You are on page 1of 9

PERSPECTIVES

in cognitive arousal; and incongruity reso-


SCIENCE AND SOCIETY
lution associated with amusement 6,1214.
Together, these frameworks for humour
The neural basis of humour point to the breadth and depth of humours
role and functionality in human experience.
processing Here, we propose that rigorous scientific
study of the neural basis of humour will shed
light on the importance of humour to the
Pascal Vrticka, Jessica M.Black and Allan L.Reiss human experience. Our understanding of
how individual variation influences commu-
Abstract | Humour is a vital component of human socio-affective and cognitive nication and adaptation (both in health and in
functioning. Recent advances in neuroscience have enabled researchers to illnesses in which humour may be adversely
explore this human attribute in children and adults. Humour seems to engage a affected; for example, brain disorders such as
core network of cortical and subcortical structures, including temporo-occipito- autism, major depression and schizophrenia)
parietal areas involved in detecting and resolving incongruity (mismatch between may also be enhanced through brain-based
research. The aim of this article is to provide a
expected and presented stimuli); and the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic
comprehensive summary of the recent func-
system and the amygdala, key structures for reward and salience processing. tional MRI (fMRI) findings that shed light on
Examining personality effects and sex differences in the neural correlates of the neurobiological basis of humour apprecia-
humour may aid in understanding typical human behaviour and the neural tion in humans and to outline directions for
mechanisms underlying neuropsychiatric disorders, which can have dramatic future research. We provide a brief overview
of the relevant mental and social operations
effects on the capacity to experience social reward.
that are inherent to humour processing, fol-
lowed by a detailed discussion of brain-based
Laughter occurs in all cultures worldwide an essential component of humour, particu- fMRI studies that examine humour and its
and is a universal component of the human larly so if playful6. In line with this theory, potential clinical relevance. We conclude with
experience1,2. In human infants, laughter a central function of humour is to maintain a brief summary and offer perspectives on
is one of the first social vocalizations3, and social order and to reinforce social bond- the future direction of neuroscientific studies
laughters early onset (at approximately ing 9 by allowing people to express disa- ofhumour.
4months of age) in response to the actions greeable feelings in a more positive way. A
of others suggests that it has innate com- related theory, tension-relief, conceptual- Evolution and benefits of humour
ponents1,2,4. In contrast to laughter, which izes humour as a mechanism for physi- Humour can lead to positive emotion15 and
is generally understood to be a reflex-like ological release of tension6. Hence, people is thought to serve important evolutionary
physiologicalbehavioural response, humour experience humour and engage in laughter socio-emotional purposes. Extant find-
is believed to represent a rather complex because it dispels pentup stress9. Another ings highlight its key role in building and
higher-order emotional process5. Specifically, potential role of humour in society is sug- maintaining relationships, emotional health
humour is a broad term that refers to any- gested by linking it with sexual selection and cognitive function6. Humour helps us
thing that people say or do that is considered theory 10,11. In so doing, humour is proposed to communicate ideas, attract partners,
funny and tends to make others laugh, as to act as a fitness indicator that provides boost mood and cope in times of trauma
well as the mental processes that go into mating partners with information about and stress1622. These beneficial manifesta-
both creating and perceiving such an amus- underlying mate quality, especially for tions are complemented at the physiological
ing stimulus, and also the affective response women judging men. In addition to these level, with humour acting as a natural stress
in the enjoyment of it (REF.6). Despite such three theories on the functional role of antagonist that can potentially enhance
multiple usages and definitions of humour, humour, one prominent cognitive humour the cardiovascular, immune and endocrine
nearly all of us can easily recognize humour theory attempts to account for how peo- systems2327. Examples of such positive
when we experience it7,8. ple understand humour. The incongruity effects of humour on physiology include
There are many theories that explain detection and resolution theory suggests faster cardiovascular recovery, decreased
the pervasive role of humour in society. that humour requires two elements: the cortisol levels after stress and improved
Among these, three theories are related to introduction of incongruity, created by the natural killer cell activity. Furthermore, as a
the functional role of humour in a proximal simultaneous presence of two habitually prototypical positive human cognitive state,
(psychology and physiology) or ultimate incompatible elements, which produces humour can increase life satisfaction by
(natural selection) sense. According to the an unexpected violation of expectations, building resilience28. Resilience is defined as
superiority theory, aggression is regarded as convention, fact or intention, and results the ability of most people, when exposed to

860 | DECEMBER 2013 | VOLUME 14 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

