You are on page 1of 19

Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

www.elsevier.com/locate/jaa

Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation


of variation in material culture and the archaeological record
a,* b
Jelmer W. Eerkens , Carl P. Lipo
a
Department of Anthropology, University of California, Davis, One Shields Avenue Davis, CA 95616, USA
b
Department of Anthropology, California State University Long Beach, 1250 Bellower Boulevard, Long Beach, CA 90840, USA

Received 9 November 2004; revised 18 March 2005


Available online 4 October 2005

Abstract

Archaeologists are adept at analyzing variation in artifacts. The discipline has well established and tested methods to
track change through time and to evaluate the function of artifacts that depend upon measures of variation in the archae-
ological record. Although a critical concept, the means by which variation in material culture is generated is not well
understood. This paper explores one source of variation, copying errors, and systematically examines how cultural trans-
mission processes act to amplify, reduce, or maintain such variation. Using simple models, we generate expected distribu-
tions for the amount of variation that occurs through time under varying circumstances. This variation is caused by small
errors that are transmitted from one person to another in the propagation and replication of cultural traits. These baseline
values provide useful null models for explaining variation in prehistoric assemblages of artifacts. We use measurements of
projectile points from Owens Valley and Woodland ceramics from Illinois to demonstrate the value of this approach.
2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Cultural transmission; Evolutionary archaeology; Copying errors; Owens Valley; Projectile points; Woodland ceramics

Over the last 25 years there has been a renewed of cultural change (e.g., Atran, 2001; Aunger, 2000,
interest among social scientists to explain human 2002; Bentley and Shennan, 2003; Boyd and Richer-
culture change within an evolutionary framework, son, 1995a,b; Boyer, 1999; Henrich, 2001, 2004;
especially using cultural transmission theory. Build- Henrich and Boyd, 2002; Mesoudi et al., 2004;
ing on the foundation of culture historians (e.g., OBrien and Lyman, 2000; Plotkin, 2002; Shennan,
Kroeber, 1916) and the works of evolutionary scien- 2000, 2002; Sperber, 1996; Wheeler et al., 2002).
tists such as Boyd and Richerson (1985) and Caval- To date, the main emphasis in evolutionary re-
li-Sforza and Feldman (1981), the number of articles search on the archaeological record has been on
following this approach has steadily increased. Re- identifying the processes that guide and regulate
cent and lively debate over the pathways, patterns the transmission of the cultural traits. We have,
and constraints of cultural transmission demon- for example, increased our understanding of what
strate a continued interest in the evolutionary study we should expect to see in cases of transmission that
are strongly structured by conformist biasing and
*
Corresponding author. other kinds of sorting (e.g., Bettinger and Eerkens,
E-mail address: jweerkens@ucdavis.edu (J.W. Eerkens). 1999; Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Henrich and

0278-4165/$ - see front matter 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jaa.2005.08.001
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 317

Gil-White, 2001; Shennan, 2002; Shennan and Wil- assemblages through time (Lyman et al., 1997), a
kinson, 2001) as well as in the absence of these pro- step critical to the formation of culture history as
cesses (e.g., Bentley et al., 2004; Lipo, 2001; Lipo an explanatory framework in archaeology. In this
et al., 1997; Lipo and Madsen, 2001; Neiman, sense, studies of artifact variation form a vital part
1995). Clearly, this research is vital and has pro- of the methodology of our discipline. Work in this
duced a robust set of techniques for explaining var- area has continued since the early part of the twen-
iability in material culture. As we show below, if we tieth century (Dunnell, 1986). On the basis of these
are to employ evolution as a means to explain eorts, we now have excellent tools for studying
change of cultural traits, it is important to know change through time based on variation in material
how such traits are transmitted. Transmission, how- culture including ceramic decoration and composi-
ever, is only one of three necessary components of tion, projectile point morphology, and architecture
evolution, the others being the generation of varia- (e.g., Cochrane, 2002; Cordell, 1993; Ford, 1938;
tion and dierential success between variants Graves and Cachola-Abad, 1996; Lipo, 2001; Lipo
(Lewontin, 1974). Among the three, the generation et al., 1997; Neiman, 1995; OBrien and Lyman,
of variation has received much less attention from 2003; OBrien et al., 2001; OBrien and Holland,
anthropologists. Variation is the raw material upon 1990).
which selection operates to cause changes in the fre- Variation in artifacts, however, is not restricted
quency of cultural traits through time. Though to dimensions that change through time. Even prior
transmission can operate in the absence of varia- to the advent of culture history as a cohesive para-
tion, evolution (i.e., change) cannot take place. This digm, archaeologists were interested in the study
paper builds on our previous research (Eerkens and of technological variability in artifacts (e.g.,
Lipo, in press) that examines the generation of var- Holmes, 1891, 1894). Much later, the new archaeol-
iation as a result of small errors when cultural traits ogists of the 1960s and 1970s began to change the
are replicated. Specically, we consider the eects of focus of research from temporal sequences to the
cultural transmission processes and how they act on means by which behavior is reconstructed. New
sources of variation. We focus our attention on the emphasis was placed on the study of artifacts as rep-
reproduction of material culture, though our argu- resentations of functional activities (e.g., Binford,
ments could be extended to other aspects of human 1962, 1973; Hill, 1977a; Plog, 1976; Wobst, 1977).
culture. In the study of ceramics, for example, researchers
began to investigate the role ceramics played in
Variation in archaeological studies the organization of household, craft, subsistence
and social activities (e.g., Blitz, 1993; Costin and
Archaeologists have used variation in material Hagstrum, 1995; Ericson et al., 1972; Evans, 1978;
culture to study prehistory since the inception of Hill, 1977b; Kramer, 1985, 1979; Mills, 1989; Schif-
the discipline (e.g., Evans, 1850, 1875; Holmes, fer, 1990; Schier et al., 1994; Skibo, 1992; Skibo
1890, 1891, 1894; Rau, 1896; Wilson, 1891). Begin- et al., 1989; Smith, 1985; Turner and Lofgren,
ning with the emergence of culture history as an 1966). Overall, in the last 30 years we have seen
explanatory paradigm, interest in variation has been an increasing body of literature on the means for
primarily focused on its use in temporal and spatial relating artifacts and structures to past functional
frameworks for tracking change through time and activities (e.g., Ahler, 1979; Beck, 1995; Binford
interaction across space (e.g., Ford, 1935; Kidder, and Binford, 1966; Dunnell, 1978a; Levin, 1976;
1917; Kroeber, 1916, 1919; Spier, 1917). Alfred Meltzer, 1981; Scheinsohn and Ferretti, 1995; Ski-
Kroeber (1916, p. 15), for example, noticed that bo, 1992; Symens, 1986; Weissner, 1983).
the relative abundance of corrugated pottery in Despite our reliance on and adeptness in using
the area around Zuni Pueblo in the American variation to study the past, we know little about
Southwest seemed to be correlated with the age of the processes related to its source. We have limited
a site; the greater the abundance of this kind of pot- knowledge about the conditions which encourage
tery the more recent the site was interpreted to be. the generation of variation and the conditions under
Variation in the abundance of certain pottery types, which variants disappear. In addition, we have not
he recognized, seemed to be a function of change developed rm theoretical grounds to determine
over time. This observation about variation allowed whether variation is generated as part of a single
Kroeber to construct a method for ordering ceramic process or is dierentially produced along indepen-
318 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

