You are on page 1of 11

What Learning a Second Language Might

Teach Us about Auditory Training


Joe Barcroft, Ph.D.,1 Mitchell Sommers, Ph.D.,2 and NancyTye-Murray, Ph.D.3

ABSTRACT

In this article, we apply theory and research from the field of second
language acquisition (SLA) to issues in auditory training for adults with
postlingual deafness. Two areas of SLA theory and research are discussed.
The first area concerns five hypotheses proposed by S. Krashen about SLA.
We apply these hypotheses to issues in auditory training, such as the issue of
using instructional techniques that promote development of implicit knowl-
edge of target structures, the role of meaning-bearing comprehensible input,
and the importance of creating an affectively positive learning environment.
The second area concerns research on acoustic variability and second
language (L2) learning. This research reinforces Krashens hypothesis about
attending to the nature of input during SLA, but does so with regard to how
spoken input may be acoustically varied to facilitate acquisition. Studies have
demonstrated that presentation formats with talker variability are effective
for training learners on L2 phonemic contrasts and that presentation
formats with talker, speaking-style, and speaking-rate variability (but not
amplitude and fundamental-frequency variability) yield positive additive
effects on L2 vocabulary learning. In light of these findings, we discuss how
acoustically varied presentation formats may be used in auditory training.

KEYWORDS: Input hypothesis, explicit and implicit learning, acoustic


variability

Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the participant will be able to apply theory and research in the
field of SLA to issues in auditory training.

1
Department of Romance Languages and Literatures, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130-4899.
Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri; 2Department E-mail: barcroft@artsci.wustl.edu.
of Psychology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Auditory Training; Guest Editor, Robert W. Sweetow,
Louis, Missouri; 3Department of Otolaryngology, Central Ph.D.
Institute for the Deaf at Washington University School of Semin Hear 2007;28:151162. Copyright # 2007 by
Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri. Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue,
Address for correspondence and reprint requests: Joe New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 5844662.
Barcroft, Ph.D., Department of Romance Languages and DOI 10.1055/s-2007-973441. ISSN. 0734-0451.
Literatures, Campus Box 1077, One Brookings Drive,
151
152 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 28, NUMBER 2 2007

R esearchers in the field of second lan- logical word forms onto a preexisting linguistic
guage acquisition (SLA) seek to understand system. Many of the implications discussed,
processes involved in the acquisition of non- however, may be applicable to auditory training
native languages. Questions addressed by SLA with other populations as well.
researchers commonly overlap with those in
linguistics and psychology and may overlap
with questions related to auditory training. KRASHENS FIVE HYPOTHESES
For example, one important commonality be- From the late 1970s through the mid 1980s,
tween second language (L2) learning and audi- Krashen14 proposed five hypotheses that im-
tory training is the need for learners to encode pacted greatly on the field of SLA and L2
new form, or signal, and to map form onto instruction. These five hypotheses painted a
meaning. L2 learners encode novel forms in the general picture of what is needed for successful
L2 and map these forms onto meaning. Sim- SLA and how, from a general perspective, SLA
ilarly, clients who receive auditory training takes place. Within the field of SLA, Krashens
attend to new forms of the speech signal (that hypotheses have helped to stimulate a great deal
result from processing through a sensory aid) of new research, such as research on principles
and map these forms onto meaning, even if the of L2 input processing (see, e.g., VanPatten5).
new forms in question are part of a previously In terms of L2 instruction, the principles have
acquired language that the person with hearing provided impetus and underlying rationale for
loss has come to perceive differently. communicative language teaching, an instruc-
In this article, we explore potential appli- tional approach that emphasizes the use of
cations of theory and research on SLA to a linguistic form to convey meaning within an
variety of issues in auditory training. More affectively positive learning environment.
specifically, we consider five hypotheses about Table 1 provides a brief summary of the basic
SLA14 and recent research on the use of tenets of five hypotheses with regard to how
acoustically varied input during L2 learning in they have been applied to SLA. Possible im-
an effort to apply both general principles and plications for auditory training are described in
specific research findings from SLA to issues in the sections below.
auditory training. We limit our focus to audi-
tory training for adults with postlingual hearing
loss who have received cochlear implants or The Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis
hearing aids, as this group closely parallels the Krashens first hypothesis about SLA is that L2
L2 learner who is required to map new phono- learners are able to develop two distinct types of

Table 1 Summary of Krashens Hypotheses


Hypothesis Basic Tenets

Acquisition-learning hypothesis Learned (explicit) L2 knowledge is independent


from acquired (implicit) L2 knowledge.
Natural order hypothesis L2 learners acquire the rules of a language in
a predictable order.
Monitor hypothesis L2 learners may use explicit knowledge as
a monitor or editor during language production.
Input hypothesis L2 learners acquire language when they receive
comprehensible samples of
the target language.
Affective filter hypothesis Affective factors (e.g., increased anxiety) can
prevent input from becoming available to
the L2 learners language acquisition device.

