST = Digit
Span Total; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; TMT = Trail Making Test; ASRS = Automatic Speed Raw Score; CSRS =
Controlled Speed Raw Score; SAT = Selective Attention Task; SSAT = Simple Selective Attention Task; RT = Reaction Time; CSAT
= Complex Selective Attention Task.
a
Effect size = Cohens d.

participants performed more slowly on the com-


plex than the simple condition, F(1, 48) = 435.28,
p < .001, 2p = .90. There was also a significant
interaction effect, with participants with TBI show-
ing greater increases in RT from simple to complex
tasks compared to control participants, F(1, 48) =
5.65, p = .02, 2p = .11.
For the 2&7 (Figure 3), the TBI group cancelled
significantly fewer targets than did the control
group, F(1, 48) = 32.18, p < .001, 2p = .40, and
both groups cancelled significantly fewer targets
under the controlled than the automatic condition,
Figure 1. Mean reaction times (and standard error of the mean) F(1, 48) = 45.90, p < .001, 2p = .49. There was a
on the 0-, 1- and 2-back loads of the n-back working memory
task for TBI and control groups.
significant Group Condition interaction, F(1, 48)

Figure 3. Mean number of items completed (and standard error


Figure 2. Mean reaction times (and standard error of the mean) of the mean) on the Automatic and Controlled conditions of the
on the Simple and Complex conditions of the Computerized Ruff 2&7 Selective Attention Test (2&7) for the TBI and control
Selective Attention Task (SAT) for the TBI and control groups. groups.
1030 DYMOWSKI ET AL.

differential impairments of RT in TBI and control


participants on the complex tasks, namely the
SAT, 2&7, and Hayling tests. ANCOVAs were
conducted to covary for speed of processing on
complex tasks (Felmingham et al., 2004). To con-
trol for speed of processing in selective attention,
we used the 2&7 automatic condition (ASRS) as a
covariate for the controlled condition (CSRS),
and the SAT simple condition (SSAT) as a cov-
ariate for the complex condition (CSAT). Hayling
Initiation was used as a covariate for Hayling
Inhibition when investigating the effect of speed
Figure 4. Mean response time (and standard error of the mean)
on response inhibition. The TMT and n-back
on the Initiation and Inhibition conditions of the Hayling Test
for the TBI and control groups. were not further analyzed, as no interaction
effects were found between group and task
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

complexity.
= 9.22, p = .004, 2p = .16, with controls showing On the SAT, after adjusting for the simple con-
greater decrement in speed on controlled relative to dition (SSAT), there was no significant difference
automatic processing than the TBI group. between the two groups on the complex condition
On the Hayling Test (Figure 4), the TBI group (CSAT), F(1, 47) = 0.99, p = .32. There was a
performed more slowly than controls, F(1, 48) = strong relationship between the simple and com-
17.21, p < .001, 2p = .26, and both groups per- plex condition times, with an 2p of .51.
formed more slowly on the Inhibition condition On the 2&7, covarying for speed of processing
than the Initiation condition, F(1, 48) = 17.85, p (ASRS) removed the significant group difference
< .001, 2p = .27. There was an interaction effect on the controlled condition (CSRS), F(1, 47) =
whereby patients with TBI showed a greater 0.29, p = .60. There was a strong relationship
increase in speed on the Inhibition when compared between 2&7 automatic and controlled condition
to the Initiation task relative to controls, F(1, 48) = scores, as indicated by an 2p of .79.
10.46, p = .002, 2p = .18. On the Hayling Test, a significant group effect
For the TMT (Figure 5), the TBI group were on the Inhibition Task remained after controlling
slower than the control group, F(1, 48) = 8.95, p = for speed on the Initiation Task, F(1, 47) = 4.72, p
.004, 2p = .16, and both groups took longer to = .04, 2p = .09. Again, there was a strong relation-
complete TMT-B than TMT-A, F(1, 48) = 140.65, ship between the Hayling subtests, with an 2p
p < .001, 2p = .75. However, there was no sig- of .31.
nificant interaction, F(1, 48) = 1.13, p = .29. These findings indicate that speed explained the
group differences in the complex conditions of the
selective attention tasks, but a specific deficit in
Association of speed with group differences
response inhibition remained after controlling for
speed.
We were interested to investigate the contribution
of slowed information processing to the

