You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Affective Disorders


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / j a d

Review

Meta-analysis of the placebo response in antidepressant trials


Winfried Rief a,, Yvonne Nestoriuc a, Sarah Weiss a, Eva Welzel a, Arthur J. Barsky b, Stefan G. Hofmann c
a
Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, Germany
b
Brigham and Women's Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
c
Boston University, Boston, MA, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Improvements in placebo groups of antidepressant trials account for a major part of the
Received 8 December 2008 expected drug effects. We aimed to determine overall effect sizes of placebo and drug effects in
Received in revised form 19 January 2009 antidepressant trials, and to analyze whether the placebo effect in antidepressant trials also
Accepted 19 January 2009
occurs for patient self-perception, general psychopathology, and quality of life.
Available online 26 February 2009
Methods: Search terms covered different variants of pharmacotherapy for patients with
depressive disorders from January 1980 to December 2005 in the databases Medline/Pubmed,
Keywords:
Placebo PsychInfo and CENTRAL, a.o. We included RCTs with a placebo group and an antidepressant
Antidepressant group in people with depression.
Depression Results: We computed within group effect sizes for several outcome variables and integrated
Meta-analysis them using random-effect models. A total of 96 studies were included. Mean effect size in the
placebo group for primary outcome variables was d = 1.69 (95% CI= 1.541.84) compared to 2.50
in the drug group (95% CI= 2.302.69). There was a major difference between placebo effect sizes
assessed with observer ratings (d = 1.85, 95% CI = 1.692.01) versus patient self-perception
(d = 0.67; 95% CI = 0.490.85). The effect sizes in placebo groups in 2005 were more than twice
as great as those in 1980, but only for observer ratings, not for patient self-ratings. The result was
partly due to increased homogeneity of samples of recently published trials.
Conclusions: The placebo effect accounted for 68% of the effect in the drug groups. Whereas
clinical trials need to control the placebo effect, clinical practice should attempt to use its full
power.
2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
2. Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
2.1. Searching . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
2.2. Selection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
2.3. Data abstraction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
2.4. Validity assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
2.5. Data synthesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1. Study characteristics. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1.1. Validity ratings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1.2. Overall effect size of the placebo effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1.3. Self-rating versus observer rating . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
3.1.4. Publication year effect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5

Corresponding author. Philipps University of Marburg, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Gutenbergstrasse18 D35032 Marburg. Fax: +49 6421 282 8904.
E-mail address: riefw@staff.uni-marburg.de (W. Rief).

0165-0327/$ see front matter 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.029

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2 W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18

3.1.5. Further moderators of the placebo response. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5


3.1.6. Sensitivity analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
3.1.7. Sample homogeneity, inclusion criteria, and publication year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
4.1. Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
4.2. Implications for research and clinical practice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Role of funding source . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
Conict of interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8