Abbreviations: L1, first language; L2, second language.


LEARNING A SECOND LANGUAGE AND AUDITORY TRAINING/BARCROFT ET AL 153

knowledge about the L2 that they are acquir- potential for incidental learning. That is, simply
ing. Krashen refers to one type of knowledge as by being exposed to speech in their environ-
learning, or the act of obtaining conscious ment, even if it is background sound such as a
information about one or more aspects of the television playing or other people speaking,
language. An example of learning in this sense they may learn to recognize speech. Recent
would be if an L2 learner of English consciously work by Stelmachowicz et al,7 for example,
learns a rule indicating that the third-person s found a significant improvement in novel
should be added to verbs in sentences with word learning (i.e., learning of nonsense words
third-person singular subjects. This type of presented in a 4-minute slide slow) by hearing-
knowledge is distinct from a second type of impaired children following a 10-dB sound
knowledge, which Krashen refers to as acquis- pressure level (SPL) increase in presentation
ition. In contrast to learning, according to level. By implication, the audiologist might
Krashen, acquisition refers to the subconscious consider lowering the electrode thresholds
process of developing an internalized linguistic when programming a cochlear implant device
system over time, much like the process that so that more sound is available to the user.
children undergo during first language (L1) During auditory training, decreased emphasis
acquisition. An example of acquisition in this might be placed on explicit learning. Clinicians
sense would be if a learner of L2 English may engage in more informal instruction (i.e.,
becomes able to use English third-person s training activities might be incorporated into
correctly based on exposure to this structure in other activities, such as conversation or learn-
the input over time without ever learning a ing) as opposed to formal instruction (i.e.,
conscious rule related to the structure. training activities might not involve highly
According to Krashen, learning and ac- structured, drill-like activities).
quisition are two mutually exclusive knowledge From a broader perspective, application of
sources such that any learned knowledge cannot Krashens strong no-interface position to audi-
become acquired knowledge. This position is tory training requires consideration of an ap-
known as a strong no-interface position. A proach to auditory training that emphasizes
strong interface position, on the other hand, meaningful interaction during which improve-
would posit that learned knowledge can become ment in sound discrimination takes place as a
acquired knowledge quite readily. Weaker ver- natural by-product of the communicative use of
sions of the no-interface position would posit language. Some activities for auditory training,
that although much learned knowledge cannot such as continuous discourse tracking,8 lend
become acquired knowledge, a limited subset of themselves to this type of focus on real-world
learned knowledge may do so. meaning. Other activities, such as same-differ-
In the following discussion, we will use the ent discriminations, involve less focus on real-
term implicit learning to refer to what Krashen world meaning. Clinicians may note how cli-
referred to as acquisition and the term explicit ents respond differently when they are engaging
learning to refer to what Krashen referred to as in the use of language for authentic communi-
learning. Ellis6 defined implicit learning as ac- cation as opposed to only form-oriented drills
quisition of knowledge about underlying struc- in which focus on form becomes divorced from
ture of a complex stimulus environment by a focus on meaning. If a primary goal of auditory
process which takes place naturally, simply and training is to improve communication in the
without conscious operations and defined ex- real world, an approach that emphasizes suc-
plicit learning as a more conscious operation cessful perception of target structures in truly
where the individual makes and tests hypoth- meaningful contexts should help clients to
eses in a search for structure (p.1). develop this type of competence. From the
perspective of a strong no-interface position,
IMPLICATIONS OF THE LEARNING-ACQUISITION this type of competence depends on the devel-
HYPOTHESIS FOR AUDITORY TRAINING opment of unconscious, implicit knowledge, as
With advancements in device processing strat- the focus of the language user (listener or
egies, cochlear implant users now have greater speaker) outside of auditory training sessions
154 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 28, NUMBER 2 2007