DISCUSSION

The aim of this study was to investigate factors


contributing to attentional impairment after TBI,
particularly the influence of deficits in speed of
thinking and strategic control on a range of atten-
tional tasks requiring controlled processing, includ-
ing working memory, selective attention, mental
flexibility, and response inhibition. While reduced
information processing speed was pervasive across
tasks, impaired strategic control of attention was
only evident on a response inhibition task (i.e. the
Figure 5. Mean response time (and standard error of the mean)
on Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B conditions for the TBI Hayling Test) after accounting for slowed proces-
and control groups. sing speed.
PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1031

Participants with TBI demonstrated reduced covariance analyses revealed that the difference
speed of information processing on the SDMT, between groups on the CSAT was accounted for
TMT-A, and all conditions of the Hayling Test, by reduced speed of information processing
2&7, SAT, and n-back tasks relative to healthy (SSAT). This is in line with previous research
controls. This finding corroborates previous using structural equation modelling, which showed
research, consistently identifying reduced speed of the main contribution to group differences on the
information processing as a common sequela post- CSAT was slowed speed of thinking, with only a
TBI (Felmingham et al., 2004; Frencham et al., weak contribution of impaired working memory
2005; Mathias & Wheaton, 2007; Ponsford & (Willmott et al., 2009). Yet in the current study
Kinsella, 1992; Willmott et al., 2009). participants with TBI did not make more errors or
Working memory was assessed in the current missed responses with increased complexity, as has
study with DS and the n-back tasks. There were been previously demonstrated (Willmott et al.,
no significant differences between the groups on 2009; Ziino & Ponsford, 2006). When compared
any DS measure (DSF, DSB, DSS, or DST), to Willmott et al. (2009), this sample of partici-
which is indicative of intact functioning of the pants with TBI made fewer errors (M = 5.38, M =
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

phonological loop (Serino et al., 2006). On the 2.52, respectively) and missed responses (M = 1.38,
n-back task, a measure of the central executive of M = 0.40, respectively) on the CSAT. Thus, the
working memory, participants with TBI had accuracy profile of the current TBI group was
slower RTs than controls, and both groups were similar to that of the healthy control group, sug-
slower with increased working memory load. gesting that speed but not accuracy was impaired
However, the TBI group was not disproportional- with increased complexity.
ity slower at increased loads than was the control On the 2&7 the control participants demon-
group, which is contrary to the hypothesis, but in strated a greater reduction in speed with increased
line with previous research (Asloun et al., 2008; complexity from the automatic to controlled pro-
Perlstein et al., 2004). Furthermore, participants cessing condition relative to TBI participants.
with TBI showed more missed responses than did While this finding is opposed to the hypothesized
controls on all conditions of the n-back. These direction of change, it is commensurate with pre-
results are in contrast to Perlstein et al. (2004), vious research (Willmott et al., 2009). Willmott
who found that groups did not differ in the number et al. (2009) suggested that such a finding poten-
of missed responses on the n-back, but, rather, that tially indicates that individuals with TBI require
participants with moderate to severe TBI made controlled processing for both the automatic and
more errors at higher working memory loads (2- controlled conditions, and thus are unable to ben-
and 3-back) relative to controls. Potentially, efit from the reduced cognitive demands of the
Perlstein et al. (2004) elicited more errors through automatic condition. Indeed, neuroimaging of par-
inclusion of non-target repeats as foils. The ticipants with DAI after TBI engaged in tasks
increase in missed responses in the current study requiring strategic control has demonstrated
may also reflect the shorter stimulus onset asyn- greater neural recruitment of regions of the pre-
chrony of 3000 ms compared to 4000 ms used by frontal cortex and posterior cortices at earlier
Perlstein et al. (2004), resulting in insufficient time stages of difficulty compared to controls (Turner
for participants with TBI to process and respond & Levine, 2008; Turner, McIntosh, & Levine,
appropriately. Alternatively, missed responses may 2011). Alternatively, it has been suggested that
indicate greater inattention, distractibility, or dis- the findings may be indicative of greater motiva-
engagement from all n-back conditions in partici- tion or mental effort in the group with TBI, or that
pants with TBI relative to controls. Thus, these the task was not sufficiently complex to tap strate-
results demonstrate impaired speed but not deficits gic control (Asloun et al., 2008; Vallat-Azouvi,
in strategic control on working memory tasks Weber, Legrand, & Azouvi, 2007).
after TBI. The Hayling Test results demonstrated that par-
The hypothesized disproportionate increase in ticipants with TBI were disproportionally slower
RTs with increased task complexity for partici- with increased complexity compared to controls
pants with TBI relative to controls was observed on Hayling Inhibition, which remained after cov-
on two of five tasks. Specifically, participants with arying for speed (Hayling Initiation). These results
TBI demonstrated disproportionately slower RT suggest that the differences between groups were
on the SAT with additional working memory due to impairment of a specific attentional compo-
load (CSAT: color and letter/number) relative to nent rather than to deficits in speed of processing.
the simple task (SSAT: color). Additional Participants with TBI also made more errors than
1032 DYMOWSKI ET AL.