dent dimensions that vary based on the content and the literature, but despite the fact that they are
environment of transmission. named, the unit of culture and how it is transmitted
Several recent studies have attempted to ll this is still not well understood. However, this is not a
gap. Drawing on the models of Boyd and Richerson deciency per se. The lack of a readily identiable
(1985), researchers such as Shennan and Wilkinson physical unit for cultural transmission is a conse-
(2001), Bentley and Shennan (2003), and Kohler quence of the fact that the physical forms of cultural
et al. (2004) have explored how social processes, information come in a variety of sizes and scales and
such as conforming to norms or copying prestigious are constantly changing. There are no boundaries
individuals, can sometimes reduce the amount of on the types of physical entities that can carry infor-
variation within sets of artifacts. Models proposing mation; particulate inheritance, the form common
how variation is increased within an evolutionary in genetic transmission, is not required. For evolu-
framework, however, are less common despite the tion to happen, and for us to be able to study it,
fact that one of the most striking aspects of the all that matters is that information is passed from
archaeological record is the tremendous increase in one individual to another and that there are one
the range of artifacts used from 2.5 million years or more sources of variation for this information.
ago until the present. Neimans classic study Although biologists routinely refer to the gene
(1995) of stylistic variation of Woodland ceramics, as a concrete empirical entity, the lack of bound-
Shennan and Wilkinsons (2001) analysis of Linear- aries is true for both cultural and genetic forms of
bandkeramik pottery, and Kohler et al.s (2004) transmission. Biologists still struggle to dene and
study of Southwestern pottery are notable exception understand a gene. When studying all processes
in this regard. Using measures of ceramic sherd of transmission, we must keep the physical package
thickness, Neiman examined how random drift pro- separate from the information being transmitted.
cesses operate on selectively neutral variation and Genes in this view are conceptual measurement
how such processes aect the number of types units, constructed only for purposes of analysis
(i.e., diversity) and their longevity within an assem- not things that are found discretely in nature.
blage. Similarly, Shennan and Wilkinson (2001) and This is true for any entity we might conceive of
Kohler et al. (2004) examine patterns of pottery for cultural transmission. When we study cultural
styles and infer the social processes that shaped their transmission it is not the physical package or suite
generation and transmission through time, nding of characters that matter but rather the cultural
either greater than expected or less than expected information that the transmission units convey.
diversity in ceramic assemblages. Both studies com- The denition of the gene proposed by Williams
pare archaeological data against that generated (1966) provides a good starting point for discussing
from null models using computer simulation, the unit of cultural transmission. Williams dened
which assume purely random copying and transmis- the gene as the unit that segregates and recombines
sion. Such null models are useful because they pro- with appreciable frequency. Extending Williams
vide a way to contextualize archaeological data, that denition, Pocklington and Best (1997, p. 81) state
is, a way to understand apparent patterns (e.g., that cultural transmission units are the largest
Brantingham, 2003). Our study takes a similar tack, units of socially transmitted information that reli-
but explores metrical variation within types, instead ably and repeatedly withstand transmission. In this
of the types themselves. sense, cultural units represent any measurable units
that we can delineate within the suite of cultural
Sources of cultural variation variation that displays heritable continuity, mea-
sured as a greater than random degree of coherence
How is variation in material culture generated of information traced through time and across
and retained? Answering this question requires us space. Importantly, the units of transmission are
to consider how culture is transmitted. Unlike ideational and not directly observable. They consist
genetic transmission, which is based on the duplica- of information and are conceptually analogous to
tion of relatively well-studied molecules of DNA recipes for behavior, artifacts, and ideas (Lyman
and RNA, there are no agreed-upon empirical units and OBrien, 2003). Studies of cultural transmission
of cultural transmission. Cultural units are some- must focus on identifying patterns of repeated co-
times referred to as memes (after Dawkins, 1976) occurring attributes while also measuring the eect
or culturgens (after Lumsden and Wilson, 1981) in of transmission on the production of variation.
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 319

Elsewhere (Eerkens and Lipo, in press) we have ten considered to be biased and/or intentional
attempted to conceptually categorize the sources variation, or the result of invention, discovery,
of cultural variation and dene classes for the pro- or innovation. For our purposes intent is not
cesses in which new variants are introduced into pertinent to the discussion. Variation is either de-
the pool of variation. This framework is summa- rived from simple copying error not perceived by
rized in Table 1. In the table, the vertical axis repre- individuals, or it is modied by internal cognitive
sents the location of the variation within the processes, regardless of the intention of the inheritor
transmission process. Variation can be dened to of culture. The latter may or may not be conscious
occur in three dierent conceptual locations during decisions by the inheritor. That is, they may copy a
transmission and materialization: (1) in the trans- prestigious individual but purposefully modify the
mission of an instruction set itself (i.e., how to do behavior according to their own worldview (e.g.,
something), (2) during the execution of that instruc- Gabora, 2004), for example, adopting a particular
tion set (i.e., actually doing it), or (3) as a result of clothing style but changing the color scheme, or
heterogeneity in the raw material out of which a they may subconsciously adopt the modal behavior
variant is generated. The second two categories within a population.
are related to the eects of the medium in which We argue that these two processes on the left
transmission occurs rather than being related to a and right side of Table 1 generate distinctly dier-
mistake on the part of the producer of culture. ent kinds of variance. The former generates primar-
Thus, in reproducing a piece of music, variation in ily random variation and is analogous to mutation
the nal product can come from an inaccurate in genetic transmission. There is no predened or
transmission of the song itself (e.g., a person hears predictable direction to the variants produced by
or remembers the song in a way that is distinct from these processes. Over time random error accumu-
the way actually played), from a slightly dierent lates, increasing the tails of the distribution of vari-
playing of the song (e.g., a pianist hits the wrong ants. If people make errors in copying the
key during a rendition of the song despite having instruction set (rst entry in the rst column of Ta-
an accurate copy of the song in his/her head), or ble 1), we might think of this as inaccuracy in learn-
from the use of a dierent instrument or perfor- ing or approximation, most likely due to errors in
mance hall, for example with dierent acoustic perception. For example, this inaccuracy would oc-
properties, to recreate the music. cur if something is perceived as larger or as smaller
The horizontal axis in Table 1 classies the mech- than it really is. On the other hand, the error could
anisms by which variation is produced. First, varia- occur during execution of the instruction set (sec-
tion can be produced simply as the result of an error ond entry in rst column) and would be due to
in copying. These errors are a consequence of inac- imprecision in manual dexterity. Alternatively, if
curacies in observation and not perceived or intend- the variation is produced as a result of the hetero-
ed. In this case, the observer simply copies geneity in raw materials or the medium used (third
instructions but inadvertently introduces errors that entry in rst column) we might refer to this as com-
he/she is unaware of. Alternatively, variation can be positional or structural variation. This category
produced by cognitive mechanisms that sort and would include when there are aws in the raw mate-
recombine information in order to create new forms rials that cause unintended variation, despite hav-
that may be evident and perceptible to the replica- ing exact copies and correct execution of the
tor. The latter process of variation generation is of- instruction set.

Table 1
Schematic for the generation of variation in material culture
Location of variation Process of variation generation
Copying error below perception threshold Cognitive processes above perception threshold
Transmission of instruction set Error in learning Discovery/recombination of instruction sets
Execution of instruction set Error in implementation/manual dexterity Innovation or interpretation
(e.g., Yo Yo Mas interpretation of Bach)
Medium of execution Material heterogenity Translation (e.g., screenplay version of a book)
Note. Invention is eectively recombinationrearrangement of existing information/instructions/memes (Gabora, 2000). Also, varia-
tion in this table is historicaland thus homologous.
320 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

It is expected that the scale of variation due to in material culture comes about through relatively
copying error, especially the rst and second entries small changes that build up over time, and not
in column 1, should be relatively small (Eerkens, through radical departures from existing technolo-
2000; Eerkens and Bettinger, 2001). Below, we pres- gies (Shennan, 1989, 1991). This process has been
ent a model for predicting how small. These gures well studied by scholars of western industrialized
are especially relevant in prehistoric settings before technology and is variously referred to as re-inven-
the advent of weighing scales, meter sticks, written tion (Rogers, 2003), recombinant technology
instructions, and other standards that provide (Hargadon, 2003), or self-conscious design (Alex-
means and checks for reducing variation in copying ander, 1970).
errors. Variation due to dierences in raw material Although there may be other ways to classify the
(third entry in rst column) may have more widely generation of variation, this model provides a start-
varying magnitude, depending on the structure of ing point for parsing out dierent kinds of variation
the material and inherited rules for material and their consequences in the archaeological record.
selection. Here, we would like to focus on the rst column in
The second mechanism (second column of Table Table 1, particularly the rst two rows. One of the
1), on the other hand, produces variation that is of reasons why we think this area of investigation is
much greater magnitude and can be directional due of particular interest is because we can generate
to the cognitive mechanisms that act to sort varia- quantiable expectations about the degree of varia-
tion. As Gabora (2000) describes, our evolved tion that should be involved. Determining the de-
brains do not solve problems through random or gree to which random variation is introduced into
exhaustive experimentation with all possible solu- the transmission and execution of instructions gives
tions. Thus, an individual may create a new tool a means for studying how change occurs in the ab-
to solve a particular problem, but would be unlikely sence of other factorsa null model against which
to randomly create objects of dierent sizes and we can evaluate change. Thus, if there is change
shapes until a workable form was discovered. Rath- through time in the variation within artifact types,
er than being random, cultural variation from the how much of this change can we attribute to the
second column of Table 1 starts with inherited var- simple eect of copying errors? Similarly, when
iability and modications are generated strategi- there is less variation than expected due to these
cally, using an internal model of the relationships copying errors, we can then begin to attribute this
amongst the various elements of the problem do- lack of change to other factors.
main, and contextually, responding to the specics
of how the present situation diers from previously Human perception and error
encountered ones (Gabora, 2000). This inherited
internal model relates to the worldview that The human senses (e.g., sight, hearing, taste, etc.)
Gabora denes in a later work (2004). We predict operate by measuring various physical phenomena
that the scale of variation generated by this process in our physical environment (e.g., wavelengths of
will be much higher relative to copying error. reected light, vibrations in the air, levels of dier-
This second mechanism for the generation of var- ent chemical compounds in food, etc.). These mea-
iation, the process of invention, discovery, and/or surements help us to perceive and adapt to our
innovation, has received signicant attention from surroundings, and allow us to manipulate objects.
scholars of technology (Basalla, 1988; Rogers, Clearly, the ability to compare and evaluate these
2003), including archaeologists (e.g., chapters in measurements and to dierentiate, for example, be-
van der Leeuw and Torrence, 1989). Although tween large animals and small ones, shallow holes
invention, discovery, and innovation invoke notions and deep pits, poisonous plants and edible ones,
of entirely new technologies or processes to solve and so on, is crucial to our survival.
problems, most diachronic studies demonstrate the Research in the psychological sciences has helped
continuous and historical nature of material tech- to identify the empirical limits of the acuity of hu-
nologies (Basalla, 1988). Even in todays technology man sensory systems, especially our ability to per-
industry in the United States, innovation almost al- ceive dierences (Coren et al., 1994; Norwich,
ways results from the merging of ideas from dier- 1983; Palmer, 1988). Due to physiological con-
ent elds and/or a reconguration of an existing straints humans have great diculty observing dif-
technology (Hargadon, 2003). Thus, most change ferences below certain threshold values. These
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 321

