You are on page 1of 15

The Impact of In-Store Greenery on

Customers
Malaika Brengman
Vrije Universiteit Brussel

Kim Willems
Vrije Universiteit Brussel and Hasselt University

Yannick Joye
University of Leuven

ABSTRACT

The incorporation of greenery in retail areas has scarcely been explored in environmental
psychology studies. In a 2 2 experimental design, the effect of in-store vegetation on consumer
emotions and responses toward the store was studied, considering the moderating role of the
information rate of the retail setting. While introducing foliage in the store environment did not
evoke feelings of excitement, it was found to elicit pleasure and to reduce stress in a complex store
interior. Given the impact of pleasure and stress on consumers approach/avoidance responses, these
findings support the potential of integrating greenery, particularly in spatially dense stores.
C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

It is widely accepted in psychology that the first level of ing environments to produce specific emotional effects
response to any environment is affective (emotional) in in the buyer that enhance his purchase probability
nature (Ittelson, 1973). Empirical research has demon- and indicated that this would become an important
strated that shopping environments can also evoke marketing tool for retailers. The more recent finding
emotional responses in consumers (Machleit & Eroglu, that almost two-thirds of all purchase decisions are
2000) and that these emotions, in turn, influence shop- made in the store (Point-of-Purchase Advertising In-
ping behaviors and outcomes (Darden & Babin, 1994; stitute (POPAI), 1998) has turned the attention of
Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Sherman, Mathur, & Smith, practitioners and marketing researchers to the point
1997). A neglected emotions-evoking element in the of purchase and store atmospherics. There is now a
shopping environment appears to be in-store greenery. growing recognition that store interiors and exteriors
Although vegetation has been demonstrated to have can be designed to create specific feelings in shoppers
positive and stress-mitigating influences in humans that can have an important cueing or reinforcing ef-
(among others, Bringslimark, Hartig, & Patil, 2009; fect on purchase (Newman & Foxall, 2003; Shapiro
Ulrich, 1993; Van Den Berg, 2009), it is unclear how and Associates, 2005). In the last two decades, there
it would affect in-store emotions and behaviors. While have been many studies concerning store atmospherics
many environmental psychology studies have inves- (Babin & Attaway, 2000; Fowler, Wesley, & Vazquez,
tigated retail settings and green environments sepa- 2007; McGoldrick & Pieros, 1998; Michon, Chebat, &
rately, the incorporation of greenery in retail areas has Turley, 2005; Sherman, Mathur, & Smith, 1997), pre-
barely been explored. Therefore, the objective of this dominantly based upon environmental psychology and
study is to determine the effect of introducing foliage specifically on the Mehrabian and Russell stimulus-
into the store environment. The role of store complex- organism-response (SOR) model (1974a), which was
ity will be considered as a potential moderating factor. first applied to the study of store atmosphere by Dono-
van and Rossiter (1982). In this context, it has been
proposed that approach/avoidance behaviors by cus-
STORE ATMOSPHERICS tomers are largely determined by individual internal
(cognitive, emotional, and physiological) responses to
Kotler (1973, p. 50) introduced the term atmospher- the store environment. Especially the pleasure and
ics to denote the conscious designing of [ . . . ] buy- arousal dimensions have been shown to be useful in

Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 29(11): 807821 (November 2012)


View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar

C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/mar.20566

807
determining shopper behavior. As such, atmospheric & Pruyn, 2008), office environments (cf. Kweon, Ulrich,
variables have been found to influence a wide variety Walker, & Tassinary, 2008), roadside views (cf. Cack-
of consumer evaluations and behaviors (Turley & Mil- owski & Nasar, 2003), school settings (cf. Han, 2009),
liman, 2000). and home environments (cf. Hartig & Fransson, 2009;
While some authors have focused on overall per- Kaplan, 2001).
ceptions of the general store interior (e.g. Akhter, An- By contrast, only very few inquiries seem to have
drews, & Durvasula, 1994; Donovan, Rossiter, Mar- focused on the effects of greenery in commercial set-
coolyn, & Nesdale, 1994; Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; tings. Noteworthy in this context are the findings by
Grossbart, Hampton, Rammohan, & Lapidus, 1990), Kathleen Wolf (2003, 2004a, 2004b, 2005, 2008), who
others have investigated the effects of specific in-store carried out a multistudy research program, focusing
stimuli. Hence, some store interior variables such specifically on the impact of urban vegetation and trees
as music and scent have received ample attention, in streetscapes. She found that retail environments
whereas others have not received the attention they were more pleasing when there were trees in the streets
probably deserve (Turley & Milliman, 2000). With re- as compared to when the streets were treeless. Judg-
gard more specifically to point-of-purchase and dec- ments on retailers and merchandize appeared to be
oration variables (cf. the classification of Berman & more positive in streetscapes with urban greenery. This
Evans, 1995), attention has focused merely on prod- also seemed to be the case for inferences about prod-
uct displays and point-of-purchase information as well uct value, product quality, and merchant responsive-
as promotional signs, while neglecting decorative ele- ness, leading to enhanced expectations of shopping ex-
ments, such as artwork and wall decorations (Turley periences in vegetated commercial settings. Also, pa-
& Milliman, 2000). The impact in particular of one tronage intentions appeared higher in commercial set-
specific decorative element, that is, in-store greenery, tings with trees as compared to settings without trees.
which is the main focus of the current paper, strangely Although positive price increments were associated
appears to have been entirely ignored by academic with the presence of trees (+9% for small cities and
researchers. +12% for large cities), respondents claimed they would
travel farther to visit a business district with trees.
They also reported to be willing to stay longer once
there.
THE IMPACT OF GREENERY IN THE Experimentally testing the effects of, for example,
STORE ENVIRONMENT plants on consumer behavior in a shopping mall, Bu-
ber, Ruso, and Gadner (2006) found that consumers
The intuition that nature has positive and healing influ- were more inclined to enter the mall when it featured
ences on humans has been around since time immemo- vegetation. Moreover, the presence of greenery trans-
rial. These last few decades there has been a steady lated into higher exploration rates (e.g. more detailed
increase of environmental psychology research into the inquiries about offers). Increases in physical contact
positive impact of nature and greenery on human atti- with products and with other people in the shopping
tudes, moods, and behavior in different settings. For ex- mall were also apparent when plants were present.
ample, different studies show that humans prefer veg- These results corroborate and extend environmental
etated settings over artifact-dominated environments psychology results about greenery from residential and
and that greenery can reduce stress in humans. These other settings to retail environments.
effects are frequently explained as a remnant of a While Joye, Willems, Brengman, and Wolf (2010) in-
shared evolutionary history, during which human be- troduced the notion of Biophilic Store Design to denote
ings were deeply dependent on nature for their subsis- the use of (references to) nature in the design of retail
tence (e.g. Heerwagen & Orians, 1993; Ulrich, 1993). environments in order to tap into the purported ben-
According to this view, humans have evolved positive eficial effects of greenery, no empirical studies seem
affective responses toward green elements and set- so far to have investigated the impact of in-store
tings, such as [ . . . ] flowers, ripe fruits, savanna (open foliage.
forests that give easy visual access), growth and leaf Essentially two main findings from the environmen-
patterns of healthy savanna trees, [and] closed forest tal psychology literature studying the beneficial effects
canopy (shelter), [ . . . ] (Thornhill, 1998, p. 562). Hard- of greenery are of interest for the current paper (see also
wired positive affective responses to these elements Joye et al., 2010). The first is that nature consistently
and conditions conveyed an adaptive benefit to our an- leads to preference/liking reactions in humans. Differ-
cestors because they could offer them resource and ent studies have confirmed that scenes featuring green-
refuge opportunities. Colarelli and Dettmann (2003) ery are preferred over nonnatural, artifact-dominated
provide an overview of how these evolved preferences settings, even if the latter are well tended (see Ulrich,
are already commonly being used by marketers in ad- 1993, for a review). Based on these findings, it can be
vertising practice. expected that stores with in-store vegetation will be
As to research on the impact of vegetation on hu- perceived as more pleasurable compared to stores with-
man behavior, the literature provides a wide range of out vegetation, which results in the formulation of the
study contexts, such as hospitals (cf. Dijkstra, Pieterse, following hypothesis:

808 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
H1: The presence of in-store vegetation leads to
increased feelings of pleasure in consumers.

