You are on page 1of 3

A Case of Misplaced Nostalgia

Ulric Neisser Emory University

To counter Banaji and Crowder's (1989) claim that the A single counterexample would suffice to refute null hy-
naturalistic study of memory has not been productive, potheses like these: I will list four for good measure. To
this reply cites four significant lines of research: Ross's be sure, there is something arbitrary about any such list;
(1989) theory of bias in recalling personal traits, Nelson's other students of memory in natural contexts have their
(1986) studies of children's event representations, Bah- own favorite studies, and I do not claim any unique priv-
rick's (1984) long-term follow-up of school learning, and ilege for mine. Some of the studies mentioned here com-
new work on "flashbulb memories" by Neisser and bine theory and data in a way that should appeal even to
Harsch. Banaji and Crowder's analogy to chemistry is Banaji and Crowder; others have produced results that
misleading." In biology, a more appropriate model, the would never have emerged from standard laboratory
importance of.field studies is taken for granted. And not studies. One (which, of course, I like especially) is partly
all the progress of the last 12 years has been on the eco- my own work. Four should be enough.
logical side; "traditional" memory research has now gone 1. People often have occasion to recall what they
well beyond the limitations noted by Neisser (1978), and were like at some earlier point in time. How bad were
both approaches are now moving ahead together. my headaches last week? How fast did I read before I
took this study-skills course? Was I happy in my college
days? Michael Ross (1989) developed a systematic theory
of such estimates. In his view they are largely derived
I have bad news for Banaji and Crowder (September from the trait's present value (my pain today, my reading
1989). It's too late: The good old days are gone, the genie speed now, my current happiness), taken together with
is out of the bottle. The situation I described and deplored an implicit hypothesis about its stability or change over
in 1978 ("If X is an interesting or socially significant as- time. If I have no reason to believe that my headaches
pect of memory, then psychologists have hardly ever have changed, for example, then recall of last week's pain
studied X," Neisser, 1978, p. 4) will never return, however will be biased toward today's level. If I have just been
much they may long for it. The present state of affairs is through a treatment program, however, recall of pain lev-
nearly the opposite. Nowadays, if X is an ecologically els before it began will be biased upward from today's
common or socially significant domain of memory, level--after all, the program must have done some good!
some'body is probably studying it intensively. Estimates In a recent issue of Psychological Review, Ross (1989)
of one's own traits at earlier points in time, memory in reported about a dozen studies, covering a wide range of
young children (and recollections of childhood), retrieval personally significant traits, that support his theory. (An
of material learned in school, recall of unusual or emo- independent measure of the original trait was available
tional experiences--these are only a few of the naturalistic in each case.) This work, which offers unexpected and
domains in which important findings have recently ap- yet systematic insights into a commonplace use of mem-
peared. The psychologists who made those findings will ory, meets every standard methodological criterion.
surely go on with their work; no amount of nostalgic 2. The use of everyday events as targets may be op-
grumbling about the proper methods of science is likely tional in studying adult memory, but it is essential with
to deter them. This being rather obviously the case, two young children. Using naturalistic methods, Katherine
questions present themselves: (a) Why does it bother Ba- Nelson (1986) and her associates showed that recall of
naji and Crowder so much? (b) How does it fit into the familiar routines as well as of specific episodes can be
study of memory as a whole? elicited from two-year-olds, and perhaps even (Nelson,
But perhaps it is not the case. Banaji and Crowder 1988) from one-year-olds. Such recall is quite sketchy at
(1989) simply denied it. According to them, the study of first, but interestingly, it is rarely wrong. It takes an in-
everyday memory (an awkward phrase, I think) has not creasingly rich narrative form during development, as
produced any important results at all. That claim must parents instruct their children in the social uses of mem-
be refuted before we proceed; otherwise my argument ory (Hudson, in press). Some characteristics of young
would rest on shaky ground. Luckily, Banaji and Crowder children's m e m o r y - - i t s dependence on external cueing;
phrased it so sweepingly that refutation is easy: its focus on things that adults would take for granted--
No delivery has been made on these claims: No theories that may help to explain the "childhood amnesia" that will
have unprecedented explanatory power have been produced; no overtake their recall later (Fivush & Hamond, in press).
new principles of memory have been discovered; and no methods 3. Harry Bahrick's (1984) studies of memory for
of data collection have been developed that add sophistication school-learned material are now well-known. By locating
or precision. (p. 1185; all italics added) people who had studied Spanish at various times in th~