extraordinary levels of stress and trauma, to Functional neuroanatomy of humour humour processing positively correlates with
maintain normal psychological and physi- During the past 15years, several fMRI stud- subjective funniness ratings (FIG.2; TABLE1).
cal functioning and avoid serious mental ies (predominantly in adults) have probed This is in agreement with results from one
illness29. Although the above work provides the neural substrates of humour apprecia- fMRI investigation36 showing that subjective
some evidence that humour can have heal- tion in humans (TABLE1). In these studies, funniness rating or labelling during humour
ing effects, it is important to recognize that the stimulus modalities used can be funda- appreciation does not disrupt the brains
more rigorous research is still needed, par- mentally dissociated into two groups: verbal response to humour but even seems to
ticularly studies including control groups and visual. Verbal stimuli comprise either have sustaining effects, particularly in brain
and applying established (psychological) written or auditory information and can be regions associated with emotion.
definitions of humour 30,31. further differentiated into phonological ver- Despite the use of various stimulus modal-
Although we share some of the basic sus semantic jokes, funny versus nonsense ities recruiting distinct brain areas in a task-
properties underlying laughter and smiling or garden path punchlines, and (non)funny specific manner, as well as the application of
with other hominids, mainly related to their versus (un)ambiguous sentences versus different analysis approaches (main effects
association with a social safety and play sig- noise. In turn, visual stimuli differ mainly contrasts versus parametric modulation),
nal (BOX1), humour defined as mental play according to their manner of presentation, there is a convergence of the findings indi-
with words and objects and conceptualized as which can be static (for example, cartoon cating two dissociable, albeit interdepend-
enjoying incongruity (REF.32) is recognized images) or dynamic (for example, short ent, core processes of humour appreciation
as a human-specific characteristic4. movie clips) (also see BOX2). Accordingly, inhumans.
Given these considerations, it is surpris- humour appreciation has been found to A cognitive component is reliably asso-
ing that far less research attention is paid to activate a large set of cortical and subcorti- ciated with (residual30) incongruity detec-
elucidating the development and function cal brain areas subserving many cognitive tion and resolution, which is also referred
of positive emotional states in humans, such and emotional functions (see below and to as humour comprehension in a recent
as those attained through humour, than FIG.1). Similar activation patterns seem to verbal humour processing model14. This
of basic negative emotional states such as emerge when using parametric data analysis cognitive component is thought to rely
fear 3335 (but also see REF.26). procedures to capture brain areas in which fundamentally on basic visual, auditory
and/or verbal processing (in a task- and
stimulus-modality-dependent manner),
Box 1 | The evolution of humour in humans: genetic and cultural influences as maintained by activity in the visual and
auditory cortices; and on the activation of
Humour in humans is thought to be tightly linked structurally and conceptually with the expression language and semantic knowledge areas,
of Duchenne laughter1, a genetically predisposed4,91 and inherently positive emotion that elicits
including the (particularly left) inferior fron-
genuine or real smiles92. Derived from the primate relaxed open-mouth play face93, such
Duchenne laughter is primarily elicited in situations in which a sudden unexpected change in
tal gyrus (IFG; Brodmann area 45 (BA45),
events occurs within a safe social surrounding. This includes rough-and-tumble play, tickling, BA46 and BA47) and the temporal pole
physical mishaps and pleasant surprise in infants (for example, a peekaboo face expressed by a (TP; BA38). In the case of stimuli requiring
parent), which are referred to as proto-humour (REF.11). By contrast, Duchenne laughter elicited the juxtaposition of mental states (theory
by incongruity-based conceptual humour, primarily in adults, is associated with formal attempts of mind (ToM)), humour comprehension
at inducing laughter11. However, proto-humour and Duchenne laughter are often tightly linked will also recruit activity in cortical midline
with one another. Darwin associated humour with tickling of the mind (REF.94), and there seems structures, including the medial prefrontal
to be a relationship between the propensity to laugh when tickled and to laugh at and use humour cortex (mPFC), posterior cingulate cortex
(in adults)95. Together, the spontaneous laughter of human infants, tickling and formal adult (PCC) and precuneus (PREC), as well as the
humour all share what is essentially a phylogenetically and ontogenetically conserved structure
(anterior and posterior) superior temporal
and context, referred to as non-serious social incongruity11.
Despite their evolutionary origin being linked with genetic predispositions, Duchenne laughter
gyrus (STG) and superior temporal sulcus
and humour are not considered to be completely resistant to modulatory influence. In this (STS). Finally, because incongruity can also
context, cultural norms and related learning mechanisms are mentioned as key shaping factors11. involve error detection or monitoring, dorsal
A good illustration of such relations is the cultural variation observed in the subject matter of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activation
humour, which can vary from toilet- and sex-based humour to political humour and span a wide has been reported in such contexts (for
spectrum of cultural institutions and customs11. Furthermore, it is known that learning processes references, see TABLE1). After a thorough
within different cultures strongly influence the context, frequency, intensity and expression of review of the literature, we suggest that all
laughter as a function of display rules and varying norms and customs11. Along these lines, the of these mechanisms converge towards a
comparison of three studies investigating the genetic versus environmental components of core processing area of incongruity detec-
humour in adult monozygotic versus dizygotic twins from the United Kingdom, Australia and
tion and resolution, which not only includes
North America is noteworthy96. The principal measure was individual differences in humour
expression according to four distinct humour styles, of which two are positive (affiliative and
the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ; BA22,
self-enhancing) and two are negative (aggressive and self-defeating). The results revealed that, BA39 and BA40) but also extends ventrally
for studied twins from Australia and the United Kingdom, additive genetic and environmental into the temporo-occipito-parietal junction
factors accounted for the variance in all four humour styles. For twins from North America, (TOPJ; BA37, BA39 and BA40)37. This area
additive genetic and environmental factors accounted for the variance in the two positive of the human brain receives multimodal
humour styles but not for the two negative humour styles, for which variance was accounted for input from different sensory afferents and
solely by environmental factors. Such findings probably demonstrate cross-cultural differences in is also known to be activated during self-
what are deemed to be acceptable uses of humour and suggest that there is more cultural related processing and ToM. Furthermore,
pressure surrounding negative rather than positive humour. However, future studies from it is involved in the detection of unexpected
different cultures are needed to confirm and extend these findings.
stimuli of behavioural relevance and linked

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 14 | DECEMBER 2013 | 861

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

Table 1 | List of all fMRI (and one PET) studies on humour in humans included in the meta-analyses*
Stimulus modality Contrast depicted Summary of main findings Refs
Studies including main effects of humour contrasts
Visual static ToM cartoon versus non-ToM cartoon Activity during ToM stories and ToM cartoons overlapped in the 102
mPFC (paracingulate cortex)
Verbal written versus ToM versus non-ToM for stories versus
visual static cartoons
Visual static versus verbal ToM versus non-ToM for cartoons
written versus stories
Verbal auditory Semantic funny versus non-funny The processing of semantic versus phonological jokes produced 100
differential activity in the posterior MTG, posterior ITG and IPG but
Phonological funny versus non-funny overlapped in the vmPFC
Semantic versus phonological funny
Phonological versus semantic funny
Funny versus baseline (semantic and
phonological)
Visual dynamic Natural amusement funny versus Laughter or smile induced by visual comics (as opposed to 103
instructed smiling non-funny voluntary movement) increased activity in the visual cortex, ATP, (PET study)
uncus, OFC and mPFC
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Humour increased activity in the TOC, IFG, ATP, SMA, dACC, 104
mesocorticolimbic reward areas, hypothalamus and AMG
Visual dynamic Humour comprehension Humour comprehension (getting the joke) entailed increased 105
activity in the inferior frontal and posterior cortices, whereas
Humour elaboration humour elaboration (experience of mirth) activated the insula and
AMG
Visual dynamic Funny versus non-funny Passive viewing of funny (versus neutral) films led to increased 36
activity in the insula, ATP, STG, MTG and CUN
Visual static ToM versus physical cartoons ToM (versus physical) cartoons entailed increased activity in the 69
PREC, IPL and MTG (in healthy control individuals only)
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Humour (versus neutral) increased BOLD signal change in the FG, 106
STG, MTG, IFG and cerebellum
Visual static with caption Humour for language-based gag High-level visual areas activated more strongly during visual 97
versus sight gag humour; classic language areas activated more strongly during
language-dependent humour; a common network activated
Visual static without Humour for sight gag versus for both types of humour comprising the AMG and midbrain
caption language-based gag (associated with amusement)
Visual static overall Funny versus non-funny
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Humour (versus neutral) entailed stronger BOLD signal change in 107
the sensorimotor cortex, SMA, PFC, TOPJ, MTG, ATP, AMG, PHG,
thalamus, putamen, midbrain and cerebellum
Visual static ToM versus semantic versus visual Semantic puns and incongruity resolution activated a (left-sided) 98
puns versus irresolvable incongruity network including the TPJ, IFG and vmPFC; visual puns showed
more activity in the extrastriate cortex; ToM cartoons increased
activation in the SFG, mPFC, TPJ, aSTS, ATP and FG
Visual static Incongruity resolution versus Incongruity resolution (versus nonsense) recruited the mPFC, SFG 58
nonsense and TPJ
Visual static Cartoon versus neutral Humour (versus neutral) increased activity in the angular gyrus, 63
SFG, ACC, PREC, thalamus, MTG and cerebellum
Visual dynamic High versus low funniness High (versus low funny) clips elicited stronger activation in the 108
mesocorticolimbic areas, TPJ, SMA and IFG
Verbal auditory Funny versus non-funny Humour (versus neutral) increased BOLD signal change in the 109
MTG, midbrain, AMG, cingulate, visual cortex, ATP, OFC, FG, IFG,
SFG and ACC
Visual dynamic (children) Funny versus non-funny Funny (versus non-funny) movies increased activity in the TOPJ and 37
midbrain; funny (versus positive) movies entailed stronger activity
Funny versus positive in the STG and SMG