is going to be on the use of language (percep- whether two sounds are the same or different),
tion or production) for truly communicative identification (ability to label some auditory
purposes. stimuli), and comprehension (ability to under-
stand meaning in a spoken message). The goal
of auditory training is to advance the client
The Natural Order Hypothesis from one stage to the next. Structured listening
A second hypothesis proposed by Krashen is tasks may initially present sounds and words
that L2 learners acquire the rules of a language that are comprised primarily of low-frequency
in a predictable order such that some rules tend sounds (e.g., /m/ and /mam/), with the assump-
to be acquired before others. An example of this tion that individuals learn to recognize those
would be Dulay and Burts9 demonstration that items before items comprised of higher fre-
L2 learners of English, regardless of their L1 quencies. With the advent of more sophisti-
(Spanish or Chinese), tended to follow a similar cated processing strategies, it is perhaps time to
sequence in their acquisition of various struc- reassess points made by Krashen and others (see
tures in English (> refers to earlier in acquisi Corder10) with regard to the learners internal
tion): (1) pronoun case > (2) article (a, the) syllabus.
> (3) progressive (-ing) > (4) contractible cop- For example, it may be that a new cochlear
ula (s) > (5) past regular (-ed) > (6) past irreg- implant user is immediately capable of com-
ular > (7) long plural (-es) > (8) possessive (s) prehension activities and that auditory train-
> (9) 3rd person (-s). From an instructional ing might focus on sound or word distinctions
perspective, the order in which L2 learners can that are presented in the context of meaning-
be expected to acquire different structures ful conversation or narrative comprehension
sometimes is referred to as the learners syllabus rather than in more drill-oriented formats (see
or the internal syllabus of the learner.10 Instruc- Tye-Murrays book13 for a review of tradi-
tors may be at a greater advantage if they are tional discrimination and identification activ-
more aware of what to expect with regard to the ities). Samedifferent discrimination tasks and
learners internal syllabus and acquisition or- word identification in closed-set formats may
ders. If the sequencing of a language course is at no longer be the optimal introduction to train-
odds with what we expect in terms of acquis- ing. Instead, an initial activity might focus on
ition orders, more difficulties may arise. Aware- comprehension activities. For instance, a client
ness of research findings on acquisition orders might be asked to comprehend the gist of a
on the part of instructors and L2 course devel- spoken sentence such as The boys walked to
opers can help to address this issue from a more school. Initially, it might not matter that the
informed perspective. The natural order hy- client heard the word boys as boy. Later, it
pothesis claims that learners have a predisposi- might be appropriate to focus the clients
tion to acquire structures in a particular order. attention on making fine-grained distinctions,
When provided with sufficient amounts of like listening for the /z/ and /s/ sounds that
meaning-bearing comprehensible input by the distinguish plural from singular forms. It also
instructor (and other sources of input), learners may be appropriate for clinicians to focus their
will acquire structures in this order naturally. clients attention on high-frequency sounds
early on in the program as most cochlear im-
IMPLICATIONS OF THE NATURAL ORDER plant processing strategies code high frequen-
HYPOTHESIS FOR AUDITORY TRAINING cies. For instance, a client who uses a cochlear
In many auditory training curricula,11,12 the implant may have the capability of discrimi-
assumption is made that persons who are learn- nating the words sip and ship, an initial
ing to listen, whether it is after a sudden consonant distinction that is not perceptible to
hearing loss or after receipt of a listening device, most hearing-aid users who have significant
pass through a hierarchy of auditory skill levels. hearing loss. A comprehensive assessment of
These levels may include sound awareness the listening capabilities of a new cochlear
(awareness of when sound is present and not implant user, as well as information about
present), discrimination (ability to indicate how his or her device is programmed, should
LEARNING A SECOND LANGUAGE AND AUDITORY TRAINING/BARCROFT ET AL 155

inform the clinician about an individuals in- Bradlow, Akahane-Yamada, Pisoni, and
ternal syllabus. Tohkura15 provide some evidence for positive
effects of phonemic training on speech produc-
tion in the absence of explicit instruction about
The Monitor Hypothesis speech production. In Bradlow et als study,
A third hypothesis put forth by Krashen is native Japanese learners of L2 English were
that although explicit knowledge (learning) trained on the English r/l distinction using
cannot become implicit knowledge (acquisi- minimal pairs (e.g., rocklock) discrimination
tion), L2 learners may use explicit knowledge with feedback. Pre- and posttraining measures
as a monitor or editor during language pro- of perception (accuracy of identifying novel r/l
duction. As Krashen14 explains, After a po- minimal pairs) and production (ratings of r/l
tential utterance is produced by acquired production by native English speakers) revealed
competence, the performer can refer to con- significant improvements in both perception
scious rules and make certain corrections be- and production, despite receiving no explicit
fore the utterance is spoken or written training on production. These results establish
(p. 46). Krashen also notes that to use explicit an important link between the production and
knowledge to monitor production, L2 learners perception of L2 and suggest that clinicians
must have enough time to do so, must know may use productive (in addition to receptive)
the conscious rule in question, and must be measures as an additional index of the benefits
focusing on form. of sensory aids.