limits vary depending on what sensory system is in the absence of a xed reference template, these er-
being used to measure (i.e., perceive) the phenome- rors can accumulate quite dramatically and be sub-
non of interest, and dier slightly from person to ject to change by dierent evolutionary forces,
person, but are surprisingly constant. As a result resulting in visible change over time (Simons et al.,
of these limits, variation below these thresholds is 2000). Below we model the generation and propaga-
virtually imperceptible and everything is interpreted tion of copying error to determine how much varia-
as the same. Limits in perception are produced by tion we may expect over time from such processes.
the logical structure of individual cognition as well
as physiological constraints (van Doorn et al., Modeling the generation of variation
1984). For example, the eye is predominantly com-
posed of water and its composition limits optical Given simple rules about cultural transmission
quality and the available spectral window. These we can build expectations for the amount of varia-
hardwired limits produce constraints that have tion present in artifact assemblages due to copying
implications for the production of variation in arti- errors. As a starting point, we take 3% as the small-
facts, and hence, change through time in the amount est dierence that might be detectable for two mea-
of variation we can expect in sets of artifacts (see sures, the Weber Fraction for visual measurement
below). of line length. This value represents the magnitude
For humans, errors in perception are always rel- of values that are thought to be inherent in human
ative to the size or intensity of the phenomenon cognition (Coren et al., 1994; see also Eerkens and
being measured, unlike machines such as mechani- Bettinger, 2001). What this means is that if individ-
cal weighing scales which have absolute error terms uals cannot tell the dierence between the length of
(e.g., plus or minus 1 gram). For example, in order two bifaces that are within 3% of each other, and
to tell if two lines are dierent in length, one must be transmission processes consist of simple copying
about 3% longer than the other (Coren et al., 1994; or imitation, it stands to reason that people may
see also Eerkens and Bettinger, 2001). If the lines be o by up to 3% during the transmission of infor-
are within 3% of one another they will appear equal mation about what size to make an artifact. As a re-
in length to the naked eye, though this does not ap- sult, we can expect a certain amount of drift during
ply when an external standard, such as a ruler, is the process of transmission, whether horizontal,
used as the method of measurement or when the vertical, or oblique. How much drift can we expect?
lines are placed directly next to each other (in which Modeling this process using simple simulations of
case one serves as a ruler to measure the other). This the transmission of information provides one way
3% value is referred to as the Weber Fraction for to answer this question.
estimation of length. Each sensory system has a un-
ique Weber Fraction indicating the acuity of the hu- Unbiased transmission
man body to distinguish dierence for that
particular sensory input. In our simplest simulations we focused only on
Since the manufacture of discrete objects is heav- vertical transmission of information. The simulation
ily dependent upon memory and manual dexterity, consisted of several lineages with direct replacement
inaccuracy in human perception, especially of size, in each generation. Each parent passes informa-
is likely to be relevant to archaeological studies of tion to a single ospring about an attribute. This
artifact change. If there are physiological limits to conguration is akin to asexual reproduction with
the human ability to perceive dierences, then it is one-to-one replacement of a parent by an ospring
reasonable to assume that small amounts of error in each generation. The attributes are arbitrary
below the limits of detection will be introduced dur- but could be imagined as the length of an arrow-
ing transmission. This small amount of error is head. In each subsequent generation, error is add-
introduced in any copying event in which informa- ed to the attribute value and transmitted to the next
tion is communicated about what an artifact should generation, in a Markov chain fashion. In this way,
look like and how it should be made. We refer to the values for arrowhead length could change over
this source of variation as copying error. time. We can describe this situation with a simple
Although the error is imperceptible at each trans- time series equation
mission event, it is cumulative and can become per-
ceptible over time. As we show in our simulations, Y t 1 Y t Y t  c  N 0; 1; 1
322 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

where Y(t) represents the attribute at any time t, and one way of tracking the amount of variation within
Y(t + 1) represents the attribute at the next point in a population of measures. The CV is a dimensionless
time (i.e., t + 1 or the next generation). In the equa- measure of variation across populations and
tion above, c represents the error rate divided by represents the sample standard deviation divided
two and N(0, 1) is a normal random variable with by the sample mean multiplied by 100. If we calcu-
mean of zero and variance of one. Thus, the length late the CV in our simulation we can see that it
of the arrowhead at time t + 1 is the length at the increases monotonically through time. In fact, we
previous time plus a small amount of error. This er- can express the CV as a function of the number of
ror is normally distributed around the value 0 and is generations that have passed (i.e., time) and the error
scaled by both the error rate (3%) and previous rate. The general formula describing the relationship
attribute value. The amount of error, therefore, between CV, error rate, and time is:
can be positive or negative. Over time, the cumula- p
tive results of this error will behave in a stochastic CV ec2 t  1; 2
fashion. This particular stochastic process has been
well studied by mathematicians and is known more where e equals 2.7183 (sometimes called Eulers con-
generally as a multiplicative linear white noise pro- stant), c is the error rate divided by two, and t is time
cess (Gardiner, 1985, pp. 103104). measured in the number of generations. This equa-
We modeled this process across 400 generations tion implies that the CV strictly increases but that
with 10 individuals in each generation (i.e., 10 lineag- the rate of increase slows over time, much like a
es), where there is no interaction between contempo- parabola turned on its side. The dashed line depicting
rary individuals (i.e., vertical transmission only). Ten CV in Fig. 1 shows the relation between these
such Markov chains are shown in Fig. 1. As seen, parameters.
each population drifts around the mean, increasing These basic simulations show that, in the absence
or decreasing slightly with each generation. While, of interaction and selection but with copying error,
individual lineages can drift quite far from the mean, variation will be transmitted and amplied over
the overall average stays about the same. This is a re- time. This is due simply to imprecision in how hu-
sult well known from biological studies on genetic mans are able to visually measure, remember, and
drift (e.g., Wright, 1970). What clearly changes over replicate artifacts. The rate of CV increase, howev-
time, however, is the amount of variation between er, slows down over time, roughly as the square root
the ten lineages. The coecient of variation (CV) is of the number of generations.

20.0

CV

16.0
Value

12.0

Average

8.0

4.0
1 51 101 151 201 251 301 351
Generation

Fig. 1. Average and CV of 10 drifting Markov chains under unbiased transmission.


J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 323

Table 2
Classication of cultural transmission sorting mechanisms by inheritance direction and number of people inherited from
Direction of inheritance Traits copied from:
Individual (n = 1) Subset of individuals (1 < n < N) Population (n = N)
Vertical Direct transmission (i.e., parent) Direct transmission (i.e., both parents) N/A
Oblique Direct transmission (e.g., teacher) Conformist transmission (i.e., group) Conformist transmission
(e.g., parental generation)
Horizontal Prestige-biased transmission Prestige-biased transmission Conformist transmission
(e.g., individual) (e.g., peer clique) (e.g., peers)
Notes. N refers to all individuals from which copies can be made. This number varies depending on direction of inheritance.