Verbal written Funny versus garden path Funny (versus garden path) sentences increased activity in the 101
AMG, midbrain and PHG
Verbal written Funny versus nonsense Funny (versus nonsense) sentences entailed stronger activity in the 14
SFG and IPL

862 | DECEMBER 2013 | VOLUME 14 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

Table 1 (cont.) | List of all fMRI (and one PET) studies on humour in humans included in the meta-analyses*
Stimulus modality Contrast depicted Summary of main findings Refs
Visual dynamic (children) Funny versus non-funny Funny (versus neutral) entailed increased activity in the IPL, 46
midbrain and bilateral PCG
Funny versus positive Funny (versus positive) entailed stronger activity in the STG, TPJ,
midbrain, pSTS and OCC
Studies including parametric modulation of funniness
Verbal auditory Funny versus baseline Activity in the vmPFC overlapping for both phonological and 100
semantic jokes correlated positively with funniness ratings
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Activity in response to humour in the mesocorticolimbic areas, 104
IFG, ATP and TOC correlated positively with funniness ratings
Visual dynamic Funny versus non-funny BOLD signal change to funny films correlated positively with 36
funniness ratings in the SFG, IFG, ACC, insula, STG, thalamus,
caudate, putamen and cerebellum
Visual static Funny versus non-funny BOLD signal change to humour correlated positively with funniness 106
ratings in the cerebellum, FG, STG, IFG, MTG, AMG and PHG
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Increased scores of funniness correlated positively with brain 97
activity in response to humour in the STS, MTG, mesocorticolimbic
areas, hippocampus, STG, SFG and cingulate
Visual static Funny versus non-funny Positive correlation between funniness ratings and humour 110
appreciation in the mPFC, insula, basal ganglia, STG and
cerebellum
Visual static Funny versus neutral Funniness ratings correlated positively with humour activity in the 63
SFG, ACC and lingual gyrus
Visual dynamic High versus low funny Activity in response to funny movies correlated positively with 108
funniness ratings in the cerebellum, TPJ, SMA, mesocorticolimbic
areas, STS, PHG, ITG, AMG, motor cortex and ATP
ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AMG, amygdala; aSTS, anterior STS; ATP, anterior temporal pole; BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; CUN, cuneus; dACC,
dorsal ACC; FG, fusiform gyrus; fMRI, functional MRI; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; IPG, inferior pre-central gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; ITG, inferior temporal
gyrus; mPFC, medial PFC; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; OCC, occipital cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PCG, post-central gyrus; PET, positron emission
tomography; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PREC, precuneus; pSTS, posterior STS; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor
area; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; TOC, temporo-occipital cortex; ToM, theory of mind; TOPJ, temporo-
occipito-parietal junction; TPJ, temporo-parietal junction; vmPFC, ventral mPFC. *The investigations are separated into those using a main effects contrast
approach (FIG.1) and those using a parametric modulation analysis with subjective funniness scores (FIG.2). Some studies included both types of analyses. The
stimulus modality used, principal contrast (or contrasts) and main findings are also listed. Studies are sorted by year of publication, ascending.

to increased connectivity with ventral fron- blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) funny stimuli to a neutral control condition
toparietal areas associated with attention and signal in mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic but not to a similarly positive state without
decision making 3841. The TOPJ therefore areas are known to increase during various humour. There are only two investigations
seems ideally suited for incongruity detec- reward-related responses45, and such activa- to date that used such a positive-state con-
tion and resolution. However, it should tions are also commonly reported by means trol37,46. Although these studies were con-
be noted that incongruity detection and of correlational analyses with subjective fun- ducted in children, they show evidence that
resolution have not yet been functionally niness ratings (FIG.2; TABLE1). Accordingly, humour appreciation differs from a more
and anatomically dissociated, because they this is generally understood to represent a generalized response to reward; this differ-
occur in rapid temporal succession (virtu- positive feeling of mirth or reward in the ence is probably related to the satisfaction
ally at the same time), making it difficult to course of humour appreciation, which is of detecting and resolving the incompatible
separate them with current fMRI methods. also referred to as humour elaboration14. elements of humour. More extensive testing
Investigations using electroencephalography The exact nature of positivity associated of optimal control conditions for humour
or magnetoencephalography might be better with humour, however, is not yet completely studies is required.
suited to address this issue4244. understood. This is probably for several Humour is also reliably associated
An emotional component is also con- reasons. First, increased subjective ratings of with activation of the amygdala (FIGS1,2).
sistently found to be involved in humour funniness also correlate with BOLD signal Although the human amygdala is known
appreciation. Although this emotional change during humour processing in cogni- to be involved in reward-related mecha-
component recruits the insula, the ventral tive areas comprising the TPJ, TP, mPFC, nisms47, its functional profile is more
ACC and the supplementary motor area ACC, PCC and PREC (FIG.2). Therefore, comprehensively understood to resemble
(SMA), it is primarily associated with heightened funniness scores could also be a relevance detector 4850. The amygdala
increased activity in mesocorticolimbic linked to humour properties other than is attributed a key role in selecting from
dopaminergic brain areas (that is, the the basic sense of reward typically linked a constant incoming stream of diverse
ventral tegmental area, substantia nigra, with dopaminergic signalling. Second, and information those inputs that are most
nucleus accumbens, ventral striatum and related to this notion, most neuroimaging relevant to the goals or intentions of the
ventral mPFC) (FIG.1). Changes in the studies of humour appreciation compare organism at a given moment in time. Such