did controls, which is further indicative of GENERAL DISCUSSION, LIMITATIONS, AND


impaired response inhibition after brain injury. FUTURE DIRECTIONS
The Hayling Test requires participants to inhibit
an automatic response before generating a novel The SAT and Hayling Test demonstrated the pre-
response, necessitating both response inhibition dicted Group Condition interaction, with parti-
and idea generation skills. The Stroop Test simi- cipants with TBI more disadvantaged with
larly requires participants to inhibit an automatic increased complexity than were controls. On the
response in favor of a less familiar response, but it 2&7 participants with TBI appeared unable to
has generally not been found to demonstrate a benefit from the minimal processing demands of
disproportionate distraction effect (Felmingham the automatic condition. Alternatively, the 2&7,
et al., 2004; Ponsford & Kinsella, 1992; Ros TMT and n-back may not have been sufficiently
et al., 2004). Potentially the addition of idea gen- challenging or complex to recruit strategic control
eration makes the Hayling more sensitive than the of attention. More challenging tasks tapping func-
Stroop Test to deficits in response inhibition after tional attentional difficulties experienced in every-
TBI. Furthermore, the Hayling Test tapped two day life by participants with TBI may be required
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

executive processes simultaneouslynamely, inhi- to elicit deficits in strategic control.


bition and idea generation (Strauss, Sherman, & The present data strongly support the pervasive
Spreen, 2006)which possibly made this task influence of reduced speed of information proces-
more challenging and better able to tap everyday sing on attentional performance after TBI.
functioning difficulties in individuals with TBI Specifically, speed accounted for group differences
than other tasks requiring controlled processing on the complex conditions of the selective attention
used in this study (Spikman, Deelman, & van tasks, including the Ruff 2&7 CSRS and the
Zomeren, 2000). Alternatively, examination of the CSAT. Yet the findings also indicate that strategic
error profiles suggests that controls were more control influences performance beyond the influ-
likely than individuals with TBI to adopt a strategy ence of speed of thinking, whereby group differ-
(i.e., naming objects in the room or listing items ences on the Hayling Test Inhibition task remained
from a predefined category) to avoid providing an after controlling for speed. There were no differ-
automatic response. This suggests that individuals ential group differences in RT on working memory
with TBI may demonstrate impairment in either and mental flexibility tasks. The relative influence
the generation or the implementation of strategies of speed and strategic control on attention may be
(Shallice, 2002). Hence, the Hayling Test possibly task-dependent or attention-type-specific.
captures strategy production or implementation We acknowledge that the current results included
better than do the other tasks assessing strategic a small sample of participants who were treated at
control employed in this study. Epworth HealthCare. Thus, the results may not be
The results of the TMT indicate that participants representative of the broader population with TBI.
with TBI were slower than controls, but did not Furthermore, the small sample size may have
perform differentially across conditions. Explicitly, resulted in reduced power to detect deficits in strate-
participants with TBI were not more disadvantaged gic control beyond the influence of speed. Lack of
by increased working memory load and flexibility significant findings in the covariance analyses should
requirements than healthy control participants. therefore be interpreted cautiously. This study
Previous research findings on group differences in included a heterogeneous sample, whereby the
attention flexibility (TMT-B) after controlling for majority of individuals had sustained a severe TBI,
speed (TMT-A) are mixed. The current results are and two individuals had sustained a complicated
in line with Felmingham et al. (2004), who found mild TBI (both with intracranial abnormality evi-
that group differences on TMT-B were removed dent on neuroimaging). While individuals with com-
after covarying for TMT-A, but contrast with find- plicated mild TBI typically have poorer outcomes
ings of Ros et al. (2004), who reported that deficits than those with uncomplicated TBI (Iverson et al.,
in strategic control remained after controlling for 2012; Lange et al., 2009), it is possible that the
speed. The speed of completion of TMT-B in this nature of the cognitive impairments differs across
study (M = 85.36) was similar to Felmingham et al. this sample, and deficits in strategic control may be
(2004) (M = 79.0 for focal pathology; M = 109.2 for limited to the most severely injured individuals.
diffuse pathology), which were quicker than the Despite attempts to fractionate strategic control
results of Ros et al. (2004) (M = 162.03). The into subcomponents both theoretically (Baddeley,
differences in speed of completion on TMT-B for 2002; Shallice, 2002) and via factor analytic tech-
participants with TBI across studies likely explains niques (Mirsky, Anthony, Duncan, Ahearn, &
the differential findings. Kellam, 1991; Ros et al., 2004), the mechanisms
PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1033