Biased transmission transmission, individuals conform to the average


value (with attending error) from the entire previous
Of course, the transmission of culture rarely generation, that of their parent. Using the average
operates in such a fashion. People often transmit value is only one way of framing a conformist mod-
information obliquely and/or horizontally using a el; modal or median values produce similar results.
range of techniques (i.e., transmission rules) such In our simulations, the probability of conforming
as conformist transmission, prestige-biased trans- to the average was held constant for all individuals
mission, guided variation, and/or indirectly biased within a particular simulation run. Dierent runs al-
transmission (e.g., Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Cav- lowed us to vary the probability of conformance for
alli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981; see review by Hen- each transmission event from 0 to 100%. For exam-
rich and McElreath, 2003). Each of these kinds of ple, in one simulation individuals might have a 5%
sorting mechanisms can potentially inuence the chance of conforming to the average of the previous
range of variation that occurs within a population. generation while in another they might conform
Table 2 presents one way in which cultural trans- 50% of the time. We refer to this value as the
mission sorting mechanisms can be classied. Rules strength of conformance.
for sorting may depend on the direction of inheri- In theory, a conformist transmission process
tance. In most transmission models (e.g., Boyd should have the eect of reducing the amount of var-
and Richerson, 1985; Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, iation within the population compared to unbiased
1981) rules for inheritance are determined by transmission. This is because lineages that run o
whether a trait is being adopted from a peer (i.e., to one extreme or the other, as in Fig. 1, will have a
horizontal transmission), from someone of a previ- chance of returning back to the average value at each
ous generation (i.e., oblique transmission), or from generation. The probability of this occurrence de-
a parent (i.e., vertical transmission). In addition, pends on the strength of conformance. If the strength
traits can be adopted from a single individual of conformity is 0%, transmission will be unbiased
(e.g., a prestigious individual or a parent), some and variation will increase as discussed above. If
subgroup of the population (e.g., a clique) or from the strength of conformity is 100%, variation will re-
the entire population (e.g., an average population main constant, equal only to the error rate.
value). These factors create a set of eight potential Fig. 2 shows the eects of six dierent levels of
transmission scenarios. In the case of cultural trans- strength of conformance on the CV. Data points
mission, however, the status of parents is indeter- represent average CV values calculated for 10 dier-
minate: anyone can serve as a parent for traits ent simulations. As shown, the CV increases over
and biological parents only have the potential to the generations in each simulation until a threshold
be one of the prestigious individuals. In our next is reached. After this threshold, CV stays constant
set of simulations, we focus on the eects of con- in equilibrium. The greater the strength of confor-
formist and prestige-biased transmission on vari- mity the faster it converges on the equilibrium val-
ance during trait inheritance. ue.1 A simple mathematical formula (i.e.,

Conformist transmission 1
In an innitely large population this threshold value would
represent an asymptote. Given a nite population and a
To model conformist transmission we set up a stochastic process, however, this value is actually reached within
similar simulation to the one above. In conformist the simulations.
324 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

0.3

0.25
1% Conformity
Coefficient of Variation

0.2

0.15

5% Conformity
0.1

10% Conformity

0.05
25% Conformity
50 & 100% Conf.

0
0

0
50
1

10

15

20

25

30

35

40
Generation

Fig. 2. Eects of dierent levels of conformist transmission on CV.

analytical solution) describing either the number of of variation within populations, as expected. The
generations required to reach this value or the equi- amount of variation removed depends on the
librium value itself is not evident to us. However, strength of conformist transmission. Surprisingly,
when the simulation data are plotted a clear rela- only a small amount (5%) of conformity is required
tionship between the nal CV and the strength of to reduce the nal CV by over half its original value
conformity is evident. Fig. 3 shows this relationship. when there is only unbiased transmission. This re-
A regression through these points suggests that the sult suggests, as other recent studies have also
CV is roughly equal to the inverse of the cube root shown (e.g., Henrich, 2004; Henrich and Boyd,
of the strength of conformity, multiplied by a 2002; Henrich and McElreath, 2003; McElreath
constant. et al., 2003; Shennan, 2000), that even small changes
Overall, the CV values from the conformist in how some people obtain information can have
transmission simulations show that this method of signicant eects on the structure and composition
acquiring cultural information reduces the amount of culture over time. Moreover, the use of conform-

0.25

0.2
Coefficient of Variation

0.15

0.1

0.05

0
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1
Strength of Conformity

Fig. 3. Eects of conformist transmission on nal CV.


J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 325

ist transmission greatly reduces the eect of drift parent they copy an alternative. In conformist trans-
and keeps attributes near the initial starting values mission this alternative is the mode or an approxi-
over time. mation of the average within the population, in
prestige-biased transmission it is a randomly select-
Prestige-biased transmission ed prestigious individual.
Again, even small amounts of such prestige-bi-
In prestige-biased transmission, traits are not ased transmission act to greatly reduce the amount
equally likely to be adopted from all individuals of variation in each generation, but not as much
within a population. Instead, certain individuals as conformist transmission. This result occurs be-
those who are prestigioushave a disproportion- cause in conformist transmission individuals must
ate probability of having traits copied. Our simple copy the average value of the previous generation
simulation assumes that all individuals are equally which does not vary much from generation to gen-
likely to be prestigious. In each generation a single eration. In contrast, individuals engaged in pres-
prestigious individual is chosen at random. In the tige-biased transmission could copy traits from an
following generation instead of adopting the trait individual with highly unusual values. As a result,
of their parent, each individual has a chance to prestige-biased transmission allows for much great-
adopt the trait of this prestigious person. The prob- er drift in attributes over time, unlike conformist
ability with which individuals do this is set for each transmission. Increasing the number of prestigious
simulation, but can be varied from simulation to social models individuals can copy from (i.e., in-
simulation (i.e., the strength of prestige-biased stead of only one as we have simulated) will serve
transmission). If we keep prestige within certain lin- to increase variation even further from that shown
eages rather than picking prestigious individuals in Fig. 4. Thus, as the number of prestigious
randomly each generation, individuals will be biased models approaches the population size the results
to copy this attribute values within the prestigious will look more and more like unbiased transmission
lineage instead of the average. However, the overall with respect to the generation of variation.
eect in our simulations will be the same as in con- In sum, many dierent biasing transmission pro-
formist transmission. cesses will act to reduce variation over time. In our
Results from the prestige simulations show that simulations above we have started with a homoge-
the eects on CV are very similar to those of con- nous population and allowed variation to build up
formist transmission (Fig. 4). This result was expect- over time. If, on the other hand, variation is high
ed because both models work in a similar way; when to begin with, the introduction of conformist and
individuals do not inherit a trait directly from their prestige-biased transmission can act to quickly

0.35

0.30
1% Prestige

0.25

0.20
5% Prestige
CV

0.15

0.10 10% Prestige

0.05 25% Prestige


50% Prestige
100% Prestige
0.00
0

0
50
1

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

Generation

Fig. 4. Eects of dierent levels of prestige-biased transmission on CV.


326 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

reduce variation, even with copying error. How out of 16 projectile points in the calculation of the
quickly this happens will depend, of course, on the CV, such running data make change appear gradu-
relative strength of these modes of acquiring cultur- al, when in fact it may be abrupt. Thus, the gradual
al information. increase and decrease of variation shown in Fig. 5 is
partly a product of the way the CV data are
Case studies generated.
Fig. 5 shows running CV for basal width and
Simulations provide the basis for generating null thickness of Rose Springs points. These two attri-
models. Their usefulness relies on application in the butes were selected for analysis because they repre-
empirical world. We now use the simulated data to sent dierent kinds of patterns in CV over time.
help explain the range of variation in two archaeo- We also examined other attributes, but do not re-
logical case studies. The case studies are not present- port them here because they mimic patterns in these
ed as exhaustive analyses of artifacts in a particular other attributes.
cultural setting (such analyses will form the basis of In the upper part of Fig. 5, variation in basal
future work). They are merely used to show how we width decreases through time, especially after 1250
can apply the ideas discussed above to examples BP. This pattern suggests that basal width was not
using real archaeological data. Each study examines subject to copying error alone, in which case we
changes in variance in assemblages of material arti- would expect rising CV. Instead selection or win-
facts over time and evaluates whether copying error nowing of forms seems to have taken place over
is sucient to explain the data, or if other variance time. For example, the use of either conformist or
amplifying or reducing cultural transmission pro- prestige-biased transmission (or both) around 1250
cesses must be considered. BP could also account for such winnowing. In fact,
if we assume an error rate of 5% during copying and
Owens Valley projectile points production of points, a rate determined experimen-
tally by Eerkens (2000), 25 years per generation, and
The rst study focuses on projectile point varia- a large population (over 20 point makers), we can
tion in California and the Great Basin. Rose Spring estimate the strength of conformity it would take
or Rosegate points (Bettinger and Taylor, 1974; to reduce the CV from 0.27 at 1250 BP to 0.17 at
Thomas, 1981) were introduced into Owens Valley 750 BP, roughly 20 generations. Using Eq. (2) and
around 1500 years ago and were used until roughly the results from the conformist transmission model
650 BP. These points have been well studied because above, it would take a conformity rate of about
many have been recovered and they represent the 10% at each transmission event. In other words,
transition from atlatl to bow and arrow hunting only 10% of the population would need to copy
(e.g., Bettinger and Eerkens, 1997, 1999; Delacorte, the average of the previous generation while the
1999; Fenenga, 1953; Lanning, 1963, p. 249; Thom- remaining 90% could inherit this trait directly from
as, 1981; Yohe, 1992). Moreover, most are made out their parents, with attending copying error in both
of obsidian, a material which allows for an indepen- cases. Thus, despite the addition of copying error,
dent measurement of age of manufacture using the CV would decrease through time by this amount
hydration techniques (Ericson, 1989; Hall and Jack- with a small amount of conformity.
son, 1989; Hull, 2001; Jones et al., 1997; Meighan, Thickness in Rose Springs projectile points be-
1983). haves quite dierently, getting increasingly more
Over 100 individual Rose Springs points from variable through time. Such an increase is akin to
Owens Valley with hydration measurements were our simulations for copying error above. In fact
arranged from youngest to oldest based on their we can determine how much copying error is re-
age estimates. We calculated a running CV across quired by using Eq. (2) and solving for c, the error
the age-stratied sample (16 projectile points repre- rate. Doing so, we nd that to increase the CV from
senting each data point). In other words, the data 0.15 at 1250 BP to 0.20 at 750 BP, representing
are organized much like paleoclimatic graphs where roughly 20 generations, would require 5.8% of copy-
a running average through data points, such as tree ing error in each generation, close to experimentally
ring width, represents trends through time in past determined rates (Eerkens, 2000). This suggests that
climate, not actual climate. Since successive data the increase in variation we see in thickness may be
points along the x-axis are correlated, sharing 15 due primarily, if not completely, to errors in copy-
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 327