1. Introduction studies show a decrease in effectiveness of the drugs with


publication year (e.g., statins, Gehr et al., 2006).
Placebo responses are general phenomena in medicine, Treatment efcacy in psychiatry can be determined using 2
and they seem to be particularly powerful in depression. different methods, namely observer ratings and patient's self-
However, the exact size of improvement in placebo groups ratings. A special characteristic of antidepressant trials is the use
compared to drug groups remains unclear because of serious of observer ratings to assess depression. Most drug trials focus
shortcomings of existing reports (e.g., strong selection of on results of the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD) or the
included studies, poor methodology based on percentage Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS). Some
scores, and inclusion of many studies with missing data). studies, however, use additional self-rating scales that allow
Moreover, it is unclear whether the improvements in placebo investigators to examine improvement from the patient's
groups can only be found for primary outcome variables (e.g., perspective. More information is needed to determine whether
depression), or whether they can also be found for other placebo effects in antidepressant trials are limited to observer
psychopathological variables and quality of life. ratings, or whether they also occur in patient self-ratings.
For the purpose of this paper, we will use the term placebo Moreover, it is unclear whether the publication year effect can
response to refer to improvements that occur in the placebo also be found for patient self-ratings.
groups of clinical trials. Despite some critical evaluations The aim of this meta-analysis was to analyze the placebo
(Hrbjartsson and Gtzsche, 2001), the placebo response is effect when rated by patient self-report as compared to
still impressive in medicine and psychiatry. The most provok- observer ratings. Moreover, the placebo response was not
ing examples of placebo effects in medicine come from sham only computed for depression, but also for general psycho-
surgery (Moseley et al., 2002; McRae et al., 2004), but the pathology and quality of life. Special emphasis was given to
placebo effect is also impressive in relieving pain (Petrovic possible moderating effects, such as publication year, diag-
et al., 2002) and reducing hypertension (Preston et al., 2000). nosis, drug groups, and others.
The placebo response was also examined in antidepres-
sant trials. Half of the drug group and 30% of the placebo 2. Method
group met the responder criterion of a 50%-reduction in
depression scores from baseline (Rush, 1993; Walsh et al., The reporting of methods follows the QUORUM Checklist.
2002). The positive effects attained with selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) that are explained by placebo are 2.1. Searching
estimated to be as high as 82% depending on the study
inclusion strategy and analysis methods used (Joffe et al., We searched Medline/Pubmed, PsychInfo, and the
1996; Kirsch and Sapirstein, 1998; Kahn et al., 2000; Kirsch Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL),
et al., 2002). A recently published study shows an inverse using the following search procedures and key words:
relationship of placebo response and depression severity, pharmacotherapy or drug therapy or drug treatment or
with less placebo response in very seriously depressed antidepressant or antidepressive and depression or affective
patients (Kirsch et al., 2008). When antidepressants were disorder or depressive or depressed or dysthymia or dysthy-
compared not only to inert placebos, but also to active placebo mic. The publication language was limited to English or
conditions imitating the side effects of antidepressants, the German, publication type = journal article. Results were
differences in the efcacy of antidepressants and active restricted to hits including the search terms: randomized or
placebos were much less pronounced (Moncrieff et al., randomised, and placebo, and double blind or triple blind.
2004). However, this analysis was based on only a few, older In the Cochrane database of systematic reviews, all reviews
studies. dealing with pharmacotherapy of depression were checked for
When comparing different antidepressant trials, it became further references.
evident that there was tremendous variation in the size of the
placebo response. An intriguing change of the placebo 2.2. Selection
response with time was reported by Walsh and colleagues
(Walsh et al., 2002) who analyzed 57 trials and found a Publication year was limited to January 1980 (introduction
correlation of r = 0.45 between the effect size in the placebo of DSM-III) until December 2005. Only studies addressing the
group and publication year. The reasons for this publication treatment of adult patients with unipolar depressive disorder
year effect are still poorly understood. In contrast, other were considered. Studies were excluded if depression was

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18 3

exclusively a comorbid disorder of a medical condition (e.g., 2.3. Data abstraction


post-stroke depression; depression after heart attacks).
Concurrent medication was allowed if less than 20% of A structured coding plan was developed prior to the meta-
patients received it. The placebo groups had to have a analysis. The coding plan included a systematic denition of
minimum of 20 patients. In typical RCT designs with 2 groups 43 items addressing general study characteristics (e.g.,
(drug vs placebo) and 2 assessment times (pre vs post), this authors, publication year), study setting, study design (e.g.,
allows us to detect effect sizes of the interaction term group treatment duration, medication, randomization, blinding,
time according to Cohen's d of N0.33 (p b .05; power = .80); assessment instruments), report of detailed results, and
therefore, even moderate treatment effects were detectable in sample characteristics (e.g., diagnosis, comorbidity, drop-out).
the included studies (Cohen, 1988). Additionally, the studies
had to report sufcient data to compute prepost-effect sizes 2.4. Validity assessment
(means, at least one standard deviation or prepost-test
statistics). The utilization of at least one instrument assessing Because the frequently used JADAD-score (Jadad et al.,
depression was required. 1996) does not cover all aspects of validity, we expanded it

Fig. 1. Trial search.