34 January 1991 American Psychologist


Copyright 1991 by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. 0003-066X/91/$2.00
Vol. 46, No. 1, 34-36
past (and controlling other variables with covariance Perhaps they can; certainly they have no conception
techniques), Bahrick was able to trace out 50-year for- of function in psychology. Discussions of what mecha-
getting functions. Although some aspects of those func- nisms are "good for" leave them frankly puzzled (Banaji
tions were predictable from traditional theories of mem- & Crowder, 1989, p. 1191). Fortunately, other psychol-
ory (e.g., effects of the level of original learning), others ogists have found the concept less mysterious. Modern
were not: The rate of forgetting dropped to zero after 5 interest in functional and ecological analysis is not con-
years or so, producing a 25-year plateau in performance. fined to memory alone; it has already influenced research
These data, which could not have been obtained by stan- on perception (Gibson, 1979), conceptualization (Medin
dard laboratory methods, have obvious theoretical and & Wattenmaker, 1987; Mervis, 1987; Rosch, 1978), de-
practical significance. velopment (Ceci & Bronfenbrenner, 1985), and cognitive
4. Banaji and Crowder (1989) believe that so-called linguistics (Lakoff, 1987). Instead of merely recombining
flashbulb memories, such as recalling how one first heard the elements and testing the hypotheses of a traditional
the news of the explosion of the space shuttle Challenger, mental chemistry, as Banaji and Crowder would appar-
"present nothing unexpected to conventional laboratory ently recommend, such investigators try to study variables
work on memory" (p. 1190). They probably mean that that play functional roles in natural settings. It is hard to
although such memories are roughly accurate, no special believe that we would be better offwithout the discoveries
memory mechanism is involved. That is the view of that they have made.
McCloskey, Wible, and Cohen (1988), whose study they Anyway, that is not one of the options. The ecological
cited. But recent work suggests that the real puzzle about approach is here to stay, in the study of memory as else-
flashbulb memories is quite different: Why do people so where in psychology. There are certain to be more and
often have vivid recollections that are entirely incorrect? more naturalistic studies in the years to come. Many of
Nicole Harsch and I asked Emory University freshmen, them will be less than outstanding in quality, but that's
on the morning after the Challenger disaster, how they part of science: No one rejects evolutionary biology out
had heard the news the day before (Harsch & Neisser, of hand just because some Darwinian studies are flawed.
1989; Neisser & Harsch, in press). Three years later, 44 I understand Banaji and Crowder's (1989) impatience
of them were asked to recall it again. Most gave very with weak studies. My own critique of traditional memory
plausible and confident accounts, but about one third of research (Neisser, 1978) was fueled by impatience of a
those accounts were dead wrong. Although errors do often similar kind. But it is inappropriate to react, as they did,
occur in eyewitness testimony (Loftus, 1979), neither folk by arguing that the only acceptable scientific protocol is
psychology nor conventional theories of memory had led that of the fully controlled laboratory study. There is more
us to expect so many utterly false reports. Not all forms than one way to skin a phenomenon.
of everyday memory are prone to such errors. For ex-
ample, they do not occur in recall of randomly selected Where Does It Fit?
events (Brewer, 1988). Clarification of these issues will The ecological approach is not the only source of new
probably require continued naturalistic research: The ideas about remembering. The "traditional" study of
strong emotions and long retention intervals characteristic memory itself has undergone radical changes since I
of flashbulb memories are not easily established under characterized it so harshly in 1978. At that time it was
laboratory conditions. chiefly focused on explicit recognition or recall of isolated
Why Does it Bother Banaji and Crowder? items from lists. Today, a glance at the Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology." Learning, Memory, and Cognition
Banaji and Crowder's (1989) use of analogies from chem- shows that such research has become almost an endan-
istry and physics is no accident. Despite a rather curious gered species. Currently popular topics include implicit
disclaimer (p. 1191), they obviously believe, as almost memory, mental imagery, motor skills, story schemata,
everyone once did, that psychology should seek universal social scripts, object recognition, and cognitive maps--
context-free laws like those of the classical sciences. But many of them appreciably closer (than the old methods
physics is no longer the unchallenged model of science. were) to the sorts of things people do everyday. Whether
Modern biology has demonstrated the enormous power laboratory study of such problems will produce what Ba-
of functional analysis coupled with naturalistic obser- naji and Crowder (1989) called "truly generalizable prin-
vation. Banaji and Crowder asked their readers, mock- ciples" (p. 1192) remains to be seen--or rather, to be
ingly, to imagine doing chemistry without laboratory tested across the relevant ecological settings. (Taken by
controls. Can they imagine evolutionary biology, or ecol- themselves, encapsulated laboratory experiments can
ogy, or ethology, without field studies? Without some never ensure true generalizability.) But even without such
analysis of the function of body parts and behavior pat- tests, some of these studies clearly meet what Eugene
terns? Winograd (1988) has called "the one requirement for sci-
entific research we all implicitly follow: our understanding
of memory is enhanced" (p. 18).
Correspondence concerningthis article should be addressed to Ulric Much ecologically oriented work on memory also
Neisser, Department of Psychology,Emory University,Atlanta, GA meets that test. The research described here is only a
30322. sample. There is not enough space to describe the many

