You are on page 1of 19

Academy of Management Journal

2007, Vol. 50, No. 4, 923940.

BEING EMOTIONAL DURING DECISION MAKINGGOOD OR


BAD? AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION
MYEONG-GU SEO
University of Maryland

LISA FELDMAN BARRETT


Boston College

This paper examines the link between affective experience and decision-making per-
formance. In a stock investment simulation, 101 stock investors rated their feelings on
an Internet Web site while making investment decisions each day for 20 consecutive
business days. Contrary to the popular belief that feelings are generally bad for
decision making, we found that individuals who experienced more intense feelings
achieved higher decision-making performance. Moreover, individuals who were better
able to identify and distinguish among their current feelings achieved higher decision-
making performance via their enhanced ability to control the possible biases induced
by those feelings.

Folk theories abound when it comes to the topic Slovic, Finucane, Peters, & MacGregor, 2002) and
of how feelings affect decision making (Slovic, thus need to be properly regulated (Gross & John,
2001). Traditionally, emotionality has been por- 2003). Others maintain that feelings play an adap-
trayed as the opposite of rationality and/or effec- tive role in decision making (Damasio, 1994) and
tiveness in a managerial setting (Ashforth & benefit personal well-being (Aspinwall & Taylor,
Humphrey, 1995; Putnam & Mumby, 1993). Orga- 1997; Fredrickson, 2001). In the present study, we
nizations have frequently asked their employees provide evidence that might help to resolve this
and managers to keep their affective experiences at debate by suggesting that whether affective feelings
work within a relatively neutral range or to express are functional or dysfunctional for decision making
their feelings only according to narrowly defined is largely dependent upon how people experience
organizational rules (Hochschild, 1983; Morris & those feelings and what they do about them during
Feldman, 1996). A similar prescription is popular decision making. On the basis of a broad perspec-
in the field of finance. Investors are frequently in- tive on individual differences in affective informa-
structed to put their feelings under control, mean- tion processing (Gohm, 2003; Gohm & Clore, 2000),
ing that they need to avoid or suppress strong feel- we propose that individuals can experience intense
ings (Babin & Donovan, 2000). feelings during decision making while simulta-
Scientific debate over whether subjective experi- neously regulating the possible biases induced by
ences of emotion are functional or maladaptive has those feelings, both of which may positively con-
been ongoing (Gohm & Clore, 2002). Some argue tribute to their decision-making performance. We
that feelings are a source of unwanted bias (Shiv, empirically examined the proposed relationships
Loewenstein, Bechara, Damasio, & Damasio, 2005; in a stock investment simulation combined with an
experience-sampling procedure.
This study extends previous research on affect
This research was supported by NSF DRMS #0215509 and decision making in three ways. First, it pro-
to Lisa Feldman Barrett and a Boston College Dissertation vides direct empirical evidence regarding how
Research Grant to Myeong-Gu Seo. We give our special feelings influence individuals decision-making
thanks to Jean Bartunek for her excellent guidance and performance in a high-fidelity simulation that
support for this study as well as her helpful comments on simultaneously captures the aspects of psycholog-
this article. We also thank Brad Kirkman, Debra Shapiro,
ical realism (Berkowitz & Donnerstein, 1982) and
Susan Taylor, and the anonymous reviewers for their
helpful comments and feedback. Thanks also go to Fran
the benefits of experiments (such as internal valid-
Grabowski, Damien Dong, Blanca Madrid, and William ity). Second, we examine contrasting perspectives
Connell for their assistance in data collection and anal- in the literaturethe potentially functional and
ysis. Finally, we give special thanks to Jinwook Choi, dysfunctional (bias-inducing) roles of feelings in
who developed and programmed the Web server for this decision makingin a single study design. Finally,
study. this study demonstrates that the degree to which
923
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holders express
written permission. Users may print, download or email articles for individual use only.
924 Academy of Management Journal August

affective feelings are functional or dysfunctional conscious attention and allocation of working
for decision making varies considerably between memory, both of which are necessary for the exten-
individuals in a predictable way. In this article, we sive cognitive processes involved in decision mak-
use feelings as a broad term referring to various ing (Damasio, 1994; Kitayama, 1997; Wells & Mat-
affective states, including mood, viewed as a pro- thews, 1994). For example, Damasio (1994)
longed and diffuse affective state associated with asserted that feelings boost the conscious attention
no particular object, and discrete emotions, viewed and continued working memory required for any
as intense prototypical affective experiences di- reasoning or deciding. Ketelaar and Clore (1997)
rected toward certain objects, such as anger and also suggested that an important function of mo-
fear (Forgas, 1995; Russell, 2003). mentary feelings is to shift attention from less-
pressing goals to more urgent ones.
Second, feelings can facilitate the decision-mak-
LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES
ing processes involved in selecting and prioritizing
The literature on affect and decision making choices relevant to situational requirements (e.g.,
points to two contrasting perspectives regarding Damasio, 1994; Ketelaar & Clore, 1997; Schwarz,
the role of affective experience in decision making. 1990; Schwarz & Clore, 1988). One of the common
The first, which we call feeling-as-bias-inducer, dilemmas a decision maker faces is that potentially
suggests that individuals feelings induce various infinite factors and options surround every deci-
forms of bias into the decision-making process that sion, each with conflicting advantages and disad-
skew their decisions in certain ways. In this view, vantages, making it extremely difficult or even im-
feelings can be harmful to decision-making perfor- possible to make an optimal decision within a
mance. There are several ways that affective feel- given time frame (Ketelaar & Clore, 1997). Pleasant
ings can bias decision making. First, feelings can and unpleasant feelings can help decision makers
affect the content of information retrieved in the to resolve this dilemma by invoking distinguish-
brain during decision making (e.g., Erber, 1991; able frames of mind (Morris, 1989; Schwarz, 1990;
LeDoux, 1993; Meyer, Gayle, Meeham, & Harman, Schwarz & Clore, 1983, 1988; Raghunathan &
1990). For example, a body of research supports a Pham, 1999) that in turn enable and facilitate their
mood congruence recall effect, which refers to selectively attending to and efficiently prioritizing
peoples tendency to recall materials from memory cues in terms of their relevance to the adaptive
that are consistent with their affective state at the requirements in a given situation (e.g., Ketelaar &
time of recall (e.g., Meyer et al., 1990). Second, Clore, 1997). In particular, Damasio (1994) argued
feelings can directly color cognitive judgments re- that the human affective system plays a critical role
quired for decision making. Numerous studies have in peoples quickly generating and selecting among
shown that momentary feelings influence various a potentially infinite number of alternative options
social judgments (see Forgas [1995] for a review). by providing immediate affective evaluations of
For example, one general effect, the mood congru- each options relative goodness or badness for their
ence judgment effect, is that people tend to make personal well-being.
judgments that are consistent with their affective Finally, considerable evidence exists that mo-
states at the time of judgment (e.g., Johnson & Tver- mentary feelings influence how people process in-
sky, 1983; Meyer et al., 1992). A third body of formation during decision making, which in turn
research suggests that affective feelings can directly promotes decision-making effectiveness in particu-
bias individual choices (e.g., Gray, 1999; Shah, lar contexts. For example, people in pleasant affec-
Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002). For example, stud- tive states tend to categorize stimuli in a broader,
ies have shown that intense unpleasant feelings more inclusive, and more flexible fashion (Murray,
often lead people to favor short-term enhance- Sujan, Hirt, & Sujan, 1990), which often enhances
ments, focusing on what is best in the moment, creativity (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987) and
regardless of possibly negative long-term conse- performance on complex tasks (Isen & Means, 1983;
quences (Gray, 1999). Staw & Barsade, 1993). In contrast, people in un-
Other researchers have proposed a feeling-as-de- pleasant affective states tend to engage in more
cision-facilitator view. That is, affective feelings effortful, systematic, piecemeal information pro-
can improve decision-making performance by facil- cessing (Conway & Giannopoulos, 1993; Edwards &
itating and even enabling decision-making pro- Weary, 1993), which leads to effective decision
cesses. Researchers have identified several ways making when decisions require accurate, unbiased,
though which feelings can facilitate decision mak- and realistic judgments (e.g., Sinclair, 1988) or sys-
ing. First, scholars from several disciplines have tematic execution of a structured decision protocol
suggested that affective reaction is a core driver of (Elsbach & Barr, 1999).
2007 Seo and Barrett 925

Reconciliation: Individual Differences in the degree to which individuals experience intense