Fig. 5. Variation in Rose Springs projectile points from Owens Valley.

ing. In other words, from the initial value at the time with the type of sorting caused by prestige and con-
of invention or introduction of the point type in formist transmission. Perhaps certain individuals
Owens Valley, thickness values seem to be drifting experimented with dierent forms and arrived at im-
in a random-like process due to copying error, proved forms in this newly introduced technology.
therefore, increasing slowly in variation through Aspiring hunters may have opted to copy the technol-
time. Other variation-increasing explanations such ogy of these successful variants or copy their approx-
as experimentation or innovation are unlikely and imation of the average or mode within the
not necessary to account for these changes. population, rather than have to expend eort experi-
These ndings are in line with what we know menting (i.e., innovating) themselves. This kind of
about projectile technologies and previous archaeo- pattern is expected. Patterns of exploration and sort-
logical ndings in the region. It is not surprising that ing are commonplace in many instances of evolution
variation in an attribute like basal width decreases when new kinds of forms are introducedwhether
due to selection while thickness changes more in a those forms are technological (e.g., Basalla, 1988;
drift-like fashion. In previous analyses we found Hargadon, 2003) or biological (e.g., Gould, 1990)
that the introduction of the bow and arrow in On the other hand, within certain tolerances,
Owens Valley appeared to correspond with patterns thickness has less inuence on the function of a pro-
of variation consistent with heavy experimentation jectile point and thus is more likely to vary as
(Bettinger and Eerkens, 1999). Since basal width expected for a stylistic attribute (sensu Dunnell,
controls the hafting of a point to a foreshaft and 1978b; Weissner, 1983). With less contribution to
strongly aects the function of an arrow, it stands the overall function, greater magnitudes and rates
to reason that inecient shapes would be quickly of copying errors in thickness could have been toler-
winnowed from the pool of variants. Such winnow- ated. As a result, variation would have been free to
ing would be accelerated if it occurred in tandem increase in this dimension.
328 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

Illinois Woodland Pot Sherds independent observations. As a result, the CV is


much more variable around the trend line. This is
The second case study uses data published by not a product of sample size of pot sherds, as small
Braun (1977, 1985) and subsequently analyzed by samples (812) have the same range of CV values as
Neiman (1995) for Woodland-period ceramics (ca. large samples (over 50). Instead, this variability
2500900 BP) from Illinois. Each pottery assem- about the trend line is inherent in the data.
blage is associated with a unique radiocarbon-dated Braun (1985, 1987, 1991) focused mainly on
feature. While Neiman (1995) used the data to sherd thickness in his work. In particular, he exam-
examine drift and innovation of ceramic types ined rates of change in the average thickness of
(i.e., classicatory or non-continuous attributes), sherds. Using the rst-order derivative of a time ser-
we focus on two attributes that Braun measured ies curve through the average sherd thickness, he
on a continuous scale akin to the simulations pre- extracted ve ceramic trends indicating dierent
sented above, thickness and pot diameter. We used selective regimes for thickness (Braun, 1987). The
these two attributes precisely because they are the most signicant of these include a period of selec-
only two measured on a continuous scale, and for tion for thicker pots between 2200 and 1900 BP
that reason are comparable to our model and (from 7.5 to about 8.5 mm), a strong and prolonged
simulations. period of thinning pots between about 1900 and
We calculated a CV for each assemblage for these 1600 BP (from 8.5 to 6.0 mm), and a shorter and
two attributes and sorted the assemblages by radio- weaker period of selection for thinner pots between
carbon date. Fig. 6 plots the data. Thus, unlike the 1500 and 1300 BP (from 6.0 to 5.5 mm). Braun re-
projectile point data, successive data points in Fig. 6 ferred to these as Trend 1, Trend 2, and Trend 4,
are not correlated and are composed entirely of respectively.

Fig. 6. Variation in Woodland ceramics from Illinois.


J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 329

We expected to see that changes in variation here, keeping variation nearly constant over time.
somehow followed these selective regimes. Periods Conformist and/or prestige-biased transmission
of high selection should be followed by periods dis- may also have been factors in these prehistoric
playing a reduction in variation. Unfortunately, as developments. Additional research would be neces-
seen in Fig. 3, there seem to be only minor changes sary to tease these processes apart.
in variation for thickness, despite the fact that aver- Changes in the CV of diameter measurements
age thickness changed substantially over this time over time are more pronounced. Variation appears
(Braun, 1977, 1987, 1991). Our binomial regression to be rather high in the earliest assemblages and
indicates a slight increase in variation during the drops perceptibly from 2500 BP until around
early part of the sequence between 2400 and 1900 1800 BP, is steady between 1800 BP and 1400 BP,
BP, similar to the results obtained by Neiman and rises noticeably after this date. Overall, this
(1995, :20). Signicantly, the magnitude of this in- pattern suggests pots were initially fairly variable
crease is less than we would expect from copying er- but that over time became increasingly standard-
ror, suggesting that some variation-reducing ized in diameter, likely as certain sizes and shapes
processes were in eect during this period. The in- were removed from the pool of variants. Again,
crease in variation corresponds temporally to these patterns may be explicable by invoking pro-
Brauns ceramic trend 1, which represents an in- cesses such as conformist or prestige-biased trans-
crease in average thickness (Braun, 1987). Thus, mission that resulted in the decrease in variation.
the increase in thickness is accompanied by a slight Note that such processes can operate in a comple-
increase in variation as well, though less of an in- mentary fashion to others, such as production stan-
crease than we would expect by copying errors dardization and intensity of production as recently
alone. This pattern is consistent with the expecta- outlined by Roux (2003). Thus, production special-
tion that a sorting process such as conformist or ists can invoke certain transmission processes to
prestige-biased transmission favored thicker pots, achieve standardization or increase production
dampening the eects of the transmission of copying intensity.
errors. In any event, the changes are not clearly After 1400 BP, however, variation increased
explicable as the result of random stochastic pro- again from a CV of ca. 0.3 at 1400 BP to a CV of
cesses alone. 0.45 at 1000 BP. If we again assume 25 radiocarbon
The increase in variation in Fig. 3 is followed by years per generation these 400 years represent about
a leveling o after 1900 BP, corresponding to 16 generations. As with the projectile points above,
Brauns trend 2, and a slight decrease in the later we can compare this increase to our simulated
part of the sequence, after 1400 BP, corresponding curves and use Eq. (2) to solve for the error rate nec-
to Brauns trend 4. Both the leveling o and de- essary to achieve such an increase. If we assume the
crease in variation imply that greater selective or increase in CV was due solely to copying errors, we
winnowing processes were at work to oset the ef- would need an error rate of approximately 15.7% to
fects of copying errors. These eects may be what see such an increase. Even given noise in our data
Braun identied as selective regimes that acted set this is much larger than experimentally deter-
to change the average thickness of sherds. mined rates (Eerkens, 2000). Thus, the increased
Overall, the patterns in CV suggest that thickness variation in pot diameters witnessed from 1800 to
of Woodland pots was not transmitted in a drift-like 1400 BP was likely due to processes that were above
process, but instead were subject to fairly stable our hypothetical perception threshold. The intro-
rates of selection. This is consistent with our under- duction of variationalso known as discovery,
standing of functional traits such as thickness. It is invention, or innovationis one process by which
expected that vessel thickness is directly related to this increase could have come about, though other
performance of the vessel (e.g., Juhl, 1995), and is variation-increasing forces may also have been at
only practical within a range of values for a partic- work such as those listed on the right side of Table
ular pot type. If a relatively stable number of pot 1. Such innovation may have come about for a
shapes were made, variation in thickness may have number of reasons, including changes in the func-
remained quite stable over time despite changes in tion or social role of pots, or an increase in the num-
modal values. Thus, individual selection and remov- ber of potters and how they learned, copied, and
al of pots with inferior thickness for whatever pur- interacted with one another. Again, future research
pose they were put to may have played a role could seek to specify such factors.
330 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