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
4 W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18

and coded additional aspects of internal validity (randomiza- used. Equivalent scores were computed according to Walsh et
tion, intent to treat analysis, report of means and standard al. (Walsh et al., 2002). Typical self-rating scales that were
deviations), construct validity (blinding, diagnosis, treatment used in the studies included the Depression subscale of the
adherence, assessment instruments), and external validity Symptom Checklist SCL-90R (Derogatis, 1994) (10 studies),
(report of selection of participants, participant characteristics, the Beck Depression Inventory BDI (Beck and Steer, 1987) (9
exclusion criteria, treatment duration). A random selection of studies), and the Zung Depression Scale (Zung, 1965) (4
30 original studies was presented to two raters (SW, EW) and studies).
inter-rater reliability for study characteristics and validity
characteristics were computed. 3.1.1. Validity ratings
In contrast to previous reviews on the placebo effect in
2.5. Data synthesis antidepressant trials that reported substantially lower rates of
complete data sets, we successfully detected relevant means
We computed within group effect sizes for the drug and and standard deviation scores for nearly three quarters of the
placebo groups. These effect sizes were dened as the mean trials (72 of 96 trials). The inter-rater reliability for the study
difference from pre-to post-treatment divided by the pooled quality characteristics was r = 0.95 which can be considered
standard deviation (e.g., Hartmann et al., 1992). In cases of excellent. The inter-rater reliability for other variables of the
missing post-scores, we used Becker's formula (Becker, 1988) data selection process ranged from 0.85 (strategy of analysis) to
which is based on the difference of the post-and pre-scores 1.00 (study identication characteristics, publication year, etc.).
related to the standard deviation of the baseline. We used a
study weighting procedure according to sample size (Hedges 3.1.2. Overall effect size of the placebo effect
and Olkin, 1985). Homogeneity statistics using Q-statistics The overall effect size of the placebo effect was 1.69 (95%
(DerSimonian and Laird, 1986) revealed signicant deviation CI=1.541.85), as compared to d=2.50 (95% CI =2.302.69) in
of the mean effect. Therefore, we applied random effect the drug group. The ratio of the effect sizes suggests that 67.6%
models for the integration of all effect sizes. Publication bias of the improvements in the drug group were attributable to
was controlled for by computing safe-n rates, stem-leaf- the placebo effect. There was a high correlation between the
diagrams and funnel plots (Becker, 1988; Egger et al., 1997). effect sizes in the placebo and drug groups (r = 0.69; p b .001),
Predened subgroup analyses were performed to test indicating that reported improvements in placebo and drug
whether the ndings were sensitive to the type of anti- groups were strongly interdependent. Stem-leaf diagrams
depressants administered, the subtype of depression diag- revealed a normal distribution with most effect sizes lying in
nosis, and the type of outcome variables. Furthermore, the the area between 1.00 and 2.8, which indicates that publica-
potential inuence of continuous moderator variables (such tion bias does not explain the results. The effect sizes of the
as publication year, study validity, treatment duration, mean placebo response were not inuenced by the category of drug
age of patients) was analyzed in regression models. that was investigated, with mean effect sizes ranging from
1.65 (SSRI-placebo) to 1.73 (other antidepressants). However,
3. Results the placebo response was signicantly greater in major
depression (d = 1.83; 95% CI = 1.671.99) than in dysthymia
3.1. Study characteristics (d = 1.11; 95% CI = 0.491.58). Interestingly, this diagnostic
specicity remained even after controlling for depression
As shown in Fig. 1, 3322 different studies fullled the rst severity (see below). The overall placebo effect was highest
level of inclusion criteria. After excluding studies based on for the primary outcome variable (depression). However,
information presented in study abstracts, 406 complete study substantial effect sizes also emerged for anxiety, general
reports were considered in more detail. The nal sample psychopathology and quality of life (see Table 1).
consisted of 96 trials1 that reported sufcient data to compute
effect sizes. The placebo groups of these studies comprised 3.1.3. Self-rating versus observer rating
9566 people. Approximately half of the studies were pub- In the placebo groups, there was a substantial difference
lished after 1996, 68% were conducted in the United States, between effect sizes for improvements rated by observers
and the mean sample size was 86 participants. The mean (d = 1.85; 95% CI = 1.692.01; 93 studies) compared to those
discontinuation rate was 30.4%. Seventy-nine of the 96 studies rated by patients (d = 0.67; 95% CI = 0.490.85; 28 studies).
addressed patients with major depression, 7 addressed Pearson's correlation between self-and observer ratings was
patients with dysthymia, and 10 studies addressed patients r = 0.57 for the 24 studies that reported both types of ratings.
with less specied or minor depressive disorders. In the active The difference between self-ratings and observer ratings was
drug groups, the following medications were administered:
SSRIs (36), tri-or tetra-cyclic antidepressants (18), MAO
inhibitors (5), herbal remedies (10), and others (27). Table 1
Average effect sizes for improvements in the placebo groups.
Most studies (86 of 97; 89%) utilized the Hamilton
Depression Scale (HAMD). Because various versions of the Variable K/n Mean d 95% CI
HAMD were used, we focused on the HAMD-17 version and Depression 97/9,469 1.69 1.541.84
estimated equivalent scores if a different version had been Anxiety 17/2,633 0.98 0.711.25
General Psychopathology 4/258 0.68 0.081.29
1 Quality of Life 12/1,214 1.00 0.571.43
A complete list of included publications can be requested from the rst
author. K = number of studies; n = total sample size; mean d = average effect size.