A second finding is that nature has a stress-reducing


or restorative potential for human individuals. For
example, in a widely cited experimental study, Ulrich
et al. (1991) found that after exposure to a stressor, in-
dividuals recovered faster and more deeply from stress
when they were exposed to a natural/green setting
than when they had viewed an urban scene (Ulrich
et al., 1991; see also Cackowski & Nasar, 2003; Dijk-
stra, Pieterse, & Pruyn, 2008; Ulrich, 1984). The stress-
reducing value of greenery has been found to occur in
many types of settings and during different activities. Figure 1. A two-dimensional view of arousal.
As shopping is frequently perceived as a stressful ac-
tivity (see further), it can be expected that the pres-
ence of in-store vegetation will lead to reduced feelings
of stress in consumers, which leads to the following tense. Also the two-dimensional circumplex models,
hypothesis: which capture the underlying structure of emotions by
means of an evaluation and activation dimension
H2a: The presence of in-store vegetation leads to (cf. Mano, 1999; Russell & Lemay, 2000) appear to fit
reduced feelings of stress in consumers. Thayers (1989) two-dimensional view of energetic and
tense arousal when viewed as 45 rotations of one
another (Yik, Russell, & Feldman Barrett, 1999). In
accordance with Wundt (1896), Schimmack and Grob
AROUSAL IN THE STORE ENVIRONMENT: (2000) provide support for a three-dimensional model,
A TWO-DIMENSIONAL PERSPECTIVE which recognizes a separate pleasure-displeasure
dimension, alongside the energetic and tense
In a retailing context, the arousal construct has been arousal dimensions (see also Schimmack & Reisenzein,
shown to be rather ambiguous (e.g. Donovan, Rossiter, 2002).
Marcoolyn, & Nesdale, 1994; Groppel-Klein & Baun, Considering a two-dimensional view of arousal, such
2001; Van Kenhove & Desrumaux, 1997). Claiming that as the one developed by Walters, Apter, and Svebak
excitement represents a pervasively positive reaction (1982), it may be expected that in-store vegetation
in a retail setting, Babin and Attaway (2000) see no will affect the two dimensions differently. Although
reason to include a separate arousal dimension in their the stress reducing properties of greenery have already
study. Also Groppel-Klein (1998) argues that arousal been extensively demonstrated (for noncommercial en-
and pleasure seem to converge in a retail setting, as vironments), in-store vegetation as a decorative ele-
positive activation appears to be implicitly registered ment can also be expected to cause some excitement
in the arousal construct. Nevertheless, empirical stud- in shoppers as it brings some variety to the store. Es-
ies by Bost (1987) and Groppel (1991) reveal that con- pecially in hedonic shopping situations, consumers are
sumers in a retail setting can also experience a pleasant expected to be in a more paratelic or arousal seeking
state of low arousal (i.e. relaxation). Too much arousal, state (Kaltcheva & Weitz, 2006). This idea is summa-
also at the point of sale, can therefore be experienced rized in the following hypothesis:
as hectic and unpleasant (i.e. tension). For this rea-
son, Groppel-Klein (1998) recommends not to neglect H3a: The presence of in-store vegetation leads
the emotional dimension of relaxation experienced to increased feelings of excitement in con-
in the shopping environment. In accordance with this sumers.
suggestion, a "two-dimensional" view of arousal has
been rationalized by several researchers (Schimmack
& Reisenzein, 2002; Thayer, 1986; Walters, Apter, &
Svebak, 1982; see Figure 1), and it has been demon- THE MODERATING ROLE OF THE
strated that the arousal construct consists of two INFORMATION RATE OF THE
separate uncorrelated arousal dimensions (Brengman ENVIRONMENT
& Geuens, 2004; Gorn, Chattopadhyay, Yi, & Dahl,
1997): one with a positive hedonic tone (i.e. boredom In line with Groppel-Kleins assertion that an op-
excitement) and a second with a negative valence (i.e. timized store-interior presents an interaction of acti-
relaxationtension). This conceptualization is in line vating and de-activating stimuli (1998, p. 309), the
with the system of emotions proposed by Wundt (1896), impact of vegetation in a store can be expected to
which actually incorporates three bipolar dimensions: be moderated by the amount of arousal evoked by
unpleasantpleasant, quietexcited, and relaxed the store environment. Therefore, the stress-reducing

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 809


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
properties of in-store vegetation are assumed to be THE IMPACT OF IN-STORE GREENERY
more prevalent in a more arousing store environ- ON SHOPPING BEHAVIOR
ment, while the excitement evoking properties are ex-
pected to be more pronounced in less arousing store This study relies on a widely accepted environmental
environments. psychology model, namely the SOR model, as put for-
Given that a direct link is conceived between the in- ward by Mehrabian and Russel (Mehrabian and Rus-
formation rate of an environment and its arousability sel 1974a). Ever since this model has been introduced
(see Donovan & Rossiter, 1982), a moderating effect can to the context of store atmospherics by Donovan and
be expected from the information rate or complexity Rossiter (1982), numerous retail and consumer psychol-
of the store on the impact of vegetation in that store. ogy researchers have applied it in empirical studies on
Based on information theory, Mehrabian and Russell the effects of store environmental cues (i.e. the stim-
(1974b) characterize the various stimuli of the envi- uli) on consumer emotions (i.e. the organism), and
ronment in terms of the information rate or load of subsequent consumer behavior (i.e. the responses;
the environment, which they define as the quantity of cf. Donovan, Rossiter, Marcoolyn, & Nesdale, 1994;
information drawn from, or perceived in, the environ- Kaltcheva & Weitz, 2006; Turley & Milliman, 2000).
ment per unit of time, measured by its degree of nov- In accordance to this conceptual model, it is expected
elty (i.e. newness, unexpectedness) and complexity that on the one hand, feelings of pleasure as well
(i.e. the number of elements or features and the extent as excitement evoked by the store environment will
of motion or change in the environment). Accordingly, lead to approach behaviors" while they will have a
the more varied, complex, novel, surprising, and lively negative impact on "avoidance behaviors. Feelings of
the environment, the higher its information rate will stress on the other hand, will lead to avoidance be-
be. In the same vein, Bitner (1992) makes a distinction haviors and will negatively affect approach behav-
between lean and elaborate environments, based on iors. The final hypotheses in this SOR-based study are
the physical complexity of the servicescape (i.e. the consequently
physical surroundings of the place where a service is
delivered or consumed). In this context, Bitner (1992)
H4a: Feelings of pleasure induced by the store en-
describes lean environments as very simple, with few
vironment have a positive impact on con-
elements, few spaces, and few forms and elaborate
environments as very complicated with many elements sumer approach responses toward the store.
and many forms. The notion of spatial density has
also been used to denote the number and configuration H4b: Feelings of pleasure induced by the store en-
of spatial elements such as merchandise and fixtures in vironment have a negative impact on con-
a retail environment, (e.g. Machleit, Kelaris, & Eroglu, sumer avoidance responses toward the store.
1994; Van Rompay, Galetzka, Pruyn, & Moreno Garcia,
2008). H5a: Feelings of excitement induced by the store
Incorporating greenery in the store interior can environment have a positive impact on con-
therefore be expected to have a stronger soothing
sumer approach responses toward the store.
or calming effect in more arousing complex stores,
whereas a stronger exciting effect can be anticipated
in lean or simple stores. Accordingly, the two previ- H5b: Feelings of excitement induced by the store
ous hypotheses can be refined environment have a negative impact on con-
sumer avoidance responses toward the store.
H2a: The presence of in-store vegetation leads to
reduced feelings of stress in consumers. H6a: Feelings of stress induced by the store en-
vironment have a negative impact on con-
H2b: The reduction in stress evoked by in-store sumer approach responses toward the store.
greenery is more pronounced in a complex
store environment than in a simple store en- H6b: Feelings of stress induced by the store envi-
vironment. ronment have a positive impact on consumer
avoidance responses toward the store.
H3a: The presence of in-store vegetation leads
to increased feelings of excitement in con-
sumers.
RESEARCH OBJECTIVES

The purpose of this study is to explore the im-


H3b: The increase in excitement evoked by in-
pact of introducing greenery in the store environment
store greenery is more pronounced in a sim- on consumer emotions and on their subsequent ap-
ple store environment than in a complex proach/avoidance behaviors, according to Mehrabian
store environment. and Russels environmental psychological SOR model

810 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Figure 2. Research hypotheses.