Affective Information Processing feelings during decision making, which is called
affective reactivity (e.g., Larsen, 2000), and the de-
These two streams of research suggest that at any
gree to which they regulate the bias-generating in-
given moment, affective experience has the poten-
fluences of their current feelings, which we call
tial to both help and hurt those making important
affective influence regulation (e.g., Forgas, 2000;
decisions. We argue that whether affective feelings
Gohm, 2003; Gohm & Clore, 2000), independently
actually hurt or help decision making can be
and interactively contribute to more favorable
largely determined by how individuals experience
decision-making outcomes. In addition, we pro-
and handle those feelings in more or less functional
pose that another dimension of individual differ-
or dysfunctional ways. This stance is consistent ences in affective information processing, emotion
with a broader perspective on individual differ- differentiation (also called emotion granularity
ences in affective information processing (Barrett, [Barrett, 2004] or emotional clarity [Gohm &
1998; Feldman, 1995; Gohm, 2003; Gohm & Clore, Clore, 2000]), is an important predictor of affective
2000, 2002). According to this perspective, individ- influence regulation. Emotion differentiation is de-
uals differ not only in how they experience feel- fined as the degree to which an individual can
ingsfor example, in the extent to which they ex- identify, distinguish, and describe specific feeling
perience intense feelings but also in what they do states (Barrett, 1998; Barrett, Gross, Christensen, &
about those feelings: that is, in the extent to which Benvenuto, 2001; Feldman, 1995; Salovey, Mayer,
they attend to the information feelings convey and Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995). We hypothesize
integrate it into their judgments, decisions, and below that emotion differentiation positively and
behaviors. More importantly, this framework sug- indirectly affects decision-making performance
gests that how people experience their feelings and via its positive influence on affective influence
what they do with their feelings are conceptually regulation.
separate and relatively independent processes
(Gohm, 2003; Gohm & Clore, 2000).
We argue that the two competing perspectives Affective Influence Regulation and Decision-
in the research literature on emotion focus on the Making Performance
two different processes in affective information A number of scholars have found that individu-
processing within individuals. The feeling-as- als differ in how they regulate their affective expe-
decision-facilitator perspective focuses on how rience and its broader consequences in their judg-
people experience their feelings during decision ments, choices, and behaviors (Erber & Erber, 2000;
making, since it suggests that feelings per se inher- Gohm, 2003; Gottman & Fainsilber-Katz, 1989;
ently facilitate decision making, regardless of what Gross, 1998; Larsen, 2000). Of particular impor-
people do about those feelings (feelings per se may, tance to decision making is affective influence reg-
for example, facilitate decision making by enhanc- ulation. Forgas and his colleagues (e.g., Forgas,
ing working memory capacity [Damasio, 1994]). In 2000; Forgas & Ciarrochi, 2002) provided a theoret-
contrast, the feeling-as-bias-inducer perspective fo- ical explanation of why and how individuals differ
cuses on the other process, what people do about in affective influence regulation. They argued that
their experienced feelings. For example, a number individuals constantly engage in two types of affec-
of studies have evidenced that the bias-inducing tive information processing modes in a temporal
effects of feelings disappear when people attribute sequence. One is open, constructive processing
their current feelings to the correct causes (Forgas & (called substantive processing) in which people
Ciarrochi, 2002; Schwarz & Clore, 1983). This view process both affective and nonaffective information
implies that not all experienced feelings introduce extensively and in an open-ended fashion. How-
bias to decisions; the effects of feelings depend on ever, they are generally unaware of their current
how people handle those feelings during decision feeling states and their possibly bias-inducing in-
making. fluences and thus experience extensive and direct
We attempt to reconcile and integrate the two infusion of their affective feelings into their judg-
competing perspectives within the broader frame- ments and choices (e.g., mood congruence judg-
work of individual differences in affective informa- ment). The other kind of processing (called moti-
tion processing by proposing that individuals can vated processing) is a more controlled, directed
experience intense feelings during decision making information-processing strategy in which the bias-
and simultaneously regulate the possibly bias- inducing effects generally disappear or are reversed
inducing effects of those feelings on their deci- as people become aware of and actively manage
sions. Moreover, we hypothesize below that both their affective experience. Forgas (2000) further
926 Academy of Management Journal August

suggested that although people constantly and ex- feeling-as-decision-facilitator perspective, intense
tensively use both types of processing in processing feelings experienced during decision making may
affective information, they may differ in the extent facilitate the cognitive processes involved in deci-
to which they make transitions from the open, sub- sion making by, for example, promoting enhanced
stantive processing to the controlled, motivated attention, working memory allocation, and alterna-
processing of their affective information and thus tive generation and selection (Damasio, 1994; Kete-
differ in the degree to which their current feelings laar & Clore, 1997; Kitayama, 1997). In addition,
induce biases into their judgments and decisions such intense feelings may also have important mo-
during decision making. tivational implications for decision making. Seo,
The judgments and choices of individuals high Barrett, and Bartunek (2004) suggested that affec-
in affective influence regulation are less likely to be tive feelings may constantly influence three core
influenced by their affective feelings during deci- dimensions of task motivation within individuals:
sion making. The feeling-as-bias-inducer perspec- direction (choice of action), effort (intensity of ac-
tive discussed above would suggest that such indi- tion), and persistence (duration of action). In par-
viduals are likely to achieve higher performance in ticular, they argued that the intensity of feelings
most decision-making tasks in which decision (activation), regardless of whether they are pleasant
makers accurate and unbiased judgments in given or unpleasant, may generate a sense of energy or
situations are the primary determinant of decision urgency for action that leads people to devote a
performance, because their decisions will be more greater amount of effort to a given task and that this
protected from the possible biases induced by their effect can occur without their conscious awareness
feelings. In contrast, individuals low in affective and control. An increase in effort generated by in-
influence regulation may perform worse than oth- tense feelings in turn may lead to better decision
ers in decision making, because their current feel- performance to the extent that performance is ef-
ings constantly influence their judgments and fort-dependent. Therefore, we hypothesize that af-
choices to greater degrees. These judgments and fective reactivity may be positively related to deci-
choices in turn hinder them from basing decisions sion-making performance:
on an accurate mental representation of reality.
Therefore, we hypothesize that affective influence Hypothesis 2. Individuals higher, rather than
regulation is positively related to decision-making lower, in affective reactivity during decision
performance: making achieve higher decision-making per-
formance.
Hypothesis 1. Individuals higher, rather than
lower, in affective influence regulation achieve
higher decision-making performance. Interaction between Affective Influence
Regulation and Affective Reactivity
Although affective influence regulation and af-
Affective Reactivity and Decision-
fective reactivity are mutually distinct individual
Making Performance
characteristics, they may not influence decision-
A number of researchers have suggested that in- making performance in a purely independent
dividuals differ in how they respond to various fashion if their underlying processesthe bias-
affective cues in their environments (Gohm & generating effect of feeling and the intensity of
Clore, 2000, 2002; Larsen, 2000). For example, experienced feelingare systematically related to
some people are more reactive to negative environ- each other within individuals. From a conceptual
mental cues than to positive ones (e.g., Larsen & point of view, a systematic association between the
Ketelaar, 1991). Some people react with more in- two underlying processes is possible because the
tense feelings to both pleasant and unpleasant bias-generating effect of affective feeling implies
events in their lives (Larsen, Diener, & Emmons, the presence of affective feeling of at least a certain
1986). In this study, we focus on a general dimen- degree of intensity. Past empirical research consis-
sion of affective reactivity often called affect tently suggests a partial association between the
intensity (Larsen & Diener, 1987) or emotional intensity of feeling and its bias-generating effect;
intensity (Gohm, 2003), defined as the magnitude there is neither complete association nor complete
of affective feelings experienced during decision independence between the two. For example, the
making. mere presence of pleasant or unpleasant feeling,
By definition, individuals with higher affective regardless of its intensity, is sufficient to color judg-
reactivity are likely to experience more intense ments and choices (see Isen [2002] for a review).
feelings during decision making. According to the Similarly, intense feelings often but not always
2007 Seo and Barrett 927