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18 5

also found in the drug groups (self-rating d = 1.12 versus analyzed (see Fig. 2b). Patients' perception of self-improve-
observer rating d = 2.89). Interestingly, the self-ratings and ment in the placebo groups was not inuenced by publication
observer ratings of proportion of positive effects in the year (r =.08; NS). In the drug groups, the same tendency was
placebo group relative to the drug group were comparable found (see Fig. 2a, b; bottom), although due to more variance
(observer: 66.5%; self-rating: 66.6%). this effect was less clear and failed to reach signicance.

3.1.4. Publication year effect 3.1.5. Further moderators of the placebo response
With our large sample size, we were able to replicate the Using bivariate regression models, we analyzed further
strong linear association between publication year and effect correlates of the effect size of the placebo response. We could
sizes of the placebo group (see Fig. 2a). In fact, the mean effect not conrm any signicant inuence for the average age of
sizes in the placebo groups of the studies published around the sample ( = .18), percentage of female participants
2005 were more than twice as great as those published before ( = 0.17), validity of the study ( = 0.06), or treatment
1985. The correlation between effect size and publication year duration in weeks ( = 0.06). However, there seemed to be a
was 0.41 (p b .001; compared to 0.45 in the original study signicant inuence of diagnosis (major depression versus
reporting this effect (Walsh et al., 2002)). However, this effect dysthymia; = 0.30; p b 0.003), and severity of depression
was not found when self-rating scales for depression were ( = 0.26; p b 0.02). Therefore, we entered publication year,

Fig. 2. a) Association of effect size with publication year (observer ratings; placebo groups at top, drug groups at the bottom). b) association of effect size with
publication year (self-report; placebo groups at top, drug groups at the bottom).

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
6 W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18