(1974a). Considering the potential moderating role of tic differential scale items: simplecomplex, sparse
the spatial density or complexity of the store en- dense, patternedrandom, clearconfusing, plain
vironment, this study aims to ascertain how in-store ambiguous, and uncrowdedcrowded (adapted from
vegetation affects the shoppers feelings of pleasure, ex- Mehrabian & Russell, 1974b; range: 3 to +3; Cron-
citement, as well as stress and how these evoked feel- bachs = 0.86). An independent samples t-test demon-
ings engender further responses toward the store. An strates that the information rate in the "complex"
overview of the studys research hypotheses is provided store environment was significantly higher than in the
in Figure 2. "lean" store environment (0.992complex vs. 0.444lean ,
p < 0.001). While the integration of in-store greenery
appears to increase the information rate of the envi-
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY ronment somewhat, an ANOVA with post-hoc Scheffe-
test, comparing the information rate between all con-
ditions, does not indicate any significant differences
Experimental Design between the environments with or without vegeta-
tion: the information rate of the complex store envi-
A 2 2 factorial experimental design was set up to test
ronment remains significantly higher (1.06with vegetation
these hypotheses, within a fashion retail context. Ma-
and 0.93without vegetation ) than the information rate of
nipulations included two conditions of store environ-
the lean store environment (0.33with vegetation and
mental complexity (lean vs. complex), with and with-
0.57without vegetation ). An independent samples t-test also
out in-store vegetation (see Figure 3). Two separate
does not reveal any significant difference in informa-
pretests of the stimuli confirmed the adequateness of
tion rate between store environments with or with-
the manipulations (reported underneath). The exper-
out vegetation (0.35with vegetation vs. 0.25without vegetation ,
iment that was subsequently conducted on the basis
p = 0.764).
of these stimuli was monadic in nature (i.e. each re-
According to OKeefe (2003), there is no need to check
spondent evaluated only one store). Participants were
the manipulation of inserting "greenery in the store envi-
exposed to a picture of the store and were asked to
ronment," because whether respondents noted the pres-
imagine shopping in that store. This is a procedure
ence of vegetation or not, potential effects of integrating
that has been shown to accurately simulate reactions
greenery may occur unconsciously, without consumers
toward real environments (Bateson & Hui, 1992). A
even noticing its presence. Still, in the pretest respon-
meta-analysis by Stamps (1990) found a 0.86 correla-
dents were asked to give their opinion with respect to
tion between environmental preferences obtained by
the greenery present in the photographs of the store en-
photographs and preferences obtained by exposures to
vironments. The response categories concerned (1) "If
real environments.
there were plants, I didnt notice them at all," (2) "If
there were plants, I didnt notice them," (3) "If there
were plants, they are in harmony with the environ-
Manipulation Checks
ment," (4) "Plants were quite prominently present,"
In order to check whether the experimental manipu- and (5) "Plants were very prominently present." An
lations were successful, an online pretest of the four independent samples t-test indicates that vegetation
photographic stimuli was conducted in a between- was noted more in environments with versus with-
subjects design using a convenience sample of 81 re- out vegetation (1.94with vegetation vs. 1.10without vegetation ,
spondents. The average respondent age was 34.17 years p < 0.001). Still the pre-test findings also show that
old (range: 2066) and 39.5% of the participants were even in the condition with vegetation, 52.8% of respon-
male. dents indicated not to have noticed the plants at all. An
The "information rate of the store environ- ANOVA with post-hoc Scheffe test reveals that plants
ment" was measured by means of six seman- appeared somewhat more prominently present in the

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 811


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
lean/green condition (2.41) as compared to in the com- married consumers. With respect to age, only respon-
plex/green (1.53) or no-green conditions (1.14complex store dents aged 18+ are included in this study. The proce-
and 1.05lean store ). As the plants used in both the lean dure of assigning one of the four photos to each par-
as the complex store environment conditions are iden- ticipant was randomized. A detailed comparison be-
tical, namely (1) a Spider Plant (i.e. Chlorophytum Co- tween the different subsamples reveals an even distri-
mosum), (2) a Lucky Bamboo (Dracaena Sanderiana), bution of socio-demographic characteristics over the dif-
(3) a Bamboo Palm Tree (Chamaedorea Seifrizii), and ferent conditions (see the Chi-square tests reported in
(4) a Ficus Benjamina "Samantha," the only difference Table 1).
was the way in which they were arranged within the
store setting. Consequently, no differences are expected
in perceived attractiveness between the different con- Measures
ditions. Still, this is put to a formal test during an-
other manipulation check. Applying a within-subject In order to measure the emotions evoked by the
research design, respondents were asked to rate the store environment, participants were asked how the
plants in the photographs in both vegetation settings fashion store depicted in the photo made them feel.
on a 5-point Likert scale in terms of their attractive- These feelings were captured by means of 7-point Lik-
ness. The sequence in which both photographs were ert scaled items, measuring pleasure, stress, and
presented was randomized. For this second manipula- excitement. Adjectives were selected on the basis
tion check, another convenience sample was used con- of previous operationalizations of the three dimen-
sisting of 40 respondents, among which 35% males, sions of core affect (cf. Schimmack & Grob, 2000;
age range 1861, average age = 35.67. Participants Schimmack & Reisenzein, 2002). Pleasure was mea-
were explicitly asked to ignore the surrounding store sured by means of five items: agreeable, unpleas-
environment and to focus only on the plants. The at- ant (reversed), enjoyable, depressed (reversed), and
tractiveness of the plants in both photographs appears good (Cronbachs = 0.866). Stress was measured
significantly positively correlated (paired samples by means of three items: nervous, stressed, and
correlation r = 0.689, p < 0.001). The results of a irritated ( = 0.869). Excitement was measured
paired samples t-test indicate that the mean attrac- by three items: excited, lively, and stimulated
tiveness rating of the plants in the simple store en- ( = 0.787).
vironment (2.53; SD = 0.751) and the attractiveness Although significant, the correlation between the
of the plants in the complex store environment (2.42; two subdimensions of arousal is confirmed to be very
SD = 0.747) do not differ significantly (t (39) = 1.071, low (Pearsons r = 0.126). While Excitement proves
p = 0.291). to be positively related to Pleasure (r = 0.505), a clear
negative correlation between Pleasure and Stress
is apparent (r = 0.740). Mean scores are calculated
Participants for each of these emotion states for use in further
analyses.
The data were electronically gathered between Novem- Subsequently, approach and avoidance responses
ber 2007 and January 2008. The sample was drawn were measured using the Donovan and Rossiters
randomly from a large online consumer panel in the (1982) measurement scale that was developed specif-
Netherlands (comprising over 100,000 people who have ically for application in a retail context. Approach
registered to participate in market research studies). was measured by means of the five 7-point Likert-
An email invitation was sent to a random selection of scale items: I like this store environment, I would
these panel members requesting them to participate in enjoy shopping in this store, I would spend a lot of
this online study. A stratified sampling procedure was time browsing in this store, In this store I might end
used in order to reach certain quota (in terms of age, up spending more money than I originally set out to
gender, and region) and was continued until 5000 com- spend, and In this store, I would feel friendly and
plete cases were obtained. After data cleaning, 4293 talkative to a stranger who happens to be near me ( =
participants were retained of which 1838 (42.8%) men 0.930). Avoidance intentions were captured by means
and 2455 (57.2%) women. The average age was 42.2 of three items: I would avoid ever having to return to
years old (range 1869); 55.7% of the subjects in the this store, I would want to avoid looking around or
sample had a lower education level, while 44.3% had exploring this environment, and In this store I might
enjoyed higher education. A summarizing overview of try to avoid other people, and avoid to talk to them (
the total sample as well as the four subsamples in terms = 0.869).
of sociodemographic characteristics is provided in Although the correlation between both behavioral
Table 1. Also some corresponding key numbers pertain- subconstructs may appear quite high (Pearsons r =
ing to the total Dutch population are provided (Statis- 0.727), they are considered separately as they have
tics Netherlands online, 2010). As can be observed from been conceptualized to constitute two distinct under-
these data the total sample is quite representative for lying dimensions of the approach/avoidance construct,
the Dutch population. There are slightly more females each evolving from opposing learning histories (Foxall,
in the sample and somewhat more highly educated and 1997; Foxall & Greenley, 2000). Again, for each of these