IMPLICATIONS OF THE MONITOR HYPOTHESIS


FOR AUDITORY TRAINING The Input Hypothesis
With regard to aural rehabilitation and A fourth hypothesis proposed by Krashen is the
speech-language therapy in general, including input hypothesis. This hypothesis is central to
speech training, this hypothesis might predict Krashens explanation, in general terms, of how
that clinicians should not expect explicit in- L2s are acquired. The input hypothesis asserts
formation that they provide to clients (e.g., that we acquire language by comprehending
explicit information about how phonemic con- messages in comprehensible input, or samples of
trasts work in a language) to become implicit the target language that can be understood by
knowledge. This type of explicit information the learner. The input hypothesis also posits a
may be used by clients only to reflect upon or general mechanism, or a general metaphor, for
recall how the system works (monitoring) and how language acquisition takes place. According
cannot replace the implicit knowledge gained to this hypothesis, language acquisition pro-
through use of the target structure in commu- gresses when learners attend to input (samples
nicative contexts. With regard to auditory of the target language) that contains linguistic
training in particular, clinicians may notice data slightly beyond their current level of com-
that some clients, perhaps adults in particular, petence. Krashen refers to this type of input as
are interested in learning explicit information i 1. Krashen14 explains this general mecha-
about how a phonemic contrast or other aspect nism in the following manner: . . . if an acquirer
of speech works. The clinician may provide is currently at stage i, it is hypothesized that s/
this type of information to clients, perhaps for he can acquire i 1 if s/he understands input
the fact that the information itself is inherently containing i 1 (p. 46). The notion of i 1
interesting. Applying the monitor hypothesis, therefore serves as a general metaphor for how
however, explicit information of this nature L2 learners progress from lower to higher levels
will be available to clients only as a means of of proficiency in the gradual process of SLA.
monitoring how they should perceive speech
accurately and not as a means of developing IMPLICATIONS OF THE INPUT HYPOTHESIS FOR
the ability to do so in an increasingly fluid AUDITORY TRAINING
manner during communication in naturalistic The input hypothesis holds several implications
contexts. for auditory training. For instance, cycling is
156 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 28, NUMBER 2 2007

sometimes incorporated into the auditory train- (page 46). In a general sense, the main instruc-
ing program. The clinician works with the tional implication of this hypothesis is to create
client until the individual reaches a prescribed a classroom environment that is affectively
benchmark of success. Later, say in one or two positive, that increases the self-esteem of the
weeks, the clinician returns to the training learners, and that encourages the learner to feel
objective to provide reinforcement and addi- more like a member of the group that speaks
tional learning.16 The notion of building from the target language. In this way, following the
success also might be evident if the clinician Affective Filter Hypothesis, learners in the
interweaves activities that require listening only classroom will be less likely to raise their affec-
with activities that require both listening and tive filter and block input containing data
watching. For instance, a clinician might needed for the learner to progress in the L2
present a training task using vision and audition acquisition process.
to ensure that the client understands the task
and is familiar with the vocabulary and lan- IMPLICATIONS OF AFFECTIVE FILTERING FOR
guage structures. Once the client experiences AUDITORY TRAINING
success in an audition-plus-vision condition, Clinicians sometimes note anecdotally that
the task can be repeated in an audition-only auditory training has reduced their clients
condition. In this kind of scenario, the client is anxiety levels during everyday listening. With
continually pressed to perform just beyond his some individuals, listening becomes less effort-
or her current abilities while still receiving an ful following training, as they are able to devote
opportunity to experience success. The input more mental energy to comprehending the
hypothesis, as does the acquisition-learning meaning of the message and less effort toward
hypothesis, underscores the importance of identifying sounds and words spoken (see
meaningful listening. If the input is not mean- Witt18 for a case report). One major benefit
ingful, the spoken form in question cannot be an individual derives from auditory training is
attached to meaning. Techniques that might be an overall greater self-confidence in his or her
employed for making spoken language compre- ability to recognize speech, in addition to the
hensible include a structured communication series of specific perceptual improvements that
activity like Quest? AR.17 In Quest?AR, the a client gains over time. The individual may be
clinician asks a series of scripted questions (for less likely to raise what Krashen refers to as an
auditory training, it would be in an audition- affective filter, and more likely to process audi-
only format), and the client responds. The tory input with full mental powers. For new
general implication here is that in auditory device users, many of whom feel unsure about
training, like L2 learning, form should not be their new listening skills and carry the baggage
divorced from meaning. The input needs to be of having experienced communication break-
both meaning-bearing and comprehensible. downs repeatedly, a clinicians praise and en-
couragement might decrease anxiety and
increase the new users ability to process input.
The Affective Filter Hypothesis Means by which a clinician may lessen anxiety
The fifth and final of Krashens hypotheses is include focusing on the positive, providing test
the affective filter hypothesis. This hypothesis data (and error data from listening tests) about
posits that affective factors can prevent input how much better the individual performs with a
from becoming available to the L2 learners device than without it, and providing multiple
language acquisition device. Krashen14 explains opportunities for the client to experience suc-
this hypothesis more specifically as follows: If cess.
the acquirer is anxious, has low self-esteem,
does not consider him/herself to be a potential
member of the group that speaks the language, EFFECTS OF ACOUSTIC
s/he may understand the input, but it will not VARIABILITY
reach the language acquisition device a block, In the preceding section, we used Krashens
the Affective Filter, will keep the input out theoretical framework to discuss how general
LEARNING A SECOND LANGUAGE AND AUDITORY TRAINING/BARCROFT ET AL 157