Conclusions iation over time. Moreover, we aimed to examine


how cognitive sorting processes amplify, reduce, or
The simulation results provide a means for gener- keep constant variation introduced due to copying
ating potential explanations about our observations error. Modeling these processes enables us to estab-
of temporal changes in variance in projectile points lish baseline measurements against which we can
in Owens Valley, California and Woodland ceram- compare artifacts from the archaeological record
ics in Illinois. Without null-hypotheses to generate to provide more powerful explanations for the data
expectations it is unclear whether the changes we we generate.
see in these cases are culturally or behaviorally sig- Archaeological explanations require theoretically
nicant or whether they are simply due to drift-like robust descriptions. The approach taken here gives
processes. Null hypotheses give us a means of ask- us tools for making measurements of artifacts in a
ing questions of our data (e.g., Brantingham, way that can then be explained. Our theoretical
2003). Unless there are good reasons, Occams Ra- structure provides a falsiable hypothesis for chang-
zor should generally apply. That is, one should es in artifact variation through time. This is a key
not provide complex arguments or invoke multiple component of the formal structure of archaeological
and complex entities when trying to explain some- explanations: we have no means for directly observ-
thing when a simple answer will suce. The use of ing artifact variation as the result of copying errors
null hypotheses can help us in this regard. related to particular artisans. Instead, we must de-
In the case of Rose Spring projectile points, sim- scribe the world in theoretically explicable ways
ple errors in copying can fully account for the var- and build hypotheses that we can use to explain
iation witnessed in thickness measurements. It is these descriptions (sensu Lewontin, 1974). For this
not necessary to invoke other explanations, though reason, modeling the behavior of aggregate mea-
of course we cannot rule out that other factors surements like the CV or the average is desirable be-
may have been at play. This was not the case for cause we can rarely examine change in the
basal width measurements, where variation was archaeological record at the scale of the individual.
much less than expected due to drift-like processes. We need methods and measurements that model
There, variance-reducing mechanisms must have change at the level of aggregates, even though such
been in eect. In the case of vessel diameter in Late changes are the composite result of innumerable
Woodland pots, small copying errors are also not decisions by individuals.
enough to explain an increase in variation over Furthermore, it is important that we collect data
the last 400 years of the sequence. Thus, other var- at the proper scale to employ such baseline measure-
iation-increasing factors must have been at play, ments. For example, when archaeologists collect
such as experimentation or invention, leading categorical data about artifacts, as is most common-
to an increase in the range of diameter measure- ly done (e.g., types or presence/absence of
ments for ceramic vessels. some trait) it is dicult to track the kinds of
Our point in this paper was not to generate full small-scale processes we talk about here. Types are
explanations for the specics of Great Basin points ideational structures built by us for measuring vari-
or Woodland pots. We do not suggest that hunters ation in the archaeological record (Dunnell, 1971).
or potters always produced their tools entirely from Our types are merely measurement tools without
memory, without comparison to external standards, any necessary relation to the units used by past pop-
or that their ideas about what the ideal size and ulations. One of the issues with using decorative ele-
shape for stone tools and pots was constant ments or presence/absence attributes (e.g., serration
throughout their lives. Nor do we argue that chang- on projectile points) is that it is more likely that
es in settlement patterns, social organization, and these will be subject to modication by cognitive
the like are irrelevant to the structure of variation processes such as invention, rather than simple
in artifacts (though we believe copying error to be copying error. This does not mean that change in
a human universal that can transcend such chang- these kinds of attributes cannot be modeled by
es). Instead, our goal was to introduce a framework errors accumulated during the copying process. As
for investigating copying errors and their eects shown by Neiman (1995) relative abundances of
when transmitted through time. As we have seen, such types clearly drift due to copying errors
imperceptibly small dierences passed on during when objects are made in large enough numbers.
transmission can lead to signicant increases in var- However, establishing baseline measurements for
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 331

the actual innovation rate for types is poorly References


known and requires further research, simulation,
and consideration. Ahler, S.A., 1979. Functional analysis of nonobsidian chipped
stone artifacts: terms, variables, and quantication. In:
To date, the most amenable attributes for track-
Hayden, B. (Ed.), Lithic Use-Wear Analysis. Academic Press,
ing changes in variation due to copying errors are New York, pp. 301328.
those that are measured on continuous scales such Alexander, C., 1970. Notes on the Synthesis of Form. Harvard
as length, width, thickness, angle, weight, and so University Press, Boston.
on. It is usually possible to collect such information Atran, S., 2001. The trouble with memes: inference versus
from discrete object-type artifacts. For example, we imitation in cultural creation. Human Nature 12, 351381.
Aunger, R., 2000. The life history of culture learning in a face-to-
can easily measure line thickness on decoration mo- face society. Ethos 28, 138.
tifs instead of categorical statements about presence Aunger, R., 2002. The Electric Meme: A New Theory of How We
or absence of some design. Most cultural historical Think. Free Press, New York.
types, however, reduce metric variation to attribute Basalla, G., 1988. The Evolution of Technology. Cambridge
classes. This makes it dicult to model expected University Press, Cambridge.
Beck, C., 1995. Functional attributes and the dierential persis-
changes through time given copying error, because tence of Great Basin dart forms. Journal of California and
we have no basis upon which to calculate an expect- Great Basin Anthropology 17, 222243.
ed rate of variation generation (i.e., innovation in Bentley, R.A., Hahn, M.W., Shennan, S.J., 2004. Random drift
this case). and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London Series BBiological Sciences 271, 14431450.
Finally, not all archaeological problems require
Bentley, R.A., Shennan, S., 2003. Cultural evolution and
an understanding, or the application, of cultural stochastic network growth. American Antiquity 68, 459485.
transmission theory. However, we believe that the Bettinger, R.L., Eerkens, J.W., 1999. Point typologies, cultural
application of such models can in many cases pro- transmission, and the spread of bow-and-arrow technology
vide great insight into the explanation of the in the prehistoric Great Basin. American Antiquity 64,
archaeological record, particularly when we are 231242.
Bettinger, R.L., Eerkens, J.W., 1997. Evolutionary implications
tracking changes through time. The processes by of metrical variation in Great Basin projectile points. In:
which people obtain and transmit cultural informa- Barton, C.M., Clark, G.A. (Eds.), Rediscovering Darwin:
tion fundamentally structures many elements of Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation. Amer-
material culture. In particular, it has strong eects ican Anthropological Association, Arlington, pp. 177191.
on variation in artifacts, something archaeologists Bettinger, R.L., Taylor, R.E., 1974. Suggested revisions in
archaeological sequences of the Great Basin in interior
have long been interested in. Thus, cultural trans- Southern California. Nevada Archaeological Survey Research
mission has important implications for many Papers 5, pp. 126.
aggregate assemblage measures such as the number Binford, L., 1962. Archaeology as anthropology. American
of types, average size, median diameter, CV, and Antiquity 28, 217225.
standard deviation, among others, measures that Binford, L., 1973. Interassemblage variability: the Mousterian
and the functional argument. In: Renfrew, C. (Ed.), Expla-
are commonly reported in the archaeological liter- nation of Culture Change: Models in Prehistory. Duckworth,
ature. To this end, we believe that frameworks that Liverpool, pp. 217226.
can be used to tease apart dierent cultural trans- Binford, L.R., Binford, S.R., 1966. A preliminary analysis of
mission processes will enable us to explain the functional variability in Mousterian of Levallois facies.
archaeological record in a more scientically struc- American Anthropologist 68, 238295.
Blitz, J.H., 1993. Big pots for big shots: feasting and storage in a
tured manner. Mississippian community. American Antiquity 58, 8096.
Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J., 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary
Acknowledgments Process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J., 1995a. Life in the fast lane: rapid
cultural change and the human evolutionary process. In:
We thank Loukas Barton, Robert Bettinger,
Changeux, J.P., Unavaillon, J. (Eds.), Origins of the Human
Daniel O. Larson, Mark Madsen, Richard McElre- Brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 155169.
ath, Pete Richerson, and two anonymous reviewers Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J., 1995b. Why does culture increase
for comments on previous drafts, useful discussions human adaptability?. Ethology and Sociobiology 16 125
and insight on the subject of variation generation. 143.
Finally, we owe much gratitude to Mark Grote Boyer, P., 1999. Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: how
evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission.
for helping us derive equations from the model American Anthropologist 100, 876889.
and discussions in general. Any mistakes, of course, Brantingham, P.J., 2003. A neutral model of stone raw material
remain wholly our own. procurement. American Antiquity 68, 487509.
332 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