generate biases when people are aware of and thus ferentiation characterized by large positive correla-
actively regulate such intense feelings (see Forgas tions among positive affect items and among
[1995] for a review). negative items).
Therefore, at least to the extent that affective A number of scholars have suggested that emo-
intensity is systematically associated with deci- tion differentiation has an important implication
sion-making biases, affective reactivity may influ- for effective use and regulation of affective experi-
ence decision-making performance in interaction ences, particularly for reducing the possibly bias-
with affective influence regulation. More specifi- inducing effects of momentary feelings (Barrett et
cally, affective reactivity may more strongly facili- al., 2001; Barrett & Gross, 2001; Ciarrochi, Catuti, &
tate decision-making performance for those indi- Mayer, 2003; Gohm, 2003; Salovey et al., 1995). For
viduals who are higher in affective influence example, several researchers have argued that ex-
regulation, since they can better regulate any bias- periences of specific, differentiated emotional
generating effects that arise during decision mak- states are less subject to misattribution errors (Kelt-
ing. In addition, since the association is still partial, ner, Locke, & Audrain, 1993; Schwarz, 1990) be-
the interaction effect may not completely replace cause these states are typically associated with a
the main effects of affective influence regulation causal object, whereas global affective states are not
and affective reactivity on decision-making perfor- (Russell & Barrett, 1999). In addition, other scholars
mance. Thus, we hypothesize a moderating effect (Barrett et al., 2001; Barrett & Gross, 2001; Ciarrochi
of affective influence regulation on the relationship et al., 2003) have suggested that greater emotion
between affective reactivity and decision-making differentiation is associated with more highly acti-
performance in addition to the two main effects: vated discrete emotional knowledge, which pro-
vides a wealth of information regarding the behav-
Hypothesis 3. The relationship between affec- ioral repertoire for dealing with an affective
tive reactivity and decision-making perfor- experience and coping with the larger situation.
mance is stronger for those individuals who are Thus, they suggest, individuals with high emotion
higher, rather than lower, in affective influence differentiation should have an advantage in regu-
regulation. lating their affective experience and its potentially
negative influences on their choices and behaviors.
Accordingly, we hypothesize that emotion differ-
Emotion Differentiation and Affective
entiation is positively related to affective influence
Influence Regulation
regulation. Specifically, individuals who are more
From a practical standpoint, a great deal of un- attentive to and better able to identify and distin-
certainty remains regarding what individuals guish among their current affective statesinstead
should do to better regulate the influence of their of ignoring them or viewing them globallyare
affective feelings on decision making, even if sci- likely to better regulate the possibly bias-generating
entific evidence supports our hypothesis that effects of their affective feelings during decision
greater affective influence regulation will lead to making. As a result, more emotionally differenti-
higher decision-making performance. Here we pro- ated individuals will achieve higher decision-mak-
pose a way to reduce the uncertainty and, in doing ing performance via their enhanced ability to reg-
so, we contradict the popular prescriptions and ulate their affective influence on their decisions.
organizational practices that encourage people to This argument leads us to further hypothesize that
ignore or suppress their feelings to better regulate affective influence regulation mediates the rela-
their affective influence (Putnam & Mumby, 1993). tionship between emotion differentiation and deci-
A key dimension on which individuals differ in sion-making performance:
processing affective information is emotion differ-
entiation. In several studies using an experience- Hypothesis 4. Affective influence regulation
sampling procedure, for example, Barrett and her mediates the relationship between emotion dif-
colleagues (Barrett, 1998, 2004; Feldman, 1995) ferentiation and decision-making performance.
found that some individuals tend to describe their
affective experiences in a discrete, differentiated
METHODS
fashion (high emotion differentiation characterized
by smaller correlations among positive affect items To examine the dynamics of affective experience
and among negative affect items), whereas other and its effects on decision making in a real-life
individuals represent their affective experience in setting, we developed and ran an Internet-based
an undifferentiated fashion, treating a range of like- stock investment simulation. We chose this domain
valence terms as interchangeable (low emotion dif- of behavior because the task of stock investing in-
928 Academy of Management Journal August

volves a series of decision-making activities that meetings and/or electronic advertisement via mem-
have clearly observable variations in key dimen- bership e-mail directories). A total of 118 members
sions of decision making, such as risk taking. In volunteered by the deadline date to participate in
addition, stock investing allowed us to isolate in- the stock investment simulation. The first author
dividual-level effects of affective experience from initially met the participants as groups, gave them
potential group-level, organization-level, and insti- the detailed instructions for participating in the
tution-level factors that might affect decision-mak- stock investment simulation, and told them that the
ing outcomes. investment simulation was a part of a larger study
We combined the investment simulation with an exploring how peoples thoughts and feelings in-
experience-sampling procedure (Barrett, 1998; Bar- fluence investment decisions. Thus, he noted, they
rett & Barrett, 2001; Feldman, 1995) in which in- would be asked to report their current thoughts and
vestors rated their feelings and thoughts directly on feelings during the simulation. Participants re-
the Internet Web site while simultaneously per- ceived remuneration of between $100 and $1,000
forming investing activities. Experience-sampling for participating; the amount depended on their
procedures, in which feelings are measured at the investment performance in the simulation (de-
time they are being experienced, minimize the cog- scribed further below). Their ages ranged from 18 to
nitive biases that can affect memory-based self-re- 74 (x 24.7, s.d. 13.2) and, as is typical in most
ports (Wheeler & Reis, 1991; Reis & Gable, 2000). investment clubs, the majority of the participants
This bias reduction is particularly important for were male (86 men / 80%). Their investment expe-
studying affective experience, because researchers rience was 4.3 years on average (s.d. 7.4), ranging
have detected memory biases when using standard from 0 to 50 years (0 years, 16%; 0 1 year, 20%;
retrospective self-report measures (e.g., Barrett, 23 years, 26%; 4 5 years, 15%; 510 years, 16%;
1997). Moreover, by measuring momentary affec- more than 10 years, 7%).
tive feelings and all other variables multiple times
(here, 1720 times) for each individual, we could
Measurement
examine the within-individual characteristics in af-
fective information processing (affective reactivity, Affective reactivity. Drawing on the recent con-
affective influence regulation, and emotion differ- ceptual and empirical examination of core affective
entiation) as well as the between-person effects of structure by Barrett and Russell (1998), we selected
those within-person characteristics on decision 22 affect-related adjectives that represented the cir-
performance. cumplex structure of core affect: two indicated
The first author ran the stock investment simu- pleasant feelings (happy and satisfied), five in-
lation for 20 business days (four weeks). The par- dicated pleasant, activated feelings (excited,
ticipants were initially given hypothetical cash of joyful, enthusiastic, proud, and inter-
$10,000. During the simulation, they were allowed ested), two indicated activated feelings (aroused
to invest the whole or a part of this hypothetical and surprised), five indicated unpleasant, acti-
cash on any of 12 anonymous stocks selected from vated feelings (irritated, afraid, angry, ner-
the national stock market for this simulation. Once vous, and frustrated), two indicated unpleasant
a day during the simulation period, participants feelings (sad and disappointed), two indicated
logged onto the stock investment simulation Web unpleasant, deactivated feelings (depressed
site; viewed current market and stock information, and tired), two indicated deactivated feelings
which the author updated daily, using national (quiet and still), and two indicated pleasant,
sources; checked their current investment perfor- deactivated feelings (calm and relaxed).
mance; and finally made their investment deci- Each day during the simulation period (thus, 20
sions about which and how many shares of the 12 times), participants used a 5-point scale (0, not at
stocks to buy or sell for the day. Just before making all, to 4, extremely so) to indicate the extent to
their investment decisions for the day, they re- which each adjective described their current feel-
ported their current affect. ings. Of the 22 adjectives used, 18 represented af-
fective reactivity: nine pleasantly valenced affect
items (excited, joyful, enthusiastic, proud,
Participants
interested, happy, satisfied, calm, and re-
The first author contacted six investment clubs laxed) and nine unpleasantly valenced affect
located in the northeastern United States, each items (irritated, afraid, angry, nervous,
with at least 40 members, and advertised the in- frustrated, disappointed, sad, tired, and
vestment simulation via public announcement depressed). We derived an affective reactivity in-
(e.g., face-to-face presentations during regular dex each day for each participant by taking the
2007 Seo and Barrett 929