January 1991 American Psychologist 35


e x c e l l e n t studies o f oral t r a d i t i o n , prospective r e m e m - Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories
bering, conversational use o f the past, personal recall, a n d reveal about the mind. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Lofius, E. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
eyewitness t e s t i m o n y t h a t have also e n h a n c e d o u r u n - versity Press.
d e r s t a n d i n g i n the last few years. F a r f r o m " c o m p r o m i s i n g McCloskey, M., Wible, C. G., & Cohen, N. J. (1988). Is there a special
g e n u i n e a c c o m p l i s h m e n t s o f o u r y o u n g e n d e a v o r " (Banaji flashbulb memory mechanism? Journal of experimental psychology:
& Crowder, 1989, p. 1185), this w o r k is a n i n t e g r a l p a r t General 117, 171-181.
Medin, D. L., & Wattenmaker, W. D. (1987). Category cohesiveness,
o f those a c c o m p l i s h m e n t s . N o serious s t u d e n t o f m e m o r y theories, and cognitive archeology. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and
c a n afford to i g n o r e it. conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in cate-
gorization (pp. 25-62). New York: Cambridge University Press.
REFERENCES
Mervis, C. B. (1987). Child-basic object categories and early iexical de-
Bahrick, H. P. (1984). Semantic memory content in permastore: Fifty velopment. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development:
years of memory for Spanish learned in school. Journal of Experi- Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 201-233).
mental Psychology: General 113, 1-29. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Banaji, M. R., & Crowder, R. G. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday Neisser, U. (1978). Memory: What are the important questions? In
memory. American Psychologist, 44, 1185-1193. M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects
Brewer, W. E (1988). Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical of memory (pp. 3-24). London: Academic Press.
events. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsid- Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (in press). Phantom flashbulbs: False recol-
ered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory lections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd & U.
(pp. 21-90). New York: Cambridge University Press. Neisser (Eds.), Flashbulb memories: Recalling the "Challenger" ex-
Ceci, S. J., & Bronfenbrenner, U. (1985). Don't forget to take the cupcakes plosion and other disasters. New York: Cambridge University Press.
out of the oven: Strategic time-monitoring, prospective memory, and Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in devel-
context. Child Development, 56, 175-190. opment. Hillsdale, N J: Eribaum.
Fivush, R., & Hamond, N. R. (in press). Autobiographical memory Nelson, K. (1988). The ontogeny of memory for real events. In U. Neisser
across the preschool years: Toward reconceptualizing childhood am- & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and
nesia. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering traditional approaches to the study of memory (pp. 244-276). New
in young children. New York: Cambridge University Press. York: Cambridge University Press.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecologicalapproach to visual perception. Boston: Roseh, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd
Houghton Mittlin. (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27-48). Hiiisdale, NJ: Erl-
Harsch, N., & Neisset; U. (1989, November). Substantial and irreversible baum.
errors in flashbulb memories of the Challenger explosion. Poster pre- Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of
sented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Atlanta, GA. personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341-357.
Hudson, J. A. (in press). The emergence of autobiographic memory in Winograd, E. (1988). Continuities between ecological and laboratory
mother-child conversation. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), approaches to memory. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remem-
Knowing and remembering in young children. New York: Cambridge bering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study
University Press. of memory (pp. 11-20). New York: Cambridge University Press.

36 J a n u a r y 1991 A m e r i c a n Psychologist

You might also like