812 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Table 1. Sociodemographical Sample Characteristics.
Condition 1 Condition 2 Condition 3 Condition 4
Without Without With With
Vegetation/ Vegetation/ Vegetation/ Vegetation/ Total Population NLa

Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar


Simple Complex Simple Complex Sample (N2010 =
(n = 1079) (n = 1027) (n = 1080) (n = 1107) (n = 4293) 16,577,612)

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS


Abs. Rel. (%) Abs. Rel. (%) Abs. Rel. (%) Abs. Rel. (%) 2 test Abs. Rel. (%) Abs. Rel. (%)

Sex
Male 437 40.5 454 44.2 453 41.9 494 44.6 2 1838 42.8 8,205,443 49.5
Female 642 59.5 573 55.8 627 58.1 613 55.4 0.173 2455 57.2 8,372,169 50.5
Age
Min./Max 18 69 18 69 18 69 18 69 18 69 0 >99
Mean/SD 41.4 13.1 42.3 12.8 42.3 13.4 42.8 13.0 42.2 13.1 39,2 -
< 20 43 4.0 41 4.0 34 3.1 30 2.7 2 148 3.4 3,927,975 23.7
2040 493 45.7 420 40.9 480 44.4 458 41.4 0.181 1851 43.1 4,195,201 25.3
4065 512 47.5 540 52.6 536 49.6 588 53.1 2176 50.7 5,916,067 35.7
6580 31 2.9 26 2.5 30 2.8 31 2.8 118 2.7 1,890,472 11.4
>80 - - - - - - - - - - 647,890 3.9
Educational level
Lower 599 55.5 602 58.6 604 55.9 587 53.0 2 2392 55.7 6,905,000 63.4
Higher 480 44.5 425 41.4 476 43.0 520 47.0 0.079 1901 44.3 3,987,000 36.6
Civil status
Married 694 64.3 676 65.8 684 63.3 707 63.9 2 2761 64.3 9,603,148 58.3
Other 385 35.7 351 34.2 396 36.7 400 36.1 0.667 1532 35.7 6,882,639 41.7
a
Based on Statistics Netherlands online.
Abs. = absolute numbers; Rel. = in relative terms.

813
Figure 3. Manipulations of store complexity and in-store vegetation.

subconstructs, mean scores are calculated, to be used conducted with in-store vegetation and store com-
in further analyses. plexity as fixed factors and evoked feelings of plea-
sure, stress, and excitement as dependent vari-
ables. In order to control for any confounding effects of
ANALYSES AND RESULTS the sociodemographic variables age and gender, these
were added to the analysis as covariates. Although the
So as to be able to formulate an answer to the research multivariate tests support the main effects of in-store
questions in this study, the analyses will be discussed vegetation (Wilks Lambda = 0.997, F (1, 4289) = 3.89,
in two stages. First, the hypotheses with regard to the p = 0.009) and complexity (Wilks Lambda = 0.971,
impact of in-store greenery on perceived pleasure, F (1, 4289) = 42.41, p < 0.001), the overall interac-
stress, and excitement in both lean and complex tion effect between the two factors appears to be only
store environments will be tested. Next, the mediat- marginally significant (Wilks Lambda = 0.998, F (1,
ing effects of these consumer emotions on subsequent 4289) = 2.17, p = 0.090). Nevertheless, a closer look
approach and avoidance responses will be addressed. at the univariate analyses provides clear evidence of
some pronounced interaction effects, as will be illus-
The Impact of In-Store Greenery on trated further. Both covariates, age (Wilks Lambda
Consumer Emotions, Considering the = 0.995, F (1, 4289) = 6.95, p < 0.001) as well as
gender (Wilks Lambda = 0.984, F (1, 4289) = 23.90,
Moderating Role of Store Complexity
p < 0.001), also appear to have a significant impact
In order to test Hypotheses H1 through H3, a two-way on the emotional reactions toward the store environ-
multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was ments.

814 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Figure 4. The moderating role of store complexity on Figure 5. The moderating role of store complexity on stress
pleasure evoked by in-store greenery. reduction by in-store greenery.

Pleasure Evoked by In-Store Greenery. Regarding to alleviate stress only in a complex retail setting (per-
the impact of in-store greenery on evoked pleasure (H1), ceived stress = 3.51(without vegetation) vs. 3.35(with vegetation) , t
a notable interaction effect of store complexity that was (2107.785) = 2.84, p = 0.005), which is in line with the
not anticipated can be observed (F (1, 4289) = 6.41, p = present studys hypotheses.
0.011).
Figure 4 clearly illustrates that adding greenery in
Excitement Evoked by in-Store Greenery. Although
the store interior can evoke higher feelings of pleasure,
it was tentatively hypothesized that the presence of
but that this effect is distinctly more pronounced in
in-store vegetation would lead to increased feelings of
complex store environments. As a matter of fact, an in-
excitement in consumers (H3a) and that this increase
dependent samples t-test reveals that the introduction
would be more pronounced in simple store environ-
of vegetation does not generate a significant effect in
ments than in complex retail settings (H3b), the find-
the lean store environment (t (2152.547) = 0.50, p =
ings do not reveal any effect of in-store greenery on
0.617). In a complex retail setting, on the other hand,
elicited excitement. A possible explanation is that veg-
the positive effect of foliage on perceived pleasure is
etation, basically plants, are not enough to surprise a
clearly evident (perceived pleasure = 3.86(without vegetation)
shopper. Not only may consumers be too familiar with
vs. 4.05(with vegetation) , t (2115.773) = 3.98; p < 0.001).
such natural elements to feel excited by them, previous
So the integration of greenery in the store environment
research also indicated that such elements are actually
apparently does not result in an equally beneficial effect
experienced as only moderately fascinating (as opposed
on the consumers perceived pleasure in complex versus
to, say, watching television; see Herzog, Black, Foun-
lean retail settings. This implies that the first hypothe-
taine, & Knotts, 1997).
sis is only partially supported, that is, in complex store
interior conditions only. As is observable, however, the
lean store interior was generally rated to be more
pleasurable than the complex store environment (F
(1, 4289) = 51.57, p < 0.001), which may have provided
too little room for improvement by integrating in-store
vegetation.