diagnosis, and severity of depression in one regression improvements in the placebo groups. In the studies included
analysis model. This analysis conrmed the major inuence in our analysis, we could not conrm signicant associations
of publication year ( = 0.48; p b 0.001) and diagnosis between inclusion cut-off scores for depression and publica-
( = 0.29; p b 0.003), but failed to show an additional tion year (r = 0.01; ns) or mean baseline depression severity
inuence of depression severity ( = 0.14; p = 0 .07). and publication year (r = .195; ns). Therefore the associa-
tion between depression standard deviation and publication
3.1.6. Sensitivity analysis year can be only attributed to increasing homogeneity.
A funnel plot conrmed that the greater the sample size,
the closer the effect size was to the average effect size. This 4. Discussion
argues against a publication bias; if publication bias played a
major role, large sample size trials would supposedly reveal We conducted a meta-analysis of improvements in
lower effect sizes than small sample size trials (Egger et al., placebo groups in 96 trials containing a total of 9,566
1997). Failsafe-rates were computed to determine how many depressed patients. We found that the improvements in the
unpublished studies would be needed to reduce the effect size placebo groups corresponded to 67% of the improvements in
to d = 0.01 thereby eliminating the positive effect in the the active drug groups. We further conrmed that the
placebo group. It was found that 16,300 unpublished studies reported placebo effect sizes increased tremendously from
reporting no positive placebo effects would be needed. To 1980 to 2005. However, this strong publication year effect was
reduce the mean effect size of 1.69 to a low effect size of 0.21, a only found when the primary outcome variable was an
failsafe-rate of 683 studies would be needed. Both rates observer rating of depression. Self-ratings of depression were
conrm the stability of improvements in the placebo groups. less prone to publication year effects. Indeed, the placebo
response in self-ratings was only of medium magnitude, but
3.1.7. Sample homogeneity, inclusion criteria, and publication year corresponded to other outcome measures (psychopathology,
Effect sizes and signicant ndings for improvements can quality of life).
be increased if more homogeneous samples are included. Our analysis conrms that the placebo effect can differ
Homogeneity of samples is reected in the standard deviation substantially depending on study design and outcome
of scores. The lower the standard deviation (e.g., at baseline), measures (Antonaci et al., 2007). The difference between
the higher the effect size if the average improvement is self-ratings and observer ratings in evaluating clinical out-
comparable. Therefore, we analyzed whether there was a comes of depression trials is intriguing, and raises questions
linear association between publication year and standard about which is the more valid assessment approach. Some
deviation of depression scores at baseline. Fig. 3 shows that, in scientists question whether people with depression can
fact, a linear association between publication year and accurately detect small changes in mood, and therefore
homogeneity of samples existed (r = 0.30; p b .01). However, argue in favor of observer ratings. Others, however, including
the variation was less than 25% of the values from 1980; Fava and colleagues (Fava et al., 2003), believe that clinicians
therefore, this reduction of heterogeneity can only partly tend to overestimate changes in patients, and therefore
account for the substantial changes of the effect sizes for endorse the use of additional assessment strategies.
The strong publication year effect found for observer
ratings questions their validity. Following the view of Fava
and others (Fava et al., 2003), this effect might indicate
increasing overestimation of positive effects by clinicians. One
possibility is that doctors became enthusiastic about the
efcacy of the antidepressant drugs, and developed higher
expectations for improvement. It is unlikely that the drug-and
placebo effects actually increased more than 100% over the
last 2 decades. Walsh et al. (Walsh et al., 2002) mention that
the Hamilton cut-off score for study inclusion has also
increased over the years, and that investigators might have
tended to overestimate depression severity at baseline in
order to include patients in studies. This might be associated
with a substantial decrease in depression scores between the
rst and second assessments (Khan et al., 2007). While this
effect might contribute in part to increasing effect sizes over
time, it is unlikely that it explains the entire publication year
effect. In most studies, the placebo response not only occurs
between the rst and second assessments, but rather,
improvements increase continuously. Furthermore, we were
able to show that increasing effect sizes for improvements in
antidepressant trials were partly due to the decreasing
heterogeneity of the samples. While this process might
improve internal validity, samples of clinical trials differ
Fig. 3. Decreasing heterogeneity of depression with publication year more and more from clinical practice. However, the change of
(r = 0.30; p b .01). Abbreviation: SD = standard deviation. heterogeneity over the years is smaller than the increase of