principles of SLA might inform practices in with conditions in which words were spoken by
auditory training. We turn next to a specific the same talker or at the same speaking rate. A
instance of extending Krashens proposals about key manipulation in this study was the inter-
the role of input in SLA and why not all input is stimulus interval (ISI; i.e., time between items
created alike with regard to its effects on in a list). Nygaard et al reasoned that at short
acquisition. Specifically, we summarize research ISIs, listeners would not have sufficient time to
on L2 phonemic training19,20 and recent work encode either rate or talker information and
from our laboratories on acoustic variability and therefore would not exhibit a benefit of varia-
L2 word learning21,22 and then propose how bility. At longer ISIs, however, the researchers
research in both of these areas might have predicted that participants would have suffi-
important implications for auditory training. cient time to encode the variability and the
The findings of this research has demonstrated additional rate and talker information would
that the use of acoustic variability, or acousti- serve as supplemental retrieval cues to increase
cally varied presentation formats, is an effective recall performance. Consistent with these pre-
instructional technique for teaching learners L2 dictions, Nygaard et al found no benefit (and in
phonemic contrasts and new L2 vocabulary. some cases, a cost) for variable input (multiple
These findings reinforce Krashens hypothesis talkers and multiple rates) at ISIs below
about attending to the nature of input during 1000 milliseconds (ms), but improved recall
SLA but, in this instance, with regard to the performance for variable input at ISIs above
extent to which input is acoustically varied in 1000 ms. These findings suggest that at least
addition to being meaning bearing and com- under some circumstances varied, rather than
prehensible. consistent, input can produce improved mem-
A traditional view of learning is that re- ory performance for familiar L1 words.
peated presentations of verbal material increase Interestingly, research on L2 learning also
the strength of association between the pre- has demonstrated that acoustically varied input
sented items.23 According to this perspective, if can produce positive effects on learning L2
we wanted to teach the L2 learner of Spanish phonemic contrasts and L2 vocabulary learn-
the new form-meaning relationship pez ing. In the following sections, we provide back-
fish, we might repeatedly pair these two items ground about research on acoustic variability in
until the relationship had been acquired. Sim- L1 speech processing and describe recent re-
ilarly, if we wanted to teach the new user of a search supporting the beneficial effects of
sensory aid the relationship between the modi- acoustic variability in memory for L1 words,
fied (due to perception via the new device) L2 phonemic training, and L2 vocabulary
signal and a preexisting concept (e.g., fish) learning. Finally, we discuss how these findings
during auditory training, we would again re- might be applied to auditory training.
peatedly pair these two items. A hallmark of
both L1 and L2 training from this perspective
is input consistency, whereby repeated presen- Acoustic Variability and L1 Speech
tations of the identical stimuli add information Processing
to the organizational structure originally cre- Studies on L1 speech processing have demon-
ated for that item. Some studies on human strated that presenting words in acoustically
memory, however, have demonstrated that varied formats negatively affects performance
under some circumstances memory for an on L1 speech processing tasks such as vowel
item can improve when varied, as opposed to perception,26 word recognition, and word nam-
consistent, exemplars are presented during a ing.27,28 These effects have been produced us-
learning phase.24,25 To provide one example ing talker, speaking rate, or voice type as sources
of this stimulus variability effect, Nygaard, of acoustic variability. Other research, in con-
Sommers, and Pisoni25 used a serial recall task trast, has demonstrated that some sources of
to investigate whether variations in speaking variability render no effects on these same
rate and talker characteristics (voice) could dependent measures. Sommers, Nygaard, and
improve memory for spoken words compared Pisoni29 found null effects for amplitude
158 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 28, NUMBER 2 2007

variability on L1 word identification, as did portant extension to the work on acoustic