Stress Alleviation by In-Store Greenery. With re-


gard to the stress-reducing properties of in-store vege-
tation, a moderating effect of store complexity was ex-
pected (H2a and H2b), which is confirmed by the studys
findings (F (1, 4289) = 4.28, p = 0.039). Figure 5 clearly
illustrates that adding greenery in the store interior
can alleviate feelings of stress and that this effect is
distinctly more pronounced in complex store interiors.
As a matter of fact, an independent samples t-test re-
veals that the introduction of vegetation does not gener-
ate a significant effect in the lean store environment (t Figure 6. The moderating role of store complexity on
(2155.445) = 0.10, p = 0.922). Foliage is found to be able approach responses by in-store greenery.

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 815


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
tions, is only significant in complex stores (approach
= 3.05 (without vegetation) vs. 3.22 (with vegetation) , t (2132) =
3.06, p = 0.002). The effect of introducing vegetation
in a lean store environment appears not significant as
illustrated in Figure 6 (t (2153.694) = 0.56, p = 0.574).
Regarding the impact of in-store greenery on con-
sumers "Avoidance" responses toward the store, an
even more notable interaction effect with store com-
plexity can be discerned (F (1, 4289) = 6.24, p = 0.013),
besides a significant main effect of store complexity (F
(1, 4289) = 28.23, p < 0.001) and a marginally signif-
icant effect of in-store vegetation (F (1, 4289) = 3.04,
p = 0.082). Figure 7 illustrates that adding greenery
in the store interior can reduce consumers avoidance
of a complex store (avoidance = 3.97 (without vegetation) vs.
Figure 7. The moderating role of store complexity on 3.79 (with vegetation) , t (2132) = 2.30, p = 0.003). The effect
avoidance responses by in-store greenery. of introducing vegetation in a lean store environment
appears however not significant (t (2148.593) = 0.53,
p = 0.598).
The Impact of In-Store Greenery on
Shopping Behavior, Considering the
Moderating Role of Store Complexity The Mediating Role of Emotions Evoked in
the Store Environment
The second stage of this study aims to investigate
the mediating role of the different emotions evoked by According to the guidelines offered by Zhao, Lynch, and
greenery in the store environment (i.e. pleasure, stress, Chen (2010) and Hayes and Preacher (2011), a boot-
and excitement) on shoppers approach and avoidance strapping analysis (with 5000 bootstrapping samples
intentions (H4H6) and this again for both lean and and a 95% confidence level) is conducted to examine
complex store environments. Thus, both the direct mediation effects and to reveal the indirect as well as
and indirect effects of the integration of greenery in the direct effects of vegetation in the store environment
a store on shoppers approach and avoidance behaviors on shopping intentions. Individual tests are performed
will be examined. for the dependent variables Approach and Avoid-
ance. The variables Pleasure, Stress, and Excite-
ment are entered as possible mediators to the model
Direct Effects (using MEDIATE for SPSS), while the direct effects
of in-store vegetation and store complexity, as well
A MANCOVA analysis with approach and avoid- as the interaction effect vegetation complexity are
ance as dependent variables and in-store greenery examined. In order to control for any confounding ef-
and store complexity as fixed factors (again includ- fects of the sociodemographic variables age and gender,
ing age and gender as covariates) reveals significant these were again added to the analyses as covariates. A
main and interaction effects. Actually, this multivari- comprehensive overview of the findings is displayed in
ate test supports the overall interaction effect between Tables 2 and 3.
the factors (Wilks Lambda = 0.999, F (1, 4289) = 3.12, From this bootstrapping analysis, a similar picture
p = 0.044) as well as main effects of in-store vegeta- emerges concerning the direct effects. While no direct
tion (Wilks Lambda = 0.998, F (1, 4289) = 3.42, p = main effects of vegetation on approach or avoidance
0.033) and store complexity (Wilks lambda = 0.993, F intentions can be revealed, store complexity does ap-
(1, 4289) = 14.73, p < 0.001). pear to have a negative direct effect on approach (
With respect to in-store vegetations impact on = 0.244, p = <0.001), which can apparently be miti-
consumer Approach responses toward the store, a gated by adding greenery to the store environment (
marginally significant interaction effect with store com- = +0.137, p = 0.072). Store complexity also appears
plexity can be discerned (F (1, 4289) = 3.25, p = 0.071), to have a direct avoidance inducing effect ( = 0.331,
besides significant main effects of in-store vegetation p < 0.001), which can be reduced by adding in-store
(F (1, 4289) = 6.80, p = 0.009) and store complexity (F vegetation ( = 0.212, p = 0.013).
(1, 4289) = 21.18, p < 0.001). In line with the previ- Subsequently, it is examined whether this effect
ously formulated expectations, adding greenery to the is mediated by the consumer emotions evoked in
store interior appears to increase consumers approach the store environment. While no indirect main ef-
intentions toward the store. Store complexity on the fects of vegetation in the store environment can
other hand decreases consumers approach intentions. be revealed on approach or avoidance intentions ei-
As a matter of fact, a closer look at the results reveals ther, the results support the existence of indirect
that the positive effect of greenery on approach reac- effects of store complexity and the interaction

816 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Table 2. Direct and Indirect Effects on Approach Intentions.
Approach
Direct effects Indirect Effects Approach
R2 adj = 0.024 Pleasure Stress Excitement R2 adj = 0.667
Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p

Vegetation - n.s. - n.s. - n.s. - n.s. - n.s.


Complexity 0.244 <0.001 0.315 <0.001 +0.505 <0.001 - n.s. - n.s.
Vegetation Complexity +0.137 0.072 +0.166 0.011 0.158 0.039 - n.s. - n.s.
Gender 0.352 <0.001 0.274 <0.001 +0.229 <0.001 0.200 <0.001 0.079 0.001
Age +0.005 0.005 +0.004 0.006 0.007 <0.001 - n.s. +0.003 0.004
Pleasure +0.721 <0.001
Stress 0.066 <0.001
Excitement +0.297 <0.001

Approach Approach
Direct Effects Indirect Effects through
Pleasure Stress Excitement
Coefficient p Effect CIlow CIup Effect CIlow CIup Effect CIlow CIup

Vegetation - n.s. - 0.047 0.082 - 0.007 0.007 - 0.026 0.022


Complexity 0.244 <0.001 0.227 0.295 0.160 0.033 0.053 0.017 - 0.039 0.013
Vegetation Complexity +0.137 0.072 +0.120 0.026 0.213 +0.010 0.005 0.024 - 0.014 0.056

Table 3. Direct and Indirect Effects on Avoidance Intentions.


Avoidance
Direct Effects Indirect Effects Avoidance
R2 adj = 0.015 Pleasure Stress Excitement R2 adj = 0.621
Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p Coefficient p

Vegetation - n.s. - n.s. - n.s. - n.s. - n.s.


Complexity +0.331 <0.001 0.315 <0.001 +0.505 <0.001 - n.s. - n.s.
Vegetation Complexity 0.212 0.013 +0.166 0.011 0.158 0.039 - n.s. - n.s.
Gender +0.254 <0.001 0.275 <0.001 +0.229 <0.001 0.200 <0.001 - n.s.
Age 0.005 0.003 +0.004 0.006 0.007 <0.001 - n.s. - n.s.
Pleasure 0.663 <0.001
Stress +0.323 <0.001
Excitement 0.084 <0.001
Avoidance Avoidance
Direct Effects Indirect Effects through
Pleasure Stress Excitement
Coefficient p Effect CIlow CIup Effect CIlow CIup Effect CIlow CIup

Vegetation - n.s. - 0.076 0.043 - 0.035 0.032 - 0.007 0.008


Complexity +0.331 <0.001 +0.209 0.145 0.271 +0.166 0.128 0.210 - 0.004 0.012
Vegetation Complexity 0.212 0.013 0.110 0.195 0.022 0.052 0.103 0.002 - 0.017 0.004

between greenery and store complexity on approach while Stress appears to have the least explanatory
and avoidance intentions, through evoked pleasure power (0.066, p < 0.001).
and/or stress. With regard to Avoidance, the multiple regres-
As a matter of fact, the multiple regression analysis sion analysis reveals that 62.1% of the variance (ad-
demonstrates that 66.7% of the variance in Approach justed R2 ) is explained by the variance in the explana-
(adjusted R2 ) is explained by the variance in the ex- tory variables Pleasure, Stress, and Excitement,
planatory variables Pleasure, Stress, and Excite- which all appear to contribute significantly. While the
ment. Each of these emotional dimensions appears to standardized beta coefficients reveal that pleasure is
contribute significantly to approach intentions. The again the most important explanatory factor (0.663,
standardized beta coefficients reveal that pleasure p < 0.001), stress (0.323, p < 0.001) comes second,
is the most important explanatory factor (0.721, p < and excitement appears to contribute least (0.084,
0.001), followed by Excitement (0.297, p < 0.001), p < 0.001).