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18 7

improvement effect sizes, therefore indicating that variations trials. It is highly questionable whether the nonspecic effects
of heterogeneity can only partly explain the publication year acting in placebo groups are the same as the nonspecic
effect. effects acting in active drug groups. If placebos are not
Interestingly, we found differences in placebo responses completely inert, but instead mimic the side effects of the
depending on diagnosis. Patients with dysthymia reported drug (active placebos), they are more effective. Therefore, it
lower placebo responses than patients with major depression. is not surprising that the difference between the improve-
This is not only due to depression severity (which was ments in drug groups compared to placebo groups diminish to
controlled for in regression analyses), but depended on insignicant levels if the effect of active placebos is compared
depression type. Dysthymia is chronic by denition, while to that of antidepressants (Moncrieff et al., 2004). In other
major depression tends to follow a more cyclic course, words, if people experience side effects, they might experi-
thereby allowing more variability in placebo responses. ence more non-specic, placebo effects. However, in typical
Different placebo responses depending on diagnostic group placebo controlled trials, this increase of nonspecic effects
have also been shown to exist in other areas of mental only occurs in the drug group, but not in the placebo group.
disorders (Huppert et al., 2004).
The question arises as to which mechanisms might be 4.1. Limitations
responsible for the placebo response. Rapaport and others
(Rapaport et al., 2000) assume that placebo responders differ A general limitation of meta-analyses is the quality of the
from drug responders, therefore indicating that the mechan- included studies. However, we did not nd any association
isms of improvement under placebo might differ from the between study quality and the reported results. The total
mechanisms of improvement under drug inuence. However, sample size and the number of studies included in our meta-
other studies have found that many predictors and correlates of analysis were sufciently high for the primary outcome
change are similar between placebo and antidepressant drug variable. Publication bias is also a point of consideration in
responders (Leuchter et al., 2002; Mayberg et al., 2002); this meta-analyses, and has been shown to inuence system-
notion is conrmed by the high correlation of placebo group atically the report of positive effects for antidepressants
effect sizes and drug group effect sizes in our analysis (r = 0.69). (Turner et al., 2008). We controlled for publication bias
A positive placebo response depends on the activation of computing fail-to-safe rates, and we were able to show that
specic brain areas (Benedetti, 1996; Bingel et al., 2006). publication bias was unlikely to account for the substantial
Classical conditioning might also explain the placebo response. improvements in placebo groups. Although we had to exclude
Classical conditioning depends on prior experience. While prior many studies because of insufcient data (Fig. 1), we still
experience might facilitate the placebo response, it has also included more studies than other meta-analyses on anti-
been shown that the placebo response can be triggered by depressants, and the reported fail-to-safe rates conrm that
expectation alone (Benedetti et al., 2003; Levine et al., 2006). the results are robust. However, the results of self-rating
A crucial question for the placebo response in antidepres- scales in our analysis were based on only one-quarter of the
sant trials is whether the effect is simply a regression to the studies included, which increases the risk of type-II-errors.
mean. It has been shown that the higher the depression score, Furthermore, different self-rating scales were investigated,
the greater the placebo response (r = 0.27, Walsh et al., while observer ratings were mainly based on the Hamilton
2002). However, our results demonstrate that depression scale. This might have contributed to a blunting of systematic
severity does not explain the major effects, and the results of inuences such as the publication year effect in self-ratings.
Kirsch et al. (Kirsch et al., 2008) also contradict the Further studies should directly compare self-and expert
assumption that a mere regression-to-the mean effect ratings using the same items (e.g., the MADRS versus
explains placebo responses in depression. Moreover, regres- MADRS self-report). Therefore, our meta-analysis leaves
sion to the mean cannot account for the observed publication some questions unanswered. Although we can demonstrate
year effect, the differences depending on diagnosis, or the a tremendous difference between the results of patient self-
differences depending on ascertainment strategy. reports and observer ratings, we cannot completely explain
Another methodological shortcoming is the lack of control this difference. The same is true for the publication year effect,
groups that show the natural course of the disorder. In fact, in a which is only partly explained by ascertainment strategy and
critical analysis of the placebo response, Hrobjartsson and reduction of sample heterogeneity. Therefore, these effects
Gtzsche (Hrbjartsson and Gtzsche, 2001) compared results should be analyzed further in future studies.
of placebo groups to natural course groups and found
substantially lower placebo effects than expected. Three years 4.2. Implications for research and clinical practice
after assessment, remission rates for major depression can be
up to 70% (Rhebergen et al., 2009); in contrast, other studies These results have several implications for research and
have described stable depression severity for samples with clinical practice. As long as it remains unclear whether self-
depression scores comparable to those in our included studies ratings or observer ratings are the more valid instrument,
(Stoolmiller et al., 2005). Therefore only natural course control both types of instruments should be used in clinical trials.
groups can elucidate the long-term inuence of placebo However, only one fourth of the studies included in this
reactions, but these groups are rarely included in depression overview used self-rating scales for depression. For clinical
trials due to ethical concerns. practice, it should be kept in mind that the placebo response
Differences between the mechanisms acting in placebo in the treatment of depression is substantial. Therefore,
groups compared to those acting in the drug groups have clinicians should attempt to retain and optimize the non-
further implications for the rationale of placebo-controlled specic effects. Indeed, Benedetti (Benedetti et al., 2006)