Sommers and Barcroft28 for fundamental-fre- variability and memory for L1 words because
quency variability. One explanation for this they demonstrate that at least some sources of
overall pattern of results is that only those acoustic variability can improve L2 learners
sources of acoustic variability that affect pho- ability to learn novel phonetic forms.
netically relevant properties of speech will pro-
duce decrements in L1 speech processing.29
Acoustic Variability and L2 Vocabulary
Learning
Acoustic Variability and Memory for A more recent series of experiments21,22 as-
L1 words sessed the effects of acoustic variability on L2
In contrast, considerable evidence is now avail- vocabulary learning. Barcroft and Sommers21
able to suggest that acoustic variability can examined the effects of variability in voice
improve memory for L1 words presented in type and talker on the ability of English
word lists. Mullennix, Pisoni, and Martin27 speakers to learn Spanish vocabulary. Speak-
demonstrated, for example, that memory for ing style (also referred to as voice type) and
words spoken by multiple talkers was signifi- talker were selected as sources of variability
cantly better than memory for words spoken by because they represent both intraspeaker
only a single talker. Similarly, Goldinger, Pi- (speaking style) and interspeaker (talker) var-
soni, and Logan24 found that when listeners are iations that occur naturally. The general
given sufficient time to encode voice character- methodology used in these experiments was
istics, serial recall is better for items spoken by to compare vocabulary learning in conditions
multiple talkers as compared with single talkers. with no variability (one talker or one speaking
This improved memory performance for acous- style), moderate variability (three talkers or
tically varied stimuli has been attributed to three speaking styles), and high variability (six
listeners ability to encode both linguistic and talkers or six speaking styles). For all sources
indexical information and to use the latter as an of variability, speed and accuracy of picture-
additional retrieval cue. to-L2 recall and L2-to-L1 translation were
dependent measures. The findings for both
sources of variability indicated a positive and
Acoustic Variability and L2 Phonemic additive effect of acoustic variability; the par-
Training ticipants were faster and more accurate in
Another body of research suggests that acous- both types of recall when they learned the
tically varied presentation formats also can be words in acoustically varied compared with
useful for teaching L2 phonemic contrasts, such acoustically consistent formats. Moreover,
as when training native Japanese speakers on learning performance with high variability
the English contrast between liquid consonants was better (faster and more accurate) than
/r/ and /l/. A series of studies has demonstrated for moderate variability, which, in turn, was
the effectiveness of using acoustically varied, as better than with no variability.
compared with acoustically consistent, presen- Sommers and Barcroft22 extended the in-
tation formats during training for this English vestigation of acoustic variability and L2 vo-
contrast19,20,30,31 (see also Hardison32). Of par- cabulary learning to three previously untested
ticular importance to the issue of auditory sources of variability: overall amplitude, funda-
training is that these studies demonstrate that mental frequency, and speaking rate. Sommers
(1) variable input during training facilitates and Barcroft hypothesized that two of these
listeners ability to perceive the contrasts pro- sources of variability, overall amplitude and
duced by novel talkers, (2) variability improves fundamental frequency, may not affect L2 vo-
long-term retention of the contrasts, and (3) cabulary learning. This prediction was based on
variability during perceptual training can lead earlier experiments29 designed to test the pho-
to improved production as assessed by intelli- netic relevance hypothesis for the effects of
gibility ratings. These findings provide an im- acoustic variability on L1 speech processing.
LEARNING A SECOND LANGUAGE AND AUDITORY TRAINING/BARCROFT ET AL 159