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 817


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
The bootstrap mediation tests (at = 0.05) clearly dense store interiors. In such store settings, vegetation
make the case that pleasure and stress" significantly is also found to directly affect consumers approach and
mediate the effects of store complexity" and vegeta- avoidance behaviors in a favorable way (positively resp.
tion complexity on approach as well as on avoid- negatively), providing further support for its strate-
ance intentions. According to Zhao, Lynch, and Chen gic role. This study also demonstrates that the posi-
(2010) this implies indirect-only mediation (cf. Baron tive impact of integrating greenery in complex store
and Kennys full mediation). Thus, store complex- environments on approach and avoidance intentions is
ity appears to have an indirect negative effect on ap- fully mediated by its impact on feelings of pleasure and
proach through reduced feelings of pleasure (0.227) stress.
and increased stress (0.033). The integration of in-
store greenery in complex store environments (i.e. veg-
etation complexity) turns out to have a positive effect LIMITATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR
on approach through evoked pleasure (+0.120) and
FURTHER RESEARCH
reduced stress" (+0.010). With regard to avoidance,
store complexity appears to have an indirect avoid- In this study, emotional responses to the store environ-
ance inducing effect through reduced feelings of plea- ment were measured by means of self-report question-
sure (+0.209) and increased stress (+0.166). The in- naires. As some authors point out, verbal scales may
tegration of in-store greenery in complex store environ- not be appropriate to capture arousal. Future studies
ments (i.e. vegetation complexity) turns out to di- should therefore also include other arousal indicators,
minish avoidance through evoked pleasure (0.110) such as the measurement of electrodermal activity (cf.
and reduced stress 0.052). Groppel-Klein & Baun, 2001).
Another limitation to this study concerns the ex-
ternal validity of the findings. Unfortunately, as a di-
DISCUSSION rect consequence of the main concern for internal valid-
ity, laboratory experiments are generally rather weak
Based on the findings of this study, it can be con- in generalizability. In particular, it would be valuable
cluded that introducing foliage in the store interior to replicate the present study based on a set of pho-
can elicit feelings of pleasure and reduce stress, even tographs, rather than just one, defining each condition.
though these effects could only be discerned in a com- Moreover, an extension of this study to field experi-
plex store interior. As a matter of fact, no significant ments is called for to determine the ecological valid-
effect of in-store greenery on evoked feelings of plea- ity and generalizability of this studys findings. In such
sure or stress could be revealed in the lean store en- field studies actual in-store behaviors can be observed
vironment condition. While it was expected that the (cf. Newman & Foxall, 2003).
stress-alleviating potential of in-store vegetation would As the experiment was conducted in a fashion re-
be reduced in lean store interiors (as they cause less tail context, this study should be replicated in other
arousal in general), it was not anticipated that it would retail contexts, such as grocery stores. It will be particu-
disappear completely. The fact that a significant effect larly interesting to investigate whether the findings ob-
of in-store greenery on evoked pleasure could not be tained in a hedonic shopping situation will also hold in a
discerned in the lean store condition either, may be more utilitarian shopping context. Because utilitarian
explained by the observation that the lean store inte- shopping is essentially functional and goal directed
rior was already rated as generally quite pleasurable, and also often experienced as a chore" (see Geuens,
which left little room for improvement. Contrary to the Brengman, & SJegers, 2003), it seems probable that
tentative expectations, no impact was found of in-store stress reduction will have even more pronounced ef-
greenery on feelings of excitement elicited by the store. fects and relevance during this type of shopping. On the
The findings of this study confirm that feelings of other hand, hedonistic shoppers may often show more
pleasure" evoked by the store environment are an im- appreciation for aesthetic attributes in shops, such as
portant determining factor for approach as well as in-store vegetation. While enduring shopping motiva-
avoidance responses toward the store. With regard tions are related to a persons personality and are rel-
to the two arousal dimensions, it was established that atively stable over time (Mooradian & Olver, 1996),
both of them significantly affect approach and avoid- situational shopping motivations are temporary and
ance behaviors. Nonetheless, a clearly divergent impact differ depending on a particular consumer situation
was identified: while feelings of excitement appear to such as time pressure, social surroundings, or task def-
provide a stronger drive for approach intentions, feel- inition (e.g. urgent vs. regular purchase; Van Kenhove,
ings of stress seem to trigger avoidance intentions De Wulf, & Van Waterschoot, 1999). In this regard, it
to a greater extent. could also be very interesting to manipulate the situa-
Given the impact of pleasure and stress on consumer tional motivation of the shopper (run vs. fun shopping
approach/avoidance responses, the findings of the cur- trip) to discover the different effects of introducing veg-
rent study support the potential of integrating green- etation in the store. Even the shoppers stress level be-
ery in retail store environments, especially in spatially fore entering the store could be manipulated by means