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 14 | DECEMBER 2013 | 863

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

Box 2 | Different types of humour used in fMRI research


The extant literature using functional MRI (fMRI) to study humour
includes different types of stimuli for humour induction. Below is a a
summary of the most prominent categories, which are dissociated by
stimulus modality (verbal versus visual) and presentation mode (static
versus dynamic).
Static visual stimuli
Visual pun. This kind of humour uses visual resemblance as the main
element inducing incongruity, which is usually achieved by one (or
more) part of an image having different possible meanings. In the
provided example (see the figure, part a), the diagonal line can stand
for the sea (activated through the fin) or the mountain (activated
through the skis).
Semantic pun. Such humour is based on pure semantic relations and not
visual resemblance (as in a visual pun). In the provided example (see the
figure, part b), the patient has died, which can be seen on the monitor in
the form of an angel flying away. There is no visual resemblance
between the angel and the expected flatline, which indicates no
heartbeat on an electrocardiogram.
b
Theory of mind humour. In contrast to visual and semantic puns, this kind
of humour requires mentalizing abilities in order to get the joke. In the
given example (see the figure, part c), one has to understand that the
woman does not know what will happen to her, whereas the man does;
such discrepancy in subjective knowledge represents the central element
of incongruity.
Language-dependent visual humour. Under certain circumstances,
incongruity during visual humour perception is introduced through the
image caption and not the image (that is, the drawing, cartoon, and so on).
This is differentiated from sight gags containing a legend that are still
experienced as funny when the caption is removed97.
Control conditions for static visual humour usually consist of images
with an irresolvable incongruity98 or stimuli in which the incongruent
(funny) element has been removed97.
Dynamic visual stimuli
Such humour is usually presented by means of short movie clips, taken
either from professional comedy programmes or from amateur footage,
including scenes in which humans and/or animals display unusual
behaviour and/or the shown action takes an unexpected (incongruous) c
twist. Some studies use full-length episodes of a comedy series, in which
humour will not be purely visual: there will also be an incompatibility
between behaviour and speech comparable to language-dependent
visual humour (see above).
Control stimuli normally include comparable neutral scenes with no
funny elements. In rare cases, positive or rewarding but non-funny scenes
have been used as more appropriate controls37,46,61.
Verbal stimuli
There is a wide variety of verbal jokes that originate from different
structural levels of language99. These are phonetic, (lexico-)semantic,
morphological, phraseological and syntactical. Phonetic and semantic
jokes are the main types of jokes used in fMRI humour research.
Phonetic jokes. The most commonly used underlying principle is
ambiguity caused by an identical or very similar pattern of sounds
conveying different meanings. For example: Why did the golfer wear two
sets of pants? He got a hole in one. (REF.100).
Semantic jokes. In this type of humour, there is either a deviation
against lexico-semantic rules or a violation at the pragmatic Different baselines have been used for verbalReviews
Nature jokes. Usually, the
| Neuroscience
communicative level that introduces ambiguities in the interpretation punchline is exchanged with a logical or congruent statement. However,
of the described situation. This can be achieved by the use of synonyms in some cases, the punchline has been modified to include either
or antonyms (words with the same or opposite meaning, respectively), nonsense or garden path-like elements; the latter introduces non-funny
homonyms (phonetically identical but semantically different words), and irresolvable incongruity14,101.
polysemy (in which a lexical item has a range of different meanings) or
paronymy (in which words sound similar but mean different things)99. Figure is reproduced, with permission, from REF.98 (2008) Taylor & Francis.

864 | DECEMBER 2013 | VOLUME 14 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

(the opposite of neuroticism) and experi-


Right hemisphere Left hemisphere ence seeking (related to extraversion) may
enhance humour processing, as indicated
Lateral by increased activity in the lateral prefrontal
and temporal cortices, hippocampus and
mesocorticolimbic circuits. Conversely, high
introversion has been found to correlate with
amygdala activity during humour apprecia-
tion. These preliminary data indicate that
both cognitive and emotional processes
during humour appreciation can be moder-
ated by personality traits in adults and are in
agreement with behavioural data showing
Medial that extraversion is related to the amount of
positive affect in response to humour 59,60.
Comparable data from earlier developmental
stages are scarce, but a recent study provides
preliminary evidence that, in 613year-old
children, humour processing is already mod-
erated by temperament traits such as emo-
tionality, shyness and sociability 61. Overall,
these findings suggest that humour appre-
ciation may be susceptible to individual
differences in personality in both children
and adults, highlighting the need for further
research into the developmental trajectories
Visual static (cartoons) Verbal auditory (spoken jokes)
of this uniquely human positive cognitive
Visual dynamic (movies) Verbal written (written jokes) state by means of larger cross-sectional and/
or longitudinal studies.
Nature Reviews | Neuroscience
Figure 1 | Meta-analysis of activations during humour processing. The meta-analysis results in The findings noted above are comple-
the figure show that humour processing recruits a large set of cortical and subcortical brain areas mented by data from two fMRI investi-
that maintain both the cognitive and the emotional components of humour in a stimulus modality- gations probing sex differences in adult
and task-dependent manner. Literature review suggests that humour appreciation converges on
humour processing 62,63. The first study
two core processes: incongruity detection and resolution, as maintained by the temporo-occipito-
parietal junction; and reward and salience processing involving mesocorticolimbic areas and the
showed that the TOPJ, TP and IFG were
amygdala. Each sphere represents peak coordinates of reported activation clusters from humour stud- activated during humour appreciation in
ies using functional MRI (TABLE1) in Talairach coordinates, projected on a template single-subject both sexes. However, activity in the IFG was
flattened structural brain image. Computed contrasts to derive these activations include funny ver- stronger in females, who displayed addi-
sus non-funny, theory of mind (ToM) funny versus non-ToM funny, semantic funny versus phonologi- tional BOLD signal change in mesocorti-
cal funny (and vice versa), natural amusement versus instructed smiling, language funny versus colimbic reward areas. These findings were
sight gag funny (and vice versa), funny versus irresolvable incongruity, incongruity resolution interpreted as indicating a greater degree of
versus nonsense, funny versus positive, and funny versus garden path (for a definition of humour executive processing and language-based
conditions, please refer to BOX2). decoding, as well as a greater reward net-
work response and possibly less reward
biological value seems to be prominently Personality, sex and brain disorder expectation in females62. The notion of
related to the processing of salience, sig- Considerable evidence from data in adults stronger emotional reactivity in females
nificance, ambiguity and unpredictability 50. indicates that humour is linked with during humour perception was supported
Accordingly, humour appreciation is likely positive individual, as well as group, out- by the second study in adults63. An examina-
to activate the amygdala because it contains comes6,11,20,22,5153. Conversely, humour per- tion of sex differences in humour apprecia-
not only several of the above processing ception has been observed to be reduced in tion in 22 children (aged 613years) found
components related to basic incongruity conditions associated with negative mood similar activation patterns to those reported
detection and resolution but also a posi- states, such as major depression54, and in in adults46. Stronger activity in the midbrain
tive interaction signal with high intrinsic people suffering from social anxiety 55. and amygdala in response to humour was
social significance. Amygdala involve- One way to investigate such associations observed in girls compared with boys, who
ment in humour appreciation therefore is to examine brain activity in response to in turn displayed stronger activation in the
underscores the importance of humour as humour as a function of personality traits ventral mPFC. This supports the notion of
a social process for humans and highlights that are risk factors for psychopathology, increased reward response and salience in
the susceptibility of this process to various such as introversionextraversion and girls, perhaps owing to less reward anticipa-
moderating influences, such as personality, neuroticism56. To date, two fMRI inves- tion during the task. Overall, such emerging
sex and the presence of neuropsychiatric tigations in adults57,58 have used such an data on sex differences in humour apprecia-
disorders that modify the biological value approach. Preliminary results showed that tion in both adults and children support
ofhumour. positive traits such as emotional stability the fitness indicator hypothesis of humour