According to the phonetic relevance hypothe- a woman with a southern drawl (and hence,
sis, listeners will encode and retain indexical spoken with a heavily diphthongized vowel and
properties of the speech signal such as talker a higher fundamental frequency). In a Natural
characteristics and speaking rate that affect Speech Program, through the process of implicit
acoustic features, such as format frequencies learning (Krashens first hypothesis), Tye-Mur-
and transitions, which are used for phonetic ray et al33 proposed that listeners abstract the
identification. In contrast, sources of variability invariant aspects of words and their component
that do not alter phonetically relevant speech sounds by means of repeated exposures and
features, such as overall amplitude (overall am- varying contexts. This process should result in
plitude affects perceived loudness, but does not overall better listening performance in everyday
alter formant frequencies or other phonetically environments. In the original Natural Speech
relevant parameters of the speech signal), either Program, training activities included same-dif-
will be ignored or will be processed more ferent discrimination tasks and three-interval
automatically (in the same way that an auto- forced-choice drills.
matic gain control functions to maintain a In todays auditory training curriculum,
constant loudness level). In applying the pho- training programs that incorporate acoustic
netic relevance hypothesis to studies of acous- variability might be supplemented by activities
tic variability and L2 vocabulary learning, that are more meaningful, such as comprehen-
Sommers and Barcroft22 reasoned that if the sion-level activities (Krashens hypotheses 1, 2,
beneficial effects of talker and speaking-style and 4). Clinicians could use technology such as
variability are a result of listeners encoding and digitized speech samples stored on a computer
retaining phonetically relevant sources of vari- to present speech spoken by a variety of talkers,
ability (i.e., they serve as an additional retrieval with sounds and words presented in a variety of
cue), then variability based on sources that do contexts, as a means of incorporating acoustic
not affect phonetically important speech fea- variability into the learning experience. The
tures may not affect L2 vocabulary acquisition. training materials may be arranged along a
Consistent with this prediction, Sommers and hierarchy of listening difficulty, in considera-
Barcroft found that neither variations in overall tion of the clients internal syllabus (Krashens
amplitude nor variations in fundamental fre- hypothesis 2). Outside of the clinical setting,
quency affected L2 vocabulary learning based clients might be encouraged to interact with a
on the same dependent variables used by Bar- variety of talkers and in a variety of environ-
croft and Sommers.21 Speaking-rate variability, ments to reinforce skills developed in the clin-
however, produced positive effects on L2 vo- ical setting.
cabulary learning, as would be predicted by the
phonetic relevance hypothesis.
FUTURE RESEARCH ON ACOUSTIC
VARIABILITY AND AUDITORY
USE OF ACOUSTIC VARIABILITY TRAINING
IN AUDITORY TRAINING Whereas extant research on acoustic variabil-
Auditory training programs that rely on acous- ity and L2 learning suggest that talker, speak-
tic variability include the Natural Speech Pro- ing-style, and speaking-rate (but not
gram proposed by Tye-Murray, Tyler, Lansing, amplitude and fundamental-frequency) varia-
and Bertschy.33 Its premise was that individuals bility might help to facilitate auditory train-
learn by exploring and extracting invariance ing, new research on the effects of these
from their environment.34,35 Clients who re- sources of variability on auditory training is
ceive a cochlear implant learn to listen through clearly needed, to avoid overextending the
repeated exposure to sounds and words in current L2 findings. We are in the planning
different contexts and by different talkers. For stages for conducting new research on the use
example, the word rice is still the word rice, of talker variability during auditory training
regardless of whether it is spoken by a man with for clients who have cochlear implants and
a general American English accent or spoken by clients who are hearing impaired. If new
160 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 28, NUMBER 2 2007