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
8 W. Rief et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 118 (2009) 18

demonstrated that the elimination of positive expectation Jadad, A.R., Moore, R.A., Carroll, D., Jenkinson, C.P., Reynolds, D.J., Gavaghan,
D.J., McQuai, H.J., 1996. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized
effects made analgesic therapies less effective and that higher clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Controlled Clinical Trials 17, 112.
drug dosages were needed in those patients as compared to Joffe, R.T., Sokolov, S., Streiner, D., 1996. Antidepressant treatment of
others with positive treatment expectations. Waber et al. depression: a meta-analysis. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 41, 613616.
Kahn, A., Warner, H.A., Brown, W.A., 2000. Symptom reduction and suicide
were able to show that the analgesic placebo effect is higher if risk in patients treated with placebo in antidepressant clinical trials.
people expect the drug to be expensive and valuable (Waber Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 311317.
et al., 2008). Therefore the systematic application of positive Khan, A., Schwartz, K., Kolts, R.L., Ridgway, D.L.C., 2007. Relationship between
depression severity entry criteria and antidepressant clinical trial
expectations can help to optimize medication use. outcomes. Biological Psychiatry 62, 6571.
Kirsch, I., Sapirstein, G., 1998. Listening to prozac but hearing placebo: a
Role of funding source meta-analysis of antidepressant medication. Prevention and treatment 1
Nothing declared. (article 0002a).
Kirsch, I., Moore, T.J., Scoboria, A., Nicholls, S.S., 2002. The emperor's new
Conict of interest drugs: an analysis of antidepressant medication data submitted to the U.S.
food and drug administration. Prevention and Treatment 5(article 23).
No conict declared.
Kirsch, I., Deacon, B.J., Huedo-Medina, T.B., Scoboria, A., Moore, T.J., Johnson,
B.T., 2008. Initial severity and antidepressant benets: a meta-analysis of
References data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLOS Medicine 5
(2), e45.
Leuchter, A.F., Cook, I.A., Witte, E.A., Morgan, M., Abrams, M., 2002. Changes
Antonaci, F., Chimento, P., Diener, H.C., Sances, G., Bono, G., 2007. Lessons
in brain function of depressed subjects during treatment with placebo.
from placebo effects in migraine treatment. Journal of Headache and Pain
American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 122129.
8, 6366.
Levine, M.E., Stern, R.M., Koch, K.L., 2006. The effects of manipulating
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., 1987. Beck Depression Inventory. Manual. The
expectations through placebo and nocebo administration on gastric
Psychological Cooperation, San Antonio.
tachyarrhythmia and motion-induced nausea. Psychosomatic Medicine
Becker, B.J., 1988. Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British
68, 478486.
Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 41, 257278.
Mayberg, H.S., Silva, J.A., Brannan, S.K., Tekell, J.L., Mahurin, R.K., McGinnis, S.,
Benedetti, F., 1996. The opposite effect of the opiate antagonist naloxon and
Jerabek, P.A., 2002. The functional neuroanatomy of the placebo effect.
the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia. Pain 64,
American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 728737.
535543.
McRae, C., Cherin, E., Yamazaki, T.G., Diem, G., Vo, A.H., Russell, D., Ellgring, J.H.,
Benedetti, F., Pollo, A., Lopiano, L., Lanotte, M., Vighetti, S., Rainero, I., 2003.
Fahn, S., Greene, P., Dillon, S., Wineld, H., Bjugstad, K.B., Freed, C.R., 2004.
Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor,
Effects of perceived treatment on quality of life and medical outcomes in a
and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. Journal of Neuroscience 23,
double-blind placebo surgery trial. Archives of General Psychiatry 61,
43154323.
412420.
Benedetti, F., Arduino, C., Costa, S., Vighetti, S., Tarenzi, L., Rainero, I.,
Moncrieff, J., Wessely, S., Hardy, R., 2004. Active placebos versus antidepres-
Asteggiano, G., 2006. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in
sants for depression. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Review (1).