Braun, D.P., 1977. Middle WoodlandEarly Late Woodland Down Boundaries: Anthropological Approaches to Cultural
Social Change in the Prehistoric Central Midwestern US Transmission and Material Culture. University of Arizona
University Microlms, Ann Arbor. Press, Tucson.
Braun, D.P., 1985. Ceramic decorative diversity and Illinois Ericson, J.E., 1989. Toward ow-specic obsidian hydration
woodland regional integration. In: Nelson, B.A. (Ed.), rates: Coso Volcanic Field, Inyo County, California. In:
Decoding Prehistoric Ceramics. Southern Illinois University Hughes, R.E. (Ed.), Current Directions in California Obsid-
Press, Carbondale, pp. 128153. ian Studies. Contributions of the University of California
Braun, D.P., 1987. Coevolution of sedentism, pottery technology, Archaeological Research Facility No. 45, Berkeley, CA, pp.
and horticulture in the central Midwest, 200 B.C.A.D. 600. 1322.
In: Keegan, W.F. (Ed.), Emergent Horticultural Economies Ericson, J.E., Read, D.W., Burke, C., 1972. Research design: the
of the Eastern Woodlands. Southern Illinois University Press, relationships between the primary functions and the physical
Carbondale, pp. 153181. properties of ceramic vessels and their implications for
Braun, D.P., 1991. Why decorate a pot? Midwestern household ceramic distributions on an archaeological site. Anthropology
pottery, 200 B.C.A.D. 600. Journal of Anthropological UCLA 3, 8495.
Archaeology 10, 360397. Evans, J., 1850. On the date of British coins. The Numismatic
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Feldman, M.W., 1981. Cultural Transmis- Chronicle and the Journal of the Numismatic Society 12, 127
sion and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton 137.
University Press, Princeton. Evans, J., 1875. On the coinage of the ancient Britons and natural
Cochrane, E.E., 2002. Separating time and space in archaeolog- selection. Royal Institution of Great Britain, Proceedings 7,
ical landscapes: An example from Windward Society Islands 24-32.
Ceremonial Architecture. In: Ladefoged, T.N., Graves, M.W. Evans, R.K., 1978. Early craft specialization: an example from
(Eds.), Pacic Landscapes: Archaeological Approaches. Eas- the Balkan Chalcolithic. In: Redman, C.L. (Ed.), Social
ter Island Foundation Press, Los Osos, CA, pp. 189209. Archaeology: Beyond Subsistence and Dating. Academic
Cordell, A.S., 1993. Chronological variability in ceramic paste: a Press, New York, pp. 113129.
comparison of Deptford and Savannah Period pottery in the Fenenga, F., 1953. The weight of chipped stone points: a clue to
St. Marys River region of Northeast Florida and Southeast their functions. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 9,
Georgia. Southeastern Archaeology 12, 3358. 309333.
Coren, S.L., Ward, L.M., Enns, J.T., 1994. Sensation and Ford, J.A., 1935. Ceramic decoration sequence at an Old Indian
Perception. Harcourt Brace, Fort Worth. Village Site, near Sicily Island, Louisiana. Louisiana State
Costin, C.L., Hagstrum, M.B., 1995. Standardization, labor Geological Survey, Department of Conservation, Anthropo-
investment, skill, and the organization of ceramic production logical Study 1.
in late prehispanic highland Peru. American Antiquity 60, Ford, J.A., 1938. A chronological method applicable to the
619639. Southeast. American Antiquity 3, 260264.
Dawkins, R., 1976. The Selsh Gene. Oxford University Press, Gabora, L.M., 2000. The beer can theory of creativity. In:
Oxford. Bentley, P., Corne, D. (Eds.), Creative Evolutionary Systems.
Delacorte, M.G. 1999. The Changing Role of Riverine Environ- Morgan Kaufman Publishers, San Francisco, pp. 126.
ments in the Prehistory of the Central-Western Great Basin: Gabora, L.M., 2004. Ideas are not replicators but minds are.
Data Recovery Excavations at Six Prehistoric Sites in Owens Biology and Philosophy 19, 127143.
Valley, California. Report prepared by Far Western Anthro- Gardiner, C.W., 1985. Handbook of Stochastic Methods for
pological Research Group, Davis, California. Physics, Chemistry, and the Natural Sciences, second ed.
Dunnell, R.C., 1971. Systematics in Prehistory. Free Press, New Springer, New York.
York. Gould, S.J., 1990. Wonderful Life: The Burgess Shale and the
Dunnell, R.C., 1978a. Archaeological potential of anthropolog- Nature of History. W.W. Norton, New York.
ical and scientic models of function. In: Dunnell, R.C., Hall, Graves, M.W., Cachola-Abad, C.K., 1996. Seriation as a method
E.S. (Eds.), Archaeological Essays in Honor of Irving of chronologically ordering architectural design traits: a
Benjamin Rouse. Mouton, The Hague, pp. 4173. example from Hawaii. Archaeology in Oceania 31, 1932.
Dunnell, R.C., 1978b. Style and function: a fundamental dichot- Hall, M.C., Jackson, R.J. 1989. Obsidian hydration rates in
omy. American Antiquity 43, 192202. California. In: Hughes, R.E. (Ed.), Current Directions in
Dunnell, R.C., 1986. Five decades of American archaeology. In: California Obsidian Studies. Contributions of the University
Meltzer, D., Fowler, D., Sablo, J. (Eds.), American Archae- of California Archaeological Research Facility No. 45,
ology Past and Future. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, Berkeley, CA, pp. 3158.
DC, pp. 2349. Hargadon, A., 2003. How Breakthroughs happen: The Surprising
Eerkens, J.W., 2000. Practice makes within 5% of perfect: the role Truth about how Companies Innovate. Harvard Business
of visual perception, motor skills, and human memory in School Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
artifact variation and standardization. Current Anthropology Henrich, J., 2001. Cultural transmission and the diusion of
41, 663668. innovations: adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural
Eerkens, J.W., Bettinger, R.L., 2001. Techniques for assessing transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change
standardization in artifact assemblages: can we scale material and much of sociocultural evolution. American Anthropolo-
variability? American Antiquity 66, 493504. gist 103, 9921013.
Eerkens, J.W., Lipo, C.P., in press. Cultural Transmission of Henrich, J., 2004. Demography and cultural evolution: why
copying errors and the evolution of variation in Woodland adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in
pots. In: Bowser, B., Horne, L., Stark, M. (Eds.), Breaking Tasmania. American Antiquity 69, 197214.
J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334 333