average of the pleasantly valenced affect items ( relation to the stock market (Bodie et al., 2001).
.90) when pleasant affect was the dominant sub- This is also a well-known parameter of a stocks
jective state and the average of the unpleasantly potential risk. The average of the betas in a partic-
valenced affect items ( .86) when unpleasant ipants stock portfolio indicated the level of risk
affect was the dominant state (Barrett et al., 2001; that the participant chose in constructing the port-
Larsen & Diener, 1987). Out of a total of 1,868 affect folio. A higher average beta indicated greater risk
reports collected, in 1,072 cases (57%) pleasant taking. The third risk indicator was the average
feelings were the dominant affective state for a one-year return of a stock portfolio. The one-year
given day, and in 796 cases (43%), unpleasant feel- return, generally considered a parameter of a
ings were predominant. These numbers suggest a stocks potential profitability and associated risk,
relatively good balance in our sampling between pointed to the level of profitability and risk that a
predominantly pleasant and predominantly un- participant chose in constructing his or her stock
pleasant affective states. These affective reactivity portfolio. A higher average one-year return indi-
index scores were further averaged over times cated greater risk taking. A factor analysis (with the
(days) for each participant. A higher score in this principal component extraction method) showed
averaged affective reactivity index indicated that these three parameters constituted one factor
greater affective intensity experienced during stock that explained 61 percent of the total variance. We
investment decision making. used the factor score (calculated by the regression
Affective influence regulation. We computed method) as a general index for the risk taking rep-
an index of affective influence regulation for each resented in a given stock portfolio, with a higher
participant to capture the extent to which the de- score indicating greater risk taking.
gree of pleasantness and the degree of activation, Finally, we computed two regression coefficients
the two fundamental dimensions of core affect, in- for each participant, one by regressing the risk-
fluenced the level of risk that a person chose in taking index on the degree of pleasantness and the
making his or her investment decisions. This com- other by regressing the same risk-taking index on
putation took several steps. the degree of activation over time for each individ-
From each participants daily report of core ual. The coefficient for the regression of pleasant-
affective experience, we first computed the degree ness on risk taking varied from 3.40 to 1.12, with
of pleasantness ( .85) by subtracting the mean a mean of 0.03 and a standard deviation of 0.51,
for the nine unpleasantly valenced affect items and the coefficient for the regression of activation
(used to construct our affective reactivity measure) on risk varied from 2.31 to 2.75, with a mean of
from the mean for the nine pleasantly valenced 0.09 and a standard deviation of 0.65. This pattern
affect items (used to construct the affective reactiv- of values suggested that affective influence (the
ity measure). Similarly, we computed each per- bias effect) on risk taking could go in either direc-
sons degree of activation ( .61) by subtracting tion; an increase in pleasantness or activation can
the mean of deactivated affect items (tired, de- make some individuals take greater risks (resulting
pressed, quiet, still, relaxed, and calm) in positive values for the regression coefficients)
from the mean of activated affect items (excited, but make other individuals avoid risks (negative
interested, joyful, enthusiastic, proud, coefficients). Thus, we took the absolute values for
aroused, surprised, irritated, afraid, an- these regression coefficients, to consider only the
gry, nervous, and frustrated). magnitude, not the direction, of affective influence
Second, from each participants daily stock in- on risk taking. These two regression coefficients
vestment portfolio, we computed three parameters were highly correlated with each other (r .44),
that indicated the degree of risk chosen by the and a factor analysis (principal component extrac-
participant in making his or her stock investment tion method) showed that they constituted one fac-
decision on a given day. One was diversification, a tor explaining 72 percent of the total variance. We
well-known financial strategy used to avoid risk used the factor scores (calculated by the regression
(Bodie, Kane, & Marcus, 2001), which we measured method) as an index for affective influence regula-
by computing a Herfindahl index, the sum of the tion. For conceptual consistency, we reversed the
squares of all percentage weights invested in differ- index scores in such a way that higher scores indi-
ent stocks (0 index 1). A higher score indicated cated higher affective influence regulation (less af-
greater risk taking. The second risk indicator was fective influence on risk taking).
the averaged beta coefficient of a selected stock Emotion differentiation. From the 20-day core
portfolio. The beta of each stock, which partici- affective experience ratings of each participant, we
pants saw every day during the simulation period, computed two emotion differentiation indexes, one
is a measure of the volatility of the stocks price in for pleasant feelings (positive emotion differentia-
930 Academy of Management Journal August

tion) and the other for unpleasant feelings (negative esized relationships among the key variables. One
emotion differentiation), following Barrett et al. was participants age (in years) and the other was
(2001). For the positive emotion differentiation in- previous stock investment experience (in months).
dex, we first calculated the correlations between Age is an important factor influencing affective ex-
the three affect items calm, happy, and ex- perience and its influence on cognitive processes
cited over time for each participant. These affect (e.g., Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade,
items were chosen because they represent a range 2000), whereas experience may influence both de-
of prototypical pleasant affective states (Barrett, cision makers affective experience and their deci-
1998; Barrett et al., 2001). Large correlations re- sion-making performance (e.g., Lo, 2002).
flected large degrees of co-occurrence and thus lit-
tle differentiation, whereas smaller correlations
Procedures
reflected smaller degrees of co-occurrence and
more differentiation (Barrett, 1998). We performed Each day during the simulation period, partici-
Fisher r-to-Z transformations on all correlations be- pants visited our Internet Web site once, between
fore additional computations and then computed 6:00 p.m. and 9:00 a.m. the next morning. After
and averaged one set of correlations for each par- logging in using their code names, they saw the
ticipant. A similar procedure was followed for the daily stock market information, including the daily
three prototypical negative affect items sad, an- changes and the past-five-day trends of the three
gry, and nervous. For conceptual consistency, major market indexes (e.g., the Dow Jones, NAS-
we reversed the scores of both the positive emotion DAQ, and S&P 500), as well as the changes and
differentiation index and the negative emotion dif- trends of a local market index for the simulation;
ferentiation index in such a way that higher scores this was a composite index of the 12 anonymous
indicated higher emotion differentiation. stocks that we had randomly selected from the
We treated these two indexes separately for both national stock market on the basis of varying de-
conceptual and empirical reasons. First, individu- grees of risk and profitability and of various indus-
als often experience more pressure to understand tries and company sizes. The local market index
and actively regulate their emotions when they ex- was highly correlated with the national market in-
perience negative rather than positive emotions dexes (r .8) and maintained a relatively good
(Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1996; Pratto & John, balance of ups and downs (14 ups and 6 downs)
1991). As a result, individuals often better differen- during the simulation period.
tiate among negative feelings than positive feelings Next, participants saw a Web page that contained
(Fredrickson, 2001, 2003). This implies not only the daily updated information (on the basis of daily
that positive emotion differentiation and negative closing price) on these 12 stocks. Information on
emotion differentiation can develop in a mutually each was limited to its current price (initially set at
independent fashion within individuals, but also $100.00 per share but changed in proportion to the
that the two may play different adaptive roles in stocks actual price change thereafter), daily price
emotion regulation (Barrett et al., 2001). Consis- change (expressed as a percentage), average price
tently, these two indexes were virtually uncorre- change rate for the past five days, beta coefficient (a
lated with each other in our data (r .02). stocks volatility in relation to the market), one-year
Decision performance. Decision performance stock performance (percent change in stock price
was measured as the average daily stock invest- over the trailing 52 weeks), price-earnings ratio (a
ment return generated by each participant as a ratio of stock price to its trailing 12-month earnings
result of the daily investment decisions made per share), and company size (sales volume). The
throughout the simulation. Each participants stock individual stock names were manipulated (e.g.,
investment return was determined daily by the stock A, B, and C) in such a way that participants
amount that he or she had earned or lost so far as a could not identify the real names.
percentage of $10,000, the initial amount of hy- On the next page, participants saw a report that
pothetical cash provided for each to invest. To summarized their investment performance and ex-
discourage opportunistic efforts to simply capital- pected reward so far. All participants began the
ize on stock market fluctuations, this investment simulation with a designated reward of $200, but
return (expressed as a percentage) was further they earned or lost money each day depending on
adjusted by the average performance of the 12 their investment performance, which was deter-
stocks (the local market index also expressed as a mined by their overall investment returnthe
percentage). amount in percentage that they earned or lost by
Control variables. We controlled for two vari- investing their initial capital (hypothetical cash of
ables in this study that might influence the hypoth- $10,000) adjusted by the local market index.
2007 Seo and Barrett 931