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 14 | DECEMBER 2013 | 865

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

Taken together, these data on personal-


Right hemisphere Left hemisphere ity, sex differences and psychopathological
conditions associated with humour appre-
Lateral
ciation demonstrate the psychological,
psychiatric or even neurological variation
related to both the cognitive and the emo-
tional components of humour. Moreover,
many of these effects can be observed
in both adults and young children. Such
findings highlight the clinical relevance
of investigating humour appreciation by
means of neuroimaging methods at different
developmentalstages.
Medial
Conclusions and perspective
Humour is a ubiquitous component of
human cognition, communication and
interaction. It has numerous potential ben-
eficial effects on personal, psychological and
physical well-being, and positively influences
social and group processes.
On a functional neuroanatomical basis,
humour appreciation recruits a wide range
of brain areas, which differentially activate
as a function of distinct humour-inducing
stimulus modalities and task requirements.
Visual static (cartoons) Verbal auditory (spoken jokes) Visual dynamic (movies)
Nonetheless, all of these auxiliary mecha-
Figure 2 | Meta-analysis of activations involving parametric modulation of humour. The meta- nisms seem to converge towards two core
analysis results in the figure show that the degree of funniness reported by participants positively processes of humour appreciation: incongru-
correlates with brain activity in response to humour in a large set of cortical
Nature and subcortical
Reviews brain areas
| Neuroscience ity detection and resolution (the cognitive
maintaining both the cognitive and the emotional components of humour. These areas include the component); and a feeling of mirth or reward
temporo-parietal junction, mesocorticolimbic circuit and amygdala. Each sphere represents peak (the emotional component). Whereas the
coordinates of reported activation clusters from humour studies using functional MRI (TABLE1) in cognitive component seems to rely principally
Talairach coordinates, projected on a template flattened structural brain image. Parametric modula- on activity in the TOPJ, the emotional com-
tion analyses were always based on funny versus non-funny contrasts. ponent appears to involve mesocorticolimbic
dopaminergic pathways and the amygdala.
function related to sexual selection theory response to jokes requiring the attribution Our perspective is that none of the regions or
(see above and REF.11) by illustrating that of mental states (BOX2). Therefore, in both networks underlying human humour appre-
females may be more receptive to, and dis- autism and schizophrenia, humour appre- ciation (for example, sensory processing,
play less reward expectation from, humour, ciation deficits seem to be predominantly working memory, incongruity detection and
regardless of age. Further research to confirm related to disturbed ToM and/or socio- resolution, and reward) evolved individually
and extend such findings isneeded. emotional integration mechanisms, and thus or in concert with another expressly for that
Behavioural, imaging and brain lesion cognitive humour processing (see above). function. Rather, the combination of several
findings associated with humour and psy- In accordance with such findings, there are of these regions and/or networks in the ser-
chopathology have also been published. data from patients with focal brain damage vice of humour appreciation became increas-
For example, one line of research reports linking humour-processing deficits with ingly prominent in human society because of
deficient humour processing in autism6467. impaired (right) frontal lobe function67. its importance in processing social informa-
These results are linked to the individuals Finally, a link between altered humour tion. Although the differentiation between
difficulties in understanding the social appreciation and cataplexy has been cognitive and emotional components of
aspect of humour requiring ToM and, noted72,73. Cataplexy refers to episodes of humour is not new 1, evidence from recent
more generally, in integrating cognitive and sudden and transient loss of muscle tone neuroimaging studies strongly supports this
affective information. Prominent neural triggered by strong emotions, usually occur- differentiation.
substrates for such functions are the TPJ ring in association with the complex sleep Future studies focused on the neurosci-
and (right) IFG, the activity of which has wake disorder called narcolepsy. Results ence of humour should address several
been found to be altered in people with from fMRI investigations in patients suffer- outstanding areas of research. Perhaps most
autism64,66,67. ing from cataplexy converge in suggesting an importantly, more data are needed to eluci-
Other research indicates impaired overdrive of the emotional humour circuitry date the development of humour throughout
humour appreciation in schizophrenia6871. (ventral striatum and amygdala), which the human lifespan and its modulation by
Affected individuals display reduced humour might be linked with a compensatory sup- various factors such as culture, personality,
recognition associated with impaired ToM pression of hypothalamus activity by cortical sex, age and intelligence quotient (IQ). Such
abilities and diminished mPFC activity in inhibitory regions. investigations are particularly desirable if