underlying attentional control remain debated. Cohen, J. D., Perlstein, W. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom,
Hence, it is currently unclear whether various com- L. E., Noll, D. C., Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (1997).
Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a
ponent processes of strategic control are differen-
working memory task. Nature, 386, 604608.
tially affected after TBI. Functional magnetic doi:10.1038/386604a0
resonance imaging (fMRI) and other imaging tech- Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege and prestige:
nologies provide advanced techniques for delineat- Occupations in Australia. Melbourne: Longman
ing component processes of strategic control as Cheshire.
Felmingham, K. L., Baguley, I. J., & Green, A. M.
well as exploring underlying neuropathology and
(2004). Effects of diffuse axonal injury on speed of
neurochemical alterations responsible for deficits information processing following severe traumatic
in complex attention and speed after TBI. brain injury. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 564571.
doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.564
Frencham, K. A. R., Fox, A. M., & Maybery, M. T.
CONCLUSIONS (2005). Neuropsychological studies of mild traumatic
brain injury: A meta-analytic review of research since
1995. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
The current study strongly supports the vast array Neuropsychology, 27, 334351. doi:10.1080/13803390
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

of research demonstrating slowed information pro- 490520328


cessing after TBI. There was also evidence of Iverson, G. L., Lange, R. T., Wljas, M.,
impaired strategic control of attention on response Liimatainen, S., Dastidar, P., Hartikainen,
inhibition tasks after controlling for speed. Hence, K. M.,. . . hman, J. (2012). Outcome from com-
plicated versus uncomplicated mild traumatic brain
various component processes of attention or atten- injury. Rehabilitation Research and Practice.
tional tasks are differentially influenced by speed doi:10.1155/2012/415740
and strategic control after TBI. Further investiga- James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New
tion and replication of differential effects on tasks York: Dover.
requiring multiple executive processes and strategy Kinsella, G. J. (2008). Traumatic brain injury and pro-
cessing speed. In J. DeLuca & J. H. Kalmar (Eds.),
production and implementation is warranted in Information processing speed in clinical populations
order to guide rehabilitative efforts. (pp. 173194). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Lange, R. T., Iverson, G. L., & Franzen, M. D. (2009).
Neuropsychological functioning following compli-
cated vs. uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury.
REFERENCES Brain Injury, 23(2), 8391. doi:10.1080/
02699050802635281
Asloun, S., Soury, S., Couillet, J., Giroire, J.-M., Joseph, Levin, H. S., Williams, D. H., Eisenberg, H. M., High,
P.-A.,Mazaux, J.-M., & Azouvi, P. (2008). W. M. J., & Guinto, F. C. (1992). Serial magnetic
Interactions between divided attention and working- resonance imaging and neurobehavioural findings
memory load in patients with severe traumatic brain after mild to moderate closed head injury. Journal
injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 55,
Neuropsychology, 30(4), 481490. doi:10.1080/ 255262.
13803390701550144 Lewis, F. D., & Horn, G. J. (2013). Traumatic brain
Azouvi, P., Jokic, C., van der Linden, M., Marlier, N., injury: Analysis of functional deficits and posthospi-