On the next page, participants were asked to rate participants reported interruptions during the ses-
the various feelings that comprised their current sions (57 cases) or data transfer errors (6 cases). As
affective state. On the subsequent page, partici- a result, we used 1,870 cases of data completed by
pants made their own investment decisions for the 101 participants for data analysis in computing the
daywhich stocks to sell and which to buy. As individual-level indexes for emotion differentia-
noted, each participant was initially given $10,000 tion, affective influence regulation, affective reac-
in hypothetical cash. They were allowed to invest tivity, and daily decision performance.
all or a part of the cash on any of the 12 stocks in We used structural equation modeling (SEM),
the local market as long as the cash balance did not implemented in EQS (Bentler & Wu, 1998), to test
go below zero, and they were also allowed to trade the hypothesized relationships among the variables
those stocks freely, with no transaction costs. The (including both mediating and moderating effects)
Web page had been designed in such a way that it precisely by considering all the relationships
automatically performed all mathematical calcula- among the key variables simultaneously in estimat-
tions required for investment decision making and ing parameters. Using SEM, we fitted several
instantly checked for mistakes (e.g., overinvest- nested models to the data according to our hypoth-
ment). The current (national and local) market and eses, assuming that all the variables in the models
stock information that participants had seen in the were observed (manifest) variables. Following
previous pages also became available for reference Hatcher (1998), we used several indexes of model
on a separate Web page. fit, including (1) the chi-square goodness-of-fit sta-
Before logging out, participants saw their invest- tistic, (2) the root-mean-square error of approxima-
ment summary in a table and were asked to de- tion (RMSEA), (3) the goodness-of-fit index (GFI),
scribe the reasons behind their investment deci- (4) the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI), (5)
sions for the day in a text box. Finally, they the normed fit index (NFI), and (6) the comparative
reported whether, when, and how long they had fit index (CFI).
experienced any type of interruptions while per-
forming these tasks for the day. This process was
RESULTS
repeated daily during the simulation period (20
times). Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations,
and ranges of the variables and their correlations.
Data Analysis
The Basic Hypothesized Model and Testing the
Of the 118 investors recruited for this study, 108
Main Effects
participants completed the stock investment simu-
lation task. They generated 2,059 cases, each of The first model that we tested, the basic hypoth-
which included all measures generated by one par- esized model, specifies the primary hypothesized
ticipant going through one investment session per relationships among the key variables. This model
day. We dropped 7 participants because of non- directly tested the two main effect hypotheses (Hy-
compliance with instructions (they showed a sys- potheses 1 and 2) and was used as a basis for
tematic pattern of random responses) and elimi- further testing the interaction (moderation) hypoth-
nated an additional 63 cases (3%) because the esis (Hypothesis 3) and the mediation hypothesis

TABLE 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlationsa
Variable Mean s.d. Maximum Minimum 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Age in years 25.05 13.63 74 18


2. Experience in months 52.96 90.97 600 0 .77**
3. Decision performance 0.46 2.50 7.87 7.42 .21* .07
4. Affective reactivity 1.41 0.57 3.49 0.33 .16 .01 .23*
5. Affective influence regulation 0.00 1.00 0.85 6.38 .19 .10 .27** .02
6. Positive emotion differentiation 0.70 0.20 1.23 0.14 .09 .11 .04 .20* .12
7. Negative emotion 0.69 0.27 1.16 0.02 .01 .04 .14 .23* .23* .02
differentiation

a
n 101.
* p .05
** p .01
932 Academy of Management Journal August

(Hypothesis 4). As presented in Figure 1, our basic supported Hypothesis 1: participants who were
hypothesized model contains four main paths to be less influenced by their current feelings in deter-
estimated, paths from affective influence regulation mining the level of risk in their daily stock portfo-
to decision performance (path a in Figure 1; Hy- lios (had higher affective influence regulation)
pothesis 1), from affective reactivity to decision achieved higher daily investment returns on the
performance (path b; Hypothesis 2), from positive average throughout the simulation. Hypothesis 2
emotion differentiation to affective influence regu- was also supported; the path coefficient from affec-
lation (path c), and from negative emotion differen- tive reactivity to decision performance (path b) was
tiation to affective influence regulation (path d). positive and significant (b 0.85, t 2.01, p
We added two more paths to be estimated for con- 0.05). Participants who experienced affective feel-
trol purposes, one from age to decision perfor- ings with greater intensity during the investment
mance and the other from experience to decision simulation achieved higher investment returns.
performance. In specifying the model parameters,
we allowed all pairs of the exogenous variables in
Moderation Model
the model (affective reactivity, positive emotion
differentiation, negative emotion differentiation, To test Hypothesis 3, stating that affective influ-
age, and experience) to covary (Hatcher, 1998). We ence regulation moderates the relationship be-
present the standardized path coefficients esti- tween affective reactivity and decision perfor-
mated by SEM in Figure 1. mance, we developed a second model, the
The SEM results suggested that this model fitted moderation model, following a procedure similar
the data well, with all fit indexes meeting the cri- to one suggested by Ping (1995). In this model, we
teria (2 7.43, df 5, p 0.19; GFI .98, AGFI added an interaction term, affective reactivity by
.89, RMSEA .07, CFI .98, NFI .95). The affective influence regulation, to the basic hypoth-
path coefficient from affective influence regulation esized model and specified a direct path from this
to decision performance (path a) was positive and interaction term to decision performance. We also
significant (b 0.56, t 2.40, p 0.05) and thus allowed this interaction term to be correlated with

FIGURE 1
Basic Hypothesized Path Model with Standardized Path Coefficientsa

a
n 101.
* p .05
2007 Seo and Barrett 933

affective reactivity as well as with the error term of affective influence regulation (path d), and one
affective influence regulation. from affective influence regulation to decision per-
The SEM results suggest that the moderation formance (path a), while retaining all other speci-
model fits well to the data, with most fit indexes fications in the partial mediation model.
meeting the criteria (2 13.41, df 9, p 0.15; The SEM results suggest that the partial media-
GFI .97, AGFI .87, RMSEA .07, CFI .97, tion model fits the data well (2 4.55, df 3, p
NFI .93). However, the path coefficient of the 0.21; GFI .99, AGFI .88, RMSEA .07, CFI
interaction term was not significant and near zero .99, NFI .97). However, the change in the value of
(b 0.01, t 0.05). Thus, Hypothesis 3 was not chi-square between this model and the hypothe-
supported, suggesting that both affective reactivity sized model is marginal and nonsignificant (2
and affective influence regulation contributed to 2.88, df 2). In addition, neither of the two added
decision performance additively, not interactively. direct path coefficients, one from positive emotion
differentiation to decision performance (b 0.18,
t 0.15) and one from negative emotion differen-
tiation to decision performance (b 1.55, t
Mediation Model
1.70), was statistically significant, and a previ-
By default, the basic hypothesized model speci- ously significant indirect path from affective reac-
fies that affective influence regulation fully medi- tivity to decision performance (path b) became non-
ates the relationships between positive emotion significant (b 0.68, t 1.55). To check whether
differentiation and decision performance and be- these nonsignificant results came from simply en-
tween negative emotion differentiation and deci- tering too many variables (six) into the equation,
sion performance. Thus, this model provides the given the small sample size (n 101), we further
basis for testing Hypothesis 4 (stating that affective dropped the control variables from the partial me-
influence regulation mediates the relationship be- diation model and reran the SEM analysis. How-
tween emotion differentiation and decision perfor- ever, the two added direct paths remained nonsig-
mance). The SEM results show that, although the nificant (t 0.34 and t 1.61, respectively), and
path coefficient from positive emotion differentia- the indirect path from affective reactivity to deci-
tion to affective influence regulation (path c) is sion performance (path b) became significant (b
positive, it is not significant (b 0.58, t 1.22). 0.87, t 2.02). The nonmediated model did not fit
However, the path coefficient from negative emo- the data well, with several fit indexes failing to
tion differentiation to affective influence regulation meet the requirements (2 7.06, df 1, p 0.01;
(path d) is both positive and significant (b 0.85, GFI .98, AGFI .47, RMSEA .25, CFI .95,
t 2.33, p 0.05); participants who reported their NFI .95) and with the two direct paths from
negative affective feelings in a more differentiated positive and negative emotion differentiation to de-
fashion were less influenced by their affective feel- cision performance being nonsignificant (t 0.21
ings in determining the level of risk in their daily and t 1.08, respectively).
stock portfolio. Thus, we retained the basic hypothesized mod-
Because both the path from negative emotion elthe fully mediated modelas having the best
differentiation to affective influence regulation fit, as predicted in Hypothesis 4. However, the non-
(path d) and the path from affective influence reg- significance of the path from positive emotion dif-
ulation to decision performance (path a) are signif- ferentiation to affective influence regulation makes
icant, a required condition for mediation is met. To Hypothesis 4 only partially supported; affective in-
further test the mediation hypothesis, we created fluence regulation fully mediates the effect of neg-
two other alternative models, the partial mediation ative, but not positive, emotion differentiation on
model and the nonmediated model, and compared decision performance.
them with the basic hypothesized model, as recom-
mended by Kelloway (1998). In the partial media-
DISCUSSION
tion model, we specified two direct paths, one from
positive emotion differentiation to decision perfor- Going contrary to the popular belief that the
mance and the other from negative emotion differ- cooler head prevails, the results of this study
entiation to decision performance, while retaining make it evident that feelings and emotions experi-
all other specifications in the basic hypothesized enced during decision making can have positive
model. In the nonmediated model, we dropped effects on decision-making performance. In this
three indirect paths, one from positive emotion dif- study, people with hot headsthose who expe-
ferentiation to affective influence regulation (path rienced their feelings with greater intensity during
c), one from negative emotion differentiation to decision makingachieved higher decision-mak-
934 Academy of Management Journal August