866 | DECEMBER 2013 | VOLUME 14 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

they incorporate longitudinal study designs. 1. Wild,B., Rodden,F.A., Grodd,W. & Ruch,W. Neural 31. Ruch,W. & Hehl,F.J. in The Sense of Humor:
correlates of laughter and humour. Brain 126, Explorations of a Personality Characteristic (ed.
However, we add the caveat that the current 21212138 (2003). Ruch,W.) 109142 (Mouton de Gruyter, 2007).
fMRI scanner environment limits the extent 2. Lefcourt,H.M. in Coping and Copers: Adaptive 32. Moreall,J. The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor
Processes and People (ed. Snyder,C.R.) 6892 (SUNY Press, 1987).
to which humour can be studied, particularly (Oxford Univ. Press, 2000). 33. Burgdorf,J. & Panksepp,J. The neurobiology of
in comparison to naturalistic settings. Other 3. McGhee,P.E. Humor: Its Origin and Development positive emotions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30,
(Freeman, 1979). 173187 (2006).
functional imaging modalities, such as func- 4. Caron,J.E. From ethology to aesthetics: evolution as 34. Taber,K.H., Redden,M. & Hurley,R.A. Functional
tional near-infrared spectroscopy 7476, might a theoretical paradigm for research on laughter, anatomy of humor: positive affect and chronic mental
humor, and other comic phenomena. Humor 15, illness. J.Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 19,
provide methodological advantages from this 245281 (2002). 358362 (2007).
standpoint. It also seems vital to extend exist- 5. Lefcourt,H.M. & Martin,R.A. Humor and Life Stress: 35. Uekermann,J., Daum,I. & Channon,S. Toward a
Antidote to Adversity (Springer, 1986). cognitive and social neuroscience of humor processing.
ing data with research using appropriate con- 6. Martin,R.A. The Psychology of Humor: An Integrative Soc. Cogn. 25, 553572 (2007).
trol conditions and differentiating between Approach (Elsevier Academic Press, 2007). 36. Hutcherson,C.A. etal. Attention and emotion: does
7. Ruch,W. & Carrell,A. Trait cheerfulness and the sense rating emotion alter neural responses to amusing and
incongruity detection and resolution, and of humour. Pers. Individ. Dif. 24, 551558 (1998). sad films? Neuroimage 27, 656668 (2005).
proto-humour versus formal humour 8. Korotkov,D., Perunovic,M., Claybourn,M. & Fraser,I. 37. Neely,M.N., Walter,E., Black,J.M. & Reiss,A.L.
Humor, Stress and Health (Nova Science Publishers, Neural correlates of humor detection and appreciation
(BOX1). Such experiments will shed more light 2011). in children. J.Neurosci. 32, 17841790 (2012).
on the neural bases of human humour appre- 9. Wilkins,J. & Eisenbraun,A.J. Humor theories and the 38. Blanke,O. & Arzy,S. The outofbody experience:
physiological benefits of laughter. Holist. Nurs. Pract. disturbed self-processing at the temporo-parietal
ciation. In addition, we note that the present 23, 349354 (2009). junction. Neuroscientist 11, 1624 (2005).
article emphasizes the clinical relevance of 10. Wilbur,C.J. & Campbell,L. Humor in romantic 39. Mitchell,J.P. Activity in right temporo-parietal
contexts: do men participate and women evaluate? junction is not selective for theory-ofmind. Cereb.
humour related to neuropsychiatric disor- Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 918929 (2011). Cortex 18, 262271 (2008).
ders (such as autism, schizophrenia, anxiety, 11. Gervais,M. & Wilson,D.S. The evolution and 40. Geng,J.J. & Mangun,G.R. Right temporoparietal
functions of laughter and humor: a synthetic junction activation by a salient contextual cue
depression and cataplexy). However, there are approach. Q.Rev. Biol. 80, 395430 (2005). facilitates target discrimination. Neuroimage 54,
many more domains in which the positive 12. Shultz,T.R.Role of incongruity and resolution in 594601 (2011).
childrens appreciation of cartoon humor. J.Exp. Child 41. Vossel,S., Weidner,R., Driver,J., Friston,K.J. &
relationship between humour and coping and Psychol. 13, 456477 (1972). Fink,G.R. Deconstructing the architecture of dorsal
resilience could be more extensively explored 13. Hurley,M.M., Dennett,D.C. & Adams,R.B. Inside and ventral attention systems with dynamic causal
Jokes: Using Humor to Reverse-Engineer the Mind modeling. J.Neurosci. 32, 1063710648 (2012).
in the future. As briefly mentioned above, it (MIT Press, 2011). 42. Coulson,S. & Kutas,M. Getting it: human event-
has been suggested that humour exerts many 14. Chan,Y.C. etal. Towards a neural circuit model of related brain response to jokes in good and poor
verbal humor processing: an fMRI study of the neural comprehenders. Neurosci. Lett. 316, 7174 (2001).
beneficial effects on physical and mental substrates of incongruity detection and resolution. 43. Korb,S., Grandjean,D., Samson,A.C., Delplanque,S.
health, which is of potential interest in several Neuroimage 66, 169176 (2012). & Scherer,K.R. Stop laughing! Humor perception with
15. Ruch,W. in The Oxford Companion to the Affective and without expressive suppression. Soc. Neurosci. 7,
medical settings, including procedures with Sciences (eds Sander,D. & Scherer,K.R.) 2728 510524 (2012).
ill children, older individuals or those in pal- (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009). 44. Marinkovic,K. etal. Right hemisphere has the last
16. Gavrilovic,J. etal. Coping strategies in civilians during laugh: neural dynamics of joke appreciation. Cogn.
liative care19,7782. Furthermore, humour-based air attacks. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 38, Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 113130 (2011).