& Bussel, B. (1996). Working memory and supervi- tal rehabilitation outcomes. Journal of Special
sory control after severe closed-head injury. A study Operations Medicine, 13(3), 5661.
of dual task performance and random generation. MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., &
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the
Neuropsychology, 18(3), 317337. doi:10.1080/026990 dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex
50902970711 in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 18351838.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: doi:10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
Oxford University Press. Markianos, M., Seretis, A., Kotsou, A., Baltas, I., &
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new compo- Sacharogiannis, H. (1992). CSF neurotransmitter
nent of working memory? Trends in Cognitive metabolites and short-term outcome of patients in
Sciences, 4(11), 417423. coma after head injury. Acta Neurologica
Baddeley, A. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. Scandinavica, 86, 190193. doi:10.1111/j.1600-
In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of 0404.1992.tb05064.x
frontal lobe function (pp. 246260). New York: Markianos, M., Seretis, A., Kotsou, A., &
Oxford University Press. Christopoulos, M. (1996). CSF neurotransmitter
Bigler, E. D. (2001). Quantitative magnetic resonance metabolites in comatose head injury patients during
imaging in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head changes in their clinical state. Acta Neurochirurgica,
Trauma Rehabilitation, 16(2), 117134. 138, 5759. doi:10.1007/BF01411725
Burgess, P., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Mathias, J. L., & Wheaton, P. (2007). Changes in atten-
Brixton Tests. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley tion and information-processing speed following
Test Company. severe traumatic brain injury: A meta-analytic
1034 DYMOWSKI ET AL.

review. Neuropsychology, 21(2), 212223. Serino, A., Ciaramelli, E., Di Santantonio, A., Malag,
doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.212 S., Servadei, F., & Ldavas, E. (2006). Central execu-
McDowell, S., Whyte, J., & DEsposito, M. (1997). tive system impairment in traumatic brain injury.
Working memory impairments in traumatic brain Brain Injury, 20(1), 2332. doi:10.1080/
injury: Evidence from a dual-task paradigm. 02699050500309627
Neuropsychologia, 35(10), 13411353. doi:10.1016/ Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning.
S0028-3932(97)00082-1 Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, Biological Sciences, 298, 199209. doi:10.1098/
M. B., & Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the ele- rstb.1982.0082
ments of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Shallice, T. (2002). Fractionation of the supervisory sys-
Neuropsychological Review, 2(2), 109145. tem. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles
doi:10.1007/BF01109051 of frontal lobe function (pp. 261277). New York:
Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test Oxford University Press.
(NART): Test manual. Windsor: NFER Nelson. Shores, E. A., Marosszeky, J. E., Sandanam, J., &
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to Batchelor, J. (1986). Preliminary validation of a
action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. scale for measuring the duration of post-traumatic
Center for Human Information Processing Technical amnesia. Medical Journal of Australia, 144,
Report No. 99. San Diego, CA: University of 569572.
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