Henrich, J., Boyd, R., 2002. On modeling cognition and culture: Lipo, C.P., Madsen, M., 2001. Neutrality, style and drift:
why cultural evolution does not require replication of building method for studying cultural transmission in the
representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2, 87112. archaeological record. In: Hurt, T.D., Rakita, G.F.M. (Eds.),
Henrich, J., Gil-White, F.J., 2001. The evolution of prestige: Style and Function: Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary
freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the Archaeology. Bergin and Garvey, Westport, Connecticut,
benets of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human pp. 91118.
Behavior 22, 165196. Lipo, C.P., Madsen, M., Dunnell, R., Hunt, T., 1997. Population
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., 2003. The evolution of cultural structure, cultural transmission, and frequency seriation.
evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 12, 123135. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 16, 33.
Hill, J., 1977a. Explanation of Prehistoric Change. University of Lumsden, C.J., Wilson, E.O., 1981. Genes, Mind, and Culture:
New Mexico, Albuquerque. The Coevolutionary Process. Harvard University Press,
Hill, J., 1977b. Individual variability in ceramics and the study of Cambridge, MA.
prehistoric social organization. In: Hill, J., Gunn, J. (Eds.), Lyman, R.L., OBrien, M.J., 2003. Cultural traits: units of
The Individual in Prehistory. Academic Press, New York, pp. analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of
55108. Anthropological Research 59, 225250.
Holmes, W.H., 1890. On the evolution of ornamentAn Lyman, R.L., OBrien, M.J., Dunnell, R.C., 1997. The Rise and
American lesson. American Anthropologist 3, 137146. Fall of Culture History. Plenum Press, New York.
Holmes, W.H., 1891. Manufacture of stone arrow-points. McElreath, R., Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J., 2003. Shared norms
American Anthropologist 4, 4958. and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology
Holmes, W.H., 1894. Natural history of aked stone implements. 44, 122129.
In: Wake, C.S. (Ed.), Memoirs of the International Congress Meighan, C.W., 1983. Obsidian dating in California: theory and
of Anthropology. Schulte, Chicago, pp. 120139. practice. American Antiquity 48, 600609.
Hull, K.L., 2001. Reasserting the utility of obsidian hydration Meltzer, D.J., 1981. A study of style and function in a class of
dating: a temperature-dependent empirical approach to tools. Journal of Field Archaeology 8, 313326.
practical temporal resolution with archaeological obsidians. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., Laland, K.N., 2004. Perspective: is
Journal of Archaeological Science 28, 10251040. human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from
Jones, M., Sheppard, P.J., Sutton, D,G., 1997. Soil temperature the perspective of The Origins of Species. Evolution 58, 111.
and obsidian hydration dating: a clarication of variables Mills, B.J., 1989. Integrating functional analyses of vessels and
aecting accuracy. Journal of Archaeological Science 24, 505 sherds through models of ceramic assemblage formation.
516. World Archaeology 21, 133147.
Juhl, K., 1995. The Relation between Vessel Form and Vessel Neiman, F., 1995. Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective:
Function: A Methodological Study. Arkeologisk Museum i inferences from decorative diversity and inter-assemblage
Stavanger, Stavanger. distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic assemblages. American
Kidder, A.V., 1917. A design-sequence from New Mexico. Antiquity 60, 137.
National Academy of Sciences, Proceedings 3, 369370. Norwich, K.H., 1983. On the theory of Weber fractions.
Kohler, T.A., Van Buskirk, S., Ruscavage-Barz, S., 2004. Vessels Perception and Psychophysics 42, 286298.
and villages: evidence for conformist transmission in early OBrien, M.J., Darwent, J., Lyman, R.L., 2001. Cladistics is
village aggregations. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Paleo-
23, 100118. indian points from the southeastern United States. Journal of
Kramer, C., 1985. Ceramic ethnoarchaeology. Annual Review of Archaeological Science 28, 11151136.
Anthropology 14, 77102. OBrien, M.J., Holland, T.D., 1990. Variation, selection, and the
Kramer, C., 1979. An archaeological view of a contemporary archaeological record. Archaeological Method and Theory 2,
Kurdish village: domestic architecture, household size, and 3179.
wealth. In: Kramer, C. (Ed.), Ethnoarchaeology. Cambridge OBrien, M.J., Lyman, R.L., 2000. Applying Evolutionary
University Press, Cambridge, pp. 139163. Archaeology. Kluwer Academic, New York.
Kroeber, A.L., 1916. Zuni Culture sequences. American Museum OBrien, M.J., Lyman, R.L., 2003. Cladistics and Archaeology.
of Natural History, Anthropological Papers 18, 1-37. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.
Kroeber, A.L., 1919. On the principle of order in civilizations as Palmer, J., 1988. Very short-term visual memory for size and
exemplied by changes of fashion. American Anthropologist shape. Perception and Psychophysics 43, 278286.
21, 235263. Plog, S., 1976. The inference of prehistoric social organization
Lanning, E.P., 1963. Archaeology of the Rose Spring Site (INY- from ceramic design variability. Michigan Discussions in
372). University of California Publications in American Anthropology 1, 147.
Archaeology and Ethnology 49, 237336. Plotkin, H.C., 2002. Learning from culture. In: Wheeler, M.,
Levin, M.E., 1976. On the ascription of functions to objects, with Ziman, J., Boden, M. (Eds.), The Evolution of Cultural
special reference to inference in archaeology. Philosophy of Entities. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 339358.
the Social Sciences 6, 227234. Pocklington, R., Best, M.L., 1997. Cultural evolution and units
Lewontin, R.C., 1974. The Genetic Basis of Evolutionary of selection in replicating text. Journal of Theoretical Biology
Change. Columbia University Press, New York. 188, 7987.
Lipo, C.P., 2001. Science, Style and the Study of Community Rau, C., 1896. The Archaeological Collections of the United
Structure: An Example from the Central Mississippi River States National Museums in Charge of the Smithsonian
Valley. British Archaeological Reports, International Series, Institution. Smithsonian Contributions to Knowledge XXII,
No. 918, Oxford. Article 4. Washington, DC.
334 J.W. Eerkens, C.P. Lipo / Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24 (2005) 316334

Rogers, E.M., 2003. Diusion of Innovations, fth ed. Free Press, Sperber, D., 1996. Explaining Culture: a Naturalistic Approach.
New York. Blackwell, Oxford.
Roux, V., 2003. Ceramic standardization and intensity of Spier, L., 1917. An outline for a chronology of Zuni Ruins.
production: quantifying degrees of specialization. American Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural
Antiquity 68, 768782. History 18 (3), 207331.
Scheinsohn, V., Ferretti, J.L., 1995. The mechanical properties of Symens, N., 1986. Functional analysis of selected stone artifacts
bone materials in relation to the design and function of from the Magdalenian site at Verberie, France. Journal of
prehistoric tools from Tierra Del Fuego, Argentina. Journal Field Archaeology 13, 213222.
of Archaeological Science 22, 711. Thomas, D.H., 1981. How to classify the projectile points from
Schier, M.B., 1990. Inuence of surface treatment on heating Monitor Valley, Nevada. Journal of California and Great
eectiveness of ceramic vessels. Journal of Archaeological Basin Anthropology 3, 743.
Science 17, 373381. Turner II, C.G., Lofgren, L., 1966. Household size of prehistoric
Schier, M.B., Skibo, J.M., Boelke, T.C., Neupert, M.A., Western Pueblo Indians. Southwestern Journal of Anthro-
Aronson, M., 1994. New perspectives on experimental pology 22, 117132.
archaeology: surface treatments and thermal responses of van Doorn, A.J., van de Grind, W.A., Koenderink, J.J. (Eds.),
the clay cooking pot. American Antiquity 59, 197217. . Limits in Perception: Essays in Honour of Maarten Bouman..
Shennan, S.J., 1989. Cultural transmission and cultural change. VNU Science Press, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
In: van der Leeuw, S.E., Torrence, R. (Eds.), Whats New? A van der Leeuw, S.E., Torrence, R. (Eds.), 1989. Whats New?: A
Closer Look at the Process of Innovation. Unwin Hyman, Closer Look at the Process of Innovation.. Unwin Hyman,
London, pp. 330346. London.
Shennan, S.J., 1991. Tradition, rationality and cultural transmis- Weissner, P., 1983. Style and social information in Kalahari San
sion. In: Preucel, R. (Ed.), Processual and Postprocessual projectile points. American Antiquity 48, 253276.
Archaeologies: Multiple Ways of Knowing the Past. Center Wheeler, M., Ziman, J., Boden, M.A. (Eds.), 2002. The
for Archaeological Investigations, Southern Illinois Universi- Evolution of Cultural Entities. Oxford University Press,
ty at Carbondale, Carbondale, pp. 197208. Oxford.
Shennan, S.J., 2000. Population, culture history and the dynamics Williams, G.C., 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection: A
of culture change. Current Anthropology 41, 811835. Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton
Shennan, S.J., 2002. Genes, Memes and Human History. Thames University Press, Princeton.
and Hudson, London. Wilson, T., 1891. Forms of ancient arrow-heads. American
Shennan, S.J., Wilkinson, J.R., 2001. Ceramic style change and Anthropologist 4, 5863.
neutral evolution: a case study from Neolithic Europe. Wobst, H.M., 1977. Stylistic behavior and information exchange.
American Antiquity 66, 577594. In: Cleland, C. (Ed.), For the Director: Research Essays in
Simons, D.J., Franconeri, S.L., Reimer, R.L., 2000. Change Honor of James B. Grin. University of Michigan Anthro-
blindness in the absence of a visual disruption. Perception 29, pology Paper 61, Ann Arbor, pp. 317342.
11431154. Wright, S., 1970. Random drift and the shifting balance theory of
Skibo, J.M., 1992. Pottery Function: A Use-Alteration Perspec- evolution. In: Kojima, K. (Ed.), Mathematical Topics in
tive. Plenum, New York. Population Genetics. Springer, Berlin, pp. 131.
Skibo, J.M., Schier, M.B., Reid, K.C., 1989. Organic-tempered Yohe, R.M., 1992. A reevaluation of Western Great Basin
pottery: an experimental study. American Antiquity 54, 122 cultural chronology and evidence for the timing of the
146. introduction of the bow and arrow to Eastern Califor-
Smith, M.F.J., 1985. Toward an economic interpretation of nia based on new excavations at the Rose Spring Site
ceramics: relating vessel size and shape to use. In: Nelson, (CA-INY-372). Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Depart-
B.A. (Ed.), Decoding Prehistoric Ceramics. Southern Illinois ment of Anthropology, University of California,
University Press, Carbondale, pp. 254309. Riverside.

You might also like