(psycho)therapy and counselling may help 128133 (2003). 45. Fehr,E. & Camerer,C.F. Social neuroeconornics: the
17. Martin,R.A. Humor, laughter, and physical health: neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn. Sci.
to promote healthy relationships in general, methodological issues and research findings. Psychol. 11, 419427 (2007).
including marriage and family settings in Bull. 127, 504519 (2001). 46. Vrticka,P., Neely,M., Walter Shelly,E., Black,J. M. &
18. Martin,R.A. Sense of humor and physical health: Reiss, A. L. Sex differences during humor appreciation
particular 8386. Finally, the use of humour has theoretical issues, recent findings, and future in child-sibling pairs. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 291304
been suggested to be beneficial in education directions. Humor 17, 119 (2004). (2013).
19. Marziali,E., McDonald,L. & Donahue,P. The role of 47. Murray,E.A. The amygdala, reward and emotion.
for both learning and testing, as well as in coping humor in the physical and mental health of Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 489497 (2007).
the workplace, where it can enhance social older adults. Aging Ment. Health 12, 713718 48. Sander,D., Grafman,J. & Zalla,T. The human
(2008). amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection.
functioning, ease negotiation and mediation, 20. Neuhoff,C.C. & Schaefer,C. Effects of laughing, Rev. Neurosci. 14, 303316 (2003).
and support leadership8790. Such preliminary smiling, and howling on mood. Psychol. Rep. 91, 49. Vrticka,P., Sander,D. & Vuilleumier,P. Lateralized
10791080 (2002). interactive social content and valence processing
findings on the beneficial effects of humour 21. Nezlek,J.B. & Derks,P. Use of humor as a coping within the human amygdala. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6,
on physical and mental health need to be fur- mechanism, psychological adjustment, and social 358 (2013).
interaction. Humor 14, 395413 (2001). 50. Pessoa,L. & Adolphs,R. Emotion processing and the
ther evaluated under stringent scientific con- 22. Samson,A.C. & Gross,J.J. Humour as emotion amygdala: from a low road to many roads of
ditions. A deeper understanding of the neural regulation: the differential consequences of negative evaluating biological significance. Nature Rev.
versus positive humour. Cogn. Emot. 26, 375384 Neurosci. 11, 773782 (2010).
bases of humour appreciation nonetheless (2012). 51. Rosner,F. Therapeutic efficacy of laughter in medicine.
seems relevant for many different contexts, 23. Bennett,M.P., Zeller,J.M., Rosenberg,L. & Cancer Invest. 20, 434436 (2002).
McCann,J. The effect of mirthful laughter on stress 52. Wooten,P. Humor: an antidote for stress. Holist. Nurs.
and has the potential to positively affect the and natural killer cell activity. Altern. Ther. Health Pract. 10, 4956 (1996).
well-being of a wide range of individuals. Med. 9, 3845 (2003). 53. Keltner,D. & Bonanno,G.A. A study of laughter and
24. Berk,L.S. etal. Neuro-endocrine and stress hormone dissociation: distinct correlates of laughter and
Pascal Vrticka and Allan L.Reiss are at the Center for changs during mirthful laughter Am. J.Med. Sci. 298, smiling during bereavement. J.Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73,
Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research, Department 390396 (1989). 687702 (1997).
of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford 25. Fry,W. F.Jr. The physiologic effects of humor, mirth, 54. Uekermann,J. etal. Executive function, mentalizing
and laughter. JAMA 267, 18571858 (1992). and humor in major depression. J.Int. Neuropsychol.
University, Stanford, California 94305, USA. 26. Fredrickson,B.L. & Levenson,R.W. Positive Soc. 14, 5562 (2008).
Jessica M.Black is at the Graduate School of Social emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular 55. Samson,A.C., Lackner,H.K., Weiss,E.M. &
sequelae of negative emotions. Cogn. Emot. 12, Papousek,I. Perception of other peoples mental
Work, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, 191220 (1998). states affects humor in social anxiety. J.Behav. Ther.
Massachusetts 02467, USA. 27. Lefcourt,H. M., Davidson-Katz,K. & Kueneman, K. Exp. Psychiatry 43, 625631 (2012).
Humor and immune-system functioning. Humor 3, 56. Widiger,T.A. & Trull,T.J.Personality and
Allan L.Reiss is also at the Department of Radiology 305321 (1990). psychopathology: an application of the fivefactor
and Pediatrics, Stanford University, Stanford, 28. Cohn,M.A., Fredrickson,B.L., Brown,S.L., model. J.Pers. 60, 363393 (1992).
California 94305, USA. Mikels,J.A. & Conway,A.M. Happiness unpacked: 57. Mobbs,D., Hagan,C.C., Azim,E., Menon,V. &
positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building Reiss,A.L. Personality predicts activity in reward and
P.V.and J.M.B.contributed equally to this work. resilience. Emotion 9, 361368 (2009). emotional regions associated with humor. Proc. Natl
Correspondence to A.L.R. 29. Russo,S.J., Murroughl,J.W., Han,M.H., Acad. Sci. USA 102, 1650216506 (2005).
email: areiss1@stanford.edu Charney,D.S. & Nestler,E.J. Neurobiology of 58. Samson,A.C., Hempelmann,C.F., Huber,O. &
resilience. Nature Neurosci. 15, 14751484 (2012). Zysset,S. Neural substrates of incongruity-resolution
doi:10.1038/nrn3566 30. Ruch,W. in A Primer of Humor (ed. Raskin,V.) and nonsense humor. Neuropsychologia 47,
Published online 30 October 2013 17100 (Mounton de Gruyter, 2008). 10231033 (2009).