818 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
of different scenarios. It may also be a good idea to con- in-store moods (see also dAstous, 2000). In this respect,
sider the shoppers Optimum-Stimulation-Level in this Geuens, Brengman, and SJegers (2003, p. 246) found
respect. that when consumers were asked to imagine their ideal
Since a moderating role of the information rate of food store, they reported to want to shop in an envi-
the store environment regarding the impact of in-store ronment that takes away the stress instead of imposing
vegetation on arousal was expected, the spatial den- more stress . . . . It is obvious that retailers can benefit
sity of the store was manipulated. Clearly, the lean from minimizing stress episodes in customers or from
store in this study was found to be rather pleasurable providing them with "breathers" from such stress, for
already, leaving little room for improvement, for ex- instance by integrating in-store vegetation.
ample, by adding greenery into the store. It is reason- As the store environment has been shown to influ-
able to assume therefore that the impact of greenery ence not only customers, but also store employees (Bit-
on perceived pleasure will be more apparent in less ner, 1992), an even more pronounced beneficial impact
pleasant lean store interiors. In agreement with Van can be expected from the integration of vegetation in the
Rompay et al. (2008), the findings of the present study retail environment because of its stress-reducing effects
also request further research to determine the impact on the sales staff. Stress reduction among sales assis-
of spatial density on pleasure. tants and employees can lead to more positive moods,
Because various kinds of vegetation are appreci- which can translate into increased helpfulness and
ated differently (Lohr & Pearson-Mims, 2006), it can friendliness toward customers and into more job satis-
be assumed that they may produce different effects in faction, which are obviously important for retail service
shoppers. Further studies should investigate the effects quality (Vazquez, Rodriguez-Del Bosque, Diaz, & Ruiz,
of diverse types of foliage (e.g. leafy plants, conifers, 2001). Other possible benefits are fewer stress-related
palms, flowers). Less common, more dramatic plants or health problems affecting employees; reduced costs as-
flowers may actually evoke more excitement (carnivo- sociated with health problems (i.e. absenteeism); in-
rous plants for example). It would also be interesting creased productivity, etc.
to find out whether the same effects hold for artificial Considering that many potential benefits can be ex-
as compared to real vegetation. Moreover the impact of pected from the integration of vegetation in store in-
representations of nature in wall decorations and art- teriors, further research on this pertinent topic seems
work could also be examined. justified.
Finally, it can be worthwhile to investigate the ad-
verse effects of in-store greenery: in-store vegetation
can, for example, block out the view on marketing-
relevant information; it can reduce shopper circulation
REFERENCES
and so cause crowding; it can hinder easy access to cer-
tain products; it can be valued negatively if it is not Akhter, S. H., Andrews, J. C., & Durvasula, S. (1994). The in-
fluence of retail store environment on brand related judg-
well tended or if it does not fit the specific context.
ments. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 1, 67
76.
Aylott, R., & Mitchell V.-W. (1998). An exploratory study of
OVERALL CONCLUSIONS grocery shopping stressors. International Journal of Retail
and Distribution, 26, 362373.
Since shopping is often experienced as quite stressful Babin, B. J., & Attaway, J. S. (2000). Atmospheric affect as
(Fram & Ajami, 1994; Fram & Axelrod, 1990), the pos- a tool for creating value and gaining share of customer.
itive and stress-reducing effects that may be evoked by Journal of Business Research, 49, 9199.
green interventions could potentially be very valuable Babin, B. J., & Darden, W. R. (1996). Good and bad shop-
ping vibes: Spending and patronage satisfaction. Journal
in a retail context. The segment of shoppers entering
of Business Research, 35, 201206.
a store already in a negative mood state represents no
Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D.A. (1986). Moderator-mediator vari-
less than 10% of the total shopping population (Maxwell ables distinction in social psychological research: Concep-
& Kover, 2003). Moreover, once a customer enters the tual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of
store, there are numerous factors, both within and be- Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 11731182.
yond the control of retail management, which might Bateson, J. E. G., & Hui, M. K. (1992). The ecological validity
turn the act of shopping into a stressful and poten- of photographic slides and videotapes in simulating the ser-
tially irritating experience (dAstous, 2000; Aylott & vice setting. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 271280.
Mitchell, 1998). Shopping is often a goal-directed activ- Berman, B., & Evans, J. R. (1995). Retail management. Upper
ity (e.g. buying a certain good) that is constrained by Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bitner, M. J. (1992). Servicescapes: The impact of psychical
certain factors (e.g. limits on time or budget). When ob-
surroundings on customers and employees. Journal of Mar-
stacles are encountered during this process, this may
keting, 56, 5770.
cause stress (e.g. Machleit & Eroglu, 2000, p. 101), Bost, E. (1987). Ladenatmosphare und konsumentenverhal-
which in turn may lead to avoidance behavior toward ten. Heidelberg, Germany: Physica.
the particular retail context. Babin and Darden (1996) Brengman, M., & Geuens, M. (2004). The four dimensional
encourage retailers not only to try to create positive impact of color on shoppers emotions. In B. E. Kahn &
consumer emotions, but above all to prevent negative M. Frances (Eds.), Advances in consumer research, volume

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 819


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
31 (pp. 122128). Valdosta, GA: Association for Consumer pilot study using electrodermal activity as indicator. In M.
Research. C. Gilly & J. Meyers-Levy (Eds.), Advances in consumer
Bringslimark, T., Hartig, T., & Patil, G. G. (2009). The psycho- research, Vol. 28 (pp. 412419). Valdosta, GA: Association
logical benefits of indoor plants: A critical review of the ex- for Consumer Research.
perimental literature. Journal of Environmental Psychol- Grossbart, S. L., Hampton, R., Rammohan, B., & Lapidus, R. S.
ogy, 29, 422433. (1990). Environmental dispositions and customer response
Buber, R., Ruso, B., & Gadner, J. (2006). Evolutionares de- to store atmospherics. Journal of Business Research, 31,
sign von Verkaufsraumen Wasser, Pflanzen, Tiere und 225241.
Sichtschutz als verhaltenssteuernde Gestaltungselemente. Han, K.-T. (2009). Influence of limited visible leafy indoor
In P. Schnedlitz, R. Buber, T. Reutterer, A. Schuh, & C. plants on the psychology, behavior, and health of students
Teller (Eds.), Innovationen in marketing und handel (pp. at a junior high school in Taiwan. Environment and Behav-
361378). Vienna: Linde. ior, 41, 658692.
Cackowski, J. M., & Nasar, J. L. (2003). The restorative effects Hartig, T., & Fransson, U. (2009). Leisure home ownership,
of roadside vegetation. Implications for automobile driver access to nature, and health: A longitudinal study of urban
anger and frustration. Environment and Behavior, 35, 736 residents in Sweden. Environment and Planning, 41, 82
751. 96.
Colarelli, S. M., & Dettmann, J. R. (2003). Intuitive evolu- Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2011). Indirect and direct ef-
tionary perspectives in marketing practices. Psychology & fects of a multicategorical causal agent in statistical medi-
Marketing, 20, 837865. ation analysis. Working Paper, The Ohio State University
dAstous, A. (2000). Irritating aspects of the shopping environ- and Vanderbilt University, retrieved April 10, 2012, from
ment. Journal of Business Research, 49, 149156. http://www.afhayes.com/public/hp2011.pdf.
Darden, W. R., & Babin, B. J. (1994). Exploring the concept of Heerwagen, J. H., & Orians, G. H. (1993). Humans, habi-
affective quality: Expanding the concept of retail personal- tats, and aesthetics. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Eds.),
ity. Journal of Business Research, 29, 101110. The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 138172). Washington: Island
Dijkstra, K., Pieterse, M. E., & Pruyn, A. (2008). Stress- Press.
reducing effects of indoor plants in the built healthcare Herzog, T. R., Black, A. M., Fountaine, K. M., & Knotts, D.
environment: The mediating role of perceived attractive- J. (1997). Reflection and attentional recovery as distinctive
ness. Preventive Medicine, 47, 279283. benefits of restorative environments. Journal of Environ-
Donovan, R. J., & Rossiter, J. R. (1982). Store atmosphere: An mental Psychology, 17, 165170.
environmental psychology approach. Journal of Retailing, Ittelson, W. H. (1973). Environment perception and contem-
58, 3457. porary perceptual theory. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Envi-
Donovan, R. J., Rossister, J. R., Marcoolyn, G., & Nesdale, A. ronment and cognition (pp. 141154). New York: Seminar
(1994). Store atmosphere and purchasing behaviour. Jour- Press.
nal of Retailing, 70, 283294. Joye, Y., Willems, K., Brengman, M., & Wolf, K. (2010). The
Fowler, D. C., Wesley, S. C., & Vazquez, M. E. (2007). Sim- effects of urban retail greenery on consumer experience: Re-
patico in store retailing: How immigrant Hispanic emic in- viewing the evidence from a restorative perspective. Urban
terpret U.S. store atmospherics and interactions with sales Forestry & Urban Greening, 9, 5764.
associates. Journal of Business Research, 60, 5059. Kaltcheva, V. D., & Weitz, B. A. (2006). When should a retailer
Foxall, G. R. (1997). The emotional texture of consumer envi- create an exciting environment? Journal of Marketing, 70,
ronments: A systematic approach to atmospherics. Journal 107118.
of Economic Psychology, 18(5), 505523. Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: Psy-
Foxall G. R., & Greenley, G.E. (2000). Consumers emotional chological benefits. Environment and Behavior, 33, 507
responses to service environments. Journal of Business Re- 542.
search, 46, 149158. Kotler, P. (1973). Atmosphere as a marketing tool. Journal of
Fram, E., & Ajami, R. (1994). Globalization of markets Retailing, 49, 4864.
and shopping stress: Cross-country comparisons. Business Kweon, B.-S., Ulrich, R. S., Walker, V. D., & Tassinary, L.
Horizons, 37, 1723. G. (2008). Anger and stress: The role of landscape posters
Fram, E., & Axelrod, J. (1990). Distressed shopper. American in an office setting. Environment and Behavior, 40, 355
Demographics, 12, 4445. 381.
Geuens, M., Brengman, M., & SJegers, R. (2003). Food retail- Leo J. Shapiro & Associates (2005). Store atmospherics
ing, now and in the future. A consumer perspective. Journal provide competitive edge. Chain Store Age, 81, 74
of Retailing and Consumer Services, 10, 241251. 89.
Gorn, G. J., Chattopadhyay, A., Yi, T., & Dahl, D. W. (1997). Lohr, V. I., & Pearson-Mims, C. H. (2006). Responses to scenes
Effects of color as an executional cue in advertising: Theyre with spreading, rounded, and conical tree forms. Environ-
in the shade. Management Science, 43, 13871400. ment & Behavior, 38, 667688.
Groppel, A. (1991). Erlebnisstrategien im einzelhandel. Machleit, K. A., & Eroglu, S. A. (2000). Describing and mea-
Analyse der zielgruppen, der ladengestaltung und der suring emotional response to shopping experience. Journal
warenprasentation zur vermittlung von einkaufserlebnis- of Business Research, 49, 101111.
sen. Heidelberg: Physica. Machleit, K. A., Kelaris, J. J., & Eroglu, S. A. (1994). Human
Groppel-Klein, A. (1998). The influence of the dominance per- versus spatial dimensions of crowding perceptions in retail
ceived at the point-of-sale on the price-assessment. In B. environments: A note on their measurement and on their
G. Englis & A. Olofsson (Eds.), European advances in con- effect on shopper satisfaction. Marketing Letters, 5, 183
sumer research, Vol. 3 (pp. 304311). Provo, UT: Associa- 194.
tion for Consumer Research. Mano, H. (1999). The influence of pre-existing negative affect
Groppel-Klein, A., & Baun, D. (2001). The role of consumers on store purchase intentions. Journal of Retailing, 75, 149
arousal for retail stores Results from an experimental 172.