California. Smith, A. (1991). Symbol digit modalities test. Los


Olver, J. H., Ponsford, J. L., & Curran, C. A. (1996). Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Outcome following traumatic brain injury: A com- Spikman, J. M., Deelman, B. G., & van Zomeren, A. H.
parison between 2 and 5 years after injury. Brain (2000). Executive functioning, attention and frontal
Injury, 10(11), 841848. doi:10.1080/02699059 lesions in patients with chronic CHI. Journal of
6123945 Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(3),
Park, N. W., Moscovitch, M., & Robertson, I. H. 325338. doi:10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;
(1999). Divided attention impairments after trau- FT325
matic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 37, 11191133. Spikman, J. M., van Zomeren, A., & Deelman, B. G.
Perlstein, W. M., Cole, M. A., Demery, J. A., (1996). Deficits of attention after closed-head injury:
Seignourel, P. J., Dixit, N. K., Larson, M. J., & Sloweness only? Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Briggs, R. W. (2004). Parametric manipulation of Neuropsychology, 18(5), 755767. doi:10.1080/
working memory load in traumatic brain injury: 01688639608408298
Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of the Spitz, G., Bigler, E. D., Abildskov, T., Maller, J. J.,
International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 724 OSullivan, R., & Ponsford, J. L. (2013). Regional
741. doi:10.10170S1355617704105110 cortical volume and cognitive functioning following
Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1991). The use of a rating traumatic brain injury. Brain and Cognition, 83,
scale of attentional behaviour. Neuropsychological 3444. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.06.007
Rehabilitation, 1(4), 241257. doi:10.1080/09602019 Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A
108402257 compendium of neuropsychological tests:
Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1992). Attentional deficits Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.).
following closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and New York: Oxford University Press.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 822838. Styles, E. A. (2006). The psychology of attention (2nd
doi:10.1080/01688639208402865 ed.). New York: Psychology Press.
Povlishock, J. T. (1992). Traumatically induced axonal Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multi-
injury: Pathogenesis and pathobiological implica- variate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson
tions. Brain Pathology, 2(1), 112. Education.
Raz, A. (2004). Anatomy of attentional networks. The Turner, G. R., & Levine, B. (2008). Augmented neural
Anatomical Record (Part B: New Anatomy), 281B, activity during executive control processing following
2136. doi:10.1002/ar.b.20035 diffuse axonal injury. Neurology, 71, 812818.
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000325640.18235.1c
as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual Turner, G. R., McIntosh, A. R., & Levine, B. (2011).
and Motor Skills, 8, 271276. doi:10.2466/ Prefrontal compensatory engagement in TBI is due to
PMS.8.7.271-276 altered functional engagement of existing networks
Ros, M., Periez, J. A., & Muoz-Cspedes, J. M. and not functional reorganization. Frontiers in
(2004). Attentional control and slowness of informa- Systems Neuroscience, 5(9). doi:10.3389/
tion processing after severe traumatic brain injury. fnsys.2011.00009
Brain Injury, 18(3), 257272. doi:10.1080/ Vallat-Azouvi, C., Weber, T., Legrand, L., & Azouvi, P.
02699050310001617442 (2007). Working memory after severe traumatic
Ruff, R. M., & Allen, C. C. (1995). Ruff 2 & 7 Selective brain injury. Journal of the International
Attention Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Neuropsychological Society, 13, 770780.
Assessment Resources. doi:10.1017/0S1355617707070993
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and Veltman, J. C., Brouwer, W. H., van Zomeren, A. H., &
automatic human information processing: I. van Wolffelaar, P. C. (1996). Central executive
Detection, search and attention. Psychological aspects of attention in subacute severe and very
Review, 84(1), 166. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1 severe closed head injury patients: Planning,
PROCESSING SPEED AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL AFTER TBI 1035

inhibition, flexibility, and divided attention. 575623). Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer,
Neuropsychology, 10(3), 357367. doi:10.1037/0894- Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
4105.10.3.357 Willmott, C., Ponsford, J., Hocking, C., & Schnberger,
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale M. (2009). Factors contributing to attentional impair-
Fourth Edition Australian and New Zealand Language ments after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology,
Adapted Edition: Sydney: Pearson Education. 23(4), 424432. doi:10.1037/a0015058
Whyte, J., Ponsford, J., Watanabe, T., & Hart, T. Ziino, C., & Ponsford, J. (2006). Selective attention def-
(2010). Traumatic brain injury. In W. R. icits and subjective fatigue following traumatic brain
Frontera (Ed.), Delisas physical medicine and injury. Neuropsychology, 20(3), 383390. doi:10.1037/
rehabilitation: Principles and practice (5th ed., pp. 0894-4105.20.3.383
Downloaded by [Universitat Oberta de Catalunya] at 04:43 30 October 2017

You might also like