Alzheimer's disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain 121,
Moseley, J.B., O'Malley, K., Petersen, N.J., Menke, T.J., Brody, B.A., Kuykendall,
133144.
D.H., Hollingsworth, J.C., Ashton, C.M., Wray, N.P., 2002. A controlled trial
Bingel, U., Lorenz, J., Schoell, E., Weiller, C., Bchel, C., 2006. Mechanisms of
of arthroscopic surgery for osteoarthritis of the knee. New England
placebo analgesia: rACC recruitment of a subcortical antinociceptive
Journal of Medicine 347, 8188.
network. Pain 120, 815.
Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K.M., Ingvar, M., 2002. Placebo and opioid
Cohen, J., 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
analgesia-imaging a shared neuronal network. Science 295, 17371740.
Erlbaum Academic Press, Hillsdale, NJ.
Preston, R.A., Materson, B.J., Reda, D.J., Williams, D.W., 2000. Placebo-
Derogatis, L.R., 1994. SCL-90. Administration, Scoring and Procedures. Author,
associated blood pressure response and adverse effects in the treatment
Massachussetts.
of hypertension. Archives of Internal Medicine 160, 14491454.
DerSimonian, R., Laird, N., 1986. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Controlled
Rapaport, M.H., Pollack, M., Wolkow, R., Mardekian, J., Clary, C., 2000. Is
Clinical Trials 7, 177188.
placebo response the same as drug response in panic disorder? American
Egger, M., Smith, G.D., Schneider, M., Minder, C., 1997. Bias in meta-analysis
Journal of Psychiatry 157, 10141016.
detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal 315,
Rhebergen, D., Beekman, A.T.F., de Graaf, R., Nolen, W.A., Spijker, J.,
629634.
Hoogendijk, W.J., Pennix, B.W.J.H., 2009. The three-year naturalistic
Fava, M., Eden Evins, A., Dorer, D.J., Schoenfeld, D.A., 2003. The problem of the
course of major depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder and double
placebo response in clinical trials for psychiatric disorders: culprits,
depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 115 (3), 450459.
possible remedies, and a novel study design approach. Psychotherapy
Rush, A.J., 1993. Clinical Practice Guideline 5. Depression in Primary Care Vol. 2.
and Psychosomatics 72, 115127.
Treatment of Major Depression. AHCPR Silver Spring.
Gehr, B.T., Weiss, C., Porzsolt, F., 2006. The fading of reported effectiveness. A
Stoolmiller, M., Kim, H.K., Capaldi, D.M., 2005. The course of depressive
meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMC Medical Research
symptoms in men from early adolescence to young adulthood:
Methodology 6. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-11861125.
identifying latent trajectories and early predictors. Journal of Abnormal
Hartmann, A., Herzog, T., Drinkmann, A., 1992. Psychotherapy of bulimia
Psychology 114, 331345.
nervosa: what is effective? Journal of Psychosomatic Research 36,
Turner, E.H., Mathhews, A.M., Linardatos, E., Tell, R.A., Rosenthal, R., 2008.
159167.
Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its inuence on
Hedges, L.V., Olkin, I., 1985. Statistical Methods for Meta-analysis. Academic
apparent efcacy. New England Journal of Medicine 358, 252260.
Press, Orlando, Fla.
Waber, R.L., Shiv, B., Carmon, Z., Ariely, D., 2008. Commercial features of
Hrbjartsson, A., Gtzsche, P.C., 2001. Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of
placebo and therapeutic efcacy. JAMA 299, 10161017.
clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. New England
Walsh, B.T., Seidman, S.N., Sysko, R., Gould, M., 2002. Placebo response in
Journal of Medicine 344, 15941602.
studies of major depression: variable, substantial, and growing. Journal
Huppert, J.D., Schultz, L.T., Foa, E.B., Barlow, D.H., Davidson, J.R.T., Gorman, J.M.,
of the American Medical Association 287, 18401847.
Shear, M.K., Simpson, H.B., Woods, S.W., 2004. Differential response to
Zung, W.W.K., 1965. A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General
placebo among patients with social phobia, panic disorder, and
Psychiatry 12, 6370.
obsessivecompulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 161,
14851487.

Downloaded for Anonymous User (n/a) at Chinese University of Hong Kong from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on September 22, 2017.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright 2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like