820 BRENGMAN, WILLEMS, AND JOYE


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Maxwell, S., & Kover, A. (2003). Negative affect: The dark Ulrich, R. S. (1993). Biophilia, biophobia, and natural land-
side of retailing. Journal of Business Research, 56, 553 scapes. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Eds.), The biophilia
559. hypothesis (pp. 73137). Washington: Island Press.
McGoldrick, P. J., & Pieros, C. P. (1998). Atmospherics, Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M.
pleasure and arousal, the influence of response mod- A., & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to
erators. Journal of Marketing Management, 14, 173 natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmen-
197. tal Psychology, 11, 201230.
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974a). An approach to envi- Van den Berg, A.E. (2009). Restorative effects of nature: To-
ronmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. wards a neurobiological approach. In T. Louts, M. Reiten-
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974b). A verbal measure of bach, & J. Molenbroek (Eds.), Human diversity, design for
information rate for studies in environmental psychology. life. Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Phys-
Environment and Behavior, 6, 233252. iological Anthropology (pp. 132138). Delft, the Nether-
Michon, R., Chebat, J.-C., & Turley, L.W. (2005). Mall atmo- lands: Faculty of Industrial Design Engineering, Delft Uni-
spherics: The interaction effects of the mall environment versity of Technology.
on shopping behaviour. Journal of Business Research, 58, Van Kenhove, P., & Desrumaux, P. (1997). The relationship
576583. between emotional states and approach or avoidance re-
Mooradian, T. A., & Olver, J. M. (1996). Shopping motives sponses in a retail environment. The International Review
and the Five Factor Model: An integration and preliminary of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, 7, 351368.
study. Psychological Reports, 78, 579592. Van Kenhove, P., De Wulf, K., & Van Waterschoot, W. (1999).
Newman, A. J., & Foxall, G. R. (2003). In-store customer be- The impact of task definition on store-attribute saliences
haviour in the fashion sector: Some emerging methodologi- and store choice. Journal of Retailing, 75, 125137.
cal and theoretical directions. International Journal of Re- Van Rompay, T., Galetzka, M. J. L., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Moreno
tail & Distribution Management, 31, 591600. Garcia, J. (2008). Human and spatial dimensions of retail
OKeefe D. (2003). Message properties, mediating states, and density: Revisiting the role of perceived control. Psychology
manipulation checks: Claims, evidences, and data analy- & Marketing, 25, 319335.
sis in experimental persuasive message effects research. Vazquez, R., Rodriguez-Del Bosque, I. A., Diaz, A. M., & Ruiz,
Communication Theory, 13, 251274. A. V. (2001). Service quality in supermarket retailing: Iden-
POPAI Europe. (1998). The POPAI Europe consumer buying tifying critical service experiences. Journal of Retailing and
habits study. Point-of-Purchase Advertising Institute Re- Consumer Services, 8, 114.
tail Marketing In-Store Services Limited. Watford, Herts: Walters, J., Apter, M. J., & Svebak, S. (1982). Color preference,
Author. arousal, and theory of psychological reversals. Motivation
Russell, J. A., & Lemay, G. (2000). Emotion concepts. In M. and Emotion, 6, 193215.
Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions Wolf, K. L. (2003). Public response to the urban forest in inner-
(pp. 491503). New York/London: The Guilford Press. city business districts. Journal of Arboriculture, 29, 117
Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. (1976). Environmental vari- 126.
ables in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, Wolf, K. L. (2004a). Nature in the retail environment: Compar-
3, 6263. ing consumer and business response to urban forest condi-
Schimmack, U., & Grob, A. (2000). Dimensional models of core tions. Landscape Journal, 23, 4051.
affect: A quantitative comparison by means of structural Wolf, K. L. (2004b). Trees and business district preferences: A
equation modeling. European Journal of Personality, 14, case study of Athens. Journal of Arboriculture, 30, 336346.
325345. Wolf, K. L. (2005). Trees in the small city retail business dis-
Schimmack, U., & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Experiencing activa- trict: Comparing resident and visitor perceptions. Journal
tion: Energetic arousal and tense arousal are not mixtures of Forestry, 103, 390395.
of valence and activation. Emotion, 2, 412417. Wolf, K. L. (2008). Community context and strip mall retail:
Sherman, E., Mathur, A., & Smith, R. B. (1997). Store envi- Public response to the roadside landscape. Transportation
ronment and consumer purchase behavior: Mediating role Research Record. Journal of the Transportation Research
of consumer emotions. Psychology & Marketing, 14, 361 Board, 2060, 95103.
378. Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriss der psychologic (Fundamentals
Stamps, A. E. (1990). Use of photographs to simulate environ- of Psychology). Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.
ments: A meta-analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999).
907913. Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and
Thayer, R. E. (1986). Activation and reactivation: Current beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
overview and structural analysis. Psychological Reports, 600619.
58, 607614. Zhao, H., Lynch, J.G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and activa- and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis.
tion. New York: Oxford. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197206.
Thornhill, R. (1998). Darwinian aesthetics. In C. Crawford &
D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: The authors would like to thank Mr. Johan Hellemans for his
Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 543572). Mahwah, NJ: support in the data collection. His market research and insight
Lawrence Erlbaum. agency Avisio (Antwerp Belgium) provided the opportunity
Turley, L. W., & Milliman, R. E. (2000). Atmospheric ef- to collect a considerable dataset to test this studys hypotheses.
fects on shopping behavior: A review of the experimen-
tal evidence. Journal of Business Research, 49, 193
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to:
211.
Malaika Brengman, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Pleinlaan 2,
Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence
1050 Brussels, Belgium (malaika.brengman@vub.ac.be).
recovery from surgery. Science, 224, 420421.

IN-STORE GREENERYS IMPACT ON CUSTOMERS 821


Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

You might also like