ing performance. This result also provides direct not interactively, influenced decision-making per-
counterevidence to Shiv and colleagues (2005) re- formance. Our additional finding that affective in-
cent finding that feelings can lead to suboptimal fluence regulation and affective reactivity were vir-
financial decision making in a narrowly defined tually uncorrelated (r .02) further explains the
situation (when the expected gain is greater than underlying reason: experiencing feelings (a func-
the expected loss). Consistently with another pop- tional process) and doing something with those
ular belief, Dont let your emotions run your life, feelings (a dysfunctional process) may be mutually
however, we found that individuals who better independent within an individual. These results
kept their feelings from having direct impacts on offer strong empirical support for a broader and
their decisions achieved higher decision-making integrative perspective on individual difference in
performance. This result confirms the dominant affective information processing (Gohm, 2003;
view in the literature on affect and decision making Gohm & Clore, 2000) that provides an important
that affective experiences produce various biases in theoretical basis for moving beyond simplistic
judgments and choices that must be properly regu- views on whether affective feelings are functional
lated to enhance decision-making performance. Yet or dysfunctional to decision making. Instead, it
the popular prescription for successful emotion helps researchers to explore and examine how var-
regulation, Ignore your emotions, appears, in ious individual-level characteristics (skills, traits,
view of our results, not to be the right answer for and abilities) create differences in how people ex-
effective regulation of feelings and their influence perience and handle their affective feelings during
on decision making. Instead, the results suggest decision making, thus ultimately determining their
exactly the opposite: individuals who better under- decision-making performance.
stood what was going on with their feelings during Third, this study may also contribute to the lit-
decision making and thus reported them in a more erature on emotional intelligence, ones ability to
specific and differentiated fashion were more suc- monitor ones own and others feelings and emo-
cessful in regulating the feelings influence on de- tions, to discriminate among them and to use this
cision making and, as a result, achieved higher information to guide ones thinking and actions
investment returns. (Salovey & Mayer, 1990: 189). This concept has
emerged as an area of intense interest, in both the
academic (Brackett & Mayer, 2003; Law, Wong, &
Theoretical Implications
Song, 2004) and lay (Goleman, 1995) communities.
Our findings extend previous research on affect In spite of the excitement regarding the heuristic
and decision making in three ways. First, this study value of emotional intelligence, however, there
extends the decision making literature, which has have been few rigorous scientific investigations re-
generally ignored the role of affective feelings or, at garding the underlying psychological components
best, focused only on their bias-inducing role (see and processes that constitute it (Barrett & Salovey,
Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, and Welch [2001] and 2002; Law et al., 2004). This study provides empir-
Slovic et al. [2002] for reviews). This study suggests ical evidence that emotion differentiation and af-
that both feelings and the ways people handle them fective influence regulation are two essential pro-
during decision making have important conse- cess components of emotional intelligence (Barrett
quences for decision-making outcomes. In particu- & Gross, 2001) that positively influence individual
lar, the results showed a strong support for an al- performance outcomes.
ternative view that feelings and emotions can One additional finding in this study that also has
enable and facilitate decision-making processes. an important theoretical implication is that people
This area has been relatively understudied in deci- achieved the benefit of successful affective influ-
sion making research. ence regulation from understanding and differenti-
Second, past studies on affect and decision mak- ating among their current negative feelings, but not
ing have been fragmented in the sense that they from differentiating among positive feelings. This
have focused on either the functional role of affec- result was consistent with the finding in Barrett
tive experience (e.g., Isen et al., 1987) or its dys- and her colleagues (2001) study that negative emo-
functional role (e.g., Au, Chan, Wang, & Vertinsky, tion differentiation, not positive emotion differen-
2003; Shiv et al., 2005). This study provides inte- tiation, led to greater self-regulation of emotions.
grative evidence that both the functional and dys- One explanation for these results is that the adap-
functional effects of affective feelings on decision tive pressure to respond to and actively regulate
making operate simultaneously within individuals, feelings is greater for negative feelings than for pos-
as the results showed that affective influence regu- itive feelings (Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1996; Pratto
lation and affective reactivity independently, but & John, 1991). Thus, when people experience their
2007 Seo and Barrett 935

current negative feelings as qualitatively distinc- strain experiencing and expressing feelings (Ash-
tive, they are more likely to actively regulate such forth & Humphrey, 1995). They should actively
feelings and their possibly negative consequences, remove such cognitive, normative, and behavioral
whereas they can be less responsive to positive barriers in their organizations, which might involve
feelings, regardless of whether they experience a tremendous amount of reeducation and unlearn-
those positive feelings as fully differentiated or un- ing. We hope the findings of this study can be used
differentiated feeling states. as legitimate bases for business leaders and man-
agers initiating such reeducation and unlearning.
However, a more challenging issue is how to min-
Managerial Implications imize the possibly negative influences of affective
feelings, once affective experience and expression
There are two ways in which the findings of this became more encouraged and less constrained in
study challenge the dominant view in managerial
workplaces.
discourse and practice that feelings are potentially
This study suggests one particular way in
dangerous factors hindering effective decision
which managers and employees can reduce the
making and thus must be suppressed or con-
possibly bias-generating effects of their current
strained in organizations (Ashforth & Humphrey,
1995; Putnam & Mumby, 1993). First, our findings affective states (and thus increase decision-mak-
suggest that affective experiences have the poten- ing performance). That is, they might increase the
tial to both facilitate and hinder decision making degree to which they attend to and clearly differ-
within individuals and to do so simultaneously. entiate among their current affective states dur-
The problem of the dominant view and related ing decision making. This can be achieved by
managerial practice is not that they are entirely conducting frequent self-audits (Forgas & Ciarro-
wrong, but that they are attempts to minimize the chi, 2002) of current feelings during decision
potentially negative effects of peoples feelings to- makingasking oneself, for instance, How am I
gether with all of the potentially positive effects, feeling right now? and trying to precisely de-
such as enhanced decision efficiency, engagement, scribe current feelings and understand why they
and creativitythus throwing the baby out with are being experienced. Employees and managers
the bath water. Second, and also contrary to the might also attempt to increase their general levels
dominant view, participants who were more igno- of emotional self-awareness (Ciarrochi et al.,
rant about and thus less able to identify their spe- 2003; Lane, Quinlan, Schwartz, Walker, & Zeit-
cific feeling states at the moment of decision mak- lin, 1990). To do this, they might need to acquire
ing performed worse in this study by being richer categorical knowledge of different emo-
influenced more by the feelings that they ignored. tional states (e.g., describing diverse distinctive
Instead, better performers were more attentive to feeling states), their underlying meanings (e.g.,
their current feeling states and better able to de- when those feelings are experienced and what
scribe them clearly during decision making. In par- people tend to think and do when they are expe-
ticular, the better performers could better distin- riencing those feelings), and the relationships of
guish among their negative feeling states, where the the states with each other (e.g., how feeling ner-
press for affective regulation is greatest (Barrett et
vous is similar to and different from feeling
al., 2001).
afraid, sad, excited, calm, and so forth).
Our study informs an alternative approach organ-
Mangers and employees could then use such en-
izations could take to feelings. This approach
hanced knowledge in describing their current
would be to foster managers and employees expe-
riencing and expressing their moods and emotions feelings clearly and in a well-differentiated fash-
to maximize the positive outcomes of those feel- ion (e.g., being able to say I am feeling angry as
ings, and simultaneously help them minimize the opposed to I am feeling bad). Our findings
feelings potential negative impacts. This approach place particular emphasis on developing manag-
is similar to bounded emotionality (e.g., Martin, ers and employees ability to describe and dif-
Knopoff, & Beckman, 1998) but speaks more di- ferentiate negative feelings during decision mak-
rectly to productivity issues than to employee well- ing. Such differentiation and expression may
being. More specifically, to foster experiencing and require a completely different set of abilities or
expressing various feelings and emotions in organ- skills from those required to differentiate positive
izations, managers and leaders may need to care- feelings (negative emotion differentiation was
fully reexamine common beliefs, norms, languages, uncorrelated with positive emotion differentia-
and practices that devalue, discourage, and con- tion in this study).
936 Academy of Management Journal August