research documents that benefits of acoustic 12. Stout G, Windle J. Developmental Approaches to
variability on L2 learning extend to auditory Successful Listening II. Englewood, CO: Resource
training, such a finding would afford two Point; 1992
important benefits. First, it would provide 13. Tye-Murray N. Foundations of Aural Rehabilita-
tion: Children, Adults, and Their Family Members.
direct evidence of the effectiveness of incor- Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Thomson Learning;
porating more acoustic variability within 2004
auditory training programs. Second, it would 14. Krashen S. The input hypothesis and its rivals.
suggest that at least some areas of research on In: Ellis NC, ed. Implicit and Explicit Learning
L2 learning may serve as initial testing of Languages. London: Academic Press; 1994:
ground for addressing issues in auditory 4577
training. 15. Bradlow AR, Akahane-Yamada R, Pisoni DB,
Tohkura Y. Training Japanese listeners to identify
English /r/ and /l/: Long-term retention of learning
in perception and production. Percept Psychophys
ABBREVIATIONS 1999;61:977985
16. Vergara KC, Miskiel LW, Oller K. CHATS: The
ISI interstimulus interval Miami Cochlear Implant Auditory and Tactile
L1 first language Skills Curriculum. Miami, FL: Intelligent Hearing
L2 second language Systems; 1994
SLA second language acquisition 17. Erber NP. Auditory Training. Washington, DC:
Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf;
SPL sound pressure level
1982
18. Witt S. Effectiveness of an Intensive Aural
Rehabilitation Program for Adult Cochlear
REFERENCES Implant users: A Demonstration Project [masters
thesis]. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa; 1997
1. Krashen S. The monitor model for adult second 19. Logan JS, Lively SE, Pisoni DB. Training Japanese
language performance. In: Burt Dulay MK, listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: A first
Finocchiaoro HCM, eds. Viewpoints on English report. J Acoust Soc Am 1991;89:874886
as a Second Language. New York: Regents; 1977 20. Lively SE, Logan JS, Pisoni DB. Training Japanese
2. Krashen S. Second Language Acquisition and listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: II. The role
Second Language Learning. Oxford: Pergamon; of phonetic environment and talker variability in
1981 learning new perceptual categories. J Acoust Soc
3. Krashen S. Principles and Practice in Second Am 1993;94:12421255
Language Acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon; 1982 21. Barcroft J, Sommers MS. Effects of acoustic
4. Krashen S. The Input Hypothesis. London: Long- variability on second language vocabulary learning.
man; 1985 Studies Second Lang Acquisition 2005;27:387414
5. Van Patten B. Input Processing and Grammar 22. Sommers M, Barcroft J. An integrated account of
Instruction. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1996 the effects of acoustic variability in L1 and L2:
6. Ellis NC. Implicit and Explicit Learning of evidence from amplitude, fundamental frequency,
Languages. London: Academic Press; 1994 and speaking rate variability. Applied Psycholin-
7. Stelmachowicz PG, Pittman AL, Hoover BM, guistics 2007;28:231249
Lewis DE. Novel-word learning in children with 23. Young DR, Bellezza FS. Encoding variability,
normal hearing and hearing loss. Ear Hear 2004; memory organization, and the repetition effect.
25:4756 J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1982;8:545559
8. DeFilippo CL, Scott BL. A method of hearing and 24. Goldinger SD, Pisoni DB, Logan JS. On the locus
evaluating the reception of ongoing speech. of talker variability effects in recall of spoken word
J Acoust Soc Am 1978;63:11861192 lists. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1991;17:
9. Dulay H, Burt M. Natural sequences in child 152162
second language acquisition. Lang Learn 1974;24: 25. Nygaard LC, Sommers MS, Pisoni DB. Effects of
3753 stimulus variability on perception and representa-
10. Corder SP. The significance of learners errors. Int tion of spoken words in memory. Percept Psycho-
Rev of Appl Linguist 1967;5:161169 phys 1995;57:9891001
11. Erber NP, Lind C. Communication therapy: 26. Assmann PF, Nearey TM, Hogan J. Vowel
theory and practice. J Acad Rehab Audiol 1994; identification: orthographic, perceptual, and acous-
27(Suppl):267287 tic aspects. J Acoust Soc Am 1982;71:975989
LEARNING A SECOND LANGUAGE AND AUDITORY TRAINING/BARCROFT ET AL 161

27. Mullennix JW, Pisoni DB, Martin CS. Some 31. Bradlow AR, Pisoni DB, Akahane-Yamada R,
effects of talker variability on spoken word Tohkura Y. Training Japanese listeners to identify
recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 1989;85:365378 English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual
28. Sommers MS, Barcroft J. Stimulus variability and learning on speech production. J Acoust Soc Am
the phonetic relevance hypothesis: Effects of 1997;101:22992310
variability in speaking style, fundamental fre- 32. Hardison DM. Acquisition of second-language
quency, and speaking rate on spoken word speech: effects of visual cues, context, and talker
identification. J Acoust Soc Am 2006;119:2406 variability. Appl Psycholinguist. 2003;24:495
2416 522
29. Sommers MS, Nygaard LC, Pisoni DB. Stimulus 33. Tye-Murray N, Tyler R, Lansing C, Bertschy M.
variability and spoken word recognition: I. Effects Programs in action: evaluating the effectiveness of
of variability in speaking rate and overall amplitude. auditory training stimuli using a computerized
J Acoust Soc Am 1994;96:13141324 program. Volta Rev 1990;92:2530
30. Lively SE, Pisoni DB, Yamada RA, et al. Training 34. Gibson EJ. Perceptual Learning and Development.
Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1969
III. Long-term retention of new phonetic catego- 35. Gibson JJ. The Ecological Approach to Visual
ries. J Acoust Soc Am 1994;96:20762087 Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1979

You might also like