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 14 | DECEMBER 2013 | 867

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved


PERSPECTIVES

59. Ruch,W. Extraversion, alcohol, and enjoyment. Pers. introductory review. J.Near Infrared Spectrosc. 20, 97. Watson,K.K., Matthews,B.J. & Allman,J.M. Brain
Individ. Dif. 16, 89102 (1994). 7592 (2012). activation during sight gags and language-
60. Ruch,W. & Deckers,L. Do extraverts like to laugh: 77. Fernandes,S.C. & Arriaga,P. The effects of clown dependent humor. Cereb. Cortex 17, 314324
an analysis of the Situational Humour Response intervention on worries and emotional responses in (2007).
Questionnaire (SHRQ). Eur. J.Pers. 7, 211220 children undergoing surgery. J.Health Psychol. 15, 98. Samson,A.C., Zysset,S. & Huber,O. Cognitive
(1993). 405415 (2010). humor processing: different logical mechanisms in
61. Vrticka, P., Black, J. M., Neely, M., Shelly, E. W. & 78. Dean,R.A.K. & Gregory,D.M. Humor and laughter nonverbal cartoons an fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci.
Reiss, A. L. Humor processing in children: influence of in palliative care: an ethnographic investigation. 3, 125140 (2008).
temperament, age and IQ. Neuropsychologia http:// Palliat. Support. Care 2, 139148 (2004). 99. Servaite,L. The anatomy of a joke. Tiltai 4, 8186
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.028 79. Dean,R.A.K. & Gregory,D.M. More than trivial: (2005).
(2013). strategies for using humor in palliative care. Cancer 100. Goel,V. & Dolan,R.J. The functional anatomy of
62. Azim,E., Mobbs,D., Jo,B., Menon,V. & Reiss,A.L. Nurs. 28, 292300 (2005). humor: segregating cognitive and affective
Sex differences in brain activation elicited by humor. 80. Bokarius,A. etal. Attitude toward humor in patients components. Nature Neurosci. 4, 237238 (2001).
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 1649616501 experiencing depressive symptoms. Innov. Clin. 101. Chan,Y.C., Chou,T.L., Chen,H.C. & Liang,K.C.
(2005). Neurosci. 8, 2023 (2011). Segregating the comprehension and elaboration
63. Kohn,N., Kellermann,T., Gur,R.C., Schneider,F. & 81. Phipps,S. etal. Resilience in children undergoing stem processing of verbal jokes: an fMRI study.
Habel,U. Gender differences in the neural correlates cell transplantation: results of a complementary Neuroimage 61, 899906 (2012).
of humor processing: implications for different intervention trial. Pediatrics 129, E762E770 102. Gallagher,H.L. etal. Reading the mind in cartoons
processing modes. Neuropsychologia 49, 888897 (2012). and stories: an fMRI study of theory of mind in verbal
(2011). 82. Goodenough,B. etal. Study protocol for a randomized and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 1121
64. Semrud-Clikeman,M. & Glass,K. The relation of controlled trial of humor therapy in residential care: (2000).
humor and child development: social, adaptive, and the Sydney Multisite Intervention of LaughterBosses 103. Iwase,M. etal. Neural substrates of human facial
emotional aspects. J.Child Neurol. 25, 12481260 and ElderClowns (SMILE). Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, expression of pleasant emotion induced by comic
(2010). 20372044 (2012). films: a PET study. Neuroimage 17, 758768
65. Samson,A.C. & Hegenloh,M. Stimulus characteristics 83. de Koning,E. & Weiss,R.L. The relational humor (2002).
affect humor processing in individuals with Asperger inventory: functions of humor in close relationships. 104. Mobbs,D., Greicius,M.D., Abdel-Azim,E.,
syndrome. J.Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 438447 Am. J.Family Ther. 30, 118 (2002). Menon,V. & Reiss,A.L. Humor modulates the
(2010). 84. Shaughnessy,M.F. & Wadsworth,T.M.Humor in mesolimbic reward centers. Neuron 40, 10411048
66. Kana,R.K. & Wadsworth,H. M. The archeologists counseling and psychotherapy: a (2003).
career ended in ruins: hemispheric differences in pun 20yearretrospective. Psychol. Rep. 70, 755762 105. Moran,J.M., Wig,G.S., Adams,R.B., Janata,P. &
comprehension in autism. Neuroimage 62, 7786 (1992). Kelley,W.M. Neural correlates of humor detection
(2012). 85. Pasquali,E.A. Learning to laugh: humor as therapy. and appreciation. Neuroimage 21, 10551060
67. Shammi,P. & Stuss,D.T. Humour appreciation: a role J.Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 28, 3135 (2004).
of the right frontal lobe. Brain 122, 657666 (1999). (1990). 106. Bartolo,A., Benuzzi,F., Nocetti,L., Baraldi,P. &
68. Bozikas,V.P. etal. Humor appreciation deficit in 86. Rieger,A. & McGrail,J.P. Coping humor and family Nichelli,P. Humor comprehension and appreciation:
schizophrenia: the relevance of basic neurocognitive functioning in parents of children with disabilities. an fMRI study. J.Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 17891798
functioning. J.Nerv. Ment. Dis. 195, 325331 Rehabil. Psychol. 58, 8997 (2013). (2006).
(2007). 87. Romero,E.J. & Cruthirds,K.W. The use of humor in 107. Wild,B. etal. Humor and smiling: cortical regions
69. Marjoram,D. etal. A visual joke fMRI investigation the workplace. Acad. Manag. Persp. 20, 5869 selective for cognitive, affective, volitional components.
into Theory of Mind and enhanced risk of (2006). Neurology 66, 887893 (2006).
schizophrenia. Neuroimage 31, 18501858 (2006). 88. Bonaiuto,M., Castellana,E. & Pierro,A. Arguing and 108. Franklin,R.G.Jr & Adams,R.B.Jr. The reward of a
70. Polimeni,J. & Reiss,J.P. Humor perception deficits in laughing: the use of humor to negotiate in group good joke: neural correlates of viewing dynamic
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 141, 229232 discussions. Humor 16, 183223 (2003). displays of standup comedy. Cogn. Affect. Behav.
(2006). 89. Chabeli,M. Humor: a pedagogical tool to promote Neurosci. 11, 508515 (2011).
71. Tsoi,D.T.Y. etal. Humour experience in learning. Curationis 31, 5159 (2008). 109. Bekinschtein,T.A., Davis,M.H., Rodd,J.M. &
schizophrenia: relationship with executive dysfunction 90. Beckman,H., Regier,N. & Young,J. Effect of Owen,A.M. Why clowns taste funny: the relationship
and psychosocial impairment. Psychol. Med. 38, workplace laughter groups on personal efficacy beliefs. between humor and semantic ambiguity. J.Neurosci.
801810 (2008). J.Prim. Prev. 28, 167182 (2007). 31, 96659671 (2011).
72. Reiss,A.L. etal. Anomalous hypothalamic responses 91. Provine,R.R. Laughter: A Scientific Investigation 110. Goel,V. & Dolan,R.J. Social regulation of affective
to humor in cataplexy. PLoS ONE 3, e2225 (2008). (Viking, 2000). experience of humor. J.Cogn. Neurosci. 19,
73. Schwartz,S. etal. Abnormal activity in hypothalamus 92. Panksepp,J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations 15741580 (2007).
and amygdala during humour processing in human of Human and Animal Emotions (Oxford Univ. Press,
narcolepsy with cataplexy. Brain 131, 514522 1998).
(2008). 93. Fry,W.F. The biology of humor. Humor 7, 111126 Acknowledgements
74. Cui,X., Bray,S., Bryant,D.M., Glover,G.H. & (1994). We thank P. K. Mazaika for his help during literature review
Reiss,A.L. A quantitative comparison of NIRS and 94. Darwin,C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and and M. Saggar for his contribution to the humour meta-anal-
fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 54, Animals (John Murray, 1872). ysis. This work was partly financed by an Advanced Postdoc.
28082821 (2011). 95. Frilund,A.J. & Loftis,M.J. Relations between tickling Mobility fellowship from the Swiss National Science
75. Cui,X., Bryant,D.M. & Reiss,A.L. NIRS-based and humourous laughter: preliminary support the Foundation (number 136480) to P.V. and a grant from the
hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal DarwinHecker hypothesis. Biol. Psychol. 30, Stanford and Lucile Packard Foundation for Childrens Health
coherence in superior frontal cortex during 300319 (1990). to A.L.R
cooperation. Neuroimage 59, 24302437 (2012). 96. Baughman,H.M. etal. A behavioral genetic study of
76. Cutini,S., Moro,S.B. & Bisconti,S. Functional near humor styles in an Australian sample. Twin Res. Hum. Competing interests statement
infrared optical imaging in cognitive neuroscience: an Genet. 15, 663667 (2012). The authors declare no competing interests.

868 | DECEMBER 2013 | VOLUME 14 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro

2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved

You might also like