Limitations and Future Research Directions plified our study results. Exploring both the con-
scious and subconscious processes of affective
Additional research is needed to address limita-
information processing and its effects on decision-
tions of this study and to advance understanding of
making performance seems a valuable future re-
the role of affective feelings in decision making.
search direction.
First, this study was based on a correlational re-
Fifth, although our cash reward ($100 1,000)
search design, which makes it impossible to deter-
may have been useful in making the simulation
mine the precise causal directions among the key
more realistic and inducing a broad spectrum of
variables. Supplemental studies with experimental
affective experiences during the simulation, it
designs in which affective feelings are experimen-
could have influenced some of the results by affect-
tally induced in participants are needed for this ing participants affective experience, decision
determination of causality. making, and performance simultaneously. More-
Second, we measured three individual character- over, the remuneration structure for the partici-
istics of affective information processing in this pants in this study was designed in a way that
studynamely, affective reactivity, emotion differ- prevented them from incurring any serious loss. As
entiation, and affective influence regulation by a result, the expected return for participating in this
directly computing scores from participants daily simulation was highly skewed toward the gain side
mood reports and daily stock investment decisions. (up to $1,000 cash remuneration), and the worst
This is a strength of this study, since the measure- performer still got paid $100 ($100 was subtracted
ments did not rely on participants perceptions of from the initial payment of $200). This imbalance
their affective characteristics, which have been in possible gains and losses in the remuneration
found to be quite different from their actual affec- structure may have led participants to be more
tive characteristics (Gohm & Clore, 2002). The flip sensitive to rewards and to experience more posi-
side of the strength is that these measures are too tive feelings than negative feelings. Future studies
domain-specific to effectively capture participants using different affect-inducing methods and/or a
general affective characteristics and/or may not be remuneration structure balanced between possible
reliably replicated in other studies or research con- gains and losses are needed to confirm our key
texts. In addition, calculations were based on cer- findings.
tain untested assumptions (e.g., linearity and equal Finally, more research is needed to explore and
variance), which could have compromised our re- examine individual-level characteristics that might
sults. Thus, a better approach future studies could have further influenced the results of this study.
adopt would be to use both the objective measures For example, in the experimental studies of Smith
and some other subjective measures of similar af- and Petty (1996), the bias-generating effect of neg-
fective characteristics, such as the emotional clarity ative affect (mood congruence recall) was found
scales introduced by Gohm and Clore (2000). only among individuals with low self-esteem, not
Third, although feelings are a booster for short- among those with high self-esteem. This result sug-
term memory capacity (Kitayama, 1997), they also gests that self-esteem positively moderates the re-
constitute salient information that takes up short- lationship between negative emotion differentia-
term memory capacity (Mackie & Worth, 1989). As tion and affective influence regulation by helping
a result, extremely intense feelings can substan- individuals generate counteremotional (positive)
tially absorb and thus directly interfere with an thoughts once they have experienced negative feel-
individuals short-term memory capacity or ability ings and consciously recognized them.
to attend, which might hurt decision-making per-
formance (Barrett, Tugade, & Engle, 2004; Mackie &
Worth, 1989, 1991; Necka, 1997). Thus, the rela- Conclusion
tionship between affective intensity and decision- Feelings are an indispensable part of peoples
making performance may be nonlinear (taking an individual and organizational lives and, more im-
inverted U-shape), and future research needs to portantly, powerful entities that can both benefit
determine the precise relationship. and harm choices and decisions. Yet the popular
Fourth, our focus in this study is the effects of approach has predominantly focused on under-
affective experience, which is consciously accessi- standing and minimizing the dysfunctional aspects
ble and describable, on decision-making perfor- of feelings. This study not only suggests that
mance. However, affective processes include both whether they are actually beneficial or harmful to
conscious and subconscious processes (Bargh & decisions may largely depend upon how people
Chartrand, 1999; Winkielman, Zajonc, & Schwarz, experience, treat, and use their feelings during de-
1997), and the latter may have suppressed or am- cision making, but also points to an alternative
2007 Seo and Barrett 937

approach in which both functional and dysfunc- Barrett, L. F., & Salovey, P. 2002. Introduction. In L. F.
tional effects of feelings are equally acknowledged Barrett & P. Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom in feeling:
and simultaneously managed to maximize their Psychological processes in emotional intelligence:
positive effects and minimize their negative effects. 1 8. New York: Guilford Press.
We invite more scholarly investigation of this alter- Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. 2004. Indi-
native approach and the ways in which it can be vidual differences in working memory capacity and
applied to various individual and organization- dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological
al practices. Bulletin, 130: 553573.
Bentler, P., & Wu, E. 1998. EQS for Windows. Encino,
CA: Multivariate Software.

REFERENCES Berkowitz, L., & Donnerstein, E. 1982. External validity is


more than skin deep: Some answers to criticisms of
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. 1995. Emotion in the laboratory experiments. American Psychologist, 37:
workplace: A reappraisal. Human Relations, 48: 97 245257.
125.
Bodie, Z., Kane, A., & Marcus, A. J. 2001. Essentials of
Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. 1997. A stitch in time: investments. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological
Bulletin, 121: 417 436. Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. 2003. Convergent, dis-
criminant, and incremental validity of competing
Au, K., Chan, F., Wang, D., & Vertinsky, I. 2003. Mood in measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and
foreign exchange trading: Cognitive processes and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29: 11471158.
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 91: 322338. Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. 2002. Organizational behav-
ior: Affect in the workplace. In S. T. Fiske, D. L.
Babin, C. E., & Donovan, W. J. 2000. Investing secrets of Schacter, & C. Zahn-Wexler (Eds.), Annual review
the masters: Applying classical investment ideas
of psychology, vol. 53: 279 307. Palo Alto, CA: An-
to todays turbulent markets. New York: McGraw
nual Reviews.
Hill.
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade,
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. 1999. The unbearable
J. R. 2000. Emotional experience in everyday life
automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54:
across the adult life span. Journal of Personality
462279.
and Social Psychology, 79: 644 655.
Barrett, L. F. 1997. The relationship among of momentary
Ciarrochi, J., Catuti, P., & Mayer, J. D. 2003. The distinc-
emotional experiences, personality descriptions,
tiveness and utility of a measure of trait emotional
and retrospective ratings of emotion. Personality
awareness. Personality and Individual Differences,
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23: 1100 1110.
34: 14771490.
Barrett, L. F. 1998. Discrete emotions or dimensions? The
role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition Conway, M., & Giannopoulos, C. 1993. Dysphoria and
and Emotion, 12: 579 599. decision making: Limited information use for evalu-
ations of multiattribute targets. Journal of Personal-
Barrett, L. F. 2004. Feelings or words? Understanding the ity and Social Psychology, 64: 613 623.
content in self-report ratings of experienced emo-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Damasio, A. R. 1994. Descartes error: Emotion, reason,
87: 266 281. and the human brain. New York: Avon Books.

Barrett, L. F., & Barrett, D. J. 2001. An introduction to Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. 1993. Depression and the
computerized experience sampling in psychology. impression-formation continuum: Piecemeal pro-
Social Science Computer Review, 19(2): 175185. cessing despite the availability of category informa-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Barrett, L. F., & Gross, J. J. 2001. Emotion representation
64: 636 645.
and regulation: A process model of emotional intel-
ligence. In T. Mayne & G. Bonnano (Eds.), Emotion: Elsbach, K. D., & Barr, P. S. 1999. The effects of mood on
Current issues and future directions: 286 310. individuals use of structured decision protocols.
New York: Guilford. Organization Science, 10: 181198.
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J. J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, Erber, R. 1991. Affective and semantic priming: Effect of
M. 2001. Knowing what youre feeling and knowing mood on category accessibility and inference. Jour-
what to do about it: Mapping the relation between nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27: 480
emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. 498.
Cognition and Emotion, 16: 713724. Erber, R., & Erber, M. W. 2000. The self-regulation of
Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. 1998. Independence and moods: Second thoughts on the importance of hap-
bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal piness in everyday life. Psychological Inquiry, 11:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74: 967984. 142148.
938 Academy of Management Journal August

Feldman, L. A. 1995. Valence focus and arousal focus: affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Per-
Individual differences in the structure of emotional sonality and Social Psychology, 85: 348 362.
experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Hatcher, L. 1998. A Step-by-step approach to using the
chology, 69: 153166. SAS system for factor analysis and structural
Forgas, J. P. 1995. Mood and judgment: The affective equation modeling (3rd ed.). Cary, NC: SAS Insti-
infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117: tute.
39 66. Hochschild, A. R. 1983. The managed heart. Berkeley:
Forgas, J. P. 2000. Managing moods: Towards a dual- University of California Press.
process theory of spontaneous mood regulation. Psy- Isen, A. M. 2000. Positive affect and decision making. In
chological Inquiry, 11: 172177. M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
Forgas, J. P. 2001. Introduction: Affect and social cogni- emotions (2nd ed.): 417 435. New York: Guilford
tion. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and Press.
social cognition: 124. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. 1987.
Forgas, J. P., & Ciarrochi, J. V. 2002. On managing moods: Positive affect facilitates creative problem-solving.
Evidence for the role of homestatic cognitive strate- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51:
gies in affect regulation. Personality and Social Psy- 11221131.
chological Bulletin, 28: 336 345. Isen, A. M., & Means, B. 1983. Positive affect as a variable
Fredrickson, B. L. 2001. The role of positive emotions in in decision making. Social Cognition, 2: 18 31.
positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory Johnson, E., & Tversky, A. 1983. Affect, generalization,
of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56: and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality
218 226. and Social Psychology, 45: 20 31.
Fredrickson, B. L. 2003. The value of positive emotions: Kelloway, E. K. 1998. Using LISREL for structural
The emerging science of positive psychology is com- equation modeling: A researchers guide. Thou-
ing to understand why its good to feel good. Amer- sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ican Scientist, 91: 330 335. Keltner D., Locke, K. D., & Audrain, P.C. 1993. The in-
Gohm, C. L. 2003. Mood regulation and emotional intel- fluence of attributions on the relevance of negative
ligence: Individual differences. Journal of Personal- feelings to personal satisfaction. Personality and
ity and Social Psychology, 84: 594 607. Social Psychology Bulletin, 19: 2129.
Gohm, C. L., & Clore, G. L. 2000. Individual differences in Ketelaar, T., & Clore, G. L. 1997. Emotion and reason: The
emotional experience: Mapping available scales to proximate effects and ultimate functions of emotion.
processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspec-
letin, 26: 679 697. tives on personality and emotion: 355395. New
York: Elsevier.
Gohm, C. L., & Clore, G. L. 2002. Affect as information:
An individual-difference approach. In L. Barrett & P. Kitayama, S. 1997. Affective influence in perception:
Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom of feeling: Psychologi- Some implications of the amplification model. In G.
cal processes in emotional intelligence: 89 113. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on
New York: Guilford Press. personality and emotion: 193257. New York:
Elsevier.
Goleman, D. 1995. Emotional intelligence. New York:
Bantam. Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E., Walker,
P. A., & Zeitlin, S. B. 1990. The levels of emotional
Goleman, D., Boyatzis, R., & McKee, A. 2001. Primal awareness scale: A cognitive-developmental mea-
leadership: The hidden driver of great performance. sures of emotion. Journal of Personality Assess-
Harvard Business Review, 79(11): 4251. ment, 55: 124 134.
Gottman, J. M., & Fainsilber-Katz, L. 1989. Effects of Larsen, R. J. 2000. Toward a science of mood regulation.
marital discord on young childrens peer interaction Psychological Inquiry, 11: 129 141.
and health. Developmental Psychology, 25: 373
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. 1987. Affect intensity as an
381.
individual difference characteristic: A review. Jour-
Gray, J. R. 1999. A bias toward short-term thinking in nal of Research in Personality, 21: 139.
threat-related negative emotional state. Personality
Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. 1986. Affect
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25: 6575.
intensity and reactions to daily life events. Journal
Gross, J. J. 1998. The emerging field of emotion regula- of Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 803 814.
tion: An integrative review. Review of General Psy-
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. 1991. Personality and suscep-
chology, 2: 271299. tibility to positive and negative emotional states.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. 2003. Individual differences in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61:
two emotion regulation processes: Implications for 132140.
2007 Seo and Barrett 939

Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., & Song, L. J. 2004. The construct Pratto, F., & John, O. P. 1991. Automatic vigilance: The
and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and attention-grabbing power of negative social informa-
its potential utility for management studies. Journal tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
of Applied Psychology, 89: 483 496. 61: 380 391.
LeDoux, J. E. 1993. Emotional memory systems in the Putnam, L. L., & Mumby, D. K. 1993. Organizations,
brain. Behavioral Brain Research, 58: 69 79. emotion and the myth of rationality. In S. Fineman
Lo, A.W. 2002. The psychophysiology of real-time finan- (Ed.), Emotion in organization: 36 57. London:
cial risk processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- Sage.
science, 14: 323339. Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. 1999. All negative
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anx-
2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127: iety and sadness on decision making. Organiza-
267286. tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
79: 56 77.
Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. 1989. Processing deficits
and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. 2000. Event sampling and other
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57: methods for studying daily experience. In H. T. Judd
27 40. & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods
in social and personality psychology: 190 222.
Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. 1991. Feeling good, but not
New York: Cambridge University Press.
thinking straight: The impact of positive mood on
persuasion. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social Russell, J. A. 2003. Core affect and the psychological
judgments: 201219. London: Pergamon. construction of emotion. Psychological Review,
110: 145172.
Martin, J., Knopoff, K., & Beckman, C. 1998. An alterna-
tive to bureaucratic impersonality and emotional la- Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. 1990. Emotional intelligence.
bor: Bounded emotionality at the body shop. Admin- Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9: 185
istrative Science Quarterly, 43: 429 469. 211.
Meyer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., &
T. W. 1992. Mood-congruent judgment is a general Palfai, T. P. 1995. Emotional attention, clarity, and
effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the
ogy, 63: 119 132. Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In J. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emo-
tion, disclosure, and health: 125154. Washington,
Meyer, J. D., Gayle, M., Meeham, M. E., & Harman, A. K.
DC: American Psychological Association.
1990. Toward a better specification of the mood-
congruency effect in recall. Journal of Experimental Schwarz, N. 1990. Feelings as information: Informational
Psychology, 26: 465 480. and motivational functions of affective states. In
E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. 1996. The dimensions,
motivation and cognition: Foundations of social
antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor.
behavior: 527561. New York: Guilford.
Academy of Management Review, 21: 986 1010.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. 1983. Mood, misattribution
Morris, W. N. 1989. Mood: The frame of mind. New
and judgments of well-being: Informative and direc-
York: Springer-Verlag.
tive functions of affective states. Journal of Person-
Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. 1990. The ality and Social Psychology, 45: 513523.
influence of mood on categorization: A cognitive
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. 1988. How do I feel about it?
flexibility interpretation. Journal of Personality and
The informative functions of affective states. In K.
Social Psychology, 59: 411 425.
Fiedler & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and
Necka, E. 1997. Attention, working memory and arousal: social behavior: 44 62. Toronto: Hogrefe Interna-
Concepts apt to account for the process of intelli- tional.
gence. In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science per-
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. 2003. Mood as information: 20
spectives and personality and emotion: 503553.
years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14: 296 303.
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Seo, M., Barrett, L. F., & Bartunek, J. M. 2004. The role of
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1996. The communi-
affective experience in work motivation. Academy
cative theory of emotions: Empirical tests, mental
models, and implications for social interaction. In of Management Review, 29: 423 439.
L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. 2002.
Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regula- Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and conse-
tion: 363393. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. quences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality
Ping, R. A. 1995. A parsimonious estimating technique and Social Psychology, 83: 12611280.
for interaction and quadratic latent variables. Jour- Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., &
nal of Marketing Research, 32: 336 347. Damasio, A. R. 2005. Investment behavior and the
940 Academy of Management Journal August

negative side of emotion. Psychological Science, 16: Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. 1991. Self-recording of everyday
435 439. life events: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Per-
Sinclair, R. C. 1988. Mood, categorization breath, and sonality, 59: 339 354.
performance appraisal: The effects of order of infor- Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. 1997. Sub-
mation acquisition and affective state on halo, accu- liminal affective priming resists attributional inter-
racy, information retrieval, and evaluations. Organi- ventions. Cognition and Emotion, 11: 433 465.
zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
42: 22 46.
Slovic, P. 2001. Psychological study of human judgment:
Implications for investment decision making. Jour-
nal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 2: 160 Myeong-Gu Seo (mseo@rhsmith.umd.edu) is an assistant
172. professor in the R. H. Smith School of Business at the
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. University of Maryland. He received his Ph.D. in organ-
2002. The affect heuristic. In G. D. Griffin & D. Kah- ization studies from Boston College. His research focuses
neman (Eds.), Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and on emotion in organization, organizational change and
biases: 397 420. New York: Cambridge University development, and institutional change.
Press.
Lisa Feldman Barrett (barretli@bc.edu) is currently a
Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. 1995. Personality moderators professor of psychology and the director of the Interdis-
of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of ciplinary Affective Science Laboratory at Boston College,
self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal
as well as an assistant in research for Harvard Medical
of Personality and Social Psychology, 68: 1092
School at Massachusetts General Hospital. She received
1107.
her Ph.D. in psychology from the University of Waterloo.
Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. 1993. Affect and managerial Her major research focus concerns the nature of emotion
performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. hap- from social-psychological, psychophysiological, cogni-
pier-and-smarter hypothesis. Administrative Sci- tive science, and neuroscience perspectives.
ence Quarterly, 38: 304 331.
Wells, A., & Matthews, G. 1994. Attention and emotion:
A clinical perspective. